ArticlePDF Available
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40 29
Journal of Psychiatry and Psychiatric Disorders doi: 10.26502/jppd.2572-519X0038
Review Article Volume 2, Issue 1
Cortisol Awakening Response: An Ancient Adaptive Feature
Carlos M. Contreras1,2* and Ana G. Gutiérrez-Garcia2
1Unidad Periférica Xalapa, Instituto de Investigaciones Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México,
Xalapa, Veracruz, 91190, México
2Laboratorio de Neurofarmacología, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, 91190,
México
*Corresponding Author: Dr. Carlos M. Contreras, Dr. Sci., Laboratorio de Neurofarmacología, Av. Dr. Luis
Castelazo s/n, Col. Industrial Ánimas, Xalapa, Veracruz, 91190, México, Tel: +52 (228) 8418900, Ext: 13613; Fax:
+52 (228) 8418918; E-mail: ccontreras@uv.mx (or) contreras@biomedicas.unam.mx
Received: 31 January 2018; Accepted: 20 February 2018; Published: 27 February 2018
Abstract
Similar to other endocrine substances, cortisol secretion follows a pulsating rhythm. The cortisol awakening
response (CAR) occurs upon awakening in the absence of any apparent stressful situation or imminent danger,
which is a very intriguing feature. When confronting any stressful situation, two systems are activated. One system
is regulated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA), and the other system is regulated by cerebral
structures that control the activity of the autonomic sympathetic nervous system. Both systems receive inputs from
emotional memory circuits, namely the amygdala, the hippocampus, the medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral septal
nuclei, among others. This circuit integrates sensory information that comes from thalamic nuclei. The acquisition,
retention, and evocation of recent and remote memories that are processed by the emotional memory circuit allow
the selection of strategies for survival. The diurnal secretion of cortisol occurs near the time of awakening (i.e., after
a period of rest or sleeping) and persists for several hours in the absence of any current stressful situation. The CAR
seems to represent an ancient adaptive-allostatic feature that prepares an individual to face eventualities that are
forthcoming during the day. The CAR is regulated by hypothalamic nuclei that modulate circadian rhythm, namely
the suprachiasmatic nucleus and its connections with the paraventricular nucleus, and then activate the HPA axis.
The CAR may represent a useful preparatory process that occurs before a stressful situation. The participation of
emotional memory circuits may modify the CAR and contribute to resilient or vulnerable reactions when coping
with threatening situations.
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
Keywords: Adaptive; Allostasis; Allostatic load; Ancient; Anxiety; Cortisol; Cortisol awakening response; Stress
1. Introduction
Throughout the day, plasma cortisol levels typically peak many times. A period of low plasma concentrations
generally centers around midnight, with an abrupt rise that commonly occurs after awakening, independent of age,
gender, and other aspects [1]. The cortisol awakening response (CAR) is an indicator of adrenocortical activity that
consists of an increase in plasma cortisol within the first hour after waking. Within the first 30-40 min after
awakening, free cortisol levels rise by 50-60%, remain elevated for at least 60 min [2], and decline to a nadir
thereafter by about bedtime.
An approach to understanding the processes that allow individuals to adapt to their environment is called allostasis
[3, 4]. This concept refers to functional changes in hormones and mediators that occur in an organism that allow the
individual to confront perturbations in the internal and external milieus. These changes permit the survival of the
individual and consequently the species. Allostasis depends on the activity of two main systems: (i) hypothalamic-
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and (ii) autonomic nervous system. During the day, cortisol levels may increase in
response to emergencies, whereas the CAR may be an anticipatory response that is directed toward daily
eventualities just after awakening. However, in cases in which high levels of cortisol persist for a long period of
time, are inefficiently managed, or become exaggerated, allostatic load may occur [4-6], which can negatively
impact health.
The organism is able to respond to emergency situations through physiological adaptive changes that permit the
individual to maintain homeostasis and survive. From a psychological perspective, this response is referred to as
resilience, which reflects the ability of the living organism to face and overcome stressful situations [7]. In other
cases, some maladaptive processes may be related to vulnerability [8-10]. However, HPA and autonomic activity is
insufficient to explain resilience and vulnerability. Increases in cortisol when confronting an emergency situation
and the CAR may prepare the individual for future emotional threatening events, thus suggesting the participation of
brain circuits that are involved in emotional processing.
The present review considers the participation of emotional memory circuits in the regulation of endocrine and
autonomic responses both at rest and when confronting a threatening situation. Cortisol has been considered a
marker of stress [11], in addition to other products of autonomic nervous system activity. Acute stressors activate the
HPA axis, leading to the release of corticotropin-releasing factor into the portal circulation (Figure 1).
Adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) is then released into the plasma, and the cortical portion of the adrenal gland
is activated to deliver cortisol into the circulation. Plasma cortisol levels reach a peak approximately 15-30 min after
an environmental challenge [12].
30
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
Figure 1: A threatening situation elicits two simultaneous responses: sympathetic responses and cortisol secretion
that is regulated by the HPA axis. These two systems interact with each other. The participation of cerebral nuclei
that regulate emotional memory processing may explain susceptibility and resilience to stress. ACTH,
adrenocorticotropic hormone; HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the
hypothalamus; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex.
The cortisol response when confronting stressful situations has been extensively reviewed elsewhere; therefore, we
only briefly discuss it herein. We focus mainly on the CAR, beginning with a brief overview of the brain structures
that regulate emotional memory and its relationships with brain structures that regulate cortisol secretion. We then
discuss the specific features of the CAR, its neural control, and the cortisol response to cope with threatening
situations in other vertebrates. The hypothesis of the present treatise is that the CAR may represent a very useful
ancient adaptive response.
2. Emotional Memory Circuit
Emotional memory allows an individual to recognize signs from the environment and compare them with past
experiences to effectively judge and respond to the environment by choosing the best coping strategy [13, 14]. Such
processes involve the hippocampus and other deep temporal lobe structures, such as the amygdala [15], the
mesolimbic system [16], and interactions among these structures and the prefrontal cortex [17, 18], among other
connections. Sensory inputs relies on thalamic nuclei that are connected to cortical and subcortical cerebral circuits
[19] that regulate the emotional meaning of stimuli and endocrine and autonomic responses.
31
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
The neural circuits that regulate emotional memory comprise several interrelated structures that are located
primarily in deep layers of the temporal and frontal lobes that project to the HPA axis and cerebral regulators of
corticosterone secretion and adrenergic system activation (Figure 2).
Figure 2: Anatomical representation of emotional memory circuit. Connections between the amygdala,
hippocampus, lateral septal nucleus (LSN), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) modulate the utilization of
emotional memories. These nuclei are also connected to the locus coeruleus and hypothalamus, which are involved
in autonomic responses and cortisol secretion.
