ArticlePDF Available

Comparative effect of coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa) on memory and attention

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The comparative effects of coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa) on different attention and memory related assignments were measured in a double-blind study of 300 healthy young adult women who were randomly assigned to one of three different drinks: Group I (coffee robusta sachet dissolved in 100 ml of hot water): Group II (coffee arabica): and group III (100 ml water only). Cognitive function was assessed by standardized tests. Several monitoring cognitive tests and tasks were specifically chosen and performed to investigate the comparative effects of coffee robusta (CR) and coffee arabica (Qahwa; AC) on sleepiness (sleep and clear headed scale), attention (trail A & B, symbol digit, letter cancellation), general cognitive ability (stroop test) and memory (card test). Data was interpreted by analysis of variance (ANOVA). The present study revealed that coffee robusta has beneficial effects on attention, general cognitive ability and memory. Higher though non-significant cognitive scores were associated with coffee robusta consumption. Although, consumption of coffee arabica (Qahwa) has significant effects (P < 0.05) on sleepiness, attention, general cognitive ability and memory and it significantly improve reaction time and correct responses. Since different tasks were related to the sustained attention and working memory processes, results would suggest that coffee arabica (qahwa) could increase the memory and efficiency of the attentional system might be due to the presence of chlorogenic acids (CGA) which are found in less quantity in coffee robusta. However, more studies using larger samples and different tasks are necessary to better understand the effects of coffee robusta and arabica (Qahwa) on attention and memory.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1 23
Metabolic Brain Disease
ISSN 0885-7490
Volume 33
Number 4
Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:1203-1210
DOI 10.1007/s11011-018-0230-6
Comparative effect of coffee robusta and
coffee arabica (Qahwa) on memory and
attention
Waheeb D.M.Alharbi, Aisha Azmat &
Muhammad Ahmed
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC, part of
Springer Nature. This e-offprint is for personal
use only and shall not be self-archived in
electronic repositories. If you wish to self-
archive your article, please use the accepted
manuscript version for posting on your own
website. You may further deposit the accepted
manuscript version in any repository,
provided it is only made publicly available 12
months after official publication or later and
provided acknowledgement is given to the
original source of publication and a link is
inserted to the published article on Springer's
website. The link must be accompanied by
the following text: "The final publication is
available at link.springer.com”.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Comparative effect of coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa)
on memory and attention
Waheeb D. M. Alharbi
1
&Aisha Azmat
1
&Muhammad Ahmed
2
Received: 14 November 2017 /Accepted: 6 April 2018 /Published online: 13 April 2018
#Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018
Abstract
The comparative effects of coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa) on different attention and memory related assignments
were measured in a double-blind study of 300 healthy young adult women who were randomly assigned to one of three different
drinks: Group I (coffee robusta sachet dissolved in 100 ml of hot water): Group II (coffee arabica): and group III (100 ml water
only). Cognitive function was assessed by standardized tests. Several monitoring cognitive tests and tasks were specifically
chosen and performed to investigatethe comparative effects ofcoffee robusta (CR) and coffee arabica (Qahwa; AC) on sleepiness
(sleep and clear headed scale), attention (trail A & B, symbol digit, letter cancellation), general cognitive ability (stroop test) and
memory (card test). Data was interpreted by analysis of variance (ANOVA). The present study revealed that coffee robusta has
beneficial effects on attention, general cognitive ability and memory. Higher though non-significant cognitive scores were
associated with coffee robusta consumption. Although, consumption of coffee arabica (Qahwa) has significant effects
(P< 0.05) on sleepiness, attention, general cognitive ability and memory and it significantly improve reaction time and correct
responses. Since differenttasks were related to the sustained attention and working memory processes, results would suggest that
coffee arabica (qahwa) could increase the memory and efficiency of the attentional system might be due to the presence of
chlorogenic acids (CGA) which are found in less quantity in coffee robusta. However, more studies using larger samples and
different tasks are necessary to better understand the effects of coffee robusta and arabica (Qahwa) on attention and memory.
Keywords Coffee robusta (CR) .Coffee arabica (Qahwa) .Cognition .Memory .Attention .Psychomotor performance
Introduction
Attention is the process for enhanced processing and integra-
tion, and memory is the ability to store, retain, and recall
information and experiences (Winkler and Cowan 2005).
Recent research has concentrated on the formation of new
plant based medicine that boosts cognitive skills, as medicinal
plants are claimed to possess antioxidant and cognitive en-
hancing effects (Saenghong et al. 2012). Caffeine is one of
the widely consumed throughout the world in beverages,
foods, and as a drug for a variety of reasons, including its
stimulant-like effects on mood and cognitive performance
(Fredholm et al. 1999; Lieberman et al. 2002).
Different beverages have been used as attention and mem-
ory enhancers (having caffeine) in Asian, Indian, and Arabic
folklore like coffee arabica (Qahwa) and coffee robusta.
Arabic coffee is a traditional drink prepared in many Arab
countries throughout the Middle East by using coffee arabica
green beans (without/slight roasted) which contain 12 mg of
caffeine per gram of dry mass (Silvarolla et al. 2004). Coffee
robusta is prepared by boiling finely powdered roasted beans
(either coffee robusta or coffee arabica) possibly with sugar
(Jamieson 2001) while coffee arabica (Qahwa) is made from
coffee beans (without/slight roasted) and cardamom.
Traditionally, green coffee beans are roasted at home slightly
Key points Coffee arabica (Qahwa) is a more effective cognition en-
hancer than coffee robusta.
This difference between coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa)
might be due to the presence of chlorogenic acids (CGA) that act on brain
function, which has having beneficial effects on psychological
performance.
*Aisha Azmat
aishaazmatkhan@hotmail.com
1
Department of Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, Umm-Al-Qura
University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia
2
Department of Pharmacology, Faculty of Pharmacy, Umm-Al-Qura
University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia
Metabolic Brain Disease (2018) 33:12031210
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11011-018-0230-6
Author's personal copy
andground(Mahmoudetal.2013). Coffee robusta bean ex-
cessively roasted (250 °C) to produce more bitterness than
arabica (Qahwa) due to their pyrazine content (Wintgens
2009).
Previously, the effects of coffee have been assessed sci-
entifically (Mesas et al. 2011;Nehlig2016). Coffee has
both good and bad effects, both of which may impact on
cognitive performance. Coffee is found to increase neuron
activity in the frontal lobe, the site of working memory
which regulates the ability to pay attention (Ryan et al.
2002). There are multiple theories that the presence of caf-
feine in coffee affects short-term memory, due to an in-
crease in concentration. In addition, caffeine has a direct
effect on brain functions. It stimulates brain areas that cover
short-term memory and attention. Caffeine causes the
blood vessels in the brain to constrict, and boosts metabo-
lism. The neurons in the brain work faster when affected by
caffeine. This causes a chain reaction that leads to mental
alertness that allows for better concentration and conse-
quently, better recall of information (Hameleers et al.
2000). In conclusion, different studies revealed that coffee
robusta enhanced attention and cognitive performance un-
der the condition of stress and sleep deprivation (Patat et al.
2000; Reyner and Horne 2000).
Qahwa is a cultural drink believed to have anti-
atherosclerotic (Alsaif et al. 2007) and anti-
inflammatory effects and markers of inflammation were
significantly lowered by intake of Arabian Qahwa (Habib
et al. 2012). Coffee arabica (Qahwa) is flavor-rich
with cardamom. Cardamom (Elettaria cardamomum)
contained αterpineol, myrcene, limonene, menthone, β-
phellandrene, 1,8-cineol, sabinene and heptane and bor-
neol (Farooq et al. 2015) found to possess anxiolytic
(Masoumi-Ardakani et al. 2017) and it enhance learning
and memory retention as compared to control (Abu-
Taweel 2018).
