ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The study investigated cross‐cultural differences in variability and average performance in arithmetic, mathematical reasoning, symbolic and non‐symbolic magnitude processing, intelligence, spatial ability, and mathematical anxiety in 890 6‐ to 9‐year‐old children from the United Kingdom, Russia, and China. Cross‐cultural differences explained 28% of the variance in arithmetic and 17.3% of the variance in mathematical reasoning, with Chinese children outperforming the other two groups. No cross‐cultural differences were observed for spatial ability and mathematical anxiety. In all samples, symbolic magnitude processing and mathematical reasoning were independently related to early arithmetic. Other factors, such as non‐symbolic magnitude processing, mental rotation, intelligence, and mathematical anxiety, produced differential patterns across the populations. The results are discussed in relation to potential influences of parental practice, school readiness, and linguistic factors on individual differences in early mathematics. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? • Cross‐cultural differences in mathematical ability are present in preschool children. • Similar mechanisms of mathematical development operate in preschool children from the United Kingdom, Russia, and China. • Tasks that require understanding of numbers are best predictors of arithmetic in preschool children. What does this study add? • Cross‐cultural differences in mathematical ability become greater with age/years of formal education. • Similar mechanisms of mathematical development operate in early primary school children from the United Kingdom, Russia, and China. • Symbolic number magnitude and mathematical reasoning are the main predictors of arithmetic in all three populations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2018), 36, 255–276
©2018 The Authors British Journal of Developmental Psychology published by
John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Psychological Society
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com
Special issue paper
Cognition, emotion, and arithmetic in primary
school: A cross-cultural investigation
Maja Rodic
1,2
, Jiaxin Cui
3
, Sergey Malykh
1,4
, Xinlin Zhou
3
,
Elena I. Gynku
1
, Elena L. Bogdanova
5
, Dina Y. Zueva
1
,
Olga Y. Bogdanova
5
and Yulia Kovas
1,2
*
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Behavioural Genetics, Tomsk State
University, Tomsk, Russia
2
InLab, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, UK
3
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern
Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
4
Psychological Institute, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia
5
Unit of General and Educational Psychology, Psychology Department, Tomsk State
University, Tomsk, Russia
The study investigated cross-cultural differences in variability and average perfor-
mance in arithmetic, mathematical reasoning, symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude
processing, intelligence, spatial ability, and mathematical anxiety in 890 6- to 9-year-
old children from the United Kingdom, Russia, and China. Cross-cultural differences
explained 28% of the variance in arithmetic and 17.3% of the variance in
mathematical reasoning, with Chinese children outperforming the other two groups.
No cross-cultural differences were observed for spatial ability and mathematical
anxiety. In all samples, symbolic magnitude processing and mathematical reasoning
were independently related to early arithmetic. Other factors, such as non-symbolic
magnitude processing, mental rotation, intelligence, and mathematical anxiety,
produced differential patterns across the populations. The results are discussed in
relation to potential influences of parental practice, school readiness, and linguistic
factors on individual differences in early mathematics.
Statement of contribution
What is already known on this subject?
Cross-cultural differences in mathematical ability are present in preschool children.
Similar mechanisms of mathematical development operate in preschool children from the United
Kingdom, Russia, and China.
Tasks that require understanding of numbers are best predictors of arithmetic in preschool
children.
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License, which permits use,
distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited and is not used for commercial purposes.
*Correspondence should be addressed to Yulia Kovas, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, London SE14
6NW, UK (email: y.kovas@gold.ac.uk).
Maja Rodic and Jiaxin Cui contributed equally to this work.
DOI:10.1111/bjdp.12248
255
What does this study add?
Cross-cultural differences in mathematical ability become greater with age/years of formal
education.
Similar mechanisms of mathematical development operate in early primary school children from
the United Kingdom, Russia, and China.
Symbolic number magnitude and mathematical reasoning are the main predictors of arithmetic in all
three populations.
Early mathematics achievement has been found to be important for later mathematical
development (Entwisle & Alexander, 1989). Children who fall behind in mathematics in
early years of education tend to fall even further behind over time, with lower performing
children not being able to keep with the pace in acquiring mathematical information
(Aunola, Leskinen, Lerkkanen, & Nurmi, 2004; Desoete & Gr
egoire, 2006). This effect is
present even after controlling for the influence of the family background, child’s
intelligence, and reading ability (Starkey & Klein, 2007). The opposite pattern the
Matthew effect suggests accumulated advantage, where children who perform well
early on show even bigger advantage over time. Studying mathematical ability cross-
culturally is useful for understanding the sources of individual differences in mathematical
competence within any population, as well as for identification of universal features of
mathematical development.
The average mathematical advantage of children of all ages from AsianPacific
countries has been very well established (Gonzales et al., 2008; Imbo & LeFevre, 2009;
Mullis, Martin, & Foy, 2008; OECD, 2010). This advantage seems to exist even before the
beginning of formal schooling (Huntsinger, Jose, Liaw, & Ching, 1997; Rodic et al., 2015;
Stevenson & Stigler, 1992) and is present throughout the school years (Geary, Bow-
Thomas, Fan, & Siegler, 1993; Leung, 2006; Miura, 1987).
Explanations for the observed cross-cultural differences in mathematics performance
include linguistic factors, such as transparency of a number system and speed of
pronunciation of numbers (Dehaene, 1997); parental support (Chao, 2001; Huntsinger
et al., 1997); educational systems (Stevenson, Chen, & Lee, 1993); cultural beliefs
(Campbell & Xue, 2001); school readiness (Miller, Kelly, & Zhou, 2005); and genetic
differences among populations. Our previous research suggested that socio-demographic
and linguistic factors contribute to cross-cultural differences in performance of
preschoolers (Rodic et al., 2015).
Cross-cultural differences in non-cognitive domains have also been found (e.g., Lee,
2009). Typically, students from Asian countries show a lower level of self-efficacy and self-
concept and higher levels of mathematical anxiety than participants from Western
countries (Lee, 2009).
In addition to exploring cross-cultural differences in variation and average levels
of performance, it is important to understand whether differences exist in
interrelationships among the different domains across the populations. In particular,
it is necessary to explore whether different aspects of cognition, motivation, and
emotion regulation (such as maths anxiety) are equally strongly related to
mathematical skills in different populations and at different ages. Previous research
suggested high degree of similarity across cultures in these interrelationships
showing that, for example, symbolic magnitude processing task is related to early
arithmetic in the UK, Russian, Chinese, Kyrgyz, and Dungan 5- to 7-year-old children
(Rodic et al., 2015).
256 Maja Rodic et al.
Overall, a wealth of research in different populations shows that ability to process
numerical magnitudes of symbolic numbers is positively related to development of
arithmetic skills and mathematics achievement (De Smedt, Verschaffel, & Ghesquiere,
2009; Gilmore, McCarthy, & Spelke, 2010; Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Kolkman,
Kroesbergen, & Leseman, 2013; Zhang, Chen, Liu, Cui, & Zhou, 2016). This relationship
may be stronger in older children, as practice with mathematical operations and exposure
to numbers increases with age (Clements & Sarama, 2007; Dehaene, 1997). However,
culture-specific influences may increase or decrease the magnitude of the interrelation-
ships over time. For example, previous research has suggested cross-cultural differences
in associations between maths performance and numerical factors in preschool children
(Rodic et al., 2015). It is necessary to replicate these differences and to investigate
whether they remain significant in children at the beginning of formal maths education.
Not all previous research has been consistent in finding substantial positive
associations between mathematical skills and basic magnitude processing. For example,
number comparison performance in 9-year-old children showed only a modest associ-
ation with mathematical skills (Sz}
ucs, Devine, Soltesz, Nobes, & Gabriel, 2014). Another
study has not found significant relationships between performance on single-digit number
comparison and number reasoning, arithmetic learning, and advanced mathematics in
adults (Wei, Yuan, Chen, & Zhou, 2012). Therefore, further research is needed to identify
the sources of these inconsistencies.
Finally, intelligence and other cognitive factors typically explain no more than half of
the variability in mathematical variation. The other half of the variance is related to other
factors, including motivation, emotion, and health (Krapohl et al., 2014). Of these factors,
mathematical anxiety has consistently been negatively associated with mathematics
performance (Ashcraft, Krause, & Hopko, 2007; Eden, Heine, & Jacobs, 2013; Hembree,
1990).
The nature of the association between mathematical anxiety and performance remains
unclear. Some studies suggest that poor performance and failure lead to mathematical
anxiety (Ashcraft et al., 2007; Cipora, Szczygieł, Willmes, & Nuerk, 2015; Ma & Xu, 2004;
Maloney, Risko, Ansari, & Fugelsang, 2010), which is particularly evident in adolescence
and adulthood (Jameson, 2013). The relationship may also be moderated by motivation. A
linear relationship between mathematical anxiety and performance has been found in
individuals with low maths motivation. A curvilinear relationship was observed in highly
motivated individuals: Low and high levels of maths anxiety were associated with poor
performance and intermediate levels of maths anxiety with optimal mathematics
performance (Wang et al., 2014).
The direction of causation can also be reversed, so that greater mathematical anxiety
contributes to lower mathematics performance (e.g., Chinn, 2009; and also see reviews of
Carey, Hill, Devine, & Sz}
ucs, 2016; Dowker, Sarkar, & Looi, 2016). This could be due to a
direct effect of mathematical anxiety on mathematics-related cognitive abilities, such as
working memory (see reviews of Carey et al., 2016; Dowker et al., 2016). Moreover,
some evidence points to the bidirectional relationship between mathematical anxiety and
mathematics performance, whereby mathematical anxiety and mathematics performance
influence each other (Carey et al., 2016).
This study aimed to investigate:
(1) Whether average and variance differences exist between the UK, Russian, and
Chinese 6- to 9-year-old children in mathematically relevant traits.
