Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Scientific Reports
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Mechanisms of tactile sensory
deterioration amongst the elderly
Lisa Skedung1, Charles El Rawadi2, Martin Arvidsson1, Céline Farcet2, Gustavo S. Luengo
2,
Lionel Breton2 & Mark W. Rutland
1,3
It is known that roughness-smoothness, hardness-softness, stickiness-slipperiness and warm-cold
are predominant perceptual dimensions in macro-, micro- and nano- texture perception. However, it
is not clear to what extent active tactile texture discrimination remains intact with age. The general
decrease in tactile ability induces physical and emotional dysfunction in elderly, and has increasing
signicance for an aging population. We report a method to quantify tactile acuity based on blinded
active exploration of systematically varying micro-textured surfaces and a same-dierent paradigm.
It reveals that elderly participants show signicantly reduced ne texture discrimination ability.
The elderly group also displays statistically lower nger friction coecient, moisture and elasticity,
suggesting a link. However, a subpopulation of the elderly retains discrimination ability irrespective
of cutaneous condition and this can be related to a higher density of somatosensory receptors on the
nger pads. Skin tribology is thus not the primary reason for decline of tactile discrimination with
age. The remediation of cutaneous properties through rehydration, however leads to a signicantly
improved tactile acuity. This indicates unambiguously that neurological tactile loss can be temporarily
compensated by restoring the cutaneous contact mechanics. Such mechanical restoration of tactile
ability has the potential to increase the quality of life in elderly.
In common only with taste, touch is based on the intimate contact of a body surface with a material and as with
all senses, tactile perceptual ability declines with age1–4. Human hands and ngers are used for active exploration
of our surroundings as well as for grasping objects5,6 and are our primary “tactile probes”; ngertip interactions
are thus highly relevant for understanding tactile perception.
e so-called “sensorial” evaluation of touch (the association of sensations with subjective descriptions) is
a well-established eld7 and is paramount when communicating about touch; for example in consumer panel
studies8,9. Although the importance of physical quantities such as roughness, elastic modulus etc. is commonly
accepted, a clear-cut understanding of their individual impact on perception is lacking since it is dicult to vary
one parameter independently10–13. ere is an increased interest recently on methods based on stimuli detec-
tion; such psycho-physical techniques provide a complementary approach and rely on objective tests, such as
whether a dierence can be detected14. In general, such approaches provide quantitative data that can be com-
pared to, and ideally correlated with, individual physical quantities or combinations thereof. Skedung et al.15
observed, for example, how the friction coecient diminished with increased average roughness on printing
papers and explored the perceived similarities of calibrated wrinkled surfaces of dierent wavelengths and ampli-
tudes10. Friction coecient and pattern wavelength were directly linked to the generated tactile space and it
was also found that amplitudes as small as 15 nm could be distinguished from blank surfaces. Furthermore,
although the nger friction coecient is the physical parameter which varies, the applied load is unconsciously
regulated to maintain an optimal friction force10,15. us it is likely that from a perceptual perspective it is the
applied load which is monitored and registered. It is known that roughness-smoothness, hardness-soness and
stickiness-slipperiness are predominant perceptual (as opposed to physical) dimensions in macro texture per-
ception14,16–18. is perceptual dimensionality remains intact when the texture scale is reduced to the micro- and
nanoscale19. It is not clear whether the tactile discrimination ability for these textures remains intact with age.
Skin can be considered a biocomposite material of multiple layers. Consideration of its structure is essential
for understanding the generation and transmission of vibrations across the tissue, which are detected by the
cutaneous receptors underpinning our somatosensory system5,20 and may well be amplied by ngerprints21. e
1RISE Research Institutes of Sweden, Bioscience and Materials, Stockholm, SE-114 28, Sweden. 2L’Oréal Research
and Innovation, Aulnay-sous-Bois, 93600, France. 3KTH Royal Institute of Technology, Surface and Corrosion
Science, Stockholm, SE-100 44, Sweden. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to G.S.L.
(email: gluengo@rd.loreal.com) or M.W.R. (email: mark@kth.se)
Received: 5 December 2017
Accepted: 14 March 2018
Published: xx xx xxxx
OPEN
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
inuence of the dermis, epidermis and subcutaneous tissue has been widely studied in relation to the induced
deformation and the friction observed22–24. While less studied, there is increasing evidence of the importance of
the harder stratum corneum layer for friction25. Friction forces depend on the real area of contact, and thus the
local deformability of the stratum corneum properties impacts this parameter19,26. Lévêque et al.3 for example,
investigated how the hydration of this layer inuences its elasticity and demonstrated improved, static, cheek
spatial acuity using the two-point discrimination gap method. Other attributes of the skin surface are also known
to play a role in skin friction27, such as the micro relief12, or the presence of sweat and sebum25.
Life expectancy is increasing28, yet few studies have explored the limits of tactile perception in the elderly. Age
is known to aect both the mechanical and physical properties of the skin as well as neurophysiological capabil-
ities for the detection, transmission or interpretation of touch signals1,3,29–32. e situation is complicated by the
fact that the 2-point threshold approach used in many studies addresses static touch, but there is evidence that
dynamic touch is aected dierently33. e relative extent to which the two potential contributions (mechanical
or neural) impact sensory perception is unknown. In the correction of sensory decline in the senses of sight and
hearing, however, which also deteriorate with age in a well-documented fashion34,35, the mechanical, rather than
the neurological deciency, is systematically addressed: Spectacles correct for the deformation of the cornea and
hearing aids amplify audio signals. us the questions can be posed as to whether i) there is a similar reduc-
tion in tactile sensory discrimination of ne texture with increasing age, ii) this deterioration can be attributed
to age related changes in skin mechanical properties and iii) such mechanical deciencies can be analogously
remediated.
