ArticlePDF Available

Harnessing the creative potential of consumers: money, participation, and creativity in idea crowdsourcing

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Given the growing importance of innovation and consumer engagement, many firms are strongly interested in finding ways to encourage their consumers to generate creative new product ideas for them in their crowdsourcing initiatives. To that end, managers often use monetary rewards—one of the most commonly used managerial tools to stimulate desired behaviors. A critical question in this respect is whether the use of monetary rewards is effective in stimulating creativity and, if so, how large those rewards should be. This study aims to answer these questions. The results of an experiment suggest that introducing monetary rewards does not contribute to the number of new product ideas generated by a single consumer or the novelty of his/her ideas, and when the reward is relatively small, it can even be harmful. Monetary rewards, however, are effective in encouraging widespread participation in crowdsourcing initiatives and improving the appropriateness of the new product ideas. As a whole, these findings take us a step further toward better understanding the motivational mechanisms of consumer creativity in new product ideation.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Harnessing the creative potential of consumers: money,
participation, and creativity in idea crowdsourcing
Oguz Ali Acar
1
Published online: 26 April 2018
#The Author(s) 2018
Abstract Given the growing importance of innovation and consumer engagement,
many firms are strongly interested in finding ways to encourage their consumers to
generate creative new product ideas for them in their crowdsourcing initiatives. To that
end, managers often use monetary rewardsone of the most commonly used mana-
gerial tools to stimulate desired behaviors. A critical question in this respect is whether
the use of monetary rewards is effective in stimulating creativity and, if so, how large
those rewards should be. This study aims to answer these questions. The results of an
experiment suggest that introducing monetary rewards does not contribute to the
number of new product ideas generated by a single consumer or the novelty of his/
her ideas, and when the reward is relatively small, it can even be harmful. Monetary
rewards, however, are effective in encouraging widespread participation in
crowdsourcing initiatives and improving the appropriateness of the new product ideas.
As a whole, these findings take us a step further toward better understanding the
motivational mechanisms of consumer creativity in new product ideation.
Keywords Reward .Motivation .Ideation .Consumer creativity .Crowdsourcing .User
innovation
1 Introduction
A growing number of firms are using crowdsourcing to involve consumers in their new
product ideation efforts (Bayus 2013; Huang et al. 2014). Such practices not only hold
great potential for tapping into consumer creativity but also help firms to build stronger
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-018-9454-9
*Oguz Ali Acar
oguz.acar@city.ac.uk
1
Cass Business School, City, University of London, 106 Bunhill Row, London EC1Y 8TZ, UK
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
connections with their customers (Acar and Puntoni 2016;Bayus2013; Fuchs et al.
2010; Poetz and Schreier 2012; Stephen et al. 2016).
1
Leveraging these benefits of
crowdsourcing depends of course on being able to motivate consumers to take part in
crowdsourcing initiatives. To that end, some firms offer extremely generous rewards for
creative ideas. For example, Frito-Lay organizes BDo Us A Flavor^ideation contests
where $1 million is offered for the best new flavor suggestion for potato chips. Other
firms, however, offer only modest rewards for comparable taskse.g., on the eYeka
crowdsourcing platform, Knorr offered 2500 Euros for a revolutionary idea for a hot
savory snack. Other firms such as Starbucks and Dell provide no monetary rewards at
all for creative ideas that are submitted to their online communities (i.e., MyStarbucks
Idea and Dell IdeaStorm).
As the examples above clearly indicate, managerial practices in terms of rewarding
consumer creativity in ideation activities are far from uniform. In addition, research on
the relationship between rewards and creativity has not provided a clear answer
regarding the effectiveness of providing monetary rewards for creative tasks. As such,
it remains unclear whether firms should offer monetary rewards in their crowdsourcing
initiatives and, if so, how sizable those rewards should be. Drawing on an experiment
in which participants generated new product ideas in return for real prizes, this paper
aims to answer these questions and explores whether money can buy participation and
creativity in crowdsourcing platforms. The results suggest that monetary rewards
(compared to no monetary rewards at all) can be effective in (1) stimulating greater
participation in crowdsourcing initiatives and (2) motivating consumers to create more
useful ideas, provided the rewards are large enough. In terms of stimulating novelty and
motivating consumers to generate multiple ideas, however, even sizable rewards
appeared to have no effect, and smaller rewards could even be detrimental.
2 Related literature
2.1 Monetary rewards and creativity
Psychology research on the relationship between rewards and creativity divides broadly
into mainly into two opposing camps and provides conflicting recommendations (see
Byron and Khazanchi 2012 for a meta-analysis). While some scholars, often drawing
on self-determination theory, have postulated that rewards are detrimental to creativity
(Amabile 1996; Hennessey and Amabile 2010), others have argued that rewards in fact
enhance creativity (Eisenberger and Armeli 1997;EisenbergerandAselage2009). In
one of the few studies focusing specifically on creativity in the new product ideation
context, Burroughs et al. (2011) found that the positive effects of monetary rewards on
creativity were dependent on creativity training being provided in conjunction with the
reward. It is worth noting that earlier experimental studies on the link between rewards
and creativity focused almost exclusively on comparing the effects of offering or not
offering a reward (i.e., on the presence of a reward). Very few of the experiments which
compared the effects of different sizes of reward (Eisenberger and Armeli 1997;
Eisenberger and Selbst 1994) also involved creativity training before tasks were
1
Crowdsourcing may also have other benefits such as helping firms gain publicity.
178 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
undertaken. Given that creativity training has already been found to moderate the
relationship between rewards and creativity (Burroughs et al. 2011), it was not possible
in these studies to determine which of the effects may have been particular to reward
size and how reward size may have influenced creativity when no training was
provided.
