Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
www.nature.com/scientificreports
The Neurocognitive Architecture
of Individual Dierences in Math
Anxiety in Typical Children
Charlotte E. Hartwright
1,2, Chung Yen Looi2,3, Francesco Sella2, Alberto Inuggi
4,
Flávia Heloísa Santos5, Carmen González-Salinas6, Jose M. García Santos7,
Roi Cohen Kadosh
2 & Luis J. Fuentes
5
Math Anxiety (MA) is characterized by a negative emotional response when facing math-related
situations. MA is distinct from general anxiety and can emerge during primary education. Prior studies
typically comprise adults and comparisons between high- versus low-MA, where neuroimaging work
has focused on dierences in network activation between groups when completing numerical tasks. The
present study used voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to identify the structural brain correlates of MA in
a sample of 79 healthy children aged 7–12 years. Given that MA is thought to develop in later primary
education, the study focused on the level of MA, rather than categorically dening its presence. Using
a battery of cognitive- and numerical-function tasks, we identied that increased MA was associated
with reduced attention, working memory and math achievement. VBM highlighted that increased
MA was associated with reduced grey matter in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus. This region was
also associated with attention, suggesting that baseline dierences in morphology may underpin
attentional dierences. Future studies should clarify whether poorer attentional capacity due to
reduced grey matter density results in the later emergence of MA. Further, our data highlight the role of
working memory in propagating reduced math achievement in children with higher MA.
Math anxiety (MA) is characterised by negative emotional response such as fear and tension when facing
math-related situations, which cannot be reduced to either general anxiety or test anxiety1. It disrupts mathemat-
ical performance irrespective of gender2, and can emerge in the primary school years3,4. Depending on the extent
of MA, the negative impact of MA could have far-reaching consequences beyond academic achievements5. Given
that a signicant variation in the level of MA is contributed by genetic factors6, understanding the neurocognitive
basis of individual dierences in MA may shed light on its causal pathway.
To date, there are a limited number of neuroimaging studies, which are mainly based on adults and compar-
isons of neural response between groups of high and low levels of MA. In adults, high- compared with low-level
MA has been shown to be associated with increased activity in bilateral posterior insula, brain areas linked with
threat and pain processing, when anticipating mathematical tasks7. High- compared to low-level MA has also
been associated with stronger deactivation within the default mode network during tasks that require additional
inhibitory functions, possibly reecting depletion of working memory resources4. Further, increased activity in
frontoparietal regions in high-level MA adults when anticipating mathematical tasks has been shown to corre-
spond with reduced performance decits, suggesting the role of cognitive control in MA8. In children9, high-level
MA has been associated with hyperactivity in the right amygdala when solving mathematical problems, and
increased connectivity between this and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, areas implicated in the process-
ing and regulation of negative emotions. Compared with low-level MA, high-level MA has been linked with
decreased activity in brain areas involved in working memory and attention, including the dorsolateral prefron-
tal cortex, and reduced activity in posterior parietal areas, critical to numerical processing. Furthermore, brain
1Aston Brain Centre, School of Life and Health Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham, UK. 2Department of
Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. 3School of Experimental Psychology, University
of Bristol, Bristol, UK. 4Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy. 5Departamento de Psicología Básica y
Metodología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain. 6Departamento de Psicología Evolutiva
y de la Educación, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain. 7Servicio de Radiología, Hospital
Morales Meseguer, Murcia, Spain. Charlotte E. Hartwright and Chung Yen Looi contributed equally to this work.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to C.E.H. (email: c.hartwright@aston.ac.uk)
Received: 13 July 2017
Accepted: 22 May 2018
Published: xx xx xxxx
OPEN
Correction: Publisher Correction
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has shown the ability to improve arithmetic performance
and reduced cortisol level in those with high-level MA10. To our knowledge, there are no prior published studies
assessing the link between brain structure and MA in children or adults.
A recent review11 highlighted that there are specic aspects of numerical and executive function that might
explain varying degrees of MA: lower working memory capacity, reduced attentional control, lower inhibitory
control and a decit in low-level numerical representations. Furthermore, increasing levels of MA may negatively
aect mathematical achievement, via disruption of core executive functions and/or decit in low-level numerical
representations. Using data collected from a larger study, we sought to test the strength of association made by
those predictions in that review, to better understand the neural and cognitive factors that are associated with the
degree of anxiety towards mathematics. We aimed to identify the structural brain correlates of the level of MA
in a typical school population, and to determine how brain structure mediates the relationship between those
cognitive functions that are most strongly predictive of the level of MA. Much of the prior literature has con-
sisted of between-group comparisons and, whilst these have provided important insights on the plausible neural
mechanisms of MA, the neural and cognitive architecture that contributes to individual dierences in MA, and
its association with mathematical achievement, remains unclear.
e present study comprises brain structure and MA data from 79 healthy, Spanish children. e Behavioural
Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)12,13 was used to prole each child’s behavior in specic domains
of executive function. e BRIEF indices that were of primary interest for the present study were: inhibitory
control and impulsivity (INHBIT), ability to switch and alternate attention (SHIFT) and on-line, representational
memory (WORKING MEMORY). e BRIEF is regularly used in clinical and education settings, where higher
scores indicate greater diculty, and therefore lower capacity, in each domain. Numerical representation skills
were assessed using a number line task, consisting of positioning numbers on an analogue scale (PN)14, mathe-
matical achievement was determined using the Woodcock Johnson III Achievement (WJ)15 and the level of MA
was established via the Math Anxiety Scale (Math-AS)16. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to identify
brain-structure correlates of MA.