Previous studies have investigated cross-cultural differences in a number of mathe-
matical abilities, including counting (e.g., Fuson & Kwon, 1992; Mark & Dowker, 2015;
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 257
Miller & Stigler, 1987; Song & Ginsburg, 1988; Xenidou-Dervou, Gilmore, van der Schoot,
& van Lieshout, 2015); numerical characteristics (e.g., G
obel, Maier, & Shaki, 2015); place-
value understanding (e.g., Mark & Dowker, 2015; Miller & Stigler, 1987; Miura, Kim,
Chang, & Okamoto, 1988); number line estimation (e.g., Helmreich et al., 2011; Siegler &
Mu, 2008), digit span (e.g., Chen, Cowell, Varley, & Wang, 2009; Yang et al., 2012);
approximate number sense (e.g., Rodic et al., 2015); simple and complex arithmetic (e.g.,
Dowker, Bala, & Lloyd, 2008; Gatobu, Arocha, & Hoffman- Goetz, 2014; Geary et al., 1993;
Laski & Yu, 2014; Robinson & Beatch, 2016; Rodic et al., 2015; Shen, Vasilyeva, & Laski,
2016; Stevenson et al., 1990; Vasilyeva, Laski, & Shen, 2015; Xenidou-Dervou et al., 2015;
Zhou, Peverly, & Lin, 2005); word problems (e.g., Stevenson et al., 1990); overall
mathematical ability, including PISA and TIMSS assessments (e.g., Caro, Lenkeit, &
Kyriakides, 2016; He, Buchholz, & Klieme, 2017; Jak, 2017; Lowrie, Logan, & Ramful,
2016; Min, Cortina, & Miller, 2016; Pitchford & Outhwaite, 2016; Ryoo et al., 2014;
Schachner, He, Heizmann, & Van de Vijver, 2017; Stevenson, Lee, & Stigler, 1986; Weis,
Trommsdorff, & Mu~
noz, 2016; Zhou et al., 2005); and maths anxiety (e.g., Ahn, Usher,
Butz, & Bong, 2016).
However, previous research is not sufficient in the following three areas: First,
differences in symbolic magnitude comparison and maths reasoning have not been
investigated, with the exception of one study that focused on differences in two-digit
number comparison of Welsh and English children (Dowker et al., 2008). The advantage
of Welsh children in two-digit number comparison found in the study might stem from
irregular number system of English language.
Second, the majority of previous studies focused on cross-cultur al differences between
children from East Asia and the United States, between children from East Asia and
Western Europe, or children from two countries in Western Europe. Very few studies
examined differences in children’s ability across Russia, China, and Western Europe
(Rodic et al., 2015; Shen et al., 2016). These comparisons are informative, considering
the differences in cultural and educational practices across these populations.
Third, the cross-cultural differences in maths anxiety demonstrated in previous studies
may have come from economic differences and consequent educational differences (e.g.,
the Philippines vs. the United States in Ahn et al., 2016). Cross-cultural research is needed
to investigate potential differences in maths anxiety in similar economic circumstances.
(2) Whether cross-cultural differences exist in the interrelationships between
arithmetic skills and mathematically relevant traits in 6- to 9-year-old children.
Several studies have found a longitudinal link between non-symbolic magnitude
estimation and mathematical skills (Halberda & Feigenson, 2008; Mazzocco, Feigenson, &
Halberda, 2011), while other studies have not found this link (e.g., De Smedt & Gilmore,
2011; Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Rodic et al., 2015; Solt
esz, Sz}
ucs, & Sz}
ucs, 2010). Further
research is needed to evaluate whether the inconsistencies in the literature result from
publication bias, when evidence for the absence of the associations is not reported;
research limitations, such as small sample sizes; and differences in sample characteristics,
such as age or cultural background of participants. Differences in measures may also
contribute to the observed inconsistencies. For example, it has been found that non-
symbolic magnitude processing is related to arithmetic fluency rather than mathematical
problem-solving (Cui, Zhang, Cheng, Li, & Zhou, 2017; Zhang et al., 2016; Zhou, Wei,
Zhang, Cui, & Chen, 2015). In addition, as described above, previous studies mostly
focused on cross-cultural differences in different aspects of mathematical ability, rather
than on interrelationships between arithmetic skills and mathematically relevant
cognitive abilities. One study has investigated the interrelationships between arithmetic
258 Maja Rodic et al.
skills and symbolic and non-symbolic numerical processing in 5- to 7-year-old preschool
children from the United Kingdom, Russia, China, and Kyrgyzstan. The study found that
arithmetic skills relied on symbolic numerical processing but not non-symbolic numerical
processing (Rodic et al., 2015). The relationship between arithmetic and symbolic
numerical processing was also found in third- to sixth-grade pupils in China (Wei, Lu,
et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2016). Several previous studies investigated cross-cultural
differences in relationships between mathematics achievement and motivation, such as
maths self-concept and maths self-efficacy (e.g., Marsh, 2016). However, little research is
available on the cross-cultural differences in the links between maths ability and maths
anxiety. This study investigates the interrelationships between arithmetic skills, numer-
ical processing (both symbolic and non-symbolic), mathematical reasoning, and maths
anxiety.
The study tests two main hypotheses:
(1) There are cross-cultural differences in mathematics performance among the UK,
Russian, and Chinese 6- to 9-year-old children. Specifically, Chinese children will
outperform UK and Russian peers in symbolic numerical comparison and mathematical
reasoning. The expected differences might stem from differences in regularity of linguistic
number system, as well as advantage of Chinese children in counting abilities and concept
understanding, such as place-value understanding and number line estimation.
(2) Interrelationships between arithmetic skills and mathematically relevant traits are
similar for children in primary schools of the three countries, consistent with previous
findings (Rodic et al., 2015; Wei, Lu, et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2016).
Methods
Participants
The participants were recruited from primary schools in the United Kingdom, Russia, and
China. Children in Russia start their primary education a year later (at 7 years of age) than
children in the United Kingdom and China. To match the groups on chronological age,
Russian participants were in the second year of primary education, whereas the UK and
Chinese participants were in the third year.
The sample consisted of 890 6- to 9-year-old children from the United Kingdom, Russia,
and China. There were 73 UK participants (34 boys; mean age =97.56 months, range
80105 months); 421 Russian participants (232 boys; mean age =105.41, range =88
112 months); and 396 Chinese participants (221 boys; mean age =104.27, range =95
110 months). The children in the UK sample were recruited from five State schools in
London Greater area. The children in the Russian sample were recruited from 15 State
schools. The children in the Chinese sample came from 12 State schools. None of the
schools operated any special intake selection.
Measures and procedure
A total of eight tests were computerized using Web-based applications in the ‘Online
Psychological Experimental System (OPES)’ (www.dweipsy.com/lattice) (e.g., Cui,
Georgiou, et al., 2017; Cui, Zhang, et al., 2017; Rodic et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2016;
Zhou et al., 2015). Children completed an online battery of tests in their schools,
supervised by a researcher. The tasks (see Figure 1) were administered in the following
order: Mental rotation, Choice reaction time, Non-symbolic comparison of numerosity,
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 259
Symbolic number magnitude comparison, Simple subtraction, Number series comple-
tion, Raven’s progressive matrices, and Mathematical anxiety questionnaire. Children
were offered time to rest between the last two tasks. Mathematical anxiety questionnaire
was completed last and followed general cognitive ability (rather than numerical) test, to
minimize potential effects of prior tests on mathematical anxiety.
Children responded by pressing keys ‘Q’ or ‘P’ (and corresponding computer keys in
Russian and Chinese), which were marked with colourful stickers. Responses on the
Mathematical anxiety questionnaire were recorded by a researcher. Accuracy (ACC) and RT
(in milliseconds) were recorded for Choice reaction time, Non-symbolic comparison of
numerosity, and Symbolic number magnitude comparison tasks. For the Mathematical
anxietyquestionnaire, the total score was calculated by adding up the responses on a 5-point
Likert scale for the whole questionnaire. For the rest of the tasks (i.e., Mental rotation, Simple
subtraction, Number series completion, and Raven’s progressive matrices), the score was
calculated by subtracting incorrect from correct responses, to correct for guessing. The tasks
were grouped in the following categories: (1) general skills and IQ; (2) spatial ability; (3)
symbolic number understanding; (4) non-symbolic estimation; (5) operating with numbers
(arithmetic) and numerical reasoning; and (6) mathematical anxiety.
The study and consent procedure were approved by the Ethics Committee of
Goldsmiths, University of London.
General skills and IQ
The Choice reaction time task, adapted from the Dyscalculia screener (Butterworth,
2003), was used to measure the processing speed. A dot appeared at the 30 degree angle
on the left (15 trials) or right (15 trials) side of the fixation ‘+’, with interstimulus interval
between 1,500 and 3,000 ms. Children’s accuracy (pressing the relevant button) and
speed of responses were recorded. This task can be considered as a ‘baseline task’,
controlling for influence of individual differences in processing speed (time associated
1. Mental rotation
2. Choice reaction time task (baseline) 3. Non-symbolic co mparison of numerosity 4.Symbolic nu mber magnitude comparison
5. Simple subtraction 6. Number series completion 7. Raven’s progressive matrices
Figure 1. Illustration of tasks used in the study, in the order of presentation.
260 Maja Rodic et al.
with pressing buttons), allowing for evaluation of pure processing time for other cognitive
and mathematical processing (e.g., Butterworth, 2003, see page 13, using a simple
reaction time task as baseline).
The Raven’s progressive matrices task was adapted from the legal copy of Raven’s
Progressive Matrices and computerized on the OPES (Raven, Raven, & Court, 1998). The
task measures abstract reasoning and serves as an index of g. Children were presented
with an incomplete figure with six segments underneath it. The child had to identify the
correct segment to complete the figure’s intrinsically regular pattern. Children had to go
through as many trials as they could in 4 min. The test was shortened to 80 items,
including 44 items from the Standard Progressive Matrices (12 items from the first set and
eight items from each of the other four sets) and 36 items from the Advanced Progressive
Matrices. The shortened version has been used in previous studies to test general IQ or
abstract reasoning (e.g., Bors & Vigneau, 2003; Bouma, Mulder, & Lindeboom, 1996;
Vigneau & Bors, 2001; Vigneau, Caissie, & Bors, 2006; Wang, Sun, & Zhou, 2016; Wei, Lu,
et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2016; Zhou et al., 2015). The short time limit of 4 min was
based on previous research with children that demonstrated that having longer time did
not result in greater proportion of correct responses. For example, in one study, the
number of unanswered items was unrelated to APM (Raven’s Advanced Progressive
Matrices) score, with improvements in performance not based on a strategy to respond to
more items (Bors & Vigneau, 2003). This adaptation of the Raven Progressive Matrices has
acceptable split-half reliability according to previous studies (.84 see Cui, Zhang, et al.,
2017; and .83 see Wei, Yuan, et al., 2012).
