ArticlePDF Available

Feldstudie zur Erprobung einer Vorderen Bremsleuchte am Flughafen Berlin-Tegel

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Kann die Vordere Bremsleuchte Fußgängern und anderen Verkehrsteilnehmern die Wahrnehmung kritischer Verkehrssituationen erleichtern und so die Verkehrssicherheit erhöhen? In einer längsschnittlichen Feldstudie wurde überprüft, welche Effekte die Vordere Bremsleuchte in einem abgeschlossenen Verkehrsbereich, dem luftseitigen Teil des Flughafens Berlin-Tegel (TXL), auf die Verkehrssicherheit hat. Dafür wurden 102 Fahrzeuge für einen Zeitraum von dreieinhalb Monaten mit einer Vorderen Bremsleuchte ausgestattet und 197 Mitarbeiter vor und nach dieser Testphase zu ihren Erfahrungen mit und ihrer Einstellung zu der Vorderen Bremsleuchte befragt. Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass die Vordere Bremsleuchte nur selten zu Situationen geführt hat, in denen es zu Missverständnissen kam, und häufiger zu Situationen, in denen sie die wahrgenommene Verkehrssicherheit erhöht hat. Die Einstellung zur Vorderen Bremsleuchte war schon in der ersten Befragung sehr positiv und verbesserte sich darüber hinaus signifikant über die dreieinhalb Monate des Feldversuches hinweg. Insgesamt beurteilt eine große Mehrheit der Versuchsteilnehmer die Vordere Bremsleuchte nach praktischer Erfahrung positiv und ist der Meinung, dass sie die Verkehrssicherheit erhöht und die Kommunikation zwischen Fahrzeugführern und anderen Verkehrsteilnehmern verbessert.
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
A preview of the PDF is not available
... In another recent longitudinal field study, 102 (non-automated) vehicles on the apron of an airport were equipped with FBLs for 3.5 months. The surveyed staff members rated the FBL overall positively and stated that it improved communication and safety (Monzel, 2018). In an earlier study, participants used an FBL on their private vehicle for a month. ...
... Interestingly, they also ascribed a good hedonic quality to the FBL. These positive reactions are in line with prior studies (Monzel, 2018;Petzoldt et al., 2018;Post & Mortimer, 1971). However, as our study only investigated one very simple scenario, this positive reaction cannot be generalized to all possible interactions with FBLs in traffic. ...
Article
Full-text available
Effects of a frontal brake light (FBL, a potential external human–machine interface for automated vehicles) on participants’ self-reported willingness to cross a vehicle’s path were investigated. In a mixed design online study (vehicles in the experimental group were equipped with FBLs, there were no FBLs in the control group), participants observed videos of a vehicle approaching at different speeds from the perspective of a pedestrian standing at the curb. The vehicles exhibited either yielding behavior (braking onset 55 m or 32 m before standstill in front of the pedestrian ’s position) or non-yielding behavior (approach speed was maintained). Participants specified their willingness to cross the vehicle’s path at different distances. When the vehicle yielded (i.e., FBL was activated), willingness to cross was significantly higher in the experimental group than the control group. Notably, we further observed a significantly lower willingness to cross in the experimental group than the control group when the vehicle did not yield (i.e., FBL was deactivated). Novel external human–machine interfaces might therefore influence the interaction with vehicles not only when they are activated but also when they are deactivated.
... Despite their potential, state eHMIs have received limited attention in the literature. Still, evidence from video, field, and VR studies shows that eHMIs communicating "I am braking" are well received, help pedestrians perceive the vehicle's state, and influence perceived safety and crossing behavior [44,[54][55][56]. The possible value of state eHMIs is further corroborated by a body of research showing that providing system transparency through visual communication of what an automated system is currently doing has proven beneficial [57][58][59][60][61][62]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, there has been a debate on whether automated vehicles (AVs) should be equipped with novel external human–machine interfaces (eHMIs). Many studies have demonstrated how eHMIs influence pedestrians’ attitudes (e.g., trust in AVs) and behavior when they activate (e.g., encourage crossing by lighting up). However, very little attention has been paid to their effects when they do not activate (e.g., discourage crossing by not lighting up). We conducted a video-based laboratory study with a mixed design to explore the potential of two different eHMI messages to facilitate pedestrian-AV interactions by means of activating or not activating. Our participants watched videos of an approaching AV equipped with either a state eHMI (“I am braking”) or intent eHMI (“I intend to yield to you”) from the perspective of a pedestrian about to cross the road. They indicated when they would initiate crossing and repeatedly rated their trust in the AV. Our results show that the activation of both the state and intent eHMI was effective in communicating the AV’s intent to yield and both eHMIs drew attention to a failure to yield when they did not activate. However, the two eHMIs differed in their potential to mislead pedestrians, as decelerations accompanied by the activation of the state eHMI were repeatedly misinterpreted as an intention to yield. Despite this, user experience ratings did not differ between the eHMIs. Following a failure to yield, trust declined sharply. In subsequent trials, crossing behavior recovered quickly, while trust took longer to recover.
Article
Safe vehicles are an important pillar in reducing the number of accidents or mitigating the consequences of a collision. Although the number of autonomous safety systems in vehicles is increasing, retrofitted systems could also help reduce road accidents. A new retrofit assistance system called Front Brake Light (FBL) helps the driver to assess the intentions of other road users. This system is mounted at the front of the vehicle and works similarly to the rear brake lights. The objective of this study is to evaluate the safety performance of an FBL in real accidents at junctions. Depending on the type of accident, between 7.5% and 17.0% of the accidents analysed can be prevented. A further 9.0% to 25.5% could be positively influenced by the FBL; i.e., the collision speed could be reduced. If the FBLs were visible to the driver of the priority vehicle, the number of potentially avoidable accidents would increase to a magnitude of 11.5% to 26.2%. The range of accidents in which the consequences can be reduced increases to between 13.8% and 39.2%.