Method
Participants. Participants were recruited through two state primary schools in Murcia, Spain, as part of a
wider study17–19. e primary sample comprised 137 Spanish children, aged 7–12 years (2nd – 6th grade). Written,
informed consent was obtained from parents prior to acquiring any data, and verbal consent reobtained imme-
diately prior to data acquisition. Parents were advised that they would be informed by the hospital Radiologist
if any clinically relevant abnormalities were detected. T1-weighted structural MRI data were acquired from an
initial sample comprising 110 children whose parents gave previous informed consent. Two were not included in
the current study as they were reported to be bilingual, which may aect the measurement of math anxiety and
numerical achievement. A further 7 were excluded due to learning disabilities. Note that children with learning
disabilities were diagnosed before our study and were receiving special education from their schools. We admin-
istered tests on all children to ensure that no one felt excluded. We only analysed data of children without learning
disabilities. Following exclusion due to unsatisfactory image quality resulting from movement- or other imaging
artefacts (n = 21) or neuro-incidental ndings (n = 1), the nal sample comprised 79 children (age M = 115.20,
SD = 14.13; males = 50.6%; right-handed = 88.6%). e study was approved by the University of Murcia Ethics
Committee, and it was conducted in accordance with the approved guidelines and the Declaration of Helsinki.
Materials. Measures that were modeled in the current study include math anxiety, mathematical abilities and
working memory.
Math Anxiety. MA was assessed using the Math-AS (known also as the EAM, Escala de Ansiedade a
Matemática16). It consists of 25 items that describe situations that are commonly experienced by elementary and
high school students during their math lessons. is scale measures the variations in the degrees of math anxiety,
from absence to extreme math anxiety. is task was translated from Portuguese into European Spanish by a
speaker uent in both languages (FHS) to enable cross-cultural adaptation. Children indicated the intensity of
their response to each item on a ve-point Likert scale by crossing out one of the following: (1) None (2) Low (3)
Moderate (4) High and (5) Very High. e score was the sum of all points from the 25 questions. is measure has
been shown to have accurate validity and reliability when used with children20. Note that we could not control for
general anxiety in our analyses however, as we did not have normative data for children below 9 years old using
the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-CH)21. To be condent that we measured MA and not gen-
eral anxiety trait, we ran a correlation analysis on the standardised scores of the 50 out of 79 children. We found a
lack of correlation between general anxiety (STAI-CH) and MA, rp = 0.07, p = 0.63.
Numerical Cognition. Math Achievement. Children’s math abilities were assessed using the Spanish ver-
sion of the Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ-III) Achievement (ACH) battery15, which has been validated for the use
of children aged 6–13 years in Spain22. It comprises 4 subtests: calculation, math uency, quantitative concepts
and applied problems (see Supplementary Information: Method). e raw scores of each subtest were transformed
into W scores23 following the Rasch’s measurement model24,25. We used the composite score of all 4 subtests in
our analyses.
Numerical Representation. Numerical representation was assessed using a number line task, consisting of
positioning numbers on an analogue scale (PN)14. e participants were required to map numbers on a vertical
line that was marked with “0” at the bottom and “100” at the top. In half of the trials, the line was further marked
with 4 horizontal lines at dierent locations to assist children with number mapping. Children were required
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
3
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
to indicate the position of an Arabic numeral, orally or visually presented by the experimenter, by pointing to a
specic location on the line. ere were 12 trials in this task.
Executive Function. We used parents’ rating of children’s online memory (WORKING MEMORY), atten-
tion (SHIFT) and inhibitory control (INHIBIT) using the Spanish version of the Behavioural Rating Inventory of
Executive Function, BRIEFTM 12,13, which assesses the executive functioning of children between 5–18 years old.
It contains 86 items in eight non-overlapping clinical scales and two validity scales.
Procedure. Cognitive and Achievement Testing. Children’s performance on a range of behavioural, cognitive
and achievement tasks was assessed prior to collecting the structural MRI scans. Testing was conducted during
the Autumn term by six trained assistants with children in groups of two.
