ArticlePublisher preview available

Motivation by Positive or Negative Role Models: Regulatory Focus Determines Who Will Best Inspire Us

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

In 3 studies, the authors demonstrated that individuals are motivated by role models who encourage strategies that fit their regulatory concerns: Promotion-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of pursuing desirable outcomes, are most inspired by positive role models, who highlight strategies for achieving success; prevention-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of avoiding undesirable outcomes, are most motivated by negative role models, who highlight strategies for avoiding failure. In Studies 1 and 2, the authors primed promotion and prevention goals and then examined the impact of role models on motivation. Participants' academic motivation was increased by goal-congruent role models but decreased by goal-incongruent role models. In Study 3, participants were more likely to generate real-life role models that matched their chronic goals.
Motivation by Positive or Negative Role Models:
Regulatory Focus Determines Who Will Best Inspire Us
Penelope Lockwood
University of Toronto Christian H. Jordan and Ziva Kunda
University of Waterloo
In 3 studies, the authors demonstrated that individuals are motivated by role models who encourage
strategies that fit their regulatory concerns: Promotion-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of
pursuing desirable outcomes, are most inspired by positive role models, who highlight strategies for
achieving success; prevention-focused individuals, who favor a strategy of avoiding undesirable out-
comes, are most motivated by negative role models, who highlight strategies for avoiding failure. In
Studies 1 and 2, the authors primed promotion and prevention goals and then examined the impact of role
models on motivation. Participants’ academic motivation was increased by goal-congruent role models
but decreased by goal-incongruent role models. In Study 3, participants were more likely to generate
real-life role models that matched their chronic goals.
Positive role models, individuals who have achieved outstand-
ing success, are widely expected to inspire others to pursue similar
excellence. Accordingly, the accomplishments of star athletes,
musicians, and award-winning scientists are often showcased in an
attempt to enhance people’s goals and aspirations. People are also
assumed to be motivated by negative role models, individuals who
have experienced misfortune; public service announcements high-
light examples of AIDS patients, of smokers who have suffered
lung cancer, and of motorists who have been injured as a result of
drinking and driving, in the hope of motivating people to take the
steps necessary to avoid similarly unpleasant outcomes. Indeed,
positive role models can inspire one by illustrating an ideal,
desired self, highlighting possible achievements that one can strive
for, and demonstrating the route for achieving them (Lockwood &
Kunda, 1997, 1999); negative role models can inspire one by
illustrating a feared, to-be-avoided self, pointing to possible future
disasters, and highlighting mistakes that must be avoided so as to
prevent them (Lockwood, 2002). At different times, people may be
differentially receptive to positive and negative role models (cf.
Stapel & Koomen, 2001).
We propose that the inspirational impact of positive and nega-
tive role models may depend on the goals people are striving to
achieve when they encounter these models. Goals can take the
form of pursuing desirable outcomes or avoiding undesirable out-
comes (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1998; Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996;
Higgins, 1997). People may be especially likely to be inspired by
positive role models, who represent a desired self, when they are
bent on pursuing success, and by negative role models, who
represent a feared self, when they are intent on avoiding failure.
This prediction is based on a large body of theory and research
by Higgins and his colleagues (for reviews, see Higgins, 1997,
1998), who argue that individuals can pursue two different kinds of
regulatory goals: promotion and prevention. Promotion goals en-
tail striving to achieve an ideal self, and so produce a sensitivity to
the presence or absence of positive outcomes; strategies for
achieving promotion goals involve the eager pursuit of gains or
successes. In contrast, prevention goals entail striving to avoid
disasters, and so produce a sensitivity to the presence or absence of
negative outcomes; strategies for achieving prevention goals in-
volve the vigilant avoidance of losses or failures.
When people are driven by promotion goals, they scrutinize
their social world for information that bears on the pursuit of
success. They are especially likely to notice and recall information
relating to the pursuit of success by others (Higgins & Tykocinski,
1992). They are also especially well-attuned to emotions relating
to the successful or unsuccessful pursuit of positive outcomes (i.e.,
happiness and dejection; Higgins, Shah, & Friedman, 1997). In
addition, they tend to focus on interpersonal strategies geared
toward promoting desired outcomes (Higgins, Roney, Crowe, &
Hymes, 1994). They also tend to show especially high motivation
and persistence on tasks that are framed in terms of promotion
(Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998). It stands to reason that people
in this state of mind will also be especially susceptible to positive
role models, who exemplify positive outcomes to be pursued.
Positive role models inspire others by encouraging the pursuit of
success, a promotion strategy.