However, coffee arabica (Qahwa) has still not been inves-
tigated for its described effects, although it is traditionally
consumed in many Arab countries throughout the Middle
East. The present study, therefore, aims to observe the effect
of coffee arabica (Qahwa) on memory and compare the effect
of coffee arabica (Qahwa) and coffee robusta on psychomotor
performance.
Materials and methods
For the estimation of cholorogenic acid (CGA) in coffee
robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa), a thin layer chroma-
tography (TLC) was performed. Isolation solvent
Isopropanolformic acid (10:0.5 v/v) was used in TLC
(Wen et al. 2012).
Subjects
This study was carried out on 300 young adult women aged
1826 years old. The participants were students at Umm Al-
Qura University (UQU). They were informed about the pos-
sibility of receiving water, coffee robusta and coffee arabica
(Qahwa), when they signed up. Prior to the day of research
activity, subjects were requested to have their normal nights
sleep, to abstain from any type of beverage (caffeinated
/uncaffeinated) or any medicine for 24 h prior to testing, and
to abstain from food three hours prior to testing. A self-
reported absence of any physical or neurological conditions
that affect visual, tactile, or auditory performance, using stim-
uli criterion designed according to the National Drug Strategy,
1998 (Reid et al. 2000), was also required to meet the eligi-
bility criteria for the study. Some participants were excluded
on these latter criteria.
Materials
The participant were randomly divided into three groups.
Group I 12 g of 2 in 1 City Cafe coffee (Coffee robusta)
instant coffee + One sugar sachet of 4.6 g + 100 ml of water
added to each cup.
Group II 3.02 g of coffee arabica (Qahwa) and 2.04 g of
ground cardamom were used to make 100 ml cup without
sugar.
Group III 100 ml of drinking water.
Procedure
The research was approved by the Umm Al Qura University.
The participants filled out and signed a consent form before
the start of experiment. Task sessions were conducted between
10 a.m. and 3 p.m. on weekdays. The participants came to a
designated room and received general instruction. Before
drinking, questionnaires were given out including a nine-
question survey about their normal sleep and caffeine con-
sumption. The participants were told to choose any one cup
(100 ml of water or Coffee robusta or coffee arabica/Qahwa).
After sixty minutes, the participants were instructed that they
had started the further tests. They were also told not to write
anything until told to do so.
Sleepiness assessment scales (Karolinska sleepiness
scale KSS)
The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS; Reyner and Horne
1998), is a 9-point Likert-type self-report scale used to sub-
jectively assess sleepiness.
1204 Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210
Author's personal copy
Clear headedness rating scale (CH)
The Clear Headedness Rating Scale (CH), developed by
Bruck and Pisani (1999) is a 5-point Likert-type scale used
to subjectively assess clear headedness, or the grogginess as-
sociated with sleep inertia (Bruck and Pisani 1999).
Trail making test A and B
This test (Tombaugh 2004) was done to assess attention and
explore the ability of coordination (visuomotor) and process-
ing speed during the arrangement of both numbers and letters
(Lezak et al. 2004).
Part A
In part A, the participant should connect the numbers (125)
in ascending order (Tombaugh 2004).
Part B
In part B, participants were instructed to connect the circles in
an alternating pattern between the numbers and letters (i.e., 1-
A-2-B-3-C, etc.) in 30 s. Errors affected the participants
scores and were considered as negative scores, deducted from
positive scores (Corrigan 1987).
The symbol digit and digit symbol substitution task
(SDST)
This is a screening tool for the detection of cerebral dysfunc-
tion (Strauss et al. 2006) and divided attention (Ponsford and
Kinsella 1992). In this pencil-and-paper task, participants
were presented with a coding key in which the numbers 1 to
9 correspond to different arbitrary abstract symbols (Tietzel
and Lack 2001). During test participants are required accurate-
ly to fill in as many boxes as possible within a 30 s time
period. Scores are calculated by summing the number of cor-
rectly completed items, but points are not deducted for errors.
Letter cancellation test (LCT)
The Letter Cancellation Test (LCT) assesses selective atten-
tion and requires visual scanning and motor speed skills.
Participants were presented with a large 32 × 45 matrix of
random capital letters and asked to search for two particular
stimuli, e.g., Land Uline by line from left to right in a 30 s
time period (Lezak et al. 2004; Tietzel and Lack 2001).
Stroop effect
In psychology, the Stroop effect demonstrates the reaction
time of a task. During the task, the name of a color (e.g., Bred^
Bblue^or Bgreen^) was printed in a different color (e.g., the
word Bgreen^was printed in blue ink instead of green ink).
The participant was instructed to read the name of the color,
not the word (Stroop 1935). In the second experiment, partic-
ipants were asked to read the color of the letters instead of the
written word (Porges and Furman 2011).
Card recall test (CRT)
The participants were instructed that they would be shown
twenty different pictures for 30 s. After the two minutes were
up, the participants were given another set of pictures and
instructed to separate those pictures that were also present in
first set. This test measures prefrontal functions, assessing
different dimensions: cognitive processing speed, concept for-
mation, inhibition capacity and cognitive flexibility (Adan and
Serra-Grabulosa 2010).
Talland letter cancellation (TLC)
Talland Letter Cancellation (Bittner et al. 1985) is a test to
measure the ability to Bfocus^attention and screen out distrac-
tion. In each trial, the participants were asked to examine rows
to cross out target in 30 s.
In Trails 1 and 2 (capitals), participants drew a line on
capital letters.
In Trials 3 and 4 (spaces), participants drew a line before
and after each double space.
In Trials 5 and 6 (both), participants performed both types
of assigned targets (i.e. capitals and spaces).
Statistical analysis
A power analysis suggested that the sample size be set at N=
159 (53 per group) to achieve a power of 0.8, given a medium
effect size (Cohensd= 0.25) and an alpha of 0.05 calculated by
G*Power (Erdfelder et al. 1996). However, in the present study
number of volunteers per group N= 100 were enrolled. Result
of this analysis indicated that 100 subjects per group yielded a
statistical power of 0.97 if inputs were, 3 groups, α= 0.05, total
sample size = 300, critical F= 3.026 (d.f. = 2, 297).
All hypotheses were tested using analysis of variance
(ANOVA) tests. Alpha was set at (0.05). Significant effects
were subjected to Tukeys multiple-range tests for comparison
of treatment means with a control.
Results
The results of TLC are shown in Fig. 1. The isolation time was
10 min for separation of CGA with isopropanol:formic acid
(10:0.5 v/v) as the solvent (RF = 0.29), and both of the isola-
tions were performed at the temperature of 25 °C.
Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210 1205
Author's personal copy
Clear headedness chart and sleep chart
The Clear headedness chart test, used to assess the focus com-
ponent of attention, did not find any significant differences in
water (P= 0.96, non-significant) and coffee robusta (P=0.88,
non-significant) groups, when compared with each respective
control (before ingestion Table 1). However, the increase in
score regarding attention after coffee arabica (Qahwa) inges-
tion were statistically significant (F
(5,594)
= 3.28 P=0.03,
P< 0.05) when compared to control (before ingestion of
coffee arabica, Table 1).
A Sleep chart is used to assess sleep criteria. For the pooled
data for all treatment groups, showed a significant decrease in
response and decrease in sleepiness (F
(5,594)
=7.203 P=
0.0001, P< 0.05). Statistical analysis for the Sleep chart
shows that the mean scores decreased after ingestion of coffee
robusta (P= 0.2 non-significant) and coffee arabica (Qahwa),
and after ingestion of coffee arabica this decrease approached
significance (P < 0.05).
