ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The literature on whether varying plate size has an effect on consumption is mixed and contradictory. This meta-analysis of 56 studies from 20 papers shows that varying the size of the container holding food (e.g., plate or bowl) has a substantial effect on amount self-served and/or consumed (Cohen’s d 5 .43). More generally, we found a doubling of plate size increased the amount self-served or amount consumed by 41%. Our analysis resolves the various contradictions of past reviews: we found that the plate-size effect had a substantial effect on amount self-served (d = .51) and on amount consumed when the portion was self-served (d = .70) or manipulated along with (confounded with) plate size (d = 48). However, plate size had no effect on amount consumed when the portion size was held constant (d = .03). Overall, plate size had a stronger effect when participants were unaware that they were participating in a food study (d = .76).
Content may be subject to copyright.
THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE OF EATING
Whether Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption
Depends on Whos Serving and Whos Looking:
A Meta-Analysis
STEPHEN S. HOLDEN, NATALINA ZLATEVSKA, AND CHRIS DUBELAAR
ABSTRACT The literature on whether varying plate size has an effect on consumption is mixed and contradictory.
This meta-analysis of 56 studies from 20 papers shows that varying the size of the container holding food (e.g., plate or
bowl) has a substantial effect on amount self-served and/or consumed (Cohensd5.43). More generally, we found a
doubling of plate size increased the amount self-served or amount consumed by 41%. Our analysis resolves the various
contradictions of past reviews: we found that the plate-size effect had a substantial effect on amount self-served (d5
.51) and on amount consumed when the portion was self-served (d5.70) or manipulated along with (confounded
with) plate size (d548). However, plate size had no effect on amount consumed when the portion size was held con-
stant (d5.03). Overall, plate size had a stronger effect when participants were unaware that they were participating
in a food study (d5.76).
Does plate size affect how much we eat? The Small
Plate Movement (www.smallplatemovement.org) is
founded on the premise that smaller plates lead us
to eat less, but the evidence on the effect of plate size
1
is greatly disputed. Some researchers report that smaller
plates reduce consumption (Wansink and Kim 2005; Wan-
sink, Payne, and Werle 2008; Van Kleef et al. 2012; Wansink
and Van Ittersum 2013). But many others report nding
no effect (Rolls et al. 2004, 2007; Shah et al. 2011; Yip et al.
2013; Libotte et al. 2014). Some even report negative effects
(e.g., Robinson et al. 2015c).
Four recent reviews do little to clarify the effects of plate
(or bowl or other food-container) size on consumption. In
a qualitative review, Casazza et al. (2015) concluded that
plate size was a robust driver of self-served portion sizes.
Robinson and Matheson (2015) in another qualitative re-
view concluded that smaller diameter plates [and] bowls
reduce consumption while acknowledging that there were
some studies that showed no effect. In the only meta-
analysis (quantitative review), Robinson et al. (2014b) con-
cluded that evidence to date does not show that dishware
size has a consistent effect on food intake.Finally, Libotte
et al. (2014) in another qualitative review noted that de-
spite widespread recommendations to use plate size to con-
trol portions, the evidence to supportthis is contradictory.
In short, the effects of plate size are unclear. We there-
fore sought to answer the question of whether smaller plates
reduce consumption and, given the equivocal results, to de-
termine the conditions under which plate size affects con-
sumption.
We rst distinguished between two distinct dependent
variables: the amount of food self-served vs the amount con-
sumed. Robinson et al.s (2014b) meta-analysis focused on
formally measured or recorded food intake,while Casazza
et al. (2015) focused on self-served portions. In general,the
amount self-served is presumed to mediate the amount con-
sumed (see g. 1), an assumption conrmed in a number of
studies that examine both dependent variables (e.g., Koh
and Pliner 2009; Van Kleef et al. 2012). Wansink and Johnson
(2015) and Robinson et al. (2015b) report that studies mea-
suring both show that the amount consumed is 85%90%
of the amount self-served. However, the effect of plate size
on amount consumed might be different if portions are not
self-served. We therefore examined both dependent variables.
In terms of independent variables, we rst distinguished
between manipulations of area (i.e., plate size in its strict
Stephen S. Holden is honorary associate professor of marketing at the Macquarie Graduate School of Management, Macquarie University, Australia.
Natalina Zlatevska (nzlatevs@bond.edu.au) is senior lecturer, marketing, at University of Technology Sydney, Australia. Chris Dubelaar is professor of mar-
keting and associate dean, research and development, at Deakin University, Australia.
1. We are using the term plate sizegenerically here. It therefore includes plates, bowls, and other food containers such as packages.
JACR, volume 1, number 1. Published online January 1, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/684441
©2016 the Association for Consumer Research. All rights reserved. 2378-1815/2016/0101-0013$10.00
sense) and volume (as in bowls and packages). Some studies
manipulate the plate size in two dimensions (i.e., expand-
ing the diameter of a round plate increases its area); others
manipulate three dimensions (i.e., increasing the volume).
Importantly, both Robinson et al. (2014b) and Libotte et al.
(2014) have observed that manipulations of area appeared
to have no effect, while manipulations of volume did. This
result seems surprising as a doubling of area would be more
noticeable than a doubling of volume. Research by Chan-
don and Ordabeyeva (2009) and Ordabeyeva and Chandon
2013) supports this notion by showing that consumers ap-
pear to be more sensitive to a manipulation in one dimension
and less sensitive to manipulations in three dimensions. We
therefore sought to reexamine this unexpected result.
We also distinguish between whether it was the consump-
tion plate or the serving plate that was manipulated, a vari-
able Libotte et al. (2014) referred to as food-serving mode.
Conceptually, consumption and serving plates could inde-
pendently and even interactively affect both amount of
food self-served and amount consumed (see g. 1). Whilecon-
sumption and serving-plate manipulations could be crossed,
no one has done so to our knowledge. We anticipated that
manipulations of the serving-plate size and the consumption-
plate size would show a positive effect.
We also examined two further variables that have been
little examined in the past. The rst is to break down those
studies examining amount consumed by how the portion
size was manipulated: was the portion size xed (the same
for both plate sizes), self-served, or varied (and therefore
confounded) with plate size. Research has shown that por-
tion size has a strong effect on consumption (doubling of a
portion leads to 35% greater consumption on average; Zla-
tevska et al. 2014). We therefore sought to examine whether
plate size had an effect independent of portion size.
The second is whether the plate-size effect is mitigated
in studies where people are aware that they are participat-
ing in a food study. A number of recent studies suggest that
awareness may mitigate various food consumption effects in
general (Robinson et al. 2014a) and, more specically, plate-
size effects (Libotte et al. 2014), portion-size effects (Zla-
tevska et al. 2014), and partitioning effects (Holden and Zla-
tevska 2015).
Finally, we also extended previous analyses by address-
ing the multidimensionality and scalability of plate size by
developing an elasticity measure of the effect of plate size
on consumption.
METHOD
We have followed PRISMA principles (Preferred Reporting
Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) as the ba-
sis for reporting our method and our results (Beller et al.
2013). The interventions we examined were manipulations
of plate size (dened broadly as plate, bowl, and other food
container); the outcomes we examined were amount self-
served and amount consumed. Further details on the inter-
ventions, outcomes, and studies that were included in our
analysis are provided in the following.
Interventions
Our primary interest was in examining the effect of plate-
size manipulations, so studies needed to have at least two
levels of plate size to be included in the analysis.
2
Some stud-
ies included three or more levels, for example, small,”“me-
dium,and large(Rolls et al. 2004, 2007). In these cases, the
comparison of smallversus mediumwas entered into the
meta-analysis as one study and mediumvs. largeas an-
other. We note that Robinson et al. (2014b) did this but also
included an additional study comparing smallversus large.
We did not because it creates a problem of double-counting
the effects. It also creates one effect that is a function of a ma-
nipulation that is equal to the sum of two other manipula-
tions. This highlights that the plate size effect is scalable,
and we might expect a larger change from a larger change
in plate size. This is an issue that we address later.
In order to try and separate some of the conicting nd-
ings regarding plate size, we distinguish between various types
of interventions. First, and in line with Robinson et al. (2014b),
we distinguish between manipulation of area whether re-
ported as diameter (e.g., Rolls et al. 2007) or area (e.g., Koh
Figure 1. Plate size effects.
