ArticlePDF Available

Imagining Possible Worlds

Authors:

Abstract

The reading of fiction has been found to confer benefits, including increased empathy and understanding of others. Among ongoing research questions are those of how people engage in imagined worlds while keeping in touch with the currently perceived world, as well as how far stories were important in human evolution and how the brain is involved understanding them.
INTRODUCTION
Imagining Possible Worlds
Keith Oatley
University of Toronto
Robin Dunbar and Felix Budelmann
University of Oxford
The reading of fiction has been found to confer benefits, including increased empathy and understanding
of others. Among ongoing research questions are those of how people engage in imagined worlds while
keeping in touch with the currently perceived world, as well as how far stories were important in human
evolution and how the brain is involved understanding them.
Keywords: fiction, narrative, imagination, mental models, theory-of-mind
Fictional storytelling, in oral recitals and theatrical perfor-
mances as well as in written literature, has a long history. In the
West Aristotle (330 BCE/1970) and in the East Bharata Muni (200
BCE/1986) joined psychology with literary analyses in their pro-
posals for its understanding.
For general psychology, exploration of fiction bridges across the
cognitive psychology of imagination and abstraction, the social
psychology of understanding other minds, and the evolutionary
psychology of how social life arose and how it is maintained in
humans. For the humanities, there is increased interest in cognitive
approaches, and in understanding what goes on in the minds of
people as they engage with fiction.
In the last few decades a new phase has begun with research
both in psychology and in the humanities. In 1986, Jerome Bruner,
a cognitive psychologist, published Actual Minds, Possible
Worlds, in which he proposed that narrative thinking is the way in
which we humans understand ourselves and each other, our inten-
tions, and the vicissitudes these intentions meet. In the same year
psychologist Willie van Peer published an investigation of effects
of literary style on people as they read. In 2003, Patrick Hogan,a
literary theorist, published The Mind and its Stories in which he
surveyed stories from all round the world from before the era of
European colonialism, and found love stories, stories of conflict,
and stories of sacrifice for a community, were so common as to be
almost human universals. In this special issue we suggest that the
psychology of fiction is continuing to be built as a joint endeavor
of psychology and the humanities. The contributions presented
here span both areas.
In reading or watching a piece of fiction, we engage ourselves
in an imagined world, but without losing all attachment to our real
surroundings. How is it that the human mind can manage to live in
two worlds simultaneously, without confusing them? This is a
phenomenon we do not yet fully understand. But we do know that,
as members of a species who make plans for the future, we
imagine possible versions of the world that are based on mental
models (Johnson-Laird, 1983). Such models can be about circum-
stances that are not perceived directly. So, while sitting in an
armchair alone, a reader of novel may understand a social inter-
action.
The title of this special issue is “The Psychology of Fiction” but
fiction is a complex concept. Biopics, mockumenteraries and part-
scripted reality TV programs show how uncertain the boundary
can be between reality and fiction. Moreover, the effects studied in
this issue do not all depend on fictionality. For the most part it is
the narrative depiction of people’s intentions, interactions, and
lives, which is at the center. So fiction, here, is to be broadly
understood, and conceived not an antithesis to fact, but in terms of
mode and content: narrative accounts of people and their interac-
tions in the social world. Within this remit are theater, novels, short
stories, films, and TV series, but also, too, narratives of the kind
that occur in biography, memoir, and narrative history.
The Cognitive World of Fiction
In recent research on effects of reading, a distinction has been
made between fiction and explanatory nonfiction so that, for
instance, the substantial benefits that have been found for reading,
in improvements of vocabulary and verbal reasoning abilities, are
due largely to the reading of fiction (Mar & Rain, 2015). Then, in
another development, in terms of understanding others, which in
psychology is often referred to as theory-of-mind, or mentalizing,
Hakemulder (2000) found that people who read a fictional narra-
tive of an Algerian woman were more likely to understand her
point of view as a woman in that society than those who read a
nonfictional account of gender relationships in Algeria. Mar, Oat-
ley, Hirsh, dela Paz, and Peterson (2006) reported that people who
Keith Oatley, Department of Applied Psychology and Human Develop-
ment, University of Toronto; Robin Dunbar, Magdalen College, University
of Oxford; Felix Budelmann, Magdalen College, University of Oxford.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Keith
Oatley, Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development,
University of Toronto, 252 Bloor Street West, Toronto, ON M5S1V6,
Canada. E-mail: keith.oatley@utoronto.ca
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Review of General Psychology
© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 22, No. 2, 121–124
1089-2680/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000149
121
read fiction, as compared with nonfiction, did better on a non-
narrative test of empathy and theory-of-mind. In meta-analyses it
has found that these effects are small but replicable (Mumper &
Gerrig, 2017;Dodell-Feder & Tamir, 2018). Theoretical implica-
tions have been discussed by Oatley (2016) and De Mulder,
Hakemulder, and van der Berghe (2017).
