BookPDF Available

Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives in Autobiographical Storytelling

Authors:

Abstract

Emotions have a life beyond the immediate eliciting situation, as they tend to be shared with others by putting the experience in narrative form. Narrating emotions helps us to express, understand, and share them: the way we tell stories influences how others react to our emotions, and impacts how we cope with emotions ourselves. In Emotion and Narrative, Habermas introduces the forms of oral narratives of personal experiences, and highlights a narrative's capacity to integrate various personal and temporal perspectives. Via theoretical proposals richly illustrated with oral narratives from clinical and non-clinical samples, he demonstrates how the form and variety of perspectives represented in stories strongly, yet unnoticeably, influence the emotional reactions of listeners. For instance, narrators defend themselves against negativity and undesired views of themselves by excluding perspectives from narratives. Habermas shows how parents can help children, and psychotherapists can assist patients, to enrich their narratives with additional perspectives. Table of contents: https://assets.cambridge.org/97811070/32132/toc/9781107032132_toc.pdf
Preface, content, chapter 1 - submitted manuscript, 2/2018. This material has been published in Emotion and
narrative by Tilmann Habermas. This version is free to view and download for personal use only. Not for re-
distribution, re-sale or use in derivative works. © Tilmann Habermas 2019
https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/emotion-and-narrative/51D839F5C9C5D00F2652B2EBE20817DA
Cambridge University Press
978-1-107-03213-2 — Emotion and Narrative
Tilmann Habermas
Frontmatter
More Information
www.cambridge.org
© in this web service Cambridge University Press
Emotion and Narrative
Perspectives in Autobiographical Storytelling
Tilmann Habermas
Goethe University Frankfurt
Cambridge University Press
978-1-107-03213-2 — Emotion and Narrative
Tilmann Habermas
Frontmatter
More Information
www.cambridge.org
© in this web service Cambridge University Press
University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom
One Liberty Plaza, 20th Floor, New York, NY 10006, USA
477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia
314–321, 3rd Floor, Plot 3, Splendor Forum, Jasola District Centre, New Delhi – 110025, India
79 Anson Road, #06-04/06, Singapore 079906
Cambridge University Press is part of the University of Cambridge.
It furthers the University’s mission by disseminating knowledge in the pursuit of edu-
cation, learning, and research at the highest international levels of excellence.
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781107032132
DOI: 10.1017/9781139424615
© Tilmann Habermas 2019
This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of
relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place
without the written permission of Cambridge University Press.
First published 2019
Printed and bound in Great Britain by Clays Ltd, Elcograf S.p.A.
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library.
ISBN 978-1-107-03213-2 Hardback
Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of
URLs for external or third-party internet websites referred to in this publication and
does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain, accurate or
appropriate.
Cambridge University Press
978-1-107-03213-2 — Emotion and Narrative
Tilmann Habermas
Frontmatter
More Information
www.cambridge.org
© in this web service Cambridge University Press
v
Contents
List of figures and tables page vii
Preface ix
Acknowledgments xi
List of narratives xiii
1 Emotions 1
PART I Emotions in oral autobiographical narratives 17
2 Narrative structure 19
3 Narrative evaluations 48
4 Narrative perspectives 70
PART II How narratives evoke emotions 93
5 Kinds of emotional effects of narratives 97
6 Narrative perspectives guide recipient emotions 122
7 Context and incongruencies also affect emotional
response 146
PART III Narratives reflect defense against emotions, and
narrating helps cope with them 177
8 Narratives reflect narrators’ ability to bear emotions 179
9 The healing power of narrating 203
10 Working through by narrating experiences repeatedly 225
Cambridge University Press
978-1-107-03213-2 — Emotion and Narrative
Tilmann Habermas
Frontmatter
More Information
www.cambridge.org
© in this web service Cambridge University Press
vi
PART IV Transformative co-narratives by parents and therapists 257
11 Co-narrating emotional events 259
12 Co-narrating in psychotherapy 276
13 Narrative perspectives in emotions 304
References 315
Index 345
Contents
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
2
Preface
“Tell me about it” – the background to this book is an interest in how sharing emotional
experiences with others helps dealing with them, emotionally, intellectually, and practically.
Emotions provide a fine system of signals to oneself. They call for turning our attention to
something that we need to deal with. Often it is not quite clear why a situation makes us feel
uneasy or arouses a specific emotion. Often we only notice them only later on and wonder
what they are about. Strong emotional experiences as well as unclear emotional experiences
motivate us to share them with others. To share experiences we use the text format narrative.
Sharing experiences is a frequent everyday activity. A specialized professional context has
emerged at the end of the 19
th
century which has specialized in this activity, psychotherapy,
and especially the talking cures that focus on understanding experiences. My clinical
background is psychoanalysis, and although this book does not focus on psychopathology or
clinical processes, writing the book is motivated by the wish to understand some of the
mechanisms by which psychoanalytic and similar insight-oriented psychotherapies work.
Psychoanalysis has little research to offer that actually studies the moment-to-moment
mechanisms of change. Studying the uses of narratives is one possible access to therapeutic
processes.
In psychology, the sharing of emotions is often considered as mere remembering or sharing,
but not as a linguistic communication with others in time. Emotions are often conceptualized
predominantly from evolutionary, biological, and cognitive perspectives. However, I argue in
this book, emotions are communications, to others and to ourselves. Because emotions react to
and evaluate events, to understand them we need to understand the sequence of events and
their implications. I argue that this requires a narrative format. It allows making sense of
emotions and communicating them. The narrative format requires spelling out the relevant
background which is necessary to understand the meaning of events for a given individual, it
relates the events, and it allows to transport, to interpret and to evaluate anew the events.
Therefore the emotion process has a narrative quality. And narrating the emotional experiences
has the power to transform them and the emotions they engender.
Emotion shares with narrative the basic temporal structure. In addition, emotion and
narrative are structured by social perspectives. For narrative this has long been recognized, but
not so for emotions. In this book I spell out which are the emotional consequences and the
antecedents of different perspectival architectures of narratives. It remains an open task to
detail the perspectival nature of not-yet-narrativized emotions.
Because narrative is a complex linguistic product and communication and is not easily
quantified, psychology has been slow in understanding its integrative potential. Besides offering
a model for a different, namely hermeneutic and ideographic psychology (termed Narrative
Psychology), narrative has become the object also of mainstream psychology in the fields of
cognitive, memory, developmental, personality and clinical psychology. In this book I argue that
also emotion psychology could greatly profit from acknowledging and studying the narrative
form and function of emotion and of the process of coping with emotions.
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
3
Chapter 1
Emotions
Emotions have long been neglected by psychology, at the times of behaviorism and also after
the cognitive turn. Emotions were and still are a central concern of psychoanalysis. This began
with the early cathartic method of re-experiencing traumatic events and thereby releasing
pent-up emotions. It continued with Freud’s basic assumption that although emotions appear
to be unmotivated in the neuroses, “the emotional state as such is always justified” (Freud,
1896, p. 346). Freud was saying that emotions can appear outside the context in which they
made sense. He did, however, never develop a specific theory of emotions. More recent
developments in psychoanalytic emotion theory do make use of psychological emotion theories
(e.g., Benecke & Damann, 2003; Benecke, Peham & Bänninger-Huber, 2005; Döll-Hentschker,
2008).
On the shoulders of important forerunners from the 1960s such as Silvan Tomkins and
Richard Lazarus, emotion psychology began to emerge as a vibrant field in the 1970s with Paul
Ekman and Carroll Izard. It evolved into a broad and established field in the 1980s. The
important contributors include Klaus Scherer (1983), who developed a taxonomy of situation
features which are used in appraising situations. This appraisal determines the quality of an
ensuing emotion. Andrew Ortony, Gerald Clore, and Allan Collins (1988) proposed a more
theory-driven systematization of appraisal features. Nico Frijda (1986) reviewed the entire field
to propose a model of emotion process. Keith Oatley and Philip Johnson-Laird (1987) proposed
a model of emotions as signalling the progress of goal-directed action sequences at certain plan
junctures, negative emotions signalling specific plan failure, positive emotions success. Emotion
psychology continues to be one of the fields of psychology that values theory most. Therefore,
emotion psychology can easily communicate across otherwise seemingly unsurmountable
disciplinary boundaries. Its reach extends to the humanities such as philosophy, sociology, and
literary studies, and also to the sciences such as physiology and neurology.
This book is about the central roles of narrative and emotions for each other. This first
chapter provides a basic conception of emotions. It contextualizes the topic and thesis of this
book in emotion psychology. I will argue in this chapter for an understanding of emotion as a
communicative phenomenon, which places narrative in a central position in the emotion
process compared to individualistic conceptions of emotion. This idea goes back to the pre-
history of modern emotion psychology, to Charles Darwin’s (1872) book on the expression of
emotions and its elaborations by Wilhelm Wundt and George Herbert Mead. From there I will
use Richard Lazarus’ theory to embed emotions in social relationships. Finally, writings by Keith
Oatley (and later on Peter Goldie) will serve to introduce narrative as the ideal format for
understanding emotions. The basic thesis of this book is that human emotions are intricately
structured by social perspectives, which in turn, I will argue in this chapter, follows from the
communicative nature of emotions. I will unfold the details of this argument throughout the
main body of the book.
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
4
Emotion as pre-reflective communication: Darwin, Wundt, Mead
Charles Darwin (1872) framed emotions from an evolutionary perspective, constructing
parallels between human facial and gestural emotional expressions and behaviors of animals in
specific situations. He argued that human expressions of emotions were inherited remnants of
the initial phases of actions that once served to prepare the ensuing actions. The snarling of
dogs and baring of their teeth serves to prepare an attack by biting the other. Similarly, humans
bare their teeth when they get angry, although they only bite metaphorically by using language.
Darwin argued that these habitual preparatory, truncated actions expressed the underlying
emotion that motivated the action. The forms of emotion expression were passed on to the
next generation, which is actually a rather Lamarckian argument. Due to their inherited basis
these forms are universal.
Contrasting the still popular assumption that emotions are irrational und dysfunctional,
Darwin closely linked them to our inherited outfit that, at least, originally had been functional,
and is still mostly functional. The evolutionary and situational usefulness of emotions as
preparation for action is the cornerstone of functional theories of emotion (e.g., Izard, 1991).
Secondly, linking emotions to inherited bodily movements stresses the involuntary and bodily
aspects of emotions. Thirdly, relating specific facial expressions to specific emotions implies
that emotions differ qualitatively, and not only quantitatively from each other, which is a
position held today by various theories of basic emotions. Darwin laid the basis, but stopped
short of developing a communicative theory of emotions himself (Ekman, 1998).
In contrast to Darwin’s view of emotions as qualitatively distinct, Wilhelm Wundt (1887)
defined emotions in his early, physiologically oriented phase as differing quantitatively from
each other on the three subjectively perceived dimensions of valence, arousal-inhibition, and
tension-relaxation. Turning towards a psychology of culture and society, Wundt later
postulated language to be a necessary element of emotion. To phylogenetically reconstruct the
transition from instinctually regulated animal interaction and intentional human
communication, Wundt (1900) linked instinctually guided actions to an accompanying emotion.
Following Darwin, he suggested that emotions, or what he termed affect, is expressed through
physiological innervation and expressive movements of the early phases of these instinctually
guided actions. Wundt suggested that the expressive innervations and movements that most
form the basis of language are facial and gestural affective expressions of emotions, because
they not only express the intensity of emotions, but also qualities. Subsequently, these
movements may also express ideas that accompany emotions (Wundt, 1900, p. 238ff.).
According to Wundt, the qualitative transformation of a merely expressive movement into a
sign that means something to an other is made through the imitation of the expressive
movement by that other, which evokes the same emotion and idea in that other. To this the
other may associate related ideas. When these are expressed, they no longer solely constitute
an imitation, but an answer. The initially individual affect is transformed into a shared affect
through the ensuing back and forth of expressive facial and gestural movements, which keeps
changing as the dialogue of gestures continues. Because ideas begin to prevail affects, this
affective sharing turns into shared thinking, or a dialogue of ideas (Wundt, 1900, p. 240).
Consequently, Wundt identified interjections, vocatives, and imperatives as the first vocal
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
5
expressions carrying meaning, followed by imitative sounds, onomatopoetic words, and vocal
gestures.
Thus, Wundt followed Darwin in conceiving facial movements and gestures as expressions
of emotions. Similar to Darwin, he considered them to be derived from the initial phases of
truncated actions. Wundt used this conception to construct a theory of the origins of language
as the basis for culture. He shared Darwin’s idea that facial and gestural emotional movements
primarily served to express emotions and had originally been the initial part of an action. Going
beyond Darwin, Wundt suggested that in a second phylogenetic step emotional expressive
movements, especially vocal gestures, signalled an emotion and the corresponding ideas to
another organism who would then respond. Emotional expressions thus secondarily turned into
signs used for communication.
George Herbert Mead (1904) revised Wundt’s phylogenetic reconstruction of how the
expression of emotions turned into communication and then into verbal communication by
reversing their order. Mead (1910) argued that the transition of gestures from mere expression
of the physiological arousal to becoming signs carrying meaning, presupposed that they had
evolved in a preceding social interaction with others. To liberate interaction from being guided
by instinct-guided stimulus-response sequences, the other had to understand the beginning of
an action as signalling an action readiness and the complete action to come. Even animals react
to each others’ action tendencies and change the course of their own actions in response, such
as when fighting or courting.
Mead (1912) proposed that vocal gestures were ideal for rendering the meaning of one’s
own gestures accessible to oneself, because only they could be perceived by the self as easily as
by an other. Adopting another’s reaction to one’s own gesture would enable the individual to
gain consciousness of the meaning of a gesture and turn it into a (verbal) symbol that could be
used with conscious intentions to communicate (Mead, 1925).
Three aspects of this reconstruction are important for understanding emotions. Firstly,
emotions can only arise once social interaction is no longer dictated by stimulus-response
sequences, but becomes open to a negotiation of gestures, in which beginning actions are
accompanied by sensations at the fringes of the field of consciousness (James, 1890). Neither
these pre-reflective feelings nor their facial and gestural expressions are self-conscious in the
sense that they could be perceived by oneself as by an other. Therefore, they cannot easily be
expressed linguistically by naming them. Rather, as in social interaction of mammals, human
emotions usually evoke pre-reflective reactions from others in a communication of non-verbal
emotional expressions (Mead, 1934, p. 147ff.). This communication is based on the objective,
that is, pragmatic meaning of initial phases of acts. Thus, emotions are basically a
communicative phenomenon that serves to coordinate members of the same species. This
model implies that actions need to remain incomplete so that there is room for emotions in the
wake of actions. Mead (1910) attributed the incompleteness of actions to conflicts arising
within the course of action. He argued that conflicts would most likely arise when interacting
with other organisms, but not when interacting with the natural environment. In social
interactions, the other’s reaction to the individual’s beginning action motivates a change in the
course of action.
Secondly, consciously taking the perspective of an other requires inhibiting action, which
allows looking back onto one’s action tendencies and reacting to them as an other would. Only
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
6
this enables individuals to identify a vague feeling as a specific emotion. This self-consciousness
may accompany acting, but can also disappear when the individual is fully immersed in an
activity (Mead, 1912). Thus, for emotions to be reflected-upon and become conscious, they
require not only the inhibiting or truncating of a full act, but also the taking of the perspective
of a specific or a generalized other.
Third, Mead identified the taking of the perspective of an other onto one’s own action
tendencies as constituting the perspective from which James’ Me is constituted, the reflectively
conceived and judged own person. In contrast, the I represents the spontaneous action
tendencies. Insofar as spontaneous action tendencies have an evaluative component towards
the situation and its objects, so do the perspectives taken onto one’s own action tendencies
and emotions, resulting from others’ evaluative reactions. If the perspective taken is not that of
a specific other, but of a generalized other, then the individual is enabled to judge her or his
own tendencies on the basis of impersonal attitudes, rules, and moral considerations (Mead,
1934). Thus, the self-reflective evaluation of emotions also relies on perspective taking. Mead
linked both consciousness of one’s emotions as well as judgments of their desirability and
appropriateness to the taking of perspectives.
This far, emotions are considered an integral part of social interactions, supporting the
ongoing mutual accommodation of actions. Therefore, emotions are temporally located and
extended. They are motivated and point to ensuing actions. Emotions are structured by social
perspectives, indicating one individual’s action tendency towards another individual.
Consciousness of emotions implies taking the perceptual perspective of the other to whom the
emotion is signalled. Finally, judging one’s emotions requires also taking the perspective of the
others’ interests, which determine their significance for others.