An earlier technical protocol using wrinkled surfaces10,19 has thus been adapted to study in detail the changes
in tactile perception ability that occur at advanced age using active touch on ne textures. e abilities of young
and elderly participants to haptically discriminate between dierent, well dened patterned textures have been
quantied, and compared to physical properties of the skin (nger friction, nger hydration and elasticity).
Results and Discussion
Lower tactile discrimination ability in the elderly group. Six systematic textures varying in surface
pattern wavelength have been fabricated by an established method10. ey were used in a tactile perception test
where the task was to judge whether a presented surface was perceived as the same or dierent to a reference sur-
face. e nominal wavelengths of the test surfaces used in this same-dierent tactile perception test were 20 µm,
40 µm, 60 µm, 80 µm (denoted S20, S40, S60 and S80, respectively), 100 µm (Ref100) as well as a blank, smooth
surface with no systematic texture (S0). Ref 100 is closest to the limit of what can be considered “ne texture”19. A
pilot study with 10 elderly and 10 young female subjects indicated that the young group hit a threshold at 60 µm
i.e., that the diculty of the task increased signicantly for this wavelength and that S60 could not reliably be
distinguished from Ref100. e elderly group had a much lower rate of successful task completion even at 20 µm.
e results below are obtained for a larger study consisting of 30 “young” (19–25 y) and 30 elderly female subjects
(67–85 y), none of whom participated in the pilot. Six repeated tactile perception comparisons to the reference
were performed for each test surface and participant, and were presented and evaluated in a unique randomized
order (in total 36 comparisons in each perception test).
e averaged results are displayed in Fig.1 as percentage of correct responses as box plots, showing the inter-
quartile range IQR (25th and 75th quartile = 50% of the data) with the mean (square), median (line), whiskers
Figure 1. Tactile discrimination ability for the two groups. (a) Proportion of correct answers in identifying
whether the stimulus was dierent to the reference sample Ref100 (100 µm in wrinkle wavelength). e
diculty of the task clearly increases for both groups as the wavelength of the stimulus texture approaches that
of the reference. e “break point” (dened as when the success rate falls below 80%) is seen for S20 (20 µm) for
the elderly group and for S60 (60 µm) for the young group (N = 30 for both groups). (b) Box plot of the means
from each participant in the two groups (N = 2 × 30), showing the tactile discrimination ability of S20 versus
Ref100 as well as the correct responses for Ref100 versus itself (meaning “same” as the correct answer). Since the
young and the elderly groups have a very similar proportion of correct responses for the latter task, the tendency
to guess “dierent” is the same for both groups when the task is perceived as dicult.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
3
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
indicating variability within 1.5 IQR, as well as outliers. An answer was held to be correct when the test surface
was judged dierent from the reference or when the reference was judged the same when presented against itself.
e smooth surface has “perfect” scores of 100% and 99% for the young and elderly, respectively, indicating that it
was easily detected as dierent from Ref100. is can be attributed to the greater real contact area between surface
and nger during tactile interaction compared with all the other surfaces and a correspondingly increased fric-
tion coecient22. e young group successfully dierentiated S20 and S40 from Ref100 (average correct response
of 97% and 89%, respectively), and the drop in tactile discrimination ability was seen with S60 (55%). S80 is
even closer to the reference wavelength of 100 µm and not unexpectedly was even harder to distinguish (34%).
e drop in discrimination ability for the elderly group is already observed for S20 where the number of correct
responses is 63% and therefore below the 80% criterion for high performance (see below).
For the comparison of Ref100 with itself, i.e. where “same” is the correct answer, the young and elderly groups
showed a similar correct response level of 79% and 76%, respectively. e fact that this score is not 100% indicates
a tendency to answer “dierent” when the participant is uncertain. is tendency was thus factored into the deci-
sion to set the successful detection criterion to 80% correct responses. It is the same for both groups and should
not represent a bias in the group comparison.
e tactile discrimination ability (S20) was signicantly lower for the elderly group compared to the young.
For the young group, the mean value was 97%, with a standard deviation (s.d.) of 17%. For the elderly group,
the corresponding values were mean = 63%, s.d. = 48% (F(1,58) = 26.65, p < 0.001). Only two individuals in the
young group failed at reliably distinguishing S20 from Ref100, whereas 17 among the elderly failed the same
task. For S60 and S80 both groups showed diculties in perceiving dierences to the reference (<80% correct
responses).
It is thus abundantly clear that dynamic touch on ne textures exhibits an age-related decline in discrimination
ability which may well be related to the decrease in tactile discrimination sensitivity with age reported for static
touch1–4 and object discrimination3,36. e physical dimensions underpinning the active touch discrimination of
ne textures have previously been identied10 as being associated with the friction coecient (and the resulting
nger loading15), and the wavelength of the surfaces. It seems likely that the former depends on responses from
slow adapting receptors (deformation) whereas the latter depends on vibrations detected by the fast adapting
Pacinian receptors37,38. It would thus seem logical to start with frictional, or“biotribological”, studies to identify
the cause of the active touch deterioration. Skin biomechanics aect these properties strongly, and there may be
a direct link to the trends seen for static touch, which is also highly dependent on biomechanical properties. is
has been done under an ensuing heading.
Before leaving the same-difference test however, it remains to identify an additional important finding.