How it is that from fictional worlds, we can understand so much
about states of mind, emotions, and intentions, of the people who
inhabit it? Some pointers may be provided by research on chil-
dren’s play. It used to be assumed that children cannot tell the
difference between the imaginary and the real so that, as we grow
up play recedes, and we come to focus on the real. Harris (2000),
however, has shown that this idea is misleading. Young children
are well able to differentiate between events in an imagined world,
such as pouring imaginary tea into toy tea-cups, and the real world
in which nothing actually gets wet. But play does not dwindle and
die. Like other human functions, it develops: it transforms into
adult modes such as games, sports, and indeed fiction. The trans-
formation starts early. As Mar, Tackett, and Moore (2010) showed,
preschool children to whom stories were read, and who watched
fictional movies, subsequently had better theory-of-mind abilities
than those who simply watched whatever happened to be on TV.
Barnes and Bloom (2014) found that children of ages between four
and eight already preferred stories that are about other minds.
Leslie (1987) noted that theory-of-mind enabled young children
to engage in fictional play: in effect, they engaged in imagined
worlds and imagined minds. Think of hide-and-seek. When you
are hiding, you enjoy the game by imagining the one who is
seeking.
Although we know a certain amount about mentalizing and its
natural history, one thing that has eluded us since the concept was
introduced is exactly what, in cognitive terms, this phenomenon
actually is; the question is explored in this Special Issue. At
another level, a salient property of fictional narratives is their
ability to move us emotionally. The ability of actors to enable us
to empathize with characters’ predicaments should not surprise us:
responding to facial and verbal cues is how we infer others’ mental
and emotional states in our everyday world. What is more surpris-
ing is the fact that these effects can be elicited off the page by just
a few carefully chosen words. The capacity of words to move us
clearly merits a great deal more experimental attention.
To what extent do individual differences in mentalizing ca-
pacity and emotionality affect our ability to immerse ourselves
in, and enjoy, fictional worlds? Carney, Wlodarski, and Dunbar
(2014) have shown experimentally that individuals with poorer
mentalizing abilities prefer less complex stories. There are also
important questions relating to the extent to which different
storytellers can move us. The storyteller’s linguistic skills must,
of course, play a role; but from a psychological perspective a
more interesting question may be whether their own mentaliz-
ing competences and emotional responsiveness influence their
ability to conceive and tell stories that move us. An author has
to work with a reader or audience-member’s mind, and pushing
people beyond what they can process will result in bafflement
and failure to engage. Conversely, failing to challenge people
may result in a less enthusiastic response. In effect, the author-
storyteller has to know people’s competences and be able to
work with them.
The Role of Stories in Human Evolution
We know that symbolic culture played an important role from
some 100,000 years ago. Early modern humans and Neanderthals
drilled holes in sea-shells and colored them with ochre, in order to
wear them as necklaces (Bar-Yosef Mayer et al., 2009;Hoffmann
et al., 2018). As Mithen (1996) explains, what began at that time
was metaphor, in which something was something that it was not.
A shell was not a shell; it was a bead of a necklace. Following
these early indications, evidence of burials has been found
(Coolidge & Wynn, 2016). Burials imply funerals, in which people
would imagine a world beyond the grave. At such events stories
about those who had died would been told. So a person who was
dead was a person who was alive on another plane, or in memory.
Boyd (2009),inOn the Origin of Stories, and Gottschall (2012),
in The Storytelling Animal, are among a growing number of
literary scholars who explore how the ability to create stories has
been important in human evolution. Dunbar (2014) has argued that
this ability played a central role in allowing ancestral and modern
humans to bond large communities, and that this must have in-
volved a long prehistory during which the psychological compe-
tences required were built up and scaffolded.
