Content uploaded by Regan Stevenson
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Regan Stevenson on Oct 17, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Power of Positivity?
The Influence of Positive Psychological Capital Language
on Crowdfunding Performance
Aaron Anglin
Department of Management, Entrepreneurship, and Leadership
Neeley School of Business
Texas Christian University
2900 Lubbock Avenue
Fort Worth, Texas 76109
Email: a.anglin@tcu.edu
Jeremy Short
Division of Entrepreneurship and Economic Development
Price College of Business
307 W. Brooks Ave.
University of Oklahoma
Norman, OK 73019-0450
Will Drover
Division of Entrepreneurship and Economic Development
Price College of Business
307 W. Brooks Ave.
University of Oklahoma
Norman, OK 73019-0450
Regan Stevenson
Department of Management & Entrepreneurship
Kelley School of Business
1275 E. 10th Street
Indiana University
Bloomington, IN 47405
Aaron McKenny
Department of Management
UCF College of Business Administration
PO Box 161400
Orlando, FL 32816-1400
Thomas Allison
Department of Management, Information Systems, & Entrepreneurship
Carson College of Business
442 Todd Hall
Washington State University
Pullman, WA 99164
Corresponding Author: Jeremy Short, 307 West Brooks, Room 206, Norman, OK 73019-4004
Phone: (405) 325-5692; Email: jeremy.short@ou.edu
1
The Power of Positivity? The Influence of Positive Psychological Capital Language on
Crowdfunding Performance
ABSTRACT
We extend the entrepreneurship literature to include positive psychological capital — an
individual or organization’s level of psychological resources consisting of hope, optimism,
resilience, and confidence — as a salient signal in crowdfunding. We draw from the costless
signaling literature to argue that positive psychological capital language usage enhances
crowdfunding performance. We examine 1,726 crowdfunding campaigns from Kickstarter,
finding that entrepreneurs conveying positive psychological capital experience superior
fundraising performance. Human capital moderates this relationship while social capital does
not, suggesting that costly signals may, at times, enhance the influence of costless signals. Post
hoc analyses suggest findings generalize across crowdfunding types, but not to IPOs.
2
1.0 Executive Summary
The rapid rise of crowdfunding provides entrepreneurs with a new and important means
of raising funds for the creation of new ventures or the development of new products. Indeed,
annual investments in crowdfunded projects now exceed $34 billion (Massolution, 2015) and are
expected to soon overtake venture capital as the leading provider of startup funding (Barnett,
2015). Research examining entrepreneurial fundraising efforts, including crowdfunding, has
frequently drawn from signaling theory as a means to understand investment transactions
between investors and entrepreneurs (e.g., Ahlers et al., 2015; Vismara, 2016). Signaling theory
contends that investors prefer to act on information that is costly because costly signals are
believed to be indicative of higher firm quality, while costless signals will be ignored because
they can be sent by both high- and low-quality firms (Connelly et al., 2011). This assertion
appears to run counter to decades of leadership research suggesting that individuals project a
number of attributes indicative of successful leaders (e.g., confidence, optimism, or resolve).
These attributes, although costless from a signaling perspective, allow individuals to attract
support for their cause and improve perceptions of the quality of their organization (e.g., Avey et
al., 2011; Conger et al., 1991). Therefore, such qualities could possibly serve as influential
signals in investment contexts.
Costless signaling provides a theoretical lens that bridges the gap between signaling
theory culled from the finance literature and traditional leadership perspectives. Costless
signaling supports the general idea that investors would prefer costly signals, but suggests that
signals bearing little cost to acquire can be influential under certain conditions. Specifically, less
costly signals are influential when objective information is very scarce (e.g., Lin et al., 2013),
when there is a lack of explicit behavioral norms for a given context (e.g., Danilov and Sliwka,
2016), and when an audience is unsophisticated (e.g., Loewenstein et al., 2014). These three
conditions epitomize crowdfunding, suggesting that less costly signals might be particularly
valuable in crowdfunding contexts.
3
We identify language indicative of positive psychological capital — an individual or
organization’s level of psychological resources consisting of hope, optimism, resilience, and
confidence (Luthans et al., 2004) — as an important costless signal in crowdfunding. Signaling
positive psychological capital would portray an entrepreneur that is confident, resilient,
motivated, and otherwise positively oriented toward taking the needed steps to achieve their
goals. Specifically, we seek to answer two research questions: 1) To what extent, if any, do
displays of positive psychological capital influence fundraising performance in crowdfunding?
2) Do costly signals of quality — social capital and human capital — alter the relationship
between displays of positive psychological capital and crowdfunding performance?
We investigate our research questions by examining 1,726 crowdfunding campaigns from
Kickstarter—one of the world’s largest rewards-based crowdfunding platforms. Our results
indicate that increasing the use of positive psychological capital language leads to greater
crowdfunding performance. Signaling human capital strengthens this relationship, but signaling
social capital does not. We also examine how our primary finding, that positive psychological
capital language leads to greater crowdfunding performance, generalizes to another
crowdfunding context (i.e., debt-based crowdfunding) and to a traditional investment context
(i.e., IPOs). We find that that positive psychological capital language facilitates greater
performance when raising funds through debt-based crowdfunding platforms, but has no
influence in IPOs.
Our findings offer three contributions. First, we demonstrate that crowdfunding investors
value costless signals differently than traditional investors and are comfortable using such signals
to make investment decisions. Second, we add to the growing literature examining the
interaction of signals by investigating how social and human capital alter the effect of displayed
positive psychological capital on crowdfunding performance. While past literature has examined
the interactions of costly signals (e.g., Plummer et al., 2016; Stern et al., 2014), we provide the
first analysis explicitly highlighting how costless and costly signals may work together to
influence entrepreneurial fundraising efforts. Third, our work adds to the literature examining
4
how ‘people-related’ capital influences entrepreneurial outcomes by showing how key forms of
such capital may work together to facilitate fundraising.
2.0 Introduction
Crowdfunding has ushered in an era of democratized fundraising for entrepreneurs and
inventors alike (Mollick and Nanda, 2016; Short et al., 2017a). The rapid proliferation of
crowdfunding across the globe has led to a substantial increase in interest among
entrepreneurship researchers in recent years (e.g., Davis et al., 2017; Mollick, 2014;
Parhankangas and Renko, 2017). Work in crowdfunding has sought to determine the drivers of
crowdfunding performance as well as the theoretical and practical implications of crowdfunding
to entrepreneurship (McKenny et al., 2017). Research interest in this phenomenon comes at a
time where the dollar amount of crowdfunding has surpassed $34 billion in annual investments
(Massolution, 2015) and is expected to soon surpass venture capital as the leading provider of
startup financing (Barnett, 2015).
Signaling theory (Spence, 1973;2002) has been a preeminent theory in explaining
financial transactions in entrepreneurial fundraising (e.g., Davila et al., 2003; Kirsch et al., 2009;
Ozmel et al., 2013), including crowdfunding (e.g., Ahlers et al., 2015). Signaling theory suggests
that the value of a signal — activities or attributes of individuals or organizations that alter the
beliefs of, or convey information to, others in a market (Spence, 1974) — is directly related to
the cost to realize and send that signal (Connelly et al., 2011). Investors prefer to rely on costly
signals given their ability to create a separating mechanism between higher and lower quality
firms (Bergh et al., 2014). Conversely, less costly signals are of lower value because they are
easier for both high- and low-quality firms to produce, and hence easier to imitate (Connelly et
al., 2011). For example, a venture with prestigious ‘blue-chip’ executives signals higher firm
quality to prospective investors given the costly, difficult-to-imitate nature of this signal (Pollock
et al., 2010). In contrast, signals such as founders’ statements regarding their motivation or
optimism to start a new venture will be less impactful because it is not costly to make such
statements and it is easier for another founder or firm to imitate.
5
While the importance of costly signals is well established in the fundraising process, the
general predictions of signaling theory run counter to decades of leadership research indicating
that projecting attributes indicative of a successful leader, such as charisma, confidence,
optimism, or resolve, enables individuals to attract support to their cause (e.g., Avey et al., 2011;
Conger et al., 1991; LePine et al., 2016). Although they do not generally use a signaling theory
lens, work in leadership finds that projecting such attributes enhances perceptions of an
individual’s capability of achieving important goals, an individual’s authenticity, and of the
quality of the organization or cause in which they lead (e.g., Avey et al., 2011; Awamleh and
Gardner, 1999; Jensen and Luthans, 2006). Consistent with this research, a smaller stream of
work in entrepreneurship notes that investors will base investment decisions, in part, on their
own subjective impressions of an entrepreneur’s motivation and abilities irrespective of cost
(e.g., Martens et al., 2007; Parhankangas and Ehrlich, 2014). Such work indirectly implies that
costless information, such as entrepreneurial passion, positively influences investment decisions
in certain settings (e.g., Li et al., 2017). Taken together, these literature streams suggest that
projections of qualities indicative of successful leaders may act as signaling mechanisms that
influence organizational assessments even though these signals bear little cost.
An important theoretical lens that allows us to bridge the gap between signaling and
leadership perspectives is costless signaling. Costless signaling supports the central arguments of
traditional signaling theory, but suggests that signals bearing little to no cost to acquire can
nonetheless be influential under certain conditions. Costless signals (also referred to as low-cost
signals) are particularly influential when objective information about the firm is unavailable
(e.g., Lin et al., 2013), when there are fewer explicit norms of behavior in a given context (e.g.,
Danilov and Sliwka, 2016), and/or when an audience lacks sophistication (e.g., Loewenstein et
al., 2014). Under these conditions, costless signals manifest as instrumental information that can
shape impressions and beliefs about the abilities of another party (Prendergast, 2002; Trager,
2016). Costless signals, then, might be particularly important in crowdfunding where objective
6
information concerning a venture is exceedingly scarce, investors have less-established vetting
processes compared to more traditional fundraising settings, and investors are less sophisticated.
To begin narrowing the gap between what we know and what we need to know
concerning how costless signals influence crowdfunding performance, we examine how displays
of positive psychological capital communicate quality in crowdfunding campaigns and impact
crowdfunding performance. Specifically, we seek to answer the research question: To what
extent, if any, do displays of positive psychological capital influence fundraising performance in
crowdfunding? Positive psychological capital is defined as an individual or organization’s level
of psychological resources and consists of four dimensions — hope, optimism, resilience, and
confidence (Avey et al., 2011; Luthans et al., 2004). Crowdfunding appeals conveying positive
psychological capital would provide insight into an entrepreneur’s temperament and highlight
aspects of each dimension, such as an entrepreneur that is hopeful about achieving organizational
goals, optimistic about the future, resilient in the face of adversity, and confident in his/her
abilities. Communicating these qualities involves no explicit cost, but still conveys desirable
qualities concerning entrepreneurs launching a new firm or product. Further, entrepreneurs
higher in positive psychological capital are perceived as more authentic (Jensen and Luthans,
2006), which may be particularly important in crowdfunding where investors and entrepreneurs
often lack established relationships.
While signaling research has largely examined important signals in isolation (Connelly et
al., 2011), signals rarely occur in isolation in practice (Drover et al., 2018; Plummer et al., 2016;
Stern et al., 2014). As such, understanding the importance of positive psychological capital as a
costless signal also requires an understanding of its importance with respect to other signals, in
particular, costly signals. Therefore, we seek to explore a second research question: Do costly
signals of quality — social capital and human capital — alter the relationship between displays
of positive psychological capital and crowdfunding performance? We focus on social and human
capital because both are well-established costly signals important to entrepreneurial fundraising
(e.g., Ahlers et al., 2015; Baum and Silverman, 2004). Because signaling theory contends that
7
costly signals are considerably more important than costless signals in influencing investment
decisions, it is possible that costly signals could weaken or nullify the influence of costless
signals. However, both types of signals provide information in a noisy environment (i.e., where
information asymmetry is high), thus it is also possible that the signals may work together to
reduce information asymmetries similar to how costly signals work together to promote
investment (e.g., Plummer et al., 2016). Further, because some investors believe that costless
signals are less credible, the inclusion of costly signals in tandem with costless signals may
provide evidence that the costless signals are indeed credible, strengthening their influence.
We probe our research questions by examining how using language indicative of positive
psychological capital in 1,726 crowdfunding campaigns culled from Kickstarter — one of the
world’s largest rewards-based crowdfunding platforms — influences crowdfunding performance.
After conducting our primary analysis, we provide a post hoc analysis to examine whether the
positive psychological capital-performance relationship generalizes across crowdfunding
contexts (i.e., across rewards-based crowdfunding and debt-based crowdfunding contexts).
Because crowdfunding platforms attract different types of projects as well as offer differing
incentives to potential investors, generalizing findings from one type of crowdfunding to another
cannot be taken for granted (McKenny et al., 2017). Therefore, there is a need to examine the
generalizability of results found in one crowdfunding context to another. We then further extend
this investigation by evaluating the role of positive psychological capital in initial public
offerings (IPOs), providing a rare comparison of traditional versus emerging funding
mechanisms as well as providing insight into important boundary conditions for our findings.
Our work provides important implications for several literatures. First, developing
positive psychological capital as a salient costless signal in crowdfunding illuminates a potential
boundary condition to our understanding of how investors value and use information. While
investors operating in traditional financing contexts, such as venture capital, have placed little
value on costless signals (e.g., Chen et al., 2009), the crowdfunding setting strongly resembles
the conditions for which such signals should be valuable. Indeed, recent work in crowdfunding
8
implies language-based costless signals shape performance (e.g., Allison et al., 2013; Davis et
al., 2017; Parhankangas and Renko, 2017), although the literature has yet to explicitly address
the role of costless signals in crowdfunding. Our study demonstrates that crowdfunding investors
value costless signals differently than traditional investors and that crowdfunders are comfortable
making decisions based on these signals. In doing so, we also advance attributes of successful
leaders (i.e., positive psychological capital) as an overlooked type of costless signal that may
influence perceptions of organizational quality. Second, scholars have called for deeper inquiry
into how important organizational signals interact (e.g., Drover et al., 2018; Plummer et al.,
2016; Stern et al., 2014). We answer this call by developing theory concerning the interplay
between costless and costly signals and exploring how an entrepreneur’s social and human
capital may alter the effect of displayed positive psychological capital on crowdfunding
performance. Thus, we provide a first examination of how costly signals interact with costless
signals to influence entrepreneurial fundraising efforts. Third, we add to the literature examining
the importance of ‘people-related’ capital in entrepreneurship. Studies laying the foundation for
positive psychological capital include human and social capital as other critical types of capital
(e.g., Luthans et al., 2004; Luthans and Youssef, 2004) and numerous studies have shown that all
three forms of capital play an important role in entrepreneurship (e.g., Baron et al., 2016;
Davidsson and Honig, 2003). Our work adds to the literature examining how ‘people-related’
capital influences entrepreneurial outcomes by showing how key forms of ‘people-related’
capital work together in facilitating important outcomes, such as acquiring needed resources.