Individual examination of the correct response distribution of all 60 participants (for S20) in Fig.2, clearly reveals
that a signicant fraction of the elderly group displays unimpaired ability. is bimodal distribution needs to be
considered in future studies of age-related perception decline. e retention of an active touch tactile acuity has
earlier been reported in aged, blind individuals39, possibly indicating that retention is associated with use, though
this speculation is beyond the scope of this work. Using the criterion of 80% success as the demarcation between
successful and unsuccessful tactile discrimination ability, the elderly participants could be divided into a high
performing sub-group (13 individuals) and a low performing sub-group (17). us approximately 43% of elderly
group performed at the same level as the young. A direct implication of this observation is that future studies
aimed at identifying means to improve age related texture discrimination should address this distribution and
adopt an appropriate recruitment strategy.
Bio-mechanical and bio-tribological dierences in young and aged skin. Figure3 illustrates dif-
ferences in nger elasticity, nger hydration and tactile friction between the young and elderly groups. A one-way
Figure 2. Not all elderly subjects show a decrease in tactile discrimination ability. Average correct responses
for S20 of all 60 subjects, showing that 13/30 from the elderly group perform at the same level as the young. e
criterion of 80% correct responses is used to separate high and low performers. Age is plotted on the abscissa as
a convenient means to separate the two groups.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
4
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
ANOVA indicates that the nger hydration level is signicantly lower among the elderly group compared to the
young (young: mean = 66 a.u., s.d. = 23 a.u.; elderly: mean = 36 a.u., s.d. = 14 a.u.) (F(1,59) = 36.14, p < 0.001).
Skin elasticity is determined by extracting various parameters from measurements of skin deformation as a func-
tion of time. e supporting information provides details of the technique (see also Fig.S1 and TableS1) whereas
Fig.S2 shows how the parameters vary between the two groups. e elastic recovery parameter is here used as the
measure of nger elasticity since it has earlier been identied as the relevant parameter for describing age related
changes40–42. Note, that it is not an estimate of elastic modulus, but rather the reversibility of applied deforma-
tions; this (unitless) nger elasticity is signicantly lower in elderly skin (young: mean = 0.54, s.d. = 0.12; elderly:
mean = 0.34, s.d. = 0.08) (F(1,54) = 49.16, p < 0.001). Aged skin is normally reported as drier and less elastic than
young skin42–44.
Friction between a nger and the textured surfaces, (“tactile friction”)10,15,19 was measured in a continuous
reciprocating manner on surfaces S20, S60 and Ref100 (see Fig.3d). e resulting friction coecients (here, the
ratio between friction force and applied load) are shown in Fig.3c. A two-way repeated ANOVA was used to
assess the signicance of dierences between the average tactile friction coecients on the three surfaces and
the two age groups (N = 2*29). A Tukey post hoc test shows that the young group possesses a signicantly higher
tactile friction coecient on all three surfaces (p < 0.001). e large standard deviations reect the well-known
individual spread in tactile friction between individuals15,19,23, and consequently, no signicant dierences are
observed between the three surfaces at group level. However individual dierences indicate that the young vol-
unteers show a slightly greater dierence in tactile friction coecient between surfaces S20 and S60 compared
to the elderly volunteers. In both groups, the majority show highest tactile friction coecient on the smallest
wavelength surface (59% of the elderly and 69% of the young). e higher friction level of the young volunteers
is almost certainly an eect of the higher nger hydration level. Such a relation between skin hydration and skin
friction is well reported in the literature19,23,45.
As statedin the introduction, the friction coecient is unconsciously used to moderate the applied load (how
hard the nger is pressed) and this may well be the perceptual prompt. e elderly do indeed press somewhat
harder as a result of the lower friction coecients experienced though the variation at the individual level is large)
and the data is displayed in the SI. (Note that the load is not recorded during perceptual experiments, only during
tactile friction measurements).
In order to evaluate a possible link between hydration and tactile perception ability, individual correct
responses from all volunteers for S20 are plotted in Fig.4 versus the individual nger hydration value. As can
be seen, all the young participants return above 80% correct responses and the elderly participants are scattered
Figure 3. e groups display signicantly dierent bio-mechanical and bio-tribological values. e elderly
group display (a) lower nger hydration (b) lower nger elasticity and (c) lower tactile friction coecients
obtained by the continuous recording of friction force and applied load upon interaction using a (d)
ForceBoard.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
5
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
over the entire possible interval from 0% to 100% correct responses. An interesting observation is that above a
hydration level of 50 a.u, an almost exclusively high performance is observed, whereas at lower levels of hydra-
tion, both high (>80%) and low (<80%) performers are found. A similar plot is obtained with nger elasticity,
(not shown) but as can be seen in Fig.4b, nger elasticity is clearly related to the nger hydration level. ese
parameters would thus appear to be degenerate and indistinguishable in terms of their intrinsic relevance. From
a contact mechanics perspective, it is the elastic modulus that aects the true area of contact and thus the friction
coecient. Skin is a multilayer structure and the macroscopic deformation of the nger corresponds to an elastic
modulus on the scale of kPa. is is eectively maximised at very low loads, below 1 N. e friction coecient,
and the area of contact are dependent on local deformations of the Stratum Corneum (SC)26 which has a much
higher elastic modulus (ca 0.005–0.1 GPa)46As mentioned before, the elasticity parameter is not a direct measure
of the elastic modulus, but the elastic modulus of the SC also has a strong, inverse, dependence on the moisture
content (as well as the scale of the deformation)46,47, so the parameters are unambiguously linked. At very high
moisture contents the SC also exhibits plastic deformations which lead to even higher contact areas and friction
coecients, and also lower elastic reversibility – though this depends strongly on how the parameters are dened
(see SI). In addition, at higher humidity/moisture content there is the possibility for capillary condensation48 and/
or occlusion45 of liquid water in the contact. is also contributes to increased friction. e role of moisture on
friction is further discussed in the next section.
