ArticlePublisher preview available

Toward a Structure- and Process-Integrated View of Personality: Traits as Density Distributions of States

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Three experience-sampling studies explored the distributions of Big-Five-relevant states (behavior) across 2 to 3 weeks of everyday life. Within-person variability was high, such that the typical individual regularly and routinely manifested nearly all levels of all traits in his or her everyday behavior. Second, individual differences in central tendencies of behavioral distributions were almost perfectly stable. Third, amount of behavioral variability (and skew and kurtosis) were revealed as stable individual differences. Finally, amount of within-person variability in extraversion was shown to reflect individual differences in reactivity to extraversion-relevant situational cues. Thus, decontextualized and noncontingent Big-Five content is highly useful for descriptions of individuals' density distributions as wholes. Simultaneously, contextualized and contingent personality units (e.g., conditional traits, goals) are needed for describing the considerable within-person variation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
2001,
Vol. 80, No. 6, 1011-1027Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/01/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.80.6.1011
Toward a Structure- and Process-Integrated View of Personality:
Traits as Density Distributions of States
William Fleeson
Wake Forest University
Three experience-sampling studies explored the distributions of Big-Five-relevant states (behavior)
across 2 to 3 weeks of everyday life. Within-person variability was high, such that the typical individual
regularly and routinely manifested nearly all levels of all traits in his or her everyday behavior. Second,
individual differences in central tendencies of behavioral distributions were almost perfectly stable.
Third, amount of behavioral variability (and skew and kurtosis) were revealed as stable individual
differences. Finally, amount of within-person variability in extraversion was shown to reflect individual
differences in reactivity to extraversion-reievant situational cues. Thus, decontextualized and noncon-
tingent Big-Five content is highly useful for descriptions of individuals' density distributions as wholes.
Simultaneously, contextualized and contingent personality units (e.g., conditional traits, goals) are
needed for describing the considerable within-person variation.
The purpose of this article is to identify aspects of individuals'
everyday trait-relevant behavior over time that are relevant to and
explainable by personality psychology. The presumption is that,
because the same individual behaves differently on different oc-
casions, an individual's behavior over time forms a distribution;
the central proposal is that the entire distribution and its several
components are relevant to—and to be explained by—personality
psychology. Specifically, on the basis of a view of individuals as
actively reacting to context (Allport, 1937; Brown & Moskowitz,
1998;
Cantor, 1990; Diener, Larsen, & Emmons, 1984; Mischel,
1968;
Mischel & Peake, 1982; Mischel & Shoda, 1998; Mosko-
witz, 1982; Nesselroade, 1988, 1991; Revelle, 1995), it is pro-
posed, first, that the average individual routinely and regularly
expresses all levels of all traits and that this within-person vari-
ability is predictable as individual differences in reactions to
situational cues. Second, on the basis of work concerning the
predictability of behavior (e.g., Diener & Larsen, 1984; Epstein,
1979;
Moskowitz, 1982), it is proposed that although single be-
haviors are less predictable, the mean of the distribution is among
the most predictable variables in psychology. Third, on the basis of
theory that variability is itself a stable individual-differences char-
acteristic (Fiske, 1961; Larsen, 1989; Murray, 1938), it is proposed
that parameters beyond the mean are also meaningful aspects of
personality. That is, it is useful for personality to be conceived of
as density distributions as wholes rather than as only one aspect of
the distributions. To evaluate these claims empirically, three
experience-sampling studies are presented in which many individ-
I thank Anne Fleeson, Jason Fors, Alexandra Freund, Batja Mesquita,
Benjamin Peterson, Daniel Green, Elizabeth Laney, and Adriane Malanos
for their help with this line of work. I also thank the Max Planck Institute
for Human Development, Center for Lifespan Development (Director, Paul
B.
Baltes) for support during the writing of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to William
Fleeson, Department of Psychology, Wake Forest University, Winston-
Salem, North Carolina 27109. Electronic mail may be sent to fleesoww®
wfu.edu.
uals described their current behavior several times per day for 2-3
weeks.