2. We excluded studies that manipulated the size and number of con-
tainers simultaneously. Specically, Wansink and Cheney (2005), which
manipulated both the size and the number of serving bowls (2 #4L
vs. 4 #2 L), was excluded even though it was included in other reviews
(Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson and Matheson 2015). The problem is that
the size manipulation is confounded with the number manipulation, a fea-
ture of partitioningstudies that can have paradoxical and contradictory
effects (Zlatevska et al. 2014; Holden and Zlatevska 2015).
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 135
and Pliner 2009), and also volume whether reported as bowl
size (Ahn et al. 2010) or package size (Wansink and Kim
2005). Second and in line with an observation by Libotte
et al. (2014), we distinguished between whether the plate
being manipulated was the serving plate (e.g., Wansink
1996) or the consumption plate (e.g., Rolls et al. 2007).
Outcomes
Amount of food consumed was recorded in a wide variety
of ways: grams (Marchiori et al. 2012), ounces (Wansink
et al. 2006), kilojoules (Shah et al. 2011), calories (Di Santis
et al. 2013), and percentage of plate surface(Wansink and
Van Ittersum 2013). To avoid any confusion with energy in-
take (kilojoules), we only included amount consumed for
foods that were homogenous in terms of energy density. In
such cases, whether a study measured weight consumed or
energy consumed was of no consequence.
3
We also included studies that reported amount self-served
even though they were excluded in some previous reviews.
4
We note that ve studies measured both amount self-served
and amount consumed and are therefore reported for each
outcome (Koh and Pliner 2009; Van Kleef et al. 2012; Di
Santis et al. 2013; Van Ittersum and Wansink 2013; Wansink
et al. 2014).
We excluded all studies that measured perceptions and
judgments such as serving a portion to match a target
(Bryant and Dundes 2005; Van Ittersum and Wansink
2012; McClain et al. 2014; Penaforte et al. 2014). Despite
their promise, we also excluded studies that measured body
weight change (Pedersen et al. 2007; Ahn et al. 2010; Hanks
et al. 2013)
5
and food waste (Kallbekken and Sælen 2013) as
the outcomes.
Participants and Study Designs
We included both within-subject (e.g., Rolls et al. 2007) and
between-subject experimental designs (e.g., Wansink and
Van Ittersum 2013), eld and laboratory based experiments,
with both random (e.g., Van Kleef et al. 2012), and non-
random assignment of subjects to conditions (e.g., Koh and
Pliner 2009).
Search Strategy
Studies relevant for the meta-analysis were initially identi-
ed through a search of ABI/Inform, ProQuest Digital Dis-
sertations, Business Source Premier, Web of Science, Psych-
Info, SCOPUS, Google Scholar, and other databases using
the following keywords: portion size, plate size, package size,
bowl size, dishware, and container size. We also manually
searched through the following relevant journals and con-
ference proceedings where papers on portion size, plate size,
or container size have been previously published: Journal
of Marketing,Journal of Marketing Research,Journal of Con-
sumer Research,Journal of Consumer Psychology,Journal of
Public Policy and Marketing,Obesity Reviews,Annual Review
of Nutrition,American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,Body
and Society,British Journal of Sociology,Social Science and
Medicine,Appetite,Journal of Obesity Research,Advances in
Consumer Research, American Marketing Association pro-
ceedings, and the Obesity Society abstract supplements.
The references in articles found in our search were also ex-
amined to identify further studies. We also acknowledge
and thank the reviewers and editors for identifying some
articles that were in press or otherwise missed through the
above process.
Data Extraction
We recorded data from each study that would allow for the
calculation of a standardized mean difference (Cohensd; see
gs. 2a,2b). To enable the later calculation of an elasticity
coefcient, we also recorded the amount self-served and/
or consumed from the smalland largeplate size condi-
tion and the size of the plate if, and as, reported by the re-
searchers themselves.
Finally, we coded for two further variables of interest.
First, we coded for whether or not portion size was self-
served, manipulated along with (i.e., confounded with) plate
size, or held constant across plate-size conditions. Second,
we coded for whether people were aware that they were
participating in a food study.For participants to be un-
aware, the research would generally feature a nonfood cover
story, and consumption was measured covertly. While it is
difcult for a within-subjects design to disguise the fact that
food is the focus of the study as noted by Van Kleef et al.
(2012), within-subject designs with children were, in some
instances, included (e.g., Van Ittersum and Wansink 2013;
Wansink et al. 2014, study 2). These studies featured a
3. Studies measuring the consumption of nonfood items such as
bleach and detergent were excluded (Wansink 1996, study 5). A six-week
pilot study showing a decline in consumption by Robinson and Matheson
(2015) also had to be excluded due to insufcient data.
4. We note that while some previous reviewers focused on amount
consumed (Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson et al. 2014b), they did appear
to include some studies measuring amount self-served (e.g., Wansink
et al. 2006; Van Ittersum and Wansink 2013).
5. Ahn et al.s (2010) data on actual consumption were included.
136 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
eld-setting such as a school or summer-camp, and the food
manipulations were incorporated into regular meals with-
out notice to the children, so we judged the participants
were unaware.
RESULTS
Fifty-six studies reported in 20 papers representing over
3,507 subjects were included in our meta-analysis (see gs.
2a,2b; for details of the included studies and their respec-
tive effect-sizes; see appendix, available online, for full de-
tails of all studies). Using Cohensd, a measure of standard-
ized mean differences, calculated under a random effects
model, we found the overall effect of plate size across the
56 studies was d5.43 (95% condence intervals ±.11),
which would be described conventionally as a medium
effect-size (Cohen 1988).
To address the le-drawer problem (Rosenthal 1979),
we calculated the fail-safe N to be 2,828. This is the num-
Figure 2a. Effect of plate size on amount consumed.
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 137
ber of [null effect] studies that would need to be added
to a meta-analysis to reduce an overall statistically signi-
cant observed result to non-signicance(Rosenberg 2005,
464). A visual observation of the funnel plot for the k5
56 studies shows some asymmetry with some studies with
larger standard errors being overrepresented. The rst pos-
sible interpretation is that this shows publication bias. How-
ever, another possibility is that given the scalability of plate
size, the variation in standard errors may represent differ-
ent strength manipulations of plate size (Sterne et al. 2011).
That a large change in plate size might result in a larger ef-
fectis something we will capture later by calculating the plate
size elasticity of consumption. In any case, standard meta-
analytic reporting gives less weight to studies with higher
standard errors and smaller ns.
The heterogeneity of studies was considerable and the
effect-size varied from study to study as indicated by Q 5
212.3, which was much greater than the degrees of free-
dom (df 555). The I
2
index, indicating the percentage of
variation in the meta-analysis that was attributable to study
Figure 2b. Effect of plate size on amount self-served.
138 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
heterogeneity, was a substantial 74% (Higgins and Thomp-
son 2002; Huedo-Medina et al. 2006).
The overall d5.43 shows that plate size had a positive
effect on amount consumed and/or self-served. Due to the
high degree of study heterogeneity, the fact that ve stud-
ies provided measures of both amount self-served and
amount consumed (and are therefore double-counted), and
the existence of reviews reporting no plate size effect at least
in terms of those studies which manipulate only area as op-
posed to volume (Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson et al. 2014b),
we proceeded by breaking down the effect.
We rst examined plate size effects broken down by
the outcome measured (amount consumed vs. amount self-
served), dimensions manipulated (area vs. volume), and
type of plate (consumption vs. serving plate; see g. 3). This
analysis enabled us to understand more clearly the con-
icting views of whether plate size has an effect, but we
caution against interpreting this analysis as if the variables
are fully crossed in an experimental sense. As may be seen,
some cells in this analysis had no or few observations: we
found no studies manipulating the area of a serving plate,
and only one study that examined the effect of a volume
manipulation on amount consumed (Van Kleef et al. 2012).
Even for those cells with more observations, the studies
contained in each cell differed from studies in other cells
in many ways beyond those variables used to create the
subgroups.
As shown in gure 3, plate sizes had moderate to strong
effects on amounts self-served and consumed across most
cells (d5.33 up to d51.15). There was one notable excep-
tion: the manipulation of area of consumption plates had a
small (and nonsignicant) effect on amount consumed (d
A
5
.06 ±.20). This single cell where the area of a consumption
plate was manipulated appears to account for the conicting
conclusions seen in the literature. The two reviews which fo-
cused on amounts consumed (top half of g. 3) concluded
that manipulations of area (plates) had no effect relative
to manipulations of volume (bowls; Libotte et al. 2014;
Robinson et al. 2014b). However, our analyses show that area
and volume had an approximately equal effect on amount
self-served (d
area
5.49 ±.30 vs. d
volume
5.52 ±.19) (bottom half
of g. 3). The two other reviews focused on amount self-
Figure 3. Effects of plate size: summary.