3.0 Crowdfunding Performance
Crowdfunding research to date has primarily focused on the drivers of crowdfunding
performance (Short et al., 2017a). On rewards-based platforms, crowdfunding performance
typically refers to the amount of total funds raised or the ability of campaigns to meet their
funding targets in a finite timeline (e.g., Allison et al., 2017). Investors in this context pledge a
specific dollar amount toward a crowdfunding campaign in exchange for some reward in the
future (e.g., merchandise, the product itself, or insight into product development). Crowdfunding
9
campaign characteristics such as the inclusion of a video, funding goal, campaign length, and
project category all influence the performance of the crowdfunding campaign (Mollick, 2014).
Additional research has begun to show that firm orientations may influence crowdfunding
performance. For example, sustainability orientation promotes crowdfunding performance for
social ventures (Calic and Mosakowski, 2016). Work in crowdfunding has also illustrated the
importance of language use in communicating important information about an entrepreneurial
firm (e.g., Allison et al., 2015; Parhankangas and Renko, 2017). For example, in crowdfunded
microfinance, investors respond to language highlighting the venture as an opportunity to help
others (Allison et al., 2015). Finally, information providing insight into the abilities and
motivation of the entrepreneur also enhances crowdfunding performance. For example,
entrepreneurial passion (Li et al., 2017), social networks (e.g., Colombo et al., 2015), and human
capital (e.g., Ahlers et al., 2015) may all promote crowdfunding performance.
3.1 The salience of costless signals in examining crowdfunding performance
Signaling theory argues that organizations send signals that communicate the quality of
the organization to key outside stakeholders such as investors (Certo, 2003; Pollock et al., 2010).
Investors then choose to invest largely based on perceived organizational quality. Traditionally,
the cost to acquire and send a signal has been viewed as the key mechanism that separates high-
quality signalers from low-quality signalers (Connelly et al., 2011). For example, Spence’s
(1973) seminal work on signaling proposed that the costs associated with acquiring an education
make it a meaningful signal when determining job applicant quality. As such, the costliness of
signals has served as a key component in research focusing on traditional financing transactions
(e.g., Busenitz et al., 2005). Because signals bearing little to no cost should have minimal value
in communicating quality information about a firm, they should have little value aiding investors
in separating high quality firms from low quality firms (Bhattacharya and Krishnan, 1999;
Crawford and Sobel, 1982). For instance, Chen and colleagues (2009) demonstrated that
entrepreneur preparedness for business plan presentations, which is indicative of the time, effort,
and resources invested in to the company, leads to more positive evaluations from venture
10
capitalists. However, entrepreneurial passion — affective emotional displays that signaling
theory would deem costless — has no influence on venture capitalist evaluations.
Although costly signals have been the primary focus of signaling research, an emerging
stream of signaling research has identified contexts in which costless signals transmit important
information about a firm to investors (Danilov and Sliwka, 2016; Martí and Balboa, 2007).
Costless signals are particularly useful under three conditions: when there is an absence of
objective information concerning a firm (e.g., Lin et al., 2013), when there are fewer explicit
norms of behavior in a given context (e.g., Danilov and Sliwka, 2016), and when an audience
lacks sophistication (e.g., Loewenstein et al., 2014). In such cases, costless signals may be used
to make quality judgments about a firm. Notably, work in costless signaling has identified types
of language use or statements from organizational leaders as key costless signals that may be
used to evaluate a firm (e.g., Guillamon-Saorin et al., 2017). It is important to note that although
language-based signals may be ‘costless’ in that they do not incur an explicit cost to realize and
send, these signals may indeed have other associated costs (Payne et al., 2013). For example, if
these signals are disingenuous or misleading, a firm may incur substantial costs in terms of a
damaged reputation, legal costs, or lost customers.
To date, crowdfunding research has yet to explicitly leverage costless signaling as an
important theoretical lens despite the potential value this perspective holds for understanding
crowdfunding performance. While more costly signals, such as the inclusion of a professionally
developed video, past entrepreneurial success, previously successful crowdfunding campaigns,
or product prototype (e.g., Courtney et al., 2017; Devaraj and Patel, 2016), are important to
crowdfunding performance, costly signals are likely rarer than in traditional funding contexts
given that ventures are in the earliest stages of formation and that crowdfunding appeals are
presented online (Agrawal et al., 2014). In addition, investments are much smaller compared to
more traditional fundraising contexts, and thus carry less financial risk. Further, because
individuals supporting crowdfunding campaigns often have little to no investment experience,
they generally do not conduct any formal vetting of the venture (Ahlers et al., 2015). In sum,
11
crowdfunding often occurs in the absence of objective information concerning a firm, where
there are fewer explicit norms of behavior (i.e., no formal vetting requirements), and investments
are mostly made by unsophisticated investors. As such, crowdfunding epitomizes the conditions
under which costless signals may prove influential to investors, thereby influencing
crowdfunding performance. To further understand how costless signals may influence
crowdfunding performance, we introduce positive psychological capital language as an
important costless signal.
3.2 Positive psychological capital as a costless signal
Positive psychological capital spans numerous streams of inquiry such as organizational
behavior, human resource management, and entrepreneurship (Baron et al., 2016; Luthans et al.,
2007). At the individual level, positive psychological capital influences organizational
commitment, coping, performance, and the likelihood of achieving important goals (Avey et al.,
2011; Luthans et al., 2007). At the organizational level, positive psychological capital influences
innovativeness and firm performance (McKenny et al., 2013; Memili et al., 2014). Overall, a
growing body of research documents the influential role of positive psychological capital in
individual and organizational outcomes (Avey et al., 2010).
Positive psychological capital is defined as the positive psychological resource stock of
an organization and is composed of four dimensions: hope, optimism, resilience, and confidence
(McKenny et al., 2013). In contrast to human and social capital, which embody ‘what you know’
and ‘who you know’ respectively, positive psychological capital embodies ‘who you are’
(Hmieleski et al., 2015; Luthans et al., 2004). Hope is concerned both with the motivational
energy to pursue a goal and the ability to proactively and effectively plan to meet these goals
(Luthans et al., 2007), and has been linked to increases in managerial performance, an
entrepreneur’s leadership capabilities, and employee achievements (e.g., Jensen and Luthans,
2006; Peterson and Luthans, 2003). Optimism refers to expectancies about future outcomes and
attributions about past outcomes (Luthans et al., 2004). Individuals higher in optimism tend to
expect positive things to occur in the future (Carver and Scheier, 2002). Optimism has been
12
linked to employee performance (e.g., Luthans et al., 2007), the pursuit of entrepreneurial
opportunities (e.g., Dushnitsky, 2010), and entrepreneurs’ responses to failure (e.g., Ucbasaran et
al., 2010). Resilience is characterized by the ability to cope with and bounce back from
adversity, uncertainty, risk, or failure (Luthans et al., 2004). High resiliency is associated with
the ability to adapt well to change in turbulent environments (e.g., Newman et al., 2014), a
commitment to achieving organizational goals (e.g., Youssef and Luthans, 2007), and
entrepreneurs’ ability to rebound from setbacks (e.g., Hayward et al., 2010). Confidence refers to
the belief in one’s ability to achieve goals and improve on current performance (Newman et al.,
2014). Those high in confidence believe they can exercise control over outcomes and be
successful in tackling difficult tasks (Luthans et al., 2004). Confidence has been associated with
managerial, employee, and entrepreneurial performance (e.g., Hmieleski and Baron, 2008; Judge
and Bono, 2001).
Signaling positive psychological capital to another party provides insight into the
signaler’s mindset, communicating that one is capable, confident, resilient, motivated, and
otherwise positively disposed toward taking the needed steps to achieve a stated goal. These
qualities can be communicated through language use and word choice (McKenny et al., 2013).
For example, expressions of optimism contain language and statements indicative of a positive
expectancy regarding an idea or cause, while expressions of resilience utilize language indicative
of an unwillingness to give up (McKenny et al., 2013). Using words and phrases to communicate
information about one party to another is not per se costly, thus communicating positive
psychological capital through language use is considered costless.
Despite its low cost, signaling positive psychological capital can have a beneficial impact
when seeking others’ support. Individuals high in positive psychological capital are often viewed
as capable and high performing (Avey et al., 2011). Those seen as confident and capable are able
to inspire and convince others of the worthiness of a goal or cause (e.g., Luthans et al., 2007).
Further, a vast body of research suggests individuals are more willing to help those who are able
bodied and can help themselves (e.g., Eden and Aviram, 1993; Wasko and Faraj, 2000). Simply,
13
people are more likely to support an individual or group who is willing to take the needed steps
to meet an objective and express confidence the objective can be achieved than someone who
appears to lack the necessary motivation or commitment. Thus, communicating positive
psychological capital is likely to be beneficial when seeking others’ support.
Displays of positive psychological capital are distinct from other displays of positivity
that may influence the ability to raise funds. In particular, entrepreneurial passion is a positive
emotional display that has been shown to influence fundraising performance in angel investing
and crowdfunding (e.g., Li et al., 2017; Mitteness et al., 2012). While passion and positive
psychological capital overlap in that they provide an individual with goal-directed motivation
(e.g., Cardon et al., 2009; Luthans et al., 2007), the constructs differ in meaningful ways in that
passion is rooted in the literature on emotions while positive psychological capital reflects beliefs
concerning individual or team abilities and expectations about future outcomes that promote
effort and task achievement, but does not require an emotional component (Luthans et al., 2004).
This notion is empirically substantiated by numerous works indicating that positive
psychological capital is distinct from emotional constructs (e.g., Avey et al., 2008; 2010; Luthans
et al., 2007).
3.3 Positive psychological capital and crowdfunding performance
Linguistic cues have long served as a means of sending costless signals in an effort to
cultivate impressions concerning the value of a firm (e.g., Allon et al., 2011; Baginski et al.,
2016). For example, a positive linguistic tone in earnings announcements is predictive of
increasing security prices (Baginski et al., 2016), suggesting that the tone of earnings
announcements influences how investors value the firm. CEO presentations that contain
optimistic promises about the future have been shown to increase investor perceptions of the
firm, even though such promises bear little cost (e.g., Whittington et al., 2016). Retail firms that
provide specific, but costless, claims about the importance of monitoring customer service
quality are more positively evaluated by their customers (e.g., Balvers et al., 2016). Further, in
online written communication, where words can be carefully chosen and other forms of
14
communication (e.g., interpersonal interactions, body language) are muted, readers are often left
to form assessments “relying on language and content cues” (Walther, 2007, p. 2539). This
phenomenon suggests costless linguistic cues may play an elevated role in online settings. Given
that crowdfunding occurs through an online medium, it follows that costless linguistic cues may
play an elevated role in motivating investment decisions. This idea is supported by numerous
studies illustrating how use of specific types of language, which would be considered costless
from a traditional signaling perspective, shape crowdfunding performance (e.g., Allison et al.,
2013; Parhankangas and Renko, 2017; Pietraszkiewicz et al., 2017).
Positive psychological capital language portrays an organization that is hopeful regarding
its ability to meet goals, optimistic about the future, resilient in the face of adversity, and
confident in its abilities. Such qualities, while not costly to signal, are critical in launching a
successful venture. Therefore, it is likely that displaying positive psychological capital influences
the positive perceptions of an individual or firm (e.g., Friend et al., 2016; McKenny et al, 2013).
Indeed, conceptual work has theorized that positive psychological capital may act as a positive
signal, through the portrayal of confidence, optimism, and resiliency, leading to more positive
evaluations by stakeholders (Friend et al., 2016). Further, those who desire to appear competent
will attempt to signal qualities such as confidence (Holoien and Fiske, 2013), while optimism is
positively related to perceptions of leadership potential (Chemers et al., 2000). Entrepreneurs
high in positive psychological capital are perceived to be more authentic (Jensen and Luthans,
2006), which is particularly salient in crowdfunding where investors and entrepreneurs often lack
established relationships. Such arguments are generally consistent with leadership research
indicating that leaders high in positive psychological capital instill greater belief in a cause
among followers (e.g., Gooty et al., 2009; Norman et al., 2005). Further, a rich history across
political science, economics, marketing, and sociology research provides empirical evidence that
pronounced displays of confidence, optimism, and hope facilitate similar beliefs and
expectations in an audience (e.g., Olson, 2006; Strang and Soule, 1998).
15
Taken as a whole, the ability of language-based signals to shape assessments —
particularly in online settings — suggests the signaling of positive psychological capital may
lead to favorable evaluations. This body of work suggests that signaling positive psychological
capital may, in turn, communicate to investors that an entrepreneur is ready and able to meet the
challenges before them. Therefore, investors become confident and optimistic that the
entrepreneur can be successful in pursuing and achieving his or her goals. In total, positive
psychological capital language is an important costless signal where firms with higher displayed
positive psychological capital may be evaluated more positively compared to firms with lower
displayed positive psychological capital. Accordingly, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 1. There is a positive relationship between the use of positive psychological capital
language and crowdfunding performance.
3.4 The moderating influence of social capital
Investors, at times, have difficulty making sense of firm signals in noisy environments
(i.e., where information asymmetry is high) (Plummer et al., 2016). Such difficulty arises
because there may be multiple interpretations of any one piece of information (Gioia and
Chittipeddi, 1991). The online nature of crowdfunding, where information can be difficult to
verify, combined with the inexperience of investors, makes crowdfunding a particularly noisy
environment (Belleflamme et al., 2015). For instance, while expressions of positive
psychological capital may signal confidence, resilience, or psychological strength to some
investors, others may question if expressions of positive psychological capital are genuine and
may desire more information to make an investment decision.
Firms often send signals that work together in conveying information to investors
simultaneously (Pollock et al., 2010; Stern et al., 2014). In doing so, firms facilitate the
interpretation of individual signals by providing additional information that can be used to
evaluate a firm. In this way, the presence of one signal can influence how another is interpreted.