Clearly, a hypothesis to test is whether an increase or improvement in nger hydration could improve the
dynamic tactile discrimination ability in the elderly group, analogously to observations for static touch, where the
application of a moisturizer containing 5% of glycerol increased both the hydration and static touch acuity3. us,
to evaluate a possible improvement in tactile discrimination and changes in cutaneous properties, the group of 30
elderly subjects were divided into a high performing (N = 13) and a low performing (N = 17) sub-group.
Improvement in tactile ability. Two dierent humectants containing 5% and 7% of the common mois-
turizer glycerol were applied to the same index nger used in the perception test, on two dierent days. In the
treated state, S20 and Ref100 were repeatedly evaluated. As a check, the high performing group (N = 13) were
still high performing aer application of 5% glycerol. As can be seen in Fig.5a, the ability to discriminate S20
signicantly improved for the low performing group (F = 15.911, p = 0.001 for 5% and F = 18:346, p < 0.001 for
7%) aer application of humectant to the nger. e interpretation is further strengthened by the fact that the
ability to correctly identify Ref100 increased as well, indicating a higher degree of condence in the assessment.
is known humectant also improves the nger hydration of the elderly in the low performing group as can be
seen in Fig.5b. ere is a signicant increase in both nger hydration and nger elasticity that could explain the
increased ability to perceptually distinguish S20 and Ref100. Previous work has indicated that two perceptual
mechanisms seem to govern perception, where sticky/slippery has been shown to be the main dimension and
can be related to the friction coecient10,19. It has further been established that nger hydration is a major con-
tributor to the individual variations in the absolute values of the friction coecient19. ese results thus clearly
indicate that hydration and elasticity play a role in the restoration of dynamic tactile discrimination ability of ne
texture analogously to static touch on the arm3, lips49, and index nger50. e sticky/slippery perception has been
addressed by the instrumental measurements of tactile friction. A signicant dierence between the tactile fric-
tion coecients measured in untreated and treated states is observed. In the untreated state, no dierence at the
group level between S20 and Ref100 was noted. However, in the treated state a signicant dierence between the
two surfaces was obtained (F(1,16) = 4.261, p = 0.008 for 5%, and F(1,16) = 4.866, p = 0.003 for 7%), where S20
displays the higher tactile friction. ese observations indicate that the improvement in active tactile ability in
the low performing group could be related to an improvement in both nger hydration and nger elasticity. is
in turn allows the subjects to distinguish the surfaces based on a sticky/slippery perception10,19. e improvement
eect appears to be both immediate and reversible since a new “untreated” perception test performed one day
Figure 4. Biomechanical properties and tactile ability. (a) Scatter plot showing the correct responses on S20
(perceiving S20 as dierent from Ref100). ere is improvement potential for the subjects below 80% correct
responses. (b) Finger elasticity appears to increase with increasing hydration.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
6
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
post application shows that both cutaneous parameter levels and tactile discrimination ability return to the same
level as before humectant application.
Hydration is known to modify the properties of the stratum corneum51–53 and in part is responsible for a sof-
tening thereof. Such deformations in the stratum corneum facilitate pressure stress transmission to the subsurface
somatosensory system (touch corpuscles). Friction is also known to increase with the moisture level mainly due
to the contribution of skin deformation, in particular at higher pressures. Note that there is no information as to
how the topography of the nger print ridges varies with age and between the two elderly groups, but there are
no publications indicating bimodal distributions of topography amongst the elderly. Even if topography were to
vary between any of the groups, it would manifest via the nger friction parameter and would thus eectively be
degenerate with the tribological properties.
e simultaneous improvement in the tactile perception discrimination ability and cutaneous parameters
upon treatment with glycerol together with the observed dierences between young and elderly groups in the dry
state indicate that the skin state is important for active tactile perception ability. A comparison of the high and low
performing elderly sub-groups in the untreated state however, shows no signicant dierences in biomechanical
or biotribological properties, raising interesting questions as to the mechanisms of the decline in tactile acuity,
or rather its retention by the high performers. One possibility is that the high performing elderly sub-group has
developed a greater (compensating) sensitivity towards another perceptual prompt – for example the vibrations
detected in the Pacinian corpuscles37,38. e vibration occurs as the nger traverses the periodic structures and
its value depends on the wavelength. e sensitivity to these vibrations decreases as the vibration frequency
diverges from the optimal sensing frequency of the Pacinian corpuscles37,38. e fact that there is no dierence in
the biomechanical properties of the skin indicates that vibration transmission is unlikely to be the dierence. e
question however remains as to why two populations exist and this implicates an additional, neural, contribution
to age-related loss of tactile perception. is could, for example, be related to a decrease in density of peripheral
nerves and mechanoreceptors in the ngertip, or their ability to transmit signals. Based on the data above it is
Figure 5. Improvement in tactile discrimination ability with increased nger hydration. Data is shown only for
the low performing elderly group (a) Improvement in correct responses measured 30 min aer application of
humectants containing 5% and 7% glycerol (applied on two dierent days). (b) As expected, both humectants
increase the nger hydration level. (c) Finger elasticity is analogously increased. (d) Greater dierences in
tactile friction between S20 and Ref100 in the treated nger state that could explain the increase in tactile
discrimination ability based on greater dierences in sticky/slippery feel. Note that when humectant was
applied, the physical parameters were measured both before and aer. us there are “untreated” data which are
nonetheless labelled as (5%) and (7%) according to the respective, subsequent treatment.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
7
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
dicult to draw more conclusions. us, a follow up experiment was performed. A new, larger population of
elderly participants were screened using the same perceptual discrimination experiment. Two elderly groups
were selected (30 each) based on identication as poor performing (in this case less than 60% success) or high
performing (>80% success). e density of Meissner Corpuscles (MC) was measured using a microscope as a
simple, non-invasive measurement of the somatosensory condition. (Meissner Corpuscles are generally held to
be the most important of the mechanoreceptors used in touch and pressure on glabrous skin54). It was found that
the higher performing group had a statistically higher MC density (see Fig.S8). e average was 2.67 mm−2 with
a standard deviation of 0.89 mm−2. For the lower performing elderly group the average was 1.35 mm−2 with SD
0.70 mm−2. us the lower performance of this group can be condently linked to a neural decline, though we
stress that we do not link the reduced acuity directly to the lower MC density, since there is no data on the other
tactile receptors.