Analysis of Demographic, Cognitive and Achievement Data. Several of the measures resulted in positively
skewed data. Parametric statistics were combined with permutation testing as this approach, in contrast with
non-parametric analyses, has been shown to maximally reduce type I and type II errors26. Condence inter-
vals (CIs) were estimated using the bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) percentile bootstrap method (10,000
samples).
e cognitive and achievement data were analysed using SPSS, version 22. e mediation analysis was con-
ducted using the Process macro for SPSS (v2.16.3), available from http://www.processmacro.org/index.html fol-
lowing a published analysis pipeline27. Ten-thousand bootstrap resamples were used to generate bias-corrected,
95% condence intervals.
Neuroimaging Acquisition. e participants were tted with ear plugs and so foam padding used to minimize
head movement during the scan. Participants were asked to remain as still as possible for the duration of the
scan, and a parent sat beside their child throughout. A T1-weighted image was acquired for each participant
using a 1.5 T GE HDX scanner with an 8-channel, phased array, transmit-receive head coil. A 3D FSPGR BRAVO
sequence was used to achieve whole brain coverage, composed of 142 axially oriented slices with a reconstructed
voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, where TR = 12.4 ms, TE = 5.2 ms, ip angle = 12°.
Neuroimaging Analysis using Voxel-Based Morphometry. e MRI data were analyzed using the FMRIB Soware
Library (FSL, version 6.0.0; http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). Non-brain tissue was removed from the struc-
tural images and an initial weak bias eld correction applied using FSL’s anatomy pipeline (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.
uk/fsl/fslwiki/fsl_anat). ese brain extracted, bias corrected images were then fed into the second and subsequent
stages of FSL-VBM28 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLVBM), an optimised VBM protocol29. e images
were grey matter-segmented and registered to the MNI-152 standard space using non-linear registration30. e
resulting images were averaged and ipped along the x-axis to create a le-right symmetric, study-specic grey
matter template. All native grey matter images were then non-linearly registered to this study-specic template
and “modulated” to correct for local expansion (or contraction) due to the non-linear component of the spatial
transformation. e modulated grey matter images were then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with
a sigma of 3 mm, Full-Width-Half-Maximum (FWHM) ~7 mm. Finally, voxelwise general linear modelling was
applied using Randomise31 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Randomise), which permits permutation-based
non-parametric testing, correcting for multiple comparisons across space. Here, 10,000 permutations of the
data were generated to test against the null. reshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE)32 was used to identify
cluster-like structures, taking family-wise error rate (FWE) corrected p-values < 0.05. To avoid any labelling bias,
probabilistic anatomical descriptors were determined using FSL Atlas Query (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/
Atlasquery). Anatomical labels were output for voxels that had survived multiple-comparison correction. Cluster
peak information was extracted using FSL’s Cluster tool. See Supplementary Information: Method for a detailed
description of the GLM analyses.
Data availability. e research meta-data supporting this publication are available on the Open Science
Framework repository, see DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/PDFJE. e senior author, LJF, may be contacted regarding the
wider dataset.
Results
Math Anxiety and its Association with Demographic, Cognitive and Numerical Factors. Table1 out-
lines the sample’s demographic, numerical and cognitive characteristics (see also Supplementary Information: TableS1
for a detailed breakdown by grade). Typically, the level of MA within the sample was low. An independent-sam-
ples t-test determined that there was no dierence in the mean level of MA between the sexes (t(65.569) = −1.063,
p = 0.292, 95% CI −13.21, 3.569). Age was positively associated with the level of MA (r(79) = 0.237, p = 0.035, CI 0.031,
0.433), consistent with previous studies33 (see Supplementary Information: Fig.S1).
To assess the relationship between MA, numerical- and cognitive-function we conducted a series of partial
correlations. Each of the three BRIEF indices of interest (INHIBIT, SHIFT, WORKING MEMORY), plus the
PN and WJ were correlated with the Math-AS score (controlling for age and biological sex). SHIFT, WORKING
MEMORY and WJ were statistically signicant aer applying a Bonferroni correction (Table2).
Math Anxiety and its Association with the Brain. VBM was used to identify the structural correlates
of MA. A general linear model (GLM) comprising Math-AS score and the nuisance variables, age, biological
sex, recruitment source and handedness was created. Contrasts for positive and negative associations between
grey matter volume (GMV) and MA were computed, where corrections were applied for multiple comparisons
across the brain, and adjustment to correct for running two contrasts using a Bonferroni correction. A whole
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
4
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
brain analysis did not identify any regions that were positively associated with MA. Four clusters demonstrated a
negative association between GMV and MA, which encompassed only cortical grey matter (Table3 and Fig.1).
e largest cluster spanned both hemispheres across occipital and parietal cortices, including a section running
anterior-posterior along the le anterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS), in areas hIP1 and hIP3, as dened by the
Juelich Histological Atlas. A second, smaller cluster was identied in the right visual cortices, encompassing
extrastriate areas and additionally encompassing the le anterior IPS. Lastly, a small, le lateralized cluster was
identied in the inferior parietal lobule. e Bonferroni correction had split this latter cluster, resulting in a fur-
ther cluster in the lateral occipital cortex. As this comprised a single voxel (MNI: −18, −88, 42), this voxel was
excluded from subsequent analyses.