Among other temporal lobe structures, the amygdala complex is composed of many functionally heterogeneous
nuclei [20]. The amygdala nuclei have been largely considered as fundamental in the process and integration of
defensive and fear reactions [21-23]. Basolateral amygdala includes basal, lateral and accessory basal nuclei [24]
and fear reactions [25], increased anxiety state [26], during the processes of emotional learning [27] and classic
conditioning [28] relates to a higher neuronal firing rate in these regions than in absence of stimulation or resting
situations. From a behavioral point of view, electrical stimulation of amygdala produces signs of fear and anxiety,
accompanied by vegetative responses in both cats [29] and human beings [30]. Fear expression involves cortical
association areas, and thalamic and amygdaline interconnections [31]; importantly, cortisol seems to regulate the
connectivity between amygdala and at least the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) inclusively during rest conditions
[32], while amygdala-hypothalamic connections regulate vegetative activity in response to threatening situations
[33].
Among another amygdaline connections, the reciprocal innervation with hippocampus modulate the unconditioned
fear, defense reactions, goal-directed behavior and emotional memory [34, 35], with the important participation of
the two different portions of hippocampus [36]. Therefore, amygdala-hippocampus relations are crucial in the
control and regulation of episodic memory and emotional memory, and as above mentioned, through the
connections of amygdala with hypothalamus in the control of cortisol secretion. In rats, the corresponding portions
are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus, which are related to memory and emotional processing, respectively [37].
The responsivity of dorsal hippocampal neurons responders to amygdala stimulation increased 48 h after a single
session of stress, suggesting the formation of an emotional memory [38]. Increases in endogenous cortisol and
32
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
norepinephrine levels in turn increase neuronal activation in the amygdala in response to threatening images [39]. In
such cases, the higher levels of plasma cortisol when confronting a threatening situation may facilitate specific
learning that is relevant to survival [40].
Amygdala-mPFC connections are able to regulate aggressive behavior in rodents [41-43]. In rats, the mPFC
involves the cingulate, prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IL) subregions, each subregion possess different connections
and consequently different functions. In particular PL and IL differentially regulate the expression of fear [44],
among other behaviors [17], possibly due to their interconnections with amygdala [45]. mPFC subregions
differentially participate in the process of acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear [46, 47] through inhibitory
connections coming from amygdala [28], thus mediating distinct strategies to cope with environment. Inactivation of
the PL cortex impaired the expression of fear but not extinction memory. Inactivation of the IL cortex had no effect
of the expression of fear but impaired both the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear memories [46].
Activation of the PL and IL regions has yielded consistent results. The PL cortex is active during fear conditioning,
and the IL cortex becomes active during fear extinction [47].
Another structure that is connected to the amygdala, hippocampus, and mPFC is the lateral septal nucleus. Together
with the aforementioned key regions, the lateral septal nucleus also participates in the control of motivational and
autonomic responses [48], the antidepressant actions of drugs [49], anxiety [50], affective behavior, and autonomic
activity [51].
Brain structures that are related to emotional memory appear to influence and may be influenced by the actions of
cortisol secretion and sympathetic activity. In such a case, the participation of emotional memory circuits due to its
function of retention of experiences related with threatening situations may account for the formation of resilience
and vulnerability, and consequently modifying the vegetative responses, favoring or negatively impacting on the
efficacy of allostatic processes.
2.1 Cortisol awakening response and sleep
The diurnal increase in cortisol secretion is associated with the sleep/wake and light/dark cycles. The CAR is a very
constant feature that is modulated by circadian influences. In very young children, the level of morning cortisol is
positively associated with the amount of stage-2 sleep the night before and negatively associated with total sleep
time and other slow-wave-sleep stages [52].
Total sleep deprivation in healthy adults decreases the CAR in parallel with changes in the perception of energy
level, concentration, and speed of thought and a reduction of cognitive functioning despite an increase in regional
dopaminergic activity [53]. Chronic circadian misalignment significantly reduced cortisol levels and increased the
release of inflammatory factors, including tumor necrosis factor, interleukin, and C-reactive protein [54]. The
interaction between sleep and the HPA axis is complex and bidirectional. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis
hyperactivity and decreases in the duration and quality of sleep occur in insomnia, depression, Cushing’s syndrome,
and sleep-disordered breathing, among other ailments [55]. Changes in sleep duration contribute to daily variations
in cortisol and autonomic nervous system activity [56].
33
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
2.2 Neural regulation of the cortisol awakening response
The suprachiasmatic nucleus regulates the circadian rise in plasma ACTH [57,58]. Suprachiasmatic nucleus
regulates activity on paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus and exerts a decisive action on the day/night pattern of
hormonal and autonomic activity regulation [59]. This anatomical feature regulates CAR and the influence of ACTH
on suprarenal cortex [60].
Sensorial stimulation produces emotional reactions and elaborated behaviors (Figure 3). The hypothalamic
regulation CAR [61] is modulated by a multiple system of neurotransmission, mainly glutamatergic, aspartate, and
GABAergic fibers from telencephalic and forebrain regions, which are considered limbic structures [62-64], but not
from the lower brainstem. These hypothalamic nuclei control the neuroendocrine response to stress, whereas the
extended amygdala controls the autonomic responses to stress [12]. Therefore, the paraventricular nucleus may be
considered an integrator of neuroendocrine and autonomic nervous system responses and may also participate in the
integrated emotional response. The CAR may also be involved in the activation of a negative feedback loop that
results in the termination of ACTH secretion [65]. Anxiety may be a useful adaptive feature [14] that, combined
with the storage of emotional memories of prior experiences, facilitates the choice of the best strategies for survival.
Figure 3: The circadian rhythm of cortisol release occurs in the absence of a threatening situation. Therefore, it may
be considered a useful allostatic adaptive feature that prepares an individual for eventual emergency situations, with
the fundamental participation of emotional memory circuits. PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus;
SCH n: suprachiasmatic nucleus; CAR: cortisol awakening response.
34
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
2.3 Cortisol in other animal species
The cortisol response to threatening situations is not exclusive to humans or other mammals. Individuals that present
similar HPA axis function express similar responses to threatening situations, independent of species. A rise in
cortisol may indicate the development of behavioral strategies that facilitate escape from predators and functional
metabolic changes that allow survival through allostasis.
In the presence of predators or threatening situations, cortisol (or corticosterone) is released by fish [66-69],
amphibians [70], small mammals [71-74], goats [75], and seals [76]. This rise in cortisol (or corticosterone) allows
suppressive behavioral actions (e.g., freezing) in some cases and preparative defensive actions (e.g., attack) in others
[77, 78]. For example, increases in cortisol may mobilize glucose for sustained vigilance and running during periods
of reduced foraging possibilities [78]. It is currently unknown whether such increases in circulating cortisol in other
vertebrates follow a circadian rhythm or occur after periods of sleep or rest. The delivery of cortisol by the adrenal
glands and other metabolic processes may be related to a functional preparatory reaction of the organism to a
threating situation that allows individuals to adapt to their environment.
3. Conclusion
The processes that are involved in the sequence of events that allows us to cope with stress appear to represent an
adaptive process that slowly developed in our ancestral past [79]. The increase in glucocorticoid levels upon
awakening prepares the body for activity, thus enabling foraging behavior by increasing the amount of energy that is
available [60, 80]. Homo sapiens have not appreciably changed for a long time. As a species, we are exactly alike.
One function of the CAR may be to energize people in the morning [81].