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 139
served and accordingly concluded that plate size had a clear
positive effect (Casazza et al. 2015; Robinson and Matheson
2015).
While the presentation in gure 3 may encourage us to
think that the anomalous effect is driven by something spe-
cial about the interaction of area and consumption plate,
we again highlight that it would be a mistake to think of
the elements in this cell as having been randomly drawn
from the population of all possible studies. This is perhaps
highlighted by the fact that ve of the 10 studies in this cell
are from one article (Rolls et al 2007), and that there are
no observations of the effect of manipulating the serving-
plate area on amount consumed.
We then proceeded by examining theindependent effects
of ve different variables on the plate size effect: (1) out-
come variable (amount consumed vs amount self-served),
(2) dimensions manipulated (area or volume), (3) type of
plate (consumption vs serving), (4) whether subjects were
aware that they were participating in a food study or not,
and (5) how portion size was manipulated within plate size
for amount consumed. The results are presented in gure 4.
Of the ve variables examined, the effect of plate size
remains fairly consistent whether amount is self-served or
amount consumed is measured, and whether area or vol-
ume, and serving plate or consumption plate is manipulated.
However, the effect of plate size is considerably greater un-
der specic conditions as shown in the bottom two panels
of gure 4. Specically, the plate-size effect was greater
when consumers were unaware that they were participating
in a food study (d
unaware
5.76 vs d
aware
5.31). We note that
Figure 4. Effects of plate size: analysis by subgroups. * This analysis examines the effect of plate size on amount consumed only (k527).
140 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
not only was the effect larger, but it was statistically signif-
icantthe 95% condence intervals for each estimate do
not overlap. Furthermore, examining the effect of plate size
on amount consumed, the effect was stronger if the consum-
ers self-served their portions, or the portion sizes were con-
founded with the plate size. There is an important implica-
tion hereplate size does not appear to have an effect on
amount consumed if the portion size remains xed across
plate sizes. So while the effect of plate size on amount con-
sumed and amount self-served does not appear to be very
different on average, the results suggest that the effect of
plate size on amount consumed is, to a large degree, me-
diated by a portion-size effect. In the case of self-service,
the larger plates apparently encourage larger portions, and
so the consumer eats more.
We note that there are insufcient observations to say
whether the effect of unawareness is crossed with these other
variables. Of the 10 studies in which portion size was xed,
all comprised participants who were aware they were partic-
ipating in a food study. Further research is required to estab-
lish whether or not plate size might have an effect where the
portion is xed if participants are unaware.
A limitation of this subgroup analysis is that it ignores
possible interactions of the identied variables with one an-
other, although, as noted, the lack of observations and lack
of random sampling constrain any effort to conduct such
an analysis. However, the problem does highlight that the
anomalous cell seen in gure 3 where the plate size effect
has no effect, may reect the inuence of variables other
than those identied. For instance, in this anomalous cell,
nine of the 10 studies were with subjects who were aware
they were participating in a food, which tends to show a
smaller effect as seen in gure 4. Moreover, there was one
study out of 10 in this cell that showed a strong plate-size
effect (Wansink and Van Ittersum 2013, study 2; see g. 2a).
The subjects in this study self-served their portions and were
unaware they were participating in a food study.
Scaled Effect Size
To complete our analysis, we developed a scalable measure
of the plate size effect to address the problem that Cohens
dis difcult to interpret. Cohensd(and related standard-
ized mean difference measures) report the effect of con-
trolversus treatmentor, in our case, large versus small
plate size, but nothing beyond this (Chernev et al. 2010).
No allowance is made for the fact that some researchers in-
creased the plate size from the small to large condition by
200% (Marchiori et al. 2012), while others increased it by
just 30% (Rolls et al. 2004). In other words, effect size as
measured by Cohensdcannot capture a plate-size effect
that changes as a function of the change in plate size.
In an effort to address this problem, we calculated per-
centage change in plate size and the resulting percentage
change in consumption to allow for the effect-size to be ex-
pressed as a plate size elasticity of consumption. The per-
centage change in consumption was calculated as follows:
DC=CS;(1)
where DC5change in consumption (amount eaten from
larger plate 2amount eaten from smaller plate) and CS 5
consumption from smaller plate size.
We then calculated the same change parameter for plate
size, although we note that it was necessarily conditioned
on the dimensions that were manipulated. For those stud-
ies reporting plate size as a diameter, we rst converted
this into an area ((diam/2)^2*p) and then expressed change
in area for all observations as follows:
DA=AS;(2)
where DA5change in area (larger area 2smaller area) and
AS 5smaller area.
Change in volume was measured in a similar way al-
though we note that studies typically report change by ca-
pacity of the container (e.g., 100-g package or 2-L bowl) and
almost never included information about the actual physi-
cal dimensions:
DV=VS;(3)
where DV5change in volume (larger volume 2smaller
volume) and VS 5smaller volume.
We then regressed change in consumption (eq. [1]) on
the change in plate size (be it area as in eq. [2] or volume
as in eq. [3]) with no constant and with each study weighted
by the meta-analytic weights generated under a random ef-
fects model.
6
The coefcient generated represents an elas-
ticity measure which can be interpreted as the percentage
change in consumption for a doubling (100%) increase in
plate size.
6. The regressions were modeled without a constant because a zero
percent change in plate size has a zero percent change in consumption.
In any case, and following Eisenhauer (2003), we note that when included,
the constant was not signicant, and the coefcient for change in plate
size was little changed. While the presented regressions were weighted,
unweighted regressions returned virtually identical results, consistent with
the random-effects model weights being more balancedthan under xed-
effects (Borenstein et al. 2009).
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 141
Looking across all 56 studies combined (including ve
studies that provided measures of both amounts self-served
and consumed), doubling the plate size led to increases in
amounts self-served/consumed of 41% on average ( p<.001,
k556). Alternatively stated, halving the plate size led to a
29% reduction in amount self-served/consumed on average.
This scalable measure might offer some promise for explor-
ing the conditions under which plate size effect varies but
requires more observations (studies) in order to be stable.
DISCUSSION
The results from both the meta-analysis and the elasticity
analysis show that plate size has a considerable effect over-
all on amount self-served and consumed. While two previ-
ous reviews concluded that plate size had no reliable effect
on amount consumed (Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson et al.
2014b), our analysis found that there was a substantial
plate size effect on amount consumed, but only if the con-
sumer self-served their portions or portion size was varied
in line with plate-size. Plate size had no effect on average in
situations where portion sizes were held constant across
plate sizes.
Our analyses have also suggested that a major driver of
the effect is whether subjects are aware that they are par-
ticipating in a food study or not. If participants were un-
aware that they were participating in a food study, the effect
of manipulating plate-size was substantially (and signif-
icantly) larger (d
unaware
5.76 vs. d
aware
5.31), a nding in line
with a suggestion by Libotte et al. (2014). This result is
consistent with other recent reports suggesting that im-
portant demand effects operate in food studies. Zlatevska
and colleagues (Zlatevska et al. 2014; Holden and Zlatevska
2015) have showed that the effects of portions and parti-
tions respectively were reduced when research participants
were participating in a study where food was the focus. Rob-
inson and colleagues (Robinson 2014; Robinson et al. 2014a,
2015a) have conducted a meta-analysis and empirical stud-
ies showing that when subjects know they are being ob-
served, their consumption is reduced.
Overall, our analysis supports the notion that plate size
positively inuences consumption when portions are self-
served or varied in line with plate size and if consumers
are unaware that their consumption is being monitored.
But signicant areas remain to be explored more fully. Our
research highlights that more attention needs to be directed
to the distinction between amount consumed and amount
self-served and, in particular, the way in which self-served por-
tions may effectively mediate the observed plate-size effect.
