ArticlePDF Available

A Combined Read-Aloud Think-Aloud Strategy Improves Student Learning Experiences in College-Level Biology Courses

Authors:

Abstract

Reading aloud (RA) is a learning strategy commonly used to help younger students develop language comprehension skills and in adult literacy and language courses to help students master the pronunciation of words, grammar, and intonation. However, we know of no studies to date that investigate the possible benefits of RA at the college or university level. We propose that "reading aloud" (RA) immediately followed by "thinking aloud" (TA) during class can help college students enhance their reading abilities and cultivate their analytical thinking skills. This study explores the impact of a "read aloud-think aloud" (RA-TA) strategy in college-level biology courses. Students in 3 sections of 2 courses were asked about their experiences with RA via an end-of-semester survey. When pooled (N = 34 students), responses to all 5 survey questions significantly supported RA-TA. The responses to 4 survey questions very significantly supported the use of RA-TA: Students read the text more, understood it better, found the instructor's help useful, and in general learned to read difficult texts with greater comprehension. Results suggest that a RA-TA approach has the potential to greatly enhance student learning at the college level and provides a promising area for future research.
10
Journal of College Science Teaching
TWO-YEAR COMMUNITY
A Combined Read-Aloud
Think-Aloud Strategy Improves
Student Learning Experiences in
College-Level Biology Courses
By Oliver R. W. Pergams, Charmaine E. Jake-Matthews, and Liza M. Mohanty
Reading aloud (RA) is a learning strategy commonly used to help younger
students develop language comprehension skills and in adult literacy and
language courses to help students master the pronunciation of words,
grammar, and intonation. However, we know of no studies to date that
investigate the possible benets of RA at the college or university level.
We propose that “reading aloud” (RA) immediately followed by “thinking
aloud” (TA) during class can help college students enhance their reading
abilities and cultivate their analytical thinking skills. This study explores
the impact of a “read aloud–think aloud” (RA-TA) strategy in college-
level biology courses. Students in 3 sections of 2 courses were asked about
their experiences with RA via an end-of-semester survey. When pooled (N
= 34 students), responses to all 5 survey questions signicantly supported
RA-TA. The responses to 4 survey questions very signicantly supported
the use of RA-TA: Students read the text more, understood it better, found
the instructor’s help useful, and in general learned to read difcult texts
with greater comprehension. Results suggest that a RA-TA approach has
the potential to greatly enhance student learning at the college level and
provides a promising area for future research.
Community college students
typically have diverse edu-
cational backgrounds and
life circumstances (Cohen,
Brawer, & Kisker, 2014). Many stu-
dents are returning to the classroom
after being away for many years, or
are switching careers or disciplines.
Others have just completed high
school and are not experienced with
studying independently. In a diverse
classroom, helping every student
meet learning goals can be a chal-
lenge, particularly in content-heavy
subjects such as biology. An added
obstacle for students learning about
a science such as biology is the vast
number of new vocabulary words
that they must integrate in a short
period (Snow, 2010). Students often
face the same challenges in a biolo-
gy course as they would in a foreign
language course. At the same time,
learning new words is easier when
students are familiar with their con-
text. These aspects of learning can
be daunting. To serve these needs,
a teaching approach that helps them
to learn both vocabulary and course
concepts is needed. To identify such
an approach, we return to the foun-
dations of educational pedagogy.
Of the many theories in the eld
of education, Lev Vygotsky’s work
seems applicable. Vygotsky empha-
sizes the roles of social interaction
and cultural context in learning. The
learner best acquires knowledge and
skills through social connection and
interaction with others (Gredler,
2009). Many educational concepts
and practices are in turn based on
this premise, including the zone of
proximal development (ZPD) and
scaffolding.
The ZPD is dened as what the
learner cannot do alone, but can do
with support and assistance from a
more experienced and knowledge-
able individual (Rogoff, 1990). Scaf-
folding is the temporary support that
the more knowledgeable individual
provides to help the learner move
through the ZPD to higher levels of
skill and knowledge. As the learner
progresses, so does the nature of the
support provided. Ideally, descaf-
folding will eventually occur and the
learner will have acquired knowledge
and skills that can be demonstrated
independently (Collins, Brown, &
Newman, 1989).
Lin et al. (2012) conducted a
content analysis of 43 science edu-
cation scaffolding articles, yielding
three conclusions. First, the aims of
scaffolding in science education are
the development of conceptual un-
11Vol. 47, No. 5, 2018
derstanding, procedural and strategic
skills, and metacognition. Second,
many representations of scaffolding
are present in the science education
literature. These can be categorized
as visual representation, social inter-
actions, and written prompts. Third,
Lin et al. found consensus that descaf-
folding is necessary, but found little
documentation of its occurrence.