2.2 Monetary rewards in crowdsourcing
This line of research has focused mainly on identifying the optimal prize structure for
incentivizing contest participants to do their best in crowdsourcing contests. More
specifically, scholars have examined whether a fixed sum should be provided to a
single winner (a winner-takes-all award structure) or whether the prize money should
be distributed among various participants (i.e., a multiple-prize award structure) (e.g.,
Ales et al. 2017; Terwiesch and Xu 2008). Terwiesch and Xu (2008) found, for
example, that for ideation projects (the type which we focus on in this paper), a
winner-takes-all structure is the best strategy. Ales et al. (2017), however, showed that
multiple prizes may work better than winner-takes-all prizes in certain circumstances
as, for example, when the crowd perceives that popularity with consumers is important
in assessing ideas (which is often the case, as most crowdsourcing initiatives allow and
indeed encourage other consumers to vote on the ideas put forwarde.g., Lego Ideas,
BDo Us A Flavor^contest). As a whole, although this research extended our under-
standing of how prize money should be distributed among winners in crowdsourcing
contests, it did not answer the question as to what role the presence and size of
monetary rewards play in driving the generation of creative ideas.
2.3 Monetary rewards for noncreative tasks
In the context of noncreative tasks, there is extensive evidence from the economics
literature to suggest that the size of monetary reward enhances peopleseffortand
performance in the activity for which the reward is being given (e.g., Lazear 2000;
Prendergast 1999). However, this view has been challenged recently (Gneezy and
Rustichini 2000; Heyman and Ariely 2004). In their seminal paper, Gneezy and
Rustichini (2000) provide an account of how rewards can have different effects,
depending on their size. That is, unless it is sufficiently generous, a monetary reward
might lead to a poorer performance than no reward at all. It is, however, not clear
whether this can be extrapolated directly to creative tasks. This is because motivating
individuals to be creative may be different from motivating them for routine perfor-
mance, due to the particular cognitive and behavioral processes that underlie creativity:
creative tasks are more dependent on intrinsic motivation, more risky, and more
cognitively demanding (Byron and Khazanchi 2012).
3 New product ideation experiment
I recruited 302 adults (142 female) from an online crowdsourcing platform for human
intelligence tasks (HITs), Amazon Mechanical Turk, in exchange for a small payment.
The recruitment was for an unrelated filler taski.e., completing a survey about their
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188 179
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
thoughts and beliefs. Once the participants had answered the questions in the filler task,
they received a note indicating clearly that the task was over and that they would
receive their payment. At the end of the note, they were told that there was an idea
generation contest (separate from the task they had just completed) and that, if they
wanted to, they could also participate in that.
Participants saw an invitation to take part in an idea generation contest to come up
with a new flavor idea for a potato chip. This choice was informed by actual contests
such as BDo Us a Flavor^contest. Participants were randomly assigned to receive one
of three versions of the invitation, designed to manipulate the rewards. All the
information was identical, except that in the low and high reward conditions, partici-
pants were given additional information specifying the amount they could potentially
win. No such information was provided to those in the no reward condition. The main
part of the invitation read as follows:
Do you have a great flavor idea for potato chips? Share it to join us in our
endeavor to create Bthe best^potato chips in the world. We expect to receive
about 100 ideas and will ask a panel of consumers to select the top 3 most
creative flavors. We will let you know if you are the creator of one of the
Bwinners.^
As noted, in the low and high reward conditions, participants also received
details of the prizes (i.e., BHere are the prizes for winners^). Specifically, those
in the low reward category were informed that there was total prize money of $40,
and that they stood a chance of winning either $25 (first prize), $10 (second prize),
or $5 (third prize). Those in the high reward category were told that the total sum
available was $400, and that they might win either $250 (first prize), $100 (second
prize), or $50 (third prize). The decision to offer multiple rewards, rather than a
single prize for the overall winner, was prompted by previous marketing research on
the link between rewards and creativity in new product ideation (Burroughs et al.
2011) and by actual crowdsourcing practice (e.g., BDo Us A Flavor^contest and
contests on the eYeka and Tongal platforms, among others). The amounts offered
were determined based on a pretest with 42 participants (26 female) recruited from
the same population as our study participants. In the pretest, participants were asked
to rate different prizes (presented in a random order) on a seven-point scale (1 =
very low, 4 = about right, 7 = very high). The prize structure for the high reward was
perceived to be about right (M= 4.07, SD = 1.80; not significantly different from
the mid-point, p> .25), and the structure for low reward condition was perceived to
be low (M= 2.43, SD = 1.60; significantly lower than the mid-point, p< .001).
Importantly, the invitation included information about the expected number of
contestants (i.e., Bwe expect to receive about 100 ideas^), because participants
might expect more competition in the high reward condition and this might influ-
ence how they rated their chances of winning.
After they had been given this information, participants were asked whether they
wanted to take part. A total of 145 participants (79 female) decided to take part, but
only 141 of them (77 female) actually submitted at least one idea. Those who chose to
participate were given more details about the idea generation task and the rules of the
contest (i.e., the evaluation criteria and submission requirements) which were adapted
180 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
from a real crowdsourcing contest (i.e., BDo Us A Flavor^). All the participants were
given the following information:
Thank you for agreeing to share your creative idea(s) with us. Please share your
new flavor idea for a potato chip below. Please make sure your idea consists of
(1) flavor name, (2) ingredients and (3) a brief description (why you think this
makes a great flavor). A panel of consumer judges will evaluate every new flavor
idea based on:
&its potential for a delicious flavor for potato chips
&originality of the flavor compared to other flavors in the market.
The top three ideas that have the highest cumulative score in these two criteria
will be selected as winners. Feel free to enter as many ideas as you wish.
