ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

It is well established that composite facial images are perceived as more attractive compared with individual images, suggesting a preference for heterozygosity. Similarly, there is evidence that preferences for body odours might be linked to heterozygosity. Here, we tested whether blending individual body odours into composites would follow a similar pattern as observed in the perception of faces. We collected axillary odour samples from 38 individuals, which were subsequently assessed individually and as composites of two (N = 19) or four (N = 9) body odours regarding their pleasantness, attractiveness and intensity. We found no significant differences between mean ratings of individual odour samples or composites of two or four odour samples. Our results indicate that, in contrast to faces, composite body odours are not rated as more attractive. Composite body odours retain similar hedonic perceptual qualities as individual odours, thus highlighting differences in visual and chemosensory perceptual mechanisms.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Article
Human Body Odour
Composites Are Not
Perceived More Positively
than the Individual Samples
Jitka Fialova
´
Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic; National
Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czech Republic
Agnieszka Sorokowska
Institute of Psychology, University of Wroclaw, Poland; Smell & Taste
Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, TU Dresden, Germany
S. Craig Roberts
School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, UK
Lydie Kubicova
´
Faculty of Humanities, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
Jan Havlı
´c
ˇek
Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic; National
Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czech Republic
Abstract
It is well established that composite facial images are perceived as more attractive compared with
individual images, suggesting a preference for heterozygosity. Similarly, there is evidence that
preferences for body odours might be linked to heterozygosity. Here, we tested whether
blending individual body odours into composites would follow a similar pattern as observed in
the perception of faces. We collected axillary odour samples from 38 individuals, which were
subsequently assessed individually and as composites of two (N¼19) or four (N¼9) body odours
regarding their pleasantness, attractiveness and intensity. We found no significant differences
between mean ratings of individual odour samples or composites of two or four odour
samples. Our results indicate that, in contrast to faces, composite body odours are not rated
as more attractive. Composite body odours retain similar hedonic perceptual qualities as individual
odours, thus highlighting differences in visual and chemosensory perceptual mechanisms.
Corresponding author:
Jitka Fialova
´, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Vinic
ˇna
´7, 128 44 Prague, Czech Republic.
Email: jitka.fialova@natur.cuni.cz
Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License
(http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without
further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sage-
pub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
i-Perception
2018 Vol. 9(3), 1–15
!The Author(s) 2018
DOI: 10.1177/2041669518766367
journals.sagepub.com/home/ipe
Keywords
heterozygosity, averageness, odour blend, olfaction, MHC, mate preferences
Date received: 17 May 2017; accepted: 26 February 2018
Introduction
A robust body of evidence indicates that composite facial images are perceived as more
attractive compared with the mean attractiveness rating of their constituent images. The
phenomenon was first noted by Galton (1879) who used photographic superimposing
techniques to combine facial images. More recent studies have improved upon these
techniques, using computer graphics to generate composite facial images (Grammer &
Thornhill, 1994; Langlois, Roggman, & Musselman, 1994; Rhodes, Sumich, & Byatt, 1999;
Rhodes & Tremewan, 1996), and finding that composites are considered more attractive than
almost all constituent faces. Furthermore, there is generally a positive association between
the number of individual images constituting a composite image and its attractiveness and
averageness ratings (Langlois & Roggman, 1990), although average faces may not be the
most attractive (Perrett, May, & Yoshikawa, 1994).
The attractiveness of average faces may result from two different mechanisms. First, it
might be a by-product of visual system processing by which average faces can be processed
faster because they resemble a mental representation of a prototypical face (Enquist & Arak,
1994). Indeed, it appears that prototypes are processed faster and have a higher probability of
recognition (Posner & Keele, 1968; Smith, Shoben, & Rips, 1974), and visual processing is
more fluent as reflected in decreased activity in the posterior occipital cortex (Aizenstein
et al., 2000). In addition, there is a positive relationship between prototypicality and
preference, consistent with the observation that an increase in processing fluency also
increases liking (Martindale & Moore, 1988; Whitfield & Slatter, 1979). An alternative
explanation proposes that facial averageness might be a marker of heterozygosity (i.e.,
genetic diversity at certain loci; Lie, Rhodes, & Simmons, 2008; Thornhill & Gangestad,
1993). It has therefore been argued that attractiveness of average faces might be a
perceptual adaptation to favour heterozygous individuals (Thornhill & Gangestad, 1993).
Heterozygosity in some loci positively affects viability of the organism, as deleterious
alleles are predominantly expressed in recessive fashion (i.e., only in homozygotes).
Further, benefits of mating with heterozygous individuals may include reduced risk of
disease transmission and potential for high-quality paternal care (Kirkpatrick & Ryan,
1991; Roberts et al., 2005).
Facial attractiveness and averageness is associated with heterozygosity in the genes of the
major histocompatibility complex (MHC; Lie et al., 2008; Roberts et al., 2005). The MHC
complex appears to be involved in mate selection processes of various vertebrate species
including humans (Kamiya, O’Dwyer, Westerdahl, Senior, & Nakagawa, 2014; Winternitz,
Abbate, Huchard, Havlı
´c
ˇek, & Garamszegi, 2017). MHC genes are extremely polymorphic
and code for cell-surface peptides that are responsible for recognition of foreign antigens and
thereby initiating an immune response. As the MHC is expressed codominantly,
heterozygous individuals are able to present a broader spectrum of peptides and thus
provide resistance to a wider range of pathogens compared with MHC homozygotes
(Havlı
´c
ˇek & Roberts, 2009). Consistent with this, MHC heterozygotes have faces that
appear more healthy (Roberts et al., 2005). There is also some evidence showing
preferences for body odour of MHC heterozygotes: Male body odour attractiveness as
2i-Perception 9(3)
rated by women was positively associated with MHC heterozygosity, but there was no similar
pattern when men rated female body odour samples (Thornhill et al., 2003).
Based on this evidence, one may speculate whether composite body odours might
perceptually mimic odours of heterozygote individuals, as has been observed in studies of
composite faces. If so, one would predict that body odour composites—individual odours
blended and presented together—would be rated more positively than the individual samples.
A test of this prediction could have important consequences for the methodology of body
odour studies. Numerous previous studies have presented composite odours to assessors to
test potential communicatory significance of body odours. They use composite odours in an
attempt to achieve a representative odour sample associated with a particular characteristic
or state of interest while reducing the importance of individual variability in odour profiles.
Thus, the rationale behind these studies is that composite body odours (usually created by
pooling odour samples from people with a given characteristic) will contain shared qualities
based on their group attributes, such as gender (Lu
¨bke et al., 2014; Mutic, Moellers,
Wiesmann, & Freiherr, 2016) and sexual orientation (Martins et al., 2005). Similarly,
composite body odours have been used for testing various affective states such as
happiness and fear (Chen & Haviland-Jones, 2000), disgust (de Groot, Smeets, Kaldewaij,
Duijndam, & Semin, 2012), sadness (Gelstein et al., 2011), anxiety (Haegler et al., 2010;
Pause, Adolph, Prehn-Kristensen, & Ferstl, 2009; Pause, Lu
¨bke, Laudien, & Ferstl, 2010;
Pause, Ohrt, Prehn, & Ferstl, 2004; Prehn-Kristensen et al., 2009; Zernecke et al., 2011), and
stress (Dalton, Maute
´, Jae
´n, & Wilson, 2013; Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009; Prehn, Ohrt, Sojka,
Ferstl, & Pause, 2006; Radulescu & Mujica-Parodi, 2013). While the approach used in such
studies is potentially very useful, it is currently unknown whether creating such composites
might influence hedonic qualities.