Tomasello (2014,2016) has argued that cooperation is funda-
mental to being human. We arrange and enact joint plans, in which
a goal in a possible world, shared with one or a few other people,
becomes more important than individual goals. And we take part
in cultures, which involve shared understandings of spoken lan-
guage, and of moralities such as the idea of justice as fairness in
society. In order to cooperate with other individuals or to take part
in society, we need to make mental models of others, to know who
they are, to know how they are likely to behave in shared endeav-
ors and in response to potential conflicts. As Dunbar (2004) has
emphasized, it is likely that the reason we need our large brains is
to house not only mental models of those whom we know, but also
the models of others’ models of other people and ourselves. Stories
told as anecdotes and gossip (Dunbar, 1996), whether orally or in
writing, have an effect of enabling human beings to make better
mental models of others, both for joint plans and for being mem-
bers of a cooperating cultural community (Dunbar, 2018). In
novels, for instance, we are able to identify with protagonists, and
so understand them from the inside.
Neural Bases of the Audience Experience
In 2011, Zeki announced that functional MRI (fMRI) is bringing
in a revolution, comparable in scope to that begun by Galileo in his
recognition that the earth revolves round the sun. The new revo-
lution, said Zeki, is the beginning of understanding how the brain
constructs experience. In a review of literature, Mar (2011) iden-
tified brain regions that are activated (i) when people understand
others in tasks presented in narrative form, (ii) when people
understand others in tasks presented in non-narrative form, and
(iii) when people comprehend stories. Although some regions of
activation were distinct for each kind of task, a core mentalizing
network was identified, within which there were substantial areas
of overlap for all three tasks. In other words, comprehension of
stories involves the understanding of other minds.
Part of the mentalizing network (the medial prefrontal cortex), is
also part of the so-called default mode network, which includes, as
well, the posterior cingulate cortex and the angular gyrus. The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
122 OATLEY, DUNBAR, AND BUDELMANN
default mode network is deactivated when one performs certain
tasks in the outside world, but is activated during spontaneous
self-reflection and when thinking about other people. Andrews-
Hanna (2012) proposes that “engaging in spontaneous thought can
allow individuals to construct and simulate alternative scenarios,
mentally organize their plans, and prepare for what may lie ahead”
(p. 260). In an example of interdisciplinary coordination, Vessel
and Rubin who are neuroscientists, worked with Starr, a theorist of
literature and other arts, to find that the process of engaging with
art that moves one, activates the default mode network; it reaches
within (Vessel, Starr, & Rubin, 2013).
There is some evidence that mentalizing is more cognitively
demanding than processing factual information. Two studies of
2012, one using memory for friends’ traits as a social working
memory task (Meyer et al., 2012), the other using eye gaze in an
implicit false belief task (Schneider, Lam, Bayliss, & Dux, 2012),
provide evidence for an effect of cognitive load when processing
social information. More directly, Lewis, Birch, Hall, and Dunbar
(2017) showed that processing propositions about the world of
intentions (mentalizing) recruits more brain neurons and is cogni-
tively more demanding (as indexed by reaction time [RT]) than
processing factual propositions of similar complexity about the
same story. These studies speak to the fact that humans’ large
brains may have their origins in the need to handle the demands of
virtual mental worlds.
Such studies suggest avenues for further neuroscientific work on
the processing of fiction. As yet, we still know relatively little
about the neural bases of engagement with fictional worlds (for
examples of work to date, see Speer, Reynolds, Swallow, & Zacks,
2009;Phillips, 2015). It is largely unclear, for example, whether
we use different neural circuits to handle engagment in physical
and fictional worlds. Further experimental work in this area will be
welcome.
Toward This Special Issue’s Contributions...
Our aim in putting this Special Issue together has been to
explore some processes of human minds that, until recently in
psychology have not received much attention. We warmly thank
our contributors and also Gerianne Alexander and Svetlana Efremova
for their help and encouragement.
Aside from raising questions of why we spend so much time in
fictional worlds, and what effects this had in evolution and today,
this topic raises psychological questions as to how we navigate two
mental worlds simultaneously without confusing them. Our behav-
ior in this respect contrasts with mental disorders in which indi-
viduals do confuse inner states with the immediate world.
Stories may be thought of as externalized pieces of conscious-
ness that can be passed from one mind to others (Oatley & Djikic,
2017). Human consciousness remains an enigma. Investigations of
the imagined worlds of stories may enable us to come closer to
understanding it.
