ArticlePDF Available

Sleep divergently affects cognitive and automatic emotional response in children

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Sleep enhances memory for emotional experiences, but its influence on the emotional response associated with memories is elusive. Here, we compared the influence of nocturnal sleep on memory for negative and neutral pictures and the associated emotional response in 8-11-year-old children, i.e., an age group with heightened levels of emotional memory-related sleep features. During all sessions, emotional responses as measured by subjective ratings, the late positive potential of the EEG (LPP) and heart rate deceleration (HRD) were recorded. Sleep enhanced picture memory. Compared to dynamics across wakefulness, sleep decreased the emotional response in ratings and the LPP, while increasing the emotional response in HRD. We conclude that sleep consolidates immediate emotional meaning by enhancing more automatic emotional responses while concurrently promoting top-down control of emotional responses, perhaps through strengthening respective neocortical representations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia
Sleep divergently aects cognitive and automatic emotional response in
children
Elaina Bolinger
a
, Jan Born
a,b,
, Katharina Zinke
a,⁎⁎
a
Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, 72076, Germany
b
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, 72076, Germany
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Development
Children
Sleep
Emotion
EEG
Memory
ABSTRACT
Sleep enhances memory for emotional experiences, but its inuence on the emotional response associated with
memories is elusive. Here, we compared the inuence of nocturnal sleep on memory for negative and neutral
pictures and the associated emotional response in 811-year-old children, i.e., an age group with heightened
levels of emotional memory-related sleep features. During all sessions, emotional responses as measured by
subjective ratings, the late positive potential of the EEG (LPP) and heart rate deceleration (HRD) were recorded.
Sleep enhanced picture memory. Compared to dynamics across wakefulness, sleep decreased the emotional
response in ratings and the LPP, while increasing the emotional response in HRD. We conclude that sleep
consolidates immediate emotional meaning by enhancing more automatic emotional responses while con-
currently promoting top-down control of emotional responses, perhaps through strengthening respective neo-
cortical representations.
1. Introduction
Whereas research has revealed a benet of sleep on emotional
memory (e.g. Hu et al., 2006;Nishida et al., 2009;Wagner et al., 2001),
sleep's inuence on the emotional reactivity associated with a memory
has produced complex and seemingly disparate results. Sleep has been
shown to increase (Wagner et al., 2002), decrease (Pace-Schott et al.,
2011; van der Helm et al., 2011), and preserve (Baran et al., 2012;
Pace-Schott et al., 2011; Werner et al., 2015) the emotional tone as-
sociated with memories. Interestingly, a central role of rapid eye
movement (REM) sleep has consistently emerged, leading to two con-
icting theories on its role in emotional processing. The Sleep to Forget,
Sleep to Remember (SFSR) theory proposes that REM sleep specically
decreases emotional tone by providing an opportunity to process
emotional memories within a neurochemical environment that supports
memory consolidation in an emotionally neutral setting (Walker,
2009). The SFSR theory has received some support, with work sug-
gesting REM physiology is linked to decreases in emotional tone (van
der Helm et al., 2011). The second theory proposes that REM sleep
preserves emotional tone through consolidation of emotional salience
(Baran et al., 2012; Pace-Schott et al., 2011; Werner et al., 2015). In this
framework, REM sleep is assumed to strengthen the link between a
stimulus and its associated immediate emotional meaning (i.e., aversive
or safe), thereby leading to more robust emotional responses to the
stimulus. Emotional salience consolidation (ESC) might be associated
with the enhanced consolidation of emotional memory seen across
sleep.
Emotional scenarios elicit both physiological and behavioral re-
sponses. A prominent physiological response is the tendency of heart
rate to decrease during confrontation with an emotionally relevant
stimulus (Bradley et al., 2001). Encountering emotional stimuli also
elicits an enhanced central nervous response, which can be measured as
an increase in event-related response amplitude in the EEG (i.e. the late
positive potential at Pz, see Cuthbert et al., 2000). While heart rate
deceleration is more automatic in its features, in that it is preattentively
triggered and directly controlled by subcortical structures like the
amygdala and brainstem (Öhman et al., 2000), the late positive po-
tential (LPP) as well as the subjective emotional response (as assessed
by ratings) are modulated by an interplay between the amygdala and
prefrontal cortex (PFC, Hajcak et al., 2010;Ochsner et al., 2012). Im-
portantly, top-down cognitive processes readily inuence both sub-
jective ratings and the LPP (Dennis and Hajcak, 2009; Hajcak et al.,
2010).
Sleep's inuence on emotional tone might depend on the nature of
the emotional response considered, and more specically, on the degree
to which it is inuenced by cognitive processes. The level to which the
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.015
Received 8 November 2017; Received in revised form 9 May 2018; Accepted 17 May 2018
Correspondence to: Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Otfried-Müller-Str. 25, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.
⁎⁎
Correspondence to: Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Silcherstr. 5, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.
E-mail addresses: jan.born@uni-tuebingen.de (J. Born), katharina.zinke@uni-tuebingen.de (K. Zinke).
Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
Available online 19 May 2018
0028-3932/ © 2018 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY-NC-ND/4.0/).
T
HRD dierentially responds to emotional compared to neutral stimuli is
preserved over a nap (Pace-Schott et al., 2011) and HRD responses
decrease after a night of sleep in an emotionally-independent manner
(Cunningham et al., 2014). Thus, more automatically triggered emo-
tional responses might be preserved by sleep. However, more complex
emotional response measures, which recruit cognitive processes to a
greater extent, might become more accessible to top-down control and
thereby decrease over sleep. While some evidence from emotional
ratings supports this notion (van der Helm et al., 2011), other studies
examining sleep on subjective ratings measures have been contra-
dictory (Baran et al., 2012; van der Helm et al., 2011; Wagner et al.,
2001). The LPP, as another measure integrating emotional top-down
control, has thus far only been investigated in one sleep study where it
was related to the emotional enhancement in memory after REM-rich
sleep (Groch et al., 2013).
Children are an excellent model of enhanced emotional processing
during sleep as they exhibit heightened levels of a number of sleep
features which have been associated with emotional memory proces-
sing in adults (i.e. REM & NonREM theta and delta power: Kurth et al.,
2010;Nishida et al., 2009, and NonREM spindle density: Clawson et al.,
2016;Kaestner et al., 2013) and likewise a greater benet of sleep on
emotional memory consolidation than adults (Prehn-Kristensen et al.,
2013). What is more, they tend to have more emotional sleep experi-
ences as evidenced by increased nightmare and night terror frequency
(Simonds and Parraga, 1982). Though emotion robustly inuences
subjective ratings, LPP, HRD, and memory in children (Leventon et al.,
2014), the inuence of sleep on the emotional tone associated with
memories has not yet been reported in this age group.
We investigated how sleep inuences emotional reactivity in 811
year olds. Children encoded negative and neutral pictures and then
either stayed awake or slept in the laboratory during a 10-h retention
period. They then saw the original along with new pictures and were
asked to report if they recognized each picture. During encoding and
recognition testing, subjective (ratings of valence and arousal), central
nervous (LPP of the brain evoked response) and autonomic nervous
(HRD) system measures of the emotional response were assessed,
whereby the emotional response was dened as the response dierence
between negative and neutral pictures. This metric provides a measure
of emotional reaction strength that is corrected for possible dierences
in baseline reactivity (e.g. Coan and Allen, 2007;Phelps et al., 2001).
We expected that, compared to wake, measures of emotion that are
subject to greater cognitive control would decrease after sleep, while
measures of emotion that are less inuenced by cognitive control would
be preserved. In addition, we hypothesized that consolidative inu-
ences of sleep (as measured by emotional response) would be speci-
cally linked to REM sleep.
8:00h
8:00h 20:00h
Old or New?
Encoding Recognition
Old or New?
Recognition
Wake Condition
Encoding
Day
Awake
Sleep Condition
Encoding
Recognition
Night
Asleep
C) Experimental Design
A) Change in Emotional Response B) Recognition Accuracy
D) Emotional Picture Task
*
**
*
Fig. 1. A) Sleep and wake intervals dierentially aected the change in emotional response from encoding to recognition. Whereas sleep decreased emotional
response in valence ratings, the ratings did not change across the wake interval. (Note that the y-axis has been inverted to facilitate interpretation of the valence
response: a positive change is a decrease in emotional response, n= 14). The emotional response of the late positive potential (LPP) of the event-related EEG at Pz
increased over a day of wake but was preserved over a night of sleep. Therefore, relative to wake, sleep decreased the emotional response in LPP (n= 12). The
emotional response in heart rate deceleration (HRD) decreased over the wake interval but was stable after a night of sleep. Thus, relative to wake, sleep increased the
emotional response in HRD (n= 15). B) Sleep enhanced recognition accuracy in general, while wake led to better memory for negative compared to neutral pictures
(n= 16). C) A recognition task was used in a within-subjects crossover design to examine the inuence of post-encoding sleep vs wakefulness on memory and
associated emotional responses in 811 year old children. During Encoding, children viewed emotional pictures in either the evening (Sleep condition) or the
morning (Wake condition). After a 10-h retention interval, children performed a memory task during the Recognition session. During both sessions, subjective
ratings, the LPP of the EEG, and HRD were measured. D) During Encoding, children saw negative and neutral images that they rated with regard to subjective valence
and arousal using the SAM. During Recognition, children saw the original images as well as negative and neutral distractors. Trials were identical to the Encoding
session, with the exception of the additional question Is this picture old or new?which was asked after subjective ratings were performed. * represents p< .05.