In light of this, here we aimed to test the effects of composite odour creation on the
perception of odour attractiveness, pleasantness and intensity. We created odour composites
comprising two and four individual odours and tested whether these composite body odours
are rated more positively compared with the distinct odours from the same individuals.
Methods
Raters
The individual samples were assessed by 110 raters (56 men, mean age 24.1 years, range 18–
34; 54 women, mean age 23.4, range 19–35) as part of previously reported studies (Fialova
´&
Havlı
´c
ˇek, 2012; Fialova
´, Roberts, & Havlı
´c
ˇek, 2016). The composite samples were assessed
by 98 raters (50 men, mean age 23.9, range 19–33; 48 women, mean age 22.8, range 19–35).
The two sets of raters were independent—no rater assessed both individual and composites.
The raters were mostly Charles University students and were contacted via e-mail, posters or
by oral invitation. Raters reported no respiratory or other diseases at the time of the study or
any problems or medication that could influence their olfactory abilities. All women were
using hormonal contraception to avoid changes in olfactory perception during the menstrual
cycle (Martinec Nova
´kova
´, Havlı
´c
ˇek, & Roberts, 2014). Following the procedure used in
previous studies, we assumed no systematic fluctuation in olfactory ability over time due to
hormonal contraceptive use, and thus scheduling of assessments was unrestricted (e.g.,
Fialova
´et al., 2016; Kohoutova
´, Rubes
ˇova
´, & Havlı
´c
ˇek, 2011; Sobotkova
´, Fialova
´,
Roberts, & Havlı
´c
ˇek, 2017). Raters received 100 CZK (approximately US$5) as
compensation for their time.
The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of
Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the institutional
Fialova
´et al. 3
review board of Charles University, Faculty of Science. Written informed consent was
obtained from all participants.
Odour Stimuli
Twenty-six men (mean age 25.2 years; range 18–34) and 12 women (mean age 22.4; range 20–
26), mostly students at Charles University (Prague, Czech Republic), participated as odour
donors (sample size varies across men and women because the participants were originally
recruited for the purpose of two other studies). All donors were nonsmokers and reported no
dermatological or other diseases at the time of the study. No men shaved their armpits, while
all women shaved their armpits; axillary shaving was kept constant within sex as it might
affect perceived quality of the axillary odour (Kohoutova
´et al., 2011). All women used
hormonal contraception to avoid possible body odour quality fluctuations across the
menstrual cycle (Havlı
´c
ˇek, Dvor
ˇa
´kova
´, Bartos
ˇ, & Flegr, 2006; Kuukasjarvi et al., 2004).
Male and female donors were given 400 CZK (approximately US$20) and 1,000 CZK
(approximately US$40), respectively, in compensation for their time and potential
inconvenience caused by the prescribed diet and according to duration of the study and its
demands.
The odour donors were asked to avoid consuming smelly and spicy food, alcohol, smoking
or using any cosmetics on the day before and during the sampling day (48 hr overall). They
attached 100% cotton pads under each armpit using surgical tape and wore the pads for 12 h
overnight (see Havlı
´c
ˇek, Lenochova
´, Oberzaucher, Grammer, & Roberts, 2011). To avoid
odour contamination from extrinsic ambient odours, the donors were asked to wear as the
first layer of clothing a new white 100% cotton T-shirt that had been previously washed
without washing powder. In the morning, they placed the pads in ziplock plastic bags and
returned them to the experimenters. The samples were immediately placed in a freezer at
21C to prevent further microbial action and possible changes in odour quality. Freezing
has been shown to have no significant effect on hedonic ratings (Lenochova, Roberts, &
Havlicek, 2008). Time elapsed between removing the pads and onset of freezing was
approximately 1 to 2 hr. Each donor’s conformity with the instructions was checked by a
questionnaire, and no violations on the day of sampling were recorded.
To create composite body odours, we first ranked the individual samples of either sex based
on their attractiveness ratings. Male and female composite odours were then created by
blending two or four individual samples. We used previously unused pads that had been
collected at the same time from each individual; to do this, we used the pads collected from
the other armpit than the one that had been used for previous ratings of the individual samples.
Each pad was cut into half and pooled with halved pads from other same-sex individuals who
lay adjacent along the attractiveness continuum. In this way, we obtained 19 composites
comprised of 2 individual samples and 9 composites comprised of 4 individual samples.
Odour Rating Procedure
Ratings took place in a quiet, ventilated room. The samples from one randomly chosen
armpit of each odour donor were assessed individually, by the first set of raters. The
second set of raters assessed the composites, originating from the other armpit and pooled
as described earlier. Both kinds of samples were presented in 250 ml opaque jars labelled with
a code. Participants were asked to sniff each jar; ratings were recorded immediately after
sniffing each stimulus, but the time spent sniffing was not restricted. To avoid adaptation, the
samples were randomly split into subsets, and raters were given approximately 10 min break
4i-Perception 9(3)
between assessing each set. All samples were thawed before the rating session and then
assessed in a randomized order regarding their (a) pleasantness, (b) attractiveness and (c)
intensity, each on a 7-point scale. Both ends of each scale were verbally anchored by
descriptors (e.g., very unpleasant and very pleasant). If raters found any of the samples too
weak to assess, they could select an option ‘‘I cannot smell the sample’’ instead of rating using
the scales (this occurred for 4.9% of the individual samples, 3.4% of the two-composite body
odour samples, and 4% of the four-composite body odour samples). Such instances were not
included in analyses, and hence the sample sizes of analyses can vary.
Statistical Analysis
Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests showed normal data distribution for all dependent variables. We
computed mean values from the ratings of the individual samples that were used for creating
composite body odour samples and compared them with actual ratings of composite body
odour samples using paired ttests (e.g., for the two-odour composites, the ratings of 19
composites were compared with 19 average scores from their constituent individual odour
pairs). To investigate sex differences in ratings of composite body odour samples, we used a
two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with odour type and sex as a factor (two levels: two-
and four-composite body odours and two levels: male, female, respectively). To explore
whether composite body odour samples retain qualities of the individual odours, we first
ranked the individual samples of either sex based on their attractiveness ratings and then
split two- and four-composite body odours by the median value of the constituent samples.
Subsequently, we compared the above median and below median samples by an independent
samples ttest. In what follows, mean values calculated from ratings of the individual samples
and actual ratings of the composite body odour samples are referred to as calculated samples
and rated samples, respectively. We also tested for possible relationships between individual
samples and composite body odour samples using bivariate correlations.