Trail making test part A and B
The Trail Making test Awas used to assess the focus compo-
nent of attention. There were non-significant differences in
scores, (F
2,297
=1.6, P=0.2, P> 0.05). The results (Table 2)
show that that the mean correct responses increased after the
ingestion of coffee robusta and coffee arabica (Qahwa) but
non-significantly (P > 0.05) as compared to control. For the
pooled data for Trail Making B, showed a significant increase
in correct response (F
2,297
= 6.537, P= 0.0001, P<0.05).
Statistical analysis for the Trail Making B showed that the
mean correct answer increased after the ingestion of coffee
robusta (P< 0.05) and coffee arabica (Qahwa) and this in-
crease was statistically significant (P < 0.05) when compared
with control.
Symbol digit and digit symbol substitution task
(SDST) and letter cancellation
The score results of Symbol digit and letter cancellation tests
used to assess attention, revealed significance (F
2,297
=5.63,
P= 0.005, P< 0.05). Results of Tukey HSD Post-hoc Test
showed that consumption of coffee arabica (Qahwa) increases
the score significantly (P = 0.005, P < 0.05) when compared to
water (P< 0.05) as shown in Table 2. A non-significant dif-
ference between water and coffee robusta (p> 0.05) was ob-
served in both tests.
Talland test
A Talland test was used to assess attention, speed and accura-
cy. The results showed that significant differences were ob-
served between beverage groups (F
2,297
=5.39, P=0.006,
Fig. 1 TLC chromatograms of isolation solvent Isopropanolformic acid
(10:0.5 v/v), lanes 1: Coffee robusta, lane 2: CGA standard and 3: Coffee
Arabica (Qahwa)
Table 1 Comparative effect of coffee robusta and coffee arabica on memory and attention tested by Clear Headedness chart
Parameters Sleep chart scores Clear headedness chart scores
Water Before ingestion 3.04 ± 0.10 F
5594
=7.203
Effect size = 0.25
*P= 0.001
Power = 0.99
3.01 ± 0.12 F
5594
=3.28
Effect size = 0.18
*P=0.03
Power = 0.95
After ingestion 3.44 ± 0.14 3.14 ± 0.11
Coffee robusta Before ingestion 3.65 ± 0.11 3.22 ± 0.11
After ingestion 3.29 ± 0.12 3.40 ± 0.13
Coffee Arabica Before ingestion 3.66 ± 0.09 3.07 ± 0.09
After ingestion 2.96 ± 0.10*3.54 ± 0.11
*
*Indicate significant(p < 0.05) in coffee arabica before and after. Power (P) and effect size (f) was calculated by G*Power (Erdfelder et al. 1996). N = 100
subjects in each group
1206 Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210
Author's personal copy
P< 0.05) when compared with control. Statistical analysis for
the Talland test showed that the mean correct answer/score
increased after ingestion of coffee arabica (Qahwa) signifi-
cantly (P< 0.05) when compared with water (P=0.006) and
coffee robusta (P=0.02, Table2).
Stroop test
The present study indicated that different drinks consumed
had a significant effect on reaction times (F
2,297
=5.55, P=
0.006, P < 0.05) and errors (F
2,297
= 3.115, P= 0.04, P < 0.05).
Statistical analysis for the Stroop test showed that the mean
reaction time increased significantly after the ingestion of cof-
fee arabica/Qahwa (p< 0.05), and this increase in mean cor-
rect answer was being greater compared to the coffee robusta
(P= 0.02, p < 0.05) group, as shown in Table 2. The ANOVA
performed for the errors/mistake in reaction revealed a signif-
icant decrease in error was observed between water and coffee
arabica (p < 0.05). After coffee robusta ingestion performance
also showed impairments with more incorrect responses al-
though statistically nonsignificant (P=0.9),
Card recall test
The score results of card recall tests, revealed significance
(F
2,297
= 10.70, P= 0.007, P< 0.05). Tukey HSD Post-hoc
Statistical analysis for the card test showed that the mean cor-
rect answer increased after ingestion of coffee arabica (Qahwa),
and this increase approached significance (P < 0.05). Card test
was used to assess memory. The result showed significant dif-
ferences between water and Arabic coffee (P < 0.05).
Consumption of coffee decreased the mean score (4.36 ±
0.09), though non-significantly (P < 0.05) as shown in Table 2.
Discussion
The results of the present study are presented in Tables 1-2.
The results confirm that coffee robusta and coffee arabica
(Qahwa) are associated with memory and cognitive functions.
During the present study, coffee robusta and coffee arabica
(Qahwa) was ingested and different tasks were performed af-
ter thirty minutes, as it is rapidly absorbed in 3040 min and
has a half-life of between 3 and 6 h (Rogers et al. 2003).All
psychological tasks were performed on subjects between
8:00 am and 12:00 noon because short-term memory (STM)
is thought to be influenced differently throughout the day
(Mitchell and Redman 1992). In other work, when coffee
robusta has been ingested in the morning, STM performance
will be different than at the end of the day (Mitchell and
Redman 1992). Foreman et al. 1989 reported that there were
non-significant effects of coffee robusta on response times or
recall tasks. In the present results after coffee robusta
Table 2 Comparative effect of coffee and Arabic coffee on memory and attention
Parameters Scores in trail making
Tes t A
Scores in trail making
Tes t B
Symbol digit Test
scores
letter cancellation
Tes t sco re
Talland test Stroop test reaction
scores
Stroop test mistakes
score
Card recall test
scores
Water 21.85 ± 0.45 17.06 ± 0.35 18.19 ± 0.57 12.42 ± 0.35 84.73 ± 1.92 22.86 ± 0.51 1.18 ± 0.22 4.43 ± 0.12
Coffee robusta 22.14 ± 0.43 17.90 ± 0.13* 18.51 ± 0.52 13.21 ± 0.53 86.23 ± 2.00 23.42 ± 1.62 1.23 ± 0.14 4.36 ± 0.09
Coffee arabica 22.84 ± 0.31 18.36 ± 0.40
#
20.44 ± 0.44
#
16.53 ± 0.31
#$
93.88 ± 2.39
#$
27.22 ± 0.39
#
0.72 ± 0.09
#
4.92 ± 0.06
#$
F
2,297
=1.6 F
2,297
= 6.537 F
2,297
=5.63 F
2,297
= 28.56 F
2,297
= 5.392 F
2,297
=5.55 F
2,297
=3.115 F
2,297
= 10.70
Effect size = 0.41 Effect size =0.53 Effect size = 0.99 Effect size = 0.77 Effect size = 1.00 Effect size = 0.48 Effect size = 0.22 Effect size = 0.286
P=0.2 *P=0.05
#
P = 0.005
#
P = 0.0001
#
P = 0.006
#
P = 0.006
#
P = 0.04
#
P = 0.007
Power = 0.99
#
P = 0.001
Power = 0.95
Power = 0.97
$
P = 0.0001
Power = 0.97
$
P = 0.02
Power = 0.97
$
P=0.02
Power = 0.95
Power = 0.95
$
P= 0.001
Power = 0.95
#indicates significant between water and coffee arabica
*indicates significant between water and coffee robusta
$
indicates significant between coffee robusta and coffee arabica
Power (P) and effect size (f) was calculated by G*Power (Erdfelder et al. 1996). N= 100 subjects in each group. N= 100 subjects in each group
Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210 1207
Author's personal copy
consumption, the subjects have slower response than the cof-
fee arabica (Qahwa) group. The current results were in accor-
dance of (Foreman et al. 1989), who suggested that subjects
who were given coffee robusta were weak and showed slow
responses during the Stroop test. The researchers suggest that
coffee robusta may have a detrimental effect on the rapid
processing of ambiguous or confusing stimuli. Coffee robusta
administered in subjects recalled fewer words than control
subjects in the card test (Table 2), and also behind the control
subjects in recalling the lists (Terry and Phifer 1986).