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
85
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Sixteen healthy children without sleep disturbances between the
ages of 8 and 11 years (mean ± SD: 9.25 ± 1.06 years, 8 females)
participated in the experiment. Sample size was based on a previous
within-subjects study addressing the inuence of sleep on emotional
response in adults (Groch et al., 2013). All participants were physically
and mentally healthy according to parental report and the Child Be-
havior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991; Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child
Behavior Checklist, 1998), and had normal or corrected-to-normal vi-
sion. Participants had an average digit span forward of 5.57 ± 1.26
and backward of 3.94 ± 1.18 according to the digit span subscale of
the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (Petermann and Petermann, 2011).
Participants did not exhibit sleep disturbances according to parent and
child reports. The study was approved by the University of Tübingen
medical faculty ethics committee and both parents and children gave
informed verbal and written consent.
2.2. Experimental design and procedures
Recognition memory and emotional responses to pictures were
compared between a Sleep and a Wake condition, following a within
[HYPHEN]subjects crossover design where each child was tested on
both conditions with an interval of at least 12 days between each
condition. The order of conditions was randomly assigned to the chil-
dren, with 9 children of the nal sample of 16 children starting with
Sleep condition. Each condition started with an Encoding session,
where the task pictures were presented the rst time, followed by a 10-
h retention period in which the child either slept or was awake, and
then a Recognition session in which the original pictures were pre-
sented along with distractor pictures which had not been presented in
the Encoding session (Fig. 1C and D). In both sessions, valence and
arousal ratings (collected with the self-assessment manikin, SAM;
Bradley and Lang, 1994), the LPP, and HRD responses to the pictures
were measured.
Each child participated in an adaptation session before the experi-
ment proper. For this session, children and parents arrived at the lab at
18:00 h. Children lled out a questionnaire about their sleeping habits
and completed the digit span task. They also practiced the control tasks,
which were measured before each experimental condition, including a
5-min version of the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT, Roach et al.,
2006) to assess vigilance, the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS, Hoddes
et al., 1973) to assess sleepiness, the Positive and Negative Aect
Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988) to assess mood, and a ve-point
Likert-scale assessing motivation (1 = absolutely not motivated, 5 =
extremely motivated). During this time, parents were asked to ll out
the questionnaires about the child's health and sleep behavior. Children
were then prepared for bed and electroencephalographic (EEG), elec-
trocardiographic (ECG), electromyographic (EMG), and electrooculo-
graphic (EOG) recordings. They then slept for ~9 h to ensure accli-
mation to the conditions of the sleep laboratory.
For the Sleep condition, children and parents arrived ~3 h before
the child's habitual bedtime. Children prepared for bed and recording
electrodes were applied (EEG, ECG, EMG and EOG). Children per-
formed the control and main tasks of the Encoding session and then
went to sleep for ~9 h. Upon waking, the children ate a light breakfast
while electrode impedances were checked. It was ensured that the
children were awake at least 30 min before beginning the Recognition
session, in order to avoid any inuences of sleep inertia on task per-
formance.
For the Wake condition, children and parents arrived at the lab at
~7:30 h. The procedure was then identical to the Sleep condition with
the exception of a day of wake during the retention period. Children
wore an activity-tracking device (Actiwatch 2, Philips Respironics) to
ensure that they did not sleep during the wake period and families were
asked to abstain from strenuous activities over the course of the day.
Participants ate a light meal before starting the Recognition session.
2.3. Materials and task
A set of 444 pictures (taken from the International Aective Picture
System, IAPS; Lang et al., 2008, and other studies; Jackson et al., 2005;
Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009) was used in this study to create two
unique versions of the task (List A and B, order balanced) for use in each
condition. Each task version consisted of 72 negative targets and 72
neutral targets, in addition to 36 negative foils and 36 neutral foils
which were used as distractor pictures during recognition (Fig. 1D). An
additional six positive pictures were included in each list with the sole
purpose of maintaining motivation and were not included in the ana-
lyses. A pilot study, wherein a larger cohort of children (n= 41) used
the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM, Bradley and Lang, 1994) to rate the
pictures, conrmed that children generally rated the negative pictures
as more negative and more arousing (mean ± sem valence ratings:
negative = 1.68 ± 0.75, neutral = 0.78 ± 0.82, t(40) = 16.48,
p< .001; arousal ratings: negative = 4.58 ± 1.47, neutral
= 1.77 ± 1.18, t(40) = 12.32, p< .001).
During the Encoding session, target pictures were presented in a
pseudo-random order wherein consecutive iterations of picture cate-
gory (i.e. negative and neutral) were limited to three. A single trial
consisted of the following procession: a xation circle (displayed for a
randomized period between 1 and 2 s), the picture (displayed for 1.5s),
a blank screen (displayed for 6 s), valence rating (i.e. valence SAM with
the question: How unpleasant or pleasant did you feel while looking at
the picture?, coded from 5 to 5), arousal rating (i.e. arousal SAM
with the question: How calm or nervous did you feel while looking at
the picture?, coded from 1 to 9), and nally an inter-trial interval of 2 s
(Fig. 1D). At the Recognition session, target and distractor pictures
were intermixed. The trial procession was identical except for the ad-
ditional question (following the ratings): Is this picture old or new?to
which children responded with either Oldor New.Participants
were instructed to rate all pictures based on their momentary feelings
using a computer mouse and to not try to remember how they had rated
a picture if they had seen it before. The task was presented using E-
Prime®3.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, Pennsylvania,
USA).
2.4. Behavioral analysis
Recognition memory scores were calculated as recognition accu-
racy, i.e. hit rate minus false alarm rate, for negative and neutral pic-
tures, separately. Average ratings for valence and arousal were calcu-
lated according to trial type, i.e. negative target at encoding, neutral
target at encoding, successfully remembered negative target at re-
cognition (negative hits), and successfully remembered neutral target at
recognition (neutral hits). Emotional response for all measures was
calculated as the dierence between the response to negative images
and the response to neutral images at each session. This measure re-
ects the strength of an emotional response while correcting for pos-
sible dierences in baseline reactivity. The analysis was limited to trials
where the picture was correctly remembered (hits) in recognition ses-
sions.
2.5. Electrophysiological recordings and analysis
Electrophysiological data was collected using Brain Vision hardware
and software (Brain Products GmBH, Gilching, Germany). During all
sessions, EEG was recorded at F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, and P4
(referenced to linked mastoids with FC1 and FC2 as grounds) according
to the 1020 system using Ag/Cl electrodes. Two diagonally placed
EOG electrodes were used to measure eye movements. ECG bipolar
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
86
electrodes were placed on the lower left and upper right chest. Signals
were sampled at 500 Hz, and EEG electrode impedance was kept below
5kΩ.
Event-related potential (ERP) processing was performed with the
Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 Software (Brain Products GmBH, Gilching,
Germany). EEG and EOG were rst ltered between 0.3 and 35 Hz
using a zero-phase Butterworth lter (24 dB/oct) with a 50 Hz notch.
Trials were segmented from 200 ms before picture onset to 1500 ms
after picture onset and then subjected to linear detrending according to
Hennighausen et al. (Hennighausen et al., 1993). Baseline correction of
the ERP segments was performed using the 200 ms before picture onset.
ERP segments were rejected as artifacted when 1) a gradient > 40 µV/
ms was detected, 2) a voltage dierence > 150 µV occurred within the
1700-ms segment, 3) a period of low activity (< .5 µV) was detected for
a period > 100 ms, or 4) an ocular artifact was apparent by visual in-
spection. ERP analyses concentrated on the LPP whose amplitude was
taken as the peak voltage at Pz between 600 ms and 1000 ms after the
picture onset (using the average voltage in this latency bin yielded es-
sentially the same results and are not reported here).
To determine heart rate deceleration (HRD), R waves of the QRS
complexes were rst marked using a slope-based detection algorithm in
the Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 Software. Trials were extracted according
to type and then visually inspected to exclude artifacted data. The
maximum R-R interval in the 5-s interval following picture onset was
subtracted from the mean R-R interval during the 1.5-s baseline period
before picture onset. LPP and HRD responses were averaged according
to trial type (negative and neutral targets at encoding, negative and
neutral hits at recognition,see Fig. 2 for traces of the original LPP and
HRD signals).