In the entrepreneurial setting, for instance, certain signals flowing from a young venture can be
magnified or strengthened in the presence of a key external signal, such as involvement in an
16
accelerator program (Plummer et al., 2016). In a similar vein, effects stemming from
organizational status and prestige signals are amplified when in congruence with one another
(Stern et al., 2014). Thus, because positive psychological capital language may provide
beneficial information about an entrepreneur and the underlying quality of his or her concept, its
influence may change when accompanied by another potentially beneficial signal.
The entrepreneurship literature has long recognized social capital as a key signal of
quality used by investors. Social capital refers to the value received from social relationships of
individuals or collectives and the available goodwill created through personal ties (Gedajlovic et
al., 2013; Grichnik et al., 2014). Social capital takes time, effort, and resources to cultivate.
Consequently, social capital has been traditionally viewed as a costly signal (e.g., Ahlers et al.,
2015; Khoury et al., 2013). Signals relating to social capital create an endorsement effect that
indicates others have vouched for the entrepreneur (Honig et al., 2006). Further, because social
capital increases an entrepreneur’s credibility, social capital facilitates the building of rapport
between investors and entrepreneurs (Florin et al., 2003).
The current signaling literature remains unclear as to how the presence of costly signals,
such as social capital, alongside costless signals may influence investment decisions. Costly
signals are argued to inherently create more value than costless signals (Connelly et al., 2011).
As such, investors prefer to rely on costly signals. This might suggest that when costly signals
become available, investors would prioritize costly signals, weakening or negating the influence
of costless signals. However, an alternative view argues that in noisy environments more
information is preferred to less information (e.g., Stern et al., 2014; Wang and Lim, 2008).
Indeed, a key premise of signaling theory is that signals are useful because they provide
information in noisy environments, where one party desires more information about another
party (Connelly et al., 2011). Given the need for information, even if costly signals are preferred,
it is unlikely that investors would disregard costless signals that provide further insight into an
entrepreneur or venture’s prospects. This should be particularly salient in contexts, such as
crowdfunding, where costless signals are likely to be valued and costly signals are still rare. In
17
addition, because costless signals are difficult to verify, the inclusion of a costly signal in tandem
with a costless signal provides evidence supporting the credibility of the costless signal, allowing
investors to trust that the costless signal is genuine.
In this view, a costly social capital signal should strengthen the impact of a positive
psychological capital signal on crowdfunding performance. When standing alone, positive
psychological capital signals reside in a noisy environment where potential investors may have
trouble interpreting the genuineness of one’s hope, optimism, confidence, and resilience.
Therefore, while it may be perceived as a positive signal by some, others may be less inclined to
accept positive psychological capital language at face value. Because social capital signals that
an entrepreneur is trustworthy and credible, it suggests that expressions of positive psychological
capital are genuine, and therefore are reliable. Thus, the presence of social capital signals should
strengthen the influence of positive psychological capital on crowdfunding performance.
Accordingly, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 2. Social capital moderates the relationship between positive psychological capital
language and crowdfunding performance such that increases in social capital strengthen the
relationship between use of positive psychological capital language and crowdfunding
performance.
3.5 The moderating influence of human capital
Projections of human capital provide another well-established, costly signal important to
entrepreneurial fundraising (e.g., Ahlers et al., 2015; Baum and Silverman, 2004). Human capital
represents the capabilities possessed by an individual or team, such as the knowledge and skills
of the individuals launching the venture (Martin et al., 2013). These skills are often obtained
from costly investments such as obtaining an education, acquiring experience in an industry, or
developing experience through starting or growing a new business (Martin et al., 2013). The cost
of developing human capital signals suggests to investors that an entrepreneur has abilities that
make him or her more capable of successfully launching and operating a new business (e.g.,
Bruns et al., 2008).
18
While positive psychological capital language provides an indication that the
entrepreneur has characteristics associated with entrepreneurial success, human capital provides
tangible evidence of past success. For example, entrepreneurial experience provides investors
with an indication that an entrepreneur can successfully launch and grow a venture. Thus, a
human capital signal provides evidence that an entrepreneur’s confidence or optimism is
warranted because of past successes. Specifically, signaling human capital and positive
psychological capital simultaneously suggests that the entrepreneur not only has the mental
hardiness to execute on a proposed venture, but has a track record of doing so. Therefore, costly
human capital signals indicate to investors that the positive psychological capital displayed by
the entrepreneur is a reliable signal. It follows that human capital signals should strengthen the
influence of positive psychological capital. Accordingly, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 3. Human capital moderates the relationship between positive psychological capital
language and crowdfunding performance such that increases in human capital strengthen the
relationship between use of positive psychological capital language and crowdfunding
performance.
4.0 Methods
To examine the role of positive psychological capital in crowdfunding performance we
collected two random samples of crowdfunding campaigns drawn from the Kickstarter
crowdfunding platform. Kickstarter is a rewards-based crowdfunding platform, one of the top
two crowdfunding websites by volume, and has provided over USD 2.8 billion to more than
117,888 successfully funded campaigns (Kickstarter, 2017a). We drew part of our sample from a
list of 45,815 crowdfunding campaigns that were created before June 2, 2012. This sample was
originally collected in 2013 and 900 campaigns were randomly selected. This sample maximizes
comparability to recent examinations of crowdfunding phenomena using the same sampling time
frame (e.g., Mollick, 2014). From these 900 campaigns, two suspended campaigns and three
canceled campaigns were eliminated leaving a sample of 895. In addition, we collected a more
recent sample from 2016. In late 2012, Kickstarter made changes to the way crowdfunding
19
appeals must be presented on the platform (Kickstarter, 2017b). For example, projects must
include descriptions of potential risks that may impede the completion of a project and
restrictions on the use of simulations for hardware products were also released (Kickstarter,
2017b). Thus, these changes in how projects must be presented suggest a need for a more recent
sample that reflects the current requirements for how a new venture must be pitched. From the
projects created in 2016, we selected 1,000 campaigns to examine bringing the total number of
campaigns in our sample to 1,895. Once observations with missing data were removed, we were
left with 1,726 campaigns to analyze, with 48% of the observations coming before June 2, 2012
and 52% of the observations coming after this date.
4.1 Dependent variables
We examine the effect of positive psychological capital on two outcomes of interest in
crowdfunding research: whether the project’s funding target was met and the total amount of
funds raised. On Kickstarter, a funding target is set at the beginning of the campaign. If the
funding target is not met over the duration of the campaign, the investors are refunded and the
entrepreneur receives no funds (Kickstarter, 2017b). This ensures that meeting the funding target
is a salient crowdfunding performance outcome for organizations. This also enhances
comparability with other venture finance research that uses funding success as a dependent
variable (e.g., Batjargal, 2007; Davis et al., 2017). If at the end of the campaign, the funds raised
were greater than or equal to the funding target, a value of 1 was assigned to the success variable.
If the funds raised were less than the funding target, a value of 0 was assigned. Past venture
funding and crowdfunding research has also used continuous measures for the amount of money
invested in an entrepreneurial firm as a measure of evaluating funding performance (e.g.,
Cholakova and Clarysse, 2015; Gompers, 1995; Li et al., 2017). In line with this research, we
also operationalize crowdfunding performance as a continuous variable measuring the amount of
money committed to the project by investors called amount raised.
4.2 Independent Variables
20
Language associated with positive psychological capital was measured using computer-
aided text analysis (e.g., McKenny et al., 2013). Computer-aided text analysis is a member of the
content analysis family and measures the salience of constructs based on the frequency with
which words are used in a text (McKenny et al., 2016; Short et al., 2017b). For example, scholars
have examined innovativeness rhetoric by identifying the frequency with which words such as
‘innovation’ and ‘creativity’ are used within organizational texts (e.g., Moss et al., 2015). Each
instance of these words or other words associated with innovativeness would increment the
innovativeness construct by one.
We measured positive psychological capital using the word lists developed and validated
by McKenny and colleagues (2013). One word list was created for each of the positive
psychological capital dimensions: hope, optimism, resilience, and confidence. Example words
from the resilience word list include “adamant”, “dogged”, and “resolute” (McKenny et al.,
2013). We examine the project descriptions for each crowdfunding campaign and use the
DICTION 7.0 (Hart and Carroll, 2014) software to provide counts of each of the four
dimensions. Because positive psychological capital is a superordinate higher-order construct
(Luthans et al., 2007), we operationalized positive psychological capital as the sum of the results
from each of the four dimensions to provide a single positive psychological capital variable for
each crowdfunding text (e.g., Luthans et al., 2008; McKenny et al., 2013). For example, if a
profile description used 3 instances of words from the hope dictionary, 2 from the optimism
dictionary, 4 from the resilience dictionary, and 6 from the confidence dictionary, the total
positive psychological capital score would be 15. Appendix A illustrates the language associated
with each dictionary using crowdfunding examples.
4.3 Interaction variables
Our study examines four interaction variables: two reflecting social capital and two
reflecting human capital. To operationalize social capital, we follow prior precedent in
crowdfunding work and use the number of projects backed by the entrepreneur (e.g., Colombo et
al., 2015). By funding the projects of others, entrepreneurs can build social capital within
21
crowdfunding communities. In addition, the signaling literature has measured costly social
capital signals by examining endorsement effects, notably, the relationships between the
entrepreneur(s) and prestigious third parties, such as important partnerships, endorsements, or
sponsors (e.g., Khoury et al., 2013; Ozmel et al., 2013). To code for endorsements, we searched
for permutations of the words “partner”, “endorse”, and “sponsor” to identify campaigns
potentially highlighting important relationships with third parties. Each campaign was then
individually inspected to determine if the entrepreneurs were referring to a specific third-party
relationship. For those that highlighted an important relationship we coded an endorse variable
as ‘1’ and ‘0’ otherwise.
Entrepreneurial experience is commonly used as a costly indicator of human capital (e.g.,
Dimov and Shepherd, 2005). As such, an entrepreneurial experience variable was created using
a dummy variable coded ‘1’ for lead entrepreneurs with functional experience in the same or
similar context of the current venture and coded ‘0’ otherwise (Davis et al., 2017). The second
human capital variable captures crowdfunding experience. Individuals that have launched past
crowdfunding campaigns have incurred expenses (time, rewards costs, Kickstarter fees, etc.)
making the launch of a campaign costly. Further, entrepreneurs who have launched previous
campaigns have had opportunities to learn about what is needed to successfully raise funds as
well as deliver on promised rewards or products (Belleflamme et al., 2013). Therefore,
entrepreneurs with crowdfunding experience may be perceived as better able to deliver on
campaign promises. To operationalize crowdfunding experience, we use the number of past
campaigns launched by the entrepreneur represented by the created variable.
4.4 Controls
To account for the effects of other antecedents of crowdfunding performance, we
included several control variables. Crowdfunding research has found that the categories of
products or services differ in their ability to raise funds (e.g., Allison et al., 2015). To isolate
these effects, we controlled for category using the fifteen project categories available on
Kickstarter. Crowdfunding research has also found that the structure of the crowdfunding
22
campaign selected by the entrepreneur can influence crowdfunding success (e.g., Mollick, 2014).
To capture these differences, we controlled for the effect of the funding goal and campaign
duration. Because crowdfunding continues to evolve over time and Kickstarter has made
changes to how projects can be pitched since its inception, it is important to control for when a
campaign was conducted. Our sample includes projects from five years: 2009, 2010, 2011, 2012,
and 2016. Dummy variables are included for years 2010, 2011, 2012 and 2016 with 2009 being
the excluded dummy variable.
We introduced a number of controls identified by past research indicative of campaign
quality. Specifically, we control the inclusion of a video (video = 1; no video = 0), the direct
effect of entrepreneurial experience, the direct effect of past campaigns created by the
entrepreneur, the number of Facebook friends, and the word length of the campaign (e.g., Davis
et al., 2017; Parhankangas and Renko, 2017). We included an additional human capital control,
education, using a dummy variable coded ‘1’ for lead entrepreneurs who possessed a master's
degree or above and otherwise coded ‘0’ (e.g., Davis et al., 2017). If a campaign was featured
staff pick (meaning that it has been identified by the Kickstarter staff as a project they support), a
staff pick variable was coded as ‘1’, while other campaigns were coded as ‘0’. We use the
numerical terms CATA word list provided by the DICTION software program, as such language
highlights reliance on specific, objective data rather than abstract goals. We code for an outside
web presence with a web variable coded ‘1’ when the campaign provided a link to a formal
outside website and ‘0’ otherwise. Finally, sex and ethnicity of entrepreneurs may influence
funding preferences (e.g., Davis et al., 2017). Entrepreneur sex was controlled with a dummy
variable coded ‘1’ for campaigns led by a male entrepreneur and coded ‘0’ for ventures led by a
female entrepreneur. Likewise, an ethnicity dummy variable was coded ‘1’ for entrepreneurs that
appear Caucasian and otherwise coded ‘0’.
4.5 Statistical Analysis
Our amount raised dependent variable, positive psychological capital variable, two
variables used for interactions (created and backed), and several control variables followed a
23
right skewed, gamma distribution, which presents analytical challenges. For variables with non-
zero values (e.g., funding goal) we used a natural log transformation to correct for this skewness.
However, many of the skewed variables had zero values in the data preventing us from using the
natural log transformation. In these cases, we use an inverse hyperbolic sine transformation:
sinh-1(y) = log(yi+(yi2+1)1/2) (e.g., Franke and Richey, 2010; Nyberg et al., 2010). The inverse
hyperbolic sine transformation has two benefits. First, it allows us to correct for right skew in the
data, mitigating the influence of extreme observations (Bonaccorsi et al., 2013; Sauerwald et al.,
2016). Second, the interpretation of a variable transformed using this method is identical to the
natural log interpretation (Burbidge et al., 1988). Thus, this transformation allows us to interpret
variables transformed using the natural log and those using the inverse hyperbolic sine
transformation in the same way. Positive psychological capital, amount raised, created, Facebook
friends, and backed were all transformed using the inverse hyperbolic sine transformation.
We use two different statistical procedures to test our hypotheses. Because funding
success in crowdfunding is a dichotomous variable, we use logistic regression to test for the
influence of positive psychological capital on crowdfunding success. For our other dependent
variable, amount raised, we use generalized linear modeling (GLM). GLM is a generalization of
linear regression that allows for dependent variables that have an error distribution other than a
normal distribution and are estimated using maximum likelihood (McCullagh, 1984). After
examining our error distributions, we found them to be approximately normal and, thus, made no
changes to models to account for non-normality. Robust standard errors are used in all models.