Penelope Lockwood, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Christian H. Jordan and Ziva Kunda, Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
This research was supported by Social Sciences and Humanities Re-
search Council of Canada Grant 410-99-1376 awarded to Penelope Lock-
wood and Grant 410-99-0993 awarded to Ziva Kunda, by Natural Sciences
and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Grant 13843-98
awarded to Ziva Kunda, and by an NSERC doctoral fellowship awarded to
Christian H. Jordan. We are grateful to Niki Capogiannis, Keren Fyman,
Natalie Foong, Linor Gerchak, and Raymond Mar for their assistance with
data collection. We thank Michael Ross, Anne Wilson, and Joanne Wood
for their comments on an earlier version of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Penelope
Lockwood, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 St.
George Street, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada. E-mail: lockwood@
psych.utoronto.ca
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2002, Vol. 83, No. 4, 854– 864 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.854
854
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... In the present research across studies we considered the General Regulatory Focus Measure (GRFM; Lockwood et al., 2002), which centres on the reference-point definition; (Higgins & Cornwell, 2016;Summerville & Roese, 2008), or the RFQ focusing on early-age experiences and often lacking internal consistency when used outside of the US (Sassenberg et al., 2012). Vriend et al. (2023) further argue that the RFQ tends to confound goals and strategies, that the GRFM neglects non-losses, and that neither adequately addresses advancement versus security goals. ...
... Across studies, four different scales assessed participants' chronic regulatory focus. Studies 1 and 9 relied on the 14-item GRFM (Lockwood et al., 2002) and ...
... To corroborate the comparative findings from Study 1, we included the three major measures of regulatory focus: GRFM (Lockwood et al., 2002), RFQ , and RFSS (Ouschan et al., 2007). Given that Haws et al.'s composite scale (CRFM, 2010) yielded results that were comparable to the aggregate of the GRFM and RFQ in Study 1, we did not include it in Study 2. All instruments utilised a 7point Likert scale. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present work brings together perspectives from trust and the self-regulation literature to investigate how people’s regulatory focus, a form of self-regulation and motivational orientation, relates to their generalised trust. Regulatory focus theory distinguishes between two different ways that humans can self-regulate and pursue their goals: a prevention orientation (i.e., greater focus on security, ought goals, and a loss-avoidance mindset) and a promotion orientation (i.e., greater focus on growth, ideal goals, and a gain-approach mindset). People usually show a greater tendency toward one of the two orientations which can be assessed via self-reported questionnaire (often referred to as chronic regulatory focus). A secondary analysis of nine correlational studies, combined in a small-scale meta-analysis (N = 6,422), revealed a positive relationship between chronic promotion focus and generalised trust, r = .11, 95% CI [.083, .131] and a significantly larger negative relationship between chronic prevention focus and generalised trust, r = -.22, 95% CI [-.237, -.191]. A second, preregistered study (N = 478) replicated these findings on a measure of self-reported generalised trust and on trusting (cooperative) behaviour in a trust game. The present findings further our understanding of the nomological network of generalised trust. They also expand the theoretical boundaries of regulatory focus theory, highlight distinctions between conceptualisations and measures of regulatory focus, and underscore its implications for understanding the social psychological foundations of trust.
... The study of negative role models is important since they inspire others to avoid certain behaviors or negative outcomes. For instance, someone who has caused harm to another individual due to drinking and driving may encourage others to refrain from driving under the influence (Lockwood et al., 2002(Lockwood et al., , 2004. Therefore, negative role models, symbolizing a potential future loss, are more influential in the prevention of negative behaviors (Wood & VanderZee, 1997). ...
... In this case, the modeler does not want to adopt the role model's some specific features only. Some researchers suggested that the number of negative role models in an individual's life is usually higher than positive role models, and negative role models might be more influential on the behavior of the modeler (Cruess et al., 2008;Lockwood et al., 2002). As the literature on negative role models is scarce, this study aims to focus on negative role models. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the personality traits of negative role models of Turkish undergraduate psychology students. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 50 participants regarding a specific negative role model they chose. Scripts were content analyzed and interpreted in terms of the Big Five Personality Model. Inter-rater reliability analysis indicated a high level of agreement between the two coders, showing an overall Cohen’s κ of .94. Results showed that negative role models were mainly described as having low levels of conscientiousness and agreeableness. Within the theme of conscientiousness negative role models' lack of motivation, desire for achievement or life purpose, indecisiveness, and inability to deal with life difficulties were prominent. Within the theme of agreeableness negative role models’ negative attitudes and behavior such as humiliating or dominating others were mentioned. Findings indicated that negative role models existed within the close social circle of the participant (i.e., being a family member), and they were described with some positive characteristics as well. Another finding showed that female participants’ negative role models were usually female, while male participants’ negative role models were male. Findings were discussed and recommendations were presented for future studies.