Interactive reading aloud (RA)
is a teaching tool that uses scaffold-
ing. RA consists of several steps
(previewing a storybook, scaffolding
on prior knowledge, emphasizing
story elements, answering purpose-
ful questions, and summarizing what
was learned). Two forms of RA have
been used in educational practices:
reading to students and round-robin
reading (Brabham & Lynch-Brown,
2002; Trelease, 2013). In the former,
the teacher reads the book aloud to
the students, modeling appropriate
uency and intonation, while students
experience the storybook by answer-
ing and asking questions as the book
is read to them (Trelease, 2013).
Round-robin reading is similar, but
instead the students take turns read-
ing passages. RA in various forms
was observed to increase student
mastery of new vocabulary words at
both the preschool (Leung, 2008) and
elementary school (Brabham & Lynch
Brown, 2002) levels. The authors thus
felt it worth exploring the potential of
RA as a learning tool for students at
advanced educational levels.
The teacher may also demonstrate
“thinking aloud” (TA) at the same
time, by describing his or her thought
processes while reading. This mod-
els for the students how to search
for meaning in what they read. RA
to younger students has been dem-
onstrated to broaden vocabularies,
develop concept of story structure,
strengthen understanding of the con-
nection between spoken and printed
elements, sharpen critical thinking
skills, increase condence with the
reading process, model uency, and
encourage students to read on their
own (Rog, 2001).
RA is less commonly used in the
upper elementary, middle, and high
school levels, and there are relatively
few studies of its effectiveness in
helping older students learn (Trelease,
2013). We know of no prior studies of
RA at the postsecondary level. The de-
crease in the use of RA in higher grade
levels may occur for many reasons.
Some argue that RA is unengaging for
the listener. Some studies suggest that
it is anxiety producing for the reader
and encumbers working memory for
decoding, recoding, and articulation,
thus preventing the student from fully
comprehending what she or he is read-
ing. Furthermore, RA may arguably be
a linear process that does not facilitate
comprehension (Klapper, 1992). Some
feel that the slowed reading rate dur-
ing RA results in the dissection of the
language and an overall loss of mes-
sage of the writing (Eskey & Grabe,
1988; Grabe & Stoller, 2011). Kelly
(1995) noted that many teachers persist
in using RA despite these arguments
and the lack of research to support the
effectiveness of RA.
However, more recent research
provides support for RA in the class-
room, especially when it is used in an
active, rather than passive, manner.
Dreher (2003) reported that RA helps
engage students in lessons, increases
their condence, and improves their
ability to read difcult texts. Spe-
cically, these benets occur when all
students are involved in the reading
process and subsequent discussion.
Hale et al. (2007) found that RA
enhances reading comprehension in
elementary and high school students.
Furthermore, Duncan (2015) reported
that adults continue to engage in RA,
apply it to a variety of reading mate-
rials, and value it for many reasons.
These ndings are particularly salient
as they support the use of RA with
older students and support the use of
RA in other than storybooks.
The use of TA in conjunction
with RA (RA-TA) might serve as a
powerful learning tool even at more
advanced levels of study. From the
authors’ experience, this approach
seems to work best in smaller classes,
preferably with fewer than 15 students.
RA-TA provides a framework for
strengthening students’ reading skills
in an interactive way. The concern
of students becoming bored is offset
by a TA session after the reading of
a paragraph. During the TA session,
the instructor would pose questions
and provide explanation as needed.
All students would be encouraged to
contribute to the discussion by para-
phrasing, summarizing, and asking and
answering questions. RA alone may be
a more passive activity, but in concert
with TA it can engage all students in
the classroom, while providing the
scaffolding necessary to develop abili-
ties central to both reading scientic
literature and performing hands-on
science. These abilities include critical
thinking, data analysis, and integration
of prior knowledge, all of which help
students succeed in their courses and
beyond. The authors thus felt justied
in using class time to help students
develop their reading skills.
In this study, the strategy of RA in
class was implemented in three sec-
tions of two courses offered at an urban
community college, Olive-Harvey
College (OHC), one of the City Col-
leges of Chicago (CCC). CCC is one
of the nation’s largest community col-
leges and the largest in Illinois, with
6,000 faculty and staff and 120,000
students. The PI (corresponding au-
12
Journal of College Science Teaching
TWO-YEAR COMMUNITY
thor Pergams) is a tenured associate
professor at OHC, the poorest of the
seven colleges (95% minority, 84%
below poverty level (CCC unpublished
data, 2016). One course was Biology
119, Environmental Biology, a general
education course that may be taken by
biology nonmajors, although it is often
also taken by majors. A second course
in which this technique was applied
was Biology 122 (Organismal Biol-
ogy: Ecology, and Evolution), which
is a majors’ biology course usually
taken by students who will transfer to
a 4-year bachelor of science program.