Results
Ratings of the Individual Versus Composite Body Odours
We found no significant differences between mean ratings of the individual samples and
composite body odour samples (Figure 1; all ps>.16; for detailed results, see Tables 1
and 2). These means were calculated from the full sample of raters (i.e., both male and
female raters). Subsequently, we performed a two-way ANOVA for ratings of only
opposite-sex odour samples. This also did not reveal any significant differences in women’s
ratings of individual, two- and four-composite male odour samples, neither for pleasantness,
F(2, 68) ¼0.008, p¼.992; attractiveness, F(2, 68) ¼0.159, p¼.854 or intensity,
F(2, 68) ¼0.287, p¼.752. Similarly, there were no significant differences in men’s ratings of
individual, two- and four-composite female odour samples, for pleasantness, F(2, 30) ¼0.605,
p¼.553; attractiveness, F(2, 30) ¼0.726, p¼.492 or intensity, F(2, 30) ¼0.017, p¼.983.
To test whether composite body odours retain properties of the individual samples, we
split two- and four-composite body odours by the median value of the constituent samples.
We found that two-composite body odours consisting of above median samples were
perceived as more pleasant, t(16) ¼6.213, p<.001; attractive, t(16) ¼6.466, p<.001 and
intense, t(16) ¼7.756, p<.001, compared with below median samples. However,
differences were not significant in four-composite body odours either in pleasantness,
t(6) ¼0.437, p¼.678; attractiveness, t(6) ¼0.601, p¼.570 or intensity, t(6) ¼0.387,
p¼.712; see Figure 2(a) and (b). For explorative purposes, we further performed
Fialova
´et al. 5
a one-way ANOVA comparing individual four-composite samples and found significant
differences in their attractiveness, F(8, 349) ¼9.744, p<.001. A Tukey post hoc test
revealed that Sample 1 was rated significantly more attractive than Sample 5 (p¼.042)
and Samples 8 and 9 (p<.001). Moreover, Sample 2 was significantly more attractive than
all samples (p¼.05) except Sample 1 (see Figure 3).
Sex Differences in Ratings of the Composite Body Odours
A two-way ANOVA showed that male and female ratings did not significantly differ in
pleasantness, F(1, 52) ¼1.092, p¼.301; attractiveness, F(1, 52) ¼1.143, p¼.290 and
Figure 1. Mean ratings (95% CI) of calculated individual (white bars), rated two- (light grey bars) and
four- (dark grey bars) composite body odour samples concerning their pleasantness, attractiveness and
intensity. The individual samples were assessed by 110 raters and composite stimuli by 98 raters using 7-point
scale (e.g., 1 ¼very unpleasant and 7 ¼very pleasant).
6i-Perception 9(3)
intensity, F(1, 52) ¼0.575, p¼.452, nor was there any interaction between rater sex and
number of composite body odour samples, for pleasantness, F(1, 52) ¼0.040, p¼.842;
attractiveness, F(1, 52) ¼0.108, p¼.743 or intensity, F(1, 52) ¼0.060, p¼.807.
Correlation Between Individual and Composite Body Odours
We found significant positive correlations between ratings of two- and four-composite body
odour samples and mean values of the individual odours, for pleasantness (two-samples:
r¼.663, p¼.002; four-samples: r¼.783, p¼.013), attractiveness (two-samples: r¼.647,
p¼.003; four-samples: r¼.873, p¼.002) and intensity (two-samples: r¼.603, p¼.006;
four-samples: r¼.589, p¼.095; see Figure 4(a) and (b), Tables 3 and 4).
Correlation Between Ratings of Characteristics
Very strong positive correlations between pleasantness and attractiveness ratings (two-
samples: r¼.998, p<.001; four-samples: r¼.981, p<.001), and very strong negative
correlations between pleasantness and intensity (two-samples: r¼.862, p<.001; four-
Table 2. Comparison of Mean Ratings (SD) of the Individual Samples and Two- and Four-Composite Body
Odour Samples Concerning Their Pleasantness, Attractiveness and Intensity.
Characteristic Individual sample/composite M(SD) Paired samples tdfpCohen’s d
Pleasantness 1 3.16 (0.81) 1 2 0.25 18 .8 0.09
2 3.08 (1) 1 4 0.48 8 .64 0.05
4 3.2 (0.73) 2 4 1.02 8 .34 0.14
Attractiveness 1 3.1 (0.75) 1 2 0.98 18 .34 0.26
2 2.87 (0.98) 1 4 0.08 8 .94 0.07
4 3.05 (0.69) 2 4 0.87 8 .41 0.21
Intensity 1 4.06 (1.06) 1 2 0.35 18 .73 0.09
2 4.16 (1.08) 1 4 0.17 8 .87 0.1
4 3.97 (0.76) 2 4 0.62 8 .55 0.2
Table 1. Differences Between Mean Ratings (SD) of Pleasantness, Attractiveness and Intensity of the
Actually Rated Composite Body Odour Samples and Mean Values Calculated From Ratings of the Respective
Individual Samples.
Composite Characteristic M(SD)tdfpCohen’s d
2 Pleasantness Rated 3.08 (1.0) 0.56 18 .58 0.1
Calculated 3.17 (0.73)
2 Attractiveness Rated 2.87 (0.98) 1.48 18 .16 0.37
Calculated 3.13 (0.16)
2 Intensity Rated 4.16 (1.08) 0.63 18 .53 0.17
Calculated 4.03 (0.19)
4 Pleasantness Rated 3.2 (0.73) 1.11 8 .3 0.25
Calculated 3.38 (0.72)
4 Attractiveness Rated 3.05 (0.69) 2.11 8 .67 0.35
Calculated 3.29 (0.68)
4 Intensity Rated 3.97 (0.76) 0.59 8 .57 0.18
Calculated 3.81 (0.96)
Fialova
´et al. 7
Figure 3. Mean attractiveness ratings (95% CI) of four-composite body odour samples. Numbers at
individual bars indicate rank of each sample based on mean values calculated from attractiveness ratings of the
respective individual odours.
Figure 2. Mean attractiveness ratings (95% CI) of (a) two-composite body odours and (b) four-
composite body odours above (white bars) and below (grey bars) the median. Asterisk indicates level of
significance; ***p<.001 level.
8i-Perception 9(3)
samples: r¼.936, p<.001) and between attractiveness and intensity (two-samples:
r¼.856, p<.001; four-samples: r¼.913, p¼.001) were found (for more details, see
Tables 3 and 4).
Discussion
The main aim of this study was to test whether composite body odours are rated more
positively as compared with the individual samples collected from the same individuals.
Figure 4. Positive correlation between attractiveness ratings of (a) two-composite body odour samples
(r¼.63) and (b) four-composite body odour samples (r¼.87) and mean values calculated from ratings of the
respective individual odours. Dashed lines indicate 95% CI.
Fialova
´et al. 9
The rationale was that composite body odours might be more positively perceived because
they would be more ‘‘average,’’ as analogously observed in facial attractiveness studies, and
perhaps because they perceptually mimic the odour of relatively heterozygous individuals.
Contrary to expectation, we did not find significant differences between ratings of the
individual samples and both two- or four-composite body odours, and the same pattern
was observed for ratings of opposite-sex samples. Similarly, we did not find any positive
association between the number of individual body odours constituting composite odours
and its attractiveness ratings, as has been observed in facial images. However, we did find
significant positive correlations between ratings of two- and four-composite body odours and
calculated values of individual odours.
