ArticlePublisher preview available

Role Congruity Theory of Prejudice Toward Female Leaders

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

A role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders proposes that perceived incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles leads to 2 forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles. Evidence from varied research paradigms substantiates that these consequences occur, especially in situations that heighten perceptions of incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles.
Role Congruity Theory of Prejudice Toward Female Leaders
Alice H. Eagly
Northwestern University Steven J. Karau
Southern Illinois University at Carbondale
A role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders proposes that perceived incongruity between
the female gender role and leadership roles leads to 2 forms of prejudice: (a) perceiving women less
favorably than men as potential occupants of leadership roles and (b) evaluating behavior that fulfills the
prescriptions of a leader role less favorably when it is enacted by a woman. One consequence is that
attitudes are less positive toward female than male leaders and potential leaders. Other consequences are
that it is more difficult for women to become leaders and to achieve success in leadership roles. Evidence
from varied research paradigms substantiates that these consequences occur, especially in situations that
heighten perceptions of incongruity between the female gender role and leadership roles.
Leadership has been predominantly a male prerogative in cor-
porate, political, military, and other sectors of society. Although
women have gained increased access to supervisory and middle
management positions, they remain quite rare as elite leaders and
top executives. To explain this phenomenon, public and scientific
discussion has centered on the idea of a “glass ceiling”—a barrier
of prejudice and discrimination that excludes women from higher
level leadership positions (Federal Glass Ceiling Commission,
1995; Morrison, White, & Van Velsor, 1987). To further this
discussion, we advance a theory of prejudice toward female lead-
ers and test the theory in relation to available empirical research.
This integrative theory builds on social psychologists’ tradition of
studying prejudice and stereotyping and industrial–organizational
psychologists’ tradition of studying perceptions of managerial
roles.
The popularity of the glass ceiling concept may stem from the
rarity of women in major leadership posts, despite the presence of
equality or near equality of the sexes on many other indicators. A
number of statistics thus suggest equality: In the United States,
women make up 46% of all workers (U.S. Bureau of Labor
Statistics, 2001b) and 45% of those in executive, administrative,
and managerial occupations (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics,
2001a); women possess 51% of bachelor’s degrees and 45% of all
advanced degrees (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2000). In contrast,
statistics pertaining to major leadership roles consistently show
inequality: Women constitute 4% of the five highest earning
officers in Fortune 500 companies and 0.4% of the CEOs (Cata-
lyst, 2000); 13% of senators, 14% of congressional representatives,
and 10% of state governors (Center for the American Woman and
Politics, 2001); and 2% of military officers at the level of brigadier
general and rear admiral or higher (U.S. Department of Defense,
1998). Similarly small proportions of women in most high posi-
tions also characterize other industrialized and industrializing na-
tions (Adler & Izraeli, 1994; Melkas & Anker, 1997).
Explanations for this sparse representation of women in elite
leadership roles traditionally focused on the idea that a lack of
qualified women created a “pipeline problem.” This shortage of
women has been ascribed to a variety of causes, including wom-
en’s family responsibilities (Greenhaus & Parasuraman, 1999) and
inherited tendencies for women to display fewer of the traits and
motivations that are necessary to attain and achieve success in
high-level positions (e.g., Browne, 1999; S. Goldberg, 1993).
However, because the convergence of the sexes on many human-
capital variables raises questions about the sufficiency of such
explanations, it is vital to evaluate whether prejudice is one of the
causes of women’s rarity in major leadership positions. To this
end, we propose a role congruity theory of prejudice toward female
leaders that (a) extends Eagly’s (1987; Eagly, Wood, & Diekman,
2000) social role theory of sex differences and similarities into
new territory; (b) proposes novel, testable predictions about prej-
udice and its consequences; and (c) yields an effective organizing
framework for a very large number of empirical findings from
laboratories, field settings, organizations, and public opinion polls.
In this article, we first explain the theory in general terms, then
elaborate it by explaining the moderating conditions that allow it to
make detailed predictions, and finally review empirical literature
relevant to the theory.