Sleep EEG and EOG were ltered in the same manner as the ERP
data. EMG was ltered with the same parameters but with a higher
bandpass frequency range (5100 Hz). Sleep recordings were scored
according to standard criteria (Rechtschaen, 1968) to obtain the time
spent in dierent sleep stages (wake, stages 1, 2, slow wave sleep
[SWS], and REM sleep in minutes and percentage of total sleep time
[TST]). The Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 Software was used to process and
calculate power in the delta and theta bands during NonREM (S2 and
SWS) and REM, respectively. Artifact-free sleep epochs were cut into 6-
second segments with 2 s of overlap. A fast fourier transform using a
symmetric Hanning window was used to calculate power with a 0.2 Hz
resolution. Relative EEG power (%) at Fz (as done in previous work:
Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2013) was then calculated for each subject by
dividing the power in each frequency bin by the total power in the
spectrum for a specic sleep stage and multiplying by 100. Because
frequency band ranges can vary widely from person to person (e.g.
Klimesch et al., 1998;Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999), we ne-
tuned the determination of frequency ranges for the bands of interest by
visually determining for each participant the peak frequency for the
delta and spindle bands for NonREM sleep as well as theta during REM
sleep, and centering the band limits around this peak (Supplementary
Fig. S2). This procedure resulted in the following average bands:
0.42 Hz for the NonREM sleep delta band, 10.513.2 Hz for the
NonREM sleep spindle band, and 4.66 Hz for the REM sleep theta
band.
To detect discrete spindles, EEG was bandpass ltered within a
subject-specic spindle frequency range and spindle events were de-
tected according to an established thresholding algorithm (see Gais
et al., 2002 for details). Subject-specic spindle bands were visually
identied as the peak within the 915 Hz range (see Kurdziel et al.,
2013). Spindle counts were averaged over C3, Cz, and C4, similar to
Kaestner et al. (2013). Spindle density was calculated as the average
number of spindles per 30-s epoch for S2 sleep and SWS.
2.6. Statistical analyses
Physiological emotional response data from one participant had to
be excluded due to a technical failure. Ratings from two participants
were excluded from analyses because of performance values ex-
ceeding ± 2 standard deviations from the sample mean, suggesting that
they may have responded randomly to the subjective rating portion of
the task. It was nevertheless conrmed that they adequately performed
the memory task (i.e. recognition accuracies > 65%). The sleep elec-
trophysiology from frontal electrodes in one participant had to be ex-
cluded due to technical problems with the recordings. Data of 3 parti-
cipants were excluded from LPP analyses due to values that
exceeded ± 2 standard deviations from the mean.
Following the experimental within-subject cross-over design of the
study, changes in memory and emotional response were analyzed using
repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the within-sub-
jects factor Sleep (Wake vs. Sleep). The inuence of sleep on
Fig. 2. The inuence of sleep on physiological measures of emotion. A) and B) show grand average LPP traces at Pz across wake and sleep, respectively. C) and D)
show the change in R-R interval relative to the 1.5 s baseline period before image onset (post onset minus pre onset). Note that a positive change R-R interval
represents a deceleration in heart rate and that the y-axis has been inverted to facilitate interpretation of the heart response. Black vertical lines represent image
onset.
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
87
recognition accuracy was addressed using an additional Emotion cate-
gory (negative vs. neutral) within-subjects factor. For the analysis of
emotional response, a within-subjects Session factor (Encoding vs.
Recognition) was used in addition to the Sleep factor. Bonferroni-cor-
rected paired t-tests were used to follow up signicant ANOVA eects
(new α-level for comparison: 0.05/2 tests = 0.025). To determine the
specic nature of the inuence of sleep, change scores in response over
the retention interval (dened by the dierence between the respective
response at recognition minus the response at encoding) were calcu-
lated separately according to picture class (negative and neutral)
whenever a signicant interaction between Sleep and Session was
found (Bonferroni-corrected paired t-tests as described above).
Pearson's correlations were used to test for relationships between
sleep features that have been associated with emotional preservation
(REM% (Wagner et al., 2001), REM theta power (Prehn-Kristensen
et al., 2013), S2 and SWS spindle density (Kaestner et al., 2013)) and
measures which did not exhibit a change in emotional response over
sleep (LPP and HRD, calculated as emotional response at recognition
minus emotional response at encoding) as well as emotional dier-
entiation in memory (i.e. the dierence between negative recognition
accuracy and neutral recognition accuracy). We also tested for whether
emotional response measures which were preserved over sleep corre-
lated with emotional dierentiation following sleep or wake. Bonfer-
roni correction was applied such that the required α-level for sig-
nicance was .05/16 tests = 0.003.
Inuences of sleep on control variables were addressed using a Sleep
x Session ANOVA. Because this study was focused on possible inu-
ences of sleep on emotion and memory, follow up correlations were
calculated between control variables exhibiting Sleep eects (main ef-
fects or interactions) and the independent variables of interest (emo-
tional memory and emotional response for each measure).
3. Results
3.1. Recognition memory
In general, participants correctly recognized more target pictures
when they had slept after the encoding session than when they had
stayed awake (Sleep main eect for recognition accuracy: F(1,14)
= 8.52 p= .01, ƞ
p2
= 0.38, Fig. 1B). Also, negative images were gen-
erally better recognized than neutral ones (Emotion main eect: F(1,14)
= 17.51 p= .001, ƞ
p2
= 0.56). However, whereas better recognition
accuracy for negative compared to neutral pictures was found after
staying awake (p= .001), this was not the case for the Sleep condition
(p= .14, Sleep x Emotion interaction: F(1,14) = 6.17, p= .026, ƞ
p2
= 0.31), possibly reecting a ceiling eect of the generally enhanced
recognition performance in the Sleep condition.
3.2. Subjective emotional response
Children rated negative images as more negative and more arousing
at both the Sleep (valence ratings: negative = 1.88 ± 0.78, neutral
= 0.56 ± 0.55, arousal ratings: negative = 5.8 ± 1.36, neutral =
2.44 ± 1.31) and Wake (valence ratings: negative = 1.80 ± 0.79,
neutral = 0.47 ± 0.40, arousal ratings: negative = 5.43 ± 1.68,
neutral = 2.39 ± 1.02) encoding sessions (all ps < .001). Sleep de-
creased the emotional response (response to negative images minus
response to neutral images) as measured by valence ratings of the
successfully remembered pictures (p= .007). In contrast, valence rat-
ings remained unchanged across a retention interval containing wake
(p= .53, see Fig. 1A for visualized Sleep x Session interaction: F(1,13)
= 4.78, p= .048, ƞ
p2
= 0.27; Sleep main eect: ns; Session main eect:
F(1,13) = 5.32, p= .038, ƞ
p2
= 0.29; see Fig. S3 in the Supplementary
Material for emotional response at encoding and recognition, sepa-
rately). Calculating change scores (response at recognition minus re-
sponse at encoding) separately for neutral and negative images revealed
that this interaction was driven by the fact that sleep produced a less
negative rating of negative images (mean dierence ± sem:
+0.19 ± 0.13, see Supplementary Fig. S4) and, simultaneously, a
more negative valence rating of neutral images (0.18 ± 0.06, t
(13) = 3.17, p= .007) whereas no such changes were observed across
the wake retention interval (negative: 0.02 ± 0.10, neutral:
0.10 ± 0.08, p= .53). Arousal ratings exhibited a similar pattern of
decreased emotional response across Sleep compared with the Wake
condition, albeit less robustly (Sleep x Session interaction: F(1,13)
= 4.30, p= .058, ƞ
p2
= 0.25, Sleep & Session main eects: ns). The
inuence of sleep on rating behavior did not appear to be driven by
circadian eects, as the subjective emotional response did not sig-
nicantly dier between the wake encoding session, which took place
in the morning, and sleep encoding session, which took place in the
evening (valence: p= .16; arousal: p= .11).
3.3. LPP emotional response
The emotional response as measured by the LPP of the event related
potential at Pz remained stable across sleep (p= .59) but increased
over the course of the wake period (p= .008; Sleep x Session interac-
tion: F(1,11) = 5.34, p= .041, ƞ
p2
= 0.33; Sleep and Session main
eects: ns). Thus, compared with the dynamics across the wake period,
sleep led to a relative decrease in the LPP emotional response (see
Fig. 1A for visualized Sleep x Session interaction). Correspondingly,
separate change scores for negative and neutral pictures indicated that
the LPP slightly increased across sleep regardless of emotional picture
category (negative: 2.81 ± 1.61 μV, neutral: 3.78 ± 1.10 μV,
p= .60), whereas the wake period produced a particularly strong in-
crease in LPP to negative pictures (6.9 ± 0.85 μV) as compared to
neutral pictures (2.95 ± 1.00 μV, t(11) = 3.21, p= .008). The emo-
tional response tended to be smaller at encoding in the morning (Wake
condition) than at encoding in the evening session (Sleep condition;
p= .063) suggesting that the increase in emotional response in LPP
across sessions in the Wake condition may partially reect a circadian
inuence (see Fig. S3 for an overview of the emotional responses of the
LPP at encoding and recognition, as well as additional LPP analyses in
the Supplementary material). To explore this possibility, we removed
the three participants with the lowest emotional response at the wake
encoding session in order to match emotional response at encoding (t
(8) = 1.49, p= .175). This analysis revealed essentially the same
pattern with a statistical trend for the Sleep x Session interaction (F(1,8)
= 3.93, p= .083, ƞ
p2
= 0.33), which suggests that time of day alone
did not exclusively drive the resultant data patterns.