Adjusted number of correct trials was used to control for the effect of guessing in
multiple choice tests (e.g., Cirino, 2011; Hedden & Yoon, 2006; Salthouse, 1994;
Salthouse & Meinz, 1995). The score was calculated by subtracting the number of
incorrect responses from the number of correct responses, following the Guilford
correction formula ‘S =R-W/(n-1)’ (S: the adjusted number of items that the participants
can actually perform without the aid of chance; R: the number of correct responses; W:
the number of wrong responses; and n: the number of alternative responses to each item)
(Guilford & Guilford, 1936). This correction procedure has been utilized recently in
studies of mathematical cognition (Cirino, 2011; Cui, Zhang, et al., 2017; Zhou et al.,
2015) and cognition in general (Hedden & Yoon, 2006; Putz, Gaulin, Sporter, &
McBurney, 2004; Salthouse, 1994). In this study, the number of alternative answers is 2;
therefore, (n-1) =1. Consequently, the scores ranged from 80 to 80.
Spatial ability
The Mental rotation task, adapted from Shepard and Metzler (1971), was used to assess
children’s ability to mentally rotate three-dimensional images. One (target) image was
presented at the top of the screen with two potential answers on the left and right bottom
parts of the screen. Children had to select the image that was matching the top one. The
matching images were rotated from 15°to 345°. Children had to go through as many trials
(max 180) as they could in 3 min.
Symbolic number understanding
The Symbolic number magnitude comparison task, adapted from Girelli, Lucangeli, and
Butterworth (2000), used a Stroop-like paradigm. The task assessed the ability to compare
numerical values of numbers that varied in physical size (1:2 size ratio). Children had
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 261
5 seconds to judge which of the two single-digit numbers, appearing simultaneously on
the screen, was larger in numerical magnitude, ignoring the differences in physical size.
There were three types of trials: neutral (both digits were of the same physical size),
congruent (a numerically larger digit was also physically larger), and incongruent
(numerically smaller digit was physically larger). There were three sessions of 28 trials,
separated by 10-second resting periods.
Non-symbolic estimation
The Non-symbolic comparison of numerosity, adapted from Baroody and Ginsburg
(1990), was used to measure non-symbolic number sense. Two sets of dots of varying sizes
(512 dots; ratios 2:3, 5:7, and 3:4) were presented simultaneously on the screen.
Children were asked to estimate (without counting) which of the two sets contained more
dots. There were 36 trials in total. As proposed by Gebuis and Reynvoet (2011, 2012), the
following five types of visual stimulus parameters were controlled in the test: total surface
area, envelope area or convex hull, item size, density (envelope area divided by total
surface), and circumference. Even after considering the five visual properties, the
accuracy across all trials of this test was still ratio-dependent, r(113) =0.31, p<0.001
(see Zhou et al., 2015).
Operating with numbers
The Simple subtraction task, developed according to the theory in previous work
(Landerl, Bevan, & Butterworth, 2004; Girelli et al., 2000), assessed early arithmetic
ability. The problem was presented in the middle of the screen with two candidate
answers beneath it. Children had to choose the correct answer in as many problems as
they could (max 92) in 2 min. All minuends were smaller than 18, and all differences were
single-digit numbers. Correct and incorrect answers were within the range of each other
plus or minus 3.
The Number series completion task, adapted from Smith, Fernandes, and Strand
(2001), measured logical numerical reasoning. Children completed the sequence of
numbers presented on the screen (e.g., 2, 4, 6, 8), by choosing one of the two candidate
answers (e.g., 9 or 10) presented below it. Children had to go through as many trials (max
43) as they could in 4 min.
Maths anxiety
The Revised Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale (RMARS; Alexander & Martray, 1989)
included 25 items, such as ‘How would you feel having math as part of a test’. Each
statement was read out, and children reported (on the scale of 1 =not at all to 5 =very
strongly) how anxious they would feel in these situations. The full list of items in English is
presented in Table S1, together with information on adaptation for administration in
Russian and Chinese.
Results
Descriptive statistics on raw data for all tasks can be seen in Table S2. The data were age-
regressed for further analyses to eliminate any effects of chronological age. Table S3
262 Maja Rodic et al.
presents the results of the sex differences analyses, showing non-significant or negligible
effects of sex.
Cross-cultural comparisons
Table S4 presents the results of ANOVAs and Bonferroni post-hoc analyses conducted for
all measures on uncorrected scores. All tests exhibited significant cross-cultural
differences. UK children on average spent more time and achieved higher accuracy,
while Chinese children tended to use less time leading to a lower accuracy in non-time-
limited tests (choice reaction time, symbolic magnitude comparison, and non-symbolic
comparison of numerosity).
We have calculated the speedaccuracy trade-off effect for the symbolic and non-
symbolic magnitude comparison tasks, using an inverse efficiency measure IES
(Townsend & Ashby, 1978; e.g., Pouw, Van, Zwaan, & Paas, 2016; Setti, Borghi, & Tessari,
2009). IES is estimated as reaction time divided by proportion correct responses, with
higher scores indicating using more time to obtain a higher proportion of correct
responses. The IES scores for the UK, Russian, and Chinese samples were 11.96, 10.85,
and 8.66 for symbolic magnitude comparison; and 12.82, 12.44, and 10.63 for non-
symbolic numerosity comparison, respectively.
11a 2 2a
33a 4 5
67
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Mean RT
Sample
Choice reacon me
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Accuracy
Sample
Number magnitude
comparison
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Mean RT
Sample
Comparison of
numerosity
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Correct–incorrect
responses
Sample
Simple subtracon
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Correct
–incorrect
responses
Sample
Number series
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Accuracy
Sample
Comparison of
numerosity
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Accuracy
Sample
Choice reacon me
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Mean RT
Sample
Number magnitude
comparison
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Correct–incorrect
responses
Sample
Mental rotaon
–2
–1
0
1
2
UK Russ Chi
Total score
Sample
Maths anxiety
Figure 2. Mean accuracy, mean RTs, and mean correctincorrect responses with standard error bars
(1SD) for the tasks where significant differences emerged between samples: (1) Chinese >UK, Russian;
(1a) Russian >Chinese; (2) Chinese >UK, Russian; (2a) UK >Chinese, and Russian >Chinese; (3)
Chinese >UK, Russian; (3a) UK >Russian, Chinese, and Russian >Chinese; (4) Chinese >UK,
Russian; (5) Chinese >UK, Russian, and UK >Russian; (6) Chinese, Russian >UK; and (7) Rus-
sian >UK, Chinese.
Note: All data were age- and IQ-regressed and cleaned for outliers (3SD).
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 263
ANOVAs and Bonferroni post-hoc analyses were conducted for all measures on scores
corrected for raw scores of Raven’s progressive matrices (to control for chronological age
and grade differences). The majority of tasks showed significant group differences in
accuracy and/or RT (see Figure 2), with modest effect sizes, except performance on
Mental rotation (spatial ability) and Mathematical anxiety.
There were significant differences among groups (all p<.001) in Choice reaction
time, Symbolic number magnitude comparison, and Non-symbolic comparison of
numerosity. Chinese children had faster reaction time and slightly lower accuracy than
Russian/UK children (all p<.05).
Significant differences were also found among all groups (all p<.001) on Simple
subtraction and Number series completion among groups, with Chinese children
outperforming the UK and Russian children. That is, with limited time available for the
two tasks, Chinese children obtained significantly higher adjusted correct scores (correct
minus incorrect trials) than the UK and Russian children. The advantage of Chinese
children in these tasks was present for scores both uncorrected (Table S4) and corrected
for Raven’s progressive matrices (Figure 2) and Mental rotation (Table S5). This suggests
that this advantage cannot be explained by differences in gor spatial ability across the
samples.
The two tasks that did not lead to differences across samples showed normal
distributions. For Mental rotation, the values of skewness and kurtosis were as follows: for
all samples combined, .362 and .329, respectively; for the UK sample, .008 and .338;
for the Russian sample, .376 and .162; and for the Chinese sample, .193 and .091. For
mathematical anxiety task, skewness and kurtosis were as follows: for all samples
combined, .191 and .497; for the UK sample, .185 and .751; for the Russian sample,
.258 and .484; and for the Chinese sample, .103 and .696.
ANOVAs and Bonferroni post-hoc analyses were also conducted for all measures on
scores corrected for spatial ability, as measured by Mental rotation. Most of the tasks
showed significant group differences in accuracy and/or RT (see Table S5). The effect of
Mental rotation on other measures is relatively small and did not change the significance of
cross-cultural differences observed in the raw (uncorrected) data.
Regression analyses
A three-step hierarchical multiple regression was conducted separately in each sample
with Simple subtraction as the criterion. Table 1 summarizes the variables included in the
three steps and the results of the regression.
All three samples exhibited a significant role of general cognitive processing (Choice
reaction time, Mental rotation, and Raven’s progressive matrices), basic numerical
processing (Symbolic number magnitude comparison and Non-symbolic comparison of
numerosity), Number series reasoning, and gender in variation of Simple subtraction
(except only a marginal significance of general cognitive processing in the UK sample).
Next, we grouped all samples together and repeated the same analysis, with Sample as
an additional predictor. Together, RT and ACC of the Choice reaction time task, and
correct - incorrect responses for Mental rotation and Raven’s progressive matrices
explained 25.2% of the variance in Simple subtraction. RT and ACC of the Symbolic
number magnitude comparison task and the Non-symbolic comparison of numerosity
task explained additional 13.3% of the variance. Number series completion (correct -
incorrect responses) and Gender explained 11.4% of the variance. Finally, Sample
explained additional 8.5% of the variance in Simple subtraction.