5.0 Results
Before testing our hypotheses, we conducted a factor analysis to empirically assess the
appropriateness of operationalizing our positive psychological capital measure as a composite of
the content analytic scores for each of the four dimensions (e.g., Anglin et al., 2017). One factor
was retained (eigenvalue = 3.23) that explained approximately 81% of the variance in the
positive psychological capital variable and the factor loadings were as follows: hope = 0.93,
optimism = 0.95, resilience = 0.95, and confidence = 0.74. We then conducted a parallel
24
analysis, which considers normal sampling error when determining how many and which factors
should be retained (Fabrigar et al., 1999; Ruscio and Roche, 2012). The adjusted eigenvalue was
3.15 with an estimated bias of 0.08. Both analyses suggest treating positive psychological capital
as a composite of hope, optimism, resilience, and confidence is appropriate.1 The results of these
analyses are provided in Table 1 in Appendix B and a scree plot of the eigenvalues for the factor
analysis are provided in Appendix C.
All tables and figures showing our results are provided in Appendix B. Table 2 provides
the descriptive statistics for our sample, Table 3 provides the correlations for our sample, and
Figure 1 summarizes our theoretical model and the operationalization of each signal. Table 4
presents the results for the funding success dependent variable. We provide both the log odds
coefficients and the average marginal effects in Table 4. The average marginal effects (AME) —
the average change in probability for a given change in x — are particularly useful for
interpreting interactions in logistic regressions as interactions between log odds or odds ratios do
not lend themselves to intuitive interpretations (Plummer et al., 2016). Table 5 provides the
results for the amount raised variable.
Hypothesis 1 suggested that positive psychological capital will be positively related to
funding performance in crowdfunding. The coefficient for funding success was positive and
significant (b = 0.30, odds ratio = 1.37, p < 0.01; AME = 0.05, p < 0.01) and the coefficient for
amount raised is positive and significant (b = 0.34, p < 0.01), providing support for Hypothesis 1.
These results indicate that a 10% increase in the use of positive psychological capital would be
associated with an approximate 3% increase in the probability of success and an approximate
3.4% increase in the amount of funds raised. If we evaluate these effects at the sample means for
positive psychological capital (mean = 21.72) and our dependent variables (success = 0.44; funds
raised = 8721.16), a 10% increase in positive psychological capital would be associated with a
change in the success rate from 44% to 45.32% and an additional $296.52 raised.
1 At the request of an anonymous reviewer, we conducted an analysis of each positive psychological capital dimension
individually and its relationship to crowdfunding performance. We will be happy to provide the results upon request.
25
Hypothesis 2 suggested that an interaction between social capital and positive
psychological capital will be positively with associated crowdfunding performance. The
coefficient in the funding success models for both social capital variables were not significant
(psychological capital × backed: b = 0.03, odds ratio = 1.03, p > 0.05, AME = 0.00, p > 0.05;
psychological capital × endorse: b = -0.46, odds ratio = 0.63, p > 0.05, AME = -0.07, p > 0.05).
Likewise, the coefficient in amount raised models for both social capital variables were not
significant (psychological capital × backed: b = 0.02, p > 0.05; psychological capital × endorse:
b = -0.09, p > 0.05). Thus, Hypothesis 2 is not supported.
Hypothesis 3 suggested that an interaction between human capital and positive
psychological capital will be positively associated with crowdfunding performance. Both
interaction terms using the human capital variables for the success models were positive and
significant (psychological capital × created: b = 0.39, odds ratio = 1.47, p < 0.01, AME = 0.06, p
< 0.01; psychological capital × experience: b = 0.28, odds ratio = 1.32, p < 0.05, AME = 0.04, p
< 0.01), supporting Hypothesis 3. The marginal effect at the mean of positive psychological
capital for an entrepreneur who has created two past campaigns (i.e., the approximate mean of
previous campaigns) is 0.45. Because the positive psychological capital and created variables
had previously been transformed, logged values are used to compute the effect at the mean of
positive psychological capital and two past campaigns. These results suggest that a 10% increase
in the use of positive psychological capital language for an entrepreneur who has launched two
previous campaigns is associated with an additional 4.5 percentage points, which is a success
rate of approximately 48.5%. The marginal effect at the mean of positive psychological capital
for an entrepreneur with experience (entrepreneurial experience = 1) is 0.54. Therefore, using the
positive psychological capital mean as a starting point, a 10% increase in the use of positive
psychological capital language for someone with entrepreneurial experience is associated with an
additional 5.4 percentage points, which is a success rate of approximately 49.4%. Both
interaction terms for the amount raised models were positive and significant (psychological
capital × created: b = 0.23, p < 0.01; psychological capital × entrepreneurial experience: b =
26
0.41, p < 0.01), supporting Hypothesis 3. In practical terms, a 10% increase positive
psychological capital language when paired with the launch of two previous campaigns is
associated with an extra 6.29% in amount raised, which equates to $548.56 using the mean of
amount raised as a reference point. Likewise, entrepreneurial experience was associated with an
additional 4.1% in amount raised for a 10% increase in positive psychological capital language
— an additional $357.57 when using the mean of amount raised as a reference point.2
6.0 Generalizability of findings
Given the heterogeneity among crowdfunding types, we sought to test the generalizability
of our finding that positive psychological capital language is a salient predictor of crowdfunding
performance. We constructed random sample of 1,726 crowdfunding campaigns drawn from the
Kiva website. Kiva is the world's first and one of the largest debt-based crowdfunding websites,
having facilitated over USD 1.07 billion in loans to over 2.6 million entrepreneurs (Kiva, 2017).
Kiva’s business model focuses on assisting economically-disadvantaged entrepreneurs from
around the world. In this model, socially-minded individuals agree to fund a portion of a
microloan given to an entrepreneur (Allison et al., 2013). No interest is received for making this
loan, but there is an expectation of being repaid the principal. Kiva profiles do not include
videos, but include a written appeal and picture of the entrepreneur.
To examine crowdfunding performance on the Kiva platform, we examined three
dependent variables. We examine funding success and amount raised for comparability with our
Kickstarter results. In addition, several studies examining the Kiva platform evaluate the rate at
which a crowdfunding a campaign achieves its goals (e.g., Allison et al., 2015; Anglin et al.,
2014). For comparability with these studies, we assess funding speed, measured by the number
days it takes the meet the funding goal (e.g., Allison et al., 2013). We introduce several context-
specific controls. We included a control for campaigns that indicated involvement with group
lending. Group lending occurs when a borrower is placed within a lending group where all
2 Our results are robust to project size, the choice of controls, and modeling choices. These robustness tests are available from the
authors upon request.
27
members of group are all responsible for the repayment of all loans taken by other members of
that group (Brau and Woller, 2004). This method has been shown to increase repayment rates
and is seen as a signal of higher quality in microfinance (Armendáriz de Aghion and Morduch,
2000). We also included country controls for Kiva campaigns (e.g. Allison et al., 2013). Kiva
borrowers are dispersed throughout the world and prior research has suggested that geographic
location may drive funding outcomes (Allison et al., 2015). We inserted 42 dummy variables to
control for the 43 countries in our sample. In the Kiva campaigns, the duration is fixed to thirty
days. In addition, no entrepreneurs utilized existing websites, linked to Facebook, past
campaigns created are not reported, no entrepreneurs had Master’s degrees, and entrepreneurial
experience was not discussed. Thus, these controls were not included. The lack of this
information is likely reflective of the fact that individuals seeking microloans are often
impoverished, therefore opportunities for education, website creation, and other signals of
quality are quite rare.
In estimating the logistic models for the success variable, we encountered separation
issues with some of the country dummy variables. Separation occurs in logistic regression when
an independent or control variable perfectly predicts the dependent variable (Menard, 1995).
Failure to correct for separation issues may lead to biased parameter estimates and model
misspecification (Hosmer Jr et al., 2013). To fit these models and in order to ensure conservative
parameter estimates while minimizing bias, we use Firth’s method of penalized maximum
likelihood estimation for the logistic models (Firth, 1993).
Appendix C presents the results for this analysis. The positive psychological capital
variable was a significant predictor of performance when examining amount raised (b = 0.02; p
< 0.05) and when examining funding speed (b = -0.08; p < 0.01). Note that a negative coefficient
for funding speed indicates that funding took fewer days, therefore is indicative of a positive
effect. The coefficient for funding success b = -0.07; p > 0.05), was not significant. In all, we
find evidence to support our primary argument that positive psychological capital should be a
salient predictor of funding performance for two of the three performance variables.
28
6.1 Generalizability to traditional entrepreneurial fundraising sources: Initial public offerings
Our theory suggests that costless signals may be valuable sources of information to
crowdfunding investors because there are few objective costly signals available and investors are
not as sophisticated as in traditional investment settings. However, in situations where more
objective information is available and investors are more sophisticated, we expect that costless
signals should be less salient. For instance, Chen and colleagues (2009) demonstrated that
entrepreneur preparedness for business plan presentations, which would be considered costly
under the general tenets of signaling theory, leads to more positive evaluations from venture
capitalists, while entrepreneurial passion, which would be considered costless, has no influence
on venture capitalist evaluations. We sought to examine this boundary condition by testing our
hypothesis in a non-crowdfunding fundraising sample where objective information is more
readily available and investors are sophisticated. We selected a second post hoc sample of
companies that underwent an initial public offering (IPO) in the years 2011, 2012, or 2013 to
conduct this test. Just as key investment considerations in crowdfunding are conveyed by a
crowdfunding campaign text, in IPOs key investment considerations are also communicated via
a key fundraising text: the IPO prospectus (Arthurs et al., 2008). Studying the drivers of IPO
performance has been a central focus in both strategic management and entrepreneurship
research (e.g., Kroll et al., 2007). Thus, by including IPOs in our analysis we provide a reference
point to the existing literature on funding performance in which to compare the crowdfunding
results. We initially identified 560 US IPOs that occurred during this time period that had a
published prospectus. Using EdgarPro, Yahoo Finance, and Compustat data, we compiled
complete data on 432 of these IPOs for our analysis.
We examine the effect of positive psychological capital on a primary outcome of interest
in the IPO context, IPO underpricing (e.g., Daily et al., 2003; Pollock and Rindova, 2003). IPO
underpricing captures the difference between the offer price received by firm owners compared
to the closing price on the first day of trading and is a commonly used measure when
investigating short-term IPO performance (Certo et al., 2009). IPO underpricing is measured as
29
((Stock Price - Offer Price)/Offer Price) X 100 on the first day a stock traded on a national
exchange. We also introduced several context-specific controls associated IPO performance: We
controlled for organizational size operationalized by company revenues and by the number of
firm employees (e.g., Sanders and Boivie, 2004). Given that economic conditions in a given year
influence the valuation of IPOs in that year, we included dummy variables for the years 2012 and
2013 to control for the three IPO years in our sample (e.g., Payne et al., 2013). The market
exchange platform (e.g., NYSE) has been identified as a factor in IPO success (e.g., Moore et al.,
2012). Accordingly, we introduce dummy variables for each of the four market exchanges
facilitating the IPO. Finally, we controlled for industry effects using the first two digits of an
organization’s SIC code by inserting 45 dummy controls for the 46 industries in our study.
Appendix D provides the results for our IPO sample. When examining underpricing, a
negative coefficient suggests a positive relationship with fundraising performance because it
reduces IPO underpricing (Payne et al., 2013). The coefficient for positive psychological capital
in the model was negative, but not significant (b
= -0.05; p > 0.05) suggesting that positive
psychological capital language may not play a role in funding performance in the IPO context.
6.2 Positive psychological capital language in video transcriptions
Many campaigns include videos of the founders showcasing the product or service to
generate interest in the campaign (Mollick, 2014). Because a video provides an additional
opportunity to signal positive psychological capital to potential investors, in this post hoc we test
whether the positive psychological capital language use in videos also leads to crowdfunding
performance. 562 campaigns were professionally transcribed to produce an exact transcript of
the video. After accounting for missing data in our controls, we conducted our analysis on 527
campaigns using the same models as in our primary analysis. Surprisingly, positive
psychological capital in the video transcriptions has no influence on either performance variable
(success: b = -0.03, p > 0.05; amount raised: b = -0.35, p > 0.05).
30
6.3 The influence of positive psychological capital over time3
Our sample examines crowdfunding campaigns that were launched across several years.
Given that crowdfunding is a continually evolving phenomenon, it is probable the influence of
positive psychological capital has continued to evolve over time. To test this notion, we take two
steps. First, we separate the 2009-2012 and the 2016 samples and estimate the impact of positive
psychological capital on each sample. We found no significant relationships in the earlier sample
(success: b = 0.15, p > 0.05; amount raised: b = 0.10, p > 0.05), but did find significant
relationships in the 2016 sample (success: b = 0.48, p < 0.01; amount raised: b = 0.67, p < 0.01).
The effect sizes for positive psychological capital in the 2016 sample are also substantially larger
than the estimated effects sizes using the entire sample. These results might indicate that our
results are driven entirely by the 2016 sample. Alternatively, the results might indicate that
positive psychological capital is gaining in importance over time.
To further investigate how the importance of positive psychological capital may have
changed over time, we interact the positive psychological capital variable with the year dummies
and estimated the models using the combined sample. A table of the interaction terms is provided
in Appendix E. For the success dependent variable, we find a significant positive direct effect
that is weakened in early years and disappears in later years. For the funds raised models, we
find a non-significant main effect but positive and significant interactions that increase in size for
the three latter years in our sample. The joint effects in both models are the same however:
positive psychological capital increases in importance over time. Thus, the results of this analysis
provide evidence that the importance of displaying positive psychological capital has increased
as crowdfunding has continued to evolve.
7.0 Discussion
Our study demonstrates that language indicative of positive psychological capital is an
important costless signal tied to fundraising outcomes for entrepreneurs raising funds through
3 We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting that we explore how time impacts the influence of
positive psychological capital.
31
crowdfunding. We add to the signaling literature by providing evidence that positive
psychological capital may be an important costless signal, particularly in non-traditional
fundraising contexts like crowdfunding where information is scarce, there is no formal vetting
process, and investors are often less sophisticated. Indeed, past work in crowdfunding has
implied the importance of costless signals in crowdfunding (Davis et al., 2017; Li et al., 2017).