4Results
4.1 Participation
As described in the previous section, participants were first given information about the
contest and then asked whether they would like to participate. The answer to this
question was coded as 0 if a respondent chose not to participate and 1 if he or she chose
to participate. I conducted a logistic regression analysis to assess the effect of different
monetary reward conditions on participantslikelihood of taking part in the idea
generation contest. I used the high reward condition as the baseline condition and used
dummy variables to represent the low reward and no reward conditions. The high, low,
and no reward conditions had 101, 101, and 100 participants respectively. A test of the
full model against the intercept-only model was significant, suggesting that the reward
conditions affected consumerslikelihood of participating (χ
2
(2) = 14.14; p=.001).As
depicted also in Fig. 1, participants in the high reward condition were more likely to
take part in the contest than those in the no reward (61 vs. 36%; B=1.04; odds
ratio = .35; p< .001) and low reward condition (61 vs. 43%; B=.76; odds ratio = .46;
p= .008). A separate logistic regression analysis, in which the no reward condition was
used as the baseline, showed that the likelihood of participating in the contest was not
significantly different in the low reward and no reward conditions (B=.28;oddsratio=
1.32; p> .25). These results suggest that monetary rewards can be used to attract more
people to participate in a crowdsourcing contest as long as the reward is sufficiently
high.
4.2 Number of ideas generated
In the task, participants were free to generate as many ideas as they liked, and the
number of unique ideas submitted by each participant was coded. The number of ideas
(also sometimes referred to as fluency) is often considered to be indicative of creativity
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188 181
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
(Guilford 1967) and has been used extensively in the prior literature (e.g., Mehta et al.
2012; Toubia 2006). The no reward, low reward, and high reward conditions had 36,
43, and 62 participants respectively. To account for non-normality, I conducted a
nonparametric KruskalWallis test for three independent samples. The results showed
a statistically significant difference in number of ideas generated across different reward
conditions (χ
2
(2) = 7.29, p= .026). Participants in the low reward condition generated
significantly fewer ideas per person (M= 1.02, SD = .15) than those in either the no
reward (M=1.39, SD=.90; Z
Mann-Whitney
=2.78, p= .005) or high reward (M=1.32,
SD = .83; Z
Mann-Whitney
= 2.30, p= .021). No significant difference was observed
between the no reward and high reward conditions (p>.25) (Fig.2).
4.3 Novelty and appropriateness of ideas
Creative ideas are considered to have two main componentsnovelty (i.e., originality,
uniqueness) and appropriateness (i.e., usefulness, effectiveness) (Hennessey and
Amabile 2010). In line with recent marketing research (e.g., Mehta and Zhu 2016;
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
Participation (%)
No reward
Low reward
High rewar
d
Fig. 1 Effect of monetary rewards on participation
0
0.5
1
1.5
)nosreprep(saedIforebmuN
No reward
Low reward
High rewar
d
Fig. 2 Effect of monetary rewards on number of ideas
182 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Moreau and Dahl 2005), I used separate ratings by independent judges to measure these
two dimensions (instead of having a single score for creativity). In particular, I recruited
five judges from the same population as our study participants to assess the novelty and
appropriateness of each idea. The judges were given no information about the identity
of the participants or the other judges, nor were they told about the conditions of the
experiment. The only information that I provided to the judges was a spreadsheet of the
participantsideas (in random order) and details of the experimental task and evaluation
criteria. Judges were asked to rate each idea in terms of its novelty and its appropri-
ateness, using a scale developed by Moreau and Dahl (2005). For novelty, the three
items in the measure were originality, innovativeness, and creativity, and for appropri-
ateness, the items were practicality, likely effectiveness, and usefulness; all items were
scored using a seven-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very) (see Table 1for a list of
example ideas). To create a novelty and appropriateness index for each participant, I
followed the procedures outlined by Mehta and Zhu (2016,2009). I averaged each of
the five judgesratings on three items for novelty/appropriateness to obtain five
novelty/appropriateness scores for each idea. These scores were then averaged to obtain
an overall score for both novelty and appropriateness (α= .79 and α= .69 respectively).
When a participant submitted multiple ideas, an average score for novelty and appro-
priateness was calculated for that participant by summing up novelty/appropriateness
score for each of the ideas and dividing it by the total number of ideas he or she had
generated.
AKruskalWallis test for three independent samples returned a significant main
effect of monetary rewards on the novelty and appropriateness of the ideas generated
(χ
2
(2) = 9.02, p=.011 and χ
2
(2) = 12.75, p= .002 respectively). Compared to those
generated in the low reward condition (M= 4.78, SD = 1.36), the ideas generated by the
participants in the high reward condition (M= 5.35, SD = 1.07; Z
Mann-Whitney
=2.18,
p= .029) and no reward condition (M=5.54, SD=1.09;Z
Mann-Whitney
=2.75,p=.006)
were judged to be significantly more novel. The differences in novelty judgments
between the high reward and no reward conditions were not significant (p> .25). The
Tab l e 1 Selected ideas from the experiment
Some of the winning (i.e., most creative) ideas
Pad Thai: Ingredients would include flavors like tamarind, spring onion, a little brown sugar, garlic, fish sauce,
and lime. I would love to try a chip like this. I imagine them having a little sweet background with some
good umami flavors built in, with a finish of lime. I have seen quite a few different flavors of chips
developed but none of them seem to get very creative with different ideas about cuisine.
Pesto and Ricotta Ravioli: I want it to embody the flavor of a classic ricotta-stuffed ravioli smothered in pesto.
Im not sure what the ingredients would consist of, but it would need to taste like ricotta, basil, garlic, pine
nuts, oliveoil, and salt and pepper. I think this would make a great flavor because lets face it, raviolis are so
good! And pesto has to be the best sauce ever created. So when youre craving a bowl of pasta in the middle
of the day, you can reach for a convenient bag of ravioli pesto flavored potato chips!