264 Maja Rodic et al.
Table 1. Significant predictors of hierarchical multiple linear regression
Sample Significant predictors BSEb95% confidence interval FR
2
at each step
UK
N=65
Step 1 0.942 0.056
Choice reaction time (RT) 0.004 0.009 0.022 0.013
Choice reaction time (acc.) 60.261 36.267 12.214 132.736
Mental rotation 0.080 0.109 0.138 0.298
Raven’s 0.076 0.299 0.522 0.673
Step 2 4.532 0.222**
Symb. magnitude (RT) 0.013* 0.005 0.023 0.003
Symb. magnitude (acc.) 46.691 24.461 2.255 95.638
Non-symb. comparison (RT) 0.006 0.005 0.004 0.015
Non-symb. comparison (acc.) 31.861* 13.857 4.133 59.588
Step 3 5.808 0.122**
Number series 0.659*** 0.195 0.27 1.049
Gender 0.299 2.054 3.814 4.412
Russian
N=288
Step 1 11.566 0.141***
Choice reaction time (RT) 0.006 0.003 0.012 0.001
Choice reaction time (acc.) 16.533 8.694 0.581 33.647
Mental rotation 0.128** 0.048 0.034 0.222
Raven’s 0.566** 0.137 0.296 0.837
Step 2 10.175 0.109***
Symb. magnitude (RT) 0.009** 0.003 0.014 0.003
Symb. magnitude (acc.) 37.546*** 6.993 23.782 51.311
Non-symb. comparison (RT) 0.001 0.003 0.006 0.004
Non-symb. comparison (acc.) 6.824 5.089 3.194 16.841
Step 3 10.274 0.052***
Number series 0.303*** 0.074 0.157 0.448
Gender 1.540 1.010 3.529 0.448
Continued
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 265
Table 1. (Continued)
Sample Significant predictors BSEb95% confidence interval FR
2
at each step
Chinese
N=396
Step 1 21.627 0.175***
Choice reaction time (RT) 0.009** 0.003 0.016 0.003
Choice reaction time (acc.) 4.398 4.530 4.508 13.304
Mental rotation 0.243*** 0.055 0.136 0.351
Raven’s 0.357*** 0.060 0.240 0.474
Step 2 18.522 0.128***
Symb. magnitude (RT) 0.008* 0.003 0.014 0.002
Symb. magnitude (acc.) 28.290*** 6.855 14.815 41.765
Non-symb. comparison (RT) 0.000 0.002 0.004 0.005
Non-symb. comparison (acc.) 24.322*** 4.325 15.819 32.825
Step 3 14.588 0.047***
Number series 0.464*** 0.088 0.291 0.636
Gender 0.622 0.704 0.761 2.005
All samples
grouped
together
N=749
Step 1 64.494 0.252***
Choice reaction time (RT) 0.018*** 0.003 0.023 0.013
Choice reaction time (acc.) 0.155* 4.918 9.499 9.810
Mental rotation 0.181** 0.040 0.102 0.260
Raven’s 0.674** 0.064 0.548 0.800
Continued
266 Maja Rodic et al.
Table 1. (Continued)
Sample Significant predictors BSEb95% confidence interval FR
2
at each step
Step 2 41.151 0.133***
Symb. magnitude (RT) 0.022*** 0.002 0.026 0.018
Symb. magnitude (acc.) 38.076*** 5.54 27.226 48.927
Non-symb. comparison (RT) 7.889* 3.65 0.714 15.065
Non-symb. comparison (acc.) 0.003 0.002 0.000 0.003
Step 3 86.299 0.114***
Number series 0.670*** 0.051 0.569 0.771
Gender 0.411 0.611 1.610 0.788
Step 4 155.092 0.085***
Sample 6.813*** 0.547 5.739 7.887
Notes. The Choice reaction time (RT and accuracy), Mental rotation (correct - incorrect responses), Raven’s (correct - incorrect responses) were entered at step 1;
RT and accuracy of both Symbolic number magnitude comparison and Non-symbolic comparison of numerosity were entered at step 2, Number series (correct-
incorrect responses) and gender were entered at step 3, and Sample (i.e., nation) was entered at step 4; for Magnitude comparison task the trials were collapsed
together as no congruency effects were found (high performance on average in all trials); for Non-symbolic comparison of numerosity, the trials were collapsed
together as no ratio effects were found (high performance on average in all trials); the results for congruency and ratio analyses are available from the corresponding
author. The bold values indicate significant change in R
2
in each step.
*p<.05; **<.01; ***<.001. Only participants with complete data for all variables were included in this analysis.
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 267
Discussion
The first aim of this study was to investigate average and variance differences between the
UK, Russian, and Chinese early primary school children in arithmetic, related cognitive
skills, and mathematical anxiety.
Sample explained 8.5% of the variance of the speed in which the children performed
the baseline task (i.e., Choice reaction time task, which eliminates influence of individual
differences in processing speed to see the pure processing time of other general cognitive
processing and mathematical processing rather than the time to press buttons), with
Chinese children showing faster performance than the UK and Russian children. Accuracy
on this task, which was approaching ceiling (~95% for all children), produced small (1.8%)
differences.
The measure that required good understanding of the numerical value of numerals and
the capacity to order numerosities by their size also showed small (4.3%) differences in
accuracy. The accuracy on this task was high overall, with all children scoring in the range of
90%. For theRT measure of this task,21% of the variance wasexplained by Sample, indicating
significantly faster performance by the Chinese children over the other two samples.
For the non-symbolic magnitude processing task, for both accuracy and RT, ~6% of the
variance was explained by the Sample. Chinese children performed faster than the
remaining two samples. Once again, the accuracy was high (~90%). Chinese children
performed significantly less accurate than both the UK and Russian children according to
our results of speedaccuracy trade-off effect.
Overall, the RT measure seems to better discriminate children’s performance at this
stage (see Butterworth, 2003). Processing speed is an important measure, particularly for
basic overlearned mathematical abilities, and has previously been found to predict
mathematics performance in 7-year-old children (Bull & Johnston, 1997). Some studies
also suggest that slow reaction time on tasks, such as number identification, visual number
matching, magnitude comparison, and digit encoding, can be used to identify children
with mathematical difficulties (Geary et al., 1993).
For Number series completion, a numerical reasoning task, 17.3% of the variance was
explained by the Sample, with Chinese children showing the best performance.
For the Simple subtraction task, 28% of the variance was explained by the Sample. In
line with the previous research with both preschool (Rodic et al., 2015) and older
children (Imbo & LeFevre, 2009; Mullis et al., 2008; OECD, 2010), Chinese 6- to 9-year-
olds outperformed both the UK and Russian samples.
The Mental rotation task did not show any cross-cultural differences in this study. It is
possible that visuospatial advantage of Asian adults (Sakamoto & Spiers, 2014), attributed
to a potential impact of spatially complex character-based writing system on the
development of spatial ability (Flaherty & Connolly, 1995), is present only in more
advanced users of character-based writing systems. Chinese children in our sample might
not have been exposed to the spatial complexity of the character-based writing system
long enough to lead to the advantage on spatial ability tests.
The levels of Mathematical anxiety did not differ significantly between the samples. It
is also possible that mathematical anxiety is not yet pronounced at this age (Jameson,
2013). However, several previous studies, using different measures, have shown that MA
was present in younger primary students (Harari, Vukovic, & Bailey, 2013; Krinzinger,
Kaufmann, & Willmes, 2009; Ramirez, Gunderson, Levine, & Beilock, 2013). It is possible
that the self-report measure used in this study is not precise in measuring individual
differences in mathematical anxiety in young children. In particular, some children might
268 Maja Rodic et al.
not have been familiar with some of the items. The overall variability in the scores was
quite small, and the average level of anxiety was quite low. The relatively low MA scores
might be related to higher scores in subtraction task in the current study compared to
peers (e.g., lower scores were found in Zhang et al., 2016 and Zhou et al., 2015, using the
same subtraction task). Better performance may be associated with lower anxiety through
unilateral or reciprocal causal links (e.g., better performance leading to greater
confidence). Alternatively, the link may reflect other factors, such as covering simpler
material in previous lessons or teachers’ characteristics.
In line with previous research with participants of different ages (Geary et al., 1993;
Leung, 2006; Rodic et al., 2015; Song & Ginsburg, 1988), Chinese 6- to 9-year-old children
outperformed the other two samples on both simple arithmetic and mathematical reasoning
tasks, with moderate effect sizes (1728%). In comparison with the Rodic et al. (2015) study,
where Chinese preschool children showed advantage with 13% effect size, the current study
indicates the increaseof this effect (28%), suggesting that the advantage might be increasing
with age and/or formal instruction. This finding suggests that in addition to linguistic
advantage of Chinese children (Dehaene, 1997) and extra time Chinese parents spend
teaching and practising with their children before they reach school (Huntsinger, Jose,
Larson, Balsink Krieg, & Shaligram, 2000), the Chinese formal educational system might
provide additional advantage at the early primary school ages. For example, some research
suggeststhatparentsinChinaarebetterinformedbytheschoolsofwhatisexpectedoftheir
children and are thus able to provide more adequate help (Miller et al., 2005). In addition,
mothers of first graders in China show increased involvement as their children start formal
education, in comparison with US mothers (Chao, 2001).
The observed overall faster performance of Chinese children on measures of symbolic
and non-symbolic magnitude comparison may indicate more in-depth knowledge of
magnitudes and Arabic numerals, which in turn could also lead to better performance in
arithmetic problem-solving.
Overall, the results suggest that culture has an effect on performance. For example, the
results of the multiple regression on all three samples combined showed that Sample
explained 8.5% of the variance in Simple subtraction, additional to the effects of Raven’s
progressive matrices, Mental rotation, and other abilities. The actual effects of culture are
likely to be even greater as some effects may be ‘removed’ when controlling for other
abilities on which samples also differed.
The second aim of this study was to investigate whether cognitive skills that predict
arithmetic in early primary school differ between the three populations. Understanding
numbers and magnitudes, as well as mathematical reasoning, predicted arithmetic in all
three samples. This is in line with the previous research which shows that the ability to
process numerical magnitudes of symbolic numbers is important for the development of
arithmetic skills and is positively related to mathematics achievement (Booth & Siegler,
2006, 2008; Castronovo & G
obel, 2012; De Smedt et al., 2009; Gilmore et al., 2010;
Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Jordan, Kaplan, Ramineni, & Locuniak, 2009). In line with
previous studies, Non-symbolic magnitude comparison task predicted arithmetic in the
UK and Chinese samples (Libertus, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011; Mazzocco et al., 2011).
It is possible that shorter period of instruction in the Russian sample (second vs third
grade) led to an absence of this association.
In line with previous research (Gottfredson & Deary, 2004; Rohde & Thompson, 2007;
Strenberg, Grigorenko, & Bundy, 2001), Mental rotation and Raven’s progressive matrices
predicted arithmetic in the Russian and Chinese samples. The absence of the effect in the
UK sample could be due to a reduced power of a small sample size.