While some previous reviewers have suggested that bowls
or manipulations of volume have a greater effect than ma-
nipulation of area (Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson et al. 2014),
we consider this unlikely. Our results show that there is little
distinction between manipulating a plate in two dimensions
(area) or three dimensions (volume as in a bowl). More-
over, if there were to be a difference, we would expect two-
dimensional changes to have a greater effect than three-
dimensional changes in view of the compelling evidence that
the perceived size and perceived change in size in containers
is quite distinct from the actual change (Chandon and Orda-
bayeva 2009; Ordabayeva and Chandon 2013). Changes in one
dimension are generally perceived as bigger than an equiv-
alent change in three dimensions (Raghubir and Krishna
1999; Chandon and Ordabayeva 2009). In any case, very
few of the studies included in our analysis gave any consid-
eration to the perceived change. With regard to volume in
particular, we found virtually no studies that reported the
dimensions of the container. This made it impossible to
code those studies manipulating volume for the number
of dimensions changed. Given the promising opportunities
offered through the manipulation of one versus all three
dimensions as highlighted by Chandon and Ordabeyeva
(2009), we think that it would be helpful if, at the very least,
future studies examining the effect of plate size, and espe-
cially volume, report the three dimensions of the container
of both the smaller and larger plate.
In terms of future directions and implications, we think
that further exploration of the awareness by participants
of their participation in a food study deserves more atten-
tion. In their qualitative review, Libotte et al. (2014) noted
that in all the studies they reviewed where they found a
plate-size effect, participants were distracted from food-
consumption or serving,and no distraction factors were
present in the studies that did not nd a signicant effect
of plate size.They conrmed this in their own study of
plate size in a setting using a fake-food buffet: their study
produced no effect. In our review, we report no effect of
plate size on amount consumed if the portion size was held
constant but there is an important caveat: all studies con-
tributing to this result featured participants who were aware
they were in a food study. Therefore, we need studies with
unaware participants investigating whether or not plate size
has an effect when portion sizes are held constant.
In view of the potential importance of subject aware-
ness, we add our voice to Robinsons (2014) call to lie more
to participants in eating behaviour experiments.Impor-
tantly, we highlight that the blinding required is more than
142 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
simply blinding to the manipulation but also blinding to the
measurement. That is, the individual needs to be double-
blinded: unaware of the plate size manipulation and un-
aware even that they are participating in a food-consumption
experiment.
However, we also hasten to add that blinding in the clas-
sic social experimentation sense overlooks another prom-
ising possibility related to the human adaptation to novel
stimuli (Berlyne 1971). A novel stimulus attracts attention
initially, becomes familiar, and ultimately is forgotten. This
may explain how some longer-term trials with smaller dish-
ware have reported success in reducing consumption (Ahn
et al. 2010; Robinson and Matheson 2015) and weight
(Pedersen et al. 2007; Hanks et al. 2013). Ensuring partic-
ipants are blind to the manipulation or the measurement in
such trials is likely to be virtually impossible. We speculate
that awareness of the changed dishware attenuates over
time. The result is therefore consistent with the notion
that the effect works best when the participant is blind
to the intervention. The difference is that through adapta-
tion, the consumer becomes blind to long-term manipula-
tions of plate size.
In summary, smaller plates will reduce the amount self-
served to a plate. Smaller plates also reduce the amount
consumed if the consumers are self-serving to those smaller
plates or portion size is manipulated in line with the plate
size. However, simply reducing plate size and holding por-
tion size constant appears to have no effectbut this needs
further investigation with unaware consumers. The plate-
size effect is observed to be larger if the consumers do not
believe they are being watched. So the widespread, long-
term use of smaller plate sizes may help reduce consump-
tion and perhaps obesity in precisely the same way that we
have become blind to how large plate sizes have become. Con-
tinual use of smaller plate sizes may be both habit-forming
and good for your health.
THE LARGER THEME: SMALL STEPS
TOWARD OVERCOMING OBESITY
Obesity is growing (Flegal et al. 2002, 1998; Young and
Nestle 2002), and concern about obesity is growing at a pro-
portional rate. Despite enormous amounts of research and
attention directed to the obesity problem, there appear to
be no simple solutions. Perhaps this is an important nd-
ing: there is no large simple solution; rather obesity can
only be successfully tackled by a series of small steps.
Our research suggests that substituting small for larger
plates is one such small step that will help, especially if con-
sumers are self-serving to the plate, and if the change to
smaller plates is not signaled to the consumer. This research
ts within a larger eld of small ways in which the amount
we consume can be potentially limited by portions served
such as smaller plates (shown in this article), smaller food
units(Davis et al. 2016), smaller portions (Zlatevska et al
2014), partitioned portions (Holden and Zlatevska 2015),
or conversely, encouraging people to eat less by making por-
tions appear larger (e.g., Wansink and Van Ittersum 2003,
2006; McClain et al. 2014).
At a broader level, the efforts toward downsizing con-
sumed portion sizes all t within a much larger eld of small
steps used to modify the consumption environment so as to
nudge people toward healthier and less wasteful consump-
tion behaviors (e.g., Szocs and Biswas 2016; Williamson,
Block, and Keller 2016).
So there are solutions: they are small but also numerous.
The greatest challenge perhaps is how to implement these
ideas. In this regard, we see four different stakeholders we
need to address: public health policy makers, food market-
ers, consumers, and food researchers.
The rst stage in encouraging these small steps toward
a solution is through the public health policy path. But it is
difcult to imagine how these small steps might be imple-
mented by the public health authorities, especially given evi-
dence that consumers can react negatively to heavy-handed
approaches (Pham et al. 2016). The best public health ap-
proach would appear to be to provide information and ideas
for implementing small changes and leaving the changing to
others.
A second step is for marketers to actively engage in en-
couraging healthier consumer habits. While some may view
marketer involvement with some caution, they do in fact
have the capacityand in many instances, the interest
to help nudge consumers in the right direction. In this regard
then, we might encourage marketers to implement ideas
such as making healthy the default option (e.g., Peters et al.
2016).
Ultimately, change has to be implemented at the consumer
level, and for this to work, a more consumer-empowered,
bottom-up approach is probably needed. In this consumer-
centric version, consumers are encouraged (by distributed
information) to make informed decisions to help them-
selves. It is noted that in this regard, the very public outcry
about the growth in obesity may be viewed as a positive. Due
to this media attention, many consumers are searching for
solutions, even if small and subtle. The Small Plate Move-
ment (www.smallplatemovement.org) is an excellent exam-
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 143
ple of a consumer-focused intervention that encourages bet-
ter habits for better living.
Finally, food researchers can help, and our research sug-
gests directions for future efforts. While food researchers
are typically united in seeking solutions for obesity, a search
for strong, simple solutions can potentially hide smaller,
subtle solutions. Our research was motivated by the con-
siderable confusion about whether small plates work to re-
duce consumption. Many had studied the effect of small
plate sizes in various settings, which generated a wide range
of results, positive, neutral, and even negative. Even review
articles have disagreed about whether there is an effect
or not (Libotte et al. 2014; Robinson et al. 2014b; Casazza
et al. 2015; Robinson and Matheson 2015). Our research
resolved this confusion: we show that the effect of smaller
plates on amount self-served is substantial, but the effect
of smaller plates on amount consumed holds only under
specic conditions, notably where consumers self-serve
their portions or when portion size is reduced along with
plate size. More research on the small steps that can be taken,
and the conditions under which they do and do not oper-
ate, is to be encouraged.
Our research also revealed another sometimes entrenched
practice in food science that we believe needs shifting: we
found the overall effect of plate sizes was stronger if study
participants were unaware that their consumption is being
monitored. Our article adds to a growing body of research
suggesting that the effects of portion sizes and partitioning
are mitigated when subjects are aware they are in a food
study (Zlatevska et al. 2014; Holden and Zlatevska 2015).
This ts with broader reviews showing that aware partici-
pants tend to modify or constrain their consumption (Rob-
inson et al. 2014a, 2015a). Importantly then, the small and
subtle steps that can be used to modify consumption might
be missed in studies where participants are aware they are
in a food study.
The obesity problem can be resolvedthrough a series
of small steps. Our research offers some clarication around
one such small step. Use small plates, especially if you are
self-serving your food to your plate. It will encourage you
to self-serve and eat less. What about the problem that if
someone installs smaller plates in their household, every-
one will know, that is, be aware of the change? Our research
does show that smaller plates work best if people are un-
aware that their consumption is being monitored. Fortu-
nately, while humans tend to notice novel stimuli, the ip
side is that they tend to adapt to and overlook familiar
stimuli. So even if the change to small plates in a household
is a conscious decision, over time, the members of the house-
hold are likely to become unaware of the change, and we
might expect to see behavior change accordingly. There is
already some evidence to suggest that this is the case with
individuals adopting smaller dishware showing a loss of
weight over time (Pedersen et al. 2007; Hanks et al. 2013).