Concluding remarks. ere is a clear, statistically signicant reduction in the ne texture discrimination
with active touch amongst the elderly. Identifying such decline quantitatively is not trivial, but the test presented
here, provides a straightforward approach. Not only can the deterioration be measured, but its remediation by
training or treatment can also easily be assessed. e reduction in acuity amongst the elderly is matched by a
reduction in the biomechanical properties of the skin – moisture, elasticity and nger friction coecient are all
signicantly reduced. e nger friction is now known as an important perceptual identier for ne texture dis-
crimination so this provides a clear pathway for amelioration of the reduced tactile perception.
e observation of that a large proportion of the elderly retain their active touch ne texture discrimination
acuity has several important ramications. At the simplest level, it means that future studies aimed at remediating
tactile discrimination need to take this into account. More importantly, this observation casts signicant light on
the mechanism of the decline in acuity. Since the biomechanical decline of the skin properties is indistinguishable
for these two populations, the reduced performance cannot be associated directly with the age related changes
in elasticity, moisture and friction. is almost certainly rules out the sticky-slippery perceptual dimension as
the prompt for the performance. Another dimension is associated with vibrations detected by Pacinian and/
or Meissner Corpuscles. Since the mechanical skin properties in the two groups are comparable, the vibration
transmission itself can be cautiously ruled out, which strongly implies that the dierence between the two groups
has a neural decline associated with receptor sensitivity, density, or signal transmission. A reduced density of
receptors has clearly been shown for the poor performers. us, age-related decline in neural properties is the
primary explanation for the reduction in active touch acuity. Nonetheless, an improvement in the biomechanical
properties of the skin through rehydration, for example as performed here, means that the loss in discrimination
in the one dimension, can be signicantly ameliorated by an improvement in tactile discrimination ability in the
other, friction based, dimension.
Methods
Wrinkle-patterned model surfaces. Six wrinkled surfaces were used in the initial perceptual test and
were manufactured in-house based on a surface wrinkling technique presented earlier10,55. They consist of
UV-curable adhesive polymer NOA81 (Norland Optical Adhesive 81, Norland Products Inc., Cranbury, USA)
and were templated from sinusoidal textures imposed on a PDMS surface (polydimethoxysiloxane, Sylgard 184
Dow Corning, USA), with nominal wavelengths according to previous recipes19 ~20 µm (S20), ~40 µm (S40),
~60 µm (S60), ~80 µm (S80) and ~100 µm (Ref100). A blank reference surface was replicated against smooth,
unwrinkled PDMS. See FigsS3 and S4 prolometry results.
Tactile discrimination test. A same-dierent paradigm was used, i.e. the task was to judge whether two
surfaces were perceived identical or not. e method of constant stimuli was employed for the presentation
of stimuli, i.e. the participants were always presented with Ref100 rst, followed by the test stimuli and were
instructed to judge whether the second surface was perceived as the same as or dierent from the reference. Each
surface was compared with the reference surface six times in a randomized order, resulting in 36 comparisons in
total for each participant in the untreated state. e instruction was to stroke the surface back and forth with the
index nger of the dominant hand. e perception test was performed blind-folded. All subjects were allowed to
practice the test procedure on the same pairs of surfaces prior to the test. e surfaces were cleaned with acetone
both during and aer each test series.
Subjects and experimental conditions. 30 elderly women (mean: 73 ± 4.5 years; range 67–85) and 30
young women (mean: 22 ± 1.5 years; range 19–25) participated in the study. Each participant had a unique ran-
domized order of surface presentation for perception tests and tactile friction measurements. e study was per-
formed in ambient indoor air, where the mean temperature was 23 ± 1.3°C and the relative humidity 53 ± 8.7%.
Inclusion criteria, ethics and guidelines. e inclusion criteria were addressed during the recruitment
process, and were also checked at the start of the experimental session. None of the participants had any skin
disease, neurological disease, diabetes or were allergic to cosmetic products. None of the participants were preg-
nant or had been breastfeeding the last six months. Signed, informed consent was given and the subjects were
informed that they could quit the experiment at any time if they so wished. All procedures were performed in
accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments of ethical stand-
ards regarding studies with human participants. e protocols are as for reference10, and were approved by the
Research Committee of the Institute for Surface Chemistry and the Ethics Committee of Stockholm University.
Cutaneous measurements. Cutaneous status or biomechanical properties were measured by means of dif-
ferent probes from Courage & Khazaka Electronic GmbH (Cologne, Germany). Finger hydration was measured
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
8
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
with a Corneometer CM 825 in arbitrary units (a.u.), based on electrical capacitance. Each individual moisture
value is an average (mean ± standard deviation) of ve repeated measurements on the index ngertip of the
dominant hand. Finger elasticity was measured on the index nger of the dominant hand with a Cutometer MPA
580 by measuring the vertical deformation of the skin when pulled into a 2 mm diameter probe with an optical
sensor. Each measurement consisted of three suction cycles of 2 s using a constant negative pressure of 450 mbar,
followed by a 2 s period when the pressure was switched o (relaxation phase) allowing the skin to return to its
original shape. e elastic recovery parameter (also called net elasticity), R5 (Ur/Ue) is used to represent nger
elasticity in this work obtained, as it has been identied as a suitable parameter for comparing dierence between
young and aged skin41. e dierent parameters from the time/strain curves (elasticity curves) are described in
supplementary information, the parameters Ue and Ur refer to the linear elastic response on deformation and
relaxation respectively.