Directly assessing individuals' everyday trait-relevant behavior
over time is intended as important for at least three reasons. Most
important, it addresses a basic question about the nature of per-
sonality: How is trait content manifest in everyday behavior, and
do individuals differ in such manifestation? If personality psychol-
ogy is going to say something about behavior with its constructs,
we must know how that behavior is patterned and where individual
differences occur in those patterns (Barrett & Pietromonaco, 1997;
Borkenau & Liebler, 1995; Borkenau &
Ostendorf,
1998; Botwin,
1989;
Buss & Craik, 1983; Diener & Larsen, 1984; Emmons,
Diener, & Larsen, 1986; Larsen, 1989; Revelle, 1995; Schwartz,
Neale, Marco, Shiffman, & Stone, 1999). A second reason this is
important is that considerable debate in personality psychology
depends on the actual nature of behavioral distributions (e.g.,
Block, 1995; Cervone, 1991; Pervin, 1994). Specifically, the use-
fulness of structural or noncontingent approaches to explaining
behavior depends on the stability of aggregated means, whereas
the usefulness of process or contingent approaches to explaining
behavior depends on the amount of within-person variability to be
explained. With both Epstein (1994) and Mischel and Shoda
(1998) calling for compromise and integration between the struc-
tural and the process approaches to personality, it appears the field
is prepared for the end of this debate. This article proposes one
way to integrate the two factious approaches to personality by
demonstrating that trait concepts are inclusive of both impressive
levels of within-person stability and impressive levels of within-
person variability.
The third reason for assessing everyday trait-relevant behavior
is that it may reveal additional aspects of everyday behavior over
time as relevant to personality. For example, it has long been
theorized that the amount of variability itself is a stable, individual-
differences characteristic (Fiske, 1961; Larsen, 1989; Murray,
1938),
but it has not been investigated empirically for everyday
behavior. The present article operationalizes such concepts as
1011
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Recent research has shown that workaholic tendencies fluctuate from one day to another (Balducci et al., 2022;Xu et al., 2021) in the context of a stable tendency toward workaholism (Clark et al., 2021). Such findings offer empirical support for whole-trait theory (Fleeson, 2001), which proposes that state-like manifestations of any personality trait have the same affective, behavioral, and cognitive components as their corresponding traits but can vary from one moment to another around a personal average, depending on the situation (Fleeson & Jayawickreme, 2015). For example, individuals might have a general level of obsessive preoccupation with and compulsive engagement in excessive work, which they recall and evaluate retrospectively when they fill out a general workaholism survey. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this study, we adopted a multilevel perspective over workaholism, conceptualized as a personal demand driving higher perceived workload and exhaustion during typical working weeks. We adopted a diary design and tested whether workload mediates the weekly positive relationship between workaholism and emotional exhaustion. We also argued that perceptions of a family-supportive climate (family-supportive organization perceptions [FSOP]) as an organizational resource would buffer the weekly relationships between workaholism, workload, and exhaustion. We collected survey data from 167 employees from diverse organizations (one general and seven weekly measures; NL1 = 1,168). The data fully supported the proposed mediated process, showing that on weeks when employees struggled with a stronger workaholic drive, they tackled higher workloads and felt more emotionally exhausted. The data also supported a strong beneficial effect of FSOP in preventing weekly exhaustion and buffering the effects of workload on exhaustion. However, we found less conclusive results regarding the interaction between FSOP and weekly workaholism. FSOP did not affect the association between workaholism and workload and enhanced, rather than buffered, the link between workaholism and exhaustion.
... Darüber hinaus wird ausgehend von einigen Befunden (z. B. Bleidorn, 2009;Fleeson, 2001) ...
... First, our study sheds light on whether higher trait curiosity manifested in greater levels of state curiosity in line with personality theories that conceptualize personality traits as density distributions of their respective states (Fleeson, 2001). As an important extension of prior research, we tested how trait curiosity was related to both behavioral state curiosity (in the form of self-directed information seeking) and self-reported state curiosity (on the knowledge test). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
To expand their knowledge and satisfy their intellectual curiosity, people need to engage in self-directed information seeking. However, curiosity research often relies on experimental tasks that explicitly prompt information seeking instead of capturing participants’ self-initiated exploratory behaviors. The present study aimed to combine aspects of experimental curiosity research with more naturalistic exploration methods by introducing a novel experimental set-up that captured self-directed information seeking as a more naturalistic behavioral measure of curiosity. In a preregistered online study (N = 799), participants freely explored a hypertext on a historical topic while their self-directed information seeking (i.e., clicks on hyperlinks and reading time) was captured with log files. Participants then completed a knowledge test in which they also reported their confidence in their answers and their curiosity about learning the correct answers, similar to commonly used curiosity tasks. Additionally, participants’ trait curiosity was measured with a questionnaire before they read the hypertext. Using mixed-effects regression models, we found that simply choosing to seek additional information (click on a hyperlink) did not predict subsequent curiosity ratings, whereas the extent of engagement in information seeking (reading time) did. Moreover, trait curiosity moderated the relationship between confidence and state curiosity, with highly trait curious individuals maintaining higher state curiosity under low confidence, unlike those with low trait curiosity. This study presents a promising way to assess self-directed information-seeking behavior as a manifestation of curiosity and provides a comprehensive perspective on the dynamic ways in which curiosity is sparked and satisfied.