Sample course materials are included
in Appendices I–VI (available at http://
www.nsta.org/college/connections.
aspx).
The PI had previously taught these
courses in a traditional lecture-based
style, using PowerPoint slides to con-
vey much of the information. He felt
that this “sage on the stage” approach
was not engaging students enough and
not motivating them to take owner-
ship of their learning. The instructor
asked the rst group of students where
they were facing challenges. Most
responded that they had not read the
textbook on their own, or that they
had attempted to read the textbook but
did not understand it. When asked to
elaborate, most students cited one or
both of two reasons: (1) a lack of ad-
equate time, or (2) they did not enjoy
reading the textbook or article, often
because they felt overwhelmed by it.
To counter these challenges, the PI
sought a different approach. Based on
success of RA and TA in other educa-
tional settings discussed previously,
RA-TA in class seemed to be a poten-
tially effective approach to address all
three concerns (encouraging students
to read the material, helping students
to understand the course concepts,
and motivating students to become
stakeholders in their own learning).
Methods
The PI decided to directly address
the students’ apprehension of read-
ing (especially difcult textbooks)
by having them take turns reading
the textbook aloud during class. He
also wanted to encourage them to
think critically and to identify their
own areas of challenge by asking
questions as they read. A combined
“reading aloud–thinking aloud”
(RA-TA) approach was used.
Active student participation and
buy-in were gained by rst explain-
ing RA-TA and its hoped-for benets
to them. Students then were asked to
vote on a 2-week RA-TA trial period.
At the end of the trial period students
voted again on whether to retain RA-
TA for the rest of the semester. All stu-
dents agreed to abide by the majority
opinion. In each course the majority
voted in favor of retaining RA-TA for
the duration. No students objected,
so there was no need to incentivize
participation by awarding points.
Each student and the instructor
took turns reading a paragraph out
loud. Under the guidance of the
instructor, this was followed by a
think-aloud during which the class
took part in the paraphrasing and
analysis of the paragraph. Students
were asked to summarize the main
idea of the paragraph and to ask
questions on anything they did not
understand, including unfamiliar vo-
cabulary. Accompanying gures were
also discussed. During the lesson,
the instructor incorporated videos,
models, ancillary texts, internet re-
sources, etc., and paused for discus-
sion to be sure students thoroughly
understood concepts before reading
would continue. RA also provided the
instructor with instant feedback on
students’ reading ability and allowed
the instructor to model the reading
process and to demonstrate correct
pronunciation and intonation. The
goal was to increase students’ self-
condence in their reading ability,
improve their reading comprehension,
and help them develop their metacog-
nitive abilities. This also modeled for
the students how to search for other
sources of information to help them
better understand what they had read.
Analysis
Three sections of two courses were
involved in this project:
1. Biology 122 (Organismal
Biology II) taught fall 2015 (N =
8 students)
2. Biology 119 (Environmental
Biology) taught spring 2016 (N =
14 students)
3. Biology 122 (Organismal
Biology II) taught fall 2016 (N =
12 students)
There were a total of 34 students
in these three sections. The instruc-
tor gave the students a semester-end
survey to ll out (see Figure 1). The
FIGURE 1
End-of-semester survey.
Please rate the following on a 1–5
scale, with 1 = strongly disagree, 2
= somewhat disagree, 3 = neither
disagree nor agree, 4 = somewhat
agree, and 5 = strongly agree.
1. It made me read the text more
than I would have otherwise.
2. It made me understand the text
more than I would have otherwise.
3. [The instructor] elaborating on the
text was useful to me.
4. In general, I learned how to
read dicult texts with greater
comprehension.
5. I prefer a class conducted this
way over a class with lecture and
PowerPoints.
13Vol. 47, No. 5, 2018
survey sheets were proctored by a
student while the instructor was out
of the room. The total sample of 34
students is small, but p-values take
sample size into account. We used
one-sample t-tests with null hypoth-
eses of 3 on a 5-point Likert scale
(neither agree nor disagree). In other
words, we tested whether students’
responses were signicantly differ-
ent from neutral for each question.
Because previous sections of both
courses used different textbooks,
homework, and quizzes, student
grades in past sections of these
courses taught by traditional lectur-
ing could not be compared with these
three RA-TA classes.
Results
Students in Biology 122 (fall 2015,
N = 8) responded signicantly and
positively to all but two questions
(Table 1). One was “My own ‘trans-
lating’ of the text after someone else
read it helped me understand the
text better.” Later discussion with
students suggested this was because
“translating” started late in the class
(Week 11 out of 16), and the students
did not have time to get used to it.