However, the literature has yet to leverage a costless signaling lens to expand our knowledge of
such signals. By adopting a costless signaling lens, a key implication of our work is that an
understanding of the drivers of crowdfunding performance requires a deeper understanding of
costless signals and how they lead to performance.
Our work contributes to the emerging work in strategic management and
entrepreneurship examining the interaction of signals (e.g., Plummer et al., 2016; Stern et al.,
2014). Signaling research has mostly examined signals in isolation (Connelly et al., 2011; Drover
et al., 2018). However, in practice, signals rarely occur in isolation and signals are often
accompanied by other signals that may alter the influence of each (Stern et al., 2014). As such,
exploring the interaction of signals allows for a more accurate depiction of how signals relate to
organizational outcomes in naturally occurring settings. Our study provides evidence that costly
human capital signals strengthen the positive effect of costless positive psychological capital
signals, indicating that signals representative of capabilities (e.g., entrepreneurial experience) and
signals representative of motivation and psychological strength work together in facilitating
crowdfunding performance. Thus, an entrepreneur likely to successfully raise funds in
crowdfunding is one that can simultaneously demonstrate both evidence of past success and a
positive, motivated mindset.
While the interactions between human capital and positive psychological capital language
significantly impacted crowdfunding performance, social capital did not alter this relationship.
One explanation for this unexpected outcome concerns the differences in information provided
by human capital versus social capital signals. Human capital signals provide insight into
entrepreneurs’ individual capabilities (Marvel et al., 2016) and positive psychological capital
32
signals provide insight into psychological strengths. Individual capabilities and psychological
qualities are often closely related (e.g., Dimov, 2010), thus combining these signals provides a
more complete picture of the individual entrepreneurs (e.g., one that is skillful, resilient, and
motivated). In contrast, social capital signals capture ‘outside’ information about an entrepreneur
provided by others (Khoury et al., 2013). Although these signals provide endorsement or social
proof effects, they do not necessarily provide additional information concerning an
entrepreneurs’ individual characteristics (i.e., capabilities or mindsets). Instead, they tell
investors how others might view the entrepreneur. Thus, while important in evaluating the
overall potential of an entrepreneur, it is probable that these signals are considered separately
from signals pertaining to individual qualities. Theoretically, this suggests that for costly signals
to enhance the influence of costless signals, the costly signals must add more than just additional
information, they must add information that is complementary to the costless signal, allowing for
the construction of a more complete picture of valued signaler qualities. In our case, the
inclusion of human capital signals allows for a more complete picture of the entrepreneur’s
individual qualities. Together, this begins to trace out a more nuanced relationship of how
multiple signals interact.
Our post hoc analyses provide insight into important boundary conditions of our study.
We find evidence that our key finding – that positive psychological capital leads to better
fundraising performance – generalizes from our Kickstarter data to other types of crowdfunding.
However, it does not generalize to more traditional means of fundraising such as IPOs. While
exploratory, the results of the post hoc analyses are also consistent with the notion that costless
signals are more salient in contexts where there is less objective information (e.g., Lin et al.,
2013). The findings also add to the small but growing literature suggesting that indications of
personal characteristics are particularly important in contexts where investors lack sophisticated
risk assessment routines and investment preferences are largely taste-based (e.g., rewards-based
crowdfunding, peer-to-peer lending, microfinance; Ciuchta et al., 2016; Davis et al., 2017).
33
We were surprised by the absence of a relationship between positive psychological
capital in the video transcriptions and crowdfunding performance. Though this was a post hoc
test, it appears to clash with the premise that videos are a key part of crowdfunding campaigns
(e.g., Mollick, 2014). However, recent crowdfunding research that draws from the Elaboration
Likelihood Model (ELM) suggests that potential crowdfunding backers may go through a two-
step process when evaluating a crowdfunding campaign (Allison et al., 2017). This work
indicates that investors may initially be in a ‘low elaboration’ (i.e., low attention) state wherein
they are primarily responsive to surface-level cues, such as exciting graphics or an enthusiastic
presentation (Allison et al., 2017). Because videos appear at the top of most crowdfunding pages,
this is the state most backers are likely to be in when viewing the included video. If the video
grabs their attention, they may then switch to a ‘high elaboration’ (i.e., high attention) state,
where they more thoroughly evaluate other materials in the crowdfunding campaign, such as the
narrative. It is possible that because the video serves as a sorting function that other costless
signals, such as emotional displays (e.g., Li et al., 2017), may be more salient in garnering initial
investor attention. Moreover, because videos also contain non-verbal cues that are easily
observed by viewers but are not present in the written narratives, costless signals embedded in
written language are more salient once backers begin to evaluate the narratives. However, we
still know little about how signals embedded in videos relate to signals embedded in texts, which
provides opportunities for future research. For example, future research could juxtapose insights
from signaling theory and the ELM to tease out which costless signals are most salient in
grabbing an investor’s initial attention and which signals play a stronger role in the narratives
once the campaign has captured the attention of investors. Such research could also explore the
interactions between signals in videos and signals in narratives to better understand how these
signals may act as complements or substitutes for one another. In a broader vein, future research
should carefully examine the impact of visual aesthetics inherent to videos or other graphic
content that might otherwise impact crowdfunding performance.
34
Finally, our post hoc analyses indicate that the influence of positive psychological capital
language on crowdfunding performance has increased over time. We believe this result is
explained by the increasing demand for crowdfunding (e.g., Assadi, 2015; Massolution, 2015).
As demand for crowdfunding increases, entrepreneurs face greater pressure to distinguish
themselves from one another and show that they are worthy of investor funds. Because
displaying positive psychological capital provides a means for entrepreneurs to demonstrate their
worth, it follows that such displays would become more important as pressures to display one’s
worth increases. More broadly, these results also suggest that the influence of signals in
crowdfunding is not constant over time. However, to date, little work has investigated the
evolution of crowdfunding signals. Thus, we encourage future researchers to adopt a temporal
perspective to examining signals in crowdfunding to provide insight into how the influence of
signals change as crowdfunding becomes more mature.
7.1 Limitations and future research
The contributions of our research should be understood in light of the study’s limitations.
One of the challenges in crowdfunding research is the lack of data on the individual investors in
the campaigns (McKenny et al., 2017). As a result, while our results indicate that crowdfunding
campaigns with language indicative of positive psychological capital tend to outperform those
without, we cannot directly measure the impact of this language on the decision making of
individual investors using field data. However, this limitation presents an opportunity for future
research to build on our findings by using experimental designs where investor preferences can
directly be accounted for and measured (e.g., Davis et al., 2017; Drover et al., 2017b). Such work
would allow researchers to more intricately tease out the decision processes used by investors.
For example, researchers could ask potential investors to weight the signals used in making their
decisions to determine the relative importance of signals such as human, social, and positive
psychological capital.
Our work provides evidence that costless signals enable crowdfunding performance.
However, it remains unclear whether these costless signals are truly indicative of the underlying
35
quality of the firm. Given the prevalence of costless signals in crowdfunding, if costless signals
are not indicative of firm quality but can be used to drive investment then crowdfunding
platforms could become mechanisms where financial resources flow to low-quality firms. This
would lead to a waste of resources and undercut the value of crowdfunding in providing financial
capital to promising new ventures. Accordingly, it is critical that future research examine
relationships between firm quality and costless signals. For example, research examining how
crowdfunding campaigns deliver on the promises made during campaigns remains are rare (e.g.,
Mollick, 2014). Future research could examine the presence of positive psychological capital or
entrepreneurial passion in campaigns and examine the extent to which these campaigns deliver
on campaign promises. Further, future research could examine if costless signals predict other
indicators of quality, such as future growth of the firm or subsequent capital raises from
professional investors.
In addition, while we address how human and social capital interact with positive
psychological capital, we do not investigate how these forms of capital were acquired. For
example, social capital may enable the development of human capital (e.g., Coleman, 1988) and
positive psychological capital can be developed through experience (Newman et al., 2014).
Accordingly, more work remains in investigating the relationships among the different forms of
people-related capital in crowdfunding. For example, future work could examine how the
completion of previous crowdfunding campaigns leads to changes in the use of positive
psychological capital language in future crowdfunding campaigns.
Recent work examining the drivers of crowdfunding performance has found that positive
emotional displays, notably entrepreneurial passion, may lead to greater crowdfunding
performance (e.g., Davis et al., 2017; Li et al., 2017). While positive psychological capital is
distinct from emotional constructs like passion (e.g., Avey et al., 2008; 2010; Luthans et al.,
2007), thematically, passion and positive psychological capital overlap in that they both provide
an individual with increased motivation (e.g., Cardon et al., 2009; Luthans et al., 2007). Work in
organizational behavior suggests that positive psychological capital and emotions often work
36
together to facilitate desirable outcomes, such as goal achievement and job performance
(Norman et al., 2005). Future research might build from this work to examine how displays of
passion and displays of positive psychological capital collectively influence crowdfunding
performance. For instance, passion is often displayed by outwardly visible manifestations of
emotion, such as an energetic tone of voice or expressive body language (Li et al., 2017). A
future study might examine the interaction between an energetic tone of voice and positive
psychological capital language and the resulting influence on crowdfunding performance.
Further, because those high in positive psychological capital often experience more positive
emotions, future research might examine if an entrepreneur’s positive psychological capital is
predictive of the level of passion shown in a crowdfunding campaign4.
Entrepreneurs often use various mediums to convey their message on crowdfunding
platforms, including videos, pictures, and textual narratives (Drover et al., 2017a). Given that
prior research has shown that the mere presence of a video has the potential to influence funding
outcomes (Josefy et al., 2017; Mollick, 2014), future research should consider how these
mediums can be used independently to signal quality to potential backers. Indeed, prior work has
shown that subtle changes in visual content can alter the effectiveness of the message and the
behaviors of resource providers (e.g., Chan and Park, 2015; Pollack et al., 2012). Future
crowdfunding studies that leverage prior work on the content analysis of multimedia, including
audio tones, photos, and video content could further extend our work on linguistic content by
considering the relative effect of signals embedded within videos and images (e.g., Pope and
Sydnor, 2011). Further, entrepreneur updates and backer comments represent a dialogue between
the entrepreneur and the crowd that may provide researchers with further insight into how the
dynamics between the entrepreneur and the crowd shape crowdfunding decisions. Future work
might examine how the linguistic content of updates and comments as well as entrepreneur
responsiveness to backer comments relates to crowdfunding performance.
4 We thank an anonymous reviewer for directing us to this important area of inquiry.
37
Our study indicates that certain costly signals and costless signals (i.e., human capital and
positive psychological capital, respectively) work together to facilitate crowdfunding
performance. However, it currently remains unclear whether this finding would hold across other
venture financing mediums. The value of individual signals is often contextual (Connelly et al.,
2011; Plummer et al., 2016), suggesting that the joint impact of signals is also likely contextual.
Further, costless signals may play a less salient role in fundraising situations where there is more
risk involved, investors are sophisticated, and more formal means of vetting exist (e.g., venture
capital, IPO). It is possible that in contexts where costless signals are less relevant, investors may
continue to ignore low cost signals even if they are accompanied by costly signals. Therefore,
future research should continue to investigate how costly and costless signals may influence each
other in various contexts. For example, future research could examine how the presence of social
and human capital alter the influence of displaying of positive psychological capital in equity
crowdfunding, pitches to angel investors, or pitches to venture capitalists.
8.0 Conclusion
Our study is the first to investigate positive psychological capital language as an
important costless signal and explore its role in the entrepreneurial fundraising process. For
scholars, our study advances understanding of the determinants of successful crowdfunding
campaigns — introducing costless signaling as important theoretical lens for understanding
crowdfunding performance. Our study also underscores the importance of considering the
interactions of multiple signals, versus studying one signal in isolation. For entrepreneurs, our
research suggests that entrepreneurs would benefit from proactively signaling positive
psychological capital when raising funds through crowdfunding. We hope that these findings and
their associated implications lead to further academic inquiry regarding the role of positive
organizational phenomena in entrepreneurship.
38
References
Agrawal, A., Catalini, C., Goldfarb, A., 2014. Some simple economics of crowdfunding.
Innovation Policy and the Economy. 14, 63-97.
Ahlers, G.K., Cumming, D., Günther, C., Schweizer, D., 2015. Signaling in equity
crowdfunding. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 39, 955-980.
Allison, T.H., Davis, B.C., Short, J.C., Webb, J.W., 2015. Crowdfunding in a prosocial
microlending environment: Examining the role of intrinsic versus extrinsic cues.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 39, 53-73.
Allison, T.H., Davis, B.C., Webb, J.W., Short, J.C., 2017. Persuasion in Crowdfunding: An
Elaboration Likelihood Model of Crowdfunding Performance. Journal of Business
Venturing. 32, 707-725. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusvent.2017.09.002
Allison, T.H., McKenny, A.F., Short, J.C., 2013. The effect of entrepreneurial rhetoric on
microlending investment: An examination of the warm-glow effect. Journal of Business
Venturing. 28, 690-707.
Allon, G., Bassamboo, A., Gurvich, I., 2011. “We will be right with you”: Managing customer
expectations with vague promises and cheap talk. Operations Research. 59, 1382-1394.
Anglin, A.H., Allison, T.H., McKenny, A.F., Busenitz, L.W., 2014. The role of charismatic
rhetoric in crowdfunding: An examination with computer-aided text analysis. In Social
Entrepreneurship and Research Methods (pp. 19-48). Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Anglin, A.H., Reid, S.W., Short, J.C., Zachary, M.A., Rutherford, M.W., 2017. An archival
approach to measuring family influence: An organizational identity perspective. Family
Business Review. doi: 0894486516669254.
Armendáriz de Aghion, B., Morduch, J., 2000. Microfinance beyond group lending. Economics
of Transition. 8, 401-420.
Arthurs, J.D., Hoskisson, R.E., Busenitz, L.W., Johnson, R.A., 2008. Managerial agents
watching other agents: Multiple agency conflicts regarding underpricing in IPO firms.
Academy of Management Journal. 51, 277-294.
Assadi, D., 2015. Strategic approaches to successful crowdfunding. IGI Global, pp.154-163.