References
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive
role in internal mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18, 251–270. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/1073858411403316
Aristotle. (1970). Poetics (G. E. Else, Trans.). Ann Arbor: University of
Michigan Press. (Original work published 330 BCE)
Barnes, J. L., & Bloom, P. (2014). Children’s preference for social stories.
Developmental Psychology, 50, 498 –503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0033613
Bar-Yosef Mayer, D. E., Vandermeersch, B., & Bar-Yosef, O. (2009).
Shells and ochre in Middle Paleolithic Qafzeh Cave, Israel: Indications
for modern behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 56, 307–314. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.10.005
Bharata Muni. (1986). Natyasastra. English translation with critical notes
(A. Rangacharya, Trans.). Bangalore, India: IBH Prakashana. (Original
work published 200 BCE)
Boyd, B. (2009). On the origin of stories. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Carney, J., Wlodarski, R., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Inference or
enaction? The influence of genre on the narrative processing of other
minds. PLoS ONE, 9, E114172.
Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2016). An introduction to cognitive archae-
ology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 386 –392. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721416657085
De Mulder, H. N. M., Hakemulder, F., & van der Berghe, R. (2017).
Effects of exposure to literary narrative fiction. Scientific Study of
Literature, 7, 129 –169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/ssol.7.1.06dem
Dodell-Feder, D., & Tamir, D. (2018). Fiction reading has a small positive
impact on social cognition: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/xge0000395
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language.
London, England: Faber & Faber.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2004). The human story: A new history of mankind’s
evolution. London, England: Faber and Faber.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Human evolution. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The anatomy of friendship. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 22, 32–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004
Gottschall, J. (2012). The storytelling animal: How stories make us human.
Boston, MA: Harcourt Mifflin.
Hakemulder, F. (2000). The moral laboratory: Experiments examining the
effects of reading literature on social perception and moral self-concept.
Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/upal.34
Harris, P. L. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford, England:
Blackwell.
Hoffmann, D. L., Standish, C. D., García-Diez, M., Pettitt, P. B., Milton,
J. A., Zilhão, J.,...Pike, A. W. G. (2018). U-Th dating of carbonate
crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art. Science, 359, 912–
915. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.aap7778
Hogan, P. C. (2003). The mind and its stories: Narrative universals and
human emotion. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science
of language, inference, and consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation in infancy: The origin of
‘Theory of Mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412– 426. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412
Lewis, P. A., Birch, A., Hall, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2017). Higher order
intentionality tasks are cognitively more demanding. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1063–1071. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
scan/nsx034
Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story com-
prehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103–134. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., dela Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006).
Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, diver-
gent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
123
IMAGINING POSSIBLE WORLDS
social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 694 –712. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.002
Mar, R. A., & Rain, M. (2015). Narrative fiction and expository nonfiction
differentially predict verbal ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19,
419 433. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2015.1069296
Mar, R. A., Tackett, J. L., & Moore, C. (2010). Exposure to media and
theory-of-mind development in preschoolers. Cognitive Development,
25, 69 –78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.11.002
Meyer, M. L., Spunt, R. P., Berkman, E. T., Taylor, S. E., & Lieberman,
M. D. (2012). Evidence for social working memory from a parametric
functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 109, 1883–1888. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1073/pnas.1121077109
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art
and science. London, England: Thames and Hudson.
Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Leisure reading and social
cognition. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 109
120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000089
Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cogni-
tive Sciences, 20, 618 628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.002
Oatley, K. & Djikic, M. (2017). The creativity of literary writing. In J.
Kaufman, J. Baer, & V. Glaveanu (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of
creativity across domains (pp. 61–79). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/9781316274385.005
Phillips, N. M. (2015). Literary neuroscience and history of mind: An
interdisciplinary fMRI study of attention and Jane Austen. In L. Zun-
shine (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive literary studies (pp.
55– 81). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load
disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. Psychological Science, 23,
842– 847. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612439070
Speer, N. K., Reynolds, J. R., Swallow, K. M., & Zacks, J. M. (2009).
Reading stories activates neural representations of visual and motor
experiences. Psychological Science, 20, 989–999. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02397.x
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.4159/9780674726
369
Tomasello, M. (2016). A natural history of human morality. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.4159/978067491
5855
Van Peer, W. (1986). Stylistics and psychology: Investigations of fore-
grounding. London, England: Croom Helm.