The problem of obesity is growing. Smaller plates are one
of a range of small steps that can help reduce the amount
self-served and thus the amount consumed.
REFERENCES
Ahn, Hee Jung, Kyung Ah Han, Hwi Ryun Kwon, and Kyung Wan Min
(2010), Small Rice Bowl-Based Meal Plan Was Effective at Reducing
Dietary Intake, Body Weight, and Blood Glucose Levels in Korean
Women with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus,Korean Diabetes Journal,34(6),
34049.
Beller, Elaine M., Paul P. Glasziou, Douglas G. Altman, Sally Hopewell, Hilda
Bastian, Iain Chalmers, Peter C. Gøtzsche, Toby Lasserson, and David
Tovey (2013), PRISMA for Abstracts: Reporting Systematic Reviews
in Journal and Conference Abstracts,PLOS Medicine, 10 (4), 18.
Berlyne, Daniel E. (1971), Novelty and Attention: Controls for Retinal
Adaptation and for Stimulus-Response Specicity,Psychonomic Science,
25 (6), 34951.
Borenstein Michael, Larry V. Hedges, Julian P. T. Higgins, and Hannah R.
Rothstein (2009), Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chicester: Wiley.
Bryant, Rachel, and Lauren Dundes (2005), Portion Distortion: A Study
of College Students,Journal of Consumer Affairs, 39 (2), 399408.
Casazza, Krista, Andrew Brown, Arne Astrup, Fredrik Bertz, Charles Baum,
Michelle Bohan Brown, John Dawson, Nefertiti Durant, Gareth Dut-
ton, David A. Fields, Kevin R. Fontaine, David Levitsky, Tapan Mehta,
Nir Menachemi, Pk Newby, Russell Pate, Hollie Raynor, Barbara J.
Rolls, Bisakha Sen, Daniel L. Smith, Diana Thomas, Brian Wansink, Da-
vid B. Allison, and Steven Heymseld (2015), Weighing the Evidence
of Common Beliefs in Obesity Research,Critical Reviews in Food Science
and Nutrition, 55 (14): 201453.
Chandon, Pierre, and Nailya Ordabayeva (2009), Supersize in One Dimen-
sion, Downsize in Three Dimensions: Effects of Spatial Dimensional-
ity on Size Perceptions and Preferences,Journal of Marketing Research,
46 (December), 73953.
Chang, Un-Jae, Hyung-Joo Suh, Sun O. Yang, Yang H. Hong, Suk K. Young,
Jin M. Kim, and Eun Y. Jung (2012), Distinct Foods with Smaller Unit
Would Be an Effective Approach to Achieve Sustainable Weight Loss,
Eating Behaviors, 13 (1), 7477.
Chernev, Alexander, Ulf Bockenholt, and Joseph Goodman (2010), Com-
mentary on Scheibehenne, Greifeneder, and Todd: Choice Overload:
Is There Anything to It?Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (3), 426
28.
Cohen, Jacob (1988), Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences,
2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Davis, Brennan, Collin R. Payne, and My Bui (2016), Making Small Food
Units Seem Regular: How Larger Table Size Reduces Calories to Be Con-
sumed,Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1 (1), in this
issue.
144 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
Di Santis, Katherine I., Leann L. Birch, Adam Davey, Elena L. Serrano, Jun
Zhang, Yasmeen Bruton, and Jennifer O. Fisher (2013), Plate Size and
Childrens Appetite: Effects of Larger Dishware on Self-Served Por-
tions and Intake,Pediatrics, 131 (5), e145158.
Eisenhauer, Joseph G. (2003), Regression through the Origin,Teaching
Statistics, 25 (3), 76 80.
Flegal, Katherine M., Margaret D. Carroll, Robert J. Kuczmarski, and Clif-
ford L. Johnson (1998) Overweight and Obesity in the United States:
Prevalence and Trends, 19601994,International Journal of Obesity
and Related Metabolic Disorders, 22 (1), 3947.
Flegal, Katherine M., Margaret D. Carroll, Cynthia L. Ogden, and Clifford
L. Johnson (2002) Prevalence and Trends in Obesity among US Adults,
19992000,JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association,
288 (14), 172327.
Hanks, Andrew, K. Kaipainen, and Brian Wansink (2013), The Syracuse
Plate: Reducing BMI by Introducing Smaller Plates in Households,
Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 45 (4), S41.
Higgins, Julian P. T., and Simon G. Thompson (2002), Quantifying Hetero-
geneity in a Meta-Analysis,Statistics in Medicine, 21 (11), 153958.
Holden, Stephen S., and Natalina Zlatevska (2015), The Partitioning Par-
adox: The Big Bite around Small Packages,International Journal of Re-
search in Marketing, 32 (2), 23033.
Huedo-Medina, Tania B., Julio Sánchez-Meca, Fulgencio Marín-Martínez,
and Juan Botella (2006), Assessing Heterogeneity in Meta-Analysis:
Q Statistic or I
2
Index?Psychological Methods, 11 (2), 193206.
Kallbekken, Steffen, and Håkon Sælen (2013), NudgingHotel Guests to
Reduce Food Waste as a Win-Win Environmental Measure,Economics
Letters, 119 (3), 32527.
Koh, Jiaqi, and Patricia Pliner (2009), The Effects of Degree of Acquain-
tance, Plate Size, and Sharing on Food Intake,Appetite, 52 (3), 595602.
Libotte, E., Michael Siegrist, and T. Bucher (2014), The Inuence of Plate
Size on Meal Composition. Literature Review and Experiment,Appe-
tite, 82 (1), 9196.
Marchiori, David, Olivier Corneille, and Olivier Klein (2012), Container
Size Inuences Snack Food Intake Independently of Portion Size,Ap-
petite, 58 (3), 81417.
McClain, A. D., Wouter van den Bos, Donna Matheson, Manisha Desai,
Samuel M. McClure, and Thomas N. Robinson (2014), Visual Illusions
and Plate Design: The Effects of Plate Rim Widths and Rim Coloring
on Perceived Food Portion Size,International Journal of Obesity,38
(5), 65762.
Ordabayeva, Nailya, and Pierre Chandon (2013), Predicting and Manag-
ing ConsumersPackage Size Impressions,Journal of Marketing,77
(5), 12337.
Pedersen, Sue D., Jian Kang, and Gregory A. Kline (2007), Portion Con-
trol Plate for Weight Loss in Obese Patients with Type 2 Diabetes Mel-
litus,Archives of Internal Medicine, 167 (June), 127783.
Penaforte, F. R. O., C. C. Japur, R. W. Diez-Garcia, J. C. Hernandez, I.
Palmma-Linares, and P. G. Chiarello (2014), Plate Size Does Not Affect
Perception of Food Portion Size,Journal of Human Nutrition and Die-
tetics, 27 (S2), 21419.
Peters, John, Jimikaye Beck, Jan Lande, Zhaoxing Pan, Michelle Cardel,
Keith Ayoob, and James O. Hill (2016), Using Healthy Defaults in
Walt Disney World Restaurants to Improve Nutritional Choices,Jour-
nal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1 (1), in this issue.
Pham, Nguyen, Naomi Mandel, and Andrea C. Morales (2016), Messages
from the Food Police: How Food-Related Warnings Backre among
Dieters,Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1 (1), in this
issue.
Raghubir, Priya, and Aradhna Krishna (1999), Vital Dimensions in Vol-
ume Perception: Can the Eye Fool the Stomach?Journal of Marketing
Research, 36 (3), 31326.
Robinson, Eric (2014), Im Watching You: Why We Might Need to Lie
More to Participants in Eating Behavior Experiments,Appetite,83
(December), 343.
Robinson, Eric, Charlotte Hardman, Jason Halford, and Andrew Jones
(2015a) Eating under Observation: A Systematic Review and Meta-
Analysis of the Effect That Heightened Awareness of Observation Has
on Laboratory Measured Energy Intake,American Journal of Clinical
Nutrition, 102 (2), 32437.