Tactile friction measurements. Finger friction (or tactile friction) was measured with a ForceBoard
(Industrial Dynamics AB, Sweden), a universal friction and force tester equipped with one horizontal and one
tangential load cell, individually connected to the same plate of assembly. Upon interaction, a mechanical load is
converted into voltage signals that are amplied and proportional to the applied load in N. e tangential force
(friction force), and vertical force (applied load) were continuously recorded using DAQFactory soware at a rate
of 100 Hz as a nger was moved over the surfaces. Friction coecients were calculated at each data point as the
ratio of the friction force and applied load the average dynamic friction coecients of ten stroking cycles were
calculated and compared19. (Note that here “friction coecient” refers to the macroscopic interaction of the entire
nger-textured surface contact, rather than the local, “asperity contact” shear stress which should be invariant
since the surfaces are all the same material). e surfaces S20, S60 and Ref100 were measure in triplicate in each
experimental session (randomized order). e participants were instructed to use their preferred load, speed and
angle of the index nger the same for all the measurements. e surfaces were measured in randomized order.
Tests after application of a humectant. To assess the eects of skin remediation on perception, the 30
elderly participants performed a second perception test aer application of a humectant. Two dierent humectant
systems, were used in which the active moisturizing ingredient in both cases was glycerol at concentrations of 5%
and 7%, respectively. Two humectant systems, with dierent carrier compositions were employed to limit poten-
tial artefacts from the non-humectant components. e two dierent systems were tested on two dierent days,
and their order randomized. e humectant was applied with three ngers (including the index nger) onto the
cheek (as a counter surface to distribute the material evenly and in a manner perceived as “natural” by the partic-
ipants). e cutaneous parameters and tactile friction were also measured in the treated state on the index nger.
Meissner Corpuscle density. Meissner corpuscles (MC) densities were obtained according to an estab-
lished protocol described and referenced in the ESI. Optical images were obtained for each subject by sampling a
2,5 × 2 mm area over the midpoint of the volar aspect of the distal phalanx of Digit I, on the dominant hand. An in
vivo reectance Confocal Microscope (RCM) (Vivascope 1500, Lucid Inc., NY) was used to obtain all the images
at a specic depth. A fuller description is provided in the Supplementary Information.
Statistical analysis. One-way ANOVAs or two-way repeated ANOVAS have been done with Origin (phys-
ical data) and SPSS (perception data) to test signicance between variables, i.e. the eect of age, eect of humec-
tant and eect of dierent concentrations of glycerol. e Alpha level for statistically signicance was set to
p < 0.05. Due to a wide spread in absolute values obtained when measuring tactile perception ability with human
subjects as well as properties on the skin in vivo, we show all results as box plots, showing the interquartile range
IQR (25th and 75th quartile = 50% of the data) with the mean (square), median (line), whiskers indicating varia-
bility within 1.5 IQR, as well as outliers. e age dierences in the ability to correctly identify S20 and the ability
to correctly detect S40 were analysed in separate one-way ANOVAs. Tukey post hoc analyses were used to identify
which of the conditions were signicantly dierent.
References
1. ornbury, J. M. & Mistretta, C. M. Tactile sensitivity as a function of age. Journals of Gerontology 36, 34–39 (1981).
2. Wicremaratchi, M. M. & Llewelyn, J. G. Eects of ageing on touch. Postgraduate Medical Journal 82, 301 (2006).
3. Leveque, J. L., Dresler, J., ibot-Ciscar, E., oll, J. P. & Poelman, C. Changes in tactile spatial discrimination and cutaneous coding
properties by sin hydration in the elderly. J Invest Dermatol 115, 454–458, https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1747.2000.00055.x
(2000).
4. Decorps, J., Saumet, J. L., Sommer, P., Sigaudo-oussel, D. & Fromy, B. Eect of ageing on tactile transduction processes. Ageing
esearch eviews 13, 90–99, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2013.12.003 (2014).
5. Lederman, S. J. & latzy, . L. Haptic perception: A tutorial. Attention Perception & Psychophysics 71, 1439–1459 (2009).
6. Derler, S., Süess, J., ao, A. & otaru, G. M. Inuence of variations in the pressure distribution on the friction of the nger pad.
Tribology International 63, 14–20, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.triboint.2012.03.001 (2013).
7. Guest, S. et al. Perceptual and sensory-functional consequences of sin care products. Journal of Cosmetics, Dermatological Sciences
and Applications 3, 66–78 (2013).
8. Estanqueiro, M., Amaral, M. H. & Sousa Lobo, J. M. Comparison between sensory and instrumental characterization of topical
formulations: impact of thicening agents. International Journal of Cosmetic Science 38, 389–398, https://doi.org/10.1111/ics.12302
(2016).
9. Shao, F., Chen, X. J., Barnes, C. J. & Henson, B. A novel tactile sensation measurement system for qualifying touch perception.
Proceedings of the Institution of Mechanical Engineers Part H-Journal of Engineering in Medicine 224, 97–105, https://doi.
org/10.1243/09544119jeim658 (2010).
10. Sedung, L. et al. Feeling Small: Exploring the Tactile Perception Limits. Sci. ep. 3, 2617, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep02617 (2013).