Vessel, E. A., Starr, G. G., & Rubin, N. (2013). Art reaches within:
Aesthetic experience, the self and the default mode network. Frontiers in
Neuroscience, 7, 258. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00258
Zeki, S. (2011). Neuroaesthetics. Paper presented at the Centre for Cre-
ation, Content, and Technology: Reading mediated minds: Empathy
with persons and characters in media and art works, Amsterdam, the
Netherlands.
Received March 29, 2018
Accepted March 29, 2018
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
124 OATLEY, DUNBAR, AND BUDELMANN
... In recent years, boosted by advancements in computational linguistics, researchers have looked at lexical, syntactical, and literary devices in the language of fiction (e.g., Jacobs, 2015;Jockers & Thalken, 2020) and how readers are transported into these fictional worlds (e.g., Barnes, 2018, Schwerin & Lenhart, 2022Tal-Or & Cohen, 2010;Thompson et al., 2018). Theoretical and empirical work has investigated whether engaging with fiction also has spillover effects on the social cognition of readers, outside of the fictional environment (for reviews, see Dodell-Feder & Tamir, 2018;Oatley et al., 2018). These two aspects, how fiction elicits emotions and emotional resonance with the fictional characters, and how it affects readers' ability to understand and infer emotions outside of the fictional context, are likely connected (see Schwerin & Lenhart, 2022). ...
... Scholarly work on the psychology of fiction has long investigated how authors transport readers into their fictional worlds by eliciting an emotional response and allowing them to get into the minds of characters (Gerrig, 1993;Humphrey, 2002;Hjort & Laver, 1997;Zunshine, 2022). At the same time, research findings stemming primarily from cognitive science show that reading fiction also impacts readers' social cognition (for reviews, see Dodell-Feder & Tamir, 2018;Oatley et al., 2018). In this essay, I have explored the connection between these two lines of inquiry and proposed that to understand it we should look at how emotions and other mental states of characters are conveyed in fiction. ...
Article
Stories, in pictorial format, orally narrated, and later on as narrative texts, have played a key role in human evolution and to this day continue to surreptitiously teach us things and skills. In recent decades, psychologists and cognitive scientists have begun documenting the role of stories, and particularly fiction, in refining our sociocognitive skills. In this essay, I focus specifically on how stories, particularly written fiction, hone our emotion recognition skills. I present a brief overview of existing theorizing and research findings, and propose the less-is-more hypothesis, according to which emotion recognition skills are sharpened by stories that do not tell us, but rather show us the emotional life of fictional characters.
... En este sentido, según Oatley et al. (2018), la lectura puede potenciar, tanto las habilidades afectivas como las metacognitivas, lo que conduce a una reflexión constante, un análisis profundo y una construcción de significados más sólida. En consecuencia, se puede afirmar que la lectura fortalece los vínculos entre niños y adultos, mejora la inteligencia emocional y también tiene otros beneficios, como el desarrollo de la competencia comunicativa, el pensamiento crítico y la mejora del lenguaje, así como la posibilidad de recreación. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
El capítulo titulado "Conflictos socioambientales y minería en la ‘estrella hídrica del sur’ del Ecuador: El caso de la cordillera de Fierro Urco", analiza los conflictos emergentes derivados de la expansión minera en la cordillera Fierro Urco, un ecosistema estratégico ubicado entre las provincias de Loja y El Oro. El estudio se enmarca en un enfoque cualitativo, con entrevistas a actores clave y análisis de contenido mediante el software NVivo, para identificar las tensiones generadas entre comunidades, instituciones públicas y empresas mineras. El capítulo identifica tres tipos principales de conflicto: intragrupales, intergrupales y de intereses, los cuales responden a la falta de consulta previa, el otorgamiento inconsulto de concesiones, la amenaza a fuentes hídricas y la polarización comunitaria. Los autores resaltan que Fierro Urco es considerado un territorio sagrado por pueblos campesinos e indígenas, cuyas formas de vida se ven amenazadas por actividades extractivas. Como alternativa, se propone la implementación de una veeduría ciudadana para fortalecer la gobernanza local y monitorear el impacto de la minería. El capítulo evidencia cómo la minería transforma territorialidades, vulnera derechos colectivos y genera nuevas formas de acción colectiva en defensa del agua y del territorio.