Role Congruity Theory: How Gender Roles and Leader
Roles Produce Two Types of Prejudice
In general, prejudice can arise from the relations that people
perceive between the characteristics of members of a social group
A draft of this article was written while Alice H. Eagly was a visiting
scholar at the Murray Research Center of Radcliffe Institute for Advanced
Study and supported by a sabbatical award from the James McKeen Cattell
Fund. The writing of this article was also supported by National Science
Foundation Grant SBR-9729449 to Alice H. Eagly. We thank Amanda
Diekman, Judith Hall, Lauren Gibbs, Eric Hansen, Madeline Heilman,
Mary Johannesen-Schmidt, Richard Martell, Tracie Stewart, Janet Swim,
Marloes Van Engen, Claartje Vinkenburg, and Wendy Wood for comments
on an earlier version of the article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alice H.
Eagly, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheri-
dan Road, Swift Hall 313, Evanston, Illinois 60208, or to Steven J. Karau,
Department of Management, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale,
Illinois 62901-4627. E-mail: eagly@northwestern.edu or skarau@cba
.siu.edu
Psychological Review Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2002, Vol. 109, No. 3, 573–598 0033-295X/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0033-295X.109.3.573
573
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
... Societal norms deeply ingrained in traditional gender roles also shape the esports landscape. Females are typically associated with communal attributes such as being nurturing, collaborative, and empathetic, while males are linked with agency, embodying traits like competitiveness and independence (Eagly & Karau, 2002). Perceptions like these, according to previous studies (Buser et al., 2014;Niederle & Vesterlund, 2011;Petrie & Segal, 2015), may be based on real differences between males and females. ...
... There is a disparity in competitive online multiplayer games and esports, where females are underrepresented at both casual and competitive levels. Past work suggests that sex-related prejudices against female players result in expectations for females to fill supportive roles and exhibit lower levels of skill and agency (Kim, 2017), paralleling other male-dominated fields (Eagly & Karau, 2002;Ellemers, 2018). In this study, we explored sex differences through two experiments. ...
... This biased evocation is consistent with previous research showing that individuals often perceive agentic attributes (dominance, ambition, confidence, etc.) as typically male attributes (Ellemers, 2018). This effect usually leads people to perceive women as being unfit for leadership roles and to evaluate certain behaviors linked to a leadership role as being less favorable when they come from a woman since leadership is perceived as less congruent with the female gender role (Eagly & Karau, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Discrimination against women is widespread in contemporary societies, and the persistent wage gap for identical roles compared to men suggests that this bias is socially justified. We addressed this issue by proposing that individuals evoke gender stereotypes in a way that reinforces their tendency to reaffirm gender pay gaps. In two experiments, we analyzed how the mere act of evoking reasons for hiring someone produces the gender pay gap and whether stereotype-based justifications mediate this effect. In Study 1 (N = 95), we manipulated the gender of a candidate applying for a job position and identified that the simple act of justifying hiring influences the gender pay gap. In Study 2 (N = 302), participants evoked more stereotypes of competence for a male job applicant, and this evocation mediated the gender pay gap. In the discussion, we addressed the role of stereotype-based justification in gender disparity on critical decisions within work-relation contexts. The insights generated by this research program have important implications for understanding discrimination against women in the workplace and for the development of public policies aimed at promoting gender equality.
... Leadership behaviours depend, in fact, on the set of competences expected for this. It should be a question of choice by professional criteria (Eagly & Karau, 2002;Vaz, 2013). There are, according to Bendl and Schmidt (2010), invisible cultural barriers understood as a firewall or glass ceiling, focused on maintaining the status quo and the institutionally entrenched structures. ...