Early in the morning, a relatively high amount of cortisol is released, and cortisol levels dramatically increase after a
few minutes, leading to exploratory behavior, food seeking, and the facilitation of typical behavioral patterns of each
species to survive [82]. Upon awakening, our body is ready to hunt and fight, being previously prepared to support
thirst and hunger by liquids retention and increased metabolic rate, ultimately some of the main cortisol functions.
The CAR may be considered an ancient adaptive feature. Understanding the relationships between brain circuits that
modulate emotional memory and cerebral structures that modulate endocrine and autonomic responses to stress may
shed light on the processes that regulate resilience and vulnerability when coping with threatening situations.
4. Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no competing interests and no financial support to report.
5. Acknowledgements
The authors thank Michael Arends for revising and editing the English of the manuscript.
35
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
References
1. Wust S, Wolf J, Hellhammer DH, et al. The cortisol awakening response - normal values and confounds.
Noise and health 2 (2000): 79-88.
2. Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, et al. Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological
marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci 61 (1997): 2539-2549.
3. McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci 840 (1998):
33-44.
4. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev
87 (2007): 873-904.
5. McEwen BS. Allostasis, allostatic load, and the aging nervous system: role of excitatory amino acids and
excitotoxicity. Neurochem Res 25 (2000): 1219-1231.
6. McEwen BS. Interacting mediators of allostasis and allostatic load: towards an understanding of resilience
in aging. Metabolism 52 (2003): 10-16.
7. Rutter M. Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1094 (2006):
1-12.
8. Vreeburg SA, Hartman CA, Hoogendijk WJ, et al. Parental history of depression or anxiety and the cortisol
awakening response. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science 197 (2010): 180-185.
9. Karatsoreos IN, McEwen BS. Psychobiological allostasis: resistance, resilience and vulnerability. Trends in
cognitive sciences 15 (2011): 576-584.
10. Karatsoreos IN, McEwen BS. Resilience and vulnerability: a neurobiological perspective. F1000Prime Rep
5 (2013): 13.
11. Kozlov AI, Kozlova MA. Cortisol as a marker of stress. Fiziologiia cheloveka 40 (2014):123-136.
12. Kovacs KJ. CRH: the link between hormonal-, metabolic- and behavioral responses to stress. Journal of
chemical neuroanatomy 54 (2013): 25-33.
13. Contreras CM, Gutiérrez-García AG. Emotional memory and chemical communication. In: Benitez-King,
G, Cisneros-Berlanga C. The Neurobiological Sciences Applied to Psychiatry: From Genes, Proteins, and
Neurotransmitters to Behavior. Research Signpost, Kerala 171-188.
14. Gutiérrez-García AG, Contreras CM. Anxiety: an adaptive emotion. In: Durbano, F. New Insights into
Anxiety Disorders. INTECH, Rijeka 21-37.
15. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23 (2000):155-184.
16. Pani L, Porcella A, Gessa GL. The role of stress in the pathophysiology of the dopaminergic system.
Molecular psychiatry 5 (2000): 14-21.
17. Vertes RP. Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in
emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience 142 (2006): 1-20.
18. Zelikowsky M, Hersman S, Chawla MK, et al. Neuronal ensembles in amygdala, hippocampus, and
prefrontal cortex track differential components of contextual fear. The Journal of neuroscience : The
official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 34 (2014): 8462-8466.
19. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, et al. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of
normal emotion perception. Biological psychiatry 54 (2003): 504-514.
36
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
20. LeDoux J. The amygdala. Curr Biol 17 (2007): R868-R874.
21. Maren S. Neurotoxic basolateral amygdala lesions impair learning and memory but not the performance of
conditional fear in rats. The Journal of neuroscience : The official journal of the Society for Neuroscience
19 (1999): 8696-8703.
22. Nader K, Majidishad P, Amorapanth P, et al. Damage to the lateral and central, but not other, amygdaloid
nuclei prevents the acquisition of auditory fear conditioning. Learn Mem 8 (2001): 156-163.
23. Herry C, Ciocchi S, Senn V, et al. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature 454
(2008): 600-606.
24. Sah P, Faber ES, Lopez De Armentia M, et al. The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiol
Rev 83 (2003): 803-834.
25. Pelletier JG, Likhtik E, Filali M, et al. Lasting increases in basolateral amygdala activity after emotional
arousal: implications for facilitated consolidation of emotional memories. Learn Mem 12 (2005): 96-102.
26. Villarreal G, King CY. Brain imaging in posttraumatic stress disorder. Seminars in clinical neuropsychiatry
6 (2001): 131-145.
27. Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Molecular psychiatry 6 (2001): 13-34.
28. Grace AA, Rosenkranz JA. Regulation of conditioned responses of basolateral amygdala neurons.
Physiology and behavior 77 (2002): 489-493.
29. Hilton SM, Zbrozyna AW. Amygdaloid region for defence reactions and its efferent pathway to the brain
stem. The Journal of physiology 165 (1963): 160-173.
30. Gunne LM, Reis DJ. Changes in brain catecholamines associated with electrical stimulation of amygdaloid
nucleus. Life sciences 11 (1963): 804-809.
31. Romanski LM, LeDoux JE. Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits
in auditory fear conditioning. The Journal of neuroscience: The official journal of the Society for
Neuroscience 12 (1992): 4501-4509.
32. Veer IM, Oei NY, Spinhoven P, et al. Endogenous cortisol is associated with functional connectivity
between the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37 (2012): 1039-1047.
33. Iwata J, LeDoux JE, Meeley MP, et al. Intrinsic neurons in the amygdaloid field projected to by the medial
geniculate body mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli. Brain Res 383 (1986): 195-
214.
34. LeDoux JE. Evolution of human emotion: a view through fear. Prog Brain Res 195 (2012): 431-442.
35. Wang Z, Pang RD, Hernandez M, et al. Anxiolytic-like effect of pregabalin on unconditioned fear in the
rat: an autoradiographic brain perfusion mapping and functional connectivity study. NeuroImage 59 (2012):
4168-4188.
36. Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron
65 (2010): 7-19.
37. Bannerman DM, Rawlins JN, McHugh SB, et al. Regional dissociations within the hippocampus--memory
and anxiety. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 28 (2004): 273-283.
38. Contreras CM, Molina-Jiménez T, Gutiérrez-García AG. Exposure to an alarm pheromone combined with
footshock stress enhances responsivity of the medial amygdala-hippocampus circuit. Am J Psychiatry
Neurosci 2 (2014): 83.
37
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
39. van Stegeren AH, Wolf OT, Everaerd W, et al. Endogenous cortisol level interacts with noradrenergic
activation in the human amygdala. Neurobiol Learn Mem 87 (2007): 57-66.
40. Carrasco GA, Van de Kar LD. Neuroendocrine pharmacology of stress. Eur J Pharmacol 463 (2003): 235-
272.
41. Canonaco M, Valenti A, Maggi A. Effects of progesterone on [35S] t-butylbicyclophosphorothionate
binding in some forebrain areas of the female rat and its correlation to aggressive behavior. Pharmacology,
biochemistry, and behavior 37 (1990): 433-438.
