ArticlePDF Available

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals a role for the left inferior parietal lobule in matching observed kinematics during imitation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Apraxia (a disorder of complex movement) suggests that the left inferior parietal lobule plays a role in kinematic or spatial aspects of imitation, which may be particularly important for meaningless (i.e., unfamiliar intransitive) actions. Mirror neuron theories indicate that the inferior parietal lobule is part of a frontoparietal system that can support imitation by linking observed and stored actions through visuomotor matching, and have less to say about different subregions of the left inferior parietal lobule, or how different types of action (i.e., meaningful or meaningless) are processed for imitation. We used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to bridge this gap and better understand the roles of the left supramarginal gyrus and left angular gyrus in imitation. We also examined if these areas are differentially involved in meaningful and meaningless action imitation. We applied rTMS over the left supramarginal gyrus, left angular gyrus, or during a no-rTMS baseline condition, then asked participants to imitate a confederate’s actions whilst the arm and hand movements of both individuals were motion-tracked. rTMS over both the left supramarginal gyrus and the left angular gyrus reduced the velocity of participants' finger movements relative to the actor during imitation of finger gestures, regardless of action meaning. Our results support recent claims in apraxia and confirm a role for the left inferior parietal lobule in kinematic processing during gesture imitation, regardless of action meaning.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals a role for the left inferior parietal lobule in
matching observed kinematics during imitation
Arran T. Reader1, 2, * Ben P. Royce2, Jade E. Marsh2, Katy-Jayne Chivers2, Nicholas P.
Holmes3
1. Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
2. Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical
Language Sciences, University of Reading, UK
3. School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, UK
*Corresponding author: Arran T. Reader. E-mail: arran.reader@ki.se
This article accepted for publication in European Journal of Neuroscience: 10.1111/ejn.13886
2
Abstract
Apraxia (a disorder of complex movement) suggests that the left inferior parietal lobule
plays a role in kinematic or spatial aspects of imitation, which may be particularly important for
meaningless (i.e., unfamiliar intransitive) actions. Mirror neuron theories indicate that the inferior
parietal lobule is part of a frontoparietal system that can support imitation by linking observed and
stored actions through visuomotor matching, and have less to say about different subregions of the
left inferior parietal lobule, or how different types of action (i.e., meaningful or meaningless) are
processed for imitation. We used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to bridge this
gap and better understand the roles of the left supramarginal gyrus and left angular gyrus in
imitation. We also examined if these areas are differentially involved in meaningful and
meaningless action imitation. We applied rTMS over the left supramarginal gyrus, left angular
gyrus, or during a no-rTMS baseline condition, then asked participants to imitate a confederate’s
actions whilst the arm and hand movements of both individuals were motion-tracked. rTMS over
both the left supramarginal gyrus and the left angular gyrus reduced the velocity of participants'
finger movements relative to the actor during imitation of finger gestures, regardless of action
meaning. Our results support recent claims in apraxia and confirm a role for the left inferior parietal
lobule in kinematic processing during gesture imitation, regardless of action meaning.
Introduction
Neuroscientific research implicates the left parietal lobe in imitation (Caspers et al., 2010;
Molenberghs et al., 2009), but neither the precise area nor its exact role are fully established. Some
neuropsychologists studying the apraxias - disorders of complex movement in which different types
of imitation can be impaired - suggest that the left parietal lobe controls kinematic (Buxbaum et al.,
2014) or spatial (Goldenberg, 2009) aspects of imitation, and that the inferior parietal lobule (IPL)
in particular is critical for imitating meaningless actions. Theories of social interaction based on the
3
putative human mirror neuron system, by contrast, indicate that the IPL is part of a frontoparietal
system that links observed and stored actions through visuomotor matching (Rizzolatti et al., 2014),
in terms of movements (Iacoboni, 2009; Iacoboni & Dapretto, 2006) or goals (Hamilton, 2008,
2014). In this way, the IPL could provide a scaffolding for imitation. These theories, however, often
say less about the specific role of IPL subregions (supramarginal gyrus, SMG; angular gyrus, AG),
or how the left IPL might or might not be differentially involved in meaningful and meaningless
action imitation.
These differing views result from different approaches to studying imitation.