... We used the scale developed by Lockwood et al. (2002), which includes two dimensions and a total of seven items. The "promotion regulatory focus" dimension has four items, a sample item is "I often think about how to achieve good results" (Cronbach'α = 0.791). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Impoverished leadership, as a form of unethical leadership behavior, can have a wide range of negative impacts. It not only affects team morale, work efficiency, cohesion, and trust but also directly influences organizational performance, reputation, and the leader’s own career development. However, previous research has rarely explored the antecedents of impoverished leadership. Methods Based on social cognitive theory and conservation of resources theory, this study investigated the impact of error aversion climate on impoverished leadership with mixed methodologies (i.e., a scenario experiment and a questionnaire survey). Results The results showed that error aversion climate positively influences impoverished leadership; moral disengagement and ego depletion serve as mediators between error aversion climate and impoverished leadership. Besides, regulatory focus moderate the relationship between error aversion climate and moral disengagement (ego depletion). Specifically, when leaders have high prevention regulatory focus and high promotion regulatory focus, the relationship between error aversion climate and moral disengagement (ego depletion) is stronger. Regulatory focus also moderate the indirect effect of error aversion climate on impoverished leadership through moral disengagement (ego depletion). The indirect effect of error aversion climate on impoverished leadership is stronger when leaders have high prevention regulatory focus and high promotion regulatory focus. Discussion The findings provide theoretical guidance for interventions to reduce impoverished leadership and offer new insights for promoting organizational sustainability.
Article
When evaluating the potential financial effects of climate change, investors demand disclosures of the climate‐related risks and opportunities that companies need to manage. We examine how and why management control over climate change performance affects investors' evaluations of such disclosures. In a series of experiments, we find that investors believe that managerial optimism is beneficial and, thus, are more willing to invest when climate‐related disclosures focus on opportunities rather than risks. This effect, however, occurs only when management has high control over the company's future climate change performance. When that control is low, investors believe that managerial realism is beneficial and, thus, are more willing to invest when these disclosures focus on risks rather than opportunities. Our study has implications for companies and standard setters considering the consequences of focusing on either risks or opportunities in climate change reporting and the conditions under which one focus or the other may be beneficial.
Article
It has been well documented that the development of artificial intelligence (AI) greatly affects employees’ sense of uncertainty. We argue that talking about AI (AI talk hereafter) is a natural way for individual to cope with such uncertainty. However, existing literature has neglected the proactivity of this coping behavior and its potential dual effect. Drawing from self-regulatory focus theory, we thus aim to explore how AI talk exerts influence on employees’ self-concepts (i.e., situational regulatory focus) and subsequent behaviors. We conducted a two-wave questionnaire survey with a sample of 312 employees on three automatic companies. Results demonstrated that AI talk could, on one hand, induce employees’ situational promotion focus and thus increase their innovative behavior, and, on the other hand, stimulate their situational prevention focus and thus increase faking behavior. Additionally, employees’ chronic regulatory focus plays moderating role on the relationship above. Specifically, we found that for individuals with low chronic promotion focus, their situation promotion focus and innovative behavior increase as AI talk increases, and for individuals with low chronic prevention focus, their situation prevention focus and faking behavior increase as AI talk increases. We discussed theoretical and practical implications of the findings followed by future research directions.
Article
In the era of digital intelligence, companies need to capture more data to portray the user profile to “grab” the “heart” of the consumer, AI is undoubtedly a powerful tool to implement this process. Nevertheless, it has also gradually sparked concerns regarding user privacy and data. Recent studies have increasingly focused on user reactions to AI data capture, yet exploration in this area remains limited. This research contributes to the existing literature by investigating the underlying psychological processes and influencing factors that prompt users to confront AI data capture. Through four studies, we found the data capture strategies have a significant negative impact on users' intention to use AI systems, and compared with the overt data capture strategy, the covert strategy would make users' intention to use AI systems lower. The impact is mediated by psychological ownership, specifically, it is mediated by perceived control of psychological ownership rather than perceived possession. Additionally, prevention‐oriented users are more likely to feel deprived of their right to be informed and their control over data. However, AI explainability can increase users' psychological ownership and intention to use by alleviating their psychological defenses in the process. These results are conducive to promoting the resolution of AI data governance issues under digital intelligence empowerment, and providing a reference for reasonable strategies adopted by enterprises in using AI data capture.