... Book reading plays an important role in various aspects of human development, contributing to the evolution of human cognition [48] and shaping global values through narrative persuasion in the human mind [49]. Picture books, primarily designed for children, contain both visual and written stories [50]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The term ‘抜苦与楽 (Bakku Yoraku)’ means to remove suffering and give comfort. In Buddhism, this word means that Buddhas and Bodhisattvas save sentient beings from suffering and provide them with happiness. The qualitative difference in empathy between humans and Buddha is suggested to be related to the psychological function of self-compassion. I explore fantasy, metacognition and autobiographical memory, integral components of self-compassion during empathy, from psychological, neurological and biological perspectives. I discuss the possibility of using a picture book as an intervention for fostering fantasy. To create a new picture book, I must understand the percentages of adjectives, verbs and nouns used in the world’s most-read books. Subsequently, a new word is coined. A fantasy story is created using these words. This story should evoke warm and positive emotions, such as the Holy Bible or Buddhist scriptures. The narration of the story must be created from 1st person perspective. I hope that this article will be helpful to researchers, educators and clinicians seeking a peaceful future.
... In this, fiction can "improve empathy and social understanding" (Oatley, 2016, p. 625). However, the benefits can extend beyond this individual effect and this one encounter: Research has also shown that experiencing a personal connection with a text that builds empathy and understanding can result in long-term changes that can guide a person's real-life behaviors (Djikic & Oatley, 2014;Johnson, 2012;Oatley et al., 2018;Webber & Agiro, 2019). This is also expressed by Karen, who points to the "long-lasting" impact of stories that may "help to change your view on life, yourself or others." ...
Article
Full-text available
The school environment can be challenging for young people experiencing mental health concerns to express their individuality and feel accepted. There has been an increased focus on wellbeing in school-aged children, with programs targeted to building resilience and promoting inclusion. However, peer stigmatization lowers personal wellbeing by limiting social belongingness in young people with mental health concerns and this is often exacerbated by poor mental health literacy. This paper is based on a doctoral study that used a grounded theory methodology to explore adolescent responses to a novel featuring mental health concerns as a class text. This research showed that students prefer contemporary young adult novels in the English classroom to support mental health literacy due to its contemporaneity and relatability. This paper links these benefits with the role of teacher librarians in recommending young adult literature featuring mental health concerns for use in the English curriculum.
... For affordances see: Bin, 2016;Chemero, 2003;Gibson, 1977;Rucinska, 2017. On imagination see (in the representational and meta-representational traditions): Gilmore, 2019;Harris, 2000Harris, , 2022Oatley, 2011Oatley, , 2013Oatley, , 2016Oatley, , 2022Oatley, Dunbar, and Budelmann, 2018;Zunshine, 2003Zunshine, , 2006Zunshine, , 2009Zunshine, , 2017. For enactivist approaches to imagination see: Caracciolo, 2013;Medina, 2013;Ravenscroft, 2017;Weichold and Rucinska, 2021. ...
Article
Full-text available
This essay focuses primarily on Fury (2014) and to a lesser extent on Saving Private Ryan (1998), two popular war films. I suggest cognitive mechanisms that make the actions of main characters biologically plausible and intelligible. The principal ones have to do with social status and especially prestige, which is based on certain kinds of competence, especially in heroic leaders. Prestige is understood to be co-created by leaders and followers together. Heroism is understood to be depicted in these films as effective leadership in extreme circumstances so as to solve collective action problems. Heroic qualities are signaled (honestly) by means of wounds and scars. These signals are received and understood (both in the films and in their viewers) by means of social learning and social comparison judgments that track prestige. Group cohesion is also explored, especially in Fury, in terms of shared emotions, mimicry, rituals and commitments to a common task. Hatred of morally defined out-groups is also seen at work. I conclude that these films posit warfare as one of a small number of "biologically possible arrangements" that accomplish a rare concatenation of fitness promoting goals. Some consideration is also given to what makes consilience between empirical science and cinematic imagination possible.
... They also describe experiencing positive emotional states including absorption (Jamieson, 2005), which is characterized by extreme focusing, flow, and elation, and aesthetic feelings, such as a feeling of wonder, when they succeed in producing a new palindrome. These emotions and feelings could bear resemblance with those experienced during storytelling and the creation of narratives (Oatley, 2016;Oatley, Dunbar, & Budelmann, 2018), as well as to aesthetic experiences described in other artistic domains, like visual arts or music (Ferreri et al., 2021;Skov, 2019). Further, well-developed long-term interests are characterized by positive feelings and increased stored knowledge, creating added value for maintaining certain behaviours needed for achieving constructive and creative endeavours (Hidi & Renninger, 2006) (for example, see [Loewenstein, 1999] for an insightful analysis about Mountaineering passion). ...