To understand the function of these structural correlates of MA, a further GLM was constructed comprising
the Math-AS scores and nuisance variables as outlined previously, plus those variables that had earlier shown
the most robust associations with the level of MA: attentional control (SHIFT), online memory (WORKING
MEMORY) and mathematical achievement (WJ). is GLM was applied to those voxels that were previously
identied as being negatively associated with MA.
Domain Scale/Index nmean SD min max
Demographic
Age (months) — 115.20 14.13 95.00 145.00
School grade — 4 — 2 6
Sex (M/F) 40/39 — — — —
Handedness (L/R) 9/70 — — — —
Numerical Cognition
Math Anxiety Scale 79 43.52 19.35 25 101
Woodcock-Johnson III Achievement 79 128.15 29.64 65 208
Number line task 73 7.51 2.18 2.00 10.50
Executive Function (BRIEF)
Initiate 71 12.92 2.99 8 22
Working Memory*71 17.49 4.62 10 27
Plan 71 19.87 5.43 12 33
Organization 71 10.75 3.28 6 18
Monitor 71 13.63 3.20 8 22
Inhibit*71 15.34 3.53 10 27
Shi*71 12.69 3.05 8 21
Emotional Control 71 16.49 4.21 10 28
Behavioral Regulation Index 71 43.96 9.41 18 69
Metacognition Index 71 74.69 16.70 48 118
Global Executive Composite 71 119.21 24.32 79 184
Table 1. Sample Characteristics. Note. * indicates BRIEF indices of primary interest due to prior published
associations with Math Anxiety.
Domain Math Anxiety
(MA)
Numerical
representation
(PN)
Math
Achievement
(WJ)
BRIEF indices
Inhibitory C ontrol
(INHIBIT) Attentional
Control (SHIFT) Online Memory
(WORKING MEMORY)
Math Anxiety (MA) Pe ar son’s r —−0.112 −0.302 0.278 0.320 0.313
p-value — 0.351 0.008*0.021 0.007*0.009*
Numerical Representation
(PN)
Pe ar so n’s r —0.384 −0.189 −0.155 −0.321
p-value — 0.002 0.138 0.226 0.010
Math Achievement
(WJ)
Pe ar so n’s r —−0.197 −0.127 −0.465
p-value — 0.104 0.299 <0.001
Inhibitory C ontrol
(INHIBIT)
Pe ar so n’s r —0.452 0.483
p-value — <0.001 <0.001
Attentional Control
(SHIFT)
Pe ar so n’s r —0.602
p-value — <0.001
Online Memory
(WORKING MEMORY)
Pe ar so n’s r —
p-value —
Table 2. Association between Math Anxiety, Numerical and Cognitive Indices. Note. p-values reect two-tailed
partial correlation analyses, where age and biological sex was held constant. Degrees of freedom (df) = 69 for
MA * PN; df = 75 for MA * WJ; df = 67 for all BRIEF indices. * indicates associations that survive Bonferroni
corrected alpha for the 5 primary analyses of interest (rst row; Bonferroni corrected alpha, p < 0.01). B Ca
Bootstrapped 95% CI (10,000 samples) for statistically signicant, primary MA analyses: MA * WJ lower −0.455
upper −0.133; MA * SHIFT lower 0.074, upper 0.512; MA * WORKING MEMORY lower 0.052, upper 0.529.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
5
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
When controlling for attention, working memory and mathematical achievement, a large number of those
voxels initially found to be negatively associated with MA were no longer statistically signicant, particularly
within the IPS. Although this result alone is insucient to determine a dierence, the data suggest that the asso-
ciation between MA and GMV in these voxels may be mediated by one or more of the newly modelled variables.
HO Anatomical Region Hemi
(L/R) Cluster Size
(voxels) p-value t-value
MNI Coordinates
of Cluster Peak
X Y Z
Lingual Gyrus*R 217 0.006 5.62 12 −66 −6
Intracalcarine Cortex R
Occipital Fusiform Gyrus R
Temporal Occipital Fusiform Cortex R
Precuneous Cortex R
Cuneal Cortex*L 1446 0.002 5.11 −8−82 20
Cuneal Cortex R
Lateral Occipital Cortex sup div (inc. anterior intraparietal sulcus hIP1&3a) L
Precuneous Cortex L & R
Occipital Pole L & R
Supracalcarine Cortex L & R
Intrcalcarine Cortex L
Superier Parietal Lobule (inc. anterior intraparietal sulcus hIP1&3a) L
Angular Gyrus L
Lingual Gyrus L
Lateral Occipital Cortex* (inc. anterior intraparietal sulcus hIP1&3a) L 76 0.017 4.91 −42 −64 34
Angular Gyrus L
Supramarginal Gyrus, posterior division L
Table 3. Probabilistic Labels for Brain Regions where Grey Matter Volume is Negatively Associated with Math
Anxiety. Note. Anatomical labels taken from the Harvard-Oxford (HO) Atlas bundled with FSL 6.0.0. *Cluster
peak. aReects anatomical label from Jeulich Histological Atlas available with FSL 6.0.0. p- and t-values reect
cluster peak. Labels are only reported from regions which survived correction at the voxel level, and subsequent
Bonferroni correction.