Avey, J. B., Wernsing, T. S., Luthans, F. 2008. Can positive employees help positive
organizational change? Impact of psychological capital and emotions on relevant attitudes
and behaviors. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 44. 48-70.
Avey, J.B., Luthans, F., Youssef, C.M., 2010. The additive value of positive psychological
capital in predicting work attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Management. 36, 430-452.
Avey, J.B., Reichard, R.J., Luthans, F., Mhatre, K.H., 2011. Meta‐analysis of the impact of
positive psychological capital on employee attitudes, behaviors, and performance. Human
Resource Development Quarterly. 22, 127-152.
Awamleh, R., Gardner, W.L. 1999. Perceptions of leader charisma and effectiveness: The effects
of vision content, delivery, and organizational performance. The Leadership Quarterly. 10,
345-373.
Baginski, S., Demers, E., Wang, C., Yu, J., 2016. Contemporaneous verification of language:
evidence from management earnings forecasts. Review of Accounting Studies. 21, 165-197.
Balvers, R.J., Gaski, J.F., McDonald, B., 2016. Financial disclosure and customer satisfaction:
Do companies talking the talk actually walk the walk? Journal of Business Ethics. 139, 29-
45.
Barnett, C., 2015. Trends show crowdfunding to surpass VC in 2016. Retrieved on April 15,
2017, from Forbes.com.
39
Baron, R.A., Franklin, R.J., Hmieleski, K.M., 2016. Why entrepreneurs often experience low,
not high, levels of stress the joint effects of selection and psychological capital. Journal of
Management. 42, 742-768.
Batjargal, B., 2007. Network triads: transitivity, referral, and venture capital decisions in China
and Russia. Journal of International Business Studies. 38, 998–1012.
Baum, J.A., Silverman, B.S., 2004. Picking winners or building them? Alliance, intellectual, and
human capital as selection criteria in venture financing and performance of biotechnology
startups. Journal of Business Venturing. 19, 411-436.
Belleflamme, P., Lambert, T., Schwienbacher, A., 2013. Individual crowdfunding practices.
Venture Capital. 15, 313-333.
Belleflamme, P., Omrani, N., Peitz, M., 2015. The economics of crowdfunding platforms.
Information Economics and Policy. 33, 11-28.
Bergh, D.D., Connelly, B.L., Ketchen, D.J., Shannon, L.M., 2014. Signaling theory and
equilibrium in strategic management research: An assessment and a research agenda. Journal
of Management Studies. 51, 1334-1360.
Bhattacharya, U., Dittmar, A., 2001. Costless versus costly signaling: Theory and evidence from
share repurchases. Working paper, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN.
Bonaccorsi, A., Colombo, M.G., Guerini, M., Rossi-Lamastra, C., 2013. University
specialization and new firm creation across industries. Small Business Economics. 41, 837-
863.
Brau, J.C., Woller, G.M., 2004. Microfinance: A comprehensive review of the existing literature.
Journal of Entrepreneurial Finance. 9, 1-27.
Bruns, V., Holland, D.V., Shepherd, D.A., Wiklund, J., 2008. The role of human capital in loan
officers' decision policies. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 32, 485-506.
Burbidge, J. B., Magee, L., Robb, A.L., 1988. Alternative transformations to handle extreme
values of the dependent variable. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 83, 123-
127.
Busenitz, L.W., Fiet, J.O., Moesel, D.D., 2005. Signaling in venture capitalist—new venture
team funding decisions: Does it indicate long‐term venture outcomes? Entrepreneurship
Theory and Practice. 29, 1-12.
Calic, G., Mosakowski, E., 2016. Kicking off social entrepreneurship: How a sustainability
orientation influences crowdfunding success. Journal of Management Studies. 535, 738-767.
Cardon, M.S., Wincent, J., Singh, J., Drnovsek, M., 2009. The nature and experience of
entrepreneurial passion. Academy of Management Review. 34, 511-532.
Carpentier, C., Suret, J.M. 2015. Angel group members' decision process and rejection criteria:
A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Business Venturing. 30, 808-821.
Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., 2002. The hopeful optimist. Psychological Inquiry. 13, 288-290.
Certo, S.T., 2003. Influencing initial public offering investors with prestige: signaling with board
structures. Academy of Management Review. 28, 432–446.
Certo, S.T., Holcomb, T.R., Holmes, R.M., 2009. IPO research in management and
entrepreneurship: Moving the agenda forward. Journal of Management. 35, 1340-1378.
Chan, C.R., Park, H.D., 2015. How images and color in business plans influence venture
investment screening decisions. Journal of business Venturing. 30, 732-748.
Chemers, M.M., Watson, C.B., May, S. T. 2000. Dispositional affect and leadership
effectiveness: A comparison of self-esteem, optimism, and efficacy. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26(3), 267-277.
40
Chen, X.P., Yao, X., Kotha, S., 2009. Entrepreneur passion and preparedness in business plan
presentations: a persuasion analysis of venture capitalists' funding decisions. Academy of
Management Journal. 52: 199-214.
Cholakova, M., Clarysse, B., 2015. Does the possibility to make equity investments in
crowdfunding projects crowd out reward‐based investments? Entrepreneurship Theory and
Practice. 39, 145-172.
Ciuchta, M.P., Letwin, C., Stevenson, R.M., McMahon, S.R. 2016. Regulatory focus and
information cues in a crowdfunding context. Applied Psychology. 65, 490-514.
Ciuchta, M. P., Letwin, C., Stevenson, R. M., McMahon, S. R. 2017. Betting on the coachable
entrepreneur: Signalling and social exchange in entrepreneurial pitches. Entrepreneurship
Theory and Practice. doi: 1042258717725520.
Coleman, J.S., 1988. Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal
of Sociology, 94, S95–S120.
Colombo, M.G., Franzoni, C., Rossi‐Lamastra, C., 2015. Internal social capital and the attraction
of early contributions in crowdfunding. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 39, 75-100.
Conger, J.A., 1991. Inspiring others: The language of leadership. The Executive. 5, 31-45.
Connelly B.L., Certo, S.T., Ireland, R.D., Reutzel, C.R., 2011. Signaling theory: A review and
assessment. Journal of Management. 37, 39–67.
Courtney, C., Dutta, S., Li, Y., 2017. Resolving information asymmetry: Signaling, endorsement,
and crowdfunding success. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 41, 265-290.
Crawford, V.P., Sobel, J., 1982. Strategic information transmission. Econometrica: Journal of the
Econometric Society, 1431-1451.
Daily C.M., Certo S.T., Dalton, D.R., Roengpitya, R., 2003. IPO underpricing: a meta-analysis
and research synthesis. Entrepreneurship: Theory and Practice. 27, 271–295.
Danilov, A., Sliwka, D., 2016. Can contracts signal social norms? Experimental evidence.
Management Science. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2015.2336.
Davidsson, P., Honig, B., 2003. The role of social and human capital among nascent
entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing. 18, 301-331.
Davila, A., Foster, G., Gupta, M., 2003. Venture capital financing and the growth of startup
firms. Journal of Business Venturing. 18, 689-708.
Davis, B.C., Hmieleski, K.M., Webb, J.W., Coombs, J.E., 2017. Funders' positive affective
reactions to entrepreneurs' crowdfunding pitches: The influence of perceived product
creativity and entrepreneurial passion. Journal of Business Venturing. 32, 90-106.
Devaraj, S., Patel, P.C., 2016. Influence of number of backers, goal amount, and project duration
on meeting funding goals of crowdfunding projects. Economics Bulletin. 36, 1242-1249.
Dimov, D., 2010. Nascent entrepreneurs and venture emergence: Opportunity confidence, human
capital, and early planning. Journal of Management Studies. 47: 1123-1153.
Dimov, D.P., Shepherd, D.A., 2005. Human capital theory and venture capital firms: exploring
“home runs” and “strike outs”. Journal of Business Venturing. 20, 1-21.
Drover, W., Busenitz, L., Matusik, S., Townsend, D., Anglin, A., Dushnitsky, G., 2017a. A
review and road map of entrepreneurial equity financing research: Venture capital, corporate
venture capital, angel investment, crowdfunding, and accelerators. Journal of Management.
43, 1820–1853.
Drover, W., Wood, M., Corbett, A., 2018. Toward a cognitive view of signaling theory:
Individual attention and signal set interpretation. Journal of Management Studies. doi:
10.1111/joms.12282
41
Drover, W., Wood, M.S., Zacharakis, A., 2017b. Attributes of angel and crowdfunded
investments as determinants of VC screening decisions. Entrepreneurship Theory and
Practice. 41, 323-347.
Dushnitsky, G., 2010. Entrepreneurial optimism in the market for technological inventions.
Organization Science. 21, 150–167.
Eden, D., Aviram, A. 1993. Self-efficacy training to speed reemployment: Helping people to
help themselves. Journal of Applied Psychology. 78, 352-360.
Fabrigar, L.R., Wegener, D.T., MacCallum, R.C., Strahan, E.J., 1999. Evaluating the use of
exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods. 4, 272.
Firth, D., 1993. Bias reduction of maximum likelihood estimates. Biometrika. 80, 27–38.
Florin, J., Lubatkin, M., Schulze, W., 2003. A social capital model of high-growth ventures.
Academy of Management Journal, 46. 374-384.
Franke, G.R., Richey Jr, R.G., 2010. Improving generalizations from multi-country comparisons
in international business research. Journal of International Business Studies, 41, 1275-1293.
Friend, S.B., Johnson, J.S., Luthans, F., Sohi, R.S., 2016. Positive psychology in sales:
Integrating psychological capital. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice. 24, 306-327.
Gedajlovic, E., Honig, B., Moore, C.B., Payne, G.T., Wright, M., 2013. Social capital and
entrepreneurship: A schema and research agenda. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 37,
455-478.
Gioia, D.A., Chittipeddi, K., 1991. Sensemaking and sensegiving in strategic change initiation.
Strategic Management Journal. 12, 433-448.
Gompers, P.A., 1995. Optimal investment, monitoring, and the staging of venture capital. The
Journal of Finance. 50, 1461-1489.
Gooty, J., Gavin, M., Johnson, P.D., Frazier, M.L., Snow, D.B., 2009. In the eyes of the beholder
transformational leadership, positive psychological capital, and performance. Journal of
Leadership & Organizational Studies. 15, 353-367.
Grichnik, D., Brinckmann, J., Singh, L., Manigart, S., 2014. Beyond environmental scarcity:
Human and social capital as driving forces of bootstrapping activities. Journal of Business
Venturing. 29, 310-326.
Guillamon‐Saorin, E., Isidro, H., Marques, A., 2017. Impression management and non‐GAAP
disclosure in earnings announcements. Journal of Business Finance & Accounting. 44, 448-
479.
Hart, R.P., Carroll, C.E. 2014. DICTION 7.0 The Text Analysis Program.
Hayward, M.L., Forster, W.R., Sarasvathy, S.D., Fredrickson, B.L., 2010. Beyond hubris: how
highly confident entrepreneurs rebound to venture again. Journal of Business Venturing. 25,
569–578.
Hmieleski, K.M., Baron, R.A., 2008. When does entrepreneurial self-efficacy enhance versus
reduce firm performance? Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal. 2, 57-72.
Hmieleski, K.M., Carr, J.C., Baron, R.A., 2015. Integrating discovery and creation perspectives
of entrepreneurial action: the relative roles of founding CEO human capital, social capital,
and psychological capital in contexts of risk versus uncertainty. Strategic Entrepreneurship
Journal. 9, 289–312.
Holoien, D.S., Fiske, S. T. 2013. Downplaying positive impressions: Compensation between
warmth and competence in impression management. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 49(1), 33-41.
42
Honig, B., Lerner, M., Raban, Y., 2006. Social capital and the linkages of high-tech companies
to the military defense system: Is there a signaling mechanism? Small Business Economics.
27, 419-437.
Hosmer Jr, D.W., Lemeshow, S., Sturdivant, R.X., 2013. Applied logistic regression (Vol. 398).
John Wiley & Sons.
Jensen, S.M., Luthans, F., 2006. Entrepreneurs as authentic leaders: impact on employees'
attitudes. Leadership Organization Development Journal. 27, 646-666.
Josefy, M., Dean, T.J., Albert, L.S., Fitza, M.A., 2017. The role of community in crowdfunding
success: Evidence on cultural attributes in funding campaigns to “save the local theater”.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 41, 161-182.
Judge, T.A., Bono, J.E., 2001. Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self–esteem,
generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and
job performance: a meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 86, 80–92.
Khoury, T. A., Junkunc, M., Deeds, D. L. 2013. The social construction of legitimacy through
signaling social capital: Exploring the conditional value of alliances and underwriters at IPO.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 37(3), 569-601.
Kickstarter., 2017a. Kickstarter stats. Retrieved Jan 03, 2017, from http://www.kickstarter.com.
Kickstarter., 2017b. Kickstarter Basics: Getting involved. Retrieved April 05, 2017, from
https://www.kickstarter.com/help/faq/kickstarter+basics?ref=footer.
Kirsch, D., Goldfarb, B., Gera, A., 2009. Form or substance: the role of business plans in venture
capital decision making. Strategic Management Journal, 30, 487-515.
Kiva., 2017. About. Retrieved December 5, 2017, from https://www.kiva.org/about.
Kroll, M., Walters, B.A., Le, S.A., 2007. The impact of board composition and top management
team ownership structure on post-IPO performance in young entrepreneurial firms. Academy
of Management Journal. 50, 1198-1216
LePine, M.A., Zhang, Y., Crawford, E.R., Rich, B.L., 2016. Turning their pain to gain:
Charismatic leader influence on follower stress appraisal and job performance. Academy of
Management Journal. 59, 1036-1059.
Li, J.J., Chen, X.P., Kotha, S., Fisher, G., 2017. Catching fire and spreading it: A glimpse into
displayed entrepreneurial passion in crowdfunding campaigns. Journal of Applied
Psychology. 102, 1075-1090.
Lin, M., Prabhala, N.R., Viswanathan, S., 2013. Judging borrowers by the company they keep:
Friendship networks and information asymmetry in online peer-to-peer lending. Management
Science. 59, 17-35.
Loewenstein, G., Sunstein, C.R., Golman, R., 2014. Disclosure: Psychology changes
everything. Annual Review of Economics. 6, 391-419.
Luthans, F., Avolio, B.J., Avey, J.B., Norman, S.M., 2007. Positive psychological capital:
Measurement and relationship with performance and satisfaction. Personnel Psychology. 60,
541-572.