Situation appraisal and prototypical relational themes: Lazarus
The present-day dominant view conceives emotions as resulting from the evolution of a
functional system of semi-automatic action tendencies and as consisting of three elements:
First somatic, mainly physiological changes, second facial and corporal expressive movements,
and third a subjective feeling component (Frijda, 1986). This tri-componential view was already
present in Mead’s theorizing. It liberates the concept of emotion from relying exclusively on the
subjective feeling, and from any one specific element, as it can be signalled by any of the
components. This allows a functional interpretation of emotion. Also, together with the two
levels of consciousness associated with emotions, this conception allows for only apparently
contradictory concepts such as unconscious emotions. However, as emotions were not the
focus of Mead’s theorizing, he considered them only in their role for the reconstruction of a
natural history of symbolic interaction and the self.
What remains mysterious when Darwin’s, Wundt’s, and Mead’s phylogenetic theorizing is
applied to fully socialized human beings, is the initial motivation of the inhibited action. Wundt
and Mead had rooted motivation in drives. However, it appears implausible that any motivated
action involves an initial phase that may turn into an emotion if only the action is interrupted or
inhibited. Rather a flow of actions or interactions is usually structured by intentions (Oatley &
Johnson-Laird, 1987 – see below), and emotional reactions are evoked if a situation comes up
which hinders pursuing these intentions in a specific way. Emotions thus have an intentional
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
7
structure in that they always refer to a situation. This insight led Sigmund Freud to revise his
early theory of anxiety as automatically resulting from the inhibition of physical (sexual)
tension, to which emotion served as an alternative discharge path. In his revised theory, Freud
(1926) conceived anxiety as a reaction to interpreting a situation as dangerous based on earlier
life experiences. This interpretation generates a weak anxiety signal, which calls for shifting
attention to the imminent danger and motivates reacting to it. Only if the signal is ignored, or
the danger not removed, does anxiety grow in intensity.
Freud complemented the social communication view of emotion with an intrapsychic
communication view. However, in this view emotions signal an aspect of a situation to oneself
that urgently requires attention. While emotions communicate pragmatic requirements to the
self, they communicate the corresponding action tendencies to others.
In psychology, Richard Lazarus and Susan Folkman (1984) elaborated this role of emotions
for communicating to oneself evaluations of the present situation. In their model, emotions
have a motivational background, termed personal interests or concerns (Frijda, 1986). Emotions
signal a situation in which something happens that affects these personal concerns. Appraisal
theories assume that humans continuously scan situations for their relevance to their concerns.
Lazarus differentiated two phases of appraisal. Primary appraisals regard the relevance of the
situation to concerns, specifically whether the situation is congruent or incongruent with these
concerns. Primary appraisals also assess, in which way concerns are affected by the situation.
This determines which emotion is produced. If the continuous interpretation and evaluation of
situations leads to a primary appraisal, a subsequent secondary appraisal judges additional
aspects of the situation, namely blame and credit, possibilities for coping, and anticipation of
how events will turn out, positively or negatively. If this second judgment also fulfills the
conditions for evoking an emotion, the three elements of emotions, physiological arousal,
corporal expression, and feeling are produced with a specific quality, preparing the individual
for the corresponding class of actions. When a situation is judged to be threatening the
individual’s concerns and cannot be easily dealt-with, fear or anxiety results, which prepares
the individual for action directed at escaping or diverting the threat.
Most appraisal theories assume a set of basic, qualitatively different emotions. The strong
version of these theories assumes basic emotions which are universal and biologically based
(e.g., Ekman, 1984). Appraisal theories also differ as to the number and nature of the cognitive
processes involved in the appraisal process. Many theories suggest a set of situation
components, which are tested sequentially (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988; Scherer, 1984;
Scherer, Wallbott & Summerfield, 1986; Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer & Frijda, 2013). Others have
proposed more holistic matching processes, comparing situations to prototypical situations
(e.g., Russell & Barrett, 1999).
In popular depictions of emotion-eliciting situations, a human encounters a dangerous
animal such as a bear (James, 1884) or a snake (LeDoux, 1998). The vast majority of emotional
situations, however, involves other human beings with whom one communicates (Scherer et
al., 1986). Consequently, Lazarus (1991) defined prototypical emotion eliciting situations in
terms of core relational themes.
There are two communicative functions of emotions: signalling others what we are about to
do, and signalling to ourselves the presence of a situation that requires attending. Confronting
these two functions, it might seem that the taking of the perspective of others appears late in
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
8
the emotion process and therefore may not be essential to it. The appraisal of the situation
necessary for self-communication logically precedes the expression of the emotion which is
necessary for communicating it to others. However, three arguments speak for the essential
role of perspective taking in the emotion process.
In concordance with Mead, a first argument is that situation appraisals may lead to
physiological and expressive action tendencies which are not inhibited but the action is actually
carried out. In that case, the action tendency is communicated to the other while the individual
is unaware of the emotion. Often only the effect of the action, the other’s reaction, enables the
individual to realize the emotion that drove the action. For example, I may raise my voice and
attack the other before I become aware of being angry. In this constellation, emotion first
communicates to the other, and to the self only secondarily.
Second, physiological reaction and corporal expression are aspects of the same process, and
do not follow from feeling, but are, as many theories argue, the immediate object of the
sensations that form the basis of feelings (James, 1884; 1890). Identifying a feeling requires
understanding the meaning of expressive and physiological reactions for the other. Once learnt,
expressive and physiological reactions may be interpreted without taking the perspectives of
specific others in a given situation.
Perspective-taking is involved not only in the communication and becoming aware of
emotions, but, third, perspective taking is also required by the appraisal process itself, at least
in many relevant cases, if not in all cases. In the case of fear, interpreting a situation as
threatening requires understanding the intentions of others or anticipating natural events. In
the case of self-conscious emotions such as pride, shame, and guilt, the evaluation of oneself or
one’s acts by others is essential. Appraising angering situations most often requires judging
aggressors’ intentions and their capability of foreseeing the consequences of their actions. This
is necessary to judge their responsibility for the harm they have done. Understanding
saddening events often requires comparing past states of having something or being with
someone with present or future states in which this is lost and absent.
Furthermore, if emotions are basically part of an ongoing process of negotiation between
two or more individuals, the situation to be appraised most often centrally includes others’
emotions. Therefore, understanding others’ emotions is an essential element of appraisals.
Understanding emotions includes the simple identification of others’ emotion expressions. A
more complete understanding of others’ emotions requires understanding the reasons for the
emotions, by seeing how others interpret and evaluate a situation. This understanding of
others’ emotions in turn may motivate an emotional reaction by the individual.
In his later work, Lazarus (Lazarus, 1991; 1999; 2006) argued that the emotion process
needs to be extended beyond the immediate situation. While a focus on emotion expression
suggests that emotions only last seconds to minutes (Ekman, 1984, cf. Scherer, 2009), Frijda
(1986, p. 40f.) discusse a more extended duration of action tendencies, basically binding it to
the duration of the eliciting situation. Lazarus argues that the temporal frame for analysing
emotions needs to be extended to cover the reasons for different individuals to appraise similar
situations differently. Individuals interpret situations on the background of their biographical
experiences. These create highly individual expectations, sensibilities, and relationship patterns.
A critical remark by a friend may induce fear of what more is to come, anger about the breach
of perceived norms of friendship, or sadness about the friend’s negative opinion. The emotional
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
9
reaction also depends on the emotional dispositions of general anxiousness, irritability, and
depressiveness of the individual. Tomkins (1978) speaks of nuclear scripts and De Sousa (1987)
of paradigm scenarios to refer to individual prototypical situations that are extracted from
repeated emotional experiences early in life, which are used to interpret the emotional
meaning of later situations.
In addition, Lazarus (1999) argues that the concept of emotion process needs to be
temporally extended to include the entire process of coping with the emotion-eliciting situation
and with the ensuing emotion. If coping is difficult, it may extend for a long time. This is
especially true if problem-focussed coping is unsuccessful and individuals resort to assimilative
strategies of emotion-focussed coping, trying to adapt to the situation and change their
emotional evaluation. These coping efforts may be directed at any phase of the emotion
process, by reassessing the situation, reinterpreting the arousal, expression, or feeling, and by
altering the action tendency.
The strongest argument for extending the temporal frame of the emotion process is that
emotions are usually embedded in an ongoing communication, in which emotions mutually
influence each other. Communication tends to be more emotional when it is with significant
others with whom we share relationships which extend back in time and have developed
specific expectations and obligations.
These considerations led Lazarus (1999) to suggest that emotions might best be studied by
using narratives. Narratives provide a background in terms of pre-history, characters, and
context; they provide a provocation of the emotion, and a story of the attempts to cope with it.
Lazarus conceived prototypical narratives for each of the most important emotions. Indeed,
appraisal theorists use autobiographical narratives to study the kinds of situations and their
aspects that tend to evoke specific emotions (e.g., Scherer, Wallbott, & Summerfield, 1986).
However, Lazarus stopped short of suggesting that narratives might play a role in the emotion
process itself, limiting its use to serving as a tool for research, but not for the individual
confronted with an emotional situation.
Fictional narratives form sensibilities and help understand emotions:
Oatley
Among emotion psychologists, Keith Oatley (1992) has most explicitly attributed narrative a
genuine role in in the emotion process. This derives from his conception of emotions as action-
sequences which are structured by intentions and their outcome. Oatley and Johnson-Laird
(1987) couched their emotion theory in terms of goals and plans, emotions serving to evaluate
the progress of goal pursuit at junctures of the path towards goal fulfilment. Junctures are
points in the quest for reaching a goal where there is a significant change in the assessment of
the outcome of the plan.
True to the computer metaphor of cognitive psychology, Oatley conceived reading fictional
literature as a simulation of real life, the only simulation that can match the complexity of real
life. Oatley followed Rumelhart (1975), Stein and Glenn (1979), and Trabasso (Trabasso & van
den Broek, 1985) in conceiving stories as being constructed like a plan. Thus, stories are the
natural medium to explore the intricacies of human intentions and wishes, how they interact
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
10
between individuals over the course of time. And they are the ideal medium to understand
emotions. Emotions require an effort to understand the personal meaning of experiences,
because they are, more often than not, oblique to us. Although we have a hunch how we feel,
we often do not know why, or what the object of our emotion is.
For Oatley (1992), sometimes feelings are experienced without knowing why. He suggests
conceiving of emotions without necessary reference to an object, or eliciting situation. Feelings,
he argues, may be identified without their object, and then the individual may ask for the
matching object. We may feel sad or happy without knowing the reason. But this way of
speaking suggests that we have a hard time thinking of emotions without thinking of what they
evaluate. When we are sad we look for what we have lost or what we have been disappointed
by. If we do not succeed, we think of the emotion as unjustified. We may try to suppress it, and
may even think that we are too sensitive or that we are distorting reality by reacting, for
instance, with sadness to experiences which do not justify being sad. But I will discuss the need
to justify emotions later in chapters 7 and 11. Of importance at this point is that fictional
narratives which we read in books or watch as films, offer plots that lay out complex webs of
wishes and their progressive interweaving, the success of which is only known in the end.
Fictional narratives offer a playground on which we can exercise appraising, interpreting, or
simply understanding complex situations that elicit complex emotions. To understand these
emotions one needs to understand not only the immediately preceding situation, but the entire
story. The only apparently old-fashioned conclusion by Oatley (2011) is that reading novels
helps promote understanding of social interaction, relationships, other people and oneself,
especially their emotions (cf. e.g., De Mulder, Hakemulder, van den Berghe, Klaassen, & van
Berkum, 2017; Mumper & Gerrig, 2017)
Keith Oatley propagates a central role of narrative in eliciting, processing, and
communicating emotions. However, he is exclusively concerned with fictional, literary
narratives which we consume. The objects of this book, in contrast, are personal
autobiographical narratives. One difference to novels is that they are told with a claim for
truthfulness and authenticity. Another difference is that individuals not only listen to, but also
actively produce these narratives. People narrate emotional and problematic experiences
frequently (Rimé, 2009), to express emotions, to understand emotions, and to share the
experience in the hope that it will help them to cope with the event and the emotions it elicits.
The literature concerned with this function of everyday narrating, mostly ignores the specific
structure and potential of narratives, treating them as an emotional outlet or cognitive device.
Overview of the argument
My central argument is that narrative is central to emotion, that emotion in turn is also for
making a point in narratives, and that perspectives are a central structuring device for both.
After I have outlined three elements of narratives, specifically, narrative sequence, evaluation,
and perspectives, the argument will take three steps. I will start with the relation of recipients’
emotions to qualities of the narrative text, moving on to the narrators’ emotions prior to,
during, and following the narrative process and their relation to the narrative text, and finish
with the active contribution of listeners to narrating emotions.
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
11
On the receiving side, the reader/listener co-determines which emotions she or he
experiences by taking a specific perspective, like that of a protagonist, for example, an art
consumer who judges the aesthetic qualities of the narrative, or a moral stance. The first thesis
of the book is that readers’/listeners’ emotions are influenced by which perspectives narratives
offer and which they do not offer. This applies not only to readers of fiction, but also to
listeners of everyday autobiographical accounts. I propose a rough tripartite categorization of
degrees of perspective representation in texts. First, a diversified representation of different
temporal and personal perspectives (comprehensive perspective representation) evokes more
overall empathy and a moderate emotional reaction. Second, restriction to a partial subjective
protagonist perspective (dramatic narration) elicits stronger emotions of empathy and
sympathy with the past protagonist. Third, a behavioral perspective without subjective
perspectives (impersonal narration) tends to elicit interactional rather than empathic emotions
that are directed not empathically at a protagonist, but directly at the present narrator.
Narrating emotional experiences may help the narrator to master the narrated situation and
its emotional impact. The second thesis of the book is that the degree to which the narrator
succeeds in taking a variety of perspectives in narrating an event reflects the maturity of
defense mechanisms used and indicates how well the narrator has coped with the event. This
implies that narrating experiences is more helpful when diverse perspectives are represented in
the narrative. This resonates with clinical theories of impaired empathy and mentalizing in
more severe psychopathology (Kernberg, Fonagy) as well as with theories of the use of emotion
and insight in narratives for coping (Pennebaker, Stiles). Thus, it is the more or less partial
exclusion of perspectives from narratives that communicates to the listener the emotions that
the narrator has defended against.
While the first two theses abstract from the interactional nature of narrating by focussing
on the relation between text and recipient and text and narrator, the third thesis regards
listeners’ role in the shaping of the narrative text and in influencing narrators’ abilities and
proclivities. Listeners not only facilitate narrating by demonstrating interest and accepting and
reinforcing narrators’ emotional stance and evaluations of events. There are special
asymmetrical situations in which a more mature or a professionally skilled listener aims at not
only emotionally supporting the narrator, but at modifying the narrator’s story so as to help
with coping with the event and to improve the narrator’s narrative and coping abilities. The
third thesis of the book is that in socializing interactions between parents and children or
adolescents, and therapy talk between narrating patients and co-narrating psychotherapists,
listeners influence both the narrative and narrators abilities by challenging parts of the
narrative and complementing perspectives. In both cases, co-narrators focus on the processing
of emotions through the building of narratives. However, they also differ in the degree to which
listeners challenge narrators’ perceptions. Thus, this section pursues the idea that narrative co-
construction is not only one possible, but the decisive communicative process through which
emotion-regulation is learned and through which insight-oriented psychotherapies may heal
mental disorders.
The book is divided into four sections. Part I provides an overview of how emotions are
represented in narratives, providing a toolkit for later parts to relate perspectives in narrative
texts both to recipient and producer emotions. It also provides an overview of useful concepts
for the formal analysis of autobiographical narratives.
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
12
Chapter 2 presents the overall structure of narratives, especially oral narratives (Propp,
Todorov, Labov, story grammarians), and the place of emotions in narratives. The kind of
situation narrated may more or less resemble the prototypical situation for one emotion or
another.
Chapter 3 deals with ways to represent emotion in narratives. Some are not perspective-
related, such as nonverbal means, evaluative devices such as adverbs, intensifiers, lexicon
(Labov), figurative language (Kövecses) and other rhetorical means such as repetition and
ellipsis. Narrative focus is perspective-related (Genette, Bal, Schmid) and is implied by the
information listeners are provided, ranging from a behavioral (no perspectives) to a subjective
(one protagonist perspective) to an omniscient perspective (all perspectives).
Chapter 4 is more formal, and introduces linguistic means to convey explicit perspectives,
such as general evaluations, naming emotions and mental verbs (perceptual, emotional,
cognitive, volitional), which may refer to different temporal and personal perspectives. In
addition, dramatic narrating is presented as inducing emotions in the listener by pulling her or
him into the perspective of a specific protagonist (historic present, shift of origo of deictic
terms, direct speech).
Part II is dedicated to the narrative emotions of recipients in relation to the formal
properties of narratives. This part introduces various narrative emotions (chapter 5) and
discusses thesis 1 that perspective representation is a central narrative mechanism of eliciting
emotions (chapter 6), and complements it with other mechanisms (chapter 7).