11. Veijgen, N. ., Masen, M. A. & van der Heide, E. Variables inuencing the frictional behaviour of in vivo human sin. Journal of the
Mechanical Behavior of Biomedical Materials 28, 448–461, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmbbm.2013.02.009 (2013).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
9
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
12. Zahouani, H., Djaghloul, M., Vargiolu, ., Mezghani, S. & Mansori, M. E. L. Contribution of human sin topography to the
characterization of dynamic sin tension during senescence: morpho-mechanical approach. Journal of Physics: Conference Series
483, 012012 (2014).
13. Cornuault, P.-H., Carpentier, L., Bueno, M.-A., Cote, J.-M. & Monteil, G. Influence of physico-chemical, mechanical and
morphological ngerpad properties on the frictional distinction of sticy/slippery surfaces. Journal of e oyal Society Inter face 12
(2015).
14. Bergmann Tiest, W. M. & appers, A. M. L. Analysis of haptic perception of materials by multidimensional scaling and physical
measurements of roughness and compressibility. Acta Psychologica 121, 1–20 (2006).
15. Sedung, L. et al. Tactile perception: Finger friction, surface roughness and perceived coarseness. Tribology International 44,
505–512, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.triboint.2010.04.010 (2011).
16. Oamoto, S., Nagano, H. & Yamada, Y. Psychophysical dimensions of tactile perception of textures. Haptics, IEEE Transactions on
PP, 1–1, https://doi.org/10.1109/toh.2012.32 (2012).
17. Bergmann Tiest, W. M. Tactual perception of material properties. Vision esearch 50, 2775–2782, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
visres.2010.10.005 (2010).
18. Picard, D., Dacremont, C., Valentin, D. & Giboreau, A. Perceptual dimensions of tactile textures. Acta Psychologica 114, 165–184
(2003).
19. Arvidsson, M., ingstad, L., Sedung, L., Duvefelt, . & utland, M. W. Feeling ne - the eect of topography and friction on
perceived roughness and slipperiness. Biotribology 11, 92–101, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biotri.2017.01.002 (2017).
20. Eastwood, A. L. et al. Tissue mechanics govern the rapidly adapting and symmetrical response to touch. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences 112, E6955–E6963, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1514138112 (2015).
21. Wandersman, E., Candelier, ., Debrégeas, G. & Prevost, A. Texture-induced modulations of friction force: e ngerprint eect.
Physical eview Letters 107, 164301 (2011).
22. Adams, M. J., Briscoe, B. J. & Johnson, S. A. Friction and lubrication of human sin. Tribology Letters 26, 239–253 (2007).
23. Derler, S. & Gerhardt, L. C. Tribology of sin: eview and analysis of experimental results for the friction coecient of human sin.
Tribology Letters 45, 1–27, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11249-011-9854-y (2012).
24. Leyva-Mendivil, M. F., Lengiewicz, J., Page, A., Bresslo, N. W. & Limbert, G. Sin Microstructure is a ey Contributor to Its
Friction Behaviour. Tribology Letters 65, 12, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11249-016-0794-4 (2016).
25. Pailler-Mattei, C. et al. Contribution of stratum corneum in determining bio-tribological properties of the human sin. We ar 263,
1038–1043, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wear.2007.01.128 (2007).
26. Leyva-Mendivil, M. F., Page, A., Bresslo, N. W. & Limbert, G. A mechanistic insight into the mechanical role of the stratum
corneum during stretching and compression of the sin. Journal of the Mechanical Behavior of Biomedical Materials 49, 197–219,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmbbm.2015.05.010 (2015).
27. Cua, A. B., Wilhelm, . P. & Maibach, H. I. Frictional properties of human sin: elation to age, sex and anatomical region, stratum
corneum hydration and transepidermal water loss. British Journal of Dermatology 123, 473–479 (1990).
28. de Beer, J., Bardoutsos, A. & Janssen, F. Maximum human lifespan may increase to 125 years. Nature 546, E16–E17, https://doi.
org/10.1038/nature22792 (2017).
29. Stevens, J. C. Aging and spatia l acuity of touch. J Gerontol 47, P35–40 (1992).
30. Carmeli, E., Patish, H. & Coleman, . The Aging Hand. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A 58, M146–M152, https://doi.
org/10.1093/gerona/58.2.M146 (2003).
31. Besne, I., Descombes, C. & Breton, L. Eect of age and anatomical site on density of sensory innervation in human epidermis. Arch
Dermatol 138, 1445–1450 (2002).
32. Iwasai, T., Goto, N., Goto, J., Ezure, H. & Moriyama, H. e aging of human Meissner’s corpuscles as evidenced by parallel
sectioning. Oajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica 79, 185–189, https://doi.org/10.2535/ofaj.79.185 (2003).
33. Norman, J. F. et al. Aging and curvature discrimination from static and dynamic touch. PLoS ONE 8, e68577, https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068577 (2013).
34. Nadol, J. B. J. Hearing loss. New England Journal of Medicine 329, 1092–1102, https://doi.org/10.1056/nejm199310073291507
(1993).
35. ahi, J., Logan, S., Timms, C., ussell-Eggitt, I. & Taylor, D. is, causes, and outcomes of visual impairment aer loss of vision in
the non-amblyopic eye: a population-based study. Lancet 360, 597–602 (2002).
36. Norman, J. F. et al. Aging and the haptic perception of material properties. Perception 45, 1387–1398, https://doi.
org/10.1177/0301006616659073 (2016).
37. Bolanowsi, S. J. Jr., Gescheider, G. A., Verrillo, . T. & Checosy, C. M. Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch.
Journal of e Acoustical Society of America 84, 1680–1694 (1988).