Roasted Chicken Alfredo: Ingredients: Alfredo, grilled chicken flavoring. Description: Grilled chicken
flavoring would be in the chip itself and the Alfredo would be powder or seasoning on the chip. Why: It
sounds good to me. I have yet to see a chip use any kind of Alfredo flavoring.
Some of the least creative ideas (i.e., those rated low on both novelty and appropriateness)
Bacon: Bacon flavoring added to chips. Bacon makes everything better!
Jalapeno pepper flavored chips: The ingredients would be jalapeno pepper flavoring. I love this food and think
it would be a great potato chip. It probably would taste best kettle-cooked but I think any way would work.
Potato onion chips: Ingredientspotato, onion. I think potato and onion makes a good flavor.
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188 183
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
ideas generated by participants in the high reward condition were rated as being more
appropriate (M= 4.90, SD = .84) than those generated by participants in either the low
reward (M= 4.34, SD = .98; Z
Mann-Whitney
= 3.02, p= .003) or no reward condition
(M= 4.38, SD = .87; Z
Mann-Whitney
= 2.92, p= .004). No significant difference was
observed in the appropriateness scores of the ideas generated in the no reward and
low reward conditions (p>.25) (Fig. 3).
5 Discussion and conclusion
This study examines how monetary rewards influence consumersparticipation and
their level of creativity in firmscrowdsourcing initiatives. The results suggest that
offering sufficiently high rewards (as opposed to either no reward at all or low rewards)
is effective in encouraging more consumers to take part in an idea generation task and
getting them to generate more appropriate ideas. This finding is in line with many
studies in both economics and psychology that emphasize the importance of monetary
rewards in directing peoples attention and effort toward the activity for which those
rewards are being given. One explanation for the nonsignificant difference between the
no reward and low reward conditions in terms of both the appropriateness of the ideas
submitted and consumerswillingness to participate in the first place could be that these
rewards were simply not high enough to generate a significant change in behavior.
The more counterintuitive finding of this study is that introducing a monetary reward
to stimulate new product ideation might in some cases backfire and be detrimental to
creativity. This negative effect was, however, dependent on the level of monetary
reward. With high levels of reward, the offer of money was not detrimental to the
number of ideas generated by each participant and the novelty of those ideas, but
neither was it beneficial. However, when the level of reward was relatively low, this led
to fewer ideas and less novelty than when no reward was offered. One potential
explanation for this finding is that offering a monetary reward might affect participants
motivation in at least two ways. First, it could lead to sorting (Lazear 2000; Rynes et al.
2005)i.e., it might attract different kinds of participant in terms of motivation.
Specifically, consumers who participate in a crowdsourcing task for which there are
4
4.5
5
5.5
6
Idea Novelty Idea Appropriateness
Score (1-7)
No reward
Low reward
High rewar
d
Fig. 3 Effect of monetary rewards on idea novelty and appropriateness
184 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
no rewards are likely to be intrinsically motivated (i.e., they take part because they
genuinely enjoy engaging in creative idea generation) whereas those who decide to take
part in tasks for which rewards are being offered are likely to be more extrinsically
motivated (i.e., their participation is driven, at least in part, by the prospect of winning a
prize). Second, the provision of rewards might lead to an interpretive shift in partici-
pantsmotivationfrom intrinsic to extrinsicin terms of why they are undertaking
the task (Burroughs et al. 2011;Decietal.1999; Lepper et al. 1973). That is, when
rewards are available, consumers are more likely to view their reason for engaging in an
activity as being to gain a financial return, rather than to see it as an opportunity for
sheer enjoyment. As intrinsic motivation is associated with sustained effort and a focus
on fresh and original ideas (Amabile 1996;GrantandBerry2011; Hennessey and
Amabile 2010), consumers who engage in ideation when no rewards are offered are
likely to generate both a greater number of ideas and more novel ideas. The lower level
of intrinsic motivation, as a consequence of the sorting and interpretative shift effects
brought about by the rewards, may render the rewards themselves ineffective, or even
detrimental, in terms of stimulating a greater quantity of ideas and a greater degree of
novelty. Such detrimental effects are likely to occur when the level of reward provided
is not sufficient to allow extrinsic motivation to take over from intrinsic motivation in
terms of motivating participants to maintain their creative effort and focus on novel
ideas. When the rewards are sufficiently high, the decline in intrinsic motivation could
be offset by extrinsic motivation, provided the rewards are contingent on creativity
(Byron and Khazanchi 2012).
These findings have important implications for the marketing and psychology
literature. First, this study helps to provide a better understanding of the dynamics of
crowdsourcing and extends the limited literature on the motivational underpinnings of
consumer engagement and creativity in crowdsourcing initiatives. It thereby also helps
to build a greater understanding of creativity at the Bfront-end of innovation,^which
has been identified as a critical research priority (Hauser et al. 2006) and noted as an
area in which the marketing literature currently provides only limited insights
(Burroughs et al. 2011). Second, the study contributes to the psychology literature on
the link between rewards and creativity. Specifically, to my best knowledge, it provides
the first evidence to show that the effect of rewards on creativity varies depending on
the size of the rewards and that rewards can impact the components of creativity (i.e.,
novelty and appropriateness) in different ways.