Chapter 5 introduces six kinds of recipient emotions, including direct reactions to the
narrated scene and protagonists, aesthetic emotions, empathetic emotions, interactional
emotions directed at the narrator, and emotions stemming from personal autobiographical
memories that are elicited by the narrative. Narrative emotions will be differentiated from non-
narrative emotions in terms of perspectives involved. Then psychological theories of
empathetic narrative emotions, involving concepts of empathy, sympathy, identification,
immersion, transportation, and distance (Oatley, Green) will be related to text qualities and
transferred from fictional narratives to autobiographical narratives.
Chapter 6 introduces the emotional effects of different kinds and degrees of emotion
representation in narratives. Differences between everyday oral narratives and cultural
products such as literature and film are spelled out to prepare the grounds for the global thesis
that the degree and diversification of perspectives represented in a text influences recipient
emotions. This will be exemplified by autobiographical narratives. Other aspects of narratives
that also influence recipient emotions such as the severity of the complication, the moral
evaluation of characters and the general liking of characters are complemented.
Chapter 7 takes up some of the complications regarding my thesis encountered in chapter 6,
expanding the model to sustain the main thesis. Three aspects of oral narratives that may
modify the effect of perspectives on emotions are discussed. A lack of plausibility and
believability taints possible empathic appeals of narratives. Also, if protagonist or narrator
exhibit emotions that are deemed inappropriate on various grounds, recipients may be put off
and refuse to react with empathic emotions. Incongruencies in the communication of emotions
between verbal, paraverbal, and nonverbal modalities may negatively affect believability and
thereby also the emotional reaction to the narrative. Finally, I discuss how some narratives may
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
13
appear as a defensive, implausible, or insane story in everyday communication, but in other
contexts may actually be appreciated as aesthetically appealing.
Part III turns to the psychology of the narrator. The focus shifts from the effects of narrative
form on recipients to what different forms of narrating tell us about the narrator and to how
the process of narrating changes the form of narrative and what this does to the narrator.
Chapter 8 turns to the influence of the narrator’s personality and mental health on the
quality of narrative and the narrative representation of perspectives. Besides influences of the
narrators’ personality and their present emotional state, thesis 2 states that the degree of
mental disturbance is related to the degree to which narrative perspectives are excluded from
autobiographical narratives for defensive purposes. The background to this contention is the
clinical experience that the severity of psychopathology, or the immaturity of defense
mechanisms, is related to deficits in the ability to empathize (Kernberg) or mentalize (Fonagy).
This is complemented by the clinical experience (e.g., Kernberg) that the patient’s severity of
psychopathology relates to the strength of emotional counter-transference reactions of the
therapist. This is concordant with thesis 1, which proposes that as more perspectives are
excluded, involuntary emotional recipient emotions increase strengthen. I delineate the five
most important aspects of narratives that may be affected and distorted by defensive
processes.
Chapter 9 reviews theories and evidence regarding the cathartic, cognitive, and social
mechanisms of the activity of narrating emotional events that lead to improved coping and
beneficial effects on the life of the narrator (Pennebaker, Rimé). Narrating problematic
experiences is one of the central mechanisms of change in insight-oriented psychotherapies. I
then discuss which narrative form might indicate successful coping with experiences, while
considering various aspects of good stories or stories with a closure. Finally, I review evidence
on the nature of narratives that help processing problematic experiences, considering the
taking of an outside perspective and the switching of perspectives as most efficient
mechanisms.
Chapter 10 focusses specifically on the repeated narrating of experiences which is involved
in coping with specific experiences. Diverse factors such as memory, social validation, and
emotional coping influence how narratives change over repeated tellings. However, the change
of narratives in the process of coping through repeated telling is not a linear, but probably a
curvilinear process. I borrow Stiles’ model of the assimilation of problematic experiences in
psychotherapy to suggest that actually narrating an event, then integrating more perspectives
to provide a fuller account of the event, and finally reducing the narrative to a simple,
straightforward account might be the normative trajectory of narratives in successful coping.
The final part IV explores the active role of recipients as co-narrators in changing not only
narratives of specific events, but also changing narrators’ abilities to narratively process
experiences through narrating. Thesis 3 suggests that a major mechanism for the helpful adding
of perspectives to narratives (thesis 2) is an active co-narrating by recipients. I then pursue and
illustrate the thesis in two contexts that aim at changing narrators’ abilities, socialization and
psychotherapy.
Chapter 11 reviews the necessary involvement of listeners in everyday narrative practices in
experimental and sociolinguistic studies in natural contexts. Building on extensive work on the
socializing effects of mothers’ co-narrating with their preschool-age children, I propose a
Tilmann Habermas Emotion and Narrative: Perspectives on Autobiographical Storytelling
14
taxonomy specifically of co-narrative interventions aiming at helping narrators cope with their
emotions and the experiences which elicit them. Finally, I illustrate some of the co-narrative
moves with excerpts from mother-adolescent co-narrations of emotional experiences collected
by Alice Graneist (Graneist & Habermas, 2017).
Chapter 12 describes the specificity of narrating in the context of psychotherapy, and more
specifically in psychoanalytic psychotherapy and provides an overview of therapeutic
interventions described by conversation analysts. These are not specifically described in
relation to narrative. To this end, I discuss excerpts from four psychotherapies to illustrate the
functions of a variety of therapist interventions. Different kinds of interventions challenge
patients’ narratives and add perspectives to them.
Chapter 13 summarizes the argument and spells out some implications for the fields of
psychoanalysis, emotion, and narrative. I advocate the use of narrative analyses for
psychoanalytic research as an objective approach to unconscious defense processes against
emotional experiences. Emotion research could profit enormously from studying narratives as
they offer an objective empirical approach to naturally occurring phenomena. Narrative theory
could benefit from bridging the gap between artistic and everyday oral narratives, as called for
by Fludernik in her Natural Narratology (2002), and from looking at motivated distortions of
narratives to defend against emotions. The book ends with a plea and program for a narrative
emotion psychology.
References 1
References
Abbott, H. P. (2011). Narrativity. In P. Hühn et al. (eds.), The living handbook of narratology.
Hamburg: Hamburg University Press. URL = hup.sub.uni-hamburg.de/lhn/index.php
?title=Narrativity&oldid=1580 [view date: 01 Jan 2014]
Adler, J. M., Lodi-Smith, J., Philippe, F. L., & Houle, I. (2016). The incremental validity of
narrative identity in predicting well-being: A review of the field and recommendations for
the future. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 142-175.
Amsterdam, A. G., & Bruner, J. (2000). Minding the law. Harvard University Press.
Andersson, M. A., & Conley, C. S. (2008). Expecting to heal through self-expression: A
perceived control theory of writing and health. Health Psychology Review, 2, 138-162.
Andersson, M. A., & Conley, C. S. (2013). Optimizing the perceived benefits and health
outcomes of writing about traumatic life events. Stress and Health, 29, 40-49.
Andringa, E. (1986). Perspektivierung und Perspektivenübernahme. Spiel, 5, 135-146.
Angus, L. E., Boritz, T., Bryntwick, E., Carpenter, N., Macaulay, C., & Khattra, J. (2017). The
Narrative-Emotion Process Coding System 2.0: A multi-methodological approach to
identifying and assessing narrative-emotion process markers in psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy Research, 27, 253-269.
Appel, M., & Richter, T. (2010). Transportation and need for affect in narrative persuasion: A
mediated moderation model. Media Psychology, 13, 101-135.
Appelbaum, S. A. (1973). Psychological-mindedness: Word, concept and essence. The
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 54, 35-46.
Argelander, H. (1970). Das Erstinterview in der Psychotherapie. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche
Buchgesellschaft.
Aristotle (1982). Poetik. Stuttart: Reclam.
Bader, K. (2015). Das Unerzählbare erzählbar machen? Verarbeitungsprozesse im Bericht
eines Auscwhitzüberlebenden. In Schumann, E., Gülich, E., Lucius-Hoene., G., & Pfänder,
S. (Eds.), Wiedererzählen: Formen und Funktionen einer kulturellen Praxis (pp. 203-225).
Bielefeld, GE: transcript.
Bae, H. S., Lee, D., & Bae, R. E. (2014). Emotional engagement with the plot and characters: A
narrative film on hearing-impaired sexual assault victims. Narrative Inquiry, 24, 309-327.
Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittlinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and
faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 54-
75.
Baimel, A., Birch, S. A., & Norenzayan, A. (2018). Coordinating bodies and minds: Behavioral
synchrony fosters mentalizing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 281-290.
Bal, M. (1997). Narratology: Introduction to the theory of narrative (2nd, rev. ed.). Toronto,
CA: University of Toronto Press.
Balon, S., & Rimé, B. (2016). Lexical profile of emotional disclosure in socially shared versus
written narratives. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 35, 345-373.
Bamberg, M. (1997). Positioning between structure and performance. Journal of narrative
and life history, 7, 335-342.
Bamberg, M. (2008). Twice-told-tales: Small story analysis and the process of identity
formation. In T. Sugiman, K. J. Gergen, W. Wagner & Y. Yamada (Eds.), Meaning in action
(pp. 183-204). Tokyo: Springer.
Banks, M. V., & Salmon, K. (2013). Reasoning about the self in positive and negative ways:
Relationship to psychological functioning in young adulthood. Memory, 21, 10-26.
References 2
Baranger, W., & Baranger, M. (1961). The analytic situation as a dynamic field. The
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 2008, 89, 795-826.
Baron, J. M., & Bluck, S. (2009). Autobiographical memory sharing in everyday life:
Characteristics of a good story. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 105-
117.
Baron, J. M., & Bluck, S. (2011). That was a good story! Preliminary construction of the
perceived story quality index. Discourse Processes, 48, 93-118.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of
mind”? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: An experimental and social study. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Bauer, J. J., McAdams, D. P., & Sakaeda, A. R. (2005). Interpreting the good life: Growth
memories in the lives of mature, happy people. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 88, 203-217.
Bauer, P. J., Taşdemir-Özdes, A., & Larkina, M. (2014). Adults’ reports of their earliest
memories: Consistency in events, ages, and narrative characteristics over time.
Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 7688.
Baumeister, R. F., Stilwell, A., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim and perpetrator accounts of
interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives about anger. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 59, 994-1005.
Bavelas, J. B., & Gerwing, J. (2007). Conversational hand gestures and facial displays in face-
to-face dialogue. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication (pp. 283-308). London, UK:
Psychology Press.
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2000). Listeners as co-narrators. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 941.
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2002). Listener responses as a collaborative process:
The role of gaze. Journal of Communication, 52, 566-580.
Beike, D., & Wirth-Beaumont, E. (2005). Psychological closure as a memory phenomenon.
Memory, 13, 574-593.
Benczes, R. (2013). The role of alliteration and rhyme in novel metaphorical and
metonymical compounds. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 167-184.
Benecke, C., & Dammann, G. (2003). Unbewusste Emotionen. In A. Stephan & H. Walter
Eds.), Natur und Theorie der Emotionen (pp. 139-163). Paderborn, GE: Mentis.
Benecke, C., Peham, D., & Bänninger-Huber, E. (2005). Nonverbal relationship regulation in
psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 81-90.
Bengtsson, H., & Arvidsson, Å. (2011). The impact of developing social perspectivetaking
skills on emotionality in middle and late childhood. Social Development, 20, 353-375.
Ben-Ze'ev, A. (2000). The subtlety of emotions. Cambridge, MA: MITPress.
Berán, E., & Unoka, Z. (2005). Construction of self-narrative in psychotherapeutic setting: an
analysis of the mutual determination of narrative perspective taken by patient and
therapist. In U. M. Quasthoff & T. Becker (Eds), Narrative interaction (pp. 151-167).
Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Berán, E., & Unoka, Z. (2015). Reconstructing agency by shifting perspective in trauma
narrative. Language and Psychoanalysis, 4, 50-74.
Bergstein, J., & Erdelyi, M. (2008). Recognition hypermnesia: How to get it. Memory, 16, 689-
702.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw.
References 3
Berman, R., Ragnarsdóttir, H., & Strömqvist, S. (2002). Discourse stance: Written and spoken
language. Written Language & Literacy, 5, 255-289.
Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2006). Emotion and vantage point in autobiographical memory.
Cognition & Emotion, 20, 11931215.
Bertrand, R., & Espesser, R. (2017). Co-narration in French conversation storytelling: A
quantitative insight. Journal of Pragmatics, 111, 33-53.
Bion, W. R. (1962). A theory of thinking. London: Routledge.
Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an
autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42, 613-625.
Bluck, S., Levine, L. J., & Laulhere, T. M. (1999). Autobiographical remembering and
hypermnesia: A comparison of older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 14, 671-
682.
Bodie, G. D., Cannava, K. E., & Vickery, A. J. (2016). Supportive communication and the
adequate paraphrase. Communication Research Reports, 33, 166-172.
Bonanno, G. A. (2013). Meaning making, adversity, and regulatory flexibility. Memory, 21,
150-156.
Booth, W. C. (1961). The rhetoric of fiction. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Booth, W. C. (1988). The company we keep: An ethics of fiction. Oakland, CA: University of
California Press.
Boritz, T., Barnhart, R., Angus, L., & Constantino, M. J. (2017). Narrative flexibility in brief
psychotherapy for depression. Psychotherapy Research, 27, 666-676.
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P. (2003). Psychonarratology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Bortolussi, M., Dixon, P., & Sopčák, P. (2010). Gender and reading. Poetics, 38, 299-318.
Boston Change Study Group (2010). Change in psychotherapy. New York: Norton.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, 3 vols. London, UK: Penguin.
Branigan, E. (1992). Narrative comprehension and film. London: Routledge.
Brooks, P., & Gewirtz, P. (Eds.). (1998). Law's stories: Narrative and rhetoric in the law. New
heaven, CT: Yale University Press.
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73-99.
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18, 1-21.
Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Giles, G. E., Holmes, A., & Taylor, H. A. (2016). Mentally simulating
narrative perspective is not universal or necessary for language comprehension. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1592-1605.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science: A multiple code theory. New York:
Guilford.
Bucci, W. (2011). The role of subjectivity and intersubjectivity in the reconstruction of
dissociated schemas; converging perspectives from psychoanalysis, cognitive science and
affective neuroscience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 28, 247-266.
Bucci, W., & Maskit, B. (2006). A weighted referential activity dictionary. In Computing
attitude and affect in text: Theory and applications (pp. 49-60). Dordrecht, NL: Springer.
Buchholz, M. B. (2017). Über Resonanz in einer psychoanalytischen Kurzzeittherapie. In T.
Breyer, M. B. Buchholz, S. Pfänder, & A. Hamburger (Eds.), Resonanz, Rhythmus &
Synchronisierung - Erscheinungsformen und Effekte (pp. 283-318). Bielefeld, GE:
Transcript-Verlag.
Buchholz, M. B., & Kächele, H. (2016). Rhythm & bluesAmalies 152. Sitzung. Psyche, 70, 97-
133.
References 4
Buck, R. (1980). Nonverbal behavior and the theory of emotion: The facial feedback
hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 811-824.
Bühler, K. (1934). Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Jena, GE: Fischer.
Burke, K. (1931). Counter-Statement. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press [1968].
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories:
A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18, 255-
280.
Butler, A. C., Rice, H. J., Wooldridge, C. L., & Rubin, D. C. (2016). Visual imagery in
autobiographical memory: The role of repeated retrieval in shifting perspective.
Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 237-253.
Campbell, J., Nadel, L., Duke, D., & Ryan, L. (2011). Remembering all that and then some:
Recollection of autobiographical memories after a 1-year delay. Memory, 19, 406-415.
Campbell, R. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The secret life of pronouns flexibility in writing
style and physical health. Psychological Science, 14, 60-65.
Canter, D. V., Grieve, N., Nicol, C., & Benneworth, K. (2003). Narrative plausibility: The
impact of sequence and anchoring. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 21, 251-267.
Carroll, N. (2007). Narrative closure. Philosophical studies, 135, 1-15.
Chafe, W., & Tannen, D. (1987). The relation between written and spoken language. Annual
Review of Anthropology, 16, 383-407.
Chang, J. H., Huang, C. L., & Lin, Y. C. (2013). The psychological displacement paradigm in
diary-writing (PDPD) and its psychological benefits. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 155-
167.
Chatman, S. B. (1978). Story and discourse: Narrative structure in fiction and film. Cornell
University Press.
Chatman, S. B. (1990). Coming to terms: The rhetoric of narrative in fiction and film. Cornell
University Press.
Christmann, U., Wimmer, L., & Groeben, N. (2011). The aesthetic paradox in processing
conventional and non-conventional metaphors: A reaction time study. Scientific study of
Literature, 1, 199-240.