Article
Full-text available
Humans seem to be inherently driven to engage in wordplay. An example is the creation of palindromes – sentences that read the same backward and forward. This activity can be framed as a curiosity-driven behaviour, in which individuals seek for information that serves no direct purpose and in the absence of external rewards. In this fMRI case study, an experienced palindrome creator was scanned while he generated palindromes with different levels of difficulty. Palindrome creation was alternated with resting periods and with a working memory task, both serving as control conditions. Relative to resting, palindrome creation recruited frontal domain-specific language networks and fronto-parietal domain-general networks. The comparison with the working memory task evidenced partial overlap with the multiple-demand cortex, which participates in solving a variety of cognitively challenging tasks. Intriguingly, greater difficulty during palindrome creation differentially activated the right frontopolar cortex (BA 10), a region that was also linked to palindrome resolution. The latter is consistent with exploratory behaviour – in this case, with seeking new but interdependent linguistic segments within a complex internal model (i.e., a palindromic structure)– and bears resemblance with brain substrates sustaining hard logical reasoning, altogether pointing to a commonplace for curiosity in discovering new and complex relations.
... Literary reading plays an important role in many aspects of human development, such as the evolution of human cognition (Oatley et al., 2018), enhancement of social cognition (such as the theory of mind, Kidd & Castano, 2013;De Mulder et al., 2017) and reshaping of world values in humans' mind and attitudes by narrative persuasion (Braddock & Dillard, 2016). Researchers further found that popular novels provide significant psychological pleasures for readers in the act of reading, and such pleasure is constantly satisfied during the reading process (Ge & Liu, 2019a;Geng, 2016;Green & Brock, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies found that linguistic features can be used to predict the success of novels. However, which specific linguistic features better contribute to a novel’s popularity is unclear. This study addressed this issue by investigating the linguistic features of 2,008 online Chinese fantasy novels with different popularity (indicated by the Baidu Index). Specifically, word part-of-speech, personal pronouns, word complexity, and local/overall sentence semantic coherence were analyzed using a word segmentation tool (Jieba) and a latent semantic analysis software (Chinese version of Coh-Metrix). Results showed significant differences between popular and non-popular (high and low popularity) novels in the distribution of parts-of-speech, use of the second person pronoun, word complexity, and sentence semantic coherence. Moreover, the presence of the second person pronoun (“you”), local sentence semantic coherence, auxiliary words, word complexity, overall sentence semantic coherence, and adjectives better predicted the popularity of a Chinese online fantasy novel. The theoretical background and the implications of these results are detailed in the study discussion.
Article
Despite increasing interest in creative aging, little connection has been made between creative aging research and research and theory on creativity. In part 1 of this paper, we will explain why 20th century psychological concepts of creativity were inadequate to describe the impact of creative participation for older adults, and how those theories promoted stereotypes of aging. In part 2, we discuss theory and research from gerontologists on why creative activities are important, and how more inclusive theories of creativity can guide researchers in their quest to determine how creative participation can benefit older adults, and the culture at large. Sociocultural theories of creativity highlight the importance of culture and context as essential components of creativity. For example, sociocultural creativity theory argues that the cultural artifacts called crafts deserve to be considered “creative” even if they do not seem particularly novel. Crafts, which are frequently created by older people, especially women, are cultural artifacts or heirlooms that can hold meaning for generations. Sociocultural theory can guide the study of late life creativity to expand the possibilities for meaningful engagement in creativity across the lifespan.
Article
Full-text available
Despite significantly increasing research efforts, the psychological effects of reading fiction remain under debate. We present two preregistered meta-analyses synthesizing cognitive effects and correlates of reading fiction. In Meta-Analysis 1 (371 effect sizes/70 experiments), reading fiction led to significant small-sized cognitive benefits, g = 0.14, 95% confidence interval (CI) [0.06, 0.21]. This effect of fiction reading was moderated by the comparison group (effects were greater when reading fiction was compared with watching fiction or reading nothing than when reading fiction was compared with reading nonfiction) and the outcome variable (significant effects emerged for empathy and mentalizing only). In Meta-Analysis 2 (559 effect sizes/114 studies reporting correlations), lifetime exposure to print fiction was linked with significant small-sized cognitive benefits, r = .16, 95% CI [0.13, 0.19]. This effect was moderated by outcome variable (effects were greatest for verbal abilities, followed by general cognitive abilities and empathy/mentalizing/outgroup judgments), fictionality of the print material (greater effects were found for fiction than nonfiction), publication status (published work exhibited greater effects than unpublished work), type of assessment measure (larger effects emerged when neither the outcome nor print exposure were assessed via self-report, than when either the outcome or print exposure were assessed via self-report), participant group (community samples showed greater effects than student samples), study design (greater effects were found for correlational than for experimental designs), and percentage of female participants (via a negative relationship with cognitive benefits). Together, these meta-analyses provide robust evidence for a small-sized positive relationship between reading fiction and cognitive benefits.