From a practical standpoint, the insights provided here can help firms to reap the
benefits of their crowdsourcing initiatives more effectively. The results suggest that
when the main purpose of a crowdsourcing initiative is to get novel ideas, firms may not
need to offer any rewards at all. The drawback, however, is that those ideas may not be
very appropriate and not as many people may be attracted to take part. When these two
factors are important, firms should offer sufficiently high rewards to motivate people to
take part. Whatever the objective of the crowdsourcing initiative, firms should avoid
offering a low level of reward as this would be at best a waste of resources. For firms that
wish to use monetary rewards to incentivize people to participate in their crowdsourcing
initiatives, a natural next question to ask is what constitutes a really low reward and what
level of reward will be adequate. It is impossible to suggest a specific amount that will be
applicable for each and every crowdsourced task, as this would clearly depend on
various contextual factors such as exactly what the task requires (e.g., simple ideas,
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188 185
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
detailed designs, and advertising videos), how intense the competition is (e.g., whether
participants are competing with hundreds or thousands of others), whether specific
expertise is required (e.g., knowledge of a specific video editing software), and various
other factors. Firms should seek to find out what customers perceive to be an adequate
reward, as I have done in the pretest reported in this study.
I conclude by highlighting some avenues for future research. First, in this study,
a multiple-prize award structure was used, and the rewards were contingent upon
being creative. I encourage future research to explore whether the findings of this
study could be generalized for different reward structures and contingencies.
Scholars could, for example, investigate how the relationship between reward
and creativity unfolds when the rewards are provided on a winner-takes-all basis,
when they are distributed differently (e.g., shared equally between multiple win-
ners rather than awarded in order of rank), or when they are completion-contingent
(e.g., offered for everyone who submits an idea). Second, increasing the level of
reward beyond a certain point might lead to lower creativity due to
Bovermotivation^or a Bchoking under pressure^effecti.e., excessive motivation
for an activity can lead to a deterioration in performance (Mobbs et al. 2009).
Although I found no evidence of this phenomenon in my data, future research
could examine potential curvilinear (inverted U-shaped) effects of reward size on
creativity by, for example, including experimental conditions where substantially
higher levels of reward are used than was the case here. Third, several factors,
such as who is giving the money, why it is being given, and how it is given, can
also influence the symbolic meanings associated with it (Mickel and Barron
2008). Exploring the role that such meanings may play in the link between reward
size and creativity is a promising avenue for future research. Fourth, it is impor-
tant to know whether the type of task that is being crowdsourced has a role in the
rewardcreativity relationship. In particular, researchers could investigate the
effects of rewards in crowdsourcing contests that focus on innovative activities
that differ from new product ideation in terms of their complexity (e.g., R&D
problems) or the level of engagement and expertise required (e.g., creating copy
for an ad or developing a product prototype) (Acar and Puntoni 2016;Acarand
van den Ende 2015; Terwiesch and Xu 2008). Finally, the explanations I provide
for why reward size may affect creativity in different ways are conjectural. Future
research is needed to establish whether changes in the level of intrinsic or extrinsic
motivation do indeed account for these findings. In addition, researchers could
also explore other potential mediators, which may include cognitive processes
such as search variation and effort (Acar and van den Ende 2016), and affective
factors such as positive and negative mood (Baas et al. 2008).
I hope that the insights provided in this study will take us one step closer to
understanding what part motivation plays in new product ideation and will help
managers to make more informed decisions on how to reward consumers for their
creativity.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and repro-
duction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and thesource, provide a
link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
186 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
References
Acar, O. A., & Puntoni, S. (2016). Customer empowerment in the digital age. Journal of Advertising
Research, 56(1), 48.
Acar, O. A., & van den Ende, J. (2015). Understanding fear of opportunism in global prize-based science
contests: evidence for gender and age differences. PLoS One, 10(7), e0134898.
Acar, O. A., & van den Ende, J. (2016). Knowledge distance, cognitive-search processes and creativity: the
making of winning solutions in science contests. Psychological Science, 27(5), 692699.
Ales, L., Cho, S., & Korpeoglu, E. (2017). Optimal award scheme in innovation tournaments. Operations
Research, 65(3), 693702.
Amabile, T. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder: Westview Press.
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity
research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779806.
Bayus, B. L. (2013). Crowdsourcing new product ideas over time: an analysis of the Dell ideastorm
community. Management Science, 59(1), 226244.
Burroughs, J. E., Dahl, D. W., Moreau, C. P., Chattopadhyay, A., & Gorn, G. J. (2011). Facilitating and
rewarding creativity during new product development. Journal of Marketing, 75(4), 5367.
Byron, K., & Khazanchi, S. (2012). Rewards and creative performance: a meta-analytic test of theoretically
derived hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 809830.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects
of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 627668.
Eisenberger, R., & Armeli, S. (1997). Can salient reward increase creative performance without reducing
intrinsic creative interest? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(3), 652663.
Eisenberger, R., & Aselage, J. (2009). Incremental effects of reward on experienced performance pressure:
positive outcomes for intrinsic interest and creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(1), 95117.
Eisenberger, R., & Selbst, M. (1994). Does reward increase or decrease creativity? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 66(6), 11161127.
Fuchs, C., Prandelli, E., & Schreier, M. (2010). The psychological effects of empowerment strategies on
consumersproduct demand. Journal of Marketing, 74(1), 6579.
Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or dont pay at all. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3),
791810.
Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. W. (2011). The necessity of others is the mother of invention: intrinsic and prosocial
motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 7396.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hauser, J., Tellis, G. J., & Griffin, A. (2006). Research on innovation: a review and agenda for marketing
science. Marketing Science, 25(6), 687717.
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 569598.
Heyman, J., & Ariely, D. (2004). Effort for payment a tale of two markets. Psychological Science, 15(11),
787793.
Huang, Y., Singh, P., & Srinivasan, K. (2014). Crowdsourcing new product ideas under consumer learning.
Management Science, 60(9), 21382159.
Lazear, E. (2000). The power of incentives. The American Economic Review, 90(2), 410414.
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining childrens intrinsic interest with extrinsic
reward: a test of the Boverjustification^hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1),
129137.
Mehta, R., & Zhu, R. J. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances.
Science, 323(5918), 12261229.