Figure 1. Grey matter correlates of Math Anxiety. Surface rendered image reects a t-statistic cluster map
rendered onto a template brain. All coloured areas reect those grey matter voxels that were signicantly
negatively associated with MA aer multiple comparison corrections as outlined in the method. e top right
panel illustrates the results transformed and rendered onto a single participant’s annoymised T1-structural
image. All images are presented in neurological convention, where the le of the image reects the le of the
brain. Surface rendering created using Surf Ice42; Individual subject rendering created using Mango43.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
6
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
To assess this possibility, the linear directional contrasts for each newly modelled variable werecomputed. ese
data suggested that, of the additional variables added, attentional control was negatively associated with a large
proportion of those voxels that were no longer associated with MA (see Supplementary Information: Fig.S2).
However, this result did not survive correction for multiple-comparisons. Working memory and math achieve-
ment did not explain the association with MA (see Supplementary Information for detailed modelling procedure).
To further assess this, the mean GMV was extracted for each of the 3 clusters identied in the earlier VBM
analysis. ough large, the spatial arrangement of these clusters may demarcate dierentiation of function, as
each comprised relatively distinct anatomical regions: lingual gyrus, cuneal cortex and the intraparietal sulcus.
SHIFT, WORKING MEMORY and WJ were entered into a series of partial correlations to examine their rela-
tionship with GMV within each of the 3 clusters. When controlling for age, biological sex and Math-AS score,
attentional control (SHIFT) was shown to be negatively associated with GMV across the IPS, although this result
did not survive a Bonferroni correction for 9 tests. All other tests were non-signicant at the uncorrected level
(see Table4).
The Cognitive Architecture of MA and Resultant Outcomes in Numerical Achievement.
Suárez-Pellicioni and colleagues11 outline a model, the processing eciency theory34 in which intrusive thoughts
resultant from MA consume working memory. This, in turn, is argued to expend already limited attentional
resources in high MA individuals, leading to diminished performance when complex mathematical operations are
performed. Such a relationship provides a causal explanation for the association between MA (Math-AS) and mathe-
matical achievement (WJ). We tested this theoretical model using mediation analysis. When controlling for working
memory and the nuisance variables age and biological sex, the relationship between MA and math achievement was
no longer signicant (c′) (see Table5 and Fig.2). e overall mediation model found that MA, working memory, age
and biological sex explained approximately 55% of the variance in math achievement, (R2 = 0.5527, F(4, 66) = 20.39,
p < 0.0001). Consistent with this model, the mediation analysis suggested that higher levels of MA (Math-AS) were
associated with slightly elevated diculty with holding appropriate information in mind (WORKING MEMORY),
which in turn resulted in reduced math achievement (WJ).
Discussion
e current study sought to examine the neurocognitive bases of MA in typically developing children. Unlike
most prior work, however, the analyses focused on the level of MA, which is a more rigorous approach than
making comparisons based on the presence or absence or of it35. is work therefore provides both a descrip-
tion of how MA might manifest itself across a cohort of typical children, as well as a detailed evaluation of
theoretically-driven factors that might inuence the degree of MA.
Most of the children in the sample demonstrated low-levels of MA. Around 10% of the entire sample reported
moderate to high-levels of MA, and prevalence increased linearly with age. Aside from numerical representations,
all other cognitive variables that were of theoretical interest were associated with the degree of MA: the more
math anxious children demonstrated reduced inhibitory and attentional control, as well as lower working mem-
ory capacity and math achievement. ough MA appears to be associated with dierences in baseline executive
functions in children with MA, working memory was also shown to mediate the relationship between MA and
math achievement suggesting that, possibly in addition to baseline dierences, capacity issues may be exacerbated
‘online’ when working with mathematics by the physiological response to anxiety, which in turn leads to poorer
learning and performance in mathematics.
Contrary to previous studies36,37, we found no association between MA and low-level numerical representa-
tions. ere has been considerable debate regarding the presence of a low-level decit in math ability in MA11.
It may be that the task used here was not ne-grained enough to highlight any association, although note that
the data from the task used did correlate with the measure of math achievement, suggesting that the task was
tapping into numerical cognition to some degree. e current pattern of results is also indicative of experiential
dierences between high and low math anxious individuals, as MA may result in avoidance of exposure to math
Domain Math Achievement
(WJ)
BRIEF INDICES
Attentional
Control (SHIFT) Online Memory
(WORKING MEMORY)
Lingual Gyrus*Pe ar so n’s r −0.086 −0.070 0.051
p-value 0.460 0.572 0.678
Cuneal Cortex*Pe ar so n’s r −0.168 −0.123 0.086
p-value 0.148 0.318 0.487
Lateral Occipital Cortex*Pe ar son ’s r −0.112 −0.318 −0.066
/anterior Intraparietal Sulcus hIP1&3** p-value 0.334 0.008 0.592
Table 4. Partial Correlation Results for Regional Grey Matter Volume, Numerical and Cognitive Indices. Note.
p-values reect two-tailed partial correlation analyses, where MAS score, age and biological sex is held constant.