Robinson, Eric, Inge Kersbergen, Jeffrey M. Brunstrom, and Matt Field
(2014a), Im Watching You: Awareness That Food Consumption Is Be-
ing Monitored Is a Demand Characteristic in Eating-Behaviour Exper-
iments,Appetite, 83 (December), 1925.
Robinson, Eric, Sarah Nolan, Catrin Tudur-Smith, Emma J. Boyland, Jo
A. Harrold, and Jason C. G. Halford (2015b), The Not So Clean Plate
Club: Food Self-Served Wont Always Result in Food Eaten,Interna-
tional Journal of Obesity, 39 (January), 376.
Robinson, Eric, Sarah Nolan, Catrin Tudur-Smith, Emma J. Boyland, Jo
A. Harrold, Charlotte A. Hardman, and Jason C. G. Halford (2014b),
Will Smaller Plates Lead to Smaller Waists? A Systematic Review and
Meta-Analysis of the Effect That Experimental Manipulation of Dish-
ware Size Has on Energy Consumption,Obesity Reviews, 15 (10),
81221.
Robinson, Eric, Florence Sheen, Jo Harrold, Emma Boyland, Jason Hal-
ford, and Una Masic (2015c) Dishware Size and Snack Food Intake
in a Between-Subjects Laboratory Experiment,Public Health Nutrition,
May (21), 15
Robinson, Thomas N., and Donna M. Matheson (2015), Environmental
Strategies for Portion Control in Children,Appetite, 88 (May), 3338.
Rolls, Barbara J., Liane S. Roe, Kitti H. Halverson, and Jennifer S. Meengs
(2007), Using a Smaller Plate Did Not Reduce Energy Intake at Meals,
Appetite, 49 (3), 65260.
Rolls, Barbara J., Liane S. Roe, Tanja V. E. Kral, Jennifer S. Meengs, and
Denise E. Wall (2004), Increasing the Portion Size of a Packaged Snack
Increases Energy Intake in Men and Women,Appetite, 42 (1), 639.
Rosenberg, Michael S. (2005), The File-Drawer Problem Revisited: A Gen-
eral Weighted Method for Calculating Fail-Safe Numbers in Meta-
Analysis,Evolution, 59 (2), 464 8.
Rosenthal, Robert (1979), The File Drawer Problemand the Tolerance
for Null Results,Psychological Bulletin, 86 (3), 638 41.
Shah, Meena, Rebecca Schroeder, Walker Winn, and Beverley Adams-Huet
(2011), A Pilot Study to Investigate the Effect of Plate Size on Meal
Energy Intake in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Women,
Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 24 (6), 61215.
Sharp, David, and Jeffery Sobal (2012), Using Plate Mapping to Exam-
ine Sensitivity to Plate Size in Food Portions and Meal Composition
among College Students,Appetite, 59 (3), 63945.
Sterne, Jonathan A. C., Alex J. Sutton, John P. A. Ioannidis, Norma Terrin,
David R. Jones, Joseph Lau, James Carpenter, Gerta Rücker, Roger M.
Harbord, Christopher H. Schmid, Jennifer Tetzlaff, Jonathan J. Deeks,
Jaime Peters, Petra Macaskill, Guido Schwarzer, Sue Duval, Douglas G.
Altman, David Moher, and Julian P. T. Higgins (2011), Recommenda-
tions for Examining and Interpreting Funnel Plot Asymmetry in Meta-
Volume 1 Number 1 2016 145
Analyses of Randomised Controlled Trials,British Medical Journal,
343 (July), d4002.
Szocs, Courtney, and Dipayan Biswas (2016), Forks over Spoons: The Im-
pact of Cutlery on Calorie Estimates,Journal of the Association for Con-
sumer Research, 1 (1), in this issue.
Van Ittersum, Koert, and Brian Wansink (2012), Plate Size and Color Sug-
gestibility: The Delboeuf Illusions Bias on Serving and Eating Behav-
ior,Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (2), 21528.
——— (2013), Extraverted Children Are More Biased by Bowl Sizes than
Introverts,PloS One, 8 (10), e78224.
Van Kleef, Ellen, Mitsuru Shimizu, and Brian Wansink (2012), Serving
Bowl Selection Biases the Amount of Food Served,Journal of Nutrition
Education and Behavior, 44 (January), 6670.
Wansink, Brian (1996), Can Package Size Accelerate Usage Volume?
Journal of Marketing, 60 (3), 114.
——— (2006), Mindless Eating: Why We Eat More than We Think, New
York: Bantam.
Wansink, Brian, and Matthew M. Cheney (2005), Super Bowls: Serving
Bowl Size and Food Consumption,JAMA: The Journal of the American
Medical Association, 293 (14), 1727.
Wansink, Brian, and K. A. Johnson (2015), The Clean Plate Club: About
92% of Self-Served Food Is Eaten,International Journal of Obesity,29
(2), 37174.
Wansink, Brian, and Junyong Kim (2005), Bad Popcorn in Big Buckets:
Portion Size Can Inuence Intake as Much as Taste,Journal of Nutri-
tion Education and Behavior, 37 (5), 24245.
Wansink, Brian, Collin Payne, and Carolina Werle (2008), Consequences
of Belonging to the Clean Plate Club,’” Archives of Pediatric and Adoles-
cent Medicine, 162 (10), 99495.
Wansink, Brian, and Koert Van Ittersum (2003), Bottoms Up! The Inu-
ence of Elongation on Pouring and Consumption Volume.Journal of
Consumer Research, 30 (December), 45563.
——— (2006), The Visual Illusions of Food: Why Plates, Bowls, and
Spoons Can Bias Consumption Volume.Journal of the Federation of
American Societies for Experimental Biology, 20: A618
——— (2013), Portion Size Me: Plate-Size Induced Consumption Norms
and Win-Win Solutions for Reducing Food Intake and Waste,Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19 (4), 320 32.
Wansink, Brian, Koert Van Ittersum, and James E. Painter (2006), Ice
Cream Illusions. Bowls, Spoons, and Self-Served Portion Sizes,Amer-
ican Journal of Preventative Medicine, 31 (3), 240 43.
Wansink, Brian, Koert Van Ittersum, and Collin R. Payne (2014), Larger
Bowl Size Increases the Amount of Cereal Children Request, Consume,
and Waste,Journal of Pediatrics, 164 (2), 32326.
Williamson, Sara, Lauren G. Block, and Punam A. Keller (2016), Of Waste
and Waists: The Effect of Plate Material on Food Consumption and
Waste,Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1 (1), in this
issue.
Yip, Wilson, Katy R. Wiessing, Stephanie Budgett, and Sally D. Poppitt (2013),
Using a Smaller Dining Plate Does Not Suppress Food Intake from
a Buffet Lunch Meal in Overweight, Unrestrained Women,Appetite,
69 October), 1027.
Young, Lisa R., and Marion Nestle (2002), The Contribution of Expanding
Portion Sizes to the US Obesity Epidemic,American Journal of Public
Health, 92 (2), 246 49.
Zlatevska, Natalina, Chris Dubelaar, and Stephen S. Holden (2014), Siz-
ing Up the Effect of Portion Size on Consumption: A Meta-Analytic
Review,Journal of Marketing, 78 (3), 140 54.
146 Smaller Plates Reduce Consumption Holden, Zlatevska, and Dubelaar
... Importantly, both interventions garnered high acceptance rates, suggesting that either of these intervention types is likely to result in low reactance among these particular cafeteria users. However, it's worth noting that reactance can potentially be further mitigated by increasing the quantity of side dishes to compensate for the reduction in meat (Holden et al., 2015), although our study did not encounter significant reactance in the cafeteria or during the survey phase. ...
Article
Overconsumption of meat remains common in Western diets. In a cafeteria setting, we assessed the impact of two interventions on reducing meat consumption: forced active choice (explicitly choosing meat portion size) and default nudge (offering a reduced meat portion with an opt-out option). Our field study, involving 5,966 food choices and feedback from 125 cafeteria users, revealed both interventions were effective and perceived as ethical. Across 11 meat dishes, both interventions increased the selection of reduced meat portions. Notably, the default nudge had a more substantial impact, with 90.6% opting for reduced portions compared to 38.5% for active choice. Interestingly, gender differences are most pronounced during the active choice phase and reduced through a default condition. We predict that in biased food choice environments where one option is clearly favored, differences in food choices influenced by socio-demographic factors are likely to diminish.