In fact, it was unpopular, and the in-
structor discontinued it in later class-
es. Students also did not respond
signicantly to “I prefer a class con-
ducted this way over a class with lec-
ture and PowerPoints.” Subsequent
student discussion revealed that it
was difcult for students to embrace
this approach to learning because it
was novel. Traditional PowerPoint-
based lectures were the norm in the
majority of their classes, so it is not
surprising that students were still not
completely at ease with the RA-TA
approach by the end of the semester.
Students in Biology 119 (spring
2016, N = 14) responded signicantly
and positively to all but one question:
“I prefer a class conducted this way
over a class with lecture and Pow-
erPoints.” Student discussion sug-
gested that the novelty of the RA-TA
approach was less important to them.
Students in Biology 122 (fall 2016,
N = 12) responded signicantly and
positively to all but one question: “In
general, I learned how to read difcult
texts with greater comprehension,”
The question missed significance
by a very small margin (2-tailed p
= .0688). When asked about their
response, some students shared that
they felt they still needed the instruc-
tor’s guidance to help them fully
understand the readings.
TABLE 1
Results (2-tailed P values) of 1-sample t-tests on survey questions, using 3 on a 5-point Likert scale as a null
hypothesis.
Bio 122
Fall 2015 Read Text More
Understand Text
More
Pergams
Elaborating
My
“Translating”
Helped Reading
In General
Prefer Class This
Way
mean 4.13 4.13 4.88 3.69 3.69 3.13
2-tailed P 0.0379 0.0066 <0.0001 0.1885 0.0280 0.7627
(1-sample t-test w/3 as null hypothesis, N=8)
Bio 119
Spring 2016 Read Text More
Understand Text
More
Pergams
Elaborating
My
“Translating”
Helped Reading
In General
Prefer Class This
Way
mean 4.07 4.29 4.79 4.21 3.29
2-tailed P 0.006 0.0003 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.3649
(1-sample t-test w/3 as null hypothesis, N=14)
Bio 122
Fall 2016 Read Text More
Understand Text
More
Pergams
Elaborating
My
“Translating”
Helped Reading
In General
Prefer Class This
Way
mean 4.25 3.83 4.75 3.75 3.58
2-tailed P 0.0004 0.0172 <0.0001 0.0688 0.0116
(1-sample t-test w/3 as null hypothesis, N=12)
TOTAL 4.15 4.09 4.79 3.93 3.35
2-tailed P <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.0437
(1-sample t-test w/3 as null hypothesis, N=34)
14
Journal of College Science Teaching
TWO-YEAR COMMUNITY
When pooled (N = 34), responses
to all ve questions were positively
signicant, and all but one highly so
(2-tailed p < .0001). The question
that was positively significant but
not highly so was “I prefer a class
conducted this way over a class with
lecture and PowerPoints” (p = .0437).
The authors attempted to imple-
ment the RA-TA strategy in larger
classes (17, 18, 19, 29, and 32 stu-
dents), but opted not to pursue it,
based on an observed lack of student
engagement. In these larger classes,
instructors subjectively felt it was
difcult to get through the material
without substantially losing student
attention. Further research could
verify and refine this observation.
When class sizes were >15, students
had to wait too long for their turn
to read again and lost focus. It was
concluded that this RA-TA approach
is most effective in smaller classes
with fewer than 15 students.
Conclusions
Studies published within other con-
texts provide compelling evidence
for the value of RA as a learning
tool. However, most of this research
focuses on its use with younger stu-
dents. By contrast, RA is used much
less often in middle school, high
school, and postsecondary educa-
tion. Instructors in these settings
are increasingly facing pressure to
demonstrate their effectiveness as
measured by student performance
on tests (Serani & Giorgis, 2003).
RA may be viewed as too slow to
produce immediate results on ex-
aminations. In conversations with
colleagues, the authors heard a few
instructors state that they did not
have time for RA. This attitude may
partly arise from the pressure felt by
some college instructors to cover
extensive curricula in a short time.
Another comment that the authors
encountered was that RA is too “ju-
venile” or “basic” and thus is not ap-
propriate for college.
Despite these objections to RA,
the use of an RA-TA approach for
teaching science is supported by
research on the value of scaffolding
(Lin et al., 2012). Specically, RA and
TA include the three elements found
in scaffolding: visual representa-
tions, social interactions, and written
prompts. Although Lin’s ndings ap-
plied to grammar school students, the
ndings of the current study together
with Duncan’s (2015) study of the
value of RA in adults suggest that RA-
TA is useful for adult learners as well.
Significant, positive responses
from students to the postclass survey
indicate that the use of the RA-TA
method enhanced their learning ex-
periences. However, some classes
did not show a signicant positive re-
sponse to the process of “translating”
the text. This is consistent with previ-
ous research that suggests that scaf-
folding methods should be tailored to
the needs of the students being taught
(Lin et al., 2012). In accordance with
this need for tailored scaffolding, the
authors discontinued the process of
“translating the text” when students
expressed that the process was unap-
pealing and unhelpful.