Moreover, our results suggest that two-composite body odour samples retain qualities of
the constituent individual odours as we found significant differences between the lower and
upper halves of the distribution. The differences were not significant for four-composite body
odour samples, perhaps as a consequence of limited sample size, but visual inspection of the
data (see Figure 3) indicates that four-composite body odour samples do appear to retain the
qualities of the individual constituent samples.
As described earlier, our research was inspired by studies on facial attractiveness. These
studies systematically show that composite facial images are perceived as more attractive than
the constituent images (Langlois & Roggman, 1990). This was also observed to be
independent of their higher bilateral symmetry (Rhodes et al., 1999), another factor
Table 4. Correlation Between Ratings of Four-Composite Body Odour Samples (Rated—R) and Individual
Odours (Calculated—C).
Pleasantness
R
Attractiveness
R
Intensity
R
Pleasantness
C
Attractiveness
C
Intensity
C
Pleasantness R .981*** .936*** .783* .805** .638
Attractiveness R .913** .864** .873** .741*
Intensity R .672* .703* .589
Pleasantness C .992*** .914**
Attractiveness C .928***
Intensity C
Note. Asterisks indicate level of significance; *p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.
Table 3. Correlation Between Ratings of Two-Composite Body Odour Samples (Rated—R) and Individual
Odours (Calculated—C).
Pleasantness
R
Attractiveness
R
Intensity
R
Pleasantness
C
Attractiveness
C
Intensity
C
Pleasantness R .998*** .862*** .663** .652** .515*
Attractiveness R .856*** .647** .636** .486*
Intensity R .659** .631** .603**
Pleasantness C .988*** .881***
Attractiveness C .866***
Intensity C
Note. Asterisks indicate level of significance; *p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.
10 i-Perception 9(3)
contributing to perceived attractiveness (Little & Jones, 2003). The attractiveness of
composite images thus appears to be due to their higher prototypicality (Rhodes &
Tremewan, 1996); that is, they represent an average facial morphology and may thus
mimic heterozygosity in individual faces. These processes are, however, far from how the
olfactory system perceives chemical mixtures. Several previous studies have shown that odour
mixtures might be perceived quite differently from their constituents (e.g., Thomas-Danguin
et al., 2014). Some scholars thus refer to odour mixtures as having emergent perceptual
qualities that are frequently difficult to predict from the qualities of the constituents.
Furthermore, it has been shown that humans, including professional ‘‘noses,’’ perform
rather poorly in identifying individual chemicals from odour mixtures (Jinks & Laing, 1999).
Another possible mechanism is that higher attractiveness of facial composites, due to their
prototypicality, is frequently perceived as more familiar. Unfortunately, we did not collect
these ratings. Thus, whether composite body odours are perceived as more familiar remains
an open question.
Limitations
In our study, we employed as body odour donors and raters women using hormonal
contraception to avoid possible fluctuations in body odour attractiveness (Havlı
´c
ˇek,
Dvor
ˇa
´kova
´, Bartos
ˇ, & Flegr, 2006; Kuukasjarvi et al., 2004) and olfactory abilities
(Martinec Nova
´kova
´et al., 2014) during regular menstrual cycling. A previous study has
revealed shifts in MHC-related body odour in contraceptives users (Roberts, Gosling, Carter,
& Petrie, 2008), and one may thus argue that our null findings might be thus attributable to
the fact that we employed contraceptive users. However, a recent meta-analysis did not show
significant differences related to preferences for MHC dissimilarity between contraceptive
users and nonusers (Winternitz et al., 2017). In any case, the main advantage in employing
hormonal contraceptive users as body odour donors was that we aimed to limit the known
effect of cyclic fluctuations, which could drastically interfere with the process of creating
blends across individual donors of different cycle phases. We suggest that this step should
only increase, and not interfere with, the chance to observe a positive effect of composite
body odours because it reduces noise in the collected samples. It is also worth noting that
studies of facial composite images, which find robust effects, did not similarly control for
effects of hormonal contraception in women either contributing facial photographs or ratings
(Langlois & Roggman, 1990; Rhodes & Tremewan, 1996; Rhodes et al., 1999); this seems to
us to suggest a likely sensory specificity in how composites of odours and faces are perceived.
Another potential limitation is that, when assessing individual odours and two-composite
body odour samples, raters were presented with either one complete cotton pad or two
halves, but four-composite samples were created using four halved pads. The difference in
the amount of presented material may potentially affect perception of body odour intensity.
However, this does not seem to be the case, as we did not find any significant differences
between pleasantness, attractiveness and intensity ratings of the individual samples and two-
and four-odour composites.
Implications for Future Studies
Several previous studies have employed composite body odours from individuals sharing
characteristics of interest, for example, their sexual orientation (Martins et al., 2005) or
affective state (Pause et al., 2004), with the unstated assumption that shared features of the
individual odours would be perceivable in the composite stimuli. Our results indicate that
Fialova
´et al. 11
individual hedonic qualities, at least, are retained in the odour composites, although further
study is needed to investigate the retention of other trait-specific cues. We created
composites from individual odours similar in their attractiveness, as this was the most
efficient way to test the effect in question. However, it remains an open question as to
whether composites made of individual odours that are more variable in hedonic quality
would be perceived simply as an average of the constituents or whether such mixtures
would show some different, emergent quality. Previous studies investigating interactions
between individual body odour and perfume have shown that qualities of the resulting
blends cannot be reliably predicted (Sobotkova
´et al., 2017). In a related study, one of the
components was kept constant (i.e., the odour donors applied the same perfume),
nevertheless the variability in hedonic quality of the blends was comparable with that
seen in the body odours alone (Lenochova
´et al., 2012). Whether patterns observed in
studies on perfume-body odour blends can be generalized to the blends of individual
body odours should be addressed in future studies.
Remarkably, it has been repeatedly shown that even though perceived qualities of body
odour samples tend not to differ during hedonic assessment (e.g., pleasantness), they do
induce other effects on individuals exposed to these stimuli, for example, in their
autonomic response (Adolph, Schlo
¨sser, Hawighorst, & Pause, 2010), anxiety levels
(Albrecht et al., 2011) or cognitive functioning (Chen, Katdare, & Lucas, 2006; Gelstein
et al., 2011). This evidence suggests that lack of consciously perceived differences does not
exclude the possibility of odours transferring other socially relevant cues.
Finally, our findings cannot be interpreted as evidence against a link between
heterozygosity and attractiveness. Previous studies indicate that individual body odour
samples provide cues to heterozygosity (Thornhill et al., 2003), and this might be
specifically linked to heterozygosity in MHC genes that is important in MHC-based mate
choice providing direct fitness benefits (Havlı
´c
ˇek & Roberts, 2009; Winternitz et al., 2017).
Nevertheless, our results demonstrate that the validity of composite body odour samples to
test hypotheses about heterozygosity might be limited. It also provides a caution for scholars
primarily interested in testing evolutionary hypotheses that the proximate mechanisms that
underlie interpreted functional outcomes must be treated with extreme care.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all the volunteers and
´tTr
ˇebicky´ for many helpful advices and help
with statistical analysis and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article: This work was supported by a Czech Science Foundation grant (J. F. and J.
H., GAC
ˇR 18-15168S), by the project ‘‘Sustainability for the National Institute of Mental Health,’’
under grant number LO1611, with a financial support from the Ministry of Education, Youth and
Sports of the Czech Republic under the NPU I program, and statutory research funds (A. S.).