... A recent systematic review of nudge (choice architecture) interventions on meat choice and consumption found a paucity of studies conducted in real-life settings (Bianchi et al., 2018). This is problematic because there is evidence to suggest that effect sizes obtained in realworld settings may often be smaller when compared to those obtained in laboratory settings (see Holden et al., 2016;Long et al., 2015). Thus, there is a need for research probing the effectiveness of interventions to reduce meat consumption in real-life settings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Meat consumption has an adverse impact on both human and planetary health. To date, very few studies have examined the effectiveness of interventions tackling the overconsumption of meat in field settings. The present research addresses this gap by examining the impact of gain-framed labelling interventions communicating the adverse environmental consequences of meat consumption, using a multiple treatment reversal design across two university college dining halls over a period of five weeks. In College A the intervention weeks consisted of text-only or text-and-image labels communicating the adverse environmental consequences of meat consumption, and in College B patrons were exposed to either environmental or health labels (gain-framed; combining images and text). In total 13,869 (6,577 in College A and 7,292 in College B) meals (dishes) were analysed over the period of interest. Beta-binomial regressions found no statistically significant impact of the intervention periods compared to baseline on meat consumption in both College A and College B. The number of meal type options emerged as the only consistent predictor of meat consumption across models and across both colleges: meat consumption decreased with an increase in non-meat meal options. A post-study survey (College A: n = 88; College B: n = 53) revealed that patrons in both dining halls perceived environmental labels bearing both text and images as more informative and influential at changing behaviour compared to the other labelling interventions, although this did not translate into a change in behaviour. We discuss the implications of these findings for research, policy, and practice.
... Doing this sub-analysis, may partly explain some of the controversial results in previous studies. The effect of plate size depends on the study design (24). For example, using bowl or glass instead of plate, using snack instead of main meal, using laboratory condition instead of real setting, etc. (25). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Adolescence obesity and its health consequent on adulthood is affected by various environmental factors. Recently, dishware size or color is considered as a probable critical point to manage intake and weight. We investigated visual perception effects of plate size and color on fullness sensation regarding the Body Mass Index (BMI) for age percentiles in Tehran, Iran in 2019. Methods: A cross sectional study conducted on 408 students aged 13-18 year from both sexes with different socioeconomic status through convenience sampling method. Anthropometric characteristics were measured to determine BMI for Age categories based on the WHO protocol. Visual analog scale (VAS) was used to assess participants' perceived satiety when faced to images of different plate's size and color with the same food volume. SPSS 21 was used to statistical analyze. Results: 41.2% of student were on >85th percentile of BMI for age. Mean of score of VAS for medium red plate was significantly lower than medium and large white plates in both sexes (P<0.001), while mean of score of VAS for small white plate was significantly lower than large white plates only in girls (P<0.001). The adolescents who were on >85th percentile of BMI for age, gave higher score to large white plate than medium white plate and to medium white plate than medium red plate, significantly (P<0.001), while the scores given by the adolescent on 5th percentile of BMI, were not significant. Conclusion: Considering dramatic trend of obesity in adolescents in Iran, there is an urgent need to make supportive policies. Manipulating food containers seems to be an effective and simple approach to control feeling satiety. It may be a cost-effective approach to prevent obesity in adolescents before progressing to adulthood.
... Humans perceive there to be more food when it is served on a small rather than large plate (see Fig. 2; though see also (Rolls et al., 2007). Similarly, serving food in a rimless bowl can also make it look like there is more food than when exactly the same portion of food is served in a bowl with a rim instead (e.g., Holden et al., 2016;Petit et al., 2018). Relevant specifically to the case of pet food, researchers have demonstrated that untrained dogs can discriminate visually between bowls containing more versus less food (Araujo and Milgram, 2004;Horowitz et al., 2013;Prato-Previde et al., 2008); See also (Agnetta et al., 2000;Ward and Smuts, 2007). ...
Article
While the growing global obesity crisis in humans has attracted a great deal of attention from the media and healthcare professionals alike, the rapid increase in weight problems reported amongst pets is now attracting widespread recognition too. In humans, the emerging science of gastrophysics offers a number of concrete suggestions as to how people can be nudged into eating less by means of the enhanced multisensory design of both foods and the environments in which they choose to eat. In this narrative review, the potential relevance of gastrophysics to helping tackle the growing problem of overweight and obese domestic dogs is reviewed. This involves discussion of both the important similarities and difference in the way in which people and their pets perceive food, and the likely role of various product-extrinsic factors on consumption in the two cases. Nevertheless, despite the differences, a number of suggestions for future research are forwarded that may help to address the growing problem of overweight pets, and the behaviours that give rise to it.
... Meta-analyses exploring the effects of modified dishware sizes on portion selection and intakes are inconsistent due to a large amount of heterogeneity across studies (e.g., size and type of dishware items, main meal vs. non-main meal/snack food), finding no effect across age groups (15 comparisons; plates and bowls) [19] and small-to-moderate effects in adults (19 comparisons; plates, bowls, cutlery, or glasses) [20]. A broader meta-analysis involving 56 comparisons across age groups concluded that the effect of altered dishware depends on how the portion size is determined, whereby smaller plates only resulted in smaller meals when the portion was self-served [21]. More precisely, the included studies in this meta-analysis which assessed children show that larger plates and bowls resulted in larger self-served portions for unit entrées, amorphous entrées and a fruit side dish, but interestingly not for a vegetable side dish in a buffet-type line at school lunch in forty-two first-grade children [18]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: The influence of dishware on portion size perception in children and adolescents is inconclusive. This study investigated how children and adolescents with both obesity and a normal weight perceived portion size in different sized and shaped dishware items. Methods: The study included 60 children and adolescents with overweight and obesity (OBE) and 27 children and adolescents with normal weight (NW) aged from 9 to 17 years. The participants estimated quantities in three pairs of drinking glasses, one pair of bowls and two pairs of plates which varied in size and shape. The children were instructed to state intuitively which portion they would choose for big or small thirst/hunger. Thereafter they were asked to determine the exact amount by answering which dishware item contained the larger/smaller portion (cognitive evaluation). Results: There were no substantial differences in the intuitive evaluation of portion sizes between OBE and NW. During the cognitive evaluation, OBE estimated the amount of water in the glasses more correctly compared to NW (61% vs. 43%; p = 0.008); OBE estimated the amount of lentils in the bowls and on the plates significantly less correctly (39%) compared to NW (56%; p = 0.013). Conclusions: Habit formation and environmental stimuli might play a greater role in estimating food amounts in dishware than the child's and adolescent's body weight.
Article
Full-text available
The development of new dishes in the catering services market requires an understanding of consumers’ needs, expectations, and motivations for their choices. The effect of the serving method of a dessert on customers’ perceptions of its visual appeal, portion size, energy value, and expected price was evaluated. The study involved the presentation of desserts on plates of various sizes, shapes, and colors. The study was carried out among 1005 respondents using the CAWI method. Our findings revealed that along with an increasing plate size from a diameter of ϕ24–27 cm to ϕ31 cm, the ratings of the dish’s perceived appearance (p ≤ 0.001), portion size (p ≤ 0.001), and energy value (p ≤ 0.01) decreased. Plate shape influenced the perceived appearance of the dessert. When placed on a square platter, round desserts were considerably (p ≤ 0.05) less appealing. The color of the plate had a significant influence (p ≤ 0.001) on the dish’s perceived appearance and estimated monetary value, and it evoked more sensory–hedonic impressions. Red-plate and white-plate desserts were liked less than black-plate desserts, but color-plated desserts were perceived as more expensive than those served on white dishes. Consumers perceived bright desserts on white plates as traditional, natural, and boring; those on black plates as modern, appetizing, and aesthetic; and those served on red plates as artificial, unsightly, and unappetizing. Higher consumer food neophobia led to a lower rating related to appearance and price perceptions, but elevated perceptions of portion size and energy value appraisal. Our results may be used in the marketing of gastronomic dishes.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding digital menu choices in limited-option environments, such as university cafés, is crucial for promoting healthier and more sustainable food choices. We are, therefore, looking at two possible interventions or nudges—recommendation and position—and how they interact with, for example, price. In the first smartphone-based study (N = 517), participants were presented with two menu options, while the factors “recommendation”, “position”, and “price” were manipulated. We only found effects in relation to the choice of the more popular menu option. Specifically, when the popular meal was the expensive option, the recommendation had a negative effect on choice, but not when the popular meal was the cheaper option. The aim of the second smartphone-based study (N = 916) was to shed more light on the role of popularity or personal preference in relation to recommendations. We manipulated the differences in personal preference (small or large) using a ranking task presented before the menu choice. In Study 2, the interaction effect between recommendation and price for the more popular menu option could not be replicated. Instead, we found that the greater the difference in preference, the less pronounced the price effect was. Overall, some effects of the recommendations have been identified, but further research is needed to clarify the exact circumstances under which they arise.