... Whole trait theory offers both a descriptive and explanatory account of traits. The present study focuses on the descriptive side of the theory, which suggests that personality traits can be understood as a density distribution of personality states, that is, momentary enactments of personality traits (Baird et al., 2006;Fleeson, 2001). From this perspective, individuals can be described not only in terms of the mean of the density distribution but also in terms of the variation of the observed personality states. ...
Article
Full-text available
Infant temperament is usually considered biologically driven and a precursor of personality. Despite being conceived as trait measures, parent reports for assessing infant temperament use short timescales, for example, the past seven days, implying variability in temperament traits’ expressions. In two daily diary studies, we used the whole trait theory perspective to investigate whether infant temperament is observable daily and to what degree it varies within person across days. In Study 1, N = 137 mothers of infants aged 6–18 months reported on their infant’s daily (state) temperament (median number of days: 8 and total observations: 984). The results suggest a substantial within-person variation in daily infant temperament (ICCs: .41–.54). Study 2 ( N = 199 mothers, median number of days: 7, and total observations: 1375) replicated these results on the variability in infant state temperament (ICCs: .41–.51). In addition, infant state temperament was related to infant trait temperament. However, certain temperament items—primarily those assessing surgency—were frequently rated as not applicable and did not seem suitable for daily assessments. Across both studies, results indicate substantial within-person variability in daily infant temperament and a strong trait component.
Article
Full-text available
Action-state orientation (ASO) refers to regulatory modes influencing intention initiation and goal pursuit. While ASO pertains to both dispositional and time- and context-sensitive states, extant research predominantly focuses on individual differences in ASO, leaving within-person dynamics and situational antecedents underexplored. Addressing this gap, the study explored task-related fluctuations in ASO (ASO-T) and its relationship with task-related affect and anticipated task difficulty. Both the pilot study (Nperson = 62, Nmeasurement = 248) and the main study (Nperson = 165, Nmeasurement = 660) employed a biweekly data collection. However, in the main study, the assessment of anticipated task difficulty and other measures was temporally separated. Results of the pilot study indicated that the measurement of ASO-T is a reliable and valid instrument and ASO-T displays substantial within-person variation. The main study findings showed that task-related negative affect, but not positive affect, mediates the relationship between anticipated task difficulty and two forms of ASO-T (hesitation and preoccupation). Additionally, task-related positive affect was more strongly related to ASO-T than task-related negative affect. These findings confirm ASO-T as a context-sensitive and malleable construct and extend the ASO research by shedding light on task-related, situational antecedents of ASO-T.
Article
Full-text available
Although a majority of studies show that acting in a more extraverted way is beneficial for one’s well-being, there are also indications that acting counterdispositionally is emotionally draining. In the current study, we try to explain these discrepancies by comparing indirect measures of counterdispositional extraversion (one relative to the trait level and one relative to the average state level) and we introduce a novel direct measure that taps into perceived counterdispositional extraversion. By using Multilevel Polynomial Regression and Response Surface Analysis in a large ESM dataset ( N = 187 individuals and N = 17,547 repeated observations), we found that for both direct and indirect measures of counterdispositional extraversion, higher state levels of extraversion related to higher levels of positive affect (PA), and this effect appears to be stronger for individuals high on average state and trait extraversion. Findings for negative affect (NA) were less consistent, with perceived deviations from one’s typical behavior relating to higher levels of NA. Altogether, our findings show that acting in a more extraverted way is better for increasing PA, on the understanding that subjective experiences of atypical behavior can be experienced as harmful.