Mehta, R., & Zhu, M. (2016). Creating when you have less: the impact of resource scarcity on product use
creativity. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(5), 767782.
Mehta, R., Zhu, R. J., & Cheema, A. (2012). Is noise always bad? Exploring the effects of ambient noise on
creative cognition. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(4), 784799.
Mickel, A. E., & Barron, L. A. (2008). Getting Bmore bang for the buck^: symbolic value of monetary rewards
in organizations. Journal of Management Inquiry, 17(4), 329338.
Mobbs, D., Hassabis, D., Seymour, B., Marchant, J. L., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2009).
Choking on the money. Reward-based performance decrements are associated with midbrain activity.
Psychological Science, 20(8), 955963.
Moreau, C. P., & Dahl, D. W. (2005). Designing the solution: the impact of constraints on consumers
creativity. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 1322.
Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188 187
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Poetz, M. K., & Schreier, M. (2012). The value of crowdsourcing: can users really compete with professionals
in generating new product ideas? Journal of Product Innovation Management, 29(2), 245256.
Prendergast, C. (1999). The provision of incentives in firms. Journal of Economic Literature, 37(1), 763.
Rynes, S. L., Gerhart, B., & Parks, L. (2005). Personnel psychology: performance evaluation and pay for
performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 571600.
Stephen, A. T., Zubcsek, P. P., & Goldenberg, J. (2016). Lower connectivity is better: the effects of network
structure on redundancy of ideas and customer innovativeness in interdependent ideation tasks. Journal of
Marketing Research, 53(2), 263279.
Terwiesch, C., & Xu, Y. (2008). Innovation contests, open innovation, and multiagent problem solving.
Management Science, 54(9), 15291543.
Toubia, O. (2006). Idea generation, creativity, and incentives. Marketing Science, 25(5), 411425.
188 Mark Lett (2018) 29:177188
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center
GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers
and authorised users (“Users”), for small-scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all
copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By accessing,
sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of
use (“Terms”). For these purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and
students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and
conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal subscription. These Terms will prevail over any
conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription (to
the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of
the Creative Commons license used will apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may
also use these personal data internally within ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share
it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not otherwise
disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies
unless we have your permission as detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial
use, it is important to note that Users may not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale
basis or as a means to circumvent access control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any
jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association
unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a
systematic database of Springer Nature journal content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a
product or service that creates revenue, royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as
part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal content cannot be
used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large
scale into their, or any other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not
obligated to publish any information or content on this website and may remove it or features or
functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature may revoke
this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content
which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or
guarantees to Users, either express or implied with respect to the Springer nature journal content and
all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law, including
merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published
by Springer Nature that may be licensed from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a
regular basis or in any other manner not expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer
Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... The existing literature has investigated how, during the crowdsourcing process, such design elements affect the outcome of each stage of the crowdsourcing project process (see Table 1). For example, in the ideation stage, some scholars have considered how managerial choices related to the description of the problem statement or reward affect the quality or number of crowdsourced ideas (Acar 2018;Gillier et al. 2018;Lee et al. 2015;Li and Hu 2017;Pollok, Lüttgens and Piller 2019;Sieg, Wallin and Von Krogh 2010). Concerning the selection stage, some researchers have discussed how to make decisions regarding the assessment mechanisms to influence the selection of relevant ideas (Blohm et al. 2018;Karachiwalla and Pinkow 2021). ...
... For example, previous studies have recommended selecting the right crowdsourcing platform based on the diversity of experts (Colombo et al. 2013;Karachiwalla and Pinkow 2021), choosing design platform features, such as those with an online community (Ye and Jensen 2022), matching crowds to firms (Qi and Mao 2016) and revealing information (Lee et al. 2018). Past research has also recommended exploring optimal reward structures to increase participant engagement (Acar 2018;Ales, Cho and Körpeoğlu 2017;Terwiesch and Xu 2008) and to increase the quality of solutions and the number of participants (e.g., Lee et al. 2015;Li and Hu 2017). Other researchers have recommended formulating general and well-articulated problem statements to avoid affecting the quality and quantity of solutions (Pollok, Lüttgens and Piller 2019;Sieg, Wallin and Von Krogh 2010), choosing the appropriate composition of the jury to evaluate solutions (Afuah and Tucci 2012) and utilizing tools for evaluating crowdsourcing ideas (Blohm et al. 2018;Ghezzi et al. 2018;Terwiesch and Ulrich 2009;Westerski and Iglesias 2012). ...
Article
The mismatch between crowdsourcing goals and decision making across the crowdsourcing process is likely to not only generate participants' negative feelings but also unsatisfactory outcomes. However, the crowdsourcing literature has thus far mainly focused on how to make decisions at each stage of the process, overlooking the alignment between crowdsourcing goals, decision making and project outcome satisfaction. To address this gap, our research conducted in‐depth interviews with managers involved in crowdsourcing, triangulated with secondary data from real‐life contests to provide a comprehensive analysis. Specifically, we empirically examine the influence of three types of crowdsourcing goals, namely, fishing, hunting and collective production; on decision making at each stage of the crowdsourcing process, that is, ideation, selection, implementation and evaluation. We additionally observe how the alignment between crowdsourcing goals and decision making affects managers' perception and evaluation of the crowdsourcing initiative. We thus contribute to the crowdsourcing and innovation management literature by investigating project‐related crowdsourcing decision making in line with predefined crowdsourcing goals and provide a blueprint for managers toward a more effective project management style.