Article
Full-text available
This paper examines organizational change, focusing on the distinction of changes according to their scope and presenting a typology of evolutionary and strategic changes. It also offers an in-depth study of resistance to change. Through empirical research, the paper analyzes the importance of the sources of resistance to change defined theoretically, and considers both types of changes. The article shows which sources of resistance differ most, according to the scope of change, offering hints about where organizations should pay special attention when initiating a change process.
Article
Previous research has recommended several measures of effect size for studies with repeated measurements in both treatment and control groups. Three alternate effect size estimates were compared in terms of bias, precision, and robustness to heterogeneity of variance. The results favored an effect size based on the mean pre-post change in the treatment group minus the mean pre-post change in the control group, divided by the pooled pretest standard deviation.
Article
Aim of the presented research is the development of a cognitive driver assistance system, which can capture the traffic situation, analyse it, and warn the driver in case a pedestrian is a potential hazard. Hence parameters have to be identified by which the intention of the pedestrian can be unambiguously predicted. Two approaches to the topic are addressed. First, the pedestrian’s perspective was taken. The question was how crossing decisions were influenced by the parameters distance and velocity of the car. Following a signal, participants had to choose to cross the road in front of or behind the car. The data analysis showed that pedestrians relied on the distance of the car rather than the time to collision for their decision. In the second experiment the observer’s perspective raised the question what parameters humans use to predict pedestrians’ intentions. Videos of natural traffic scenes were presented. Participants had to make statements about whether the shown pedestrian would cross the street during the next moment. In a baseline and four experimental conditions, certain information was masked in the videos. Just the condition in which only the trajectory information of the pedestrian was available produced a higher error rate.
Article
http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/903/2/15776.0001.001.pdf
Article
The aim of the study was to determine the effect on driving of restricting vision. This was undertaken by comparing the driving performance of young, normal subjects under conditions of simulated visual impairment with a baseline condition. Visual impairment was simulated using goggles designed to replicate the effects of cataracts, binocular visual field restriction, and monocular vision. All subjects had binocular visual acuity greater than 6/12 when wearing the goggles and thus satisfied the visual requirements for a driver's license. Driving performance was assessed on a closed-road circuit for a series of driving tasks including peripheral awareness, maneuvering, reversing, reaction time, speed estimation, road position, and time to complete the course. Simulated cataract resulted in the greatest detriment to driving performance, followed by binocular visual field restriction. The monocular condition did not significantly affect driving performance for any of the driving tasks assessed.
Article
In Sweden the use of daytime running lights (DRL) was made mandatory on 1 October 1977 for all motor vehicles at once, during all seasons and for all areas. According to a study conducted by Andersson and Nilsson (1981) [Andersson and Nilsson. VTI Swedish Road and Transport Research Institute, Report No. 208A; 1981] the introduction of DRL resulted in a reduction of 11% of multiple accidents during daytime. In many discussions on the effectiveness of DRL, these findings have been considered as the strongest evidence that the use of DRL is an effective vehicle collision countermeasure. The present study reexamines this evidence and shows that the reported 11% effect of DRL in the Swedish study is spurious. The effect is mainly the result of the application of a model that shows selective effects of DRL through modelling of unexplained changes in the number of single accidents. It is concluded that the Swedish data fail to show a clear effect of DRL.
Article
The purpose of the study was to: (1) observe the frequency of giving way to pedestrians at the zebra crossing; (2) reveal speed adaptation problems; (3) examine if the time difference between the arrival of the pedestrian and the car have any influence on the speed of the approaching car; (4) identify so-called 'ideal' situations (in which the car brakes on the driver's own initiative in order to give way to a pedestrian) upon which recommendations can be given for implementing means to improve speed behaviour at zebra crossings. It was hypothesized that the speed behaviour of drivers approaching the zebra crossing depends on the pedestrian's arrival at the curb related to the car's expected arrival at the zebra crossing. The speed was measured on randomly selected 'free' passenger cars which approached a non-signalized mid-block zebra crossing on a two-lane arterial road. Every second a radar gun, hidden at the road side, sent the speed data to a lap-top computer in which the observer could also register pedestrians' arrival at, and start from the curb. Simultaneous video recordings were made in order to obtain a more detailed description of the interaction between the car and the pedestrian. Speed behaviour in encounters (148 observations), non-encounters with pedestrian presence (642 observations) and situations without pedestrian presence (690 observations) was compared. Situations with pedestrian priority were classified. The results show that the frequency of giving way is 5%. Drivers do not observe the law concerning speed behaviour at the zebra crossing, as they do not "adapt the speed in such way that they do not endanger pedestrians who are already on, or are about to step onto the zebra crossing". In encounters, three out of four drivers maintain the same speed or accelerate and only one out of four slows down or brakes. These results indicate that maintained high speed (even exceeding the speed limit of 50 km hour-1) is the signal from the drivers that they do not intend to give way to the pedestrian at the zebra crossing. The conclusion is that encounters between cars and pedestrians at the zebra crossing are critical situations in which the driver has to be influenced before he reaches the decision zone at 50 to 40 m before the zebra crossing in order to prevent the 'signalling by speed' behaviour. Countermeasures to improve driver behaviour at the zebra crossing are discussed.