Chused, J. F. (1991). The evocative power of enactments. Journal of the American
Psychoanalytic Association, 39, 615-640.
Cohen, J., Tal-Or, N., & Mazor-Tregerman, M. (2015). The tempering effect of transportation:
Exploring the effects of transportation and identification during exposure to controversial
two-sided narratives. Journal of Communication, 65, 237-258.
Cohler, B. J. (1982). Personal narrative and life course. Life Span Development and Behavior,
4, 205-241.
Coluccia, E., Bianco, C., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2010). Autobiographical and event memories
for surprising and unsurprising events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 177-199.
Coman, A., & Hirst, W. (2015). Social identity and socially shared retrieval-induced
forgetting: The effects of group membership. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 144, 717-722.
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical
memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261-288.
Couper-Kuhlen, E. (2012). Exploring affiliation in the reception of conversational complaint
stories. In A. Peräkylä & M.-L. Sorjonen (Eds.), Emotion in interaction (pp.113-146).
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671684.
References 5
Crawley, R. A. (2010). Closure of autobiographical memories: the effects of written
recounting from first-or third-person visual perspective. Memory, 18, 900-917.
Crespo, M., & Fernández-Lansac, V. (2016). Memory and narrative of traumatic events: A
literature review. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 8, 149-
156.
Cupchik, G. C., & Laszlo, J. (1994). The landscape of time in literary reception: Character
experience and narrative action. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 297-312.
Cupchik, G. C., Oatley, K., & Vorderer, P. (1998). Emotional effects of reading excerpts from
short stories by James Joyce. Poetics, 25, 363-377.
Curci, A., & Rimé, B. (2012). The temporal evolution of social sharing of emotions and its
consequences on emotional recovery: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 12, 1404-1414.
D’Arcy, A. (2015). Quotation and advances in understanding syntactic systems. Annual
Review of Linguistics, 1, 43-61.
Dael, N., Mortillari, M., & Scherer, K. R. (2011). Emotion expression in bnody action and
posture. Emotion, 12, 1085-1101.
Dancygier, B. (2017). Viewpoint phenomena in constructions and discourse. Glossa: a
Journal of General Linguistics, 2, 1-22.
Danziger, K. (1994). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the emotions in man and animals critical edition
(1998). London: Harper Collins.
Davies, B., & Harré, R. (1990). Positioning: The discursive production of selves. Journal for
the Theory of Social Behaviour, 20, 43-63.
Davis, K. (1986). The process of problem (re) formulation in psychotherapy. Sociology of
Health & Illness, 8, 44-74.
De Fina, A., & Georgakopoulou, A. (2012). Analyzing narrative. Discourse and sociolinguistic
perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
De Graaf, A., Hoeken, H., Sanders, J., & Beentjes, J. W. (2012). Identification as a mechanism
of narrative persuasion. Communication Research, 39, 802-823.
De Mulder, H. N., Hakemulder, F., van den Berghe, R., Klaassen, F., & van Berkum, J. J.
(2017). Effects of exposure to literary narrative fiction. Scientific Study of Literature, 7,
129-169.
De Silveira, C., & Habermas, T. (2011). Narrative means to manage responsibility in life
narratives across adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172, 1-20.
De Sousa, R. (1987). The rationality of emotion. Boston: MIT Press.
Demiray, B., Mischler, M., & Martin, M. (2017). Reminiscence in everyday conversations: A
naturalistic observation study of older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B.
Advance online publication November 28, 2017.
Deppermann, A. (2009). Therapeutisches Fragen als Hebammenkunst. Psychoanalyse - Texte
zur Sozialforschung, 2, 154-162
Deutsch, H. (1926). Okkulte Vorgänge während der Psychoanalyse. Imago, 12, 418-433.
Dimaggio, G. (2011). Impoverished self-narrative and impaired self-reflection as targets for
the psychotherapy of personality disorders. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 41,
165-174.
Dimaggio, G., Vanheule, S., Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., & Nicolò, G. (2009). Impaired self-
reflection in psychiatric disorders among adults: a proposal for the existence of a
network of semi-independent functions. Consciousness and cognition, 18, 653-664.
References 6
Döll-Hentschker, S. (2008). Die Veränderung von Träumen in psychoanalytischen
Behandlungen: Affekttheorie, Affektregulierung und Traumkodierung. Frankfurt, GE:
Brandes & Apsel.
Drew, P., & Holt, E. (1988). Complainable matters: The use of idiomatic expressions in
making complaints. Social Problems, 35, 398-417.
Drijbooms, E., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2017). Children's use of evaluative devices in
spoken and written narratives. Journal of Child Language, 44, 767-794.
Drivdahl, S. B., & Hyman Jr, I. E. (2014). Fluidity in autobiographical memories: Relationship
memories sampled on two occasions. Memory, 22, 1070-1081.
Duprez, C., Christophe, V., Rime, B., Congard, A., & Antoine, P. (2015). Motives for the social
sharing of an emotional experience. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32, 757-
787.
Eaton, K. L. (2005). Young adults’ narratives for positive and negative experiences across
different reporting conditions: An integrative analysis. Poster at the Biannual Meeting of
SRCD, Atlanta, March 2005.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das Gedächtnis: Untersuchungen zur experimentellen
Psychologie. Berlin. Duncker & Humboldt.
Echterhoff, G., & Higgins, E. T. (2017). Creating shared reality in interpersonal and intergroup
communication: The role of epistemic processes and their interplay. European Review of
Social Psychology, 28, 175-226.
Echterhoff, G., & Kopietz, R. (2018). The socially shared nature of memory: From joint
encoding to communication. In M. L. Meade, C. B. Harris, P. Van Bergen, J. Sutton, & A. J.
Barnier (Eds.), Collaborative remembering: Theories, research, and applications (pp. 113-
134). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),
Approaches to emotion, (pp. 319-344). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum..
Ekman, P. (1998). Introduction to the critical edition. In C. Darwin (1998), The Expression of
the emotions in man and animals critical edition. London: Harper Collins.
Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed. New York, NY: Times Books.
Evers, C., Hopp, H., Gross, J. J., Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S., & Mauss, I. B. (2014). Emotion
response coherence: A dual-process perspective. Biological Psychology, 98, 43-49.
Fabb, N. (2002). Language and literary structure The linguistic analysis of form in verse and
narrative. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fabb, N. (2017). Linguistics and literature. In M. Aronoff & J. Rees-Miller (Ed.), The handbook
of linguistics (pp. 463-478). New York: Wiley.
Fainsilber, L., & Ortony, A. (1987). Metaphorical uses of language in the expression of
emotions. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 2, 239-250.
Fairbairn, W. R. D. (1940). Schizoid factors in the personality. In W. R. D. Fairbairn (1952),
Psychoanalytic studies of the personality (pp.3-27). London: Routledge.
Feldman, C. F., Bruner, J., Kalmar, D., & Renderer, B. (1993). Plot, light, and dramatism:
Interpretation at three ages. Human Development, 36, 327-342.
Feldman, C. F., Bruner, J., Renderer, B., & Spitzer, S. (1990). Narrative comprehension. In B.
K. Britton & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp. 1-78).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ferrara, K. (1994). Therapeutic ways with words. New York: Oxford University Press.
Ferro, A. (1992). The bi-personal field: Experiences in child analysis. London: Routledge
[2003].
Ferro, A. (1998). Il sogno della veglia: teoria e clinica. Rivista di Psicoanalisi, 44, 117-128.
References 7
Ferro, A. (1999). Psychoanalysis as therapy and storytelling. London: Routledge [2006].
Fiehler, R. (1990). Kommunikation und Emotion: theoretische und empirische
Untersuchungen zur Rolle von Emotionen in der verbalen Interaktion. Walter de Gruyter.
Fioretti, C., & Smorti, A. (2015). How emotional content of memories changes in narrating.
Narrative Inquiry, 25, 37-56.
Fioretti, C., Pascuzzi, D., & Smorti, A. (2017). The role of the listener on the emotional
valence of personal memories in emerging adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 24,
252-262.
Fisher, W. R. (1989). Human communication as narration: Toward a philosophy of reason,
value, and action. New York: Columbia University Press.
Fivush, R., Bohanek, J. G., & Duke, M. (2008). The intergenerational self: Subjective
perspective and family history. In F. Sani (Ed.), Self continuity: Individual and collective
perspectives (pp. 131-143). New York: Psychology Press.
Fivush, R., Booker, J. A., & Graci, M. E. (2017). Ongoing Narrative Meaning-Making Within
Events and Across the Life Span. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 37, 127-152.
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal
reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77,
1568-1588.
Flader, D., & Giesecke, M. (1980). Erzählen im psychoanalytischen Erstinterview. In K. Ehlich
(Ed.), Erzählen im Alltag (pp. 209-262). Frankfurt a.M., GE: Suhrkamp.
Flader, D., & Giesecke, M. (1980). Erzählen im psychoanalytischen Erstgespräch. In K. Ehlich
(Ed.), Erzählen im Alltag (pp. 209-262). Frankfurt: Suhrkamp.
Flavell, J. H., Botkin, P., Fry, C., Wright, J., & Jarvis, P. (1968). The development of role-taking
and communications skills in children. New York: Wiley.
Fludernik, M. (1996). Towards a natural narratology. London: Routledge.
Foa, E. B., Molnar, C., & Cashman, L. (1995). Change in rape narratives during exposure
therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 675-690.
Fohn, A., & HeenenWolff, S. (2011). The destiny of an unacknowledged trauma: The
deferred retroactive effect of aprèscoup in the hidden Jewish children of wartime
Belgium. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 92, 5-20.
Fohn, A., Bouhmidi, S., Bushati, E., Mba, D., Yesilgöz, E., & Habermas, T. (2017). Given up by
parents for survival: Separation narratives by formerly persecuted elderly Belgian Jews.
Journal of Applied Research on Memory and Cognition, 6, 74-81.
Fohn, A., Heenen-Wolff, S., & Mouchenik, Y. (2014). Silence et reconnaissance sociale dans le
processus de reconstruction d’anciens enfants juifs cachés. Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance
et de l'Adolescence, 62, 293-298.
Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. W. (2016). Adversity, attachment, and mentalizing.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 64, 59-66.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1996). Playing with reality I. The International journal of psycho-
analysis, 77, 217-233.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2000). Playing with reality: III. The persistence of dual psychic
reality in borderline patients. International Journal of Pschoanalysis, 81, 853-873.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Jurist, E. L. (2004). Affect regulation, mentalization and the
development of the self. New York: other Press.
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-functioning manual, version
5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London.
Frankl, V. E. (1946). Man's search for meaning. New York: Simon and Schuster [1986].
References 8
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 823-865.
Freud, A. (1936). Das Ich und die Abwehrmechanismen. Wien: Internationaler
Psyhoanalytischer Verlag.
Freud, S. (1896). Obsessions et phobies. Leur mécanisme psychique et leur étiologie. In S.
Freud (1952), Gesammelte Werke, Vol. 1 (pp. 343-354). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1899). Über Deckerinnerungen. In S. Freud (1952). Gesammelte Werke, Vol. 1 (pp.
529-554). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1905). Bruchstücke einer Hysterieanalyse. In S. Freud (1942). Gesammelte Werke,
Vol. 5 (pp. 161-286). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1908). Der Dichter und das Phantasieren. In S. Freud (1941). Gesammelte Werke,
Vol. 7 (pp 211-224). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1914). Erinnern, Wiederholen, Durcharbeiten. In S. Freud (1946). Gesammelte
Werke, Vol. 10 (pp 125-136). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1926). Hemmung, Symptom und Angst. In S. Freud (1948). Gesammelte Werke,
Vol. 14 (pp. 111-206). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S. (1942). Psychopathische Personen auf der Bühne. In S. Freud (1987). Gesammelte
Werke, Nachtragsband (pp. 655-661). Frankfurt: Fischer.
Freud, S., & Breuer, J. (1895). Studien über Hysterie. In S. Freud (1952). Gesammelte Werke,
Band I, pp. 75-312. Frankfurt: Fischer.
Frijda, N. H. (1956). Het begrijpen van gelaatsexpressies. Amsterdam, NL: Van Oorschot.
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gabbert, F., & Wheeler, R. (2018). Memory conformity following collaborative remembering.
In M. L. Meade, C. B. Harris, P. Van Bergen, J. Sutton, & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), Collaborative
remembering: Theories, research, and applications (pp. 100-112). Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Gee, J. P. (1986). Units in the production of narrative discourse. Discourse Processes, 9, 391-
422.
Gee, J. P. (1989). Two styles of narrative construction and their linguistic and educational
implications. Discourse processes, 12, 287-307.
Gee, J. P. (1990). Social linguistics and literacies: Ideology in discourses (3rd rev ed). London:
Routledge [2008].
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Emotion perception: Putting the face in
context. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology (pp. 539-556).
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., & Feldman Barret, L. (2033). Emotion perception: Putting the
face in context. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (p.
539-556). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Genette, G. (1980). Narrative discourse. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds: On the psychological activities of reading.
New Heaven, CT: Yale University Press.
Gibbs, R. W. (2002). Feeling moved by metaphor. In S. Csabi & J. Zerkowitz (Eds.), Textual
secrets: The message of the medium (pp. 13-28). Budapest: Eotvos Lorand University
Press.
Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L. (2012). Interpreting figurative meaning. Cambridge University
Press.
References 9
Gibbs, R. W., Leggitt, & Turner, E. (2002). Why figurative language is special in emotional
communication. In S. Fussell (Ed.), The verbal communication of emotions (pp. 125-150).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Goblirsch, M. (2005). Herstellung narrativer Identitäten durch biographische Strukturierung
und Positionierung. Eine retold story aus der Jugendhilfe. GesprächsforschungOnline-
Zeitschrift zur verbalen Interaktion, 6, 196-221.
Goldie, P. (2000).The emotions. A philosophical exploration. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Goldie, P. (2003). Narrative and perspective: Values and appropriate emotions. Royal
Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 52, 201-220.
Goldie, P. (2012). The mess inside. Narrative, emotion, and the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Goodwin, C. (1986). Between and within: Alternative sequential treatments of continuers
and assessments. Human Studies, 9, 205-217.
Gordon, C. (2006). Reshaping prior text, reshaping identities. Text & Talks, 26, 545-571.
Gordon, S. L. (1989). The socialization of children's emotions: emotional culture,
competence, and exposure. In C. I. Saarni & P. Harris (Eds.), Children's understanding of
emotion (pp. 319-349). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Graneist, A., & Habermas, T. (2017). „Ponder, talk, cuddle, cry - done“ - Social emotion
regulation in adolescence. Presented at Joseph Sandler Psychoanalytic Research
Conference, 2017 March 4, Frankfurt am Main, Germany.
Graneist, A., & Habermas, T. (2018). Internal state language in emotion narratives of
adolescents and their mothers. Submitted for publication.
Gratier, M., & Trevarthen, C. (2008). Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-
infant vocal interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15, 122-158.
Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early
development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers
in Psychology, 6, 1167.
Graybeal, A., Sexton, J. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). The role of story-making in disclosure
writing: The psychometrics of narrative. Psychology and Health, 17, 571-581.
Graybeal, A., Sexton, J. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). The role of story-making in disclosure
writing: The psychometrics of narrative. Psychology and Health, 17, 571-581.
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public
narratives. Journal of personality and social psychology, 79, 701-721.
Green, M. C., & Donahue, J. K. (2009). Simulated worlds: Transportation into narratives. In K.
D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein & J. A. Suhr (ds.), Handbook of imagination and mental
simulation, 241-256. New York: Psychology Press.
Greenson, R. R. (1967). The technique and practice of psychoanalysis. New York:
International Universities Press.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Moran (Eds. ), Syntax and
Semantics, III: Speech acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.
Gröben, N. (1981). Rezeptionsforschung als empirische Literaturwissenschaft. Tübingen:
Narr.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological
Inquiry, 26, 1-26.
Gu, X., & Tse, C. S. (2016). Narrative perspective shift at retrieval: The psychological-
distance-mediated-effect on emotional intensity of positive and negative
autobiographical memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 159-173.
References 10
Guardiola, M., & Bertrand, R. (2013). Interactional convergence in conversational
storytelling: when reported speech is a cue of alignment and/or affiliation. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 705.
Gülich, E., & Lucius-Hoene, G. (2015). Veränderungen von Geschichten beim erzählen
Versuch einer interdisziplinären Annäherung an narrative Rekonstruktionen von
Schlüsselszenen. In Schumann, E., Gülich, E., Lucius-Hoene., G., & Pfänder, S. (Eds.),
Wiedererzählen: Formen und Funktionen einer kulturellen Praxis (pp. 135-175). Bielefeld,
GE: transcript.