Chapter
This indispensable collection provides extensive, yet accessible, coverage of conceptual and practical issues in research design in personality and social psychology. Using numerous examples and clear guidelines, especially for conducting complex statistical analysis, leading experts address specific methods and areas of research to capture a definitive overview of contemporary practice. Updated and expanded, this third edition engages with the most important methodological innovations over the past decade, offering a timely perspective on research practice in the field. To reflect such rapid advances, this volume includes commentary on particularly timely areas of development such as social neuroscience, mobile sensing methods, and innovative statistical applications. Seasoned and early-career researchers alike will find a range of tools, methods, and practices that will help improve their research and develop new conceptual and methodological possibilities. Supplementary online materials are available on Cambridge Core.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive maps are thought to arise, at least in part, from our intrinsic curiosity to explore unknown places. However, it remains untested how curiosity shapes aspects of spatial exploration in humans. Combining a virtual reality task with indices of exploration complexity, we found that pre-exploration curiosity states predicted how much individuals spatially explored environments, whereas markers of visual exploration determined post-exploration feelings of interest. Moreover, individual differences in curiosity traits, particularly Stress Tolerance, modulated the relationship between curiosity and spatial exploration, suggesting the capacity to cope with uncertainty enhances the curiosity-exploration link. Furthermore, both curiosity and spatial exploration predicted how precisely participants could recall spatial-relational details of the environment, as measured by a sketch map task. These results provide new evidence for a link between curiosity and exploratory behaviour, and how curiosity might shape cognitive map formation.
Article
This study investigates Tolerance of Ambiguity in project managers, focusing on how their ability to navigate ambiguous situations varies over time and influences their performance. In a four-week study of 275 project managers, we tracked changes in Tolerance of Ambiguity in project managers and found that these temporary fluctuations were significantly associated with positive mood, adaptive performance, and project progress. Factors such as self-control, coping skills, creativity, and supportive leadership emerged as key drivers of these changes. This study highlights Tolerance of Ambiguity as a dynamic, context-dependent competency in project managers, offering novel insights for enhancing project managers’ ability to navigate ambiguous situations.
Article
Full-text available
This article gives an overview of previous work on affect variability, discusses the methodological shortcomings of research on affect variability, and presents the results of an empirical study of intraindividual variability in primary emotions across time. The results of a daily assessment study using structural equation modeling and nonlinear regression analyses showed that intraindividual variability in affect is a multidimensional construct that is sufficiently stable to be considered a psychological trait and can be reliably measured by the intraindividual standard deviation. Intraindividual variability showed convergent validity with mean level scores and neuroticism but was sufficiently distinct to be considered a unique trait. This was particularly true of intraindividual variability in positive emotions; only about 10% of the variance could be accounted for by mean affect levels and the variables of the 5-factor model of personality.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the extent to which momentary reports of coping are a traitlike phenomenon and the validity of a questionnaire assessment of coping style. Participants (N = 96) completed the questionnaire, then monitored stressful events and coping activities for 2 days using a hand-held computer that administered a brief assessment every 40 min. Momentary reports exhibited a sizable traitlike component: Individual differences accounted for 42% of the variance for 2 coping items and 15–30% of the variance for 15 items. The questionnaire assessment of coping style was a poor predictor of average momentary coping; the coping style measure and the aggregated momentary measure of trait coping shared 23% of their variance for religion and 0–12% for the 16 other coping measures. Self-report assessments of trait coping are poor measures of the trait component of momentary coping and very poor predictors of coping in specific situations.
Article
Full-text available
Previous factor-analytic studies of lexical person descriptors have produced some recurrent patterns of results, but their integration has been hampered by divergences in variable sampling, such as disparate criteria for what is considered a personality descriptor. To isolate effects of variable selection on factor structures, 500 of the most familiar English person descriptors were identified. Fifteen judges provided reliable classifications of these adjectives as disposition, state, social evaluation, or physical–appearance terms. Analyses of adult self-ratings (N = 700) and acquaintance ratings (N = 201) led to a stable Big Five structure when disposition terms, or combined disposition and state terms, were analyzed. Including a wider range of terms led to two additional stable factors: Attractiveness and a factor resembling Big Seven Negative Valence. A stable 3-factor solution was relatively impervious to variable-selection effects.