Article
Full-text available
Inaction inertia reduces the likelihood of consumers accepting an opportunity after previously missing a more favorable one. This study explores how consumers' regulatory focus influences the inaction inertia effect. The authors propose that because promotion‐ (vs. prevention‐) focused individuals tend to process information at a high construal level, they are more likely to decouple the previous and current opportunities due to distal psychological distance, reducing perceived regret, and inaction inertia. Promotion‐ (vs. prevention‐) focused individuals are more likely to experience higher processing fluency when encountering the current opportunity due to regulatory fit, which forms more positive overall feelings and reduces the inaction inertia effect. Results from one pilot study and three formal experiments show that people with a promotion (vs. prevention) focus are subject to lower inaction inertia. Moreover, perceived fluency and overall feelings play a more important and robust sequential mediating role than perceived coupling and regret. The current research findings address important gaps in the literature on inaction inertia and regulatory focus, providing valuable insights for practitioners in designing advertising and marketing strategies to mitigate the inaction inertia effect.
Article
Purpose This paper aims to propose the sensory stimulation spillover effect phenomenon, defined as the process by which sensory stimulation in one area generates positive impressions and favorably impacts opinions in other areas. Specifically, this paper demonstrates that the spillover effect of sensory priming via an advertised brand impacts the viewer’s self-brand connections (the mental representation of a brand connected to an individual’s self-concept), brand attitude and brand purchase intention. Design/methodology/approach Across six experiments, 883 participants considered advertised brands from diverse product categories (food snacks, electronics and detergent). The multisensory prime in Studies 1–3 uses positively valenced sensory imagery and text, whereas the multisensory prime in Studies 4–6 is a sensory imaging task. Studies 1–4 examine the spillover effect of the multisensory prime on consumers’ self-brand connections, as well as downstream brand-related variables. Studies 5 and 6, respectively, examined the moderating roles of advertising appeal, regulatory focus (promotion vs prevention) and cognitive versus affective tone. Findings Results provide robust evidence of the proposed sensory stimulation spillover effect. Sensory priming strengthens self-brand connections and positively impacts brand attitude and purchase intention; self-brand connections mediate the relationship between a multisensory prime and brand attitude and purchase intention. The sensory stimulation spillover effect is stronger when advertisements have a promotion (vs prevention) focus and particularly for participants with a stronger intrinsic promotion (vs prevention) orientation, as well as for advertisements with an affective (vs a cognitive) tone. Research limitations/implications The authors manipulated sensory stimulation using visual images and text as well as using a multisensory-imaging task. Future work can explore the use of actual sensory stimulation, and retail spaces or public venues may provide opportunities for field experiments to study sensory stimulation in situ . Practical implications The research focuses on spillover effects in an advertising context with broader implications for consumers’ in-store shopping experiences based on multisensory store architecture and atmospherics, as well as online shopping that is impacted by multisensory information. Originality/value This paper introduces the phenomenon of sensory stimulation spillover effect, the process by which sensory stimulation in one area generates positive impressions and favorably impacts opinions in other areas and demonstrates that multisensory priming strengthens self-brand connections and downstream brand-related variables, with self-brand connections as the mediator. The results are robust across multiple product categories and are contingent upon the type of advertising appeal. The research focuses on spillover effects in an advertising context with broader implications for consumers’ in-store shopping experiences based on multisensory store architecture and atmospherics, as well as online shopping which is impacted by multisensory information.
Article
Full-text available
Study 1 demonstrated that as individuals’ promotion-related ideal strength increases, performance on an anagram task is greater for a monetary task incentive framed in terms of gains and nongains (i.e., promotion framed) than one framed in terms of losses and nonlosses (i.e., prevention framed), whereas the reverse is true as individuals’ prevention-related ought strength increases. Study 2 further demonstrated that with promotion-framed task incentives, individuals’ ideal strength increases motivation for promotion-related goal attainment means (gaining points), whereas with prevention-framed task incentives, individuals’ ought strength increases motivation for prevention-related means (avoiding losing points). These results suggest that motivation and performance are greater when the regulatory focus of task incentives and means match (vs. mismatch) the chronic regulatory focus of the performers.