12 i-Perception 9(3)
References
Adolph, D., Schlo
¨sser, S., Hawighorst, M., & Pause, B. M. (2010). Chemosensory signals of
competition increase the skin conductance response in humans. Physiology & Behavior,101,
666–671. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.08.004
Aizenstein, H., MacDonald, A., Stenger, V., Nebes, R., Larson, J., Ursu, S., & Carter, C. (2000).
Complementary category learning systems identified using event-related functional MRI. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience,12, 977–987. doi:10.1162/08989290051137512
Albrecht, J., Demmel, M., Schopf, V., Kleemann, A. M., Kopietz, R., May, J., ...Wiesmann, M.
(2011). Smelling chemosensory signals of males in anxious versus nonanxious condition increases
state anxiety of female subjects. Chemical Senses,36, 19–27. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjq087
Chen, D., & Haviland-Jones, J. (2000). Human olfactory communication of emotion. Perceptual and
Motor Skills,91, 771–781. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771.
Chen, D., Katdare, A., & Lucas, N. (2006). Chemosignals of fear enhance cognitive performance in
humans. Chemical Senses,31, 415–423. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjj046
Dalton, P., Maute
´, C., Jae
´n, C., & Wilson, T. (2013). Chemosignals of stress influence social judgments.
PLoS One,8, e77144. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077144
de Groot, J. H. B., Smeets, M. A. M., Kaldewaij, A., Duijndam, M. J. A., & Semin, G. R. (2012).
Chemosignals communicate human emotions. Psychological Science,23, 1417–1424. doi:10.1177/
0956797612445317
Enquist, M., & Arak, A. (1994). Symmetry, beauty and evolution. Nature,372, 169–172. doi:10.1038/
372169a0
Fialova
´, J., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (August, 2012). Does restriction of caloric intake affect human body odour?
Poster presented at the XXI Biennial Conference on Human Ethology, Vienna, Austria.
Fialova
´, J., Roberts, S. C., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (2016). Consumption of garlic positively affects hedonic
perception of axillary body odour. Appetite,97, 8–15. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.001
Galton, F. (1879). Composite portraits, made by combining those of many different persons into a
single resultant figure. The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland,8,
132–144.
Gelstein, S., Yeshurun, Y., Rozenkrantz, L., Shushan, S., Frumin, I., Roth, Y., & Sobel, N. (2011).
Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science,331, 226–230. doi:10.1126/science.1198331
Grammer, K., & Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection:
The role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of Comparative Psychology,108, 233–242. doi:
10.1037/0735-7036.108.3.233.
Haegler, K., Zernecke, R., Kleemann, A. M., Albrecht, J., Pollatos, O., Bruckmann, H., & Wiesmann,
M. (2010). No fear no risk! Human risk behavior is affected by chemosensory anxiety signals.
Neuropsychologia,48, 3901–3908. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.019
Havlı
´c
ˇek, J., Dvor
ˇa
´kova
´, R., Bartos
ˇ, L., & Flegr, J. (2006). Non-advertized does not mean concealed:
Body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology,112, 81–90. doi:10.1111/j.1439-
0310.2006.01125.x
Havlı
´c
ˇek, J., Lenochova
´, P., Oberzaucher, E., Grammer, K., & Roberts, S. C. (2011). Does length of
sampling affect quality of body odor samples? Chemosensory Perception,4, 186–194. doi:10.1007/
s12078-011-9104-6
Havlı
´c
ˇek, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review.
Psychoneuroendocrinology,34, 497–512. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007
Jinks, A., & Laing, D. G. (1999). A limit in the processing of components in odour mixtures. Perception,
28, 395–404. doi:10.1068/p2898
Kamiya, T., O’Dwyer, K., Westerdahl, H., Senior, A., & Nakagawa, S. (2014). A quantitative review of
MHC-based mating preference: The role of diversity and dissimilarity. Molecular Ecology,23,
5151–5163. doi:10.1111/mec.12934
Kirkpatrick, M., & Ryan, M. J. (1991). The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek.
Nature,350, 33–38. doi:10.1038/350033a0
Fialova
´et al. 13
Kohoutova
´, D., Rubes
ˇova
´, A., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (2011). Shaving of axillary hair has only a transient effect
on perceived body odor pleasantness. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,66, 569–581.
doi:10.1007/s00265-011-1305-0
Kuukasjarvi, S., Eriksson, C. J. P., Koskela, E., Mappes, T., Nissinen, K., & Rantala, M. J. (2004).
Attractiveness of women’s body odors over the menstrual cycle: The role of oral contraceptives and
receiver sex. Behavioral Ecology,15, 579–584. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh050
Langlois, J., Roggman, L., & Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about
attractive faces? Psychological Science,5, 214–220.
Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science,1,
115–121. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00079.x
Lenochova, P., Roberts, S. C., & Havlicek, J. (2008). Methods of human body odor sampling: The
effect of freezing. Chemical Senses,34, 127–138. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjn067
Lenochova
´, P., Vohnoutova
´, P., Roberts, S. C., Oberzaucher, E., Grammer, K., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (2012).
Psychology of fragrance use: Perception of individual odor and perfume blends reveals a
mechanism for idiosyncratic effects on fragrance choice. PLoS One,7, e33810. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0033810
Lie, H. C., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2008). Genetic diversity revealed in human faces. Evolution,
62, 2473–2486. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00478.x
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2003). Evidence against perceptual bias views for symmetry preferences in
human faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences,270, 1759–1763.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2445
Lu
¨bke, K. T., Croy, I., Hoenen, M., Gerber, J., Pause, B. M., & Hummel, T. (2014). Does human body
odor represent a significant and rewarding social signal to individuals high in social openness? PLoS
One,9, 1–7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094314
Martindale, C., & Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,14, 661–670. doi:10.1037//0096-
1523.14.4.661
Martinec Nova
´kova
´, L., Havlı
´c
ˇek, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2014). Olfactory processing and odor
specificity: A meta-analysis of menstrual cycle variation in olfactory sensitivity. Anthropological
Review,77, 331–345. doi:10.2478/anre-2014-0024
Martins, Y., Preti, G., Crabtree, C. R., Runyan, T., Vainius, A. A., & Wysocki, C. J. (2005). Preference
for human body odors is influenced by gender and sexual orientation. Psychological Science,16,
694–701. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01598.x
Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Strey, H. H., Frederick, B., Savoy, R., Cox, D., Botanov, Y., ...Weber, J. (2009).
Chemosensory cues to conspecific emotional stress activate amygdala in humans. PLoS One,4,
e6415. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006415
Mutic, S., Moellers, E. M., Wiesmann, M., & Freiherr, J. (2016). Chemosensory communication of
gender information: Masculinity bias in body odor perception and femininity bias introduced by
chemosignals during social perception. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 1–11. doi:10.3389/
fpsyg.2015.01980
Pause, B. M., Adolph, D., Prehn-Kristensen, A., & Ferstl, R. (2009). Startle response potentiation to
chemosensory anxiety signals in socially anxious individuals. International Journal of
Psychophysiology,74, 88–92. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.07.008
Pause, B. M., Lu
¨bke, K., Laudien, J. H., & Ferstl, R. (2010). Intensified neuronal investment in the
processing of chemosensory anxiety signals in non-socially anxious and socially anxious individuals.