However, other studies have shown that coffee robusta intake
has no effect (Herz 1999). Coffee robusta has been shown to
have beneficial effects on cognitive performance when indi-
viduals are sleep-deprived (Patat et al. 2000; Reyner and
Horne 2000) it increase subjective alertness and reduce fatigue
(Adan et al. 2008;Hewlett and Smith 2007) and reduce the
adverse effects of sleep deprivation and exposure to severe
environmental and operational stress on cognitive perfor-
mance (Lieberman et al. 2002). The same was observed in
the present study, after coffee robusta intake subjects felt alert
and clearheaded, although the difference was non-significant
(Table 1). This result was also obtained by (Hameleers et al.
2000) who found a correlation between caffeine consumption
and improved mental alertness. Caffeine helped attention to
remain sharper.
Coffee robusta has beneficial effects on reaction times and
sustained attention tasks. Studies have found it improves
motor-skill performance (driving task; Brice and Smith
2001: and handwriting;Tucha et al. 2006). These findings
are in accordance with our results, confirming that drinking
coffee robusta improves the psychological performance as
tested by Talland, digit symbol and LCT (Table 2) but non
significantly. Another source suggests that coffee robusta al-
ters the levels of CNS neurotransmitters and metabolism of
circulating free fatty acids. The end result is an increase in
blood sugar indirectly by metabolism (Williams 1991).
Sugar (glucose) is used in the present study in coffee robusta
while coffee arabica (Qahwa) was served without sugar.
Previously Serra-Grabulosa et al. (2010) quoted that com-
bined caffeine (coffee robusta) and glucose could increase
the efficiency of the attentional system studies. Serra-
Grabulosa (Serra-Grabulosa et al. 2010) used 75 g sugar that
is far higher than the levels found in sweetened coffee robusta.
It is also higher than the 5 g used in present studies thatswhy
during the present study coffee robusta was not found to in-
crease the efficiency of the attentional system studies signifi-
cantly compared to coffee arabica (qahwa), it only improves
the attention (Table 2).
Compared to water (control) and coffee robusta, the con-
sumption of coffee arabica (Qahwa) had significant beneficial
effects on the execution of simple reaction-time tasks and
sequential reaction time, alertness and memory tests (talland
test, digit symbol test, symbol digit test, card test, stroop test
and trail making test) as shown in Table 2. This current study
suggesting that coffee arabica (Qahwa) is a more effective
cognition enhancer. Coffee robusta and coffee arabica
(Qahwa) come from same plant. Green coffee beans are used
for the preparation of coffee arabica, while coffee robusta is
prepared by brewing roasted, ground beans (Vincent 1987).
Roasting cause degradation of many contents (polysaccha-
rides, sugars, amino acids and chlorogenic acids), and an in-
crease in organic acids and lipids (Cuong et al. 2014). In
coffee arabica (green seeds) the Chlorogenic acids CGA are
intact, as shown in the present TLC from this study (Fig. 1).
Chlorogenic acids (CGA) in coffee seeds completely convert
into phenol and quinolactones due to excessive roasting
(Clifford 2000; Farah et al. 2005).This CGA compounds
present in green coffee are highly absorbed and metabolized
in humans (Farah et al. 2008). Chlorogenic acids (CGA),
which are present in high concentrations in Coffee arabica/
qahwa (Fig. 1) have exhibited their action in the brain function
(Farah et al. 2005) and exert anti-amnesic activity via inhibi-
tion of an enzyme (acetylcholinesterase) and malondialdehyde
in the hippocampus and frontal cortex, so increasing memory
process (Kwon et al. 2010). Not only the CGA, Another con-
stituent of coffee arabica (Qahwa) is cardamom (Elettaria
cardamomum), previously reported to enhance learning and
memory retention as compared to control (Abu-Taweel 2018).
Another study revealed that significant attention and mem-
ory (Trail B, Digit Symbol Test and Stroop) scores were asso-
ciated with cardiac output (Putzke et al. 1998). Cardamom in
coffee arabica (Qahwa)increased the cardiac output
(Bhaswant et al. 2015). Thus the presence of CGA in coffee
arabica and cardamom both has promising beneficial effects
on attention, memory and psychological performance.
Limitation of study
Finally, it is important to emphasize that there are some limi-
tations to our study. It should be stressed that our results must
be considered as preliminary, as we only used 100 subjects (on
a voluntary basis aged between 18 and 26 years young adult
womens) per group, although statistically significant, were
found in the coffee arabica groups. The sample size used in
present study correlates medium effect size with 0.97 power,
For that reason, there is no assurance that present results could
be applicable to other populations and conditions.
Conclusion
It is concluded that coffee arabica (Qahwa) is a more effective
cognition enhancer, having intact polysaccharides, sugars
(glucose), amino acids and chlorogenic acids (CGA) while it
burns in coffee robusta due to excessive roasting. Second,
cardamom in coffee arabica (Qahwa) increases cardiac output,
1208 Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210
Author's personal copy
which has having beneficial effects on psychological perfor-
mance (attention and memory).
Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to Miss Lujain Kuwair,
Miss Lama Farsi, Miss Kholud Sannan, Miss Sheren Farsi, Miss
Omneya Sannan, Miss Eman Monef Faculty of Applied Medical
Sciences, Umm-Al-Qura University, Makkah, Saudi Arabia, for provid-
ing their assistance in collecting data.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interests The authors have no conflict of interest.
References
Abu-Taweel GM (2018) Cardamom (Elettaria cardamomum) perinatal
exposure effects on the development, behavior and biochemical pa-
rameters in mice offspring. Saudi J Biolog Sci 25(1):186193
Adan A, Serra-Grabulosa JM (2010) Effects of caffeine and glucose,
alone and combined, on cognitive performance. Hum
Psychopharmacol 25(4):310317
Adan A, Prat G, Fabbri M, Sànchez-Turet M (2008) Early effects of
caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee on subjective state and gender
differences. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:
16981703
Alsaif MA, Khan LK, Alhamdan AAH, Alorf SA, Sawsan A, Harfi H,
Al-Othman AM, Arif Z (2007) Effect of dates and Gahwa (Arabian
coffee) supplementation on lipids in Hypercholesterolemic ham-
sters. Int J Pharmacol 3:123129
Bhaswant M, Poudyal H, Mathai ML, Ward LC, Mouatt P, Brown L
(2015) Green and black cardamom in a diet-induced rat model of
metabolic syndrome. Nutrients 7(9):76917707
Bittner AC, Smith MG, Kennedy RS, Staley CF, Harbeson MM (1985)
Automated portable test (APT) system: overview and prospects.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 17(2):217221
Brice C, Smith A (2001) The effects of caffeine on simulated driving,
subjective alertness and sustained attention. Hum Psychopharmacol
Clin Exp 16:523531
Bruck D, Pisani DL (1999) The effects of sleep inertia on decision-
making performance. J Sleep Res 8(2):95103
Clifford MN (2000) Chlorogenic acids and other cinnamates nature,
occurrence, dietary burden, absorption and metabolism. J Sci Food
Agric 80:10331043
Corrigan J (1987) The hidden balance : religion and the social theories of
Charles Chauncy and Jonathan Mayhew. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge Cambridgeshire, New York
Cuong TV, Ling LH, Quan GK, Tiep TD, Nan X, Qing CX, Linh TL
(2014) Effect of roasting conditions on several chemical constituents
of Vietnam Robusta coffee the annals of the university Dunarea De
Jos of Galati fascicle vi. Food Technol 38(2):4356
Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A (1996) Gpower: A General Power
Analysis Program. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 28:111
Farah A, de Paulis T, Trugo LC, Martin PR (2005) Effect of roasting on
the formation of chlorogenic acid lactones in coffee. J Agric Food
Chem 53(5):15051513
Farah A, Monteiro M, Donangelo C, Lafay S (2008) Chlorogenic acids
from green coffee extract are highly bioavailable in humans. J Nutr
138:23092315
Farooq A, Abbas A, Khalid MA, Gilani AH (2015) Cardamom (Elettaria
cardamomum Maton) Oils. In Victor R. Preedy, (Ed.) Essential Oils
in Food Preservation, Flavor and Safety. Amsterdam: Academic
Press. ISBN 978-0-12-416641-7. Chapter 33 (pages 295301).