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 269
Conclusion and future directions
In line with previous research, Chinese children outperformed the other samples on early
arithmetic and early mathematical reasoning. No significant differences in maths anxiety
have been found. Despite the cross-cultural differences in performance, similar
mechanisms of mathematical development seem to be operating in all three populations.
The cross-sample comparisons should be interpreted in the light of potential sample
differences (age, grade, and variance differences). Further longitudinal research, with
increased sample sizes, additional populations, and assessed cultural/educational
features, is needed to clarify the sources of the observed differences.
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge the support from the British Academy, the Royal Society,
the Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning,
Beijing Normal University; and the Grant No. 16-36-01102 from the Russian Foundation for
Basic Research.
References
Ahn, H. S., Usher, E. L., Butz, A., & Bong, M. (2016). Cultural differences in the understanding of
modelling and feedback as sources of self-efficacy information. British Journal of Educational
Psychology,86, 112136. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12093
Alexander, L., & Martray, C. (1989). The development of an abbreviated version of the Mathematics
Anxiety Rating Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development,22, 143150.
https://doi.org/10.1037/t22970-000
Ashcraft, M. H., Krause, J. A., & Hopko, D. R. (2007). Is math anxiety a mathematical learning
disability? In D. B. Berch & M. M. M. Mazzocco (Eds.), Why is math so hard for some children?
(pp. 329348). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Lerkkanen, M. K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2004). Developmental dynamics of math
performance from preschool to grade 2. Journal of Educational Psychology,96, 699713.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.699
Baroody, A. J., & Ginsburg, H. P. (1990). Children’s mathematical learning: A cognitive view. In R. B.
Davis, C. A. Maher & N. Noddings (Eds.), Constructivist views on teaching and learning
mathematics,Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Monograph (pp. 7990).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teaching of Mathematics.
Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical
estimation. Developmental Psychology,41, 189201. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.189
Booth, L., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning.
Child Development,79,10161031. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01173.x
Bors, D. A., & Vigneau, F. (2003). The effect of practice on Raven’s advanced progressive matrices.
Learning and Individual Differences,13, 291312. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(03)
00015-3
Bouma, J. M., Mulder, J. L., & Lindeboom, J. (1996). Neuropsychologische diagnostiek: Handboek.
[Neuropsychological assessment: Manual]. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). Children’s arithmetic difficulties: Contributions from processing
speed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
65,124. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.2358
Butterworth, B. (2003). Dyscalculia screener. Gosport, UK: Ashford Colour Press.
Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetics across cultures. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General,130, 299315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.299
270 Maja Rodic et al.
Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Sz}
ucs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the
relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in
Psychology,6, 1987. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987
Caro, D. H., Lenkeit, J., & Kyriakides, L. (2016). Teaching strategies and differential effectiveness
across learning contexts: Evidence from PISA 2012. Studies in Educational Evaluation,49,30
41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stueduc.2016.03.005
Castronovo, J., & G
obel, S. M. (2012). Impact of high mathematics education on the number sense.
PLoS ONE,7(4), e33832. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033832
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese
Americans and European Americans. Child Development,72, 18321843. https://doi.org/10.
1111/1467-8624.00381
Chen, Z. Y., Cowell, P. E., Varley, R., & Wang, Y. C. (2009). A cross-language study of verbal and
visuospatial working memory span. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
31, 385391. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390802195195
Chinn, S. (2009). Mathematics anxiety in secondary students in England. Dyslexia,15,6168.
https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.381
Cipora, K., Szczygieł, M., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H-C. (2015). Math anxiety assessment with the
abbreviated math anxiety scale: Applicability and usefulness: Insights from the polish
adaptation. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 1833. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01833
Cirino, P. T. (2011). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology,108, 713733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.004
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007). Early childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester (Ed.),
Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 461555). New
York, NY: Information Age Publishing.
Cui, J., Georgiou, G. K., Zhang, Y., Li, Y., Shu, H., & Zhou, X. (2017). Examining the relationship between
rapid automatized naming and arithmetic fluency in Chinese kindergarten children. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology,154, 146163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.008
Cui, J., Zhang, Y., Cheng, D., Li, D., & Zhou, X. (2017). Visual form perception can be a cognitive
correlate of lower level math categories for teenagers. Frontiers in Psychology,8, 1336.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01336
De Smedt, B., & Gilmore, C. (2011). Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical
magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology,108, 278292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.003
De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquiere, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude
comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology,103, 469479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Desoete, A., & Gr
egoire, J. (2006). Numerical competence in young children and in children with
mathematics learning disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences,16, 351367. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2006.12.006
Dowker, A., Bala, S., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Linguistic influences on mathematical development: How
important is the transparency of the counting system? Philosophical Psychology,21, 523538.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080802285511
Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years?
Frontiers in Psychology,7, 508. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
Eden, C., Heine, A., & Jacobs, A. M. (2013). Mathematics anxiety and its development in the course of
formal schooling - A review. Psychology,4,2735. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.
46A2005
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1989). Early schooling as a “critical period” phenomenon. In K.
Namboodiri & R. Corwin (Eds.), Sociology of education and socialization (pp. 2755).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 271
Flaherty, M., & Connolly, M. (1995). Space perception, coordination and a knowledge of Kanji in
Japanese and non-Japanese. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the
Orient,38, 229237.
Fuson, K. C., & Kwon, Y. (1992). Korean children’s single-digit addition and subtraction: Numbers
structured by ten. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,23, 148165. https://doi.
org/10.2307/749498
Gatobu, S., Arocha, J. F., & Hoffman-Goetz, L. (2014). Numeracy and health numeracy among
Chinese and Kenyan immigrants to Canada the role of math the role of math self-efficacy. Sage
Open,4(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244014521437
Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., Fan, L., & Siegler, R. S. (1993). Even before formal instruction,
Chinese children outperform American children in mental addition. Cognitive Development,8,
517529. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(05)80007-3
Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2011). Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli. Behavior Research
Methods,43, 981986. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0097-5
Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The role of visual information in numerosity estimation. PLoS
ONE,7(5), e37426. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0037426
Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and
mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling. Cognition,115, 394406.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002
Girelli, L., Lucangeli, D., & Butterworth, B. (2000). The development of automaticity in accessing
number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,76, 104122. https://doi.org/
10.1006/jecp.2000.2564
G
obel, S. M., Maier, C. A., & Shaki, S. (2015). Which numbers do you have in mind? Number
generation is influenced by reading direction. Cognitive Processing,16(1), 241244. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0715-8
Gonzales, P., Williams, T., Jocelyn, L., Roey, S., Kastberg, D., & Brenwald, S. (2008). Highlights From
TIMSS 2007: Mathematics and Science Achievement of U.S. Fourth- and Eighth-Grade
Students in International Context (NCES 2009-001 Revised). Washington, DC.
Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current
Directions in Psychological Science,13(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.
01301001.x
Guilford, J.P., & Guilford, R. B. (1936). Personality factors S, E, and M, and their measurement.
Journal of Psychology,2, 109127. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1936.9917446
Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the ‘number sense’: The
approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year 252 olds and adults. Developmental
Psychology,44, 14571465. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012682
Harari, R. R., Vukovic, R. K., & Bailey, S. P. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young children: An
exploratory study. Journal of Experimental Education,81, 538555. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00220973.2012.727888
He, J., Buchholz, J., & Klieme, E. (2017). Effects of anchoring vignettes on comparability and
predictive validity of student self-reports in 64 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
48, 319334. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116687395
Hedden, T., & Yoon, C. (2006). Individual differences in executive processing predict susceptibility
to interference in verbal working memory. Neuropsychology,20, 511528. https://doi.org/10.
1037/0894-4105.20.5.511
Helmreich, I., Zuber, J., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., Nuerk, H. C., & Moeller, K. (2011). Language effects
on children’s nonverbal number line estimations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,42,
598613. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111406026
Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education,21,3346. https://doi.org/10.2307/749455
Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical
distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology,103,1729. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001
272 Maja Rodic et al.
Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., Larson, S. L., Balsink Krieg, D., & Shaligram, C. (2000). Mathematics,
vocabulary, and reading development in Chinese American and European American children
over the primary school years. Journal of Educational Psychology,92, 745760. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.745
Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., Liaw, F. R., & Ching, W. (1997). Cultural differences in early
mathematical learning: A comparison of Euro-American and Taiwan-Chinese families.
International Journal of Behavioural Development,21, 371388. https://doi.org/10.1080/
016502597384929
Imbo, I., & LeFevre, J. A. (2009). Cultural differences in complex addition: Efficient Chinese versus
adaptive Belgians and Canadians. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
Cognition,35, 14651476. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017022
Jak, S. (2017). Testing and explaining differences in common and residual factors across
many countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,48,7592. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0022022116674599
Jameson, M. M. (2013). The development and validation of the children’s anxiety in math scale.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,31, 391395. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282
912470131
Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: Kindergarten
number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology,45, 850
867. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014939
Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). Early numerical development and
the role of non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction,25,95103. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001
Krapohl, E., Rimfeld, K., Shakeshaft, N. G., Trzaskowski, M., McMillan, A., Pingault, J.-B., Asbury, K.,
Harlaar, N., Kovas, Y., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2014). The high heritability of educational
achievement reflects many genetically influenced traits, not just intelligence. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,111, 1527315278. https://doi.
org/10.1073/pnas.1408777111
Krinzinger, H., Kaufmann, L., & Willmes, K. (2009). Math anxiety and math ability in early primary
school years. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,27, 206225. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0734282908330583
Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical
capacities: A study of 89-year-old students. Cognition,93,99125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.c
ognition.2003.11.004
Laski, E. V., & Yu, Q. (2014). Number line estimation and mental addition: Examining the potential
roles of language and education. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,117(1), 2944.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.007
Lee, J. (2009). Universals and specifics of math self-concept, math self-efficacy, and math anxiety
across 41 PISA 2003 participating countries. Learning and Individual Differences,19, 355
365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.009
Leung, F. K. S. (2006). Mathematics education in East Asia and the West: Does culture matter?.In
F. K. S. Leung, K. D. Graf, & F. J. Lopez-Real, (Eds.) Mathematics education in different cultural
traditions: A comparative study of East Asia and the West, The 13th ICMI Study (pp. 2146).