42. Fraile IG, McEwen BS, Pfaff DW. Progesterone inhibition of aggressive behaviors in hamsters. Physiology
and behavior 39 (1987): 225-229.
43. Fraile IG, Pfaff DW, McEwen BS. Progestin receptors with and without estrogen induction in male and
female hamster brain. Neuroendocrinology 45 (1987): 487-491.
44. Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: evidence for a dorso-ventral
distinction based upon functional and anatomical characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27 (2003): 555-
579.
45. Gabbott PL, Warner TA, Jays PR, et al. Prefrontal cortex in the rat: projections to subcortical autonomic,
motor, and limbic centers. J Comp Neurol 492 (2005): 145-177.
46. Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ. Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices,
ventral hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear.
Neuropsychopharmacology: Official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 36
(2011): 529-538.
47. Senn V, Wolff SB, Herry C, et al. Long-range connectivity defines behavioral specificity of amygdala
neurons. Neuron 81 (2014): 428-437.
48. Risold PY, Swanson LW. Connections of the lateral septal complex. Brain Res Rev 24 (1997): 115-195.
49. Contreras CM, Rodríguez-Landa JF, Gutiérrez-García AG. The lowest effective dose of fluoxetine in the
forced swim test significantly affects the firing rate of lateral septal neurones in the rat. Journal of
psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 15 (2001): 231-236.
50. Yadin E, Thomas E, Grishkat HL, et al. The role of lateral septum in anxiolysis. Physiology and behavior
53 (1993): 1077-1093.
51. Sheehan TP, Chambers RA, Russell DS. Regulation of affect by the lateral septum: implications for
neuropsychiatry. Brain research Brain research reviews 46 (2004): 71-117.
52. Lemola S, Perkinson-Gloor N, Hagmann-von Arx P, et al. Morning cortisol secretion in school-age
children is related to the sleep pattern of the preceding night. Psychoneuroendocrinology 52 (2015): 297-
301.
53. Klumpers UM, Veltman DJ, van Tol MJ, et al. Neurophysiological effects of sleep deprivation in healthy
adults, a pilot study. PloS one 10 (2015):e0116906.
54. Wright KPJ, Drake AL, Frey DJ, et al. Influence of sleep deprivation and circadian misalignment on
cortisol, inflammatory markers, and cytokine balance. Brain Behav Immun 47 (2015): 24-34.
55. Balbo M, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Impact of sleep and its disturbances on hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal
axis activity. International journal of endocrinology (2010): 759234.
38
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
56. Van Lenten SA, Doane, LD. Examining multiple sleep behaviors and diurnal salivary cortisol and alpha-
amylase: Within- and between-person associations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 68 (2016): 100-110.
57. Cascio CS, Shinsako J, Dallman MF. The suprachiasmatic nuclei stimulate evening ACTH secretion in the
rat. Brain Res 423 (1987): 173-178.
58. Buijs RM, Kalsbeek A, van der Woude TP, et al. Suprachiasmatic nucleus lesion increases corticosterone
secretion. Am J Physiol 264 (1993): R1186-R1192.
59. Buijs RM, la Fleur SE, Wortel J, et al. The suprachiasmatic nucleus balances sympathetic and
parasympathetic output to peripheral organs through separate preautonomic neurons. J Comp Neurol 464
(2003): 36-48.
60. Kalsbeek A, van der Spek R, Lei J, et al. Circadian rhythms in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)
axis. Mol Cell Endocrinol 349 (2012): 20-29.
61. Vrang N, Larsen PJ, Mikkelsen JD. Direct projection from the suprachiasmatic nucleus to
hypophysiotrophic corticotropin-releasing factor immunoreactive cells in the paraventricular nucleus of the
hypothalamus demonstrated by means of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin tract tracing. Brain Res 684
(1995): 61-69.
62. Herman JP, Tasker JG, Ziegler DR, et al. Local circuit regulation of paraventricular nucleus stress
integration glutamate GABA-connections. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71 (2002): 457-468.
63. Moga MM, Moore RY. Organization of neural inputs to the suprachiasmatic nucleus in the rat. J Comp
Neurol 389 (1997): 508-534.
64. Csáki A, Kocsis B, Halász B, et al. Localization of glutamatergic/aspartatergic neurons projecting to the
hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus studied by retrograde transport of (3H)D-aspartate autoradiography.
Neuroscience 101 (2000): 637-655.
65. Jacobson L, Akana SF, Cascio CS, et al. Circadian variations in plasma corticosterone permit normal
termination of adrenocorticotropin responses to stress. Endocrinology 122 (1988): 1343-1348.
66. Barreto RE, Barbosa-Junior A, Urbinati EC, et al. Cortisol influences the antipredator behavior induced by
chemical alarm cues in the Frillfin goby. Hormones and behavior 65 (2014): 394-400.
67. Fischer EK, Harris RM, Hofmann HA, et al. Predator exposure alters stress physiology in guppies across
timescales. Hormones and behavior 65 (2014): 165-172.
68. O'Connor CM, Gilmour KM, Van Der Kraak G, et al. Circulating androgens are influenced by parental nest
defense in a wild teleost fish. Journal of comparative physiology A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and
behavioral physiology 197 (2011): 711-715.
69. Sinha AK, Liew HJ, Diricx M, et al. Combined effects of high environmental ammonia, starvation and
exercise on hormonal and ion-regulatory response in goldfish (Carassius auratus L.). Aquatic toxicology
(Amsterdam, Netherlands) 115 (2012): 153-164.
70. Narayan EJ, Cockrem JF, Hero JM. Sight of a predator induces a corticosterone stress response and
generates fear in an amphibian. PloS one 8 (2013): e73564.
71. Zhang JX, Cao C, Gao H, et al. Effects of weasel odor on behavior and physiology of two hamster species.
Physiology and behavior 79 (2003): 549-552.
72. Mateo JM. Ecological and hormonal correlates of antipredator behavior in adult Belding's ground squirrels
(Spermophilus beldingi). Behavioral ecology and sociobiology 62 (2007): 37-49.
39
J Psychiatry Psychiatric Disord 2018; 2 (1): 29-40
73. Sheriff MJ, Krebs CJ, Boonstra R. From process to pattern: how fluctuating predation risk impacts the
stress axis of snowshoe hares during the 10-year cycle. Oecologia 166 (2011): 593-605.
74. Wang Z, Wang B, Lu J. Behavioral and physiological responses of striped field mice (Apodemus agrarius)
to predator odor. Integrative zoology 6 (2011): 334-340.
75. Olsson K, Hydbring-Sandberg E. Exposure to a dog elicits different cardiovascular and behavioral effects
in pregnant and lactating goats. Acta veterinaria Scandinavica 53 (2011): 60.
76. Oki C, Atkinson S. Diurnal patterns of cortisol and thyroid hormones in the Harbor seal (Phoca vitulina)
during summer and winter seasons. General and comparative endocrinology 136 (2004): 289-297.
77. Sapolsky RM. Stress hormones: good and bad. Neurobiology of disease 7 (2000): 540-542.
78. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating
permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine reviews 21 (2000): 55-89.