Another important goal of scaf-
folding is for students to develop
independence in performing learned
skills through the process of descaf-
folding (Collins et al., 1989). In the
current study, this was assessed by
student responses to the fourth ques-
tion on the postcourse survey, “In
general, I learned how to read difcult
texts with greater comprehension.”
Although the responses to this ques-
tion for one class missed signicance
by a very small margin, there was a
significant, positive finding in the
pooled data. This supports the idea
that descaffolding is effective. Future
research might focus on examining
descaffolding more directly, perhaps
by using an assessment tool to mea-
sure students’ ability to comprehend a
scientic passage on their own.
A practical drawback is that the RA
method may be difcult to implement
in a larger class setting (>15 students).
One suggestion is to split larger
classes into smaller groups. However,
given that RA-TA is very dependent
on having a single expert reader, split-
ting classes would probably not work
unless there was an additional expert
reader. Also, without an expert reader
we do not recommend RA-TA for use
outside the classroom, such as study
halls or peer tutoring. Finally, future
studies by the authors will involve
comparison of same-course sections
in which RA-TA is used in one and
lecture-based instruction is used in the
other. This will allow for a measure of
the impact that RA-TA has on learning
outcomes in addition to its impact on
learning experiences. These future
studies will also use more detailed
surveys given at both the beginning
and end of the semester.
Acknowledgment
The authors thank the 34 students in
this study for their patience and re-
sponses, and thank Larry Aronson for
proofreading.
References
Brabham, E. G., & Lynch-Brown, C.
(2002). Effects of teachers’ reading-
aloud styles on vocabulary acquisition
and comprehension of students in
the early elementary grades. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 94,
465–473.
Cohen, A. M., Brawer, F. B., &
Kisker, C. B. (2014). The American
community college (6th ed.). San
15Vol. 47, No. 5, 2018
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S.
E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship:
Teaching the craft of reading, writing
and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick
(Ed.), Knowing learning, and
instruction: Essays in honor of Robert
Glaser (pp. 453–494). Hillside, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Dreher, S. (2003). A novel idea: Reading
aloud in a high school English
classroom. The English Journal, 93,
50–53.
Duncan, S. (2015). Reading aloud in
Lewisham: An exploration of adult
reading-aloud practices. Literacy, 49,
84–90.
Eskey, D. E. & Grabe, W. (1988).
Interactive models for second
language reading: Perspectives on
instruction. In P. L. Carrell, J. Devine,
& D. E. Eskey (Eds.), Interactive
approaches to second language
reading (pp. 223–238). Cambridge
England, Cambridge University
Press.
Grabe, W., &. Stoller F. L. (2011).
Teaching and researching: Reading
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Gredler, M. E. (2009). Learning and
instruction: Theory into practice (6th
ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Hale, A. D., Skinner, C. H., Williams,
J., Hawkins, R., Neddenriep, C.
E., & Dizer, J. (2007). Comparing
comprehension following silent and
aloud reading across elementary and
secondary students: Implications
for curriculum-based measurement.
Behavior Analyst Today, 8, 9–23.
Kelly, P. R. (1995). Round robin reading:
Considering alternative instructional
practices that make more sense.
Reading Horizons, 36, 99–115.
Klapper, J., (1992). Preliminary
considerations for the teaching of
foreign language reading. Language
Learning Journal 6, 53–56.
Leung, C. B. (2008). Preschoolers’
acquisition of scientic vocabulary
through repeated read-aloud events,
retellings, and hands-on science
activities. Reading Psychology, 29,
165–193.
Lin, T., Hsu, Y., Lin, S., Changlai, M.,
Yang, K,. & Lai, T. (2012). A review
of empirical evidence on scaffolding
for science education. International
Journal of Science and Mathematics
Education, 10, 437–455.
Rog, L. J. (2001). Early literacy
instruction in kindergarten.
Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in
thinking: Cognitive development
in social context. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Serani, F., & Georgis, C. (2003).
Reading aloud and beyond: Fostering
the intellectual life with older readers.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
If you are interested in submitting
a manuscript to one of JCST’s
columns or have a question or
comment regarding an article,
please contact the appropriate
editor at the following addresses:
The Two-Year
Community
Apryl Nenortas
Butler Community College
El Dorado, KS 67042
anenortas@butlercc.edu
Please submit directly to JCST’s
electronic submission system
(http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/
nsta)
Column Editors
Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language
and the challenge of reading for
learning about science. Science, 328,
450–452.
Trelease, J. (2013). The read-aloud
handbook (7th ed.). New York, NY:
Penguin Books.