Degrees of freedom (df) = 74 for WJ; df = 66 for BRIEF indices. BCa Bootstrapped 95% CI (10,000 samples)
for SHIFT * IPS, lower −0.513, upper −0.108. No analyses survive Bonferroni corrected alpha for 9 tests of
interest (corrected alpha, p < 0.0056). *Anatomical descriptors reect VBM cluster peak and are taken from the
Harvard-Oxford (HO) Atlas bundled with FSL 6.0.0. **Reects anatomical label from Jeulich Histological Atlas
available with FSL 6.0.0.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
7
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
problems38. Moreover, an experientially-driven reduction in math achievement does not discount a primary,
causal decit in numerical representation; it may be that more extreme levels of MA reect both causal (rep-
resentational) and aectual (experiential) mechanisms. Indeed, cross-sectional work strongly suggests the pres-
ence of a less precise representation of numerical representation in adults with high MA37.
e present study also identied that reduced grey matter volume in the IPS, lingual gyrus and cuneal
cortex was associated with increased MA. e identication of a structural correlate in relatively young chil-
dren suggests that there may be dierences in early brain structure that underpin the development of MA.
us, whilst prior functional imaging studies demonstrate how network functionality can explain dierences
in mathematical performance in people with math anxiety8,9, these data provide the rst evidence of a possible
underlying structural basis. Genetic modelling suggests that around 40% of variance in MA6 can be explained
Path Estimate p-value
BCa 95% CI
Lower Upper
Total eect (c)−0.3639 0.0058 −0.6188 −0.1091
Direct eect (c’)−0.2247 0.0743 −0.4721 0.0227
a 0.0758 0.0088 0.0197 0.1318
b−1.8378 0.0006 −2.8603 −0.8154
Indirect eect
ab −0.1392 — −0.3074 −0.0282
Table 5. Mediation Path Coecients and Condence Intervals for Math Anxiety Predicting Math
Achievement. Note. BCa condence intervals (CI) reect 10,000 samples.
Figure 2. Summary coecients for mediation model. Note. Path a = unstandardised IV to mediator; path
b = unstandardised mediator to DV; path c = unstandardised total eect (IV to DV); path c′ = unstandardised
direct eect. Coecient values rounded to 2 decimal places; full values reported in accompanying table (3).
ns = non-signicant *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001. Note. Higher working memory values indicate lower working
memory.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
8
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
by heredity, thus, a neurodevelopmental precursor is not implausible. Prior work has implicated the IPS in a
network of regions showing aberrant activity in young, math anxious children, where these patterns of activa-
tion were unrelated to intelligence, general anxiety, reading ability or working memory9. Our data suggest that
the IPS region identied in the current study might serve an attentional function, where children with reduced
grey matter in this area had lower reported attentional resources, and this reduced attentional capacity was
associated with increased MA. is is consistent with the assumption of the processing eciency theory34 that
high MA individuals may already have a limited attentional resource, and MA would further consume work-
ing memory, contributing to lower performance on complex mathematical operations. Future studies could
investigate whether this reduced grey matter in the IPS is a structural adaptation due to reduced attentional
capacity or compensatory strategies associated with MA39, or a potential biomarker on the causal pathway of
the development of MA. One model that should be examined in future research is that children with MA, or
who go on to develop MA, start o with dierences in IPS structure, which translate into a decit in baseline
attentional capacity. According to this view, a limited ability to attend to stimuli, particularly mathematical
stimuli – where demands on attentional resources are oen high due to the nature of arithmetic problems –
could result in general feelings of anxiety, which later become habitually associated with doing math, causing
the development of MA.
In addition to providing a rich description of the neurocognitive bases of the level of math anxiety, the current
study provides testable hypotheses regarding the emergence and maintenance of MA. Having a more nuanced
understanding of the neurocognitive prole of MA, including any impairments that may cause, contribute to,
or result from MA, has important implications for the development of targeted, individualized intervention.
Longitudinal and cross-sectional work to prole MA against developmental dyscalculia, which may appear qual-
itatively similar40, is required, however, to assess the validity of these assertions.
Limitations and Suggested Future Directions. e review by Suárez-Pellicioni and colleagues11 out-
lines three core explanations of MA: (1) Task-related competition for working memory resources (2) A decit
in low-level numerical representation (3) Math anxiety as an inhibition/attentional-control decit. Using data
collected from a wider study, we evaluated each of these, combining paediatric MRI data from typically develop-
ing children to advance a neurocognitive model of the level of MA. Whilst our results address some of the core
areas of interest in the cognitive literature, the present study does not, however, provide an assessment across
all cognitive domains, so cannot be considered an exhaustive evaluation of the neurocognitive architecture of
MA. Moreover, our executive function measures were derived from parental report. Although the measure of
executive function administered is used extensively in education and clinical settings, a standardized, automated
neurocognitive test battery may provide more valid data regarding performance and capacity.