79. Reser JE: Chronic stress, cortical plasticity and neuroecology. Behav Processes 129 (2016): 105-115.
80. Kalsbeek A, Yi CX, Cailotto C, et al. Mammalian clock output mechanisms. Essays in biochemistry 49
(2011): 137-151.
81. Daly M, Delaney L, Doran PP, et al. The role of awakening cortisol and psychological distress in diurnal
variations in affect: a day reconstruction study. Emotion11 (2011): 524-532.
82. Korte SM, Koolhaas JM, Wingfield JC, et al. The Darwinian concept of stress: benefits of allostasis and
costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 29
(2005): 3-38.
This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the
Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license 4.0
Citation: Carlos M. Contreras, Ana G. Gutiérrez-Garcia. Cortisol Awakening Response: An Ancient
Adaptive Feature. Journal of Psychiatry and Psychiatric Disorders 2 (2018): 29-40.
40
... Stress effects on sleep physiology and sleep patterns have been previously reported (Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018). Additionally, the interaction between HPA axis function and sleep is complex and bilateral, while the circadian cortisol rhythm is associated with sleep and wakefulness cycles (Chan & Debono, 2010;Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018;Pulopulos et al., 2020). ...
... Stress effects on sleep physiology and sleep patterns have been previously reported (Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018). Additionally, the interaction between HPA axis function and sleep is complex and bilateral, while the circadian cortisol rhythm is associated with sleep and wakefulness cycles (Chan & Debono, 2010;Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018;Pulopulos et al., 2020). A lack of sleep is associated with HPA axis activation, so HPA axis hyperactivity and the consequent reduction in sleep duration and quality are associated with insomnia (Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018; van Dalfsen & Markus, 2018). ...
... Additionally, the interaction between HPA axis function and sleep is complex and bilateral, while the circadian cortisol rhythm is associated with sleep and wakefulness cycles (Chan & Debono, 2010;Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018;Pulopulos et al., 2020). A lack of sleep is associated with HPA axis activation, so HPA axis hyperactivity and the consequent reduction in sleep duration and quality are associated with insomnia (Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018; van Dalfsen & Markus, 2018). In many studies, parents of children with disabilities and chronic diseases, such as DMT1, reported sleep disturbances, poorer sleep quality, different sleep patterns, earlier wake-up times and shorter total sleep times than parents of healthy children (Halstead et al., 2021a;Macaulay et al., 2019;Meltzer, 2008;Meltzer & Moore, 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Sleep deprivation can decrease parental well-being and degrade mental and physical health in parents of children with chronic illness. The aim of this study was to explore the associations of sleep quality, psychological stress perception, and evening salivary cortisol concentration with self-esteem, optimism and happiness in parents of children with type 1 diabetes and developmental disorders compared to parents of healthy, typically developing children. Methods We studied 196 parents of children with chronic conditions, including autistic spectrum disorder (N = 33), cerebral palsy (N = 18), Down syndrome (N = 33), and diabetes mellitus type 1 (N = 40) and parents of healthy children (N = 72). We evaluated parental sleep quality, evening salivary cortisol levels, self-esteem, optimism and happiness. Multiple linear regression models were used to assess associations between variables. Results Compared with those of the control group, the parents of children with autistic spectrum disorders had higher evening cortisol concentrations (β = 0.17; p = 0.038) and lower perceptions of happiness (β=-0.17; p = 0.017), while parents of children with type 1 diabetes had disrupted sleep quality (β = 0.25; p = 0.003). Optimism was negatively associated with the evening cortisol concentration (β=-0.18; p = 0.023) and sleep quality index (β=-0.20; p = 0.012). Conclusions Public health programs aimed at lifestyle habit improvement, respite care, and relaxation for parents of children with chronic conditions would be useful for improving parental sleep quality, self-esteem, optimism and happiness.
... Advertisement calls of the frogs in the post-exposure condition were then immediately recorded. Because corticosterone has a circadian rhythm [49,50], the frogs were continuously exposed to turbine noise for 24 h in a plastic container, and the saliva was extracted at the same time from the pre-and post-exposure conditions. To compare the immunity between the pre-and post-exposure conditions, blood from the post-exposure condition was extracted immediately after saliva extraction. ...
... However, linearity values of 1:4 sample and 1:8 sample were less than 80% of both TCA-treated and non-treated samples because the concentrations were too low. Since the absorbance curves were higher in the TCA-treated samples of corticosterone and evidence of the interfering proteins in other species from a previous study [49], TCA-treated samples of corticosterone were used in this study. Table A1. ...
Article
Full-text available
As the advantages of wind energy as an eco-friendly strategy for power generation continue to be revealed, the number of offshore wind farms also increases worldwide. However, wind turbines can induce behavioral and physiological responses in animals by emitting various types of noises. In this study, we investigated the behavioral, physiological, and immunological responses of male Japanese tree frogs (Dryophytes japonicus) when exposed to wind turbine noise. To determine the effects during the breeding season, frogs were collected from areas with and without wind turbines. Additionally, we exposed the frogs to recorded wind turbine noise at a site without a wind generator for 1 h to 24 h to analyze the short-term effects. Three types of calling patterns (dominant frequency, note duration, and call rate) were analyzed to investigate behavioral responses. Physiological responses were assessed using two steroid hormones assays, namely testosterone and corticosterone detection in the saliva. The immunity of each individual was assessed using a bacterial killing assay. The wind turbine group in the field had a higher call rate and corticosterone levels and lower immunity than the group in the field without turbines present, and all three of these variables were correlated with each other. Conversely, in the noise exposure experiment, a higher call rate was only observed post-exposure compared to pre-exposure. Thus, turbine noise seems to induce decreased immunity in Japanese tree frogs as an increase in energy investment that triggers a behavioral response rather than acting as a sole physiological response that leads to a direct increase in corticosterone. This decreased immunity due to energy tradeoff or physiological response can change the disease epidemiology of the population and create new adaptive patterns in these habitats.
... The stress response is modulated by two systems: the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the autonomic nervous system (ANS). These two systems help to address stress and return an organism to a homeostatic baseline [1]. Disruptions to these systems multiple skills over the course of the week [20]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The cortisol awakening response (CAR) is a pivotal component of the body’s stress response, yet its dynamics under repeated acute stress and its interplay with immune biomarkers remain inadequately understood. Methods: This study examined 80 second-year military medical students undergoing a 5-day intensive surgical simulation designed to elicit stress responses. Salivary samples were collected daily upon waking and 30 min thereafter to measure cortisol and a panel of cytokines using bead-based multiplex ELISA. Results: Analysis revealed a significant blunting of the CAR on the third day of training (p = 0.00006), followed by a recovery on the fourth day (p = 0.0005). Concurrently, specific cytokines such as CXCL1 (r = 0.2, p = 0.0005), IL-6 (r = 0.13, p = 0.02), IL-10 (r = 0.14, p = 0.02), and VEGF-A (r = 0.17, p = 0.003) displayed patterns correlating with the CAR, with increased strength of associations observed when assessing cytokine levels against the CAR of the preceding day (CXCL1 r = 0.41, p = 0.0002. IL-6 r = 0.38, p = 0.0006. IL-10 r = 0.3, p = 0.008. VEGF-A r = 0.41, p = 0.0002). Conclusions: These results suggest a temporal relationship between stress-induced cortisol dynamics and immune regulation. The CAR pattern demonstrated in this study may represent induction of and recovery from psychological burnout. Moreover, the observed cytokine associations provide insight into the mechanisms by which stress can influence immune function. The results may have broader implications for managing stress in high-performance environments, such as military and medical professions, and for identifying individuals at risk of stress-related immune suppression.