38. Weeraody, N. S., Mahns, D. A., Taylor, J. L. & Gandevia, S. C. Impairment of human proprioception by high-frequency cutaneous
vibration. e Journal of Physiology 581, 971–980, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2006.126854 (2007).
39. Legge, G. E., Madison, C., Vaughn, B. N., Cheong, A. M. Y. & Miller, J. C. etention of high tactile acuity throughout the life span in
blindness. Perception & Psychophysics 70, 1471–1488, https://doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.8.1471 (2008).
40. Bruns, P. et al. Tactile acuity charts: A reliable measure of spatial acuity. PLoS ONE 9, e87384, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0087384 (2014).
41. Escoer, C. et al. Age-related mechanical properties of human sin: an in vivo study. J Invest Dermatol 93, 353–357 (1989).
42. yu, H. S., Joo, Y. H., im, S. O., Par, . C. & Youn, S. W. Inuence of age and regional dierences on sin elasticity as measured by
the Cutometer. Sin es Technol 14, 354–358, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0846.2008.00302.x (2008).
43. rueger, N., Luebberding, S., Oltmer, M., Streer, M. & erscher, M. Age-related changes in sin mechanical properties: a
quantitative evaluation of 120 female subjects. Sin es Technol 17, 141–148, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0846.2010.00486.x
(2011).
44. Ishiawa, T., Ishiawa, O. & Miyachi, Y. Measurement of sin elastic properties with a new suction device (I): elationship to age,
sex and the degree of obesity in normal individuals. J Dermatol 22, 713–717 (1995).
45. Pasumarty, S. M., Johnson, S. A., Watson, S. A. & Adams, M. J. Friction of the human nger pad: Inuence of moisture, occlusion
and velocity. Tribology Letters 44, 117–137, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11249-011-9828-0 (2011).
46. Álvarez-Asencio, . et al. Nanomechanical properties of human sin and introduction of a novel hair indenter. Journal of the
Mechanical Behavior of Biomedical Materials 54, 185–193, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmbbm.2015.09.014 (2016).
47. Crichton, M. L. et al. The viscoelastic, hyperelastic and scale dependent behaviour of freshly excised individual sin layers.
Biomaterials 32, 4670–4681, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biomaterials.2011.03.012 (2011).
48. Feiler, A. A., Jenins, P. & utland, M. W. Eect of relative humidity on adhesion and frictional properties of micro- and nano-scopic
contacts. Journal of Adhesion Science and Technology 19, 165–179, https://doi.org/10.1163/1568561054352595 (2005).
49. Guest, S., Essic, G. ., Mehrabyan, A., Dessirier, J.-M. & McGlone, F. Eect of hydration on the tactile and thermal sensitivity of the
lip. Physiology & Behavior 123, 127–135, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.10.013 (2014).
50. Vega-Bermudez, F. & Johnson, . O. Fingertip sin conformance accounts, in part, for dierences in tactile spatial acuity in young
subjects, but not for the decline in spatial acuity with aging. Percept Psychophys 66, 60–67 (2004).
51. Alber, C. et al. Eects of water gradients and use of urea on sin ultrastructure evaluated by confocal aman microspectroscopy.
Biochim Biophys Acta 1828, 2470–2478, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamem.2013.06.011 (2013).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
10
SCIenTIfIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:5303 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6
52. Wildnauer, . H., Bothwell, J. W. & Douglass, A. B. Stratum Corneum Biomechanical Properties I. Inuence of elative Humidity
on Normal and Extracted Human Stratum Corneum. Journal of Investigative Dermatology 56, 72–78, https://doi.org/10.1111/1523-
1747.ep12292018 (1971).
53. Potter, A., Luengo, G. S., Santoprete, . & Querleux, B. In Sin moistur ization (ed eds awlings AV and Leyden JJ) Ch. 18, 259–278
(Informa Healthcare 2009).
54. Nol an o, M. et al. Quantication of myelinated endings and mechanoreceptors in human digital sin. Annals of Neurology 54,
197–205, https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.10615 (2003).
55. Staord, C. M. et al. A bucling-based metrology for measuring the elastic moduli of polymeric thin lms. Nature Materials 3,
545–550, https://doi.org/10.1038/nmat1175 (2004).
Acknowledgements
Marie Lise Chiron and Julien Laboureau are acknowledged for providing the humectants and Olivia Dufour
for statistical advice. Johan Andersson is acknowledged for making the wrinkled surfaces. Annika Bergström
is thanked for recruiting the participants. We acknowledge “PhD trials, Lisbon” for performing the Meissner
corpuscle measurements.
Author Contributions
L.S., M.A., G.L., C.F., M.W.R. and C.E.R. designed and planned the research. (L.B. and C.E.R designed the
neurobiologicalresearch onMeissner Corpuscles) L.S. and M.A. performed the research and conceived the data
collection with subjects and the data analyses. (with the exception of Meissner Corpuscle results) L.S., G.L. and
M.W.R. wrote the dras of the manuscript and L.B., C.F. and C.E.R. reviewed the dras. L.S. and C.E.R. made
the gures. All authors discussed and interpreted the results and contributed to dras of this nal paper. L.S.
organized the work.
Additional Information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-23688-6.
Competing Interests: C.F., C.E.R., G.L. and L.B. are employees of L’Oréal. M.A. and L.S. were full time
employees of RISE Research Institutes of Sweden at time of completion of the study which is a govt. owned
consulting organization having received nancing from L’Oréal to perform the majority of the research leading
to this study. M.R. currently receives funding from L’Oréal via KTH to perform research on an unrelated topic
and is a senior advisor at RISE on a part time basis.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2018
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY
Content may be subject to copyright.