... Certain limited and rather ambiguous conclusions on this subject can be drawn, for example, from research on the engagement of citizens (employees, consumers) in various creative idea-sharing contest and crowdsourcing initiatives. We know, for example, that the type of prize offered (monetary versus non-monetary), prize amount (low versus high), prize structure (winner-takes-it-all versus multiple prizes), award system (for participation or for novelty/creativity) and the manner of evaluating the contribution (objective versus subjective) may be of significance, with noticeable differences as regards their influence on the users' eagerness to participate, and on the quality of the contribution offered, that is the proposed idea or solution (Acar 2018;Kireyev 2020). We also observe a dynamic increase in knowledge about using gamification to foster e-participation, i.e. designing systems, services, and processes in a way that provides positive, engaging experiences similar to those implemented between gamers (Hassan, Hamari 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Disregard for scientific facts and knowledge holders has usually been identified as a distinguishing feature of the penal populists’ politics. But is penal populism always anti-intellectual? In this article, I provide some deeper insight into the role of expertise in (penal) populist activity, especially in the context of the currently observed redefinition of expertise (some call it “the death of expertise”) and rapid development of new technologies that enable easy aggregation of citizensʼ collective wisdom. Will crowdlaw-making platforms prevent (penal) populism? Or will they strengthen it by facilitating the justification of radical and unnecessary changes in (criminal) law? Is there a place for traditionally conceived experts and established knowledge in crowdsourced law-making process, and if so, what should be their role?
... Extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, stems from the desire to attain external rewards, such as monetary gains or public recognition (Ryan and Deci, 2000). Factors including monetary incentives (Acar, 2018;Ihl et al., 2019) and opportunities for career progression opportunities (Brabham, 2012) have been identified as potent motivators influencing participation and performance in crowd-sourcing contests. Tangible rewards, such as financial compensation, play a central role in driving participation in crowdsourcing and idea competitions (Giovanis et al., 2019;Hwang and Choi, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
In a dynamic business environment, the roles of contests and crowd-sourcing are increasingly acknowledged. However, the factors driving sustained participation in these arenas remain incompletely understood. To address this gap, our study investigates the factors that influence the ongoing engagement intentions of users on contest collection portals. We focus on the interplay between goal-congruent outcomes (GCO), search intention, and various motivational elements. We collected responses from 291 individuals between March 18 and 27, 2022, and conducted an analysis based on partial least squares structural equation modelling (PLS-SEM). Our analysis indicates that utilitarian motivation positively impacts both GCO and search intention. Career promotion specifically influences GCO, while rewards serve as key determinants of both GCO and search intention. Importantly, our findings underscore the role of GCO and search intention in shaping users’ intentions to continue participating. These insights offer significant implications for businesses and platform designers, emphasising the need to understand and cater to the diverse motivations of users to sustain their interest and engagement.
... Creative thinking involves the ability to think in a way that prevents the copying of existing ideas and promotes seeing alternatives to typical associations (Sassenberg et al., 2017). Companies regularly involve consumers in tasks that require creative thinking by inviting them to actively participate in crowdsourcing new ideas or customizing products (Acar, 2018). When companies engage consumers in creative thinking, research finds that such engagement leads to predominantly positive outcomes, including increased brand loyalty (O'Brien et al., 2019), more enjoyment of repeated consumption activities (You et al., 2019), and increased charitable donations (Xu et al., 2021) (see Gino & Ariely, 2012 for one notable exception). ...
Article
Full-text available
Consumer embarrassment occurs frequently and can negatively impact both consumers and marketers. The current work demonstrates that encouraging consumers to engage in creative thinking—whether generating new ideas (e.g., crowdsourcing) or through exposure to creativity-related words—is one way for marketers to address the challenges posed by consumer embarrassment. Three studies demonstrate that prompting creative thinking makes consumers feel less embarrassed in subsequent consumer contexts. Specifically, the findings suggest that when consumers are prompted to think creatively, they assess behaviors that violate social norms as more socially acceptable, lowering feelings of embarrassment across a variety of consumption contexts. This research contributes to our understanding of the many benefits of engaging consumers in creative thinking and the growing stream of work exploring tactics companies can employ to help mitigate consumer embarrassment. This research also offers practical implications for both marketers and consumers.
Article
We question the perspective that curiosity and creativity stem from a shared novelty-seeking process. We emphasize that creativity has two distinct dimensions: Novelty and usefulness, each involving separate cognitive processes. These dimensions may not necessarily mutually reinforce each other. We contend that a more comprehensive model that encompasses the full scope of the creativity construct is needed.
Article
We know that solvers self‐selecting in social innovation challenges come from diverse backgrounds including, among others, scientists, engineers, entrepreneurs, researchers, and professionals from various industries. However, we are not aware of what motives actually bring such solvers to self‐select to address these challenges. This study aims at understanding how different kinds of motivations intervene and interact with the solvers' intention to participate considering the specific context of crowdsourcing for social innovation. Drawing on the self‐determination theory, we built a theoretical framework that hypothesizes how intrinsic, extrinsic, and prosocial motivations interact with one another and affect solvers' self‐selection process in social innovation initiatives. Empirically, to investigate the theoretical framework, a survey research design involving the use of questionnaires was adopted to obtain primary data from solvers engaged in crowdsourcing initiatives for social innovation to solve Covid‐19‐related problems in the HeroX platform. We found that prosocial motivations positively affect the solvers' self‐selection process. Moreover, our results highlight that intrinsic and extrinsic motivations differently moderate the relationship between prosocial motivations and intention to participate. The results of this study offer relevant contributions to previous crowdsourcing and organizational psychology literature and provide critical implications for managers designing and organizing crowdsourcing for social innovation challenges.
Article
An empirical motive study based on the design science approach is presented. The aim of the study is to develop specific problem-solving knowledge in the form of artefacts that can be directly applied in marketing practice. The problem-solving knowledge relates to consumers’ motives for participating in product idea contests (PICs) in the fast-moving consumer goods sector. The research phases underlying the motive study are guided by a five-phase design science research process. As a result of this research process, two artefacts are developed: a multi-item motive scale and a factor analytic motive model. The motive model includes six participation motives – three intrinsic (enjoyment with creative tasks, altruism and search for new/better product solutions) and three extrinsic motives (material–financial reward, identification with the user community and positive feedback from participants/company). Finally, the motive model enables the derivation of recommendations for action for the PIC management.