Article
Full-text available
In nomothetic analyses, the cross-situational consistency of individual differences in social behavior, assessed in vivo in a camp setting, depended on the similarity in the psychological features of situations. As predicted by the social–cognitive theory of personality, idiographic analyses revealed that individuals were characterized by stable profiles of if…then…, situation–behavior relationships that formed “behavioral signatures” of personality (e.g., he aggresses when warned by adults but complies when threatened by peers. Thus, the intraindividual organization of behavior variation across situations was enduring but discriminatively patterned, visible as distinctive profiles of situation–behavior relationships. Implications were examined for an idiographic reconceptualization of personality coherence and its behavioral expressions in relation to the psychological ingredients of situations.
Article
Full-text available
Descriptions of self (e.g., “I am dominant”) may reflect not only a person's behavioral tendencies but also idiosyncratic definitions of the social concepts and categories being considered. Five studies revealed that participants who differed in their self-ratings along trait dimensions also tended to associate different behaviors and performances with those traits. For example, participants who described themselves as dominant tended to emphasize desirable over undesirable behaviors and characteristics in their definitions of the trait, whereas self-described nondominants highlighted the opposite. Participants' self-ratings on dominance were also influenced by making positive or negative examples of dominant behavior salient to them. Moreover, when participants were induced to shift self-descriptions in self-serving ways, they tended to do so by revising their prototype of the trait in question. Discussion centers on the implications of these findings for the prediction of behavior and the interpretation of some social science results.
Article
Full-text available
This study addressed two questions: Do daily fluctuations in mood exhibit a 7-day (circaseptum) cycle, and are there reliable individual differences in how entrained peoples' moods are to such a weekly cycle? Spectral analysis of daily mood over 84 occasions revealed a strong weekly rhythm in the temporal organization of mood in a sample of 74 undergraduates. A sine wave with a period of 7 days accounted for 40% of the variance in the daily mood data. Individual differences were also found in how entrained subjects' moods were to this weekly rhythm. We predicted that extraverts (compared with introverts) should be less entrained to a weekly cycle. Results suggest that the novelty- and sensation-seeking behavior of extraverts most likely serves to lessen the cyclical predictability of their day-to-day moods. The origin and psychological meaning of the 7-day week are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Compared the relative impact of major life events, chronic stressors, and minor daily stressors on daily reports of mood. Analyses are based on daily diary data combined with interview data derived from a longitudinal study of 96 women (mean age 33.1 yrs) sampled from the population of persons using the services of a neighborhood health center. Ss kept daily diaries for 28 days following initial personal interviews. The Affect Balance Scale was used as a baseline measure of psychological well-being. The most important direct determinants of mood were found to be concurrent daily stressors and physical symptoms, and previous levels of psychological well-being. Life events and chronic stressors had indirect effects on mood through these other variables. Results confirm and extend previous studies on the determinants of short-term changes in mood. The advantages and disadvantages of the temporal aggregation of time-series data are discussed. (44 ref)
Article
Full-text available
Behavioral acts constitute the building blocks of interpersonal perception and the basis for inferences about personality traits. How reliably can observers code the acts individuals perform in a specific situation? How valid are retrospective self-reports of these acts? Participants interacted in a group discussion task and then reported their act frequencies, which were later coded by observers from videotapes. For each act, observer-observer agreement, self-observer agreement, and self-enhancement bias were examined. Findings show that (a) agreement varied greatly across acts; (b) much of this variation was predictable from properties of the acts (observability, base rate, desirability, Big Five domain); (c) on average, self-reports were positively distorted; and (d) this was particularly true for narcissistic individuals. Discussion focuses on implications for research on acts, traits, social perception, and the act frequency approach.
Article
The literature on symptoms of depression has included diurnal changes in mood. The morning-worse pattern is commonly mentioned. This pattern is often associated with endogenous or vegetative symptoms (e.g., weight and appetite loss, loss of pleasure, psychomotor retardation). However, depression researchers have also identified an evening-worse pattern of mood. This pattern is sometimes thought to be associated with milder depressive symptoms, and may characterize chronic dysthymia rather than clinical depression. The present study examines a nonclinical sample to test the hypothesis that an evening-worse diurnal pattern of mood would be associated with trait neuroticism, anxiety, and subclinical depressive symptoms. An experience sampling methodology was employed to assess mood three times a day for 60 consecutive days. This allowed us to calculate a reliable aggregate score for diurnal mood patterns. The evening-worse pattern was associated with many neurotic features, with scores on depression and anxiety measures, and with a cognitive style indicative of hopelessness. Discussion focuses on how an evening-worse diurnal pattern of mood may be indicative of mild subclinical depression, chronic dysthymia, or personality traits associated with negative affectivity.