3.4. HRD emotional response
In contrast to subjective ratings and LPP, the HRD emotional re-
sponse decreased across wake (p= .003) but not across sleep (p= .67;
Sleep x Session interaction: F(1,14) = 5.20 p= .039, ƞ
p2
= 0.27, Sleep
and Session main eects: ns). Thus, compared with the dynamics across
the wake period, sleep led to relative increase in HRD emotional re-
sponse (see Fig. 1A for visualized Sleep x Session interaction). Analysis
of separate change scores for negative and neutral pictures revealed
that this interaction was driven by a decrease in HRD response to the
negative pictures (mean ± sem: 14.09 ± 6.81 ms, t(14) = 3.54,
p= .003, see Supplementary Fig. S4) but not neutral pictures
(6.24 ± 6.85 ms) across the wake retention interval, whereas across
sleep HRD decreased regardless of emotional picture class (negative:
15.73 ± 10.72 ms, neutral: 18.97 ± 8.23 ms, p= .91). HRD re-
sponses were comparable for the Encoding sessions of the Sleep and
Wake conditions ruling out substantial circadian eects (p= .13; see
Fig. S3 in the Supplementary material).
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
88
3.5. Correlations between emotional response, sleep parameters and
memory
The overnight change in the emotional HRD response (emotional
response to hits at recognition minus emotional response at encoding)
positively correlated with the degree of emotional memory dier-
entiation after sleep (r= 0.75, p= .001): participants who showed a
relative increase in HRD emotional response also showed a bigger dif-
ference between negative and neutral recognition accuracy. No other
correlations survived multiple testing correction at α= 0.003.
3.6. Sleep parameters and control variables
Sleep scoring of the polysomnographic recordings from the experi-
mental night indicated that the children slept normally (Supplementary
Fig. S1). In total, they slept around 8.58 h. Participants spent approxi-
mately 19.9 min, 162.8 min, 225.1 min and 99.6 min in sleep stages 1,
2, SWS, and REM, respectively. They entered SWS after ~4.5 min and
REM after ~95 min. Participants were awake for approximately 6.2 min
after sleep onset.
Results from control tests are summarized in Supplementary
Table 1. Motivation, vigilance, negative mood, and sleepiness in the
Encoding and Recognition sessions were not dierentially inuenced by
the Sleep and Wake conditions (ps > .26, for the respective main and
interaction eects). Motivation was higher during the Encoding ses-
sions than the Recognition sessions (Session main eect: F(1,15)
= 4.77, p= .045). Positive mood (PANAS-Positive) exhibited a similar
trend (Session main eect: F(1,15) = 3.99, p= .064), and was higher
during the Sleep than Wake condition (Sleep main eect: F(1,15)
= 7.82, p= .014). There were no consistent correlations between
PANAS-Positive scores and any of the emotional response or memory
measures (ps.059), excluding any substantial contributions of this
factor to the eects of sleep on emotional responses or recognition
performance.
4. Discussion
Beyond conrming that sleep enhances memory for pictures (e.g.
Baran et al., 2012), our study in ~10 year old children revealed a
distinct pattern of sleep eects on the emotional response associated
with these memories. Compared with a period of daytime wakefulness,
nocturnal sleep decreased the emotional response as measured by be-
havioral ratings (valence) and the LPP (by preserving a response that
would otherwise increase over wake) while increasing the emotional
response as measured by HRD (by preserving a response that would
otherwise decrease over wake). It therefore appears that sleep decreases
responses that are more subject to cognitive control, while increasing
responses that are generated more automatically (i.e. less sensitive to
top-down inuences).
This study provides support for the body of research showing that
sleep enhances memory (Diekelmann and Born, 2010), though it should
be considered that without an immediate recognition test after en-
coding it is impossible to tell whether encoding strength was inuenced
by time of day. It should be noted, however, that many studies which
included immediate recognition sessions in order to control for circa-
dian inuences show a similar improvement in memory performance
after sleep (e.g. Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2013). Unlike previous studies
(Hu et al., 2006; Nishida et al., 2009; Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009)we
did not nd a preferential enhancement of negative over neutral stimuli
by sleep. This discrepancy likely reects a ceiling eect, as average hit
rates for negative pictures in the Sleep condition reached a maximum of
91%.
Our nding that sleep decreased emotional response in valence
ratings in children, together with previous ndings in adults from van
der Helm et al. (2011), suggests that sleep leads to a decrease in the
explicit judgment of the aversiveness of a stimulus. Overall, however,
studies of the eects of sleep on valence ratings to negative stimuli
reveal a rather heterogeneous picture (Baran et al., 2012; Wagner et al.,
2002), likely reecting the complex integration of physiological signals
and concurrent behavioral goals that are inherent to cognitive emo-
tional appraisals (Schachter and Singer, 1962). Such aspects of cogni-
tive control may be more sensitive to subtle changes in experimental
context or the composition of sleep.
The eects of sleep on valence ratings were paralleled by a relative
decrease in the emotional LPP response across sleep when compared to
wakefulness. Specically, the emotional LPP response remained at a
rather constant level across sleep, while increasing across wake.
Though it should be emphasized that circadian factors may have par-
tially inuenced this pattern, these results are in accordance with
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) work reporting a de-
crease in emotional reactivity as measured by amygdala activity across
sleep (van der Helm et al., 2011). Importantly, both the LPP and
amygdala activity have been linked to emotional reactivity and pre-
frontal cortex-driven emotion regulation in children and adults
(Babkirk et al., 2015; Dennis and Hajcak, 2009; Hajcak et al., 2010;
Ochsner et al., 2012). The relatively diminished LPP after sleep there-
fore likely reects increased emotion regulation which itself might be a
consequence of a sleep-induced enhancement of the cortical re-
presentation of the stimulus (Gais et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2016;
Takashima et al., 2006).
Opposing the down-regulating eects of sleep on the emotional
response in subjective ratings and LPP, sleep produced a relative en-
hancement in the emotional heart rate deceleration (HRD) response
compared to dynamics across wake. Specically, sleep was associated
with preservation of emotional HRD response, which otherwise de-
creased across wake. Our ndings in children agree with previous
ndings in an adult study which likewise showed that HRD in response
to emotional pictures decreases across wake but is preserved across a
nap (Pace-Schott et al., 2011). Change scores also conrmed that the
preserving eect of sleep on HRD accompanied a stimulus category-
independent decrease in HRD across sleep, as seen in adults
(Cunningham et al., 2014).
Interestingly, even though a ceiling eect was seen in our memory
measure, the degree of emotional memory enhancement and the degree
of emotional HRD response preservation across the retention period
were positively correlated in the Sleep but not Wake condition. This
suggests that a similar underlying mechanism is responsible for pre-
serving the emotional HRD response and emotional memory, and in
extension, that the immediate emotional meaning of an event is in-
herently intertwined in the memory representation. Previous work has
shown that the enhanced consolidation of the entirety of an emotional
memory is linked to REM sleep (Menz et al., 2016), however, in our
study respective correlations for REM% and REM sleep theta power
with emotional preservation did not reach signicance after correction
for multiple tests.
Our study is unique in showing that rather than exerting a uniform
inuence, a night of sleep dierentially aects automatic and cognitive
aspects of the emotional response. In combination, the present ndings
suggest a scenario in which sleep aects memory and the associated
emotional response via two dierent mechanisms: On the one hand,
sleep preserves the immediate emotional meaning of events, leading to
preservation of automatic emotional responses like HRD. On the other
hand, sleep generally acts to strengthen declarative components of a
memory in a way that may also make them more accessible to retrieval
and various other cognitive functions. This episodic memory con-
solidation process leads to the strengthening of the neocortical re-
presentation of the event and has been linked to NonREM sleep, as well
as the associated slow wave and spindle activity (Diekelmann and Born,
2010). Indeed, recent work has shown that overnight consolidation of
emotional memories is associated with preservation of autonomic
emotional reactivity and reorganization of memory-associated activity
from hippocampal to neocortical structures (Liu et al., 2016). The
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
89
strengthening of respective cortical representations might improve the
top-down control of emotional responses, seen here as diminished rat-
ings of negative valence and a relative decrease in the emotional LPP
response after sleep.
Such a twofold inuence of sleep - preserving automatic emotional
response and enhancing top-down control of emotional response -
might help to bridge the divergent predictions of the ESC and the SFSR
theories that have been proposed to explain eects of sleep on emo-
tional memory (Baran et al., 2012; Pace-Schott et al., 2011; Walker,
2010; Werner et al., 2015). In support of the ESC theory we found that
sleep increases (via preservation of a response that would otherwise
decrease over wake) emotional responses that are more automatic in
nature (HRD). In support of the SFSR theory, we found that sleep leads
to a relative decrease in more cognitive emotional responses, i.e. those
that may employ top-down regulation mechanisms like subjective rat-
ings and the LPP (via an absolute decrease in valence ratings and pre-
servation of the LPP response which otherwise increases over wake).