Habermas, T. (2006). Who speaks? Who looks? Who feels? Point of view in autobiographical
narratives. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 87, 497-518.
Habermas, T. (2007). How to tell a life: The development of the cultural concept of
biography across the lifespan. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 1-31.
Habermas, T. (2008). Freuds Ratschläge zur Einleitung der Behandlung: Eine narratologische
Reinterpretation der Wirkweise der psychoanalytischen Situation. In R. Haubl, & T.
Habermas (Eds.), Freud neu entdecken (pp. 204-229). Göttingen, GE: Vandenhoeck &
Rupprecht.
Habermas, T. (2011). Autobiographical reasoning: Arguing and narrating from a biographical
perspective. In T. Habermas (Ed.), The development of autobiographical reasoning in
adolescence and beyond. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 131, 1-
17. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Habermas, T. (2013). Come funzionano i processi co-narrativi nella psicoterapia orientata
all’insight. Psichiatria e Psicoterapia, 32, 292-303.
Habermas, T. (2015). A model of psychopathological distortions of autobiographical memory
narratives: An emotion narrative view. In L. A. Watson & D. Berntsen (Eds.), Clinical
perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 267-290). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Habermas, T., & Berger, N. (2011). Retelling everyday emotional events: Condensation,
distancing, and closure. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 206-219.
Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The development of the life story in
adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748-769.
Habermas, T., & Diel, V. (2013). The episodicity of verbal reports of personally significant
autobiographical memories: Vividness correlates with narrative text quality more than
with detailedness or memory specificity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 110.
Habermas, T., & Döll-Hentschker, S. (2017). The form of the story: Measuring linguistic
aspects of narrative activity in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 27, 300-312.
Habermas, T., & Köber, C. (2015). Autobiographical reasoning is constitutive for narrative
identity: The role of the life story for personal continuity. In K. C. McLean & M. Syed
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development (pp. 149-165). Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Habermas, T., & Paha, C. (2001). The development of coherence in adolescents’ life
narratives. Narrative Inquiry, 11, 35-54.
Habermas, T., Förster, J., Lingg, K., & Bongard, S. (2015).The role of narrative perspectives in
sad narratives in eliciting emotions: Acoustic presentation and facial, physiological and
self-report emotional reactions. Goethe University Frankfurt: Unpublished report.
Habermas, T., Meier, M., & Mukhtar, B. (2009). Are specific emotions narrated differently?
Emotion,9 , 751-762.
References 11
Habermas, T., Negele, A., & Mayer, F. (2010). „Honey, you’re jumping about“ – Mothers’
scaffolding of their children’s and adolescents’ life narration. Cognitive Development, 25,
339-351.
Habermas, T., Ott, L. M., Schubert, M., Schneider, B., & Pate, A. (2008). Stuck in the past:
Negative bias, explanatory style, temporal order, and evaluative perspectives in life
narratives of clinically depressed individuals. Depression and Anxiety, 25, E121-E132.
Hall, A. E., & Bracken, C. C. (2011). I really liked that movie. Journal of Media Psychology, 23,
90-99.
Halliday, M. A. K., & Matthiessen, C. M. (2004). An introduction to functional grammar (3rd,
rev. ed.). London: Arnold.
Harber, K. D., Podolski, P., & Dyer, L. (2014). Hearing stories that violate expectations leads
to emotional broadcasting. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33, 5-28.
Harding, D. W. (1976). Words into rhythm. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of
others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128-
140.
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2012). The social signal value of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 26,
385-389.
Hareli, S., Moran-Amir, O., David, S., & Hess, U. (2013). Emotions as signals of normative
conduct. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 1395-1404.
Harris, C. B., Barnier, A. J., Sutton, J., & Keil, P. G. (2014). Couples as socially distributed
cognitive systems: Remembering in everyday social and material contexts. Memory
Studies, 7, 285-297.
Harris, M. A., Donnellan, M. B., Guo, J., McAdams, D. P., GarnierVillarreal, M., &
Trzesniewski, K. H. (2017). Parental co‐construction of 5to 13‐year‐olds' global self
esteem through reminiscing about past events. Child Development, 88, 1810-1822.
Heritage, J. (2011). Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: empathic moments in
interaction. In T. Stivers, L. Mondada & J. Steensig (Eds.), The morality of knowledge in
conversation, (pp. 159-183). New York: Cambridge University Press..
Herman, D. (2002). Story logic. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Herman, D. (2013). Storytelling and the Sciences of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Herman, D. (Ed.). (2007). The Cambridge companion to narrative. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2016). The impact of context on the perception of emotions. In C. Abell
& J. Smith (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological, and legal
perspectives (pp. 199-218). New York: Cambridge University Press..
Heyd, T. (2011). Unreliability. The pragmatic perspective revisited. Journal of Literary Theory,
5, 3-17.
Hillebrandt, C. (2011). Emotional functions of unreliable narratives. An outline for future
research. Journal of Literary Theory, 5, 19-36.
Hirst, W., & Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in conversations: The social sharing and
reshaping of memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 55-79.
Hirst, W., & Yamashiro, J. (2018). Social aspects of forgetting. In M. L. Meade, C. B. Harris, P.
Van Bergen, J. Sutton, & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), Collaborative remembering: Theories,
research, and applications (pp. 76-99). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Hirst, W., Phelps, E. A., Meksin, R., Vaidya, C. J., Johnson, M. K., Mitchell, K. J., ... & Mather,
M. (2015). A ten-year follow-up of a study of memory for the attack of September 11,
References 12
2001: Flashbulb memories and memories for flashbulb events. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 144, 604-623.
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Oakland,
CA: University of California Press.
Hodges, S. D., Kiel, K. J., Kramer, A. D., Veach, D., & Villanueva, B. R. (2010). Giving birth to
empathy: The effects of similar experience on empathic accuracy, empathic concern, and
perceived empathy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 398-409.
Hoeken, H., & Fikkers, K. M. (2014). Issue-relevant thinking and identification as mechanisms
of narrative persuasion. Poetics, 44, 84-99.
Hoeken, H., & Sinkeldam, J. (2014). The role of identification and perception of just outcome
in evoking emotions in narrative persuasion. Journal of Communication, 64, 935-955.
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hogan, P. C. (2003). The mind and its stories: Narrative universals and human emotion.
Cambridge University Press.
Hogan, P. C. (2011). Affective narratology: The emotional structure of stories. Lincol, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.
Holland, N. N. (1975). Five readers reading. New Heaven, CT: Yale University Press.
Honos-Webb, L., & Stiles, W. B. (1998). Reformulation of assimilation analysis in terms of
voices. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 35, 23-33.
Hoyt, M. A., Austenfeld, J., & Stanton, A. L. (2016). Processing coping methods in expressive
essays about stressful experiences: Predictors of health benefit. Journal of Health
Psychology, 21, 1183-1193.
Hoyt, M. A., Austenfeld, J., & Stanton, A. L. (2016). Processing coping methods in expressive
essays about stressful experiences: Predictors of health benefit. Journal of Health
Psychology, 21, 1183-1193.
Hühn, P. (Eds). (n.d.). The living handbook of narratology. Hamburg: Hamburg University.
URL = http://www.lhn.uni-hamburg.de/
Hühn, P., & Sommer, R. (2013). Narration in Poetry and Drama. In P. Hühn (ed.), The living
handbook of narratology. Hamburg: Hamburg University Press. hup.sub.uni-
hamburg.de/lhn/index.php?title=Narration in Poetry and Drama&oldid=1930
Hutto, D. D. (2008). Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding
reasons. Cambridge, MA: MITpress.
Hymes, D. (1977). Discovering oral performance and measured verse in American Indian
narrative. New Literary History, 8, 431-457.
Hymes, D. (1981). “In vain I tried to tell you” – Essays in American Indian ethnopoetics.
Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Hymes, D. (2003). Now I know only so far: Essays in ethnopoetics. Lincoln, NE: University of
Nebraska Press.
Iser, W, (1976). Der Akt des Lesens. München, GE: Fink.
Izard, C. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Guilford.
Jacob, H., Kreifelts, B., Brück, C., Nizielski, S., Schütz, A., & Wildgruber, D. (2013). Nonverbal
signals speak up: association between perceptual nonverbal dominance and emotional
intelligence. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 783-799.
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188-205.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology, Vols. I-II. New York: Holt [1950].
Jauß, H. R. (1977). Ästhetische Erfahrung und literarische Hermeneutik. Frankfurt a.M., GE:
Suhrkamp.
References 13
Jefferson, G. (1988). On the sequential organization of troubles-talk in ordinary conversation.
Social problems, 35, 418-441.
Jefferson, G., & Lee, J. R. (1981). The rejection of advice: Managing the problematic
convergence of a ‘troubles-telling’and a ‘service encounter’. Journal of Pragmatics, 5,
399-422.
Jin, S. R. (2005). The dialectical effect of psychological displacement: A narrative analysis.
Taipei: National Science Council.
Johnson, D. R., Cushman, G. K., Borden, L. A., & McCune, M. S. (2013). Potentiating empathic
growth: Generating imagery while reading fiction increases empathy and prosocial
behavior. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 306-342.
Johnstone, B. (2016). ‘Oral versions of personal experience’: Labovian narrative analysis and
its uptake. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 20, 542-560.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of
behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Josselson, R. (2000). Stability and change in early memories over 22 years: Themes,
variations, and cadenzas. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 64, 462-481.
Josselson, R. (2009). The present of the past: Dialogues with memory over time. Journal of
Personality, 77, 647-668.
Kächele, H., & Albani, C. (2001) Die Arbeit an einem zentralen Beziehungsmuster als
Musterbeispiel klinisch relevanter Empirie. In M. Cierpka & P. Buchheim (Eds.).
Psychodynamische Konzepte (pp. 169-190). Berlin, DE: Springer.
Kashima, Y., Lyons, A., & Clark, A. (2013). The maintenance of cultural stereotypes in the
conversational retelling of narratives. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 60-70.
Kazén, M., & Solís-Macías, V. M. (2016). Recall and recognition hypermnesia for Socratic
stimuli. Memory, 24, 128-145.
Keen, S. (2007). Empathy and the novel. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York:
Aronson.
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders: Psychotherapeutic strategies. New
Heaven, CT: Yale University Press.
Kertész, I. (1975). Fateless. Random House [2004].
Klauk, T., Köppe, T., & Onea, E. (2016). More on narrative closure. Journal of Literary
Semantics, 45, 21-48.
Klauk, T., Köppe, T., & Weskott, T. (2016). Empirical correlates of narrative closure. DIEGESIS,
5.1, 26-42.
Klein, M. (1928). Die frühe Entwicklung des Gewissens beim Kind. In M. Klein (1985),
Frühstadien des Ödipuskomplexes (pp.89-101). Frankfurt a.M.: Fischer Taschenbuch.
Klein, M. (1931). Frühe Angstsituationen im Spiegel künstlerischer Darstellungen.
Internationale für Psychoanalyse, 17, 497-506.
Knerich, H. (2015). Konzepte der Vorgeformtheit und Wiedererzählen (pp. 33-60). In
Schumann, E., Gülich, E., Lucius-Hoene., G., & Pfänder, S. (Eds.), Wiedererzählen: Formen
und Funktionen einer kulturellen Praxis (pp. 33-60). Bielefeld, GE: transcript.
Köber, C., & Habermas, T. (2017a). The development of temporal macrostructure in life
narratives across the lifespan. Discourse Processes, 54, 143-162.
Köber, C., & Habermas, T. (2017b). How stable is the personal past? Stability of most
important autobiographical memories and life narratives across eight years in a lifespan
sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 608-626.
References 14
Köber, C., Schmiedek, F., & Habermas, T. (2015). Characterizing lifespan development of
three aspects of coherence in life narratives: A cohort-sequential study. Developmental
Psychology, 51, 260-275.
Koerfer, A., & Neumann, C. (1982). Alltagsdiskurs und psychoanalytischer Diskurs. Aspekte
der Sozialisierung der Patienten in einen ‘ungewöhnlichen’ Diskurstyp. In H. Flader, W.-D.
Grodzicki & K. Schröter (Eds.), Psychoanalyse als Gespräch (pp 96-137). Frankfurt a.M.,
GE: Suhrkamp.
Kohlberg, L., & Kramer, R. (1969). Continuities and discontinuities in childhood and adult
moral development. Human Development, 12, 93-120.
Komeda, H., Kawasaki, M., Tsunemi, K., & Kusumi, T. (2009). Differences between estimating
protagonists’ emotions and evaluating readers’ emotions in narrative comprehension.
Cognition and Emotion, 23, 135-151.
Komeda, H., Tsunemi, K., Inohara, K., Kusumi, T., & Rapp, D. N. (2013). Beyond disposition:
the processing consequences of explicit and implicit invocations of empathy. Acta
psychologica, 142, 349-355.
Komolova, M., Wainryb, C., & Recchia, H. (2017). “She Had a Reason to Be Concerned”:
Youths Making Sense of Their Mothers’ and Friends’ Perspectives in Their Accounts of
Conflicts. Cognitive Development, 43, 201-213.
Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture, and body in human feeling
(rev. ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-
distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 187-191.
Kurri, K., & Wahlström, J. (2007). Reformulations of agentless talk in psychotherapy. Text &
Talk-An Interdisciplinary Journal of Language, Discourse Communication Studies, 27, 315-
338.
Kyung, Y., Yanes-Lukin, P., & Roberts, J. E. (2015). Specificity and detail in autobiographical
memory: Same or different constructs? Memory, 24, 272-284.
LaBounty, J., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S., Lagattuta, K., & Liu, D. (2008). Mothers' and fathers'
use of internal state talk with their young children. Social Development, 17, 757-775.
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Labov, W. (1997). Some further steps in narrative analysis. Journal of Narrative and Life
History, 7, 395-415.
Labov, W. (2001). Uncovering the event structure of narrative. In D. Tannen & J. Alatis(Eds.),
Georgetown University round table on languages and linguistics (p. 63-83). Washington
DC: Georgetown University Press.
Labov, W. (2006). Narrative pre-construction. Narrative inquiry, 16, 37-45.
Labov, W. (2013). The language of life and death: The transformation of experience in oral
narrative. Cambridge University Press.
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In
J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 1244). Seattle, WA: University of
Washington Press. (Reprinted in Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 338, 1997).
Lakoff, G., & Johnson. M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press.
Lambrou, M. (2014). Narrative, text and time: Telling the same story twice in the oral
narrative reporting of 7/7. Language and Literature, 23, 32-48.
References 15
Lamparter, U. (2013). Zeitzeugen des Hamburger Feuersturms 1943 und ihre Familien:
Forschungsprojekt zur Weitergabe von Kriegserfahrungen. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck &
Ruprecht.
Langs, R. (1976). The bipersonal field. New York: Aronson.
Larson, S. F., & László, J. (1990). Cultural-historical knowledge and personal experience in
appreciation of Iiterature. Joumal of Social Psychology, 20, 425-40.
László, J. (1986). Same story with different point of view. Spiel, 5, 1-22.
Laub, D., & Auerhahn, N. C. (1989). Failed empathy--a central theme in the survivor's
holocaust experience. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 6, 377-400.
Lazarus, R. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lazarus, R. (1999). Stress and emotion. New York: Springer.
Lazarus, R. (2006). Emotions and interpersonal relationships. Journal of Personality, 74, 9-46.
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.
Lecours, S. (2013). La Grille de l'Élaboration Verbale de l'Affect (GEVA). Scoring Manual,
Version 7. Montréal, CA: Université de Montréal.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (2005). Making marriage visible: Wedding anniversaries as the public
component of private relationships. Text, 25, 595-631.
Lejeune, P. (1975). On autobiography. University of Minnesota Press (1989).
Lerner, G. H. (1992). Assisted storytelling: Deploying shared knowledge as a practical matter.
Qualitative Sociology, 15, 247-271.
Lewin, K. (1931). Die psychologische Situation bei Lohn und Strafe. In K. Lewin (1982),
Werkausgabe, Band 6 (pp. 113-168). Bern/Stuttgart: Huber/Klett-Cotta.
Lilgendahl, J. P., & McAdams, D. P. (2011). Constructing stories of selfgrowth: How
individual differences in patterns of autobiographical reasoning relate to wellbeing in
midlife. Journal of Personality, 79, 391-428.
Linde, C. (1997). Other people’s stories: Person and evidentiality in individual and group
memory. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society pp.
459-470.
Linde, C. (2001). Narrative and social tacit knowledge. Journal of Knowledge Management, 5,
160170.
Linde, C. (2008). Working the past: Narrative and institutional memory. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Lothane, H. Z. (2011). Dramatology vs. narratology: a new synthesis for psychiatry,
psychoanalysis, and interpersonal drama therapy (IDT). Archives of Psychiatry and
Psychotherapy, 4, 29-43.