New York, NY: Springer.
Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number
system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science,14, 12921300. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x
Lowrie, T., Logan, T., & Ramful, A. (2016). Cross cultural comparison of grade 6 students’
performance and strategy use on graphic and non-graphic tasks. Learning and Individual
Differences,52,97108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.005
Ma, X., & Xu, J. (2004). The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement: A
longitudinal panel analysis. Journal of Adolescence,27, 165179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ad
olescence.2003.11.003
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 273
Maloney, E. A., Risko, E. F., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Mathematics anxiety affects counting
but not subitizing during visual enumeration. Cognition,114, 293297. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.cognition.2009.09.013
Mark, W., & Dowker, A. (2015). Linguistic influence on mathematical development is specific rather
than pervasive: Revisiting the Chinese number advantage in Chinese and English children.
Frontiers in Psychology,26, 203.
Marsh, H. W. (2016). Cross-cultural generalizability of year in school effects: Negative effects of
acceleration and positive effects of retention on academic self-concept. Journal of Educational
Psychology,108, 256273. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000059
Mazzocco, M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate
number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PLoS ONE,6(9), e23749.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023749
Miller, K. F., Kelly, M. K., & Zhou, X. (2005). Learning mathematics in China and United States: Cross-
cultural insights into the nature and course of mathematical development. In J. I. D. Campbell
(Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 163178). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Miller, K. F., & Stigler, J. W. (1987). Counting in Chinese: Cultural variation in a basic cognitive skill.
Cognitive Development,2, 279305. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(87)90091-8
Min, I., Cortina, K. S., & Miller, K. F. (2016). Modesty bias and the attitude-achievement paradox
across nations: A reanalysis of TIMSS. Learning and Individual Differences,51, 359366.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.09.008
Miura, I. T. (1987). Mathematics achievement as a function of language. Journal of Educational
Psychology,79,7982. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.79.1.79
Miura, I. T., Kim, C. C., Chang, C. M., & Okamoto, Y. (1988). Effects of language characteristics on
children’s cognitive representation of number: Cross-national comparisons. Child
Development,59, 14451450. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130659
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., & Foy, P. (2008). TIMSS 2007 International Mathematics Report:
Findings from IEA’s Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study at Eight and
Fourth grades. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
OECD (2010). PISA at glance. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264095298-en
Pitchford, N. J., & Outhwaite, L. A. (2016). Can touch screen tablets be used to assess cognitive and
motor skills in early years primary school children? A cross-cultural study. Frontiers in
Psychology,7, 1666.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01666
Pouw, W. T., Van, G. T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2016). Augmenting instructional animations with a
body analogy to help children learn about physical systems. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 860.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00860
Putz, D. A., Gaulin, S. J., Sporter, R. J., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Sex hormones and finger length:
What does 2D: 4D indicate? Evolution and Human Behavior,25, 182199. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.005
Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory,
and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and Development,14,
187202. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.664593
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and
vocabulary scales. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.
Robinson, K. M., & Beatch, J. A. (2016). Conceptual knowledge of arithmetic for Chinese- and
Canadian-educated adults. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,70, 335342.
https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000097
Rodic, M., Zhou, X., Tikhomirova, T., Wei, W., Malykh, S., Ismatulina, V., ...Kovas, Y. (2015). Cross-
cultural investigation into cognitive underpinnings of individual differences in early arithmetic.
Developmental Science,18, 165174. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12204
Rohde, T. E., & Thompson, L. A. (2007). Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability.
Intelligence,35,8392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004
Ryoo, J. H., Molfese, V. J., Heaton, R., Zhou, X., Brown, E. T., Prokasky, A., & Davis, E. (2014). Early
mathematics skills from prekindergarten to first grade: Score changes and ability group
274 Maja Rodic et al.
differences in Kentucky, Nebraska, and Shanghai samples. Journal of Advanced Academics,25,
162188. https://doi.org/10.1177/1932202X14538975
Sakamoto, M., & Spiers, M. V. (2014). Sex and cultural differences in spatial performance between
Japanese and North Americans. Archives of Sexual Behavior,43, 483491. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10508-013-0232-8
Salthouse, T. A. (1994). The nature of the influence of speed on adult age differences in cognition.
Developmental Psychology,30, 240259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.240
Salthouse, T. A., & Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal of
Gerontology,50, 297306. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/50B.6.P297
Schachner, M. K., He, J., Heizmann, B., & Van de Vijver, F. (2017). Acculturation and school
adjustment of immigrant youth in six European countries: Findings from the programme for
international student assessment (PISA). Frontiers in Psychology,8, 649. https://doi.org/10.
3389/fpsyg.2017.00649
Setti, A., Borghi, A. M., & Tessari, A. (2009). Moving hands, moving entities. Brain and Cognition,
70, 253258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.012
Shen, C., Vasilyeva, M., & Laski, E. V. (2016). Here, but not there: Cross-national variability of gender
effects in arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,146,5065. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.016
Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science,171, 701
703. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701
Siegler, R. S., & Mu, Y. (2008). Chinese children excel on novel mathematics problems even before
elementary school. Psychological Science,19, 759763. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.
2008.02153.x
Smith, P., Fernandes, C., & Strand, S. (2001). Cognitive abilities test third edition: Technical
manual. London, UK: nferNelson.
Solt
esz, F., Sz}
ucs, D., & Sz}
ucs, L. (2010). Relationship between magnitude representation, counting
and memory in 4- to 7-year old children: A developmental study. Behavioural and Brain
Functions,6, 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-13
Song, M. J., & Ginsburg, H. P. (1988). The effect of the Korean number system on young children’s
counting: A natural experiment in numerical bilingualism. International Journal of Psychology,
23, 319332. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207598808247769
Starkey, P., & Klein, A. (2007). Sociocultural influences on young children’s mathematical
knowledge. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on mathematics
in early childhood education (pp. 253276). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., & Lee, S. Y. (1993). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and
American children: Ten years later. Science,259,5358. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.
8418494
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S. Y., Chen, C., Lummis, M., Stigler, J., Fan, L., & Ge, F. (1990). Mathematics
achievement of children in China and the United States. Child Development,61, 10531066.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1130875
Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S. Y., & Stigler, J. W. (1986). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese,
and American children. Science,231, 693699. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3945803
Stevenson, H. W., & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap. New York, NY: Free Press.
Strenberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Bundy, D. A. (2001). Predictive value of IQ. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly,47(1), 141. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2001.0005
Sz}
ucs, D., Devine, A., Soltesz, F., Nobes, A., & Gabriel, F. (2014). Cognitive components of a
mathematical processing network in 9-year-old children. Developmental Science,17, 506524.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12144
Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems.
In J. Castellan & F. Restle (Eds.). Cognitive theory Vol. 3 (pp. 200239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vasilyeva, M., Laski, E. V., & Shen, C. (2015). Computational fluency and strategy choice predict
individual and cross-national differences in complex arithmetic. Developmental Psychology,
51, 14891500. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000045
Cross-cultural differences in early arithmetic 275
Vigneau, F., & Bors, D. A. (2001). Visuospatial and verbal-analytic processes in Raven’s
Progressive Matrices performance. XI Meeting of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behaviour, and
Cognitive Science (CSBBCS), Universit
e Laval, Quebec City.
Vigneau,F.,Caissie,A.F.,&Bors,D.A.(2006).Eye-movement analysis demonstrates strategic
influences on intelligence. Intelligence,34,261272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.003
Wang, Z., Hart, S. A., Kovas, Y., Lukowski, S., Soden, B., Thompson, L. A., ...Petrill, S. A. (2014). Who
is afraid of math? Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry,55, 10561064. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12224
Wang, L., Sun, Y., & Zhou, X. (2016). Relation between approximate number system acuity and
mathematical achievement: The influence of fluency. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 1966. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01966
Wei, W., Lu, H., Zhao, H., Chen, C., Dong, Q., & Zhou, X. (2012). Gender differences in children’s
arithmetic performance are accounted for by gender differences in language abilities.
Psychological Science,23, 320330. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611427168
Wei, W., Yuan, H., Chen, C., & Zhou, X. (2012). Cognitive correlates of performance in advanced
mathematics. British Journal of Educational Psychology,82, 157181. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02049.x
Weis, M., Trommsdorff, G., & Mu~
noz, L. (2016). Children’s self-regulation and school achievement
in cultural contexts: The role of maternal restrictive control. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 722.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00722
Xenidou-Dervou, I., Gilmore, C., van der Schoot, M., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M (2015). The
developmental onset of symbolic approximation: Beyond nonsymbolic representations, the language
of numbers matters. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 487. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00487
Yang,C.C.,Kao,C.J.,Cheng,T.W.,Yang,C.C.,Wang,W.H.,Yu,R.L.,... Hua, M. S. (2012). Cross-
cultural effect on suboptimal effort detection: An example of the digit span subtest of the WAIS-III in
Taiwan. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,27, 869878. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs081
Zhang, Y., Chen, C., Liu, H., Cui, J., & Zhou, X. (2016). Both non-symbolic and symbolic quantity
processing are important for arithmetical computation but not for mathematical reasoning. Journal
of Cognitive Psychology,28, 807824. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1205074
Zhou, Z., Peverly, S. T., & Lin, J. (2005). Understanding early mathematical competencies in
American and Chinese children. School Psychology International,26, 413427. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0143034305059018
Zhou, X., Wei, W., Zhang, Y., Cui, J., & Chen, C. (2015). Visual perception can account for the close
relation between numerosity processing and computational fluency. Frontiers in Psychology,6,
1364. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01364
Received 29 September 2016; revised version received 14 April 2018
Supporting Information
Additional supplemental material may be found online in the Supporting
Information section at the end of the article:
Table S1. The Revised Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale (RMARS) in
English.
Table S2. Descriptive statistics on raw scores for all tasks - UK, Russian &
Chinese samples.
Table S3. Sex differences for all tasks per country.
Table S4. ANOVA results on raw (uncorrected) scores.
Table S5. ANOVA results on scores corrected for Mental rotation.
276 Maja Rodic et al.
... Arithmetic ability is a fundamental aspect of mathematical proficiency, which children develop progressively during their school years. Numerous studies have consistently shown that East Asian children's superiority in arithmetic skills [3][4][5][6][7]. All these studies convey results in what literature defines as "exact arithmetic", which pertains to the ability to calculate the precise answer to an arithmetic problem [8]. ...