... It is important to note that, upon awakening, cortisol levels are expected to rise by 50-60%. Subsequently, there is a rapid decline in the ensuing hours, followed by a gradual decrease as night falls [43]. A flat cortisol rhythm, characterized by the absence of an early morning rise in cortisol levels, has been associated with depression. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Mental well-being plays a pivotal role within the broader spectrum of health and illness, encompassing factors such as stress, depression, and anxiety. Nature-based therapeutic interventions have emerged as a promising approach to addressing these mental health challenges. This study seeks to assess the impact of these interventions on stress, depression, and anxiety levels. Methods: We conducted an extensive search for randomized clinical trials that examined stress, anxiety, and depression levels. The selected studies underwent a rigorous risk-of-bias assessment following the guidelines outlined in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews. Results: Our review encompassed findings from eight publications. Among them, two studies measuring cortisol levels revealed significant differences between the pre-test and post-test measurements within the intervention groups. In two studies that employed the Stress Response Inventory, a significant decrease in stress levels was observed within the intervention groups in contrast to the control groups. However, no significant differences were noted in studies that utilized the Restorative Outcome Scale. In the assessment of anxiety and depression levels, three studies employed the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, while four studies utilized The Profile of Mood States scale; none of these studies demonstrated significant differences. Conclusions: The current body of evidence offers limited support for advocating nature-based therapeutic interventions as a primary approach to reducing stress, depression, and anxiety.
... Convincing evidence for an influence of amygdala, hippocampus, and mPFC on HPA axis stress responses has been found in animal as well as human studies [2][3][4] . Moreover, while the regulation of the CAR was shown to be modulated by prelimbic regions 33,64 and associations between the CAR and chronic stress have been found 34 , it was mainly reported to be unrelated to cortisol reactivity to experimentally-induced psychological stress 65 . This is in line with the assumption that the CAR is modulated by additional regulatory mechanisms, for instance a direct influence of the adrenal cortex by the suprachiasmatic nucleus 33 . ...
Article
Full-text available
The importance of amygdala, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) for the integration of neural, endocrine, and affective stress processing was shown in healthy participants and patients with stress-related disorders. The present manuscript which reports on one study-arm of the LawSTRESS project, aimed at investigating the predictive value of acute stress responses in these regions for biopsychological consequences of chronic stress in daily life. The LawSTRESS project examined law students either in preparation for their first state examination (stress group [SG]) or in the mid-phase of their study program (control group [CG]) over 13 months. Ambulatory assessments comprising perceived stress measurements and the cortisol awakening response (CAR) were administered on six sampling points (t1 = − 1 year, t2 = − 3 months, t3 = − 1 week, t4 = exam, t5 = + 1 week, t6 = + 1 month). In a subsample of 124 participants (SG: 61; CG: 63), ScanSTRESS was applied at baseline. In the SG but not in the CG, amygdala, hippocampus, and (post-hoc analyzed) right mPFC activation changes during ScanSTRESS were significantly associated with the trajectory of perceived stress but not with the CAR. Consistent with our finding in the total LawSTRESS sample, a significant increase in perceived stress and a blunted CAR over time could be detected in the SG only. Our findings suggest that more pronounced activation decreases of amygdala, hippocampus, and mPFC in response to acute psychosocial stress at baseline were related to a more pronounced increase of stress in daily life over the following year.
... A CAR might be a good way to prepare for a stressful situation. When coping with hazardous events, emotional memory circuits may change the CAR and contribute to resilient or susceptible behaviors (Contreras and Gutierrez-Garcia, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Stress is a condition that must bepaid attention to by workers and employers, 87% of the workers from Europe claimed that they suffered from stress in the workplace. In 2015 around 28% of the workers reported suffering from work-related stress, with 33% clinical manifestations of fatigue, 19% sleep disturbances and 18% anxiety. Impacts that can affect workers can be absenteeism, presenteeism, etc. This study aims to investigate salivary cortisol examination to examine work-related stress using the evidence-based case report method. Methods: A literature review was conducted on November 23, 2020 through searches on the PubMed, Cochrane, and Google Scholar databases to find all published observational studies evaluating the relationship between salivary cortisol and work-related stress. Results: After screening using inclusion criteria and reducing the duplication of articles, 5 articles were obtained. Conclusion: it can be concluded that the salivary cortisol test tool can be used as an additional objective examination in order to check stress conditions in workers, in addition to a subjective examination like a questionnaire or anamnesis on workers. It is used for a biomonitoring effect and susceptibility biomonitoring. This salivary cortisol test can also be used to help determine stress levels in workers in order to detect early occupational diseases associated with psychosocial hazards. Keywords: biomonitoring, salivary cortisol, work-related stress
... Decline over the day leads to low CORT levels by time to sleep, but with spikes and ebbs as needed which are depictable as above and below a diurnal cortisol slope line. Much research indicates that chronic distress can (1) lead to higher CORT levels across the day or (2) depress waking CORT and/or the awakening response, both of which tend to produce a flatter diurnal slope which is associated with a blunted stress response (Contreras & Gutiérrez-Garcia, 2018). Such blunted (and hence inadequate) responses are widely thought to reflect SRS dysregulation that contributes to a wide array of negative health outcomes (O'Connor, et al., 2021). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
High levels of stress are known to accelerate biological aging in susceptible individuals, often leading to a downward course of ill health and early death. This book-length review explores how and why. It is too long to expect anyone to read the whole thing, but it serves to keep track of my understandings and syntheses (as of May 2022) while delving into literatures on toxic stress, aging, life history, and evolutionary theory. A common theme in these literatures is the connection between early life adversity (ELA) and later ill health and early death. Just about the only evolutionary explanation to be found is that ELA signals infants and children to develop a “live fast, die young” strategy to beat the odds against reproduction in their harsh environments, but this siphons energy from bodily maintenance leading to ill health later in life. However, the “live fast, die young” model has been increasingly questioned based on both theory and research. Even if it is valid in some cases, it must be incomplete because it is based on individual level theoretical reasoning. But humans and most primates live in social groupings that can only exist because individuals give up some degree of autonomy as they cooperate and support each other, especially their close relatives. This review takes the very rare approach of asking what happens if we assess the mass of ELA research through the lenses of inclusive fitness and multilevel selection, which were both developed to explain the puzzle of altruism. Is it possible there’s an altruistic aspect to accelerated biological aging? We arrive at an answer of “yes” in a multilevel model of stress and aging which appears to be particularly unique in simultaneously accounting for (1) inclusive fitness as a universal design principle; (2) the existential imperative to control free-riders (a concept virtually absent in the aging and stress literatures); (3) allometric scaling with body size determining baseline species-specific metabolic rates and lifespans (as reflected in the same lifetime limit in number of heartbeats across all mammal species); (4) social status hierarchies as venues of social selection which imposes distresses and eustresses based on relative current social and prospective fitness values of individuals; and (5) the tendency of high social distress to accelerate biological aging while eustress can maintain or even decelerate it, thereby (6) channeling individuals along diverging reproductive arcs which advantage higher status individuals, but disadvantage and speed the altruistic exit-by-aging of lower status individuals along with identified predatory free-riders.