Lucius-Hoene, G., & Deppermann, A. (2000). Narrative identity empiricized: A dialogical and
positioning approach to autobiographical research interviews. Narrative Inquiry, 10, 199-
222.
Lucius-Hoene, G., & Deppermann, A. (2002). Rekonstruktion narrativer Identität. Wiesbaden,
GE: Verlag für Sozialwssenschaften.
Luminet, O., Licata, L., Klein, O., Rosoux, V., Heenen-Wolff, S., van Ypersele, L., & Stone C. B.
(2012). The interplay between collective memory and the erosion of nation states the
paradigmatic case of Belgium. Memory Studies, 5, 3-15.
MacIntyre, A. (1981). After virtue. London: Bloomsbury.
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A
move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points
References 16
of attachment research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
50(1-2), 66-104.
Mandelbaum, J. (1987). Couples sharing stories. Communication Quarterly, 35, 144-170.
Mandelbaum, J. (1989). Interpersonal activities in conversational storytelling. Western
Journal of Communication, 53, 114-126.
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Djikic, M., & Mullin, J. (2011). Emotion and narrative fiction: Interactive
influences before, during, and after reading. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 818-833.
Marin, K. A., & Rotondo, E. K. (2017). Rumination and self-reflection in stress narratives and
relations to psychological functioning. Memory, 25, 44-56.
Marsh, E. J. (2007). Retelling is not the same as recalling implications for memory. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 16-20.
Marty, P., & de M’Uzan, M. (1963). La pensée opératoire. Revue française de psychanalyse,
27, 345-356.
McAdams, D. P. (1985). Power, intimacy, and the life story. New York: Guilford.
McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100-
122.
McAdams, D. P. (2006). The redemptive self: Stories Americans live by. New York: Oxford
University Press.
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1984). What makes a good story. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research, 13, 457-480.
McClure, J., Hilton, D. J., Cowan, J., Ishida, L., & Wilson, M. (2001). When people explain
difficult actions, is the causal question how or why? Journal of Language and Social
Psychology, 20, 339-357.
McDonnell, C. G., Valentino, K., Comas, M., & Nuttall, A. K. (2016). Motherchild reminiscing
at risk: Maternal attachment, elaboration, and child autobiographical memory specificity.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 65-84.
McGregor, I., & Holmes, J. G. (1999). How storytelling shapes memory and impressions of
relationship events over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 403-419.
McLean, K. C. (2015). The co-authored self: Family stories and the construction of personal
identity. New York: Oxford University Press.
McLean, K. C., & Mansfield, C. D. (2011). To reason or not to reason: Is autobiographical
reasoning always beneficial? New directions for child and adolescent development, (131),
85-97.
McLean, K. C., & Syed, M. (2015). Personal, master, and alternative narratives: An integrative
framework for understanding identity development in context. Human Development, 58,
318-349.
McLean, K. C., & Thorne, A. (2003). Adolescents’ self-defining memories about relationships.
Developmental Psychology, 39, 635645.
McVee, M. B. (2005). Revisiting the black Jesus: Re-emplotting a narrative through multiple
retellings. Narrative Inquiry, 15, 161-195.
Mead, G. H. (1904). The relation of psychology and philology. Psychological Bulletin, 1, 375-
391.
Mead, G. H. (1910). Social consciousness and the consciousness of meaning. Psychological
Bulletin, 7, 397-405.
Mead, G. H. (1912). The mechanism of social consciousness. Journal of Philosophy,
Psychology and Scientific Methods, 9, 401-406.
Mead, G. H. (1925). The genesis of the self and social control. International Journal of Ethics,
35, 251-277.
References 17
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. University of Chicago Press: Chicago.
Meade, M. L., Harris, C. B., Van Bergen, P., Sutton, J., & Barnier, A. J. (Eds.). (2018).
Collaborative remembering: Theories, research, and applications. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. Berlin: DeGruyter.
Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., Jacobsen, T., & Knoop, C. A. (2017). The
emotional and aesthetic powers of parallelistic diction. Poetics, 63, 47-59.
Mergenthaler, E. (1996). Emotion-abstraction patterns in verbatim protocols: A new way of
describing psychotherapeutic processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
64, 1306-1315.
Merrill, N., & Fivush, R. (2016). Intergenerational narratives and identity across
development. Developmental Review, 40, 72-92.
Miall, D. S. (2006). Literary reading. New York: Peter Lang.
Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1994). Beyond text theory: Understanding literary response.
Discourse Processes, 17, 337-354.
Mickes, L. (2015). Receiver operating characteristic analysis and confidenceaccuracy
characteristic analysis in investigations of system variables and estimator variables that
affect eyewitness memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4, 93-
102.
Mickes, L. (2016). The effects of verbal descriptions on eyewitness memory: Implications for
the real-world. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 270-276.
Mills, C. W. (1940). Situated actions and vocabularies of motive. American Sociological
Review, 5, 904-913.
Minami, M. (2008). Telling good stories in different languages: Bilingual children’s styles of
story construction and their linguistic and educational implications. Narrative Inquiry, 18,
83-110.
Mishler, E. G. (2004). Historians of the self: Restorying lives, revising identities. Research in
Human Development, 1, 101-121.
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of
emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5, 119-124.
Morgan, C. A., Southwick, S., Steffian, G., Hazlett, G. A., & Loftus, E. F. (2013).
Misinformation can influence memory for recently experienced, highly stressful events.
International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 36, 11-17.
Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Leisure reading and social cognition: A meta-analysis.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 109-120.
Muntigl, P. (2004). Narrative counselling: Social and linguistic processes of change.
Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.
Muntigl, P., & Horvath, A. O. (2014). “I can see some sadness in your eyes”: When
experiential therapists notice a client’s affectual display. Research on Language and
Social Interaction, 47, 89-108.
Muntigl, P., Knight, N. K., & Angus, L. (2014). Targeting emotional impact in storytelling:
Working with client affect in emotion-focused psychotherapy. Discourse Studies, 16, 753-
775.
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news
about Challenger. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall:
Studies of "flashbulb" memories (pp. 9-31). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson, K. L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2001). Narrative structure in recounted sad memories.
Discourse Processes, 31, 307-324.
References 18
Nelson, K. L., Bein, E., Huemer, J., Ryst, E., & Steiner, H. (2009). Listening for avoidance:
Narrative form and defensiveness in adolescent memories. Child psychiatry and human
Development, 40, 561-573.
Nelson, K. L., Moskovitz, D. J., & Steiner, H. (2008). Narration and vividness as measures of
event-specificity in autobiographical memory. Discourse Processes, 45, 195-209.
Niedenthal, P. M., & Brauer, M. (2012). Social functionality of human emotion. Annual
Review of Psychology, 63, 259-285.
Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15,
467-482.
Nissim Momigliano, L. (1984). Two persons talking in a room. In L. Nissim Momigliano (1992),
Shared experience: The psychoanalytic dialogue (pp. 5-20). London: Karnac.
Norrick, N. R. (1998). Retelling stories in spontaneous conversation. Discourse Processes, 25,
75-97.
Norrick, N. R. (2000). Conversational narrative: Storytelling in everyday talk. Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Norrick, N. R. (2012). Remembering for narration and autobiographical memory. Language
and Dialogue, 2, 193-215.
Nye, C. H. (1996). Narrative re-telling in clinical treatment: a single case study. Journal of
Analytic Social Work, 3, 113-136.
Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of the emotions. Cambridge University
Press.
Oatley, K. (1995). A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of
identification in fictional narrative. Poetics, 23, 53-74.
Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as cognitive and
emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology, 3, 101-117.
Oatley, K. (2011). Such stuff as dreams: The psychology of fiction. New York: Wiley.
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition
and emotion, 1, 29-50.
Olson, G. (2003). Reconsidering unreliability: Fallible and untrustworthy narrators. Narrative,
11, 93-109.
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Özyıldırım, I. (2009). Narrative analysis: An analysis of oral and written strategies in personal
experience narratives. Journal of pragmatics, 41, 1209-1222.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Palmer, A. (2004). Fictional minds. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning
making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 136,
257-301.
Park, J., Ayduk, Ö., & Kross, E. (2016). Stepping back to move forward: Expressive writing
promotes self-distancing. Emotion, 16, 349-364.
Pasupathi, M. (2018). Stories for all ages: Narrating anger reduces distress across childhood
and adolescence. Submitted for publication.
Pasupathi, M., & Billitteri, J. (2015). Being and becoming through being heard: Listener
effects on stories and selves. International Journal of Listening, 29, 67-84.
References 19
Pasupathi, M., Billitteri, J., Mansfield, C. D., Wainryb, C., Hanley, G. E., & Taheri, K. (2015).
Regulating emotion and identity by narrating harm. Journal of Research in Personality,
58, 127-136.
Pasupathi, M., Mansour, E., & Brubaker, J. R. (2007). Developing a life story: Constructing
relations between self and experience in autobiographical narratives. Human
Development, 50, 85-110.
Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M., & Murdoch, K. (1998). How what we tell becomes what we
know: Listener effects on speakers’ long‐term memory for events. Discourse Processes,
26, 1-25.
Pasupathi, M., Wainryb, C., Mansfield, C. D., & Bourne, S. (2017). The feeling of the story:
Narrating to regulate anger and sadness. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 444-461.
Pellecchia, G., Cucchi, M., Dimaggio. G., Cavadini, D., Carcione, A., Battelli, V., Niicolò, G.,
Pedone, R., Suiccardi, T., D’Angerio, S., Ronchi, P., Maffei, C., & Smeraldi, E. (2015). The
development of the Metacognition Assessment Interview: Instrument description, factor
structure, and reliability in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Research, 200, 890-895.
Pellecchia, G., Moroni, F., Carcione, A., Colle, L., Dimaggio, G., Nicolò, G., Pedone, R.,
Procacci, M. & & Semerari, A. (2015). Metacognition assessment interview: instrument
description and factor structure. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 12, 157-165.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. In Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 211-244). New York: Academic Press.
Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). The secret life of pronouns. New York: Bloomsberry.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: toward an
understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274-281.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2011). Expressive writing: Connections to physical and
mental health. Oxford handbook of health psychology (pp. 417-437). Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243-1254.
Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive
bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 863-871.
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1991). A cognitive theory of juror decision making: The story
model. Cardozo Law Review, 13, 519-557.
Peräkylä, A. (2004). Making links in psychoanalytic interpretations: A conversation analytical
perspective. Psychotherapy Research, 14, 289-307.
Peräkylä, A. (2010). Shifting the perspective after the patient’s response to an interpretation.
The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91, 1363-1384.
Peräkylä, A., Henttonen, P., Voutilainen, L., Kahri, M., Stevanovic, M., Sams, M., & Ravaja, N.
(2015). Sharing the emotional load: recipient affiliation calms down the storyteller. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 78, 301-323.
Peräkylä, A., Henttonen, P., Voutilainen, L., Kahri, M., Stevanovic, M., Sams, M., & Ravaja, N.
(2015). Sharing the emotional load: recipient affiliation calms down the storyteller. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 78, 301-323.
Peters, K., & Kashima, Y. (2007). From social talk to social action: Shaping the social triad
with emotion sharing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 780-797.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental Psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at
a chold’s narrative. New York: Plenum.
Phelan, J. (2007). Experiencing fiction: Judgments, progressions, and the rhetorical theory of
narrative. Athens, OH: The Ohio State University Press.
References 20
Pillemer, D. B., Picariello, M. L., & Pruett, J. C. (1994). Very longterm memories of a salient
preschool event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 95-106.
Pillemer, D. B., Steiner, K. L., Kuwabara, K. J., Thomsen, D. K., & Svob, C. (2015). Vicarious
memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 233-245.
Plantinga, C. (2009). Moving viewers: American film and the spectator's experience. Oakland,
CA: University of California Press.
Polanyi, L. (1981). Telling the same story twice. Text-Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of
Discourse, 1, 315-336.
Polya, T., Laszlo, J., & Forgas, J. P. (2005). Making sense of life stories: The role of narrative
perspective in perceiving hidden information about social identity. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 35, 785-796.
Pomerantz, A. (1986). Extreme case formulations: A way of legitimizing claims. Human
studies, 9, 219-229.
Porto, M. D., & Romano, M. (2017). ‘You’ve just got to walk away’: Mixed viewpoints in radio
call-in trauma narratives. Journal of Pragmatics, 122, 35-49.
Propp, V. (1928). Morphology of the folktale. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press [2010].
Quasthoff, U. (1980). Erzählen in Gesprächen. Tübingen: Narr.
Racker, H. (1959). Transference and countertransference. London: Karnac [1988].
Raney, A. A. (2011). The role of morality in emotional reactions to and enjoyment of media
entertainment. Journal of Media Psychology, 23, 18-23.
Recchia, H. E., Wainryb, C., & Howe, N. (2013). Two sides to every story? Parents'
attributions of culpability and their interventions into sibling conflict. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 59, 1-22.
Reese, E., Bird, A., & Tripp, G. (2007). Children's Self‐esteem and Moral Self: Links to Parent–
Child Conversations Regarding Emotion. Social Development, 16, 460-478.
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past:
Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403-430.
Reese, E., Myftari, E., McAnally, H. M., Chen, Y., Neha, T., Wang, Q., Jack, F., & Robertson, S.
J. (2017). Telling the tale and living well: Adolescent narrative identity, personality traits,
and wellbeing across cultures. Child Development, 88, 612-628.
Reich, W. (1933). Character analysis. New York: Macmillan [1980].
Renik, O. (2006). Practical psychoanalysis for therapists and patients. New York: Other Press.
Ricoeur, P. (1990). Time and narrative, Vol. 3. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Ricoeur, P. (1992). Oneself as another. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Rideout, J. C. (2009). Voice, self, and persona in legal writing. Journal of the Legal Writing
Institute, 15, 67-107.
Rideout, J. C. (2013). A twice-told tale: Plausibility and narrative coherence in judicial
storytelling. Legal Communication & Rhetoric, 10, 67-88.
Riggan, W. E. (1982). Picaros, madmen, naifs, and clowns: the unreliable first-person
narration. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press.
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review.
Emotion Review, 1, 60-85.
Rimé, B., Mesquita, B., Boca, S., & Philippot, P. (1991). Beyond the emotional event: Six
studies on the social sharing of emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 5, 435-465.
Rimmon-Kenan, S. (1983). Narrative fiction: Contemporary poetics. London: Routledge.
Roediger, H. L., Bergman, E. T., & Meade, M. L. (2000). Repeated reproduction from
memory. In A. Saito (Ed.), Bartlett, culture and cognition (pp.115-134). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
References 21
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, implications, and theory.
Houghton Mifflin.
Romano, M., Porto, M. D., & Molina, C. (2013). The structure of emotion discourse: From
Labovian to socio-cognitive models. Text & Talk, 33, 71-93.
Römisch, S., Leban, E., Habermas, T., & Döll-Hentschker, S. (2014). Evaluation, involvement,
and fragmentation in narratives of distressing, angering, and happy events by
traumatized and non-traumatized women. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice and Policy, 6, 465-472.
Rose, M. R., Nadler, J., & Clark, J. (2006). Appropriately upset? Emotion norms and
perceptions of crime victims. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 203-219.
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and
counting-out rhymes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G. Brown & A. Collins (Eds),
Representation and understanding (pp. 211-236). New York: Academic.
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other
things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76, 805.
Ruusuvuori, J., & Voutilainen, L. (2009). Comparing affiliating responses to troubles-tellings
in different types of health care encounters. In M. Haakana, M. Laakso & J. Lindstrom
(Eds.), Talk in interaction. Comparative dimensions (pp. 206-230). Helsinki: Finnish
Literature Society. 37.
Sacks, H. (1974). An analysis of the course of a joke’s telling in conversation. In R. Bauman &
J. Sherzer (Eds.), Explorations in the ethnography of speaking (pp. 337353). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation, Vols. 1 & 2. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
Salem, S., Weskott, T., & Holler, A. (2017). Does narrative perspective influence readers’
perspective-taking? An empirical study on free indirect discourse, psycho-narration and
first-person narration. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2, 61.
Sandig, B. (1996). Sprachliche Perspektivierung und perspektivierende Stile. Zeitschrift für
Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik, 26, 36-63.
Sandler, J. (1976). Countertransference and role-responsiveness. International Review of
Psychoanalysis, 3, 43-48.
Sarbin, T. (1986)(Ed.). Narrative Psychology: The Storied Nature of Human Conduct. New
York.
Schafer, R. (1983). The analytic attitude. New York: Basic books.