... As already anticipated, an extensive body of research has investigated cross-cultural differences in exact arithmetic performance, consistently showing that Chinese primary students outperform their western peers [3][4][5][6][7]. Education has been posited as an important factor in these cultural differences in mathematical abilities [3,7,9]. ...
... Multi-group path analyses revealed that the patterns of relations among cognitive factors, mathematical anxiety, and arithmetic performance were similar across both groups, with one exception that visuospatial updating was a unique predictor of arithmetic estimation for Chinese children but not for Italian children. In line with previous studies [3][4][5][6][7]67], Chinese children demonstrated better performance in exact arithmetic compared to their Western peers, particularly with double-digit arithmetic tasks [89,90]. One possible explanation for this performance gap is the different arithmetic strategies used by the children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background This study aimed to investigate the cognitive and affective factors associated with cross-cultural differences in arithmetic tasks. Methods A total of 404 third- and fourth- graders were recruited from China and Italy to complete exact arithmetic, arithmetic estimation and cognitive tasks (i.e., short-term memory, executive functions, and fluid reasoning). Their mathematical anxiety was also measured. Results The results showed that Chinese children performed better than Italian children in both arithmetic tasks and in shifting task. Italian children performed better in visuospatial updating task and reported higher levels of mathematical anxiety than their Chinese peers. Multi-group path analyses showed that the patterns of relations among cognitive factors (i.e., short-term memory, inhibition and shifting), mathematical anxiety, and arithmetic performance were similar across groups. The only exception was that visuospatial updating uniquely predicted arithmetic estimation for Chinese but not for Italian children. Conclusions Chinese children outperformed their Italian peers in the exact arithmetic task, likely due to the greater emphasis on arithmetic fluency in Chinese mathematics education, both in schools and at home. They also had a slight advantage than Italian peers in the arithmetic estimation task. The unique link between updating and arithmetic estimation found in Chinese children but not Italian children suggests that, although arithmetic estimation is not emphasized in the curricula of either country, instruction and practice in exact arithmetic may enhance Chinese children’s efficiency in solving arithmetic estimation problems.
... The third line of investigation focuses on whether non-symbolic number sense differs between cultures. To our knowledge, four studies with opposing findings have examined cross-cultural differences in non-symbolic number sense of child performance (Cantrell, Kuwabara, and Smith 2015;Rodic et al. 2015Rodic et al. , 2018Tikhomirova et al. 2019). Cantrell, Kuwabara, and Smith (2015) showed similar performance and bias on a match-to-sample task for preschoolers in the USA and Japan. ...
... Rodic et al. (2015) showed that 5-7-year-old UK, Russian, and Chinese children all performed significantly better than Kyrgyz/Dongan peers in a dot-enumeration task. Later Rodic et al. (2018) showed that 6-9-year-old Chinese children had significantly faster reaction time and slightly lower accuracy than Russian/UK peers in non-symbolic comparison of numerosity task. Tikhomirova et al. (2019) revealed that the rate of growth in ANS accuracy was higher for the Russian sample than for the Kyrgyz sample and that this difference remained significant even after controlling for fluid intelligence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Formal education would promote symbolic number processing ability, but the relationship between non‐symbolic number sense and mathematical education remains controversial. The current investigation hypothesized that non‐symbolic number sense is independent from the limited human experience of mathematical education, while symbolic number processing could be influenced by related closely to mathematical education. Experiment 1 compared both non‐symbolic number sense and arithmetic computation of undergraduates majoring in STEM fields and humanities. Experiment 2 compared homologous undergraduates from China and Kyrgyzstan in non‐symbolic number sense and mathematical cognitive processing. Both of two experiments found that mathematical education was significantly associated with symbolic mathematics, but not with non‐symbolic number sense. The results suggest that while mathematical education might improve symbolic mathematics, it does not alter non‐symbolic number sense.
... Cultural differences in mathematics language have been proposed as a contributor to the differences in children's understanding of numerical concepts (e.g., Geary et al., 1996;Göbel et al., 2011;Han & Ginsburg, 2001;Kung et al., 2019;Magargee & Beauford, 2016;Miura & Okamoto, 2003;Ng & Rao, 2010;Okamoto, 2018;Purpura & Reid, 2016;Rodic et al., 2015Rodic et al., , 2018. Written numerical symbols are practically universal across cultures; however, spoken number words differ from one language to another starting with the base sequence of numbers from 1 to 10. Furthermore, there is great diversity in how number names above 10 are generated. ...
... Children learning a language that uses more simple shape terms and embeds defining properties of shapes within the shape names (e.g., Japanese) should have an easier time identifying and determining the validity of shapes than children learning a language with more complex terms that does not include linguistic cues to defining features (e.g., English). Consistent with research showing that linguistic characteristics can facilitate the development of mathematical concepts (e.g., Geary et al., 1996;Göbel et al., 2011;Han & Ginsburg, 2001;Kung et al., 2019;Magargee & Beauford, 2016;Miura & Okamoto, 2003;Ng & Rao, 2010;Okamoto, 2018;Purpura & Reid, 2016;Rodic et al., 2015Rodic et al., , 2018, our main finding indicated Japanese children outperformed U.S. children on the Shape Identification Task. There was preliminary evidence suggesting that the greater "linguistic clarity" (Han & Ginsburg, 2001) of the Japanese shape-naming convention facilitated children's shape knowledge. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study compared Japanese and U.S. preschool children’s knowledge of geometric shapes. The main goal was to explore if differences in shape-naming conventions in Japanese and English could explain differences in children’s understanding of geometric shapes. In ancient Chinese-based languages (e.g., Japanese), all standard 2D shapes (except for the rectangle) take the form of “[the number of angles]-angle-shape” (e.g., “five-angle shape” for pentagon). Not only are the shape labels relatively easy to construct, each label includes a description of a defining property within its name (i.e., the number of angles). In contrast, English shape names are relatively more complex and do not readily suggest defining shape characteristics. A language in which shape names are relatively easy to construct and communicate defining shape properties should boost children’s shape knowledge. Participants were 33 Japanese (Mage = 54.24 months, SD = 7.33 months) and 26 U.S. children (Mage = 55.58 months, SD = 8.03 months) between 42 and 68 months old. Children were asked to determine the validity of shape stimuli in a Shape Identification Task. They were also given a mental rotation task to control for spatial reasoning. Findings indicated that language-group membership and age, but not spatial reasoning, significantly predicted shape knowledge. As expected, Japanese children outperformed U.S. children in identifying geometric shapes. The findings suggest that labeling and describing geometric shapes using their defining properties could support children’s shape learning.
... Notably, as expected, we did not observe statistically significant differences among arithmetic operations in children. Considering that children included in the articles we analyzed were 9 and 10 years old on average for addition and subtraction and closer to 11 years old for multiplication, one could suggest that children's abilities in arithmetic operations greatly improve through learning during early school years (Entwisle and Alexander, 1989;Clements and Sarama, 2007;Rodic et al., 2018) and variability in brain response may be expressed in less detectable differences. The lack of differences in brain correlates among arithmetic operations may be also due to comparable experience across operation types (Evans et al., 2016;Brignoni-Pérez et al., 2021) as 9-10-year-olds should have had a couple of years of experience in addition and subtraction and 11-year-olds should have a couple of years of experience in multiplication. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mathematical operations are cognitive actions we take to calculate relations among numbers. Arithmetic operations, addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division are elemental in education. Addition is the first one taught in school and is most popular in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. Division, typically taught last is least studied with fMRI. fMRI meta-analyses show that arithmetic operations activate brain areas in parietal, cingulate and insular cortices for children and adults. Critically, no meta-analysis examines concordance across brain correlates of separate arithmetic operations in children and adults. We review and examine using quantitative meta-analyses data from fMRI articles that report brain coordinates separately for addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division in children and adults. Results show that arithmetic operations elicit common areas of concordance in fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular networks in adults and children. Between operations differences are observed primarily for adults. Interestingly, higher within-group concordance, expressed in activation likelihood estimates, is found in brain areas associated with the cingulo-opercular network rather than the fronto-parietal network in children, areas also common between adults and children. Findings are discussed in relation to constructivist cognitive theory and practical directions for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Вступ. Актуальність дослідження впливу гіперопікуючого та гіпоопікуючого стилів сімейного виховання на психічні особливості дітей з дитячим церебральним паралічем (ДЦП) зумовлена зростанням кількості випадків цієї патології, а також необхідністю забезпечення якісної психологічної підтримки таких дітей. Коли дитина з ДЦП стикається з численними фізичними та емоційними труднощами, стиль сімейного виховання стає вирішальним чинником її психосоціальної адаптації. Гіперопіка та гіпоопіка здатні значно вплинути на самооцінку, рівень самостійності, здатність до емоційного саморегулювання і соціальні навички дитини, що, у свою чергу, визначає її успішність у подальшому житті. Отже, вивчення цих стилів є важливим для розробки рекомендацій, що сприятимуть формуванню здорових психологічних умов для розвитку дітей з ДЦП і підтримки їх рідних. Мета роботи – проаналізувати вплив гіперопікуючого та гіпоопікуючого стилів сімейного виховання на психічні особливості дітей з ДЦП, зокрема їх самооцінку, емоційну стабільність і соціальну адаптацію. Основна частина. Зосереджено увагу на вихованні дітей з ДЦП з акцентом на важливості індивідуального підходу та мультидисциплінарної підтримки. Обговорено медичні, психологічні й педагогічні аспекти, включаючи адаптацію навчальних матеріалів і фізіотерапію. Особливу увагу приділено соціа­лізації та емоційній підтримці, а також ролі сім’ї у створенні розвивального середовища для дитини. Виховання таких дітей є складним процесом, що вимагає зусиль від батьків і фахівців для забезпечення максимальної автономії та інтеграції. Висновки. Стиль виховання підлітків з ДЦП має суттєвий вплив на їх психологічний і соціальний розвиток. Гіперопіка може сприяти формуванню залежності, знижуючи рівень самостійності й ускладнюючи соціальну адаптацію, тоді як гіпоопіка викликає почуття самотності та невпевненості. Важливим є знаходження балансу між підтримкою та автономією для гармонійного розвитку дітей. Необхідно розробляти для батьків корекційні програми, які навчать їх ефективних підходів до виховання, зменшать тривожність і сприятимуть створенню здорового клімату в сім’ї. Залучення батьків до обговорень допоможе краще зрозуміти потреби дітей і сформувати індивідуальні стратегії розвитку.