Luthans, F., Luthans, K.W., Luthans, B.C., 2004. Positive psychological capital: Beyond human
and social capital. Business Horizons. 47, 45-50.
Luthans, F., Norman, S.M., Avolio, B.J., Avey, J.B., 2008. The mediating role of psychological
capital in the supportive organizational climate—employee performance relationship. Journal
of Organizational Behavior. 29, 219–238.
43
Luthans, F., Youssef, C.M., 2004. Human, social, and now positive psychological capital
management: Investing in people for competitive advantage. Organizational Dynamics. 33,
143-160.
Martens, M.L., Jennings, J.E., Jennings, P.D., 2007. Do the stories they tell get them the money
they need? The role of entrepreneurial narratives in resource acquisition. Academy of
Management Journal. 50, 1107-1132.
Martí, J., Balboa, M., 2007. Characterization of the reputation of private equity managers:
Evidence in Spain. Journal of Business Venturing. 22, 453-480.
Martin, B.C., McNally, J.J., Kay, M.J., 2013. Examining the formation of human capital in
entrepreneurship: A meta-analysis of entrepreneurship education outcomes. Journal of
Business Venturing. 28, 211-224.
Marvel, M.R., Davis, J.L., Sproul, C R., 2016. Human capital and entrepreneurship research: A
critical review and future directions. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 40, 599-626.
Massolution, C.L. 2015. Crowdfunding industry report.
McCullagh, P. 1984. Generalized linear models. European Journal of Operational Research. 16,
285-292.
McKenny, A.F., Allison, T.H., Ketchen, D.J., Short, J.C., Ireland, R.D., 2017. How should
crowdfunding research evolve? A survey of the entrepreneurship theory and practice editorial
board. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 41, 291-304.
McKenny, A.F., Aguinis, H., Short, J.C., Anglin, A.H., 2016. What doesn’t get measured does
exist improving the accuracy of computer-aided text analysis. Journal of Management, doi:
0149206316657594.
McKenny, A.F., Short, J.C., Payne, G.T., 2013. Using computer-aided text analysis to elevate
constructs: An illustration using psychological capital. Organizational Research Methods. 16,
152–184.
Memili, E., Welsh, D.H., Kaciak, E., 2014. Organizational psychological capital of family
franchise firms through the lens of the leader–member exchange theory. Journal of
Leadership Organizational Studies. 21, 200-209.
Menard, S., 1995. Applied logistic regression analysis. In Sage University Paper Series on
Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences. Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA; 7–106.
Mitteness, C., Sudek, R., Cardon, M.S., 2012. Angel investor characteristics that determine
whether perceived passion leads to higher evaluations of funding potential. Journal of
Business Venturing. 27, 592-606.
Mollick, E., 2014. The dynamics of crowdfunding: An exploratory study. Journal of Business
Venturing. 29, 1-16.
Mollick, E., Nanda, R., 2016. Wisdom or madness? Comparing crowds with expert evaluation in
funding the arts. Management Science. 62, 1533-1553.
Moore, C.B., Bell, R.G., Filatotchev, I., Rasheed, A.A., 2012. Foreign IPO capital market
choice: Understanding the institutional fit of corporate governance. Strategic Management
Journal. 38, 914-937.
Moss, T.W., Neubaum, D.O., Meyskens, M., 2015. The effect of virtuous and entrepreneurial
orientations on microfinance lending and repayment: A signaling theory perspective.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 39, 27-52.
Newman, A., Ucbasaran, D., Zhu, F., Hirst G., 2014. Psychological capital: a review and
synthesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 35, S120–S138.
44
Norman, S., Luthans, B., Luthans, K., 2005. The proposed contagion effect of hopeful leaders on
the resiliency of employees and organizations. Journal of Leadership & Organizational
Studies. 12, 55-64.
Nyberg, A.J., Fulmer, I.S., Gerhart, B., Carpenter, M. A., 2010. Agency theory revisited: CEO
return and shareholder interest alignment. Academy of Management Journal. 53, 1029-1049.
Olson, K.R., 2006. A literature review of social mood. The Journal of Behavioral Finance. 7,
193-203.
Ozmel, U., Reuer, J.J., Gulati, R., 2013. Signals across multiple networks: How venture capital
and alliance networks affect interorganizational collaboration. Academy of Management
Journal. 563, 852-866.
Parhankangas, A., Renko, M., 2017. Linguistic style and crowdfunding success among social
and commercial entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing. 32, 215-236.
Parhankangas, A., Ehrlich, M., 2014. How entrepreneurs seduce business angels: An impression
management approach. Journal of Business Venturing. 29, 543-564.
Payne, G.T., Moore, C.B., Bell, R.G., Zachary, M.A., 2013. Signaling organizational virtue: an
examination of virtue rhetoric, country‐level corruption, and performance of foreign IPOs
from emerging and developed economies. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal. 7, 230–251.
Peterson, S.J., Luthans, F., 2003. The positive impact and development of hopeful leaders.
Leadership & Organization Development Journal. 24, 26–31.
Pietraszkiewicz, A., Soppe, B., Formanowicz, M., 2017. Go pro bono: Prosocial language as a
success factor in crowdfunding. Social Psychology. 48(5), 265.
Plummer, L.A., Allison, T.H., Connelly, B.L., 2016. Better together? Signaling interactions in
new venture pursuit of initial external capital. Academy of Management Journal. 59, 1585-
1604.
Pollack, J.M., Rutherford, M.W., Nagy, B.G., 2012. Preparedness and cognitive legitimacy as
antecedents of new venture funding in televised business pitches. Entrepreneurship: Theory
and Practice. 36, 915–939.
Pollock, T.G., Rindova, V.P., 2003. Media legitimation effects in the market for initial public
offerings. Academy of Management Journal. 46, 631-642.
Pollock, T.G., Chen, G., Jackson, E.M., Hambrick, D.C., 2010. How much prestige is enough?
Assessing the value of multiple types of high-status affiliates for young firms. Journal of
Business Venturing. 25, 6-23.
Pope, D.G., Sydnor, J.R., 2011. What’s in a picture? Evidence of discrimination from
Prosper.com. Journal of Human Resources. 46, 53–92
Prendergast, C., 2002. The tenuous trade-off of risk and incentives. Journal of Political
Economy. 110, 1071-1102.
Ruscio, J., Roche, B. 2012. Determining the number of factors to retain in an exploratory factor
analysis using comparison data of known factorial structure. Psychological Assessment. 24,
282.
Sanders, W.M., Boivie, S., 2004. Sorting things out: Valuation of new firms in uncertain
markets. Strategic Management Journal. 25, 167-186.
Sauerwald, S., Lin, Z. J., Peng, M.W., 2016. Board social capital and excess CEO
returns. Strategic Management Journal. 37, 498-520.
Short, J.C., Ketchen, D.J., McKenny, A.F., Allison, T.H., Ireland, R.D., 2017a. Research on
crowdfunding: Reviewing the (very recent) past and celebrating the present.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice. 41, 149-160.
45
Short, J.C., McKenny, A.F., Reid, S.W., 2017b. More than words? Computer-aided text analysis
in organizational behavior and psychology research. Annual Review of Organizational
Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 5. doi: doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032117-
104622.
Spence, M., 1973. Job market signaling. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 87, 355-374.
Spence, M., 2002. Signaling in retrospect and the informational structure of markets. The
American Economic Review. 92, 434-459.
Spence, M., 1974. Market signaling: Informational transfer in hiring and related screening
Stern, I., Dukerich, J.M., Zajac, E., 2014. Unmixed signals: How reputation and status affect
alliance formation. Strategic Management Journal. 35, 512-531.
Strang, D., Soule, S.A., 1998. Diffusion in organizations and social movements: From hybrid
corn to poison pills. Annual review of Sociology. 24, 265-290.
Trager, R.F., 2016. The diplomacy of war and peace. Annual Review of Political Science. 19,
205-228.
Ucbasaran, D., Westhead, P., Wright, M., Flores, M., 2010. The nature of entrepreneurial
experience, business failure and comparative optimism. Journal of Business Venturing. 25,
541-555.
Vismara, S., 2016. Equity retention and social network theory in equity crowdfunding. Small
Business Economics. 46, 579-590.
Walther, J.B., 2007. Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication:
Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition. Computers in Human
Behavior. 23, 2538-2557.
Wang, H., Lim, S.S., 2008. Real options and real value: The role of employee incentives to make
specific knowledge investments. Strategic Management Journal. 29, 701-721.
Wasko, M.M., Faraj, S., 2000. “It is what one does”: Why people participate and help others in
electronic communities of practice. The Journal of Strategic Information Systems. 9, 155-
173.
Whittington, R., Yakis‐Douglas, B., Ahn, K. 2016. Cheap talk? Strategy presentations as a form
of chief executive officer impression management. Strategic Management Journal, 37(12),
2413-242.
46
Appendix A.
Language indicative of positive psychological capital in crowdfunding texts.
Dimension
Representative crowdfunding text excerpts
Hope
“I have what I believe is a very funny book.”
“We hope these offerings create pause in your life and rouse you from your
waking-slumber.”
Optimism
“Egypt presents many opportunities that make it an ideal project for Dom.”
“That is what I aspire to do with 'The MO Factor'.”
Resilience
“…we want nothing less than to build the next enduring, iconic, American
denim company and capture it all on film.”
“We were determined to build a studio that took that mantra seriously.”
“We’ve always been steadfast in remaining independent and much of what
we do at Village Underground is on a not-for-profit basis”
Confidence
“We have brilliant, experienced actors and a very capable crew.”
“We are so confident in our jeans that we also have a one-year guarantee
against defects (beyond normal wear and tear). If we can't fix it, we will
replace it. So pledge with confidence!”
47
Appendix B. Main analysis and Hypotheses Tests
Figure 1. Theoretical model
Table 1. Factor and parallel analysis for positive psychological capital variable.
Factor
Eigenvalue
Proportion
Component
Loading
1
3.23
0.81
Hope
0.93
2
0.54
0.14
Optimism
0.95
3
0.16
0.04
Resilience
0.95
4
0.06
0.02
Confidence
0.74
Parallel Analysis
Adjusted Eigenvalue
3.15
Estimated bias
0.08
48
Table 2. Descriptive statistics
Variable
Mean
Std. Dev.
Min
Max
Variable
Freq.
% of
Sample
Success
0.44
0.50
0.00
1.00
2009
9
0.52
Amount Raised
8721.16
65269.84
0.00
1924018.00
2010
138
8.00
PsyCap
21.72
29.37
0.00
281.00
2011
380
22.02
Video
0.72
0.45
0.00
1.00
2012
276
15.99
Duration
35.75
14.95
1.00
91.96
2016
923
53.48
Funding Goal
18271.00
70656.47
0.76
1641791.00
Art
124
7.18
Web
0.65
0.52
0.00
2.00
Comics
64
3.71
Numerical Terms
18.89
23.73
0.00
263.00
Crafts
21
1.22
Staff Pick
0.09
0.28
0.00
1.00
Dance
31
1.8
Created
1.88
5.48
1.00
111.00
Design
113
6.55
Experience
0.17
0.38
0.00
1.00
Fashion
90
5.21
Ethnicity
0.81
0.39
0.00
1.00
Film and Video
341
19.76
Sex
0.70
0.46
0.00
1.00
Food
91
5.27
Education
0.04
0.20
0.00
1.00
Games
132
7.65
Facebook Friends
514.87
955.30
0.00
11746.00
Journalism
17
0.98
Word Length
616.70
555.73
19.00
6205.00
Music
297
17.21
Backed
5.83
20.66
0.00
538.00
Photography
43
2.49
Endorse
0.01
0.12
0.00
1.00
Publishing
170
9.85
PsyCap
Dimensions
Written Texts1
Technology
140
8.11
Hope
8.55
11.38
0.00
107.00
Theater
52
3.01
Optimism
4.94
7.00
0.00
60.00
Resilience
6.75
10.62
0.00
109.00
Confidence
1.48
2.39
0.00
22.00
Video Transcriptions (N= 527)
PsyCap
0.10
1.60
0.00
30.00
Hope
0.03
0.53
0.00
9.00
Optimism
0.03
0.49
0.00
10.00
Resilience
0.03
0.55
0.00
11.00
Confidence
0.00
0.06
0.00
1.00
1 Approximately 93% of campaigns include at least one term related to at least one of the four dimensions of
positive psychological capital, 39% incorporated at least one term related to all four dimensions, 28% used at least
one term from three dimensions, and 11% utilized at least one term from two dimensions. Hope was the most
prominently used dimension with at least one word in 89% of the campaigns. Confidence was the least used
dimension with at least one word in 56% of the campaigns.
49
Table 3. Correlations
Variablesa
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
1
Success
2
Amount Raised
0.13
3
PsyCap
0.04
0.14
4
Video
0.15
0.06
0.21
5
Duration
-0.10
0.00
-0.13
-0.06
6
Funding Goal
-0.14
0.04
0.13
0.03
0.02
7
Web
0.19
0.07
0.06
0.14
-0.03
0.03
8
Numerical
Terms
0.20
0.08
0.38
0.21
0.02
0.02
0.15
9
Staff Pick
0.30
0.21
0.19
0.16
0.00
0.00
0.13
0.18
10
Created
0.08
0.00
0.01
0.03
-0.09
-0.02
0.07
0.06
0.01
11
Experience
0.12
0.06
-0.04
-0.04
0.06
-0.01
0.14
0.08
0.08
0.20
12
Ethnicity
0.11
0.03
0.04
0.02
-0.03
-0.02
0.03
0.08
0.04
0.04
0.02
13
Sex
-0.04
0.05
0.09
0.08
0.00
0.06
0.01
0.07
0.00
0.04
0.04
0.03
14
Education
0.01
-0.01
0.04
0.00
-0.02
0.02
0.05
0.01
0.00
-0.02
0.01
0.01
-0.06
15
Facebook
Friends
0.10
-0.03
-0.02
0.00
-0.04
-0.03
0.06
-0.01
0.00
0.00
0.10
-0.05
-0.01
0.02
16
Word Length
0.12
0.14
0.81
0.20
-0.06
0.10
0.16
0.51
0.23
0.02
0.07
0.07
0.06
0.04
-0.04
17
Backed
0.16
0.04
0.09
0.05
-0.04
-0.01
0.10
0.12
0.14
0.09
0.11
0.05
0.07
0.01
0.06
0.16
18
Endorse
0.03
0.04
0.16
0.04
-0.01
0.03
0.04
0.12
0.09
0.01
0.00
-0.01
0.00
-0.03
0.01
0.17
0.01
N = 1726; aAll correlations with an absolute value greater than (0.05) are significant at p < 0.05 and an absolute value greater than (0.07) at p < 0.01
50
Table 4. Positive psychological capital and funding success.