Article
Full-text available
Two studies using different paradigms activated either ideal self-guides (a person's hopes or wishes) or ought self-guides (a person's sense of duty and responsibility) and measured Ss' concern with different forms of self-regulation: approaching matches to desired end states or mismatches to undesired end states and avoiding mismatches to desired end states or matches to undesired end states. A 3rd study asked ideal versus ought discrepant Ss to select among alternative strategies for friendship. The results suggest that a concern with approach is greater for ideal than ought self-regulation, whereas a concern with avoidance is greater for ought than ideal self-regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Social comparison processes include the desire to affiliate with others, the desire for information about others, and explicit self-evaluation against others. Previously these types of comparison activity and their corresponding measures have been treated as interchangeable. We present evidence that in certain groups under threat, these comparison activities diverge, with explicit self-evaluation made against a less fortunate target (downward evaluation), but information and affiliation sought out from more fortunate others (upward contacts). These effects occur because downward evaluation and upward contacts appear to serve different needs, the former ameliorating self-esteem and the latter enabling a person to improve his or her situation and simultaneously increase motivation and hope. Implications for the concept, measurement, and theory of social comparison are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies examined the impact of downward comparisons on the self. Worse-off others exerted an impact only when participants drew an analogy between themselves and the other. When participants did draw an analogy, the impact of the other on the self was determined by perceived vulnerability to the other's negative fate. When vulnerability was low, downward comparisons enhanced self-evaluations. When vulnerability was high, downward comparisons deflated self-evaluations, but activated a prevention orientation, boosting motivation aimed at avoiding the negative experience of the other.
Article
Full-text available
People approach pleasure and avoid pain. To discover the true nature of approach–avoidance motivation, psychologists need to move beyond this hedonic principle to the principles that underlie the different ways that it operates. One such principle is regulatory focus, which distinguishes self-regulation with a promotion focus (accomplishments and aspirations) from self-regulation with a prevention focus (safety and responsibilities). This principle is used to reconsider the fundamental nature of approach–avoidance, expectancy–value relations, and emotional and evaluative sensitivities. Both types of regulatory focus are applied to phenonomena that have been treated in terms of either promotion (e.g., well-being) or prevention (e.g., cognitive dissonance). Then, regulatory focus is distinguished from regulatory anticipation and regulatory reference, 2 other principles underlying the different ways that people approach pleasure and avoid pain.
Chapter
In the last two decades, an approach to the study of motivation has emerged that focuses on specific cognitive and affective mediators of behaviour, in contrast to more general traits or motives. This 'social-cognitive' approach grants goal-oriented motivation its own role in shaping cognition, emotion and behaviour, rather than reducing goal-directed behaviour to cold-blooded information processing or to an enactment of a personality type. This book adds to this process-oriented approach a developmental perspective. Critical elements of motivational systems can be specified and their inter-relations understood by charting the origins and the developmental course of motivational processes. Moreover, a process-oriented approach helps to identify critical transitions and effective developmental interventions. The chapters in this book cover various age groups throughout the life span and stem from four big traditions in motivational psychology: achievement motivation, action theory, the psychology of causal attribution and perceived control, and the psychology of personal causation and intrinsic motivation.
Book
This book presents a thorough overview of a model of human functioning based on the idea that behavior is goal-directed and regulated by feedback control processes. It describes feedback processes and their application to behavior, considers goals and the idea that goals are organized hierarchically, examines affect as deriving from a different kind of feedback process, and analyzes how success expectancies influence whether people keep trying to attain goals or disengage. Later sections consider a series of emerging themes, including dynamic systems as a model for shifting among goals, catastrophe theory as a model for persistence, and the question of whether behavior is controlled or instead 'emerges'. Three chapters consider the implications of these various ideas for understanding maladaptive behavior, and the closing chapter asks whether goals are a necessity of life. Throughout, theory is presented in the context of diverse issues that link the theory to other literatures.
Article
What was noted by E. J. Langer (1978) remains true today; that much of contemporary psychological research is based on the assumption that people are consciously and systematically processing incoming information in order to construe and interpret their world and to plan and engage in courses of action. As did E. J. Langer, the authors question this assumption. First, they review evidence that the ability to exercise such conscious, intentional control is actually quite limited, so that most of moment-to-moment psychological life must occur through nonconscious means if it is to occur at all. The authors then describe the different possible mechanisms that produce automatic, environmental control over these various phenomena and review evidence establishing both the existence of these mechanisms as well as their consequences for judgments, emotions, and behavior. Three major forms of automatic self-regulation are identified: an automatic effect of perception on action, automatic goal pursuit, and a continual automatic evaluation of one's experience. From the accumulating evidence, the authors conclude that these various nonconscious mental systems perform the lion's share of the self-regulatory burden, beneficently keeping the individual grounded in his or her current environment.