Thus, our study shows that rather than acting as a uniform entity,
emotional response emerges from the interaction between automatic
emotional reaction systems and cognitive processes, each of which is
divergently inuenced by sleep.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
SFB 654 (Plasticity and Sleep). We would like to thank Alexander
Prehn-Kristensen and Christian Wiesner for preparing the task and
analyzing the results of a pilot study, Hong-Viet Ngo for providing the
spindle-detection algorithm code, and Cristin Clar and Lilliam Griselda
Hernandez-Reyes for their help with data collection.
Conict of interest statement
The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Appendix A. Supporting information
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the
online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.
05.015.
References
Achenbach, T.M., 1991. Child Behavior Checklist. University of Vermont, Burlington, VT.
Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist, 1998. Elternfragebogen über das
Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen; deutsche Bearbeitung der Child Behavior
Checklist (CBCL/4-18). Einführung und Anleitung zur Handauswertung. Köln:
Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik.
Babkirk, S., Rios, V., Dennis, T.A., 2015. The late positive potential predicts emotion
regulation strategy use in schoolaged children concurrently and two years later. Dev.
Sci. 18, 832841.
Baran, B., Pace-Schott, E.F., Ericson, C., Spencer, R.M., 2012. Processing of emotional
reactivity and emotional memory over sleep. J. Neurosci. 32, 10351042.
Bradley, M.M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B.N., Lang, P.J., 2001. Emotion and motivation I:
defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion 1, 276.
Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., 1994. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the
semantic dierential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 4959.
Clawson, B.C., Durkin, J., Aton, S.J., 2016. Form and function of sleep spindles across the
lifespan. Neural Plast. 2016.
Coan, J.A., Allen, J.J., 2007. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. Oxford
university press.
Cunningham, T.J., Crowell, C.R., Alger, S.E., Kensinger, E.A., Villano, M.A., Mattingly,
S.M., Payne, J.D., 2014. Psychophysiological arousal at encoding leads to reduced
reactivity but enhanced emotional memory following sleep. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
114, 155164.
Cuthbert, B.N., Schupp, H.T., Bradley, M.M., Birbaumer, N., Lang, P.J., 2000. Brain po-
tentials in aective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and af-
fective report. Biol. Psychol. 52, 95111.
Dennis, T.A., Hajcak, G., 2009. The late positive potential: a neurophysiological marker
for emotion regulation in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 13731383.
Diekelmann, S., Born, J., 2010. The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11,
114126.
Gais, S., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Darsaud, A., Desseilles, M., Rauchs, G.,
Schabus, M., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., 2007. Sleep transforms the cerebral
trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 1877818783.
Gais, S., Mölle, M., Helms, K., Born, J., 2002. Learning-dependent increases in sleep
spindle density. J. Neurosci. 22, 68306834.
Groch, S., Wilhelm, I., Diekelmann, S., Born, J., 2013. The role of REM sleep in the
processing of emotional memories: evidence from behavior and event-related po-
tentials. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 99, 19.
Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., Olvet, D.M., 2010. Event-related potentials, emotion, and
emotion regulation: an integrative review. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 129155.
Hennighausen, E., Heil, M., Rösler, F., 1993. A correction method for DC drift artifacts.
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 86, 199204.
Hoddes, E., Zarcone, V., Smythe, H., Phillips, R., Dement, W., 1973. Quantication of
sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology 10, 431436.
Hu, P., Stylos-Allan, M., Walker, M.P., 2006. Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional
declarative memory. Psychol. Sci. 17, 891898.
Jackson, P.L., Meltzo, A.N., Decety, J., 2005. How do we perceive the pain of others? A
window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage 24, 771779.
Kaestner, E.J., Wixted, J.T., Mednick, S.C., 2013. Pharmacologically increasing sleep
spindles enhances recognition for negative and high-arousal memories. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 25, 15971610.
Klimesch, W., Russegger, H., Doppelmayr, M., Pachinger, T., 1998. A method for the
calculation of induced band power: implications for the signicance of brain oscil-
lations. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 123130.
Kurdziel, L., Duclos, K., Spencer, R.M., 2013. Sleep spindles in midday naps enhance
learning in preschool children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 1726717272.
Kurth, S., Jenni, O.G., Riedner, B.A., Tononi, G., Carskadon, M.A., Huber, R., 2010.
Characteristics of sleep slow waves in children and adolescents. Sleep 33, 475.
Lang, P., Bradley, M., Cuthbert, B., 2008. International Aective Picture System (IAPS):
Aective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual (Technical Report A-8).
University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
Leventon, J.S., Stevens, J.S., Bauer, P.J., 2014. Development in the neurophysiology of
emotion processing and memory in school-age children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10,
2133.
Liu, Y., Lin, W., Liu, C., Luo, Y., Wu, J., Bayley, P.J., Qin, S., 2016. Memory consolidation
recongures neural pathways involved in the suppression of emotional memories.
Nat. Commun. 7, 13375.
Menz, M.M., Rihm, J.S., Büchel, C., 2016. REM sleep is causal to successful consolidation
of dangerous and safety stimuli and reduces return of fear after extinction. J.
Neurosci. 36, 21482160.
Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R.L., Walker, M.P., 2009. REM sleep, prefrontal theta,
and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb. Cortex 19, 11581166.
Ochsner, K.N., Silvers, J.A., Buhle, J.T., 2012. Functional imaging studies of emotion
regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emo-
tion. In: Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. pp. 1251.
Öhman, A., Hamm, A., Hugdahl, K., 2000. Cognition and the autonomic nervous system:
orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. In: Cacioppo, John T., Tassinary, Louis G.,
Berntson, Gary G. (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology, 2nd ed. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, pp. 533575.
Pace-Schott, E.F., Shepherd, E., Spencer, R.M., Marcello, M., Tucker, M., Propper, R.E.,
Stickgold, R., 2011. Napping promotes inter-session habituation to emotional stimuli.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 95, 2436.
Petermann, U., & Petermann, F. 2011. WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-:
Deutschsprachige Adaptation nach D. Wechsler: Pearson Assessment and
Information.
Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H., 1999. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and
desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 18421857.
Phelps, E.A., O'Connor, K.J., Gatenby, J.C., Gore, J.C., Grillon, C., Davis, M., 2001.
Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 4,
437441.
Prehn-Kristensen, A., Goder, R., Chirobeja, S., Bressmann, I., Ferstl, R., Baving, L., 2009.
Sleep in children enhances preferentially emotional declarative but not procedural
memories. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 104, 132139.
Prehn-Kristensen, A., Munz, M., Molzow, I., Wilhelm, I., Wiesner, C.D., Baving, L., 2013.
Sleep promotes consolidation of emotional memory in healthy children but not in
children with attention-decit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One 8, e65098.
Rechtschaen, A., 1968. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring
system for sleep stages of human subjects. Public Health Serv.
Roach, G.D., Dawson, D., Lamond, N., 2006. Can a shorter psychomotor vigilance task be
used as a reasonable substitute for the tenminute psychomotor vigilance task?
Chronobiol. Int. 23, 13791387.
Schachter, S., Singer, J., 1962. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of
emotional state. Psychol. Rev. 69, 379.
Simonds, J.F., Parraga, H., 1982. Prevalence of sleep disorders and sleep behaviors in
children and adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 21, 383388.
Takashima, A., Petersson, K.M., Rutters, F., Tendolkar, I., Jensen, O., Zwarts, M.,
McNaughton, B., Fernandez, G., 2006. Declarative memory consolidation in humans:
a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 103, 756761.
van der Helm, E., Yao, J., Dutt, S., Rao, V., Saletin, J.M., Walker, M.P., 2011. REM sleep
depotentiates amygdala activity to previous emotional experiences. Curr. Biol. 21,
20292032.
Wagner, U., Fischer, S., Born, J., 2002. Changes in emotional responses to aversive pic-
tures across periods rich in slow-wave sleep versus rapid eye movement sleep.
Psychosom. Med. 64, 627634.
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Born, J., 2001. Emotional memory formation is enhanced across
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
90
sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn. Mem. 8,
112119.
Walker, M.P., 2009. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
1156, 168197.
Walker, M.P., 2010. Sleep, memory and emotion. Prog. Brain Res. 185, 4968.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A., 1988. Development and validation of brief measures
of positive and negative aect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54,
10631070.
Werner, G.G., Schabus, M., Blechert, J., Kolodyazhniy, V., Wilhelm, F.H., 2015. Preto
postsleep change in psychophysiological reactivity to emotional lms: latenight REM
sleep is associated with attenuated emotional processing. Psychophysiology 52,
813825.
E. Bolinger et al. Neuropsychologia 117 (2018) 84–91
91
... Finally, at a population level, overweight subjects with hyper-stimulated sympathetic drive have shown enhanced cognitive performance [26]. On the contrary, what leads to low HR, such as isotonic exercise and sleep, tends to favour memory, while sleep deprivation reduces cognitive abilities [27][28][29][30][31][32]. Experimental studies are mandatory in order to clarify the mechanism underlying the HR ↔ cognition relationship put in evidence in the present pilot epidemiological analysis. ...