Controls
Main Effects
Social Capital Moderators
Human Capital Moderators
Variablesa
1
AME
2
AME
3
AME
4
AME
5
AME
6
AME
Video
0.78**
(0.14)
0.13**
(0.02)
0.74**
(0.15)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.64**
(0.15)
0.09**
(0.02)
0.74**
(0.15)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.73**
(0.14)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.73**
(0.14)
0.12**
(0.02)
Duration
-0.35*
(0.16)
-0.06*
(0.02)
-0.35*
(0.16)
-0.06*
(0.02)
-0.40*
(0.16)
-0.06*
(0.02)
-0.35*
(0.16)
-0.06*
(0.02)
-0.35*
(0.16)
-0.06**
(0.02)
-0.34*
(0.16)
-0.05*
(0.03)
Funding
Goal
-0.64**
(0.05)
-0.10**
(0.01)
-0.66**
(0.05)
-0.11**
(0.01)
-0.66**
(0.05)
-0.10**
(0.01)
-0.66**
(0.05)
-0.11**
(0.01)
-0.66**
(0.05)
-0.10**
(0.01)
-0.66**
(0.05)
-0.10**
(0.01)
Website
0.74**
(0.12)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.75**
(0.12)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.61**
(0.13)
0.09**
(0.02)
0.75**
(0.12)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.73**
(0.12)
0.12**
(0.02)
0.75**
(0.12)
0.12**
(0.02)
Numerical
Terms
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00**
0.00
0.01*
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.01)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.01
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
Staff Pick
3.50**
(0.37)
0.56**
(0.05)
3.50**
(0.37)
0.56**
(0.05)
3.34**
(0.39)
0.49**
(0.05)
3.51**
(0.37)
0.56**
(0.05)
3.55**
(0.37)
0.56**
(0.05)
3.50**
(0.37)
0.56**
(0.05)
Created
0.21
(0.14)
0.03
(0.02)
0.20
(0.15)
0.03
(0.02)
-0.23
(0.16)
-0.03
(0.02)
0.20
(0.15)
0.03
(0.02)
-0.86**
(0.30)
-0.14**
(0.05)
0.28
(0.15)
0.04
(0.02)
Experience
0.17
(0.19)
0.03
(0.03)
0.18
(0.19)
0.03
(0.03)
0.29
(0.21)
0.04
(0.03)
0.19
(0.19)
0.03
(0.03)
0.48*
(0.21)
0.08*
(0.03)
-0.50
(0.37)
-0.08
(0.06)
Ethnicity
0.45**
(0.17)
0.07**
(0.03)
0.47**
(0.17)
0.08**
(0.03)
0.37*
(0.17)
0.05*
(0.02)
0.48**
(0.17)
0.08**
(0.03)
0.49**
(0.17)
0.08**
(0.03)
0.48**
(0.17)
0.08**
(0.02)
Sex
-0.36**
(0.14)
-0.06**
(0.02)
-0.34*
(0.14)
-0.05*
(0.02)
-0.30*
(0.14)
-0.04*
(0.02)
-0.34*
(0.14)
-0.05*
(0.02)
-0.36*
(0.14)
-0.06**
(0.02)
-0.34*
(0.14)
-0.05*
(0.02)
Education
0.28
(0.32)
0.05
(0.05)
0.25
(0.32)
0.04
(0.05)
0.12
(0.36)
0.02
(0.05)
0.26
(0.33)
0.04
(0.05)
0.26
(0.33)
0.04
(0.05)
0.27
(0.32)
0.04
(0.02)
Facebook
Friends
0.05**
(0.02)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.02
(0.02)
0.00
(0.00)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.01**
(0.00)
Word
Length
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
Categoriesb
Yearsc
Constant
2.60**
(0.87)
2.45**
(0.86)
2.07*
(0.98)
2.49**
(0.86)
2.94**
(0.93)
2.53**
(0.86)
PsyCap
0.30**
(0.10)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.18
(0.11)
0.03
(0.02)
0.30**
(0.11)
0.05**
(0.02)
0.19
(0.11)
0.03
(0.02)
0.24*
(0.11)
0.04*
(0.02)
Backed
0.59**
(0.15)
0.09**
(0.02)
PsyCap ×
Backed
0.03
(0.05)
0.00
(0.01)
Endorse
2.40
(1.88)
0.38
(0.30)
PsyCap ×
Endorse
-0.46
(0.48)
-0.07
(0.08)
PsyCap ×
Created
0.39**
(0.10)
0.06**
(0.02)
PsyCap ×
Experience
0.28*
(0.13)
0.04*
(0.02)
Pseudo R2
0.291
0.295
0.345
0.296
0.30
0.30
Log
Likelihood
-839.01
-834.77
-774.98
-833.37
-827.92
-832.40
N
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; aStandard errors reported in parenthesis; b15 Categories, 14 controls, not reported; c5 Years,
4 controls, not reported
51
Table 5. Positive psychological capital and amount raised.
Variablesa
Controls
Main Effects
Social Capital Moderation
Models
Human Capital Moderation Models
7
8
9
10
11
12
Video
1.29**
(0.15)
1.23**
(0.15)
1.11**
(0.14)
1.23**
(0.15)
1.23**
(0.15)
1.21**
(0.15)
Duration
-0.02
(0.15)
-0.02
(0.15)
-0.03
(0.15)
-0.02
(0.15)
-0.02
(0.15)
-0.01
(0.15)
Funding Goal
0.03
(0.05)
0.02
(0.05)
0.07
(0.05)
0.02
(0.05)
0.02
(0.05)
0.02
(0.05)
Website
1.06**
(0.12)
1.06**
(0.12)
0.87**
(0.12)
1.05**
(0.12)
1.05**
(0.12)
1.05**
(0.12)
Numerical
Terms
0.01*
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.01*
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
Staff Pick
2.23**
(0.15)
2.22**
(0.15)
1.81**
(0.16)
2.21**
(0.15)
2.21**
(0.15)
2.18**
(0.15)
Created
0.03
(0.14)
0.02
(0.14)
-0.35**
(0.13)
0.01
(0.14)
-0.62*
(0.30)
0.12
(0.14)
Experience
-0.10
(0.19)
-0.10
(0.19)
-0.04
(0.18)
-0.09
(0.19)
0.09
(0.20)
-1.14**
(0.36)
Ethnicity
0.87**
(0.17)
0.88**
(0.17)
0.78**
(0.16)
0.88**
(0.17)
0.89**
(0.17)
0.88**
(0.17)
Sex
-0.51**
(0.13)
-0.48**
(0.13)
-0.44**
(0.12)
-0.48**
(0.13)
-0.49**
(0.13)
-0.48**
(0.13)
Education
0.20
(0.28)
0.17
(0.28)
0.07
(0.27)
0.18
(0.28)
0.18
(0.28)
0.21
(0.28)
Facebook
Friends
0.08**
(0.02)
0.08**
(0.02)
0.04*
(0.02)
0.08**
(0.02)
0.08**
(0.02)
0.08**
(0.02)
Word Length
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
Categoriesb
Yearsc
Constant
3.01**
(1.02)
2.81**
(1.05)
1.93
(1.08)
2.84**
(1.05)
3.00**
(1.02)
2.95**
(1.06)
PsyCap
0.34**
(0.10)
0.24*
(0.11)
0.34**
(0.10)
0.29**
(0.10)
0.26*
(0.10)
Backed
0.59**
(0.11)
PsyCap ×
Backed
0.02
(0.04)
Endorse
0.98
(2.04)
PsyCap ×
Endorse
-0.09
(0.47)
PsyCap ×
Created
0.23**
(0.09)
PsyCap ×
Experience
0.41**
(0.12)
Deviance
10364.74
10293.68
9542.32
10284.44
10254.93
10225.78
Log Likelihood
-3996.10
-3990.17
-3924.76
-3989.39
-3986.91
-3984.46
N
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; aStandard errors reported in parenthesis; b15 Categories, 14 controls, not reported; c5 Years,
4 controls, not reported
52
Appendix C. Scree and Interaction Plots
0 1 2 3
Eigenvalues
1 2 3 4
Number
Scree plot of eigenvalues
Interactions between Entrepreneurial Experience and Positive Psychological Capital
.2 .4 .6 .8 1
Pr(Successful)
0 5.69
PsyCap
Experience = 0 Experience = 1
Predictive Margins with 95% CIs
46810 12
Funds Raised
0 5.69
PsyCap
Experience = 0 Experience = 1
Predictive Margins with 95% CIs
53
Interactions between Created and Positive Psychological Capital
0 .2 .4 .6 .8 1
Pr(Successful)
0 5.69
PsyCap
Log of Created = 0 Log of Created = 4.7
Predictive Margins with 95% CIs
0 5 10 15
Funds Raised
0 5.69
PsyCap
Log of created = 0 Log of Created = 4.7
Predictive Margins with 95% CIs
54
Appendix D. Descriptive statistics and results for Kiva and IPO samples
Descriptive statistics and correlations for Kiva sample.
Variablesa
Mean
S.D.
Min
Max
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1
Successful
0.99
0.07
0.00
1.00
2
Amount Raised
813.11
805.72
0.00
9100.00
0.08
3
Funding speed
5.18
8.01
0.00
53.14
0.05
0.18
4
Group
0.12
0.32
0.00
1.00
0.03
0.40
-0.04
5
Loan Amount
816.58
803.88
50.00
9100.00
0.02
1.00
0.18
0.40
6
Sex (1=female)
0.27
0.44
0.00
1.00
0.01
-0.04
-0.32
0.12
-0.04
7
Payments
14.61
11.19
1.00
78.00
0.03
-0.09
0.04
-0.12
-0.09
0.11
8
Numerical
18.97
11.47
0.00
71.43
0.12
-0.07
-0.05
0.00
-0.08
-0.00
0.27
9
Word length
140.78
83.12
2.00
1473.00
0.12
0.15
-0.02
0.27
0.14
0.06
-0.06
-0.10
10
Psychological Capital
1.12
1.37
0.00
9.00
0.06
-0.03
-0.07
0.06
-0.04
0.03
0.07
0.00
0.31
N = 1726; aAll correlations with an absolute value greater than 0.05 are significant at p < 0.05, those with an absolute value greater than 0.07 are significant at p < 0.01
Kiva results.
Variablea,b,c
Successful
(Controls)
Successful
Amount raised
(Controls)
Amount raised
Funding speed
(Controls)
Funding speed
Group
-1.61
(1.14)
-1.55
(1.42)
-0.01
(0.01)
-0.01
(0.01)
-0.12
(0.08)
-0.12
(0.08)
Loan Amount
0.51
(0.87)
0.50
(0.87)
0.97**
(0.01)
0.98**
(0.01)
0.58**
(0.03)
0.58**
(0.03)
Sex
-0.22
(0.91)
-0.21
(0.91)
-0.03
(0.03)
-0.03
(0.03)
0.69**
(0.05)
0.69**
(0.05)
Payments
-0.11
(0.80)
-0.09
(0.82)
-0.01
(0.01)
-0.01
(0.01)
0.09**
(0.03)
0.09**
(0.03)
Numerical
-0.01
(0.04)
-0.01
(0.04)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00
(0.00)
-0.00
(0.00)
Word Length
2.22**
0.59
2.19**
(0.63)
0.01**
(0.00)
0.00**
(0.00)
0.07*
(0.03)
0.10**’
(0.03)
Constant
8.90
(6.81)
-8.81
(6.85)
-2.19
(1.89)
-2.19
(1.89)
-3.92
(0.30)
-3.97
(0.30)
Psychological Capital
-0.07
(0.92)
0.02*
(0.01)
-0.08**
Penalized Log
Likelihood/Log Likelihood
-51.31
-51.30
-1126.64
-1126.00
-2245.74
-2242.97
Sample
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
1726
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; aStandard errors reported in parenthesis; b42 Country controls included that are not reported; c16 categories not reported
55
Descriptive statistics and correlations for IPO sample.
Variables
Mean
S.D.
1
2
3
4
5
1
Underpricing
8.71
178.05
2
Psychological capital
1402.49
501.66
-0.01
3
Revenuesa
622.00
2260.00
-0.01
0.26**
4
Employees
3678.23
15935.45
-0.01
0.10*
0.62**
5
Numerical
61.23
40.78
0.02
-0.01
0.11*
0.14**
6
Word Length
119650.40
43722.08
0.01
0.87**
0.21**
0.11*
-0.08
aReported in millions, USD; * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01
Psychological capital and funding performance in IPOs.
Variablesa,b
Controls only
IPO underpricing
Employee count
0.01
(0.05)
0.03
(0.04)
Revenues
0.08*
(0.04)
0.08*
(0.04)
Numerical language
-0.01
(0.04)
-0.01
(0.04)
Word Length
-0.03
(0.05)
0.01
(0.12)
Year 2012
-0.10
(0.17)
-0.10
(0.16)
Year 2013
-0.03
(0.17)
-0.02
(0.15)
NYSE
-0.17
(0.15)
-0.17
(0.15)
OTCBB
0.23
(0.33)
0.23
(0.33)
AMEX
-0.48*
(0.21)
-0.49*
(0.22)
Constant
0.15
(0.18)
0.18
(0.23)
Two digit SIC codec
Psychological capital
-0.05
(0.10)
Log Likelihood
-585.29
-585.20
N
432
432
aStandard errors reported in parenthesis; bNASDAQ is the excluded market; c45 Industry controls included that are not reported.
* p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01
56
Appendix E. Exploring positive psychological capital over time interactions
Success
Amount raised
PsyCap
1.20**
(0.16)
-0.74
(0.43)
PsyCap and Year Interactions
2010
-1.14*
(0.46)
0.82
(0.49)
2011
-1.03*
(0.42)
0.88*
(0.44)
2012
-0.72
(0.43)
1.21**
(0.46)
2016
-0.75
(0.42)
1.35**
(0.45)
* p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01