... A steeper CAR has previously been associated with anxiety symptoms and disorders both longitudinally (Saridjan et al., 2014) and concurrently (Boggero et al., 2017). The CAR is thought to be a distinct marker of HPA-axis functioning, responsive to stress perception and anticipation of daily stressors (Contreras and Gutierrez-Garcia, 2018;Fries et al., 2009) and related to the process of awakening (Wilhelm et al., 2007). We speculate that a child exposed to PNMS, with relatively higher CAR, may be 'programmed' via sensitization of central stress response systems, to adapt and survive in expectation of a dangerous and stressful postnatal environment (Giudice, 2014). ...
Article
Background The fetal programming hypothesis suggests that prenatal maternal stress (PNMS) influences aspects of fetal development, such as the Hypothalamic Pituitary Adrenal (HPA) axis, enhancing susceptibility to emotional problems. No study (to our knowledge) has investigated this pathway considering development of preschool anxiety symptoms. Using data from the Queensland Flood study (QF2011), our objective was to determine whether toddler HPA-axis functioning mediated the association between aspects of flood-related PNMS and child anxiety symptoms at 4-years, and whether relationships were moderated by the timing of the stressor in utero or by the child’s sex. Methods Women, pregnant during the 2011 Queensland floods (N = 230), were recruited soon afterwards and completed questionnaires regarding their objective hardship (e.g., loss of personal property), subjective distress (post-traumatic-like symptoms) and cognitive appraisal of the disaster. At 16 months, indexes of the child’s diurnal cortisol rhythm (awakening response, total daily output, diurnal slope [N = 80]), and stress reactivity (N = 111), were obtained. At 4-years, N = 117 mothers reported on their own mood and their children’s anxiety symptoms; of these, N = 80 also had valid child cortisol reactivity data, and N = 64 had diurnal cortisol rhythm data. Results A greater cortisol awakening response at 16 months mediated the relationship between subjective PNMS and anxiety symptoms at 4-years. Greater toddler daily cortisol secretion predicted more anxiety symptoms, independent of PNMS. The laboratory stressor did not elicit a cortisol response. PNMS effects were not dependent upon child sex nor on gestational timing of flood exposure. Conclusions Indexes of diurnal cortisol in toddlerhood may represent vulnerability for anxiety symptoms in preschoolers, both independent of, and following, exposure to disaster-related prenatal maternal subjective distress.
Article
Dysregulation of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA axis) has been associated with various psychiatric conditions. The most interesting parameter of the HPA axis function is cortisol awakening response (CAR). Few data exist about the CAR in anxiety or personality disorders and findings are often contradictory showing blunted or increased CAR compared with control groups. The goal of this study was to determine whether patients with neurotic and personality disorders show a specific CAR pattern. The study population comprised 130 patients, mainly females (71.5%), with the primary diagnosis of a neurotic disorder or personality disorder according to ICD-10 admitted for psychotherapy in a day hospital. Pre-treatment cortisol levels were measured in three saliva samples collected in one day. The Symptom Checklist “O” and MMPI-2 were used to assess the pre-treatment levels of patients’ symptoms and personality traits. The study revealed a high percentage of CAR non-responders (cortisol increase of less than 2.5 nmol/l) in the study group (43.1%), particularly in females. 49% of them were CAR non-responders compared with 28% in males and 25% in the general population, respectively. CAR non-responders did not differ from the remainder in clinical characteristics. Four different CAR patterns were found in the study group: negative (26.9%), blunted (26.1%), normal (25.4%) and elevated (21.6%) as well as a particular type was not related to clinical characteristics of the patients. The study suggests that abnormal CAR types are observed in patients with neurotic and personality disorders and further research into the mechanism of the findings is required.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Daily stress can cause detrimentally high circulating levels of cortisol. Although habituation to this response can occur, it does not necessarily mean resilience. The operating room may be a natural site for the study of stress. Objective: The aim of the present study was to compare the impact of surgical stress in three protagonists of the operating room who play different roles: surgeon, patient, and stretcher-bearer. Methods: Twelve triads (patient, stretcher-bearer, and surgeon) of volunteers were selected. Urine samples were taken to determine the level of urinary cortisol as an indicator of stress. The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) was applied in all subjects before surgery. Results: The statistical analysis indicated that surgeons had the highest urinary cortisol levels, with no difference in cortisol levels between stretcher-bearers and patients. No differences in scores on the STAI-State (which evaluates the level of anxiety in response to a contingency) were found among the three experimental groups, and the lowest STAI-Trait scores (which evaluate anxiety as a personality trait) were found in surgeons. Conclusion: These data suggest that surgeons, through years of professional practice, develop a certain degree of resilience to perceived anxiety, but this resilience does not prevent the elevation of biochemical markers of anxiety. Therefore, although outward signs of anxiety are not manifest, strategies should be implemented to reduce anxiety in this group of professionals.
Article
Full-text available
Prolonged psychological stress and accompanying elevations in blood cortisol are known to induce hypometabolism and decreasing synaptic density in the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This article evaluates and explores evidence supporting the hypothesis that these, and other, selective effects of prolonged stress constitute a neuroecological program that adaptively modifies behavior in mammals experiencing adverse conditions. Three complementary hypotheses are proposed: 1) chronic stress signifies that the prevailing environment is life-threatening, indicating that the animal should decrease activity in brain areas capable of inhibiting the stress axis; 2) stress signifies that the environment is unpredictable, that high-level cognition may be less effective, and that the animal should increase its reliance on defensive, procedural and instinctual behaviors mediated by lower brain centers; and 3) stress indicates that environmental events are proving difficult to systemize based on delayed associations, and thus the maintenance of contextual, task-relevant information in the PFC need not be maintained for temporally-extended periods. Humans, along with countless other species of vertebrates, have been shown to make predictive, adaptive responses to chronic stress in many systems including metabolic, cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and even amygdalar and striatal systems. It is proposed in this article that humans and other mammals may also have an inducible, cerebrocortical response to pronounced stress that mediates a transition from time-intensive, explicit (controlled/attentional/top-down) processing of information to quick, implicit (automatic/preattentive/bottom-up) processing.