Scheff, T. J. (1979). Catharsis in healing, ritual, and drama. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Schegloff, E.A. (1982). Discourse as an interactional achievement: some uses of ‘‘uhhuh" and
other things that come between sentences. In: Tannen (Eds.), Analyzing Discourse: Text
and Talk (pp. 71-93). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Scheidt, C. E., & Lucius-Hoene, G. (2015). Kategorisierung und narrative Bewältigung
bindungsbezogener Traumaerfahrungen im Erwachsenenbindungsinterview. In C. F.
Scheidt, G. Lucius-Hoene, A. Stukenbrock, & E. Waller (2015). Narrative Bewältigung von
Trauma und Verlust (pp. 26-38). Stuttgart: Schattauer Verlag.
Scheler, M. (1923). Wesen und Form der Sympathie. Bonn, GE: Cohen.
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process
approach. In K. R. Scherer &P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293-317).
Hilldsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 22
Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of emotions: A review of research paradigms.
Speech Communication, 40, 227-256.
Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component
process model. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1307-1351.
Scherer, K. R. (2015). When and why are emotions disturbed? Suggestions based on theory
and data from emotion research. Emotion Review, 7, 238-249.
Scherer, K. R., Wallbot, H. G., & Summerfield, A. B. (1986). Experiencing emotions: A cross
cultural study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, and Paris: Éditions de la Maison
des Sciences de l’homme.
Schiff, B. (2005). Telling it in time: Interpreting consistency and change in the life stories of
Holocaust survivors. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 60,
189-212.
Schiff, B. (2017). A new narrative for psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Schiff, B., Skillingstead, H., Archibald, O., Arasim, A., & Peterson, J. (2006). Consistency and
change in the repeated narratives of Holocaust survivors. Narrative Inquiry, 16, 349-377.
Schmid, W. (2005). Narratology: An introduction. Berlin: deGruyter, 2010.
Schmid-Kitsikis, E. (1985). Sviluppo cognitivo e processo di mentalizzazione. Ricerche di
Psicologia, 9, 17-49.
Schütz, A. (1932). Der sinnhafte Aufbau der sozialen Welt. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp [1974].
Schütze, F. (1983). Biographieforschung und narratives Interview. Neue Praxis, 13, 283-293.
Schwitalla, J. (1997). Gesprochenes Deutsch. Berlin: Erich Schmidt Verlag.
Seehausen, M., Kazzer, P., Bajbouj, M., & Prehn, K. (2012). Effects of empathic paraphrasing
extrinsic emotion regulation in social conflict. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 482.
Seih, Y. T., Chung, C. K., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). Experimental manipulations of
perspective taking and perspective switching in expressive writing. Cognition & Emotion,
25, 926-938.
Selman, R. L. (1980). Growth of interpersonal understanding. San Francisco, CA: Academic
Press.
Selting, M. (2017). The display and management of affectivity in climaxes of amusing stories.
Journal of Pragmatics, 111, 1-32.
Semerari, A., Cucchi, M., Dimaggio, G., Cavadini, D., Carcione, A., Battelli, V., ... & Ronchi, P.
(2012). The development of the Metacognition Assessment Interview: Instrument
description, factor structure and reliability in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Research,
200, 890-895.
Shafer, D. M., & Raney, A. A. (2012). Exploring how we enjoy antihero narratives. Journal of
Communication, 62, 1028-1046.
Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books.
Shen, D. (2011). Unreliability. . In P. Hühn et al. (eds.), The living handbook of narratology.
Hamburg: Hamburg University Press. URL = Hamburg: Hamburg University Press.
htp://hup.sub.uni-hamburg.de/lhn/index.php?-tle=Unreliability&oldid=1529 [viewed
1/17/2013]
Shen, D., & Xu, D. (2007). Intratextuality, extratextuality, intertextuality: Unreliability in
autobiography versus fiction. Poetics Today, 28, 43-87.
Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual
Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 379-405.
Shields, S. A. (2005). The politics of emotion in everyday life: "Appropriate" emotion and
claims on identity. Review of General Psychology, 9, 3-15.
References 23
Shklovsky, V. (1919). Art as device. In Shklovsky, V. (1990), Theory of prose (pp.1-14).
Normal, IL: Dalkey Archive Press.
Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic
patients. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 22(, 255-262.
Silverstein, M. (1984). On the pragmatic ‘poetry’of prose: Parallelism, repetition, and
cohesive structure in the time course of dyadic conversation. In D. Schiffrin (Ed.),
Meaning, form, and use in context: Linguistic applications (pp. 181-199). Norwood, NJ.:
Ablex.
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest the curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
17, 57-60.
Skowronski, J. J., Sedikides, C., Xie, W., & Zhou, X. (2015). Changing the working self alters
the emotions prompted by recall. Memory, 23, 254-267.
Skowronski, J., Gibbons, J., Vogl, R., & Walker, W. R. (2004). The effect of social disclosure on
the intensity of affect provoked by autobiographical memories. Self and Identity, 3, 285-
309.
Smith, G. M. (2003). Film structure and the emotion system. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Smorti, A., & Fioretti, C. (2016). Why narrating changes memory: a contribution to an
integrative model of memory and narrative processes. Integrative Psychological and
Behavioral Science, 50, 296-319.
Spackman, M. P., & Gerrod Parrott, W. (2001). Emotionology in prose: A study of
descriptions of emotions from three literary periods. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 553-573.
Steblay, N. K., & Dysart, J. E. (2016). Repeated Eyewitness Identification Procedures With the
Same Suspect. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 284-289.
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school
children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53-120).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1981). What's in a story: An approach to comprehension and
instruction. Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report; no. 200. Urbana, IL:
University of Illinois.
Sterba, R. (1934). The fate of the ego in analytic therapy. The International Journal of Psycho-
Analysis, 15, 117-126.
Sternberg, M. (1992). Telling in time (II): Chronology, teleology, narrativity. Poetics Today,
13, 463-541.
Stiles, W. B. (2011). Coming to terms. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 367-384.
Stiles, W. B., Honos-Webb, L., & Lani, J. A. (1999). Some functions of narrative in the
assimilation of problematic experiences. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1213-1226.
Stiles, W. B., Morrison, L. A., Haw, S. K., Harper, H., Shapiro, D. A., & Firth-Cozens, J. (1991).
Longitudinal study of assimilation in exploratory psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Research, Practice, Training, 28, 195-206.
Stivers, T. (2008). Stance, alignment, and affiliation during storytelling: When nodding is a
token of affiliation. Research on language and social interaction, 41, 31-57.
Stockwell, P. (2009). Texture: A cognitive aesthetics of reading. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh
University Press.
Streit Olness, G., Ulatowska, H., Carpenter, C., Williams-Hubbard, L., & Dykes, J. (2005).
Holistic assessment of narrative quality: A social validation study. Aphasiology, 19, 251-
262.
References 24
Styers, M. K., & Baker-Ward, L. (2013). Finding the light at the end of the tunnel: Age
differences in the relation between internal states terms and coping with potential
threats to self. Memory, 21, 27-43.
Tal-Or, N., & Cohen, J. (2010). Understanding audience involvement: Conceptualizing and
manipulating identification and transportation. Poetics, 38, 402-418.
Tan, E. S. (1995). Film-induced affect as a witness emotion. Poetics, 23, 7-32.
Tan, E. S. (1996). Emotion and the structure of narrative film: Film as an emotion machine.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tannen, D. (1982). Oral and literate strategies in spoken and written narratives. Language,
58, 1-21.
Tannen, D. (1986). Introducing constructed dialogue in Greek and American conversational
and literary narrative. In F. Coulmas (Ed.), Direct and indirect speech (p. 311-332). Berlin:
deGruyter.
Tannen, D. (1989). Talking voices repetition, dialogue, and imagery in conversational
discourse. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Target, M., & Fonagy, P. (1996). Playing with reality II. The International journal of psycho-
analysis, 77, 459-479.
Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and
empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 1-18.
Tenenbaum, H. R., Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., & Dunne, G. (2008). The effects of explanatory
conversations on children's emotion understanding. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 26, 249-263.
Thoits, P. A. (1985). Self-labeling processes in mental illness: The role of emotional deviance.
American Journal of Sociology, 91, 221-249.
Thoits, P. A. (2004). Emotion norms, emotion work, and social order. In A. S. R. manstrad, N.
Frijda & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 359-
378). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Thomä, D. (1998). Erzähle dich selbst. Lebensgeschichte als philosophisches Problem.
München: Beck.
Thomsen, D. K., & Pillemer, D. B. (2017). I know my story and I know your story: Developing a
conceptual framework for vicarious life stories. Journal of Personality, 85, 464-480.
Todorov, T. (1969). Grammaire du „Décaméron“. Frankfurt: Mouton.
Tolins, J., & Tree, J. E. F. (2014). Addressee backchannels steer narrative development.
Journal of Pragmatics, 70, 152-164.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and
sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28,
675-691.
Tomkins, S. S. (1978). Script theory: Differential magnification of affects. In Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation, 26, 201-236.
Tomkins, S. S. (1991). Affect imagery consciousness, volume III. In Tomkins, S. S. (2008).
Affect imagery consciousness the complete edition. New York: Springer.
Toolan, M. (1988). Narrative - A Critical Linguistic Introduction. London: Routledge.
Trabasso, T., & Van Den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative
events. Journal of memory and language, 24, 612-630.
Trabasso, T., Stein, N. L., Rodkin, P. C., Munger, M. P., & Baughn, C. R. (1992). Knowledge of
goals and plans in the on-line narration of events. Cognitive Development, 7, 133-170.
References 25
Trevarthen, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In Stein
Bråten (Ed.). Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 15-46).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ulatowska, H. K., Streit Olness, G., Samson, A. M., Keebler, M. W., & Goins, K. E. (2004). On
the nature of personal narratives of high quality. Advances in Speech Language
Pathology, 6, 3-14.
Uspensky, B. A. (1972). Structural isomorphism of verbal and visual art. Poetics, 2, 5-39.
Van Abbema, D. L., McConell, P. M., Flanagan, E. H. & Mitchell M. (2005). What matters? The
impact of report mode and event valence on male and female autobiographical
narratives in late adolescence. Poster at the Biannual Meeting of SRCD, Atlanta, March
2005.
Van den Broek, P., Lorch, E. P., & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children's and Adults' Memory for
Television Stories: The Role of Causal Factors, StoryGrammar Categories, and
Hierarchical Level. Child Development, 67, 3010-3028.
Van Laer, T., De Ruyter, K., Visconti, L. M., & Wetzels, M. (2013). The extended
transportation-imagery model: A meta-analysis of the antecedents and consequences of
consumers' narrative transportation. Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 797-817.
Van Peer, W., Hakemulder, J., & Zyngier, S. (2007). Lines on feeling: foregrounding,
aesthetics and meaning. Language and Literature, 16, 197-213.
Vansina, Jan M. (1985). Oral tradition as history. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.
Varvin, S., & Stiles, W. (1999). Emergence of severe traumatic experiences: An assimilation
analysis of psychoanalytic therapy with a political refugee. Psychotherapy Research, 9,
381-404.
Vehvilainen, S. (2003). Preparing and delivering interpretations in psychoanalytic interaction.
Text, 23, 573-606.
Vehviläinen, S. (2008). Identifying and managing resistance in psychoanalytic interaction. In
A. Peräkylä (Ed.), Conversation analysis and psychotherapy (pp. 120-138). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Verduyn, P., Delaveau, P., Rotgé, J. Y., Fossati, P., & Van Mechelen, I. (2015). Determinants of
emotion duration and underlying psychological and neural mechanisms. Emotion Review,
7, 330-335.
Visch, V. T., Tan, E. S., & Molenaar, D. (2010). The emotional and cognitive effect of
immersion in film viewing. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1439-1445.
von Koppenfels, M. (2007). Immune Erzähler: Flaubert und die Affektpolitik des modernen
Romans. Wilhelm Fink.
Von Wright, G. H. (1971). Explanation and understanding. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University
Press.
Voss, C. (2004). Narrative Emotionen: Eine Untersuchung über Möglichkeiten und Grenzen
philosophischer Emotionstheorien. Berlin: de Gruyter.
Voutilainen, L., & Peräkylä, A. (2016). Interactional practices of psychotherapy. In M. O’Reily
& J. N. Lester (Eds.), The Palgrave Handbook of Adult Mental Health (pp. 540-557).
London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Vromen, S. (2016). Linking religion and family: Memories of children hidden in Belgian
convents during the holocaust. In M. L. Seeberg, I. Levin, & C. Lenz (Eds.), The Holocaust
as active memory (pp, 15-28). London: Routledge.
Vygotski, L. S. (1925). Psychology of art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [1974].
Vygotsky, L. S., & Luria, A. R. (1930). Studies on the history of behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
[1993].
References 26
Wainryb, C., & Recchia, H. E. (2014). Parent-child conversations as contexts for moral
development: Why conversations, and why conversations with parents. In C. Wainryb &
H. E. Recchia (Eds.), Talking about right and wrong: Parent-child conversations as
contexts for moral development (pp. 3-20). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wang, Q., Doan, S. N., & Song, Q. (2010). Talking about internal states in motherchild
reminiscing influences children's self-representations: A cross-cultural study. Cognitive
Development, 25, 380-393.
Warner, L. R., & Shields, S. A. (2009). Judgements of others’ emotional appropriateness are
multidimensional. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 876-888.
Waters, T. Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (2014). Autobiographical memory functions served by
different event types. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 185-195.
Waters, T., Köber, C., & Fivush, R. (2018). A meta-analysis of studies of parental narrative
memory scaffolding of children. In preparation.
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the
effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation.
Psychological Bulletin, 138, 775-808.
Weiste, E., & Peräkylä, A. (2013). A comparative conversation analytic study of formulations
in psychoanalysis and cognitive psychotherapy. Research on Language and Social
Interaction, 46, 299-321.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The
frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331354. doi:10.
1037/0033-2909.121.3.331.
White, M., & Epston, D. (1990). Narrative means to therapeutic ends. New York: Norton.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining
function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13,
103-128.
Winnicott, D. W. (1963). The development of the capacity for concern. Bulletin of the
Menninger Clinic, 27, 167-176.
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Play and reality. London: Tavistock.
Wundt, W. (1887). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie (3rd rev ed.). Leipzig:
Engelmann.
Wundt, W. (1900). Völkerpsychologie. Erster Band: Die Sprache (Part 1). Leipzig: Engelmann.
Yurtdaş, G. T. (2012). Repetitions in Turkish: Talk among friends. Forum: Qualitative Social
Research, 13.2, 22.
Zifonun, G., Hoffmann, L., & Strecker, B. (1997). Grammatik der deutschen Sprache, Vols. 1-3.
Berlin, GE: Walter de Gruyter.
Zillman, D., & Cantor, J. R. (1977). Affective responses to the emotions of a protagonist.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 155-165.
Zillmann, D. (1995). Mechanisms of emotional involvement with drama. Poetics, 23, 33-51.
Zillmann, D. (2006a). Dramaturgy for emotions from fictional narration. In J. Bryant & P.
Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of entertainment (pp. 215-238). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zillmann, D. (2006b). Empathy: Affective reactivity to others’ emotional experiences. In J.
Bryant & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of entertainment (pp. 151-181). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Zunshine, L. (2008). Theory of mind and fictions of embodied transparency. Narrative, 16,
65-92.
... Despite decades of research, there are many unresolved epistemological issues in the theorisation of emotions (Cerulo and Scribano, 2022). Ongoing discussions include the link between emotions and actions (Bericat, 2015), the nature of emotions and feelings (Turner and Stets, 2006), the connection between subconscious processes and collective phenomena (Habermas, 2018), and the relation between emotions and discursive scenarios (Kleres, 2011). The latter is particularly important for this article since it addresses how prevailing narratives enable or dissuade experiences and social practices, such as racism (Ahmed, 2017), social change and masculinity (Logoz et al., 2023), and attitudes towards women . ...
... In other words, they do not reside in subjects or situations but are as (Ahmed, 2017: 11) emphasise 'produced in circulation'. Therefore, emotions can be understood as structured by social scenarios, which informs individuals' action tendencies towards others (Habermas, 2018). ...
... In this study, we underline the argument developed by sociologists and anthropologists who have suggested that emotions should not be reduced to mere mental experiences, but be seen as social practices (Ahmed, 2017;Katz, 2002;Rosaldo, 1994;White, 1993). As emphasised by contemporary scholars of emotions and narrative (Cerulo and Scribano, 2022;Habermas, 2018;Kusenbach and Loseke, 2013;Turner and Stets, 2006), going beyond the minutiae of experience and linking emotions instead to narratives that sustain them might contribute to a broader understanding of many social phenomena. Researching narrated emotions can thus widen the analytic lens in the study of femicide. ...