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-416641-7.00033-X
Foreman MD, Gillies DA, Wagner D (1989) Impaired cognition in the
critically ill elderly patient: clinical implications. Crit Care Nurs Q
12(1):6173
Fredholm BB, B_ttig K, Holm_n J, Nehlig A, Zvartau EE (1999) Actions
of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors that contrib-
ute to its widespread use. Pharmacol Rev 51:83133
Habib SS, Ahmed SM, Al Hadlaq A, Marzouk A (2012) Effect of drink-
ing Arabian Qahwa on fractional exhaled nitric oxide levels in
healthy nonsmoking Saudi adults. Ann Thorac Med 7(3):153156.
https://doi.org/10.4103/1817-1737.98849
Hameleers PA, Van Boxtel MP et al (2000) Habitual caffeine consump-
tion and its relation to memory, attention, planning capacity and
psychomotor performance across multiple age groups. Hum
Psychopharmacol 15(8):573581
Herz RS (1999) Caffeine effects on mood and memory. Behav Res Ther
37(9):869879
Hewlett P, Smith A (2007) Effects of repeated doses of caffeine on per-
formance and alertness: new data and secondary analyses. Hum
Psychopharmacol 22:339350
Jamieson RW (2001) The essence of commodification: caffeine depen-
dencies in the early modern world. J Soc Hist 35(2):269294
Kwon SH, Lee HK, Kim JA, Hong SI, Kim HC, Jo TH, Park YI, Lee CK,
Kim YB, Lee SY, Jang CG (2010) Neuroprotective effects of
chlorogenic acid on scopolamine-induced amnesia via anti-
acetylcholinesterase and anti-oxidative activities in mice. Eur J
Pharmacol 649(13):210217
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D (2004)
Neuropsychological assessment. Part I. THEORY AND
PRACTICE OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT.
Oxford University Press, Oxford
Lieberman HR, Tharion WJ, Shukitt-Hale B, Speckman KL, Tulley R
(2002) Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive perfor-
mance and mood during U.S. navy SEAL training. Sea-air-land.
Psychopharmacology 164:250261
Mahmoud ON , Al-Qahiz NM, Ismail MS (2013) Different doses of
arabic coffee improve serum lipid profile, uric acid and liver en-
zymes of experimental rats. Food and Public Health 3(4): 228
233. https://doi.org/10.5923/j.fph.20130304.07
Masoumi-Ardakani Y, Hossein M, Amin M, Esmaeilpour KE, Hamed G,
Solmaz K, Gholamreza S (2017) The effect of Elettaria
cardamomum extract on anxiety-like behavior in a rat model of
post-traumatic stress disorder. Biomed Pharmacother 87:489495
Mesas AE, Leon-Munoz LM, Rodriguez-Artalejo F, Lopez-Garcia E
(2011) The effect of coffee on blood pressure and cardiovascular
disease in hypertensive individuals: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 94:11131126
Mitchell PJ, Redman JR (1992) Effects of caffeine, time of day and
user history on study-related performance. Psychopharmacology
109(12):121126
Nehlig A (2016) Effects of coffee/caffeine on brain health and disease:
what should I tell my patients? Pract Neurol 16(2):8995
Patat A, Rosenzweig P, Enslen M, Trocherie S, Miget N, Bozon MC,
Allain H, Gandon JM (2000) Effects of a new slow release for-
mulation of caffeine on EEG, psychomotor and cognitive func-
tions in sleep-deprived subjects. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin
Exp 15:153170
Ponsford J, Kinsella G (1992) Attentional deficits following closed-head
injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14(5):822838
Porges SW, Furman SA (2011) The early development of the autonomic
nervous system provides a neural platform for social behavior: a
polyvagal perspective. Infant Child Dev 20(1):106118
Putzke JD, Williams MA, Rayburn BK, Kirklin JK, Boll TJ (1998) The
relationship between cardiac function and neuropsychological status
among heart transplant candidates. J Card Fail 4:295303
Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210 1209
Author's personal copy
Reid A, Lynskey M, Copeland J (2000) Cannabis use among Australian
adolescents: findings of the 1998 National Drug Strategy Household
Survey. Aust N Z J Public Health 24(6):596602
Reyner LA, Horne JA (1998) Falling asleep whilst driving: are drivers
aware of prior sleepiness? Int J Legal Med 111(3):120123
Reyner LA, Horne JA (2000) Early morning driver sleepiness: effective-
ness of 200 mg caffeine. Psychophysiology 37:251256
Rogers PJ, Martin J, Smith C, Heatherley SV, Smit HJ (2003) Absence of
reinforcing, mood and psychomotor performance effects of caffeine
in habitual non-consumers of caffeine. Psychopharmacology
167(1):5462
Ryan L, Hatfield C, Hofstetter M (2002) Caffeine reduces time-of-day
effects on memory performance in older adults. Psychol Sci 13(1):
6871
Saenghong N, Wattanathorn J, Muchimapura S, Tongun T, Piyavhatkul
N, Banchonglikitkul C, Kajsongkram T (2012) BZingiber officinale
improves cognitive function of the middle-aged healthy women,^
evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine, vol. 2012,
Article ID 383062:9
Serra-Grabulosa JM, Adan A, Falcón C, Bargalló N (2010) Glucose and
caffeine effects on sustained attention: an exploratory fMRI study.
Hum Psychopharmacol 25(78):543552
Silvarolla MB, Paulo M, Luiz CF (2004) Plant biochemistry: a naturally
decaffeinated arabica coffee. Nature 429(6994):826
Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O (2006) A compendium of neuropsy-
chological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford,
Oxford University Press, New York
Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp
Psychol 18(6):643662
Terry WS, Phifer B (1986) Caffeine and memory performance on the
AVLT. J Clin Psychol 42(6):860863
Tietzel AJ, Lack LC (2001) The short-term benefits of brief and long naps
following nocturnal sleep restriction. Sleep 24(3):293300
Tombaugh TN (2004) Trail making test a and B: normative data stratified
by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19:203214
Tucha O, Walitza S, Mecklinger L, Stasik D, Sontag TA, Lange KW
(2006) The effect of caffeine on handwriting movements in skilled
writers. Hum Mov Sci 25(45):523535
Vincent J (1987) Coffee, vol 2: technology. Elsevier, London
WenJ, Kang L, Liu H, Xiao Y, Zhang X, Chen Y (2012) A validated UV-
HPLC method for determination of chlorogenic acid in
Lepidogrammitis drymoglossoides (baker) Ching, Polypodiaceae.
Pharm Res 4:148153
Williams JH (1991) Caffeine, neuromuscular function and high-intensity
exercise performance. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 31(3):481489
Winkler I, Cowan N (2005) From sensory to long-term memory: evi-
dence from auditory memory reactivation studies. Exp Psychol
52(1):320
Wintgens JN (2009) Cultivation. In: Coffee: growing, processing, sus-
tainable production: a guidebook for growers. processors, traders,
and researchers. 2nd Edn, Wiley-VCH, Germany. 799800
1210 Metab Brain Dis (2018) 33:12031210
Author's personal copy
... Despite the enhancing effects reported in these two studies, many have reported a smaller magnitude or no effects of caffeine on other WM measures: Haskell-Ramsay et al. (2018) did not find any main effect of caffeine on a visuospatial WM task. Hameleers et al. (2000) and Alharbi et al. (2018) included a letter-digit substitute task, assessing processing speed and WM capacity (Van der Elst et al., 2012). While the former did not find any effect of habitual caffeine consumption, the latter found that a single dose of caffeine from a specific type of coffee, café arabica (Qahwa), a traditional Arabic and Middle Eastern coffee made from raw or lightly roasted beans and cardamom improved performance. ...