Article
Full-text available
Cortisol and inflammatory proteins are released into the blood in response to stressors and chronic elevations of blood cortisol and inflammatory proteins may contribute to ongoing disease processes and could be useful biomarkers of disease. How chronic circadian misalignment influences cortisol and inflammatory proteins, however, is largely unknown and this was the focus of the current study. Specifically, we examined the influence of weeks of chronic circadian misalignment on cortisol, stress ratings, and pro- and anti- inflammatory proteins in humans. We also compared the effects of acute total sleep deprivation and chronic circadian misalignment on cortisol levels. Healthy, drug free females and males (N=17) aged 20-41 participated. After three weeks of maintaining consistent sleep-wake schedules at home, six laboratory baseline days and nights, a 40-h constant routine (CR, total sleep deprivation) to examine circadian rhythms for melatonin and cortisol, participants were scheduled to a 25-day laboratory entrainment protocol that resulted in sleep and circadian disruption for eight of the participants. A second constant routine was conducted to reassess melatonin and cortisol rhythms on days 34-35. Plasma cortisol levels were also measured during sampling windows every week and trapezoidal area under the curve (AUC) was used to estimate 24-h cortisol levels. Inflammatory proteins were assessed at baseline and near the end of the entrainment protocol. Acute total sleep deprivation significantly increased cortisol levels (p<0.0001), whereas chronic circadian misalignment significantly reduced cortisol levels (p<0.05). Participants who exhibited normal circadian phase relationships with the wakefulness-sleep schedule showed little change in cortisol levels. Stress ratings increased during acute sleep deprivation (p<0.0001), whereas stress ratings remained low across weeks of study for both the misaligned and synchronized control group. Circadian misalignment significantly increased plasma tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α), interleukin 10 (IL-10) and C-reactive protein (CRP) (p<0.05). Little change was observed for the TNF-α/IL-10 ratio during circadian misalignment, whereas the TNF-α/IL-10 ratio and CRP levels decreased in the synchronized control group across weeks of circadian entrainment. The current findings demonstrate that total sleep deprivation and chronic circadian misalignment modulate cortisol levels and that chronic circadian misalignment increases plasma concentrations of pro- and anti- inflammatory proteins. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Total sleep deprivation (TSD) may induce fatigue, neurocognitive slowing and mood changes, which are partly compensated by stress regulating brain systems, resulting in altered dopamine and cortisol levels in order to stay awake if needed. These systems, however, have never been studied in concert. At baseline, after a regular night of sleep, and the next morning after TSD, 12 healthy subjects performed a semantic affective classification functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task, followed by a [11C]raclopride positron emission tomography (PET) scan. Saliva cortisol levels were acquired at 7 time points during both days. Affective symptoms were measured using Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Index (STAI) and visual analogue scales. After TSD, perceived energy levels, concentration, and speed of thought decreased significantly, whereas mood did not. During fMRI, response speed decreased for neutral words and positive targets, and accuracy decreased trendwise for neutral words and for positive targets with a negative distracter. Following TSD, processing of positive words was associated with increased left dorsolateral prefrontal activation. Processing of emotional words in general was associated with increased insular activity, whereas contrasting positive vs. negative words showed subthreshold increased activation in the (para)hippocampal area. Cortisol secretion was significantly lower after TSD. Decreased voxel-by-voxel [11C]raclopride binding potential (BPND) was observed in left caudate. TSD induces widespread cognitive, neurophysiologic and endocrine changes in healthy adults, characterized by reduced cognitive functioning, despite increased regional brain activity. The blunted HPA-axis response together with altered [11C]raclopride binding in the basal ganglia indicate that sustained wakefulness requires involvement of additional adaptive biological systems.
Chapter
Full-text available
Anxiety as an adaptive response is a natural emotion that occurs in response to danger and prepares an organism to cope with the environment, playing a critical role in its survival. Among the components of anxiety, the expression of fear may inform other members of the group about the presence of imminent danger (i.e., an alarm cue). The environment is perceived by a filtering process that involves sensorial receptors. While coping with a stressful situation, an individual may simultaneously emit vocalizations, perform movements to escape, freeze, and deliver to the environment chemicals called alarm pheromones. These cues are recognized by the receptor-individual by specific sensory systems located in the legs and antennae in insects and olfactory sensorial systems in other organisms. In mammals, the sensorial information is integrated by anatomical and functional pathways, with the participation of structures related to emotional memory, namely deep temporal lobe structures. Some stimuli are perceived as relevant when they contain relevant meaning according to previous experience and learning. The participation of ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex connections then leads to the selection of an adequate strategy for survival. The perception of these cues by other individuals in the group establishes intraspecies communication and causes striking behavioral responses in the receptor subject, namely anxiety, but the consequence is likely different. While the emitting subject may be in an emergency situation that is perhaps devoid of a solution, the receptor subject may have the chance to cope with the dangerous situation by employing efficacious strategies, depending on previous experience. The aim of this chapter is to review the participation of such anatomical pathways, their neurotransmission systems, and the resulting behavioral patterns.
Article
Full-text available
Alarm substances are released under stressful situations and may constitute signals that prevent other members of the group from encountering dangerous situations by producing fear. 2-Heptanone is an alarm pheromone that increases the neuronal firing rate in temporal lobe structures that are related to fear in the rat, such as the basal amygdala. A single stress session of unavoidable electric footshock or 2-heptanone sniffing increases the responsivity of the medial amygdala-hippocampus circuit, but unknown is the timing of action of simultaneous exposure to both stressors on the firing rate and responsivity of CA1-CA3 neurons identified by their connections with the medial amygdala nucleus. Twenty-four or 48 h after a single stress session, we obtained single-unit extracellular recordings. The firing rate was higher in the 48 h group. The peristimulus histogram showed an increase in the responsivity of amygdala-hippocampus neurons, which was more pronounced 48 h after a single stress session. The present results suggest an increase in the sensitivity of this circuit after a single stress session, seemingly representing a first step in the formation of emotional memories related to a conditioned response to fear.
Article
Full-text available
Although the circuit mediating contextual fear conditioning has been extensively described, the precise contribution that specific anatomical nodes make to behavior has not been fully elucidated. To clarify the roles of the dorsal hippocampus (DH), basolateral amygdala (BLA), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in contextual fear conditioning, activity within these regions was mapped using cellular compartment analysis of temporal activity using fluorescence in situ hybridization (catFISH) for Arc mRNA. Rats were delay-fear conditioned or immediately shocked (controls) and thereafter reexposed to the shocked context to test for fear memory recall. Subsequent catFISH analyses revealed that in the DH, cells were preferentially reactivated during the context test, regardless of whether animals had been fear conditioned or immediately shocked, suggesting that the DH encodes spatial information specifically, rather then the emotional valence of an environment. In direct contrast, neuronal ensembles in the BLA were only reactivated at test if animals had been fear conditioned, suggesting that the amygdala specifically tracks the emotional properties of a context. Interestingly, Arc expression in the mPFC was consistent with both amygdala- and hippocampus-like patterns, supporting a role for the mPFC in both fear and contextual processing. Collectively, these data provide crucial insight into the region-specific behavior of neuronal ensembles during contextual fear conditioning and demonstrate a dissociable role for the hippocampus and amygdala in processing the contextual and emotional properties of a fear memory.
Article
The review considers the roles cortisol (Crt), dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), and DHEA sulfate (DHEA-S) play in the stress response. Age-related, sex-related, and circadian fluctuations in normal conditions and in acute or chronic stress are described for Crt, DHEA, and DHEA-S. The main techniques used to estimate the Crt level in the blood, urine, and saliva are described, and approaches to the interpretation of the results discussed. Special attention is paid to Crt assays in anthropological and psychological studies.