Article
Full-text available
In an innovation tournament, an organizer solicits innovative ideas from a number of independent agents. Agents exert effort to develop their solutions, but their outcomes are unknown due to technical uncertainty and/or subjective evaluation criteria. In order to incentivize agents to make their best effort, the organizer needs to devise a proper award scheme. While extant literature either assumes a winner-take-all scheme a priori or shows its optimality under specific distributions for uncertainty, this paper derives necessary and sufficient conditions under which the winner-take-all scheme is optimal. These conditions are violated when agents perceive it very likely that only few agents receive high evaluation or when a tournament does not require substantial increase in agents' marginal cost of effort to develop high-quality solutions. Yet, the winner-take-all scheme is optimal in many practical situations, especially when agents have symmetric beliefs about their evaluation. In this case, the organizer should offer a larger winner prize when he is interested in obtaining a higher number of good solutions, but interestingly the organizer need not necessarily raise the winner prize when anticipating more participants to a tournament.
Article
Full-text available
The Internet and advances in digital technologies fundamentally are transforming marketing. Armed with an abundance of information and opportunities, consumers no longer accept the role of passive recipients of marketing communication. This is turning traditional communication approaches upside down
Article
Full-text available
Conducted a field experiment with 3-5 yr old nursery school children to test the "overjustification" hypothesis suggested by self-perception theory (i.e., intrinsic interest in an activity may be decreased by inducing him to engage in that activity as an explicit means to some extrinsic goal). 51 Ss who showed intrinsic interest in a target activity during baseline observations were exposed to 1 of 3 conditions: in the expected-award condition, Ss agreed to engage in the target activity in order to obtain an extrinsic reward; in the unexpected-award condition, Ss had no knowledge of the reward until after they had finished with the activity; and in the no-award condition, Ss neither expected nor received the reward. Results support the prediction that Ss in the expected-award condition would show less subsequent intrinsic interest in the target activity than Ss in the other 2 conditions. (25 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
In an innovation tournament, an organizer solicits innovative ideas from a number of independent agents. Agents exert effort to develop their solutions, but their outcomes are unknown due to technical uncertainty and/or subjective evaluation criteria. To incentivize agents to make their best effort, the organizer needs to devise a proper award scheme. While extant literature either assumes a winner-take-all scheme a priori or shows its optimality under specific distributions for uncertainty, this paper derives necessary and sufficient conditions under which the winner-take-all scheme is optimal. These conditions are violated when agents perceive it very likely that only few agents receive high evaluation or when a tournament does not require substantial increase in agents’ marginal cost of effort to develop high-quality solutions. Yet, the winner-take-all scheme is optimal in many practical situations, especially when agents have symmetric beliefs about their evaluation. In this case, the organizer should offer a larger winner prize when he is interested in obtaining a higher number of good solutions, but interestingly the organizer need not necessarily raise the winner prize when anticipating more participants to a tournament. The online appendix is available at https://doi.org/10.1287/opre.2016.1575.
Article
Prior research has provided conflicting arguments and evidence about whether people who are outsiders or insiders relative to a knowledge domain are more likely to demonstrate scientific creativity in that particular domain. We propose that the nature of the relationship between creativity and the distance of an individual's expertise from a knowledge domain depends on his or her cognitive processes of problem solving (i.e., cognitive-search effort and cognitive-search variation). In an analysis of 230 solutions generated in a science contest platform, we found that distance was positively associated with creativity when problem solvers engaged in a focused search (i.e., low cognitive-search variation) and exerted a high level of cognitive effort. People whose expertise was close to a knowledge domain, however, were more likely to demonstrate creativity in that domain when they drew on a wide variety of different knowledge elements for recombination (i.e., high cognitive-search variation) and exerted substantial cognitive effort.
Article
This research examines how a general sense of resource availability influences consumers’ product use creativity. The authors propose and demonstrate that the salience of resource scarcity versus abundance enhances the novelty of product use solutions in independent consumption environments. An investigation of the underlying process finds that scarcity salience activates a constraint mindset which persists and manifests itself through reduced functional fixedness in subsequent product usage contexts (i.e., makes consumers think beyond the traditional functionality of a given product), consequently enhancing product use creativity. This work advances the extant creativity literature, which is currently limited to examining the effects of context-specific resource constraints, by establishing a context-independent linkage between resource availability and product use creativity. Furthermore, this research contributes to the scarcity literature, which has primarily focused on investigating the quantity and frequency of consumption, by examining the impact of scarcity on the quality of consumption solutions.
Article
Global prize-based science contests have great potential for tapping into diverse knowledge on a global scale and overcoming important scientific challenges. A necessary step for knowledge to be utilized in these contests is for that knowledge to be disclosed. Knowledge disclosure, however, is paradoxical in nature: in order for the value of knowledge to be assessed, inventors must disclose their knowledge, but then the person who receives that knowledge does so at no cost and may use it opportunistically. This risk of potential opportunistic behavior in turn makes the inventor fearful of disclosing knowledge, and this is a major psychological barrier to knowledge disclosure. In this project, we investigated this fear of opportunism in global prize-based science contests by surveying 630 contest participants in the InnoCentive online platform for science contests. We found that participants in these science contests experience fear of opportunism to varying degrees, and that women and older participants have significantly less fear of disclosing their scientific knowledge. Our findings highlight the importance of taking differences in such fears into account when designing global prize-based contests so that the potential of the contests for reaching solutions to important and challenging problems can be used more effectively.