... Non-pharmacological ways to reduce HR are to reduce stress (which increases HR) and practicing endurance isotonic exercise, which in fact improves cognitive functions and memory in particular [27,28,[34][35][36][37]. Another way is sleep, which notoriously reduces HR and, as demonstrated, consolidates immediate emotional meaning by enhancing automatic responses and promotes top-down control of emotional responses by strengthening respective neocortical representations and increases or preserves the emotional tone associated with memories [29][30][31]. A simple nap after a learning session can improve assimilation of information and memory [29]. ...
... Another way is sleep, which notoriously reduces HR and, as demonstrated, consolidates immediate emotional meaning by enhancing automatic responses and promotes top-down control of emotional responses by strengthening respective neocortical representations and increases or preserves the emotional tone associated with memories [29][30][31]. A simple nap after a learning session can improve assimilation of information and memory [29]. ...
... Cortical activity. Bolinger et al. (2018) 49 were interested in recordings of late positive potentials (LPP), which are recognized to be a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children and is modulated by conscious cognitive processes 50 . They assessed the processing and recognition of neutral, as well as emotional visual stimuli. ...
... Cortical activity. Bolinger et al. (2018) 49 were interested in recordings of late positive potentials (LPP), which are recognized to be a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children and is modulated by conscious cognitive processes 50 . They assessed the processing and recognition of neutral, as well as emotional visual stimuli. ...
... Those results support the assumption that sleep seems to enhance cortical activity, and cognitive processes in schoolaged children 49 , as well as the automatic direction of attention in response to presentation of emotional stimuli in pre-school children 51 . It would be very interesting to get more detailed insight into discriminating power of the emotional category of stimuli on neurophysiological reactions towards sleep deprivation in children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sleep and emotions are closely associated; however, the methodological challenges in the examination of sleep and the processes of emotion regulation in children and adolescents have not been investigated so far. Additionally, there is the demand to identify the levels of emotion regulating processes in which problematic or restricted sleep causes effect. Experimental sleep deprivation as well as prevalent sleep problems have been found to have negative influence on mental health and regulating functions. This review focuses first on the methodological protocols of the included studies. Subsequently, the results are summarized in the context of a multilevel model of emotion regulation. Thereafter, suggestions for future directions are given. Sleep problems and sleep deprivation are associated with a decrease of functional emotion regulating behavior and impaired emotion generation, and prolonged sleep enhances better mood and affect states, positive emotion expression, and faster sensory processing in response to emotional stimuli. This literature review highlights the limitations in current research, focusing on types of measurements, task characteristics, and data analysis. At the conclusion, suggestions are given for the future research direction in the field of sleep and emotion regulation in children and adolescents.
... showed that participants had attenuated HRD and PEP after a period of overnight sleep, in contrast to regular daytime waking activity, although this result was not specific to valenced material. Five of the nine studies showed that, in response to both valenced and neutral material, there were no post-sleep HRD or SCL differences (Kuriyama et al., 2010(Kuriyama et al., , 2013Hot et al., 2016;Bolinger et al., 2018Bolinger et al., , 2019. However, three of these studies showed that both HRD and SCL responses to the material were decreased after a period of either regular daytime waking activity or overnight sleep deprivation (Kuriyama et al., 2013;Bolinger et al., 2018Bolinger et al., , 2019. ...
... Five of the nine studies showed that, in response to both valenced and neutral material, there were no post-sleep HRD or SCL differences (Kuriyama et al., 2010(Kuriyama et al., , 2013Hot et al., 2016;Bolinger et al., 2018Bolinger et al., , 2019. However, three of these studies showed that both HRD and SCL responses to the material were decreased after a period of either regular daytime waking activity or overnight sleep deprivation (Kuriyama et al., 2013;Bolinger et al., 2018Bolinger et al., , 2019. Bolinger et al. (2018) suggested that this effect was driven by a decrease in reactivity to negative stimuli. ...
... However, three of these studies showed that both HRD and SCL responses to the material were decreased after a period of either regular daytime waking activity or overnight sleep deprivation (Kuriyama et al., 2013;Bolinger et al., 2018Bolinger et al., , 2019. Bolinger et al. (2018) suggested that this effect was driven by a decrease in reactivity to negative stimuli. The other two studies showed no change in reactivity to all stimuli after a period of either regular daytime waking activity or overnight sleep deprivation (Kuriyama et al., 2010;Hot et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Many studies have investigated whether sleep affects cognitively unmodulated reactivity to emotional stimuli. These studies operationalize emotion regulation by using subjective and/or objective measures to compare pre- and post-sleep reactivity to the same emotional stimuli. Findings have been inconsistent: some show that sleep attenuates emotional reactivity, whereas others report enhanced or maintained reactivity. Across-study methodological differences may account for discrepant findings. To resolve the questions of whether sleep leads to the attenuation, enhancement, or maintenance of emotional reactivity, and under which experimental conditions particular effects are observed, we undertook a synthesized narrative and meta-analytic approach. We searched PubMed, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Web of Science, and Cochrane Library databases for relevant articles, using search terms determined a priori and search limits of language = English, participants = human, and dates = January 2006–June 2021. Our final sample included 24 studies that investigated changes in emotional reactivity in response to negatively and/or positively valenced material compared to neutral material over a period of sleep compared to a matched period of waking. Primary analyses used random effects modeling to investigate whether sleep preferentially modulates reactivity in response to emotional stimuli; secondary analyses examined potential moderators of the effect. Results showed that sleep (or equivalent periods of wakefulness) did not significantly affect psychophysiological measures of reactivity to negative or neutral stimuli. However, self-reported arousal ratings of negative stimuli were significantly increased post-sleep but not post-waking. Sub-group analyses indicated that (a) sleep-deprived participants, compared to those who slept or who experienced daytime waking, reacted more strongly and negatively in response to positive stimuli; (b) nap-exposed participants, compared to those who remained awake or who slept a full night, rated negative pictures less negatively; and (c) participants who did not obtain substantial REM sleep, compared to those who did and those exposed to waking conditions, had attenuated reactivity to neutral stimuli. We conclude that sleep may affect emotional reactivity, but that studies need more consistency in methodology, commitment to collecting both psychophysiological and self-report measures, and should report REM sleep parameters. Using these methodological principles would promote a better understanding of under which conditions particular effects are observed.
... Although many studies find that post-learning sleep benefits overall memory performance (Baran et al., 2012;Bolinger et al., 2018;Göder et al., 2015;Hu et al., 2006;Kurz et al., 2019;Morgenthaler et al., 2014;Payne, Ellenbogen et al., 2008a;Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009, not all find more sleep-related enhancement for emotional memory than neutral memory (Ashton et al., 2019;Baran et al., 2012;Bolinger et al., 2019;Cox et al., 2018;Jones et al., 2016;Wiesner et al., 2015). One emotional memory paradigm that shows more consistent results employs the emotional memory trade-off task. ...
... The third goal was to test whether sleep interacted with common and specific internalizing symptoms measured by the bifactor S-1 model to predict emotional memory consolidation. First, because many studies have demonstrated sleep-related benefits on overall memory performance (Baran et al., 2012;Bolinger et al., 2018;Göder et al., 2015;Kurz et al., 2019;Morgenthaler et al., 2014;Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009, we hypothesized that the relationship between internalizing symptoms and worsened recognition memory across all types of scene components would be weaker after post-learning sleep compared with wakefulness. Second, we hypothesized that if internalizing symptoms were associated with more differentiated valence and arousal ratings between negative and neutral scenes, the effects of internalizing symptoms on enhanced emotional memory would be stronger after post-learning sleep. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional memory bias is a common characteristic of internalizing symptomatology and is enhanced during sleep. The current study employs bifactor S-1 modeling to disentangle depression-specific anhedonia, anxiety-specific anxious arousal, and the common internalizing factor, general distress, and test whether these internalizing symptoms interact with sleep to influence memory for emotional and neutral information. Healthy adults ( N = 281) encoded scenes featuring either negative objects (e.g., a vicious looking snake) or neutral objects (e.g., a chipmunk) placed on neutral backgrounds (e.g., an outdoor scene). After a 12-hour period of daytime wakefulness ( n = 140) or nocturnal sleep ( n = 141), participants judged whether objects and backgrounds were the same, similar, or new compared with what they viewed during encoding. Participants also completed the mini version of the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. Higher anxious arousal predicted worse memory across all stimuli features, but only after a day spent being awake—not following a night of sleep. No significant effects were found for general distress and anhedonia in either the sleep or wake condition. In this study, internalizing symptoms were not associated with enhanced emotional memory. Instead, memory performance specifically in individuals with higher anxious arousal was impaired overall, regardless of emotional valence, but this was only the case when the retention interval spanned wakefulness (i.e., not when it spanned sleep). This suggests that sleep may confer a protective effect on general memory impairments associated with anxiety.
... There is, however, contradicting research and some studies have not demonstrated that effect (e.g., Atienza and Cantero, 2008;Morgenthaler et al., 2014;Tempesta et al., 2017;Bolinger et al., 2018;Jones et al., 2018). Atienza and Cantero (2008) conducted a study with 28 participants and found that although emotional images are better remembered than neutral ones after sleep, arousal played a much stronger role than valence. ...