... Only a few studies assessed recognition STM; among these, consuming 100 mg caffeine added to decaffeinated coffee did not affect performance in either immediate recall or memory scanning as an STM recognition task (Schmitt et al., 2003). Alharbi et al. (2018) reported a tendency for a selected type of coffee in improving accuracy in picture recognition (arabica) relative to placebo, but this did not reach statistical significance. Other studies adopting STM measures reported an interaction between caffeine and age-related factors, and are described further in Section 5.2. ...
... Caffeine from the same beverage, coffee, can also have different effects due to the stage of beans, brewing process, and biochemistry profiles (Alharbi et al., 2018). In this study, participants receiving a cup of 3.02 g coffee arabica and 2.04 g ground cardamom showed performance increment in all memory tests, compared to those receiving a cup of 12 g '2 in 1 City Café' instant coffee (robusta) (with an optional 4.6 g sugar sachet). ...
Article
Caffeine is a widely used nootropic drug, but its effects on memory in healthy participants have not been sufficiently evaluated. Here we review evidence of the effects of caffeine on different types of memory, and the associated drug, experimental, and demographical factors. There is limited evidence that caffeine affects performance in memory tasks beyond improved reaction times. For drug factors, a dose-response relationship may exist but findings are inconsistent. Moreover, there is evidence that the source of caffeine can modulate its effects on memory. For experimental factors, past studies often lacked a baseline control for diet and sleep and none discussed the possible reversal of withdrawal effect due to pre-experimental fasting. For demographic factors, caffeine may interact with sex and age, and the direction of the effect may depend on the dose, individual tolerance, and metabolism at baseline. Future studies should incorporate these considerations, as well as providing continued evidence on the effect of caffeine in visuospatial, prospective, and implicit memory measures.
... Despite the enhancing effects reported in these two studies, many have reported a smaller magnitude or no effects of caffeine on other WM measures: Haskell-Ramsay et al. (2018) did not find any main effect of caffeine on a visuospatial WM task. Hameleers et al. (2000) and Alharbi et al. (2018) included a letter-digit substitute task, assessing processing speed and WM capacity (Van der Elst et al., 2012). While the former did not find any effect of habitual caffeine consumption, the latter found that a single dose of caffeine from a specific type of coffee, café arabica (Qahwa), a traditional Arabic and Middle Eastern coffee made from raw or lightly roasted beans and cardamom improved performance. ...
... Only a few studies assessed recognition STM; among these, consuming 100 mg caffeine added to decaffeinated coffee did not affect performance in either immediate recall or memory scanning as an STM recognition task (Schmitt et al., 2003). Alharbi et al. (2018) reported a tendency for a selected type of coffee in improving accuracy in picture recognition (arabica) relative to placebo, but this did not reach statistical significance. Other studies adopting STM measures reported an interaction between caffeine and age-related factors, and are described further in Section 5.2. ...
... Caffeine from the same beverage, coffee, can also have different effects due to the stage of beans, brewing process, and biochemistry profiles (Alharbi et al., 2018). In this study, participants receiving a cup of 3.02 g coffee arabica and 2.04 g ground cardamom showed performance increment in all memory tests, compared to those receiving a cup of 12 g '2 in 1 City Café' instant coffee (robusta) (with an optional 4.6 g sugar sachet). ...
Preprint
Caffeine is a widely used nootropic drug, but its effects on memory in healthy participants have not been sufficiently evaluated. Here we review evidence of the effects of caffeine on different types of memory, and the associated drug, experimental, and demographical factors. There is limited evidence that caffeine affects performance in memory tasks beyond improved reaction times. For drug factors, a dose-response relationship may exist but findings are inconsistent. Moreover, there is evidence that the source of caffeine can modulate its effects on memory. For experimental factors, past studies often lacked a baseline control for diet and sleep and none discussed the possible reversal of withdrawal effect due to pre-experimental fasting. For demographic factors, caffeine may interact with sex and age, and the direction of the effect may depend on the dose, individual tolerance, and metabolism at baseline. Future studies should incorporate these considerations, as well as providing continued evidence on the effect of caffeine in visuospatial, prospective, and implicit memory measures.
... In W.M. Alharbi's paper, he discovered that caffeine directly stimulates the Beta brain waves more as the concentration of caffeine in the coffee increases. In Aldhabi's research, Caffeine causes blood vessels in the human brain to contract and promote metabolism, and the brain's central nervous system and neurons work faster, and as a result people's attention is stronger [14]. ...
... In another research paper by Dietz, Christina, in a study of coffee-to-human cognition, pointed out that 40 mg of coffee is enough to affect attention levels [16]. In his paper, D. M. Alharbi compared the effects of two types of coffee on attention levels and found that Arabica coffee is more beneficial for increasing personal attention [14], Because of this discovery. We decided to use Arabica coffee as the experimental material. ...
... According to the author's experiment, one can collect brain waves 30 minutes after drinking coffee because it is absorbed rapidly within 30-40 minutes and has a half-life of 3 to 6 hours. All psychological tasks were performed on subjects between 8:00 am and 12:00 noon, as shortterm memory was considered to be affected differently during the day [14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study focused on the impact of local Arabica coffee on the level of attention of individual brain waves, and how coffee affects Human EEG Frequency. Local Arabica coffee is adopted in this study as a medium to wake up the Beta wave. The Personal brainwave data is then recorded through EEG equipment and classified. The result showed that local coffee is helping to improve people's attention level — the study conducted on fifty participants: twenty-five males and twenty-five females aged between twenty to thirty years old. Brainwaves or Electroencephalography are collected twice before and after drinking coffee to compare the effects of Arabica on human brain waves by using NeuroSky mindwave mobile. The paired sample t-test test was employed for comparing two groups of Beta brainwaves experiment. Besides, the k-means algorithm is used to perform data mining on brain waves, and the differential brain wave signal data is clustered and divided into three levels. The experimental results showed that there was a statistically significant difference between the two paired samples. Therefore, the results confirmed that local Arabica coffee has a direct impact on personal attention.
... A limited number of studies were conducted in Arabic countries. A study in Saudi Arabia found that compared to Robusta coffee, Arabic coffee has a better effect on cognitive function [19]. However, no study was conducted in Qatar. ...
... Using data from 10 European cohort studies, Zhou et al. conducted a Mendelian randomization meta-analysis and found that there was no evidence for causal long-term effects of coffee consumption on global cognition or memory [27]. Our findings are partly consistent with other human studies on the association between tea/coffee consumption and cognitive function [15,19,[28][29][30][31][32][33][34]. For example, in a cross-sectional study of 14,591 adults aged 35-74 years old in Brazil, coffee consumption was related to better memory performance [30]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Background: Tea and coffee consumption is associated with cognitive function in some studies. We aimed to identify tea and coffee drinking patterns and their association with mean reaction time among Qatari adults. Method: The study included 1000 adults aged 20 years and above attending the Qatar Biobank Study (QBB). Habitual tea and coffee consumption during the previous year was assessed by questionnaire. Tea and coffee drinking patterns were identified using factor analysis. In a computer-based self-administered touch screens test, mean reaction time (MRT) was used as an indicator of cognitive function. Results: The mean age of the participants was 35.8 (SD 10.3) years. Herbal tea and regular coffee consumption was inversely associated with MRT. In the multivariable model, compared with non-consumers, the regression coefficients for MRT were -34.34 (-65.36,-3.33) and -37.85 (-71.03,-4.67) for daily consumers of herbal tea and regular coffee, respectively. Of the two tea and coffee drinking patterns identified, pattern 1 (high consumption of tea, Arabic coffee, and herbal tea) was not associated with MRT but pattern 2 (high loadings of instant coffee, regular coffee and Karak) was inversely associated with MRT in the unadjusted model. There was a significant interaction between pattern 2 and LDL in relation to MRT. Pattern 2 was inversely associated with MRT among those with a low LDL.Conclusion: There was an inverse association between regular coffee and herbal tea consumption with mean reaction. There was an interaction between Western coffee pattern and LDL.