Article
The work environment is a diverse place, where people interact daily to obtain results for organisations. Organisations have been providing gender-based equity actions to promote meritocracy. The situation worsens when criteria for ascension to leadership positions are discussed, most of the time intentionally occupied by men, violating the premise of impersonality. Diversity management and social inclusion have been intensively developed, generating engagement and psychological safety for the social actors involved. The main of this paper is to test two predictive models, in a Brazilian public security organisation, that include variables related to the perception of organisational justice, organisational values, and the perception of equality in professional advancement criteria. The sample was composed of 988 Brazilian police officers, chosen through stratified random sampling. The data were analysed using descriptive statistics and multiple linear regression. The results suggest that there is a strong and positive relationship between perceived organisational justice and perception of gender-based equity of opportunities. None of the organisational values predicted the two criteria variables. As limitations, the collected data were anonymous, a reason that did not allow the comparison between means due to some clustering variable. The equity suggested that organisational values need to be, in fact, operationalised in terms of formal practices that promote them. Limitations and suggestions were made.
Article
According to research on gendered political socialisation, children internalise political gender role attitudes by observing gender-specific behaviour in their environment. Since children’s experiences at home are crucial in this context, this article investigates the effect of the parental division of labour on children’s political gender role attitudes. For analyses, a unique data set from the ‘Learning to Live Democracy 2.0: Political Socialisation at the Beginning of Lower Secondary School’ research project with more than 1,000 German fifth-graders is used. The study reveals that children who perceive a traditional division of labour at home are more likely to have traditional political gender role attitudes. By means of mediation analysis, it is also found that the effect of the parental division of labour on children’s political gender role attitudes is mediated by their general gender role attitudes. These results underline the presumed close relationship between political and gender socialisation.
Article
Purpose The purpose of the present study is to explore multimodal, i.e. verbal and nonverbal, gendered communication patterns of female physicians in senior management positions (governmental and health authorities) during a crisis. Design/methodology/approach Using a mixed multi-variable design, repeated ANOVA tests, and planned contrasts, the authors analyzed television appearances of 20 female physicians in senior management positions during the COVID-19 crisis (March/2020-April/2021). Findings The findings revealed patterns of mixed-gendered communication structures. Verbally, female physicians primarily displayed a masculine/agentic communication style of assertiveness, control, confidence and rationality. Nonverbally, however, they expressed a feminine/communal communication style of emotional attention, interpersonal sensitivity, responsiveness, kindness and empathy. Moreover, the analysis delineated integrated multimodal constructive vs. inhibitive communication strategies for crisis communication of female physicians in senior management positions. Research limitations/implications In the current research the authors did not compare females to males in health management positions, which is their follow-up project, but the authors did examine studies of males and females in management positions in the political sphere, which supported their findings. Therefore, the authors were able to demonstrate theoretical implications of multimodal gendered communication frameworks of feminine leadership. Practical implications Delineating verbal and nonverbal gendered communicative structures of effective management in health sectors can help female physicians assume positions of leadership, serve as guide models for other female physicians and contribute to improving effective communication skills during a crisis. Social implications This study contributes to the attempts of promoting gender equity in medicine and management by presenting effective communication strategies in medical crises that can help to promote female physicians’ messages development, social influence, leadership and management success in the future. Originality/value This article presents constructive, multimodal gendered communication frameworks of female physicians in senior management positions used in television appearances during the global COVID-19 crisis. Most previous studies in this area have examined either verbal or nonverbal communication mode. The value of this multimodal examination provides insights that may enhance constructive communication of female physicians in senior management positions during a crisis.
Article
We examine the interface of entrepreneurship and society by considering a novel source of gender bias (public opinion) and a novel expression of it (affective evaluations). We posit that women-led teams displaying success will trigger a “penalty for success” bias, and this will be inhibited if the team receives a “stamp of approval” from a gender congruent individual (i.e., an investor who is a man). Analysis from our first study, based on archival data, indicated that other mechanisms might be at play that we could not tease apart in our archival data, so we conducted a follow-up experimental study to further examine our initial findings. Results jointly indicate that women are viewed unfavorably unless either (a) they are validated by success or endorsement from a man, or (b) they fail but are “warm and accommodating” about it. We also find that while men benefit from being validated by success or endorsement of an opposite-gender investor, they do not face a similarly steep penalty absent that validation.