... Atienza and Cantero (2008) conducted a study with 28 participants and found that although emotional images are better remembered than neutral ones after sleep, arousal played a much stronger role than valence. Bolinger et al. (2018), conducted a study with 16 participants and did not find preferential enhancement of negative stimuli over neutral. Therefore, in a metaanalysis assessing the current literature, Lipinska et al. (2019), found sleep did not preferentially consolidate emotional memory in comparison to waking unless certain methodological criteria were observed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Research in the field of cognitive neuroscience has focused on the role of sleep in various neurocognitive processes such as memory consolidation. However, an area that has not been adequately researched is the role of dreaming in this memory process. This study aimed to determine the relationship between affect experienced in dreams and emotional memory consolidation. Considering that REM dreams are laden with emotion and that emotion enhances memory, one possibility is that dream affect could also play a role in emotional memory consolidation. We hypothesised that greater dream-related affect would be associated with greater memory retention of emotional but not neutral information. Methods 126 healthy participants, aged 18–35, were recruited for the online study, of which 103 participants had valid data (female: n =73). On the night of the study, participants viewed a series of pictures from the South African Affective Picture System (SA-APS) in an online session. Afterwards, they verbally recalled as many pictures as possible. The following morning, they were asked to recall any dreams and rate the emotional intensity of their dreams. Participants then again verbally recalled all the pictures that they could remember from the previous night. Results Contrary to the prediction, dream-related affect, regardless of valence, did not predict memory consolidation of positive or negative information. Instead, increases in dream-related affect, and especially anxiety were predictive of better memory retention of all information. The findings also showed that an increase in negative affect in dreams predicted better memory retention of negative information. Discussion Our results suggest that dream affect is an important modulator of memory consolidation processes occurring during sleep. Furthermore, increased negative affect may indicate which experiences are salient and require consolidation to form long-lasting memories that can guide future behaviour. Conclusion These findings have implications for psychiatric disorders, such as major depression, which is characterised by negative affect and increased memory sensitivity to negative stimuli.
... Such preferential enhancement allows the brain to focus on the most important information encountered during the prior day, leading to improved decisionmaking and creativity after a night of sleep [95]. This selective enhancement of emotional content has been shown in some studies with young [96] and older children [97,98] but see [99], suggesting that sleep may have a similar ability to preferentially support consolidation of important new material during development. Although there is some disagreement in the literature as to the reliability of sleep-related emotional enhancement effects in TD participants (see [100•, 101] for discussion of when this effect does and does not appear), there are several reasons why it may be critically important to separately consider how such processes operate in individuals with developmental disabilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review Sleep plays an important role in the long-term consolidation of memories across development. The current review examines sleep-dependent memory consolidation in five developmental disability diagnoses that experience significant sleep disturbances. A focus is placed on understanding how specific neural and mnemonic features of particular diagnoses contribute to distinct alterations to consolidation. Recent Findings Sleep and memory changes have been identified across diagnoses, but they differ in the extent to which sleep-related consolidation processes are specifically affected. In addition, Down syndrome may represent a unique case where sleep actually impairs memory consolidation, potentially related to profound disruptions to hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity. Summary Deficits in sleep-dependent memory consolidation are heterogeneous across developmental disabilities and are caused by a confluence of cognitive and neural factors. Sleep and memory research should target these specific population profiles. Future research should also examine sleep effects on forms of memory that may depend on alternate neural pathways, such as emotional memory.
... However, it is worth noting that most of these previous studies examined collegeaged adults (Payne et al., 2008(Payne et al., , 2012Cunningham et al., 2014;Bennion et al., 2015Bennion et al., , 2017, and the functional link between sleep and memory likely weakens with advancing age (Tucker et al., 2011;Scullin, 2013). Overall, this is in line with the existing literature that the prioritization of emotional memory during sleep is more of an exception than the norm (Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009, 2013Baran et al., 2012;Morgenthaler et al., 2014;Göder et al., 2015;Bolinger et al., 2018;Kurz et al., 2019;Davidson et al., 2021). Additionally, previous studies indicate that the proportion of rapid eye movement sleep and slow-wave sleep (Cairney et al., 2015) and the local coupling between sleep spindles and slow oscillations (Solano et al., 2021) both play crucial roles in memory consolidation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: While younger adults are more likely to attend to, process, and remember negative relative to positive information, healthy older adults show the opposite pattern. The current study evaluates when, exactly, this positivity shift begins, and how it influences memory performance for positive, negative, and neutral information. Methods: A total of 274 healthy early middle-aged (35–47), late middle-aged (48–59), and older adults (>59) viewed scenes consisting of a negative, positive, or a neutral object placed on a plausible neutral background, and rated each scene for its valence and arousal. After 12 h spanning a night of sleep (n = 137) or a day of wakefulness (n = 137), participants completed an unexpected memory test during which they were shown objects and backgrounds separately and indicated whether the scene component was the “same,” “similar,” or “new” to what they viewed during the study session. Results and conclusions: We found that both late middle-aged and older adults rated positive and neutral scenes more positively compared to early middle-aged adults. However, only older adults showed better memory for positive objects relative to negative objects, and a greater positive memory trade-off magnitude (i.e., remembering positive objects at the cost of their associated neutral backgrounds) than negative memory trade-off magnitude (i.e., remembering negative objects at the cost of their associated neutral backgrounds). Our findings suggest that while the positivity bias may not emerge in memory until older adulthood, a shift toward positivity in terms of processing may begin in middle age.
... It is also worth noting that the task also only probes participants' recognition of the faces, and not their reactivity or perception of the faces' affectivity. Therefore, we cannot specifically explore the role of sleep in the consolidation of affective components of emotional memory with our results, as has been done in some recent studies of sleep's effects on emotional memory in older children (e.g., Bolinger et al., 2018). Similarly, given that this task relies indirectly on "mean" verbal statements to imbue negative valence on otherwise unemotive face stimuli, it is possible that children's recognition of the "negative" faces in this task is being driven more by the saliency of social rule violations than by induction of negative emotions per se. ...
Article
Full-text available
Early childhood naps support emotional memory, but benefits are only observed after overnight sleep. Whether emotional memory consolidation occurs during naps, or whether napping only prepares memories for overnight consolidation is unknown. We investigated whether naps protect emotional memories from interference, indicating consolidation. Between 2018 and 2020, 63 children in western Massachusetts preschools (30 female, 33 male; 33–67 months; 23.8% Hispanic, 87.3% White) learned faces paired with negative or neutral descriptions, followed by nap or wake. Before delayed recognition, half completed an interference task. Without interference, napping benefited recognition. With interference, children recognized fewer negative faces post‐nap (compared to wake), with overnight sleep attenuating this difference. Results suggest that naps initially destabilize emotional memories, possibly reflecting partial processing that promotes long‐term consolidation.
Preprint
Full-text available
Emotional responses are dampened across sleep, and this is thought to be mediated by neural reactivation during Rapid Eye Movement (REM) Sleep. Such reactivation can be triggered by targeted memory reactivation (TMR), a technique in which a tone previously associated with a memory during wake is re-presented during subsequent sleep. Prior work has shown that TMR in REM reduces arousal responses to negative stimuli. The present study builds on this by measuring autonomic responses and brain activity as well as behaviour. Participants rated the arousal of 48 affective images, paired with semantically matching sounds. Half of these sounds were cued during REM in the subsequent overnight sleep cycle. Participants rated the images in a Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scanner with pulse oximetry 48 hours after encoding, and again after two weeks. Results showed that TMR during REM was also associated with reduced brain activity in the two primary nodes of the Salience Network (SN): the Anterior Insula and dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex (dACC), as well as the orbitofrontal cortex, subgenual cingulate, and left amygdala, all of which are known to be important for emotional processing. TMR markedly reduced the emotional heart rate deceleration (HRD) response, and also reduced subjective arousal ratings for highly arousing images, while increasing ratings for less arousing images. We conclude that REM TMR can facilitate a decrease in physiological and neurological responses to arousal. These findings have potential implications for the use of TMR in treatment of depression and anxiety disorders. Highlights TMR in REM sleep reduces Salience Network responses to emotional pictures. TMR in REM sleep reduces heart rate deceleration to emotional pictures. TMR in REM sleep reduces subjective arousal ratings of highly arousing images. TMR in REM sleep provides a promising potential avenue for treatment of PTSD.
Article
Background: Sleep-related problems often precede escalating anxiety in early adolescence. Pushing beyond broad sleep-mental health associations and toward mechanistic theories of their interplay can inform etiological models of psychopathology. Recent studies suggest that sleep de-potentiates neural (e.g., amygdala) reactivity during re-exposure to negative emotional stimuli in adults. Persistent amygdala reactivity to negative experiences and poor sleep characterize anxiety, particularly at the transition to adolescence. We propose that sleep de-potentiates amygdala reactivity in youth but fails to do so among youth with anxiety. Methods: Participants (n = 34; 18 males; age, M = 11.35, SD = 2.00) recruited from the community and specialty anxiety clinics viewed valenced images (positive, negative, and neutral) across two fMRI sessions (Study, Test), separated by a 10–12-hour retention period of sleep or wake (randomized). Mixed linear models regressed basolateral amygdala (BLA) activation and BLA- medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) functional connectivity to negative images on Time, Condition, and Anxiety Severity. Results: There were greater reductions in BLA activations to negative target images from Study to Test in the Sleep Condition, which was blunted with higher anxiety (b = -.065, z = -2.355, p = .019). No such sleep- or anxiety-related effects were observed for BLA-mPFC functional connectivity (ps > .05). Conclusions: Sleep supports de-potentiation of amygdala reactivity to negative stimuli in youth, but this effect is blunted at higher levels of anxiety. Disruptions in sleep-related affective habituation may be a critical, modifiable driver of anxiety.