Oliver R. W. Pergams (opergams@ccc.
edu) is an associate professor in the De-
partment of Natural Sciences, Olive-Har-
vey College, City Colleges of Chicago, and
a visiting professor in the Department of
Biological Sciences, University of Illinois
at Chicago. Charmaine E. Jake-Mat-
thews is a visiting lecturer in the Depart-
ment of Psychology at Purdue University
Northwest in Hammond, Indiana, and a
psychotherapist at Urban Wellness Coun-
seling in Chicago. Liza M. Mohanty is an
associate professor in the Department of
Natural Sciences, Olive-Harvey College,
City Colleges of Chicago.
Research and
Teaching
Ann Cutler
Editor
acutler@nsta.org
Please submit directly to JCST’s
electronic submission system
(http://mc.manuscriptcentral.
com/nsta)
The Case Study
Clyde F. Herreid
Department of Biology
State University of New York
Bualo, NY 14260-1300
herreid@bualo.edu
... Beyond anecdote, and a few examples within larger ethnographic studies (see for example the Quaker reading aloud practices in Mace, 2012), very little. Much is written about the benefits of reading to children (see for example, the recent Gurdon, 2019) and the uses of reading aloud as an educational tool (Gibson, 2008;Pergams, Jake-Matthews, & Mohanty, 2018;Westbrook, Sutherland, Oakhill, & Sullivan, 2018), and yet very little has been published on the reading aloud that adults may engage in across different life domains, in different formations and for different purposes. The UK Arts and Humanities Research Council-funded Reading Aloud in Britain Today (RABiT) project (https://www.ucl.ac.uk/ioe/research/projects/reading-aloud-britain-today) was developed to fill this gap and, taking Britain as its geographical context, it aimed to create the first record and analysis of contemporary adult reading aloud practices, examining the following research questions: ...
... The Mass Observation responses, in common with all Mass Observation research (see Moor and Uprichard (2014) on the "unwieldy materiality" of Mass Observation data) present an overwhelming outpouring of opinions, experiences, thoughts and stories about reading aloud. Yet what they share is striking: a united expression of surprise at being asked about adult reading aloud (with one person considering it "one of the strangest directives" and another noting "adults and reading aloud don't go together") alongside a realisation of the amount of previously unnoticed reading aloud in their lives: "my first reaction -I don't do it. ...
... Some other studies made a clear distinction between the concepts of Think-Aloud and Reading-Aloud. According to Pergams et al. (2018), Reading-Aloud is technique widely used with young students to help them improve their comprehension abilities and master their pronunciation, grammar, and intonation. Moreover, Siagian & Katemba, (2016) confirmed the effectiveness of Think -Aloud and Visual Imagery in substantially enhancing students' elt.ccsenet.org ...
Article
Full-text available
The current study's objective examines the effectiveness of using a Think-Aloud strategy in improving Saudi EFL learners' reading comprehension and attitudes towards learning. A quantitative study with a quasi-experimental design was implemented through applying two different instruments: Reading Comprehension Skills Test and Attitude Scale towards learning EFL. The study adopts a pre-post control group design where forty students were randomly assigned to either a control or an experimental group. Students of the experimental group were instructed by using metacognitive Think-Aloud strategy, whereas, the control group received traditional treatment such as skimming and scanning techniques. The findings of the study showed that the attitudes and reading comprehension skills of the experimental group improved significantly as opposed to the control group. The study gives more insight into the importance of applying a Think-Aloud strategy in teaching reading comprehension inside EFL educational context. The study also suggests recommendations for EFL teachers to increase the efficiency of applying this strategy through their teaching procedures.