... A limited number of studies were conducted in Arabic countries. A study in Saudi Arabia found that compared to Robusta coffee, Arabic coffee has a better effect on cognitive function [19]. However, no study was conducted in Qatar. ...
... Using data from 10 European cohort studies, Zhou et al. conducted a Mendelian randomization meta-analysis and found that there was no evidence for causal long-term effects of coffee consumption on global cognition or memory [27]. Our findings are partly consistent with other human studies on the association between tea/coffee consumption and cognitive function [15,19,[28][29][30][31][32][33][34]. For example, in a cross-sectional study of 14,591 adults aged 35-74 years old in Brazil, coffee consumption was related to better memory performance [30]. ...
Article
Background: Tea and coffee consumption is associated with cognitive function in some studies. Objective: We aimed to identify tea and coffee drinking patterns and their association with mean reaction time among Qatari adults. Method: The study included 1,000 adults aged 20 years and above attending the Qatar Biobank Study (QBB). Habitual tea and coffee consumption during the previous year was assessed by questionnaire. Tea and coffee drinking patterns were identified using factor analysis. In a computer-based self-administered touch screens test, mean reaction time (MRT) was used as an indicator of cognitive function. Results: The mean age of the participants was 35.8 (SD 10.3) years. Herbal tea and regular coffee consumption was inversely associated with MRT. In the multivariable model, compared with non-consumers, the regression coefficients for MRT were -34.3 (-65.4, -3.3) and -37.9 (-71.0, -4.7) for daily consumers of herbal tea and regular coffee, respectively. Of the two tea and coffee drinking patterns identified, pattern 1 (high consumption of tea, Arabic coffee, and herbal tea) was not associated with MRT but pattern 2 (high loadings of instant coffee, regular coffee, and Karak) was inversely associated with MRT in the unadjusted model. There was a significant interaction between pattern 2 and low-density lipoprotein (LDL) in relation to MRT. Pattern 2 was inversely associated with MRT among those with a low LDL. Conclusion: There was an inverse association between regular coffee and herbal tea consumption with mean reaction time. There was an interaction between Western coffee pattern and LDL.
... A study with 300 healthy volunteers comparing coffee Arabica vs. Robusta consumption found that coffee Robusta has beneficial effects on attention, general cognitive ability and memory. Contrariwise, coffee Arabica has significant positive effects on sleepiness, attention, general cognitive ability and memory (Alharbi, Azmat, and Ahmed 2018). It was also shown a linear association between coffee consumption and depression, where the risk of depression decreased by 8% (RR ¼ 0.92, 95% CI ¼ 0.87-0.97) ...
Article
Coffee is one of the most popular beverages worldwide; however, its impact on health outcomes and adverse effects is not fully understood. The current review aims to establish an update about the benefits of coffee consumption on health outcomes highlighting its side effects, and finally coming up with an attempt to provide some recommendations on its doses. A literature review using the PubMed/Medline database was carried out and the data were summarized by applying a narrative approach using the available evidence based on the literature. The main findings were the following: first, coffee may contribute to the prevention of inflammatory and oxidative stress-related diseases, such as obesity, metabolic syndrome and type 2 diabetes; second, coffee consumption seems to be associated with a lower incidence of several types of cancer and with a reduction in the risk of all-cause mortality; finally, the consumption of up to 400 mg/day (1–4 cups per day) of caffeine is safe. However, the time gap between coffee consumption and some drugs should be taken into account in order to avoid interaction. However, most of the data were based on cross-sectional or/and observational studies highlighting an association of coffee intake and health outcomes; thus, randomized controlled studies are needed in order to identify a causality link.
Preprint
Full-text available
Coffee is one of the most popular beverages in the world. Annual coffee consumption continues to increase, but at the same time, it generates a large amount of spent coffee grounds from the brewing process, that arises environmental problems. An appropriate solution to manage these spent coffee grounds becomes crucial. Our project aims to discuss the feasibility of utilizing the spent coffee ground to synthesize polylactic acid as a recycling application for spent coffee ground. This paper will discuss the properties and potential recycling applications of spent coffee grounds, the brief production process of polylactic acid, and the potential process for converting spent coffee ground to lactic acid. From our review, it is feasible to utilize spend coffee ground as the primary sources for lactic acid production by bacteria fermentation, and further produce bioplastics, polylactic acids by ring-opening polymerization. Possible ways to improve the yield and corresponding cost analysis are also discussed.
Article
Purpose There are limited studies on the effects of coffee consumption, which has become part of popular culture and is highly common among young people, on the attention levels of female university students. This paper aims to examine the effects of caffeine consumption given at different doses (0, 100 and 200 mg) to female university students ( n = 100) studying at the faculty of health sciences on attention performance with the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT). Design/methodology/approach Participants' test completion time, number of errors and corrections were tested. In addition, the frequencies of weekly coffee consumption were also recorded. Findings It was found that consumption of coffee containing 200 mg of caffeine reduced the time to complete the sections of the SCWT test ( p < 0.05), as well as the total test completion time (55.81 ± 8.67 s) compared to no coffee consumption (58.25 ± 9.46 s) and coffee consumption with 100-mg caffeine (57.56 ± 8.87 s) ( p < 0.05; effect size: 0.268). The effect of caffeine on attention level was induced by 200-mg caffeine consumption, but was unapparent at 100-mg dose. In addition, SCWT completion times were reduced in the student group with more weekly coffee consumption (=3 cups/week) at 200-mg caffeine dose. Originality/value In this context, caffeine dose is a factor that should be taken into account in determining the performance of individuals in the SCWT test. It is thought that coffee consumption can increase the concentration of students and have positive effects on academic achievement.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to identify the mechanisms underlying effects of coffee on cognition in the context of brain networks. Here we investigated functional connectivity before and after drinking coffee using graph-theoretic analysis of electroencephalography (EEG). Twenty-one healthy adults voluntarily participated in this study. The resting-state EEG data and results of neuropsychological tests were consecutively acquired before and 30 min after coffee consumption. Graph analyses were performed and compared before and after coffee consumption. Correlation analyses were conducted to assess the relationship between changes in graph measures and those in cognitive function tests. Functional connectivity (FC) was reorganized toward more efficient network properties after coffee consumption. Performance in Digit Span tests and Trail Making Test Part B improved after coffee consumption, and the improved performance in executive function was correlated with changes in graph measures, reflecting a shift toward efficient network properties. The beneficial effects of coffee on cognitive function might be attributed to the reorganization of FC toward more efficient network properties. Based on our findings, the patterns of network reorganization could be used as quantitative markers to elucidate the mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of coffee on cognition, especially executive function.