PLoS One,5, e10342. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010342
Pause, B. M., Ohrt, A., Prehn, A., & Ferstl, R. (2004). Positive emotional priming of facial affect
perception in females is diminished by chemosensory anxiety signals. Chemical Senses,29,
797–805. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh245
Perrett, D. I., May, K. A., & Yoshikawa, S. (1994). Facial shape and judgements of female
attractiveness. Nature,368, 239–242. doi:10.1038/368239a0
Posner, M., & Keele, S. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology,77,
353–363.
14 i-Perception 9(3)
Prehn, A., Ohrt, A., Sojka, B., Ferstl, R., & Pause, B. M. (2006). Chemosensory anxiety signals
augment the startle reflex in humans. Neuroscience Letters,394, 127–130. doi:10.1016/
j.neulet.2005.10.012
Prehn-Kristensen, A., Wiesner, C., Bergmann, T. O., Wolff, S., Jansen, O., Mehdorn, H. M., ...Pause,
B. M. (2009). Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety. PLoS One,4, e5987. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0005987
Radulescu, A. R., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2013). Human gender differences in the perception of
conspecific alarm chemosensory cues. PLoS One,8, 1–8. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068485
Rhodes, G., Sumich, A., & Byatt, G. (1999). Are average facial configurations attractive only because of
their symmetry? Psychological Science,10, 52–58. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00106
Rhodes, G., & Tremewan, T. (1996). Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness. Psychological
Science,7, 105–110. doi:10.1002/wcs.1316
Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Carter, V., & Petrie, M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences in
humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological
Sciences,275, 2715–2722. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0825
Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Gosling, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Carter, V., Jones, B. C., ...Petrie, M. (2005).
MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior,26, 213–226.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.09.002
Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., & Rips, L. J. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural
model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review,81, 214–241. doi:10.1037/h0036351
Sobotkova
´, M., Fialova
´, J., Roberts, S. C., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (2017). Effect of biological relatedness on
perfume selection for others: Preliminary evidence. Perception,46, 498–515. doi:10.1177/
0301006616682514
Thomas-Danguin, T., Sinding, C., Romagny, S., El Mountassir, F., Atanasova, B., Le Berre,
E., ...Coureaud, G. (2014). The perception of odor objects in everyday life: A review on the
processing of odor mixtures. Frontiers in Psychology,5, 504. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00504
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Human facial beauty Averageness, symmetry, and parasite
resistance. Human Nature,4, 237–269. doi:10.1007/BF02692201
Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCollough, J. K., & Franklin, M. (2003).
Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and
women. Behavioral Ecology,14, 668–678. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg043
Whitfield, T. W. A., & Slatter, P. E. (1979). The effects of categorization and prototypicality on
aesthetic choice in a furniture selection task. British Journal of Psychology,70, 65–75.
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1979.tb02144.x
Winternitz, J., Abbate, J., Huchard, E., Havlı
´c
ˇek, J., & Garamszegi, L. Z. (2017). Patterns of MHC-
dependent mate selection in humans and non-human primates a meta-analysis. Molecular Ecology,
26, 668–688. doi:10.1111/mec.13920
Zernecke, R., Haegler, K., Kleemann, A. M., Albrecht, J., Frank, T., Linn, J., ...Wiesmann, M. (2011).
Effects of male anxiety chemosignals on the evaluation of happy facial expressions. Journal of
Psychophysiology,25, 116–123. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000047.
How to cite this article
Fialova
´, J., Sorokowska, A., Craig Roberts, S., Kubicova
´, L., & Havlı
´c
ˇek, J. (2018). Human body
odour composites are not perceived more positively than the individual samples. i-Perception,
9(3), 1–15. doi:10.1177/2041669518766367
Fialova
´et al. 15
... Samples from the 4 most pleasant scoring odours were pooled to create a "high quality" (HQ) male odour, while pooling of the 4 lowest scoring odours formed a "low quality" (LQ) male odour. Pooling of such samples to create a composite odour minimises effects of individual differences in odour quality and preference while maintaining the average quality of the constituent samples (Fialová et al., 2018). To create these composites, each cotton pad was shredded into small pieces and mixed in equal parts with the other odours in either the HQ or LQ category, before being frozen in sealable bags. ...
Article
Voice characteristics are important to communicate socially relevant information. Recent research has shown that individuals alter their voices depending on the context of social interactions and perceived characteristics of the audience, and this affects how they are perceived. Numerous studies have also shown that the presence of bodily odours can elicit psychological changes in people. Here, we tested whether the presence of male axillary odour would influence vocal modulations in courtship contexts. We analysed differences in vocal parameters and attractiveness ratings across 950 recordings from 80 participants as they responded to opposite-sex target stimuli. Using these, we tested whether men’s and women’s vocal parameters and perceived attractiveness differed in the presence or absence of the odour. We expected women to speak with increased voice F0, and men to lower their pitch, when exposed to male body odour, especially if it were of high quality. However, neither the presence of male odour, its quality, nor the addition of androstadienone produced any consistent changes in vocal parameters. Nevertheless, rated stimulus attractiveness was predicted by F0 and especially F0 variability, suggesting that this is a key parameter in signalling attraction during human courtship, and supporting the idea that vocal modulations are context-sensitive.
... Typically, two ways of BO assessment in order to capture human BO perception have been used: rating of BO quality and BO recognition. First, BO quality is measured by pleasantness and intensity perception [38,41], which differs according to, e.g., familiarity of an odour [5]. It yet has been unclear how BO quality differs with respect to different body sites. ...
Article
Full-text available
Body odours and their importance for human chemical communication, e.g., in the mother–child relationship, are an increasing focus of recent research. Precise examination of sampling methods considering physiology and feasibility aspects in order to obtain robust and informative odour samples is therefore necessary. Studies comparing body odour sampling at different body sites are still pending. Therefore, we sampled axilla, breast, and head odour from 28 mother–infant dyads and examined whether odour perception differs with regard to the body site. The participating mothers were asked to evaluate their own and their infant’s body odour samples, as well as odours of two unfamiliar mother–infant dyads. We tested whether maternal pleasantness and intensity evaluation, as well as recognition ability of the odours differed between the body sites. In infants, the head odour exhibited slightly lower pleasantness ratings than axilla and breast, and intensity ratings did not differ between body sites. In mothers, body site affected intensity ratings but not pleasantness ratings, as the breast odour was rated as less intense compared with head and axilla. Across all body sites, mothers rated the own and their infant’s odour as less intense when compared with unfamiliar samples. Recognition ability did not differ between body sites, and in line with previous studies, mothers were able to recognize their own and their own infant’s odour above chance. In sum, our study extends the previous methodological repertoire of body odour sampling and indicates that the axilla, breast, and head of adults as well as infants serve as informative odour sources.