Article
Objective This study examined the relationship between culture and cognitive abilities by comparing WAIS-IV subtests, index scores, and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) scores across various age groups in the Taiwanese and U.S. populations. Method The Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV are comprehensively compared, examining subtest items, psychometrics, and sample characteristics. Scaled scores are compared by extracting raw scores with a scaled score of 10 from each subtest scale in the Taiwanese version and applying U.S. age norms to acquire U.S. scaled scores. Results Despite the mean FSIQ score closely aligning with the U.S. sample, notable discrepancies are evident in the Taiwanese Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) score, potentially influenced by cultural fairness of the tests. Significant variations are observed among age cohorts in the Taiwanese sample, with younger individuals excelling in Processing Speed Index, Working Memory Index, Perceptual Reasoning Index, and FSIQ, while maintaining comparable VCI scores to their U.S. counterparts. Conversely, older cohorts demonstrate lower performance across various domains, except for visuospatial reasoning and organizational skills, compared to their U.S. counterparts. These subtest variations robustly correlate with educational disparities between the Taiwanese and U.S. samples. Conclusions Despite the similarity in factor structures between the Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV, this study reveals cultural bias in both verbal and non-verbal subtests. The study highlights the intricate interplay among cognitive processing styles, cultural influences, and educational factors contributing to performance disparities.
Chapter
Studies have shown that children who perform poorly in mathematics at an early age exhibit poor mathematical performance later on. This finding, known as the Matthew effect, places great importance on early achievements in mathematics. Although preschoolers have only a little learning and practical experience in mathematics, evaluating their mathematical abilities to predict their later mathematical performances is valuable. To give a complete understanding of preschoolers’ mathematical abilities, three studies are further presented: a cross-cultural study, a parity judgment study, and a number comparison study. Then, some related factors, such as counting, language, and sports abilities, that can predict children’s mathematical and arithmetical development in depth are discussed.
Article
The attitude of parents to the child and the parenting style affect their socialization, emotional state, intelligence and moral development. The article examines the peculiarities of the parenting style that has developed in China and European countries. The article analyzes the postulates of the modern paradigm of parenting in China, which is based on Confucian philosophy and the pedagogical heritage of classical Chinese thinkers, poets and writers who formed the ancient traditions of family education. The priorities transmitted by parents in the process of upbringing are highlighted. The main directions of the interpretation of parental styles in Western Europe are considered: psychoanalytic, behaviorist and humanistic. Classifications of approaches to the education of Russian researchers are also analyzed.
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies have assessed the cognitive correlates of performance in mathematics, but little research has been conducted to systematically examine the relations between visual perception as the starting point of visuospatial processing and typical mathematical performance. In the current study, we recruited 223 seventh graders to perform a visual form perception task (figure matching), numerosity comparison, digit comparison, exact computation, approximate computation, and curriculum-based mathematical achievement tests. Results showed that, after controlling for gender, age, and five general cognitive processes (choice reaction time, visual tracing, mental rotation, spatial working memory, and non-verbal matrices reasoning), visual form perception had unique contributions to numerosity comparison, digit comparison, and exact computation, but had no significant relation with approximate computation or curriculum-based mathematical achievement. These results suggest that visual form perception is an important independent cognitive correlate of lower level math categories, including the approximate number system, digit comparison, and exact computation.
Article
Full-text available
School adjustment determines long-term adjustment in society. Yet, immigrant youth do better in some countries than in others. Drawing on acculturation research (Berry, 1997; Ward, 2001) and self-determination theory (Ryan and Deci, 2000), we investigated indirect effects of adolescent immigrants' acculturation orientations on school adjustment (school-related attitudes, truancy, and mathematics achievement) through school belonging. Analyses were based on data from the Programme for International Student Assessment from six European countries, which were combined into three clusters based on their migrant integration and multicultural policies: Those with the most supportive policies (Belgium and Finland), those with moderately supportive policies (Italy and Portugal), and those with the most unsupportive policies (Denmark and Slovenia). In a multigroup path model, we confirmed most associations. As expected, mainstream orientation predicted higher belonging and better outcomes in all clusters, whereas the added value of students' ethnic orientation was only observed in some clusters. Results are discussed in terms of differences in acculturative climate and policies between countries of settlement.
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have observed inconsistent relations between the acuity of the Approximate Number System (ANS) and mathematical achievement. In this paper, we hypothesize that the relation between ANS acuity and mathematical achievement is influenced by fluency; that is, the mathematical achievement test covering a greater expanse of mathematical fluency may better reflect the relation between ANS acuity and mathematics skills. We explored three types of mathematical achievement tests utilized in this study: Subtraction, graded, and semester-final examination. The subtraction test was designed to measure the mathematical fluency. The graded test was more fluency-based than the semester-final examination, but both involved the same mathematical knowledge from the class curriculum. A total of 219 fifth graders from primary schools were asked to perform all three tests, then given a numerosity comparison task, a visual form perception task (figure matching), and a series of other tasks to assess general cognitive processes (mental rotation, non-verbal matrix reasoning, and choice reaction time). The findings were consistent with our expectations. The relation between ANS acuity and mathematical achievement was particularly clearly reflected in the participants’ performance on the visual form perception task, which supports the domain-general explanations for the underlying mechanisms of the relation between ANS acuity and math achievement.
Article
Full-text available
Assessment of cognitive and motor functions is fundamental for developmental and neuropsychological profiling. Assessments are usually conducted on an individual basis, with a trained examiner, using standardized paper and pencil tests, and can take up to an hour or more to complete, depending on the nature of the test. This makes traditional standardized assessments of child development largely unsuitable for use in low-income countries. Touch screen tablets afford the opportunity to assess cognitive functions in groups of participants, with untrained administrators, with precision recording of responses, thus automating the assessment process. In turn, this enables cognitive profiling to be conducted in contexts where access to qualified examiners and standardized assessments are rarely available. As such, touch screen assessments could provide a means of assessing child development in both low- and high-income countries, which would afford cross-cultural comparisons to be made with the same assessment tool. However, before touch screen tablet assessments can be used for cognitive profiling in low-to-high-income countries they need to be shown to provide reliable and valid measures of performance. We report the development of a new touch screen tablet assessment of basic cognitive and motor functions for use with early years primary school children in low- and high-income countries. Measures of spatial intelligence, visual attention, short-term memory, working memory, manual processing speed, and manual coordination are included as well as mathematical knowledge. To investigate if this new touch screen assessment tool can be used for cross-cultural comparisons we administered it to a sample of children (N = 283) spanning standards 1–3 in a low-income country, Malawi, and a smaller sample of children (N = 70) from first year of formal schooling from a high-income country, the UK. Split-half reliability, test-retest reliability, face validity, convergent construct validity, predictive criterion validity, and concurrent criterion validity were investigated. Results demonstrate “proof of concept” that touch screen tablet technology can provide reliable and valid psychometric measures of performance in the early years, highlighting its potential to be used in cross-cultural comparisons and research.
Article
The primary purpose of this study was to develop an abbreviated version of the Math Anxiety Rating Scale (MARS). The result was an internally consistant and reliable 25-item scale.
Article
Anchoring vignettes are item batteries especially designed for correcting responses that might be affected by incomparability. This article investigates the effects of anchoring vignettes on the validity of student self-report data in 64 cultures. Using secondary data analysis from the 2012 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), we checked the validity of ratings on vignette questions, and investigated how rescaled item responses of two student scales, Teacher Support and Classroom Management, enhanced comparability and predictive validity. The main findings include that (a) responses to vignette questions represent valid individual and cultural differences; in particular, violations in these responses (i.e., misorderings) are related to low socioeconomic status and low cognitive sophistication; (b) the rescaled responses tend to show higher levels of comparability; and (c) the associations of rescaled Teacher Support and Classroom Management with math achievement, Student-Oriented Instruction, and Teacher-Directed Instruction are slightly different from raw scores of the two target constructs, and the associations with rescaled scores seem to be more in line with the literature. Namely, the associations among all self-report Likert-type scales are weaker with rescaled scores, presumably reducing common method variance, and both rescaled scale scores are more positively related to math achievement. The country ranking also changes substantially; in particular, Asian cultures top the ranking on Teacher Support after rescaling. However, anchoring vignettes are not a cure-all in solving measurement bias in cross-cultural surveys; we discuss the technicality and directions for further research on this technique.
Article
This investigation examined Singaporean and Australian students' performance and strategy use when solving mathematics tasks. Half of the questions in the 24-item test were drawn from the national Singaporean Grade 6 test, with the other 12 items sourced from the Australian national assessment. 1187 grade 6 students solved graphic and non-graphic mathematics tasks and reported their solution strategies later classified and coded as symbolic, pictorial or imagistic. Results revealed performance differences in favor of the Singaporean cohort on three of the four task categories. There were distinct differences between the types of strategies employed by the students across these task categories. The Singaporean students were more likely to use conditioned heuristics to solve the tasks, especially the non-graphic tasks, whereas the Australian students were reluctant to use these heuristics. Cultural differences were also found in the way students solved the tasks, especially when encountering unfamiliar tasks.
Article
Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) has been found to predict mathematics. However, the nature of their relationship remains unclear. Thus, the purpose of this study was two-fold: (a) to examine how RAN (numeric and non-numeric) predicts a subdomain of mathematics (arithmetic fluency), and (b) to examine what processing skills may account for the RAN-arithmetic fluency relationship. One hundred and sixty third-year kindergarten Chinese children (83 boys and 77 girls, mean age = 5.11 years) were assessed on RAN (colors, objects, digits, and dice), nonverbal IQ, visual-verbal paired associate learning, phonological awareness, short-term memory, speed of processing, approximate number system acuity, and arithmetic fluency (addition and subtraction). The results indicated first that RAN was a significant correlate of arithmetic fluency and the correlations did not vary as a function of type of RAN or arithmetic fluency tasks. In addition, RAN continued to predict addition and subtraction fluency, even after controlling for all other processing skills. Taken together, these findings challenge the existing theoretical accounts of the RAN-arithmetic fluency relationship and suggest that, similar to reading fluency, multiple processes underlie the RAN-arithmetic fluency relationship.