... The method allows for the examination of the design process in action (Akin, 1993) with minimal disruption of cognitive performance (Ericsson & Simon, 1993). As designers verbalize their thoughts as they occur, they may reveal what participants understand, what they pay attention to, and what guides them (Bernadowski, 2016;Bilda, Gero, & Purcell, 2006;Gotwals & Songer, 2013;Pergams, Jake-Matthews, & Mohanty, 2018;Stieff, 2011;Wiltschnig, Christensen, & Ball, 2013). This think-aloud method provides researchers with data not otherwise accessible about thoughts contributing to the creation of final explanations or solutions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Problem exploration includes identifying, framing, and defining design problems and bounding problem spaces. Intentional and unintentional changes in problem understanding naturally occur as designers explore design problems to create solutions. Through problem exploration, new perspectives on the problem can emerge along with new and diverse ideas for solutions. By considering multiple problem perspectives varying in scope and focus, designers position themselves to increase their understandings of the “real” problem and engage in more diverse idea generation processes leading to an increasing variety of potential solutions.Purpose/HypothesisThe purpose of this study was to investigate systematic patterns in problem exploration in the early design phases of mechanical engineers.Design/Method Thirty‐five senior undergraduate students and experienced designers with mechanical engineering backgrounds worked individually following a think‐aloud protocol. They explored problems and generated solutions for two of four randomly assigned design problems. After generating solutions, participants framed and rewrote problem statements to reflect their perspectives on the design problem their solutions addressed. Thematic analysis and a priori codes guided the identification of problem exploration patterns within and across problems.ResultsThe set of patterns in engineers’ problem exploration that emerged from the analysis documents alternative strategies in exploring problems to arrive at solutions. The results provide evidence that engineering designers, working individually, apply both problem‐specific and more general strategies to explore design problems.Conclusions Our study identified common patterns in the explorations of presented problems by individual engineering designers. The observed patterns, described as Problem Exploration Perspectives, capture alternative approaches to discovering problems and taking multiple problem perspectives during design. Learning about Problem Exploration Perspectives may be helpful in creating alternative perspectives on a design problem, potentially leading to more varied and innovative solutions. This paper concludes with an extended example illustrating the process of applying Problem Exploration Perspectives to move between problem perspectives to generate varied design outcomes.
... Much is written (see for example Westbrook et al., 2018;Pergams, 2018;Duncan and Paran, 2018) about reading aloud as a means to an endas a teaching and learning tool, for examplebut little about it as an end in itself: a real-life adult literacy practice. The extent and range of contemporary adult reading aloud has not been adequately documented, and we make a start on this endeavour here. ...
Article
While much is written about reading aloud to children, and as a teaching tool, far less is known about the oral reading that adults do at home, at work or in the community. This article presents the results of a national survey into whether, what, how and why adults across Britain may read aloud rather than in silence. Analysing data from 529 questionnaire responses, the article examines the frequency with which different text types are read aloud, the formations in which this is done – alone, with one other person or in a group – and the purposes of reading aloud and being read to, with attention to differences according to gender and age. Findings suggest that reading aloud happens in a number of different ways and for different purposes, across contexts and life domains, and that it has a significant relationship with aspects of the lifecourse and with identity formation and performance.
... But what do we actually know about contemporary oral reading practices? Reading aloud is frequency examined as a teaching tool and/or something done to (or with) children to prepare for the ultimate end-goal of adult, individual, silent reading (Gibson 2008;Gil, Larios, and Balibrea 2017;Hoffman, Roser, and Battle 1993;Pergams, Jake-Matthews, and Mohanty 2018). Little attention is paid to the reading aloud that adults may do at home, work and in the community. ...
Article
Reading aloud receives a great deal of attention as something done with children and as a potential teaching tool, but less is known about the oral reading that we, as adults, may do for various purposes across our everyday lives. This article explores one element of a two-year project recording and analysing contemporary adult reading aloud practices. It reports on an analysis of all 160 Mass Observation Project responses, examining the ‘atypical typicality’ of correspondents’ reactions to the topic and their own practices; their analytical acts as researchers classifying practices and investigating artistry; and the stories they tell about how we both express and create relationships with individuals and larger groups through our oral reading practices. Contemporary adult reading aloud practices, as presented, analysed and narrated by the Mass Observers, are ubiquitous, often invisible, hugely varied, and matter to us as individuals and communities.
Chapter
Thinking aloud and reading aloud are two instructional practices intended to provide learners with models of proficient language use and strategic thinking. As in any instructional practice, teachers must be clear about both purpose and anticipated outcomes when implementing a think‐aloud or a read‐aloud strategy. Teacher clarity means that the teacher knows what to teach, how to teach it, and what learning successes are expected. Hattie's meta‐analysis of thousands of studies aggregates data from many of them and identifies the effect size of teacher clarity as 0.75. This is quite significant, since 0.4 is suggested by Hattie as the “hinge point”: all percentages above that work well to promote learning and instruction. While his initial listings are often updated through inclusion of new studies, the powerful effect size of teacher clarity remains consistent. Early readers are often introduced to many of the conventions of reading – such as book handling, print conventions, and language and text structures – during a read aloud or a think aloud. While engaging students and fostering learning and academic language development, both strategies are powerful motivational supports for students throughout the grades, especially English learners because they facilitate their hearing academic language produced by a capable reader and speaker. This entry outlines the traditions of thinking aloud and reading aloud and then focuses on the use of these two instructional practices relative to vocabulary development.
Chapter
Thinking aloud and reading aloud are two instructional practices intended to provide models of proficient language use to listeners. Additionally, early readers are often introduced to many of the conventions of reading such as book handling, print conventions, and language and text structures during a read aloud or a think aloud. While engaging students and fostering learning and academic language development, both strategies are powerful motivational supports for students throughout the grades, especially English learners because they facilitate their hearing academic language produced by a capable reader and speaker. This entry outlines the traditions of thinking aloud and reading aloud and then focuses on the use of these two instructional practices relative to vocabulary development.