Article
Full-text available
Les préjugés et limites portés sur les femmes ont occasionné la rareté des femmes dans le leadership des organisations tant scolaires que parascolaires. Conscient du fait que la société semble ne pas trop faire confiance aux femmes à ce niveau de responsabilité, celles qui dirigent ces institutions sont des exemples qu’il faut interroger pour comprendre le rendement de leur leadership dans un domaine qui est encore fort masculinisé. La montée spectaculaire des femmes aux postes de responsabilité pousse les chercheurs à se questionner sur l’efficacité du leadership féminin. Comprendre comment les femmes dirigent leurs écoles est une nécessité pour la société en vue de les mettre à la place où elles ont été discriminées pendant plusieurs siècles et de donner des références à des générations montantes. L’étude a expliqué le leadership des femmes chefs d’établissements des écoles primaires dans la sous-province éducationnelle de Ngaliema de Kinshasa en RDC. L’atteinte de cet objectif était possible grâce à l’enquête par questionnaire des données dans un échantillon de commodité ou au jugement de 50 femmes-leaders, soit 79,4% de la population. L’analyse statistique (pourcentage, moyenne et écart-type) a permis l’analyse des données. Les résultats ont montré que les femmes chefs d’écoles primaires dirigent par l’exemple, la construction d’une communauté scolaire, l’élaboration, l’exécution et l’évaluation des projets éducatifs, le développement personnel et l’inclusion. Les femmes chefs d’écoles primaires privilégient la collaboration dans une logique de perfectionnement, de conscientisation et d’efficacité pour influencer le corps professoral, le personnel administratif ainsi que les parents pour un meilleur rendement de leurs élèves. Les directrices des écoles préfèrent utiliser le leadership démocratique. Le leadership employé améliore le rendement qualitatif et quantitatif de leurs élèves dans les aspects d’apprentissage, d’acquisition des compétences et des évaluations. Les femmes chefs d’écoles primaires sont motivées par le désir d’accomplissement personnel, du changement qualitatif dans leurs écoles, d’engagement social et de l’émancipation de la femme. Article visualizations: </p
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments tested and extended recent theory regarding motivational influences on impression formation (S. T. Fiske & S. L. Neuberg, 1990; J. L. Hilton & J. M. Darley, 1991) in the context of an impression management dilemma that women face: Self-promotion may be instrumental for managing a competent impression, yet women who self-promote may suffer social reprisals for violating gender prescriptions to be modest. Experiment 1 investigated the influence of perceivers’ goals on processes that inhibit stereotypical thinking, and reactions to counterstereotypical behavior. Experiments 2–3 extended these findings by including male targets. For female targets, self-promotion led to higher competence ratings but incurred social attraction and hireability costs unless perceivers were outcome-dependent males. For male targets, self-effacement decreased competence and hireability ratings, though its effects on social attraction were inconsistent.
Article
Full-text available
Stereotypes may influence judgment via assimilation, such that individual group members are evaluated consistently with stereotypes, or via contrast, such that targets are displaced from the overall group expectation. Two models of judgment—the shifting standards model and status characteristics theory—provide some insight into predicting and interpreting these apparently contradictory effects. In 2 studies involving a simulated applicant-evaluation setting, we predicted and found that participants set lower minimum-competency standards, but higher ability standards, for female than for male and for Black than for White applicants. Thus, although it may be easier for low- than high-status group members to meet (low) standards, these same people must work harder to prove that their performance is ability based.
Article
Full-text available
In this study the authors compare career and work experiences of executive women and men. Female (n = 51) and male (n = 56) financial services executives in comparable jobs were studied through archival information on organizational outcomes and career histories, and survey measures of work experiences. Similarities were found in several organizational outcomes, such as compensation, and many work attitudes. Important differences were found, however, with women having less authority, receiving fewer stock options, and having less international mobility than men. Women at the highest executive levels reported more obstacles than lower level women. The gender differences coupled with women's lower satisfaction with future career opportunities raise questions about whether women are truly above the glass ceiling or have come up against a 2nd, higher ceiling.