... Samples from the 4 most pleasant scoring odours were pooled to create a "high quality" (HQ) male odour, while pooling of the 4 lowest scoring odours formed a "low quality" (LQ) male odour. Pooling of such samples to create a composite odour minimises effects of individual differences in odour quality and preference while maintaining the average quality of the constituent samples (Fialová et al., 2018). To create these composites, each cotton pad was shredded into small pieces and mixed in equal parts with the other odours in either the HQ or LQ category, before being frozen in sealable bags. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Voice characteristics are important to communicate socially relevant information. Recent research has shown that individuals alter their voices depending on the context of social interactions and perceived characteristics of the audience, and this affects how they are perceived. Numerous studies have also shown that the presence of bodily odours can elicit psychological changes in people. Here, we tested whether the presence of male axillary odour would influence vocal modulations in courtship contexts. We analysed differences in vocal parameters and attractiveness ratings across 950 recordings from 80 participants as they responded to opposite-sex target stimuli. Using these, we tested whether men's and women's vocal parameters and perceived attractiveness differed in the presence or absence of the odour. We expected women to speak with increased voice F0, and men to lower their pitch, when exposed to male body odour, especially if it were of high quality. However, neither the presence of male odour, its quality, nor the addition of androstadienone produced any consistent changes in vocal parameters. Nevertheless, rated stimulus attractiveness was predicted by F0 and especially F0 variability, suggesting that this is a key parameter in signalling attraction during human courtship, and supporting the idea that vocal modulations are context-sensitive.
... After having been thawed for one hour prior to testing, two pad quadrants from different donors belonging to the same ethnic and sex group were disposed along the walls of wide-mouthed glass jars with lids and they were subsequently presented to the recipients. Although a previous study [19] has observed that composite body odors show similar hedonic perceptual properties as individual odors, in the present study we used two quadrants with different donors, as to reduce the effects of individual variation, given that participants were faced with an ethnic and sex discrimination task. ...
Article
Individuals of African and Caucasian descent show different chemical signatures in their body odors (BO). Does such biological difference have a perceptual correlate? We tested BO donors and raters of Afro-Portuguese (AP) and Caucasian (C) descent to investigate whether olfactory ratings reveal an ethnic bias and whether olfactory ethnic discrimination is possible. C (vs. AP) women rated the C BO as more pleasant, even when controlling for intensity. The C BO labelled as AP was rated as more intense by C raters. Although discriminability of ethnicity and sex is at chance, a nominal advantage for AP vs. C BO emerges.
... The samples were rated using verbally anchored 7-point scales for their (i) pleasantness, (ii) attractiveness, (iii) femininity and (iv) intensity. In the event that raters found any of the samples too weak to assess, they were asked to select "I cannot smell the sample" instead of using the rating scales (this occurred in 5.28% of cases, a rate similar to previous studies, e.g., [14]); these ratings were not included in further analysis. Participants recorded their rating immediately after sniffing each stimulus, but the time spent sniffing was not restricted. ...
Article
Previous studies on various vertebrates have shown that quantity and quality of food intake affect odour attractiveness as perceived by potential mates. In humans, the quality of body odour is similarly affected by ingested foods, such as by variation in meat and garlic intake. Nevertheless, it is not known whether quantity of food has an impact on human body odour attractiveness. Thus, here we tested how 48 h of complete caloric intake restriction affects the hedonic quality of human axillary odour. Odour samples (cotton pads fixed in both armpits and worn for 12 h) were obtained from healthy female donors across three conditions: i) during their habitual food regime; ii) after 48 h of complete caloric intake restriction (drinking water was provided), and iii) 72 h after restoration of caloric intake. Axillary samples were assessed by male raters regarding their pleasantness, attractiveness, femininity, and intensity. We also collected blood samples to assess physiological changes due to dietary restriction (e.g., glucose, sodium, albumin, and triacylglyceride assays) and anthropometric measurements at the same intervals as body odour samples. We found no differences in pleasantness, attractiveness and intensity between the odour samples collected at baseline and during complete caloric intake restriction. Interestingly, we found that body odours were rated more pleasant, more attractive and less intense after restoration of food intake as compared to the baseline and during caloric restriction. Our results suggest that restoration of food intake positively influences hedonic quality of human body odour which might thus provide cues to current fitness status and metabolic efficiency.
... To minimise the influence of individual donor differences on the male participants, we then created composite odours from pads worn in each of the conditions: early pregnancy, late pregnancy, post-pregnancy, and control (non-pregnant) women. Studies have shown that using composites does not positively or negatively affect the perceptual qualities of odour samples [9]. A further control condition was included, using blank (i.e. ...
Article
There is evidence across a range of bi-parental species that physiological changes may occur in partnered males prior to the birth of an infant. It has been hypothesised that these hormonal changes might facilitate care-giving behaviours, which could augment infant survival. The mechanism that induces these changes has not been identified, but evidence from several species suggests that odour may play a role. The current study investigated this in humans by recording testosterone and psychological measures related to infant interest and care in men (n = 91) both before and after exposure to odours from either pregnant women or non-pregnant control women. We found no evidence for an effect of odour cues of pregnancy on psychological measures including self-reported sociosexual orientation and social dominance scores, ratings of infant or adult faces, or testosterone levels. However, we found that brief exposure to post-partum odours significantly increased the reward value of infant faces. Our study is the first to show that the odour of peri-partum women may lead to upregulation of men's interest in infants.
Article
Background: Body odor is an important aspect in interpersonal communication. For psychological and chemical research on body odors, suitable procedures for sampling and application, and analysis of body odors are essential. In this study, different textile fabrics (polyester, cotton, and Gazin gauze) were comparatively evaluated in view of their potential suitability for body odor sampling by determining recovery rates of selected body odor substances. In addition, the impact of sample preparation and storage conditions on the recovery rates was determined. Results: The recovery rates of dimethyl disulfide, (E)-non-2-enal, 5α-androst-16-en-3-one, 6-methylhept-5-en-2-one, heptanal and 3-sulfanylhexan-1-ol were determined under different conditions of storage (storage for 30 min at room temperature or storage for 30 min at room temperature followed by freezing at - 80 °C for 4 weeks) and sample work-up (solvent extraction with and without solvent-assisted flavour evaporation, in the following: SAFE). SAFE led to overall lower recovery rates with a significant effect for (E)-non-2-enal and 5α-androst-16-en-3-one. Nevertheless, the results showed that SAFE can be an essential step when working with a complex matrix. When comparing the different fabrics, except for (E)-non-2-enal no difference between the recovery rates obtained for cotton and polyester became evident. Gazin gauze showed lower recovery rates for all compounds. Finally, our results showed that the here investigated target compounds are stable during storage for four weeks at - 80 °C. Significance and novelty: The results show on the one hand that the here investigated compounds were stable during storage and that the fiber type had limited influence on overall recovery rates. On the other hand, they highlight the limitations of using textile materials for sampling of volatile substances, especially with regard to low recovery rates for certain substances and the necessity of material pre-treatment or distillation steps for enabling GC-MS analysis after solvent extraction.
Article
Full-text available
Recent work has demonstrated that human body odour alters with changing emotional state and that emotionally laden odours can affect the physiology and behaviour of people exposed to them. Here we review these discoveries, which we believe add to a growing recognition that the human sense of smell, and its potential role in social interactions, have been underappreciated. However, we also critically evaluate the current evidence, with particular focus on methodology and interpretation of emotional odour studies. We argue that while the evidence convincingly indicates that humans retain a capacity for olfactory communication of emotion, the extent to which this occurs in ordinary social interaction remains an open question. Future studies should place fewer restrictions on participant selection and lifestyle and adopt more realistic experimental designs. We also need to devote more consideration to underlying mechanisms and to recognise the constraints that these may place on effective communication. Finally, we outline some promising approaches to address these issues, and raise some broader theoretical questions that such approaches may help us to answer.