Article
Full-text available
While studies of femicide perpetrators have focused on background factors, such as criminal history and mental health conditions, little attention has been paid to their individual experiences. Perpetrators emotions and sense-making have often been overlooked and even dismissed. With a micro-sociological approach to violence, we identify the narrated emotions involved in the perpetration of intimate femicide. The data gathered are based on 33 open-ended interviews with convicted male perpetrators from Argentina, Bolivia, Brazil, Chile, Honduras, Mexico, and Venezuela. We identify four main emotions reflecting participants' experiences of femicide: Fear, expressed through stories of women as threats to self, family, and community; helplessness, expressed through stories of men being trapped, judged, and persecuted; and pain, connected to stories of jealousy and belittlement. These lead to anger, expressed through stories of bodily reactions and losing control. The findings indicate that intimate femicide perpetrators resort to lethal violence to regulate self-worth and remediate actions they feel were disruptive. Our research demonstrates the importance of embodied and narrated emotions to understand femicides. We argue that viewing femicide as a product of a shared pervasive emotional economy highlights the role of emotions in maintaining a gendered social order.
... The design is based on specific methods used within research on narratives on recollections of personal experiences, where the information obtained from participants' narratives is typically transformed through standardized coding systems. [26][27][28][29] This study was part of a larger project based on a randomised, usual care controlled, parallel-group pragmatic clinical trial). 15 During a specific time period within the course of the study, participants were consecutively invited to take part in the substudy at the last clinical visit of the trial which forms the basis of this article. ...
... Qualitative data based on personal narratives derived from open-ended questions are a standard method within research on personal recollections which is used to better understand individuals and their experiences. The information obtained from these brief yet rich narratives is typically transformed through standardized coding systems to help us to better understand how the structure and content of personal experiences may be altered during, for instance, illness, life-events, psychopathology, etc. 26,28,30,31 Two independent coders (coder A and B) blinded to patients' treatment group coded three aspects of the narratives provided by the patients' responses; (1) Themes: The two coders coded themes based on a list of pre-defined themes (n = 19) which had been identified by a third independent coder reading carefully through all the responses. Subsequently, both coders were asked to read through all the narratives and to double-check if any potential themes were missing from the coding-list. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Immune-mediated inflammatory diseases (eg, axial spondylitis, psoriasis, psoriatic arthritis, Crohn’s disease, ulcerative colitis, and hidradenitis suppurativa) are common diseases that exert an extensive effect on the health-related quality of life, particularly when multiple concomitant conditions are present. Previous reports indicate that the traditional siloed approach to care can lead to a lack of patient centricity and inefficient disease management. In this article, we aimed to evaluate an interdisciplinary program for the treatment of immune-mediated inflammatory diseases compared to routine clinical practice. Patients and Methods This was a mixed-method study, combining qualitative and quantitative data. Patients with co-occurrence of ≥2 immune-mediated inflammatory diseases treated in an interdisciplinary clinic (n = 48) or traditional usual care (n = 17) answered open-ended questions about their care experiences. Two independent coders blinded to patients’ treatment group coded three aspects of the narratives provided by the patients’ responses: Themes, Emotional valence, and Personal growth (ie, redemption). Themes were analyzed descriptively to explore possible differences between patients assigned to the interdisciplinary clinic and patients assigned to the usual care setting. Group differences in patients’ emotional experiences were assessed, and we examined potential group differences in positive personal growth. Results Our findings indicate that an interdisciplinary combined clinic approach provides benefits for patients with multiple inflammatory diseases towards the usual setup. Patients experienced benefits on a number of specific quality-of-life themes including acceptance, optimism, disease understanding, personal growth, and better disease control. The narratives of the patients in the interdisciplinary group were significantly more emotionally positive and included more positive personal growth compared to the usual care group. Conclusion The findings indicate a patient-reported benefit, especially from the holistic approach and cross-specialty combined consultations in an interdisciplinary clinic compared to usual specialized healthcare, which was underscored by narratives highlighting an overall improved quality of life.
... This process of meaning making refers to the degree to which the protagonist of the story gains insight or learns something from an event (McAdams & McLean, 2013). Especially, more meaning making is related to conflicting or more negative life events (McLean & Thorne, 2003), resolving the negativity by narrating the experience and making sense of it (Habermas, 2018). Meaning making skills increase during adolescence, especially in mid-adolescence when youths are improving their management of contradictions and paradox in personal stories (McLean & Breen, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores functions of music listening in relation to emotion regulation and identity development in mid-adolescence. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 17 adolescents (15-16 years old), using a personally selected piece of music as a starting point. Analysis of the interviews focused on descriptions of emotional and identity related functions of their self-chosen music, complexity level of meaning making in participants' autobiographical narratives that emerged in relation to the music they had chosen, and the relation between functions and meaning making level. The results revealed two types of engagement with music entailing a connection between emotional and identity related functions. The first type involves a more basic form of interaction which is focused on influencing one's mood, and related to feelings of identification with the lively rhythm of the upbeat music they had selected. The second 146
... Narrativizing is imbued with emotion (Habermas, 2019;Hermans, 2003). For instance, autobiographical narrativizing can activate emotions of fear and anger if one's self-identity is threatened, sadness and shame if one version of the self is seen as contradicting another, or happiness and pride if multiple selves appear well aligned. ...
Article
Full-text available
How can educators equip business students to adopt an ethics of care perspective? An ethics of care perspective requires moral imagination of the self, the other, the self-other relationship, and displacement of motivation towards others. It stands in contrast to the deeply embedded firm-centric managerial perspective in business schools. Without a structured approach, students may struggle to adopt a care perspective. I propose three specifically designed narrativizing exercises that can act as vehicles for moral imagination for care by increasing student awareness of tensions between the self and others and motivating a harmonizing of these tensions. Autobiographical narrativizing surfaces discordance between a student’s managerial and non-managerial selves and invites an imaginative recomposition of these selves. Vicarious narrativizing occurs through stories of others, creating pathways between students' self-perspectives and that of others. Embodied narrativizing is a whole body and mind exercise in narrativizing the mental, visceral, and behavioural experiences during preparation for an upcoming interaction with another, along with narrativizing the actual interaction embedded in the other’s context. These three forms of narrativizing present complementary risks and benefits and cumulatively enable moral imagination for a care perspective.
... The overwhelming sharing of emotions resonated with prior research suggesting that the online environment, with its affordance of anonymity and virtual identities, lures people to speak their minds and promotes emotional profusion or overt displays of emotion (De Fina, 2016;Miller, 2008). It also reflects the narrative quality of emotion processes, especially the sharing of emotional experience (Habermas, 2019). To further unpack the nature of the shared emotions and moods, the comments under this children node of 'Topic' were further classified into were classified into 3 categories based on recognizing orientations or generally constructed notionspositive, negative, and mixed. ...
Article
Full-text available
The concept of ‘tree hole’ in social media has provided a disinhibited space for depressed individuals to communicate their depressive feelings, emotions and experiences freely. Its proliferation underscores an increasing need for such online spaces. However, their operational mechanisms, content dynamics, and implications for mental health remain under-explored, especially in the Mainland Chinese context. This study describes and interprets what is going on under ‘Zoufan’, the largest ‘tree hole’ on Weibo in China, by analyzing the patterns of online community engagement concerning discussions on depression. In an overarching project, a total of 67,614 comments under the last farewell post of ‘Zoufan’ were collected and compiled for an overview of the user’s participation patterns. This paper draws upon a set of data manually coded on the selected topics and a fine-grained analysis of linguistic features. The findings indicate a limited interest among selected users in interacting with others, with most of the content focusing on their adverse emotional states and the difficulties they face within interpersonal relationships. Moreover, the expressions contain a strong sense of negation and inhibition, often conveyed through fragmented narratives. The implications of this study help stakeholders, especially family members, educators, and mental health workers, to better understand the mental health outcomes of participation in online depression sharing. This, in turn, facilitates a more nuanced approach to addressing depression both online and in real-life interventions.
... This process of meaning making refers to the degree to which the protagonist of the story gains insight or learns something from an event (McAdams & McLean, 2013). Especially, more meaning making is related to conflicting or more negative life events (McLean & Thorne, 2003), resolving the negativity by narrating the experience and making sense of it (Habermas, 2018). Meaning making skills increase during adolescence, especially in mid-adolescence when youths are improving their management of contradictions and paradox in personal stories (McLean & Breen, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Este estudio explora las funciones de escuchar música relacionados con la regulación emocional y el desarrollo de la identidad en la adolescencia media. Se realizaron entrevistas semi-estructuradas con 17 adolescentes (15-16 años) a partir de una pieza de música elegida por cada participante. El análisis de las entrevistas se centró en las descripciones de las funciones, tanto emocionales como relacionadas con la identidad, el nivel de complejidad del significado que muestran los relatos autobiográficos de los participantes que surgieron relacionados con su música elegida, y la relación entre funciones y el nivel de elaboración de significado. Los resultados mostraron dos perfiles de uso de la música que suponen una conexión entre las funciones emocionales y de identidad. El primero es una forma más básica de interacción orientada a influir en el ánimo de uno mismo, ligada al hecho de sentirse identificado con el ritmo alegre de la música animada elegida. El segundo engloba una regulación emocional más compleja, además de una reflexión sobre uno/a mismo/a y su historia personal, facilitada por el contenido o la melodía. Los adolescentes con niveles superiores de elaboración de significado en sus relatos ligados a la música elegida, mostraron este uso más complejo de escuchar música. Se sugiere que escuchar música juega un papel importante en la interacción entre la regulación emocional y la construcción de la identidad en la adolescencia media. This study explores functions of music listening in relation to emotion regulation and identity development in mid-adolescence. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 17 adolescents (15-16 years old), using a personally selected piece of music as a starting point. Analysis of the interviews focused on descriptions of emotional and identity related functions of their self-chosen music, complexity level of meaning making in participants’ autobiographical narratives that emerged in relation to the music they had chosen, and the relation between functions and meaning making level. The results revealed two types of engagement with music entailing a connection between emotional and identity-related functions. The first type involves a more basic form of interaction which is focused on influencing one’s mood, and related to feelings of identification with the lively rhythm of the upbeat music they had selected. The second encompasses a more complex emotional regulation, as well as a reflection about the self and their personal history, facilitated by the lyrics or the melody. Adolescents with higher levels of narrative meaning making showed this more complex use of music listening. It is suggested that music listening plays an important role in the interplay between emotional regulation and identity development in mid-adolescence.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Some individuals struggle to adjust after the death of a close other. Constructing an adaptive narrative about the loss is important to adjustment but symptoms of psychopathology may interfere with this process. We examined whether psychopathology symptoms measured 2 months after the loss of a spouse or parent predicted loss narratives with lower agency and communion themes as well as more negative emotional tone and self-event connections. Method Participants included 507 adults from the Aarhus Bereavement Study who completed psychopathology symptom measures at 2, 6, 11, and 18 months after the death of their spouse or parent. Symptom measures included prolonged grief disorder (PGD), PTSD, depression, and anxiety. Eighteen months after the death, participants wrote a narrative about the loss and answered questions about its emotional qualities (i.e., tone, self-event connections). The narratives were content coded for agency and communion themes. Results Multiple regressions showed that higher PGD symptoms at 2 months predicted less adaptive loss narratives at 18 months, even after controlling for neuroticism and age. Conclusion Individuals who experience high symptom levels are struggling to construct adaptive loss narratives. This may hinder identity changes needed to accommodate their altered life circumstances.
Article
Full-text available
General Audience Summary Scholarly research examining autobiographical memory has focused primarily on recollections of personally experienced events. Yet, when facing challenging real-world circumstances, such as adapting to a new work environment, predicting the personal future, giving or receiving help in professional contexts, and dealing with personal failures, relying primarily on information provided by memories of firsthand experiences will often fall short. We propose that remembering specific events and detailed life stories recounted by others, termed vicarious memory, is an essential component of successful problem-solving and well-being. Vicarious memories enable individuals to move beyond a reliance on recollections of their own past experiences and to recognize and act upon the valuable lessons contained in memories of others’ activities and life stories. Specific examples include training community health workers to use memory sharing as a tool for instructing expectant and new mothers in successful parenting practices; attending closely to coworkers’ personal stories when adapting to a new corporate culture; remembering events from close others’ life histories to enable empathic interpersonal understanding; encouraging parents to talk with their children about their extended family history as a way to promote children’s narrative identity and positive sense of self; and nurturing hope and recovery in psychiatric patients through memory sharing with peer workers who have had their own mental health challenges. Remembering and extracting lessons contained in stories told by others provide diverse perspectives that inform future behaviors, enhance decision making, and promote interpersonal closeness, personal growth, and well-being.
Chapter
Full-text available
Wie erzählen Menschen ihre Lebensgeschichten? Und wie verändern sich diese Erzählungen im Laufe der Zeit? Shevek K. Selbert widmet sich an der Schnittstelle von Biographieforschung, Psychologie und Erzählforschung vollumfänglich wiederholten biographisch-narrativen Interviews, die im Abstand von zehn Jahren geführt wurden. Als Pionierarbeit qualitativer Längsschnittforschung entwickelt er Methoden, um die Erzählversionen miteinander zu vergleichen - und bietet einen einzigartigen Einblick in die Dynamik sowie die (auch therapeutische) Bedeutung von Selbsterzählungen für die Identitätsbildung und die Lebensgestaltung.
Book
Interpreting Figurative Meaning critically evaluates the recent empirical work from psycholinguistics and neuroscience examining the successes and difficulties associated with interpreting figurative language. There is now a huge, often contradictory literature on how people understand figures of speech. Gibbs and Colston argue that there may not be a single theory or model that adequately explains both the processes and products of figurative meaning experience. Experimental research may ultimately be unable to simply adjudicate between current models in psychology, linguistics and philosophy of how figurative meaning is interpreted. Alternatively, the authors advance a broad theoretical framework, motivated by ideas from 'dynamical systems theory', that describes the multiple, interacting influences which shape people's experiences of figurative meaning in discourse. This book details past research and theory, offers a critical assessment of this work and sets the stage for a new vision of figurative experience in human life.
Chapter
Das mehrfache Erzählen der gleichen Geschichte hat einen festen Platz in unserem Alltag und in unserer Kultur - sei es als Anekdote im Freundeskreis, als wiederholte Erzählung der Krankheitsgeschichte im Arzt-Patient-Gespräch oder auch als literarische Bearbeitung. Umso erstaunlicher ist es, dass dieses Phänomen bisher noch nicht Gegenstand interdisziplinärer Forschung war. Die Beiträge des Bandes behandeln das Wiedererzählen aus den Blickwinkeln verschiedener Forschungsrichtungen und beleuchten die Formen und Funktionen von Konstanz und Variation in den unterschiedlichen Erzählversionen in Zusammenhang mit ihren jeweils spezifischen Gesprächs- bzw. Entstehungskontexten.
Chapter
Das mehrfache Erzählen der gleichen Geschichte hat einen festen Platz in unserem Alltag und in unserer Kultur - sei es als Anekdote im Freundeskreis, als wiederholte Erzählung der Krankheitsgeschichte im Arzt-Patient-Gespräch oder auch als literarische Bearbeitung. Umso erstaunlicher ist es, dass dieses Phänomen bisher noch nicht Gegenstand interdisziplinärer Forschung war. Die Beiträge des Bandes behandeln das Wiedererzählen aus den Blickwinkeln verschiedener Forschungsrichtungen und beleuchten die Formen und Funktionen von Konstanz und Variation in den unterschiedlichen Erzählversionen in Zusammenhang mit ihren jeweils spezifischen Gesprächs- bzw. Entstehungskontexten.
Chapter
This major volume of original essays maps the place of emotion in human nature, through a discussion of the relation between consciousness and body; by analysing the importance of emotion for human agency by pointing to the ways in which practical rationality may be enhanced, as well as hindered, by emotions; and by exploring questions of value in making sense of emotions at a political, ethical and personal level. Leading researchers in the field reflect on the nature of human feelings, how and why we understand what other people feel, and the way in which our values become involved in specific emotional phenomena, such as guilt, fear, shame, amusement, or love. This collection addresses important questions in the philosophy of mind and comments on the implications of research in biology, cognitive psychology, psychoanalysis, and narrative theory for the philosophical understanding of emotions.
Book
What actually happens in counselling interactions? How does counselling bring about change? How do clients end up producing new and alternative stories of their lives and relationships? By addressing these questions and others, this book explores the narrative counselling process in the context where it is enacted: the unfolding conversation between counsellor and clients. Through a transdisciplinary approach that combines conversation analysis and systemic functional linguistic theory, Muntigl demonstrates how language is used in couples counselling, how language use changes over the course of counselling, and how this process provides clients with new linguistic resources that help them change their social relationships. This book will be a valuable resource not only for linguists and discourse analysts, but also for researchers and practitioners in the fields of counselling, psychotherapy, psychology, and medicine.