Article
Full-text available
The self-concept plays an important role in conformity to sex-typed social norms. Normative beliefs that men are powerful, dominant, and self-assertive and that women are caring, intimate with others, and emotionally expressive represent possible standards for whom people ought to be and whom they ideally would like to be. In the present research, to the extent that sex role norms were personally relevant for participants, norm-congruent experiences (i.e., those involving dominance for men and communion for women) yielded positive feelings and brought their actual self-concepts closer to the standards represented by ought and ideal selves.
Article
Full-text available
Gender differences were examined in the context of situational effects. Participants monitored interpersonal behavior for 20 days, using an event-sampling strategy. The monitored behaviors reflected dominance and submissiveness (components of agency) and agreeableness and quarrelsomeness (components of communion). The situations reflected differences in the status of work roles: interactions with boss, co-worker, and supervisee. Status influenced agency. Individuals were most agentic when with a supervisee and least agentic when with a boss. Gender did not influence agency but did influence communal behaviors. Women were more communal regardless of social role status; women were especially communal with other women, compared with men with men. Findings about agency supported a social role theory interpretation of gender differences. Results for communion were consistent with accounts of the influence of sex segregation on interpersonal relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Researchers often use sex-typed names (e.g., John vs. Joan) to identify stimulus person's sex, assuming that such names communicate sex only. In fact, however, such names also create impressions that have little or nothing to do with sex. Study 1 analyzed the age connotations, intellectual-competence connotations, and attractiveness of sex-typed names used in 230 published studies on sexism and fear of success. On each of these variables, the literature was pervasively confounded in a manner favoring male stimulus persons. Study 2 found that the name biases reported in Study 1 were positively correlated with outcome measures in a sample of sexism studies, but only when names were presented with limited other information. Possible causes of the bias are discussed, and recommendations for naming stimulus persons are presented, including a list of male names and female names matched on several key variables.
Article
Full-text available
Seven studies investigated the cognitive structure of social relationships exhibited in the patterns of substitutions that occur when people confuse a person with another. The studies investigated natural errors in which people called a familiar person by the wrong name, misremembered with whom they had interacted, or mistakenly directed an action at an inappropriate person. These studies tested the relational-models theory of A. P. Fiske (1990b, 1991) that people use 4 basic models for social relationships. All 7 studies provide support for the theory; Ss tend to confuse people with whom they interact in the same basic relationship mode. In addition, Ss confuse people of the same gender. Other factors (age, race, role term, similarity of names) generally have smaller, less reliable effects, indicating that the 4 elementary modes of relationships are among the most salient schemata in everyday social cognition.
Article
Full-text available
To examine the impact of sex composition of groups on productivity, a meta-analytic review was conducted on the findings of past laboratory research. Two classes of factors were distinguished to account for any performance differences obtained: (a) task contents or settings that favor the interests and abilities of one sex over the other and (b) group-level factors, such as men's and women's interaction, in particular, the tendency for men in groups to offer opinions and suggestions and the tendency for women to act friendly and agree with others. Overall, all-male groups were found to perform better than all-female ones. This appeared due to tasks or settings that favored men's interests and abilities over women's. The effects of group-level factors, such as interaction style, became apparent when tasks were classified in terms of the type of interaction required for completion. Female group members' interaction apparently facilitated performance at tasks requiring positive social activities, including friendliness and agreement with others. Men's interaction style slightly, although not reliably, facilitated performance on tasks requiring task-oriented behavior, such as giving opinions and suggestions. The few findings located concerning mixed-sex groups demonstrated a slight, though not significant, tendency for mixed-sex groups to outperform same-sex ones.
Article
Full-text available
30 college students and 30 professional interviewers rated and ranked bogus resumes on suitability for a managerial position. Applicant sex, physical attractiveness, and scholastic standing were systematically varied in the resumes. A 2 * 2 * 2 * 3 repeated measures analysis of variance on the ratings yielded 4 significant main effects (p < .05), while the same analysis on the rankings yielded 3 significant main effects (p < .01). Students rated applicants more favorably than professionals. Both groups preferred males to females, attractive applicants to unattractive applicants, and applicants of high scholastic standing. The latter variable accounted for the greatest proportion of variance. However, internal analyses of the rankings reveal that sex and physical attractiveness were more important than indicated by the analysis of variance. (15 ref)