Article
Full-text available
People tend to choose perfumes to complement their body odour. As kin share some body odour qualities, their ability to select complementary perfumes for relatives might be higher compared with selection for nonrelatives. We tested this in two studies, comparing selection of a perfume for a target man by himself and by either a familiar but unrelated individual (girlfriend; Study 1) or a relative (sister; Study 2). Target men applied the two perfumes (own or other’s choice) to their axillae and then wore cotton pads for 12 hr. Collected perfume-body odour blends and perfumes alone were assessed by rater panels. In Study 1, the blends were rated as nominally more pleasant when body odours were mixed with the perfumes selected by girlfriends compared with those selected by target men themselves. In Study 2, body odours mixed with perfumes selected by sisters were rated significantly more attractive than those mixed with perfumes selected by target men. No significant differences were found for attractiveness and pleasantness ratings when perfumes were rated alone, suggesting that it was the resulting blends that were uniquely different. Our results indicate that sisters might be particularly tuned to select suitable perfumes for their siblings.
Article
Full-text available
Genes of the major histocompatibility complex (MHC) in vertebrates are integral for effective adaptive immune response and are associated with sexual selection. Evidence from a range of vertebrates supports MHC-based preference for diverse and dissimilar mating partners, but evidence from human mate choice studies has been disparate and controversial. Methodologies and sampling peculiarities specific to human studies make it difficult to know whether wide discrepancies in results among human populations are real or artifact. To better understand what processes may affect MHC-mediated mate choice across humans and non-human primates we performed phylogenetically controlled meta-analyses using 58 effect sizes from 30 studies across 7 primate species. Primates showed a general trend favoring more MHC-diverse mates, which was statistically significant for humans. In contrast, there was no tendency for MHC-dissimilar mate choice, and for humans, we observed effect sizes indicating selection of both MHC-dissimilar and MHC-similar mates. Focusing on MHC-similar effect sizes only, we found evidence that preference for MHC-similarity was an artifact of population ethnic heterogeneity in observational studies but not among experimental studies with more control over socio-cultural biases. This suggests that human assortative mating biases may be responsible for some patterns of MHC-based mate choice. Additionally, the overall effect sizes of primate MHC-based mating preferences are relatively weak (Fisher's Z correlation coefficient for dissimilarity Zr = 0.044, diversity Zr = 0.153), calling for careful sampling design in future studies. Overall, our results indicate that preference for more MHC diverse mates is significant for humans and likely conserved across primates. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Human body odor is a source of important social information. In this study, we explore whether the sex of an individual can be established based on smelling axillary odor and whether exposure to male and female odors biases chemosensory and social perception. In a double-blind, pseudo-randomized application, 31 healthy normosmic heterosexual male and female raters were exposed to male and female chemosignals (odor samples of 27 heterosexual donors collected during a cardio workout) and a no odor sample. Recipients rated chemosensory samples on a masculinity-femininity scale and provided intensity, familiarity and pleasantness ratings. Additionally, the modulation of social perception (gender-neutral faces and personality attributes) and affective introspection (mood) by male and female chemosignals was assessed. Male and female axillary odors were rated as rather masculine, regardless of the sex of the donor. As opposed to the masculinity bias in the odor perception, a femininity bias modulating social perception appeared. A facilitated femininity detection in gender-neutral faces and personality attributes in association with both male and female chemosignals appeared. No chemosensory effect on mood of the rater was observed. The results are discussed with regards to the use of male and female chemosignals in affective and social communication.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Cycle-correlated variation in olfactory threshold, with women becoming more sensitive to odors mid-cycle, is somewhat supported by the literature but the evidence is not entirely consistent, with several studies finding no, or mixed, effects. It has been argued that cyclic shifts in olfactory threshold might be limited to odors relevant to the mating context. Aims: We aimed to test whether the evidence currently available points in the direction of odor-specific or, rather, general changes in olfactory sensitivity and, if the former is the case, to what group of odorants in particular. Materials and Methods: We carried out a meta-analysis of relevant studies which together used a variety of different odorants, including some found in food, body odor and some that occur in neither of these. First we tested whether there appears to be an overall effect when all studies are included. Next, we hypothesised that if cyclic changes in olfactory processing are odor-specific and tuned to biologically relevant odors, we should find changes in detection thresholds only for odorants found in body odor, or for those that are perceptually similar to it. In contrast, if threshold patterns are linked to more general fluctuations in odor processing across the cycle, we would not expect changes in relation to any particular odorant group. Results: The results support the view that there is significant cycle-correlated variation. Thresholds were in general significantly lower in the fertile than the non-fertile phases, with effect sizes consistently in this direction. This same conclusion applied to both ‘food’ and ‘musky’ odorants, despite their different evolutionary significance, and to the androgen steroids (androstadienone, androstenone, and androsterone), but could not be applied to phenyl-ethyl alcohol. Discussion: The results indicate that olfactory sensitivity may be a non-adaptive by-product of the general physiological fluctuations or differences in neural processing experienced across the cycle to a broad spectrum of odorants, rather than being specifically selected for mate choice-related odors.
Article
It is a long held assumption that women have concealed ovulation, which means that men do not know when women's menstrual cycles are in their most fertile phase. Recent empirical results have provided evidence that ovulation may not be totally concealed from pair-bonded males, but the generality and the mechanisms of the finding demand further study. To examine the possible adaptive value of the phenomenon, it is necessary to study whether the ability to detect ovulation is confined to males. We studied these questions in an experiment in which male and female raters rated the sexual attractiveness and intensity of T-shirts' odors worn by 42 women using oral contraceptives (pill users) and by 39 women without oral contraceptives (nonusers). Males rated the sexual attractiveness of nonusers highest at midcycle. However, female raters showed only a nonsignificant trend for this relationship. Neither sex rated attractiveness of the odors of pill users according to their menstrual cycle. The results indicate that men can use olfactory cues to distinguish between ovulating and nonovulating women. Furthermore, the contrasting results between pill users and nonusers may indicate that oral contraceptives demolish the cyclic attractiveness of odors. Together, these findings give more basis for the study of the role of odors in human sexual behavior. Copyright 2004.
Article
Langlois and her colleagues reported in this journal that composite faces are more attractive than the component faces used to create them, and conjectured that averageness is attractive (Langlois & Roggman, 1990; Langlois, Roggman, & Musselman, 1994). However, extremes may also be attractive (Perrett, May, & Yoshikawa, 1994). We investigated the effect of averageness (proximity to a norm or average face) on attractiveness using a computerized caricature generator to vary averageness. Attractiveness increased with averageness (Experiment 1) and was negatively correlated with distinctiveness, a subjective measure of the converse of averageness (Experiments 1 and 2). Extremes (caricatures) were not attractive. Line-drawing composites, which avoid some of the problems associated with gray-level composites, were significantly more attractive and less distinctive (more average) than individual faces (Experiment 2). These results support the claim that averageness is attractive.