Article
To provide the current student generation with an innovative online learning method, podcasts with science fiction short stories are introduced to marketing education. Findings from neurology and psychology point to positive effects of storytelling for gaining knowledge and developing interpersonal skills. Science fiction stories challenge common conceptions and enable students to engage with upcoming marketing issues in creative ways. An overview of eight marketing related science fiction stories is provided that are available for download as open access podcasts, with 23 more management and economics podcast stories available in the online appendix. To show how science fiction podcasts can be implemented in class, one of the stories is discussed in more detail. Further suggestions for implementation and possible challenges are outlined.
Chapter
Full-text available
Literary writing involves externalization of mind onto paper or computer screen, and a process of guided exploration over a space of possibilities. Among the arts, this kind of writing may come closest in structure and content to everyday consciousness; this has enabled writers and readers to explore the workings of minds in interactions with others. Artistic writing is a kind of indirect communication in which the creativity of the writer invites the creativity of the reader. In personality, writers are higher in openness but more often depressed than other members of the population. Characteristics on which literary creativity is based make writers vulnerable to emotional disorders. With the exception of conversation, nothing may have been as important in understanding ourselves and others as works of creative writing.
Article
Full-text available
Scholars from both the social sciences and the humanities have credited fiction reading with a range of positive real-world social effects. Research in psychology has suggested that readers may make good citizens because fiction reading is associated with better social cognition. But does fiction reading causally improve social cognition? Here, we meta-analyze extant published and unpublished experimental data to address this question. Multilevel random-effects meta-analysis of 53 effect sizes from 14 studies demonstrated that it does: compared to nonfiction reading and no reading, fiction reading leads to a small, statistically significant improvement in social-cognitive performance (g = .15–.16). This effect is robust across sensitivity analyses and does not appear to be the result of publication bias. We recommend that in future work, researchers use more robust reading manipulations, assess whether the effects transfer to improved real-world social functioning, and investigate mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
Neandertal cave art It has been suggested that Neandertals, as well as modern humans, may have painted caves. Hoffmann et al. used uranium-thorium dating of carbonate crusts to show that cave paintings from three different sites in Spain must be older than 64,000 years. These paintings are the oldest dated cave paintings in the world. Importantly, they predate the arrival of modern humans in Europe by at least 20,000 years, which suggests that they must be of Neandertal origin. The cave art comprises mainly red and black paintings and includes representations of various animals, linear signs, geometric shapes, hand stencils, and handprints. Thus, Neandertals possessed a much richer symbolic behavior than previously assumed. Science , this issue p. 912
Book
The idea that reading literature changes the reader seems as old as literature itself. Through the ages philosophers, writers, and literary scholars have suggested it affects norms, empathic ability, self-concept, beliefs, etc. This book examines what we actually know about these effects. And it finds strong evidence for the old claims. However, it remains unclear what aspects of the reading experience are responsible for these effects. Applying methods of the social sciences to this particular problem of literary theory, this book presents a psychological explanation based upon the conception of literature as a moral laboratory. A series of experiments examines whether imagining oneself in the shoes of characters affects beliefs about what it must be like to be someone else, and whether it affects beliefs about consequences of behavior. The results have implications for the role literature could play in society, for instance, in an alternative for traditional moral education.
Article
Friendship is the single most important factor influencing our health, well-being, and happiness. Creating and maintaining friendships is, however, extremely costly, in terms of both the time that has to be invested and the cognitive mechanisms that underpin them. Nonetheless, personal social networks exhibit many constancies, notably in their size and their hierarchical structuring. Understanding the processes that give rise to these patterns and their evolutionary origins requires a multidisciplinary approach that combines social and neuropsychology as well as evolutionary biology.