Article
Full-text available
Effects of just reading, performance reading, and interactional reading-aloud styles on learning were assessed for 117 1st graders and 129 3rd graders. Preservice teachers, trained and guided by scripted procedures, read 2 informational storybooks to students using 1 of the styles. Multivariate analyses of variance and univariate tests showed that reading-aloud styles produced statistically significant effects on vocabulary acquisition and comprehension and similar results at each grade level. Differences in comprehension means for reading style treatments were statistically significant for 1 book only. Vocabulary acquisition was facilitated more by interactional reading than performance reading. Both verbally mediated styles resulted in greater vocabulary learning than just reading. Results extend previous findings on reading-aloud styles and are congruent with sociolinguistic and transactional theories. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Chapter
This text presents up-to-date theory, research, and classroom applications in second language reading from an interactive perspective. The paperback edition presents up-to-date theory, research, and classroom applications in second language reading from an interactive perspective. The book supports the view that reading in a second language involves more than decoding; instead, reading is seen as an interactive process whereby the learner's own background and knowledge contribute to understanding reading material. These articles are written by experts in the reading field, who present process models and discuss classroom applications. The book examines traditional approaches that focus on text and decoding skills, as well as more contemporary theories that consider the reader's base knowledge.
Article
Teaching and Researching Reading was first written to help language professionals understand the complex nature of reading. Now in a thoroughly updated and improved second edition, the book expands connections from research on reading to instructional practices and teacher-initiated action research. Offering an updated overview of reading theory, it summarises key ideas and issues in first and second language contexts.
Chapter
Collins, A., Brown, J.S., & Newman, S.E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the crafts of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.) Knowing, learning, and instruction: E...
Article
This paper analyses initial findings of a qualitative pilot study of the reading-aloud practices of 17 adults in the London Borough of Lewisham. Although the dominant contemporary image of reading is that of a silent activity and within literacy provision it is frequently assumed that reading aloud is not a ‘natural’ ‘real life’ adult practice, anecdote suggests that adults do indeed read aloud, but these practices are overwhelmingly undocumented. This study is the first stage in developing a better understanding of contemporary adult reading-aloud practices. Semi-structured interviews were used to ask adults whether, what, where, how and why they read aloud. Initial findings reveal the ubiquitous nature of reading aloud in adult life, across a range of life domains (spiritual life, family life, work and learning) and for different self-selected purposes (to memorise, to understand, to write and for fellowship). Initial findings reinforce the importance of expanding our definitions and conceptualisations of reading to recognise the diversity and changing nature of real life practices, and suggest implications for our understanding of the role of reading in adult life, for literacy education and for future research.
Article
This content analysis of articles in the Social Science Citation Index journals from 1995 to 2009 was conducted to provide science educators with empirical evidence regarding the effects of scaffolding on science learning. It clarifies the definition, design, and implementation of scaffolding in science classrooms and research studies. The results show important cross-study evidence that most researchers have adopted a qualitative approach (67.44%), focused on learning context (72.09%), and used high school students as participants (53.49%). In designing scaffoldings, researchers have shown a preference for long-term explicit scaffolding using multiple representations to promote procedural and strategic skills and alternative assessments of learner performance. Nevertheless, scaffolding issues related to teacher education are unexpectedly few (11.63%) in empirical research. The results also indicate that there are too few studies to guide researchers in considering fading scaffolds for active learning (9.30%). Future directions and suggestions toward conducting research regarding scaffolding are provided.
Article
As a teacher educator in reading, I am interested in how well classroom practices which preservice students observe during fieldwork coincide with what they learn in my courses. Through discussions with my students, as well as my own classroom observations, it appears that there is not always a fit between current theory and the actual classroom practice. For example, round robin reading, the practice of one student at a time reading a portion of text aloud while other students listen, became the center of a class discussion recently.
Article
This study explored 3- and 4-year-old children's development of scientific vocabulary from participation in repeated interactive read-aloud events and retellings of three informational picture books about light and color, followed by hands-on science activities. Thirty-two children attending a YWCA preschool were matched by age and general vocabulary knowledge and randomly assigned to a retelling or no retelling condition. Repeated measures ANOVA of pretest and posttest scores on a free recall target vocabulary test revealed significantly higher scores for 4-year-olds, those with higher vocabulary knowledge, and those who retold the books. Children used significantly more target words across the three retellings.
Article
The first article in this series considered contemporary theoretical insights into the LI and FL reading process. Here we shall be focusing on more practical matters relating to topics such as text selection, ways in which learners read, how learners can be introduced to texts and what reading strategies are likely to be of most use to them.