Article
Full-text available
The ability to suppress unwanted emotional memories is crucial for human mental health. Through consolidation over time, emotional memories often become resistant to change. However, how consolidation impacts the effectiveness of emotional memory suppression is still unknown. Using event-related fMRI while concurrently recording skin conductance, we investigated the neurobiological processes underlying the suppression of aversive memories before and after overnight consolidation. Here we report that consolidated aversive memories retain their emotional reactivity and become more resistant to suppression. Suppression of consolidated memories involves higher prefrontal engagement, and less concomitant hippocampal and amygdala disengagement. In parallel, we show a shift away from hippocampal-dependent representational patterns to distributed neocortical representational patterns in the suppression of aversive memories after consolidation. These findings demonstrate rapid changes in emotional memory organization with overnight consolidation, and suggest possible neurobiological bases underlying the resistance to suppression of emotional memories in affective disorders.
Article
Full-text available
The 10 min psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) is commonly used in laboratory studies to assess the impact of sleep loss, sustained wakefulness, and/or time of day on neurobehavioral performance. In field settings, though, it may be impractical for participants to perform a test of this length. The aim of this study was to identify a performance measure that is sensitive to the effects of fatigue but less burdensome than a 10 min test. Sixteen participants (11 female, 5 male; mean age ¼ 21.7 years) slept in the sleep laboratory overnight then remained awake for 28 h from 08:00 h. During every second hour, participants completed three PVTs of differing duration (10 min, 5 min, 90 sec). For the 5 min/10 min comparison, ANOVA indicated that response time was significantly affected by test length (F1,14 ¼ 26.9, p , .001) and hours of wakefulness (F13,182 ¼ 46.1, p , .001) but not by their interaction (F13,182 ¼ 1.7, ns). There was a strong correlation between response time on the 5 and 10 min PVTs (r ¼ .88, p , .001). For the 90 sec/10 min comparison, ANOVA indicated that response time was significantly affected by test length (F1,14 ¼ 65.9, p , .001) and hours of wakefulness (F13,182 ¼ 29.7, p , .001) as well as by their interaction (F13,182 ¼ 6.0, p , .001). There was a strong correlation between response time on the 90 sec and 10 min PVTs (r ¼ .77, p , .001). The effects of hours of wakefulness on neurobehavioral performance were similar for the 5 min and 10 min PVTs. In contrast, performance on the 90 sec PVT was less affected by hours of wakefulness than on the 10 min PVT. In addition, performance on the 10 min PVT was more highly correlated with the 5 min PVT than the 90 sec PVT. These data indicate that the 5 min PVT may provide a reasonable substitute for the 10 min PVT in circumstances where a test shorter than 10 min is required.
Article
Full-text available
Since the advent of EEG recordings, sleep spindles have been identified as hallmarks of non-REM sleep. Despite a broad general understanding of mechanisms of spindle generation gleaned from animal studies, the mechanisms underlying certain features of spindles in the human brain, such as “global” versus “local” spindles, are largely unknown. Neither the topography nor the morphology of sleep spindles remains constant throughout the lifespan. It is likely that changes in spindle phenomenology during development and aging are the result of dramatic changes in brain structure and function. Across various developmental windows, spindle activity is correlated with general cognitive aptitude, learning, and memory; however, these correlations vary in strength, and even direction, depending on age and metrics used. Understanding these differences across the lifespan should further clarify how these oscillations are generated and their function under a variety of circumstances. We discuss these issues, and their translational implications for human cognitive function. Because sleep spindles are similarly affected in disorders of neurodevelopment (such as schizophrenia) and during aging (such as neurodegenerative conditions), both types of disorders may benefit from therapies based on a better understanding of spindle function.
Article
Full-text available
Sleep has a profound impact on memory consolidation. In this study, human participants underwent Pavlovian conditioning and extinction before we manipulated nocturnal memory consolidation by a split-night protocol with 80 healthy male participants in four groups. Recall after a second (recovery) night of sleep revealed that sleeping the first half of the night, which is dominated by slow-wave sleep, did not improve recall. Conversely, sleeping the second half of the night, which is dominated by rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, led to better discrimination between fear-relevant and neutral stimuli in behavioral and autonomic measures. Meanwhile, staying awake in the second half of the night led to an increase of discrimination between extinguished and neutral stimuli, which was paralleled by an activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. We conclude that sleep, especially REM sleep, is causal to successful consolidation of dangerous and safety stimuli and reduces return of fear after extinction. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: We use a split-night protocol to investigate the influence of different sleep phases on successful consolidation of conditioned fear and extinction. Such a protocol uses the fact that in humans the first half of the night is dominated by slow-wave sleep, whereas during the second half, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is more predominant. Our data show that only REM-rich sleep during the second half of the night promoted good discrimination between fear-relevant and neutral stimuli during recall, while staying awake led to a recovery of discrimination between extinguished and neutral stimuli. This suggests that sleep following extinction contributes independently to successful extinction memory consolidation.
Article
Full-text available
In the article by S. Schachter and J. Singer, which appeared in Psychological Review (1962, 69(5), 379-399) the following corrections should be made: The superscript "a" should precede the word "All" in the footnote to Table 2. The superscript "a" should appear next to the column heading "Initiates" in Table 3. The following Tables 6-9 should be substituted for those which appeared in print. (The following abstract of this article originally appeared in record 196306064-001.) It is suggested that emotional states may be considered a function of a state of physiological arousal and of a cognition appropriate to this state of arousal. From this follows these propositions: (a) Given a state of physiological arousal for which an individual has no immediate explanation, he will label this state and describe his feelings in terms of the cognitions available to him. (b) Given a state of physiological arousal for which an individual has a completely appropriate explanation, no evaluative needs will arise and the individual is unlikely to label his feelings in terms of the alternative cognitions available. (c) Given the same cognitive circumstances, the individual will react emotionally or describe his feelings as emotions only to the extent that he experiences a state of physiological arousal. An experiment is described which, together with the results of other studies, supports these propositions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep has been postulated to facilitate emotional processing of negative stimuli. However, empirical evidence is mixed and primarily based on self-report data and picture-viewing studies. This study used a full-length aversive film to elicit intense emotion on one evening, and an emotionally neutral control film on another evening while psychophysiological and experiential responses were measured. Subsequent sleep was monitored polysomnographically, and specific film scenes were presented again on the next morning. Correlation analyses revealed that participants with longer late-night REM sleep after the aversive film showed higher increase of electrodermal reactivity and less reduction of facial corrugator muscle reactivity to negative film scenes on the next morning. This indicates that REM sleep may be associated with attenuated emotional processing of prolonged and intense emotional stimuli from pre- to postsleep.
Article
Full-text available
In the adult literature, emotional arousal is regarded as a source of the enhancing effect of emotion on subsequent memory. Here, we used behavioral, electrophysiological, and psychophysiological methods to examine the role of emotional arousal on subsequent memory in school-age children. Five- to 8-year-olds, divided into younger and older groups, viewed emotional scenes as EEG, heart rate, and respiration was recorded, and participated in a memory task 24 hours later where EEG and behavioral responses were recorded; participants provided subjective ratings of the scenes after the memory task. All measures indicated emotion responses in both groups, and in ERP measures the effects were stronger for older children. Emotion responses were more consistent across measures for negative than positive stimuli. Behavioral memory performance was strong but did not differ by emotion condition. Emotion influenced the ERP index of recognition memory in the older group only (enhanced recognition of negative scenes). The findings an increasing interaction of emotion and memory during the school years. Further, the findings impress the value of combining multiple methods to assess emotion and memory in development. Development in the neurophysiology of emotion processing and memory in school-age children.
Article
The ability to use cognitive emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal may be a core component of emotional competence across development, but due to methodological challenges in measuring such strategies, they are rarely studied in young children. One neurophysiological measure, the late positive potential (LPP), has been examined in response to reappraisal as a potential neurosignature for emotion regulatory capacity in adults. The association between the LPP and emotion regulatory capacity in children is unknown. The present study examined whether the LPP during reappraisal could predict greater observed adaptive emotion regulation strategy use in school-aged children over a two-year period. Thirty-two 5- to 7-year-olds participated in two identical lab visits spaced two years apart. EEG was continuously recorded during a computerized reappraisal task in which children viewed unpleasant images paired with either reappraisal or negative stories. Next they completed a disappointing and a frustrating task during which emotion regulation strategies were observed. As predicted, children who showed reappraisal-induced reductions in the LPP at the first assessment used significantly more adaptive ER strategies concurrently and two years later. These findings provide observation-based evidence that the LPP may be a viable neurosignature for emotion regulatory capacity in children.