Future work should include measures sampled ‘online’ whilst performing numerical tasks, based on
event-related potentials, functional magnetic resonance imaging or autonomic measurement, for example. Whilst
such work has been conducted with adults, there has been little progress towards developing a dynamic view of
how individual dierences in children’s physiological responses to numerical stimuli vary as a function of MA.
Importantly, such an approach could identify further potential antecedents of MA; thus, permitting directed
early intervention ideally to prevent, or at least reduce, the negative consequences of MA. Similarly, multimodal
approaches could achieve deeper evidence regarding the functional properties of MA and, if approached longi-
tudinally, its emergence could be evaluated. Still, though longitudinal approaches permit causal inference, they
attract signicant technical and practical challenges41.
References
1. Hembree, . e nature, eects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for research in mathematics education, 33–46 (1990).
2. Devine, A., Fawcett, ., Szűcs, D. & Dower, A. Gender differences in mathematics anxiety and the relation to mathematics
performance while controlling for test anxiety. Behavioral and brain functions 8, 1–9 (2012).
3. rinzinger, H., aufmann, L. & Willmes, . Math Anxiety and Math Ability in Early Primary School Years. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 27,
206–225, https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282908330583 (2009).
4. Pletzer, B., ronbichler, M., Nuer, H.-C. & erschbaum, H. H. Mathematics anxiety reduces default mode networ deactivation in
response to numerical tass. Frontiers in human neuroscience 9, 1–12 (2015).
5. Brown, M., Brown, P. & Bibby, T. “I would rather die”: easons given by 16-year-olds for not continuing their study of mathematics.
Research in Mathematics Education 10, 3–18 (2008).
6. Wang, Z. et al. Who is afraid of math? Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry 55, 1056–1064, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12224 (2014).
7. Lyons, I. M. & Beiloc, S. L. When math hurts: math anxiety predicts pain networ activation in anticipation of doing math. PloS one
7, e48076 (2012).
8. Lyons, I. M. & Beiloc, S. L. Mathematics anxiety: separating the math from the anxiety. Cerebral cortex 22, 2102–2110 (2012).
9. Young, C. B., Wu, S. S. & Menon, V. e neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science 23, 492–501 (2012).
10. Sarar, A., Dower, A. & adosh, . Cognitive Enhancement or Cognitive Cost: Trait-Specic Outcomes of Brain Stimulation in the
Case of Mathematics Anxiety. Journal of Neuroscience 34, 16605–16610, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUOSCI.3129-14.2014 (2014).
11. Suárez-Pellicioni, M., Núñez-Peña, M. I. & Colomé, À. Math anxiety: A review of its cognitive consequences, psychophysiological
correlates, and brain bases. Cognitive, Aective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 16, 3–22, https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7
(2016).
12. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. ., Guy, S. C. & enworthy, L. (Psychological Assessment esources, Inc, 2000).
13. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. ., Guy, S. C. & enworthy, L. TEST EVIEW Behavior ating Inventory of Executive Function. Child
Neuropsychology 6, 235–238, https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152 (2000).
14. Dos Santos, F. H. et al. Number processing and calculation in Brazilian children aged 7–12 years. e Spanish Journal of Psychology
15, 513–525 (2012).
15. Woodcoc, . W., McGrew, . S. & Mather, N. Woodcock Johnson III. (e iverside Publishing Company, 2001).
16. Carmo, J. S. et al. In Sobre comportamento e cognição: aspectos teóricos, metodológicos e de formação em análise do comportamento e
terapia cognitivista (ed. Silva, W. C. M. P.) 213–221 (SP: Esetec, 2008).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
9
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
17. Sánchez-Pérez, N., Fuentes, L., López-López, J. A., Pina, V. & González-Salinas, C. How do dierent components of Eortful Control
contribute to children’s mathematics achievement? Frontiers in Psychology 6, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01383 (2015).
18. Pina, V., Castillo, A., Cohen adosh, . & Fuentes, L. J. Intentional and automatic numerical processing as predictors of
mathematical abilities in primary school children. Frontiers in Psychology 6, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00375 (2015).
19. Sanchez-Perez, N., Fuentes, L. J., Jollie, D. & Gonzalez-Salinas, C. Assessing children’s empathy through a Spanish adaptation of the
Basic Empathy Scale: parent’s and child’s report forms. Front Psychol 5, 1438, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01438 (2014).
20. Mendes, A. C. Identicação de graus de ansiedade à matemática em estudantes do ensino fundamental e médio: contribuições à
validação de uma escala de ansiedade à matemática, Universidade Federal de São Carlos, (2012).
21. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, . L., Lushene, . E., Vagg, P. . & Jacobs, G. A. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. (Consulting
Psychologists Press, 1983).
22. Diamantopoulou, S., Pina, V., Valero-Garcia, A. V., González-Salinas, C. & Fuentes, L. J. Validation of the spanish version of the
woodcoc-johnson mathematics achievement tests for children aged 6 to 13. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 30, 466–477
(2012).
23. Woodcoc, . W. Development and standardization of the Woodcock-Johnson psycho-educational battery. (Teaching esources,
1978).
24. asch, G. Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligent and Attainment Tests Copenhagen. Denmark: Institute of Educational Research
(1960).
25. Wright, B. D. & Stone, M. H. Best Test Design. asch Measurement (1979).
26. Bishara, A. J. & Hittner, J. B. Testing the signicance of a correlation with nonnormal data: comparison of Pearson, Spearman,
transformation, and resampling approaches. Psychol Methods 17, 399–417, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028087 (2012).
27. Hayes, A. F. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. [electronic resource]: a regression-based
approach. (New Yor: e Guilford Press, [2013], 2013).
28. Douaud, G. et al. Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130,
2375–2386, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm184 (2007).
29. Good, C. D. et al. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14, 21–36, https://
doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0786 (2001).
30. Andersson, J. L. ., Jeninson, M. & Smith, S. M. Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation www.fmrib.ox.ac.u/analysis/
techrep (2007).
31. Winler, A. M., idgway, G. ., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M. & Nichols, T. E. Permutation inference for the general linear model.
Neuroimage 92, 381–397, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 (2014).
32. Smith, S. M. & Nichols, T. E. reshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and
localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 (2009).
33. Dower, A., Sarar, A. & Looi, C. Y. MathematicsAnxiety: What Have We Learned in 60 Years? Frontiers in Psychology7, https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508 (2016).
34. Eysenc, M. & Calvo, M. Anxiety and Performance - the Processing Eciency eory. Cognition & Emotion 6, 409–434, https://doi.
org/10.1080/02699939208409696 (1992).
35. Preacher, . J., ucer, D. D., MacCallum, . C. & Nicewander, W. A. Use of the extreme groups approach: A critical reexamination
and new recommendations. Psychological Methods 10, 178–192, https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.10.2.178 (2005).
36. Georges, C., Homann, D. & Schiltz, C. How Math Anxiety elates to Number–Space Associations. Frontiers in Psychology 7,
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01401 (2016).
37. Maloney, E. A., Ansari, D. & Fugelsang, J. A. e eect of mathematics anxiety on the processing of numerical magnitude. e
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64, 10–16, https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.533278 (2011).
38. Ashcra, M. H. & Faust, M. W. Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cognition
and Emotion 8, 97–125, https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939408408931 (1994).
39. Eysenc, M., Derashan, N., Santos, . & Calvo, M. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion 7,
336–353, https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 (2007).
40. Mammarella, I. C., Hill, F., Devine, A., Caviola, S. & Szűcs, D. Math anxiety and developmental dyscalculia: a study on woring
memory processes. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology 37, 878–887 (2015).
41. Mills, . L. & Tamnes, C. . Methods and considerations for longitudinal structural brain imaging analysis across development.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 9, 172–190, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.04.004 (2014).
42. Surf Ice v. 10.11.16 (McCausland Center for Brain Imaging, University of South Carolina, South Carolina, 2015).
43. Mango v. 4.0 (esearch Imaging Institute University of Texas Health Science Centre, Texas, 2016).
Acknowledgements
Authors CEH FS, and RCK were supported by the European Research Council (Learning and Achievement,
award number 338065). Authors LJF, FHS, JMGS and CGS were supported by grant PSI2017-84556-P from
Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness (FEDER funding).
Author Contributions
L.J.F. conceived and designed the over-arching study. All authors contributed to the math anxiety study concept.
F.H.S. translated the Math-AS into Spanish and organized and interpreted the Math-AS scores. C.G.S. organized
and interpreted the BR.IEF questionnaire. L.J.F., A.I. and J.M.G.S. designed and implemented the MRI protocol.
L.JF., A.I., F.H.S., C.G.S. and J.M.G.S. oversaw the testing and data collection. C.E.H., C.Y.L., F.S. and R.C.K.
were given access to the subset of data described in this article to examine math anxiety, following L.J.F. taking a
sabbatical within RCK’s laboratory at the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford. C.Y.L.
contributed to the organization of test measures and raw data. C.E.H., C.Y.L. and F.S. conducted the behavioural
analysis. C.E.H. conducted the neuroimaging analyses. C.E.H. and C.Y.L. draed the manuscript. L.J.F., R.C.K.,
F.S., A.I., F.H.S., C.G.S. and J.M.G.S. provided critical revisions of the manuscript. All authors approved the nal
version of the manuscript for submission.
Additional Information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5.
Competing Interests: e authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
10
SCIEnTIFIC REpoRTS | (2018) 8:8500 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2018
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com