Article
Full-text available
Portion control utensils and reduced size tableware amongst other tools, have the potential to guide portion size intake but their effectiveness remains controversial. This review evaluated the breadth and effectiveness of existing portion control tools on learning/awareness of appropriate portion sizes (PS), PS choice, and PS consumption. Additional outcomes were energy intake and weight loss. Published records between 2006–2020 (n = 1241) were identified from PubMed and WoS, and 36 publications comparing the impact of portion control tools on awareness (n = 7 studies), selection/choice (n = 14), intake plus related measures (n = 21) and weight status (n = 9) were analyzed. Non-tableware tools included cooking utensils, educational aids and computerized applications. Tableware included mostly reduced-size and portion control/calibrated crockery/cutlery. Overall, 55% of studies reported a significant impact of using a tool (typically smaller bowl, fork or glass; or calibrated plate). A meta-analysis of 28 articles confirmed an overall effect of tool on food intake (d = –0.22; 95%CI: –0.38, –0.06; 21 comparisons), mostly driven by combinations of reduced-size bowls and spoons decreasing serving sizes (d = –0.48; 95%CI: –0.72, –0.24; 8 comparisons) and consumed amounts/energy (d = –0.22; 95%CI: –0.39, –0.05, 9 comparisons), but not by reduced-size plates (d = –0.03; 95%CI: –0.12, 0.06, 7 comparisons). Portion control tools marginally induced weight loss (d = –0.20; 95%CI: –0.37, –0.03; 9 comparisons), especially driven by calibrated tableware. No impact was detected on PS awareness; however, few studies quantified this outcome. Specific portion control tools may be helpful as potentially effective instruments for inclusion as part of weight loss interventions. Reduced size plates per se may not be as effective as previously suggested.
Article
Full-text available
This research shows when and how food-related warnings can backfire by putting consumers in a state of reactance. Across three studies, we demonstrate that dieters (but not nondieters) who see a one-sided message fo-cusing on the negative aspects of unhealthy food (vs. a one-sided positive or neutral message) increase their desire for and consumption of unhealthy foods. In contrast, dieters who see a two-sided message (focusing on both the negative and positive aspects of unhealthy food) are more likely to comply with the message, thereby choosing fewer unhealthy foods. Our research suggests that negatively worded food warnings (such as public service announcements) are unlikely to work—nondieters ignore them, and dieters do the opposite. Although preliminary, our findings also suggest that two-sided messages may offer a better solution.
Article
Full-text available
A retrospective study of kids' meals purchased at Walt Disney World was conducted to determine acceptance rates for healthy sides and beverages. Purchase data from all 145 Walt Disney World restaurants were analyzed using a log-linear model and a Poisson regression. Across all restaurants, 47.9% and 66.3% of guests accepted healthy default sides and beverages, respectively. Acceptance rates of sides and beverages were higher at quick-service restaurants (49.4% and 67.8%, respectively) compared to table-service restaurants (40.3% and 45.6%, respectively). The healthy defaults reduced calories (21.4%), fat (43.9%), and sodium (43.4%) for kids' meal sides and beverages. This study contributes by examining the use of kids' meal healthy defaults in quick-service and table-service restaurant formats at the world's largest theme park, a previously unstudied setting, and by providing the largest ever healthy default data set. The results suggest that healthy defaults can shift food and beverage selection patterns toward healthier options.
Article
Full-text available
Laboratory paradigms are commonly used to study human energy intake. However, the extent to which participants believe their eating behavior is being measured may affect energy intake and is a methodologic factor that has received little consideration. Our main objective was to examine available evidence for the effect that heightened awareness of observation has on energy intake in a laboratory setting. We systematically reviewed laboratory studies that allowed for experimental examination of the effect that heightened awareness of observation has on energy intake. From these experimental studies we combined effect estimates using inverse variance meta-analysis, calculating the standardized mean difference (SMD) in energy intake between heightened-awareness and control conditions and qualitatively synthesized potential moderators of this effect. Nine studies, providing 22 comparisons, were eligible for inclusion. These studies largely sampled young women and examined the energy intake of energy-dense snack foods. Evidence indicated that heightened awareness of observation was associated with reduced energy intake when compared with the control condition (random-effects SMD: 0.45; 95% CI: 0.25, 0.66; P < 0.0001). We found little evidence that the type of experimental manipulation used to heighten awareness moderated the overall effect. The available evidence to date suggests that heightened awareness of observation reduces energy intake in a laboratory setting. These findings suggest that laboratory studies should attempt to minimize the degree to which participants are aware that their eating behavior is being measured. © 2015 American Society for Nutrition.
Article
Full-text available
The use of smaller dishware as a way of reducing food consumption has intuitive appeal and is recommended to the general public. Recent experimental studies have failed to find an effect of plate size on food intake, although the methods used across studies have varied. The aim of the present study was to examine the effect that bowl size had on snack food consumption in a 'typical' snacking context (snacking while watching television). Between-subjects. Laboratory experiment. Sixty-one adult participants served themselves and ate popcorn while watching television. Participants were randomly assigned to serve themselves with and eat from either a small or a large bowl. The use of a smaller bowl size did not reduce food consumption. Unexpectedly, participants in the small bowl condition tended to consume more popcorn (34·0 g) than participants in the large bowl condition (24·9 g; 37 % increase, d=0·5), although the statistical significance of this difference depended on whether analyses were adjusted to account for participant characteristics (e.g. gender) associated with food intake (P=0·02) or not (P=0·07). Counter to widely held belief, the use of a smaller bowl did not reduce snack food intake. Public health recommendations advising the use of smaller dishware to reduce food consumption are premature, as this strategy may not be effective.
Article
Full-text available
We replicate the research of Do Vale et al. (2008) and Scott et al. (2008) showing that the diet-conscious tend to eat more when a portion is broken into multiple smaller partitions than when it is unpartitioned. The results show the partitioning paradox is clearer when diet-consciousness is manipulated than measured. A meta-analysis reveals that the partitioning paradox among the diet-conscious is a medium size effect, but also that partitioning has an opposite and equal size effect on the non-diet conscious: they eat more from the unpartitioned than the partitioned package.
Article
Two lab studies and three field studies indicate that plate disposability influences the amount of food wasted. The first lab study indicates that more food served on disposable (i.e., paper) plates is wasted than when the same food is served on permanent (i.e., hard plastic) plates. Study 2 employs an implicit association test (IAT) to confirm that disposable plates are strongly associated with behavior to stop eating while, simultaneously, permanent plates are more strongly associated with a keep eating behavior. Three field studies (3A, 3B, and 3C) indicate that this effect maintains when instead of being served a fixed quantity of food, participants select the amount and type of food.
Article
People compensate for small food-unit sizes by eating more units compared to regular-sized units, but the aggregate of calories people consume of smaller versus regular units is still less because each unit consumed increases perceptions of overindulgence and impulsivity. This suggests that if perceptions of a food unit’s smallness could be disrupted, people may not need to compensate, resulting in a further reduction in aggregate food chosen and consumed. In a lab and field experiment, people took the fewest calories when presented with smaller versus regular-sized pizza slices (i.e., from the same pizza pie diameter) placed on a larger table that distracted their attention away from the smallness of the pizza slices. We show that unit-size effects can be altered by food frame-size mechanisms like table diameter.
Article
Forks and spoons are present at nearly every meal in Western societies, and many foods can be appropriately consumed with either type of cutlery. We focus on foods that can be appropriately consumed with either a fork or a spoon and examine how eating with one piece of cutlery (vs. the other) influences consumers’ calorie estimates and consumption decisions. Holding bite size constant, we find that eating with a spoon (vs. a fork) leads consumers to estimate the number of calories in the food as being lower and also desire a greater volume of the food. The effect of cutlery on calories is attenuated when consumers focus on the oral sensations they experience while eating, as well as when foods do not adhere to the cutlery surface. Overall, our findings suggest that eating with a fork might be one way to encourage healthful consumption.