Preprint
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to identify the mechanisms underlying effects of coffee on cognition in the context of brain networks. Here we investigated functional connectivity before and after drinking coffee using graph theoretical analysis of electroencephalography (EEG). Twenty-one healthy adults voluntarily participated in this study. The neuropsychological tests were consecutively performed at the start of the EEG recording and 30 min after coffee consumption. Graph analyses were performed and compared before and after coffee consumption. Correlation analyses were conducted to assess the relationship between changes in graph measures and those in cognitive function tests. FC was reorganized toward more efficient network properties after coffee consumption. Performance in Digit Span tests and Trail Making Test Part B improved after coffee consumption, and the improved performance in executive function was correlated with changes in graph measures, reflecting a shift toward efficient network properties. The beneficial effects of coffee on cognitive function might be attributed to the reorganization of FC toward more efficient network properties. Based on our findings, the patterns of network reorganization could be used as quantitative markers to elucidate the mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of coffee on cognition, especially executive function.
Article
Full-text available
Cardamom is a strong antioxidant plant, so it is called the queen of spices. In the present study, we explored the potentials of cardamom on developmental, learning ability and biochemical parameters of mice offspring. Thirty pregnant mice were allocated to three groups of ten animals in each. Groups Π and Sh(cyrillic) received pilsbury's Diet containing 10 and 20% of cardamom (w/w) respectively, whereas Group I used as control. Cardomom was administered from the first day of pregnancy and was continued until post-natal day 15 (PD 15) and thereafter the mothers were switched to plain pilsbury's Diet. During the weaning period, three pups in each litter were color marked from the others, and were subjected to various tests (Physical assessment such body weight and eye opening and hair appearance; the neuromaturation of reflexes like righting, rotating, and cliff avoidance reflexes; learning ability and memory retention; estimation of monoamines neurotransmitters like dopamine and serotonin, non-enzymatic oxidative stress such as TBARS and GSH in forebrain at different ages of pups). The results indicated that the body weight gain was declining significantly. Hair appearance and eyes opening were delayed significantly. Righting, rotating, and cliff avoidance reflexes were delayed in treated animals. Exposure to cardamom led to enhance learning and memory retention as compared to control. Monoamines (DA, 5-HT) and GSH were elevated, whereas TBARS was inhibited significantly. In conclusion, perinatal cardamom exposure enhanced learning and memory as compared to control. Cardamom and its benefit compounds were transported via placenta or/and milk during lactation. Cardamom needs more researches to investigate its benefits on other kinds of behavior.
Article
Full-text available
This study was conducted to determine the effect of roasting conditions on chemical constituents of Vietnam robusta coffee. The contents of acrylamide, chlorogenic acid and tannins were higher in green coffee than in roasted coffee and decreased as roasting condition increased, which ranged from 6.53 to 91.36 μg/100g, 1.54 to 55.51 mg/g and 3.14 to 651.59 mg/10g, respectively. In addition, the content of trigonelline ranged from 1.43 to 64.24 mg/10g, which gave the highest value in green coffee, then decreased rapidly, while in the Italian roast it was not present at all. Caffeine content ranged from 15.30 to 35.91 mg/g and presented the lowest value in the case of green coffee, then increased reaching the highest value at 240 o C, after that decreasing gradually and slowly.
Article
Full-text available
Over the last decade, Food Regulation Authorities have concluded that coffee/caffeine consumption is not harmful if consumed at levels of 200 mg in one sitting (around 2½ cups of coffee) or 400 mg daily (around 5 cups of coffee). In addition, caffeine has many positive actions on the brain. It can increase alertness and well-being, help concentration, improve mood and limit depression. Caffeine may disturb sleep, but only in sensitive individuals. It may raise anxiety in a small subset of particularly sensitive people. Caffeine does not seem to lead to dependence, although a minority of people experience withdrawal symptoms. Caffeine can potentiate the effect of regular analgesic drugs in headache and migraine. Lifelong coffee/caffeine consumption has been associated with prevention of cognitive decline, and reduced risk of developing stroke, Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's disease. Its consumption does not seem to influence seizure occurrence. Thus, daily coffee and caffeine intake can be part of a healthy balanced diet; its consumption does not need to be stopped in elderly people.
Article
Full-text available
Both black (B) and green (G) cardamom are used as flavours during food preparation. This study investigated the responses to B and G in a diet-induced rat model of human metabolic syndrome. Male Wistar rats were fed either a corn starch-rich diet (C) or a high-carbohydrate, high-fat diet with increased simple sugars along with saturated and trans fats (H) for 16 weeks. H rats showed signs of metabolic syndrome leading to visceral obesity with hypertension, glucose intolerance, cardiovascular remodelling and nonalcoholic fatty liver disease. Food was supplemented with 3% dried B or G for the final eight weeks only. The major volatile components were the closely related terpenes, 1,8-cineole in B and α-terpinyl acetate in G. HB (high-carbohydrate, high-fat + black cardamom) rats showed marked reversal of diet-induced changes, with decreased visceral adiposity, total body fat mass, systolic blood pressure and plasma triglycerides, and structure and function of the heart and liver. In contrast, HG (high-carbohydrate, high-fat + green cardamom) rats increased visceral adiposity and total body fat mass, and increased heart and liver damage, without consistent improvement in the signs of metabolic syndrome. These results suggest that black cardamom is more effective in reversing the signs of metabolic syndrome than green cardamom.
Article
Full-text available
Coffee mixed with cardamo m is a traditional beverage in Arab countries. The study aimed at investigates the effect of consumption of different levels o f Arab ic co ffee on blood parameters concentration. Th irty t wo male rats (130±2.3 g) were d ivided into four equal groups (n=8), control, single dose of coffee (ACS), double dose of coffee (ACD), trip le dose of coffee (ACT) and received daily an oral dose of 2 ml normal saline, 8.6, 17.1 and 25.7 mg Arabic coffee mixture/100 g BW respectively. The experiment continued for thirty days, at the end, rats were anesthetized and killed by exsanguinations. Blood samples were co llected and used for determination of triglycerides, total cholesterol, HDLc, LDLc and VLDLc, urea, uric acid, A LT, and AST. Samp les of kidneys, heart, and liver were collected for histopathological examinations. The results revealed that there was a decrease in body weight, cholesterol, LDLc, HDLc, uric acid concentration, and liver en zy mes in rats received coffee at all levels. Liver enzy mes were found to be significantly (P<0.05) lower in A CT than control and other groups. Histopathological results indicated no evidence of kidneys dysfunction or pathological alterations in other organs. In conclusion, the consumption of Arabic coffee, at different levels, could be beneficial for patients with hypercholesterolemia or hyperuricemia.
Book
Congregationalist ministers Charles Chauncy and Jonathan Mayhew were among the most influential social and religious thinkers in Boston in the mid-eighteenth century. This 1987 study argues that Chauncy and Mayhew produced a complex but coherent body of ideas and that these ideas were organized closely and self-consciously around the principle of 'balance'. Writings on society and government are treated alongside theological works, rather than separate from them, and each man's corpus is placed against the background of English ideas as well as within the context of intellectual and social life in Boston. Investigation of the ideas of Chauncy and Mayhew in this way leads to the conclusion that although the two men believed that a cosmic principle of 'balance' organized social and religious life, they believed as well that full philosophical comprehension of this principle was beyond human capability. In order to express their understanding of cosmic order, Chauncy and Mayhew appropriated the metaphor of the 'great chain of being'.
Article
This paper summarises the occurrence in foods and beverages of the cinnamic acids, their associated conjugates and transformation products. Quantitative data are lacking for some commodities known to contain them, but it is clear that for many people coffee will be the major source. The daily dietary intake of total cinnamates may vary substantially from almost zero to perhaps close to 1 g. The data relating to their absorption and metabolism are presented along with a consideration of their possible in vivo effects. Data for true bioavailability are incomplete: in particular it is not clear whether availability differs markedly with the form of the conjugate, and whether as a consequence some dietary sources may be superior to others.© 2000 Society of Chemical Industry