Neuropsychological studies of apraxia have historically distinguished lesioned regions-of-interest,
and compared different types of imitative action (Goldenberg, 2009). However, neuroimaging
studies in healthy people have often used only a single or stereotyped action type (e.g., finger
tapping) which can make it hard to draw conclusions about different imitative capacities (but see
Bien et al., 2009; Carmo et al., 2012; Decety et al., 1997; Grèzes, 1998; Higuchi et al., 2012; Koski
et al., 2003; Krüger et al., 2014; Menz et al., 2009; Mühlau et al., 2005; Rumiati et al., 2005;
Tanaka et al. 2001; Tanaka & Inui et al., 2002).
Furthermore, neuroscience-driven work on imitation in healthy individuals is often defined
by the experimental scanning environment. Experimental paradigms typically use a single
participant responding to pre-recorded stimuli in the confines of an fMRI scanner, which tends to
limit the imitative capacity afforded by viewing and acting with the entire arm and hand. We
wanted to better understand imitation as a dynamic social experience, an approach that may be
essential to understand realistic imitation behaviour (Reader & Holmes, 2016). In addition, in
research both with healthy individuals and apraxia patients there is little use of motion-tracking for
characterising imitation in an objective fashion (with some exceptions, e.g., Braadbaart et al., 2012;
4
Campione & Gentilucci, 2011; Gold et al., 2008; Hayes et al., 2016, Hermsdörfer et al., 1996;
Krüger et al., 2014; Pan & Hamilton, 2015; Reader & Holmes, 2015; Sacheli et al., 2012, 2013,
2015a, 2015b; Wild et al., 2010; Williams et al., 2013), despite the fact that kinematics are an
important element of social interactions (Krishnan-Barman et al., 2017). With this in mind, we used
two-person motion-tracking in this experiment to better understand the links between actor and
imitator behaviour.
To bring work on imitation in neuropsychology and healthy participants together, we used
non-invasive brain stimulation to arbitrate between the roles of the two broad left IPL subregions
SMG and AG in imitation, and, secondary to this, to test whether these regions are differentially
involved in meaningful and meaningless action imitation. In particular, we were interested in
examining both imitation accuracy and kinematics in an exploratory fashion. We applied repetitive
transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the left SMG, left AG, or during a no-rTMS baseline,
then asked participants to imitate a confederate’s actions in a two-person, ecologically valid and
naturalistic motion-tracking paradigm.
Materials and methods
Participants
We recruited 12 participants from the University of Reading and the surrounding area
(mean±SE age = 23.2±1.1 years, 5 males, 2 left handed). Left-handed participants were not
excluded since, in the SMG at least, praxis representation is not related to handedness (Króliczak et
al., 2016). The experiment was approved by the University of Reading ethics committee (ref:
UREC 15/49); participants gave written, informed consent; the experiments were conducted in
accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (as of 2008).
5
Stimuli and apparatus
Positions of the participant’s right arm and hand and a confederate’s left arm and hand were
recorded using a Polhemus Liberty motion tracking system (Polhemus Inc., Colchester, VT, USA)
recording 16 channels (8 per person) with 6 degrees of freedom (x, y, z, azimuth, elevation, and
roll) at 240Hz. Trackers were attached to the shoulder (acromial end of the clavicle), elbow
(olecranon), wrist (pisiform), and the thumb and finger tips, using adhesive medical tape or
Velcro™. rTMS was applied using a PowerMAG 100 (Mag & More GmbH, Munich, Germany)
with a 70mm figure-of-eight coil.
The experiment was controlled and data acquired using custom software written in
MATLAB 2014b (Mathworks, Inc.) and using the ProkLiberty interface
(https://code.google.com/p/prok-liberty/). We used LabMan and the HandLabToolbox to document
and control experiments and analyse data. The associated repositories are, or will be, freely
available at https://github.com/TheHandLaboratory, whilst raw data are available on request.
Thirty gestures were used as stimuli. This included five meaningful hand, five meaningful
finger, and twenty matched meaningless gestures. For each meaningful gesture, two matched
meaningless gestures were created. In the case of finger gestures, this was done by changing the
fingers used and/or the orientation of the hand. In the case of hand gestures, this was done by
changing the orientation and/or position of the hand. We used more meaningless than meaningful
gestures to reduce the number of times participants were exposed to these actions, reducing the
likelihood that they would associate them with a particular meaning. The finger gestures signified
"okay", "thumbs up", "shoot", "peace", and "silence". The hand gestures signified "salute", "stop",
"shock", "looking into the distance", and "I'm listening" (Figure 1A). Emblematic gestures were
used since, unlike pantomimed actions which imply an object and require continuous motion,
6
emblematic gestures are point-to-point which is more appropriate for motion-tracking (i.e., it is
straightforward to extract a single velocity curve for analysis).
[FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE]
During the imitation task, participants sat opposite a confederate at a round plastic table
(diameter=85cm), approximately 100cm apart (Figure 1B). A Blu Tacstart point was located
20cm away from each person's abdomen. To inform the confederate of the actions they needed to
perform, a computer screen (unobservable by the participant imitator) was placed parallel to the
table, approximately 50cm to the left of the actor.
TMS site localisation
Visualisation of the participant’s brain used T1 weighted MR images alongside Brainsight
2.2.13 (Rogue Research Inc., Montreal, Canada). Due to SMG and AG size, stimulation locations
were based on guidance from previous experimental activation and cytoarchitectonic maps (Caspers
et al., 2006, 2008). The stimulation site for left SMG was area PF, located by finding the dorsal
extension of the posterior end of the Sylvian fissure and the anterior end of the intraparietal sulcus,
drawing an imaginary line between them, and stimulating the centre of this line. Evidence suggests
that area PF usually falls within these limits (Caspers et al. 2006). Since AG activation in
neuroimaging studies of imitation appears to be less frequent than SMG activation, the stimulation
site was the centre of the left AG, aiming to cover both PGa (anterior) and PGp (posterior). The AG
site was located half way between the dorsal extension of the posterior superior temporal sulcus and
the intraparietal sulcus. Mean±95% CI locations are shown in Figure 1C. For both AG and SMG the
coil was oriented orthogonal to the main orientation of the gyrus limits. The location of the coil in
7
the no-rTMS baseline condition was placed directly between the AG and SMG positions, but held
parallel against the head, such that no or minimal stimulation of the brain should occur.
rTMS parameters
Monophasic rTMS was applied to the left SMG, left AG, and in the no-rTMS condition (coil
held away from the head) at 1Hz and 110% of distance adjusted resting motor threshold (RMT,
Stokes et al., 2007). RMT (Rossini et al., 1994) was obtained at the start of the first session.
Mean±SE RMT was 69±4.1% of maximum stimulator output (MSO). The distance from M1, SMG,
and AG, to the scalp was measured using Brainsight. The no-rTMS site distance was measured
from the cortical tissue directly underlying the no-rTMS site to the scalp. Stimulation intensity was
limited to a maximum of 85% MSO in order to prevent the coil overheating. Mean±SE stimulation
intensity in each condition was as follows: SMG=70±4.1%, AG=72±4.4%, no-rTMS=71±3.9%
MSO.
Design and procedure
Participants took part in three sessions split at least a week apart. On a single day rTMS was
applied twice (once for meaningless, once for meaningful) for 15 minutes (900 pulses at 1Hz) to
either the left SMG, left AG, or in the no-rTMS baseline condition, in counterbalanced order across
participants. After each rTMS application, participants took part in either a meaningful or
meaningless action imitation task. Meaningless and meaningful actions were segregated into their
own separate trials (each following a single rTMS application), since there is evidence to suggest
that performing novel and known actions in a sequence could recruit a single processing route,
whilst presenting them separately recruits separate routes (Tessari & Rumiati, 2004; but see Press &
Heyes, 2008; Reader et al., under review). Task order was counterbalanced across stimulation sites.
8
Imitators were not given detailed instructions regarding the task constraints, and were simply told to
imitate the confederate, with the aim that this would ensure naturalistic performance in the task.
Both confederate and imitator began with their thumb and forefinger gripping their start
points. In both meaningful and meaningless imitation tasks, action images appearing on a computer
screen (in a random order, one per trial) signalled the confederate to perform the displayed action,
which they performed and maintained briefly before returning their hand to the start point. Five
seconds after presentation of the image to the confederate, a tone indicated the participant to imitate
the action they had observed, which they performed in a mirror fashion (i.e., using their right hand
to copy the confederate’s left hand). After completing the action, they returned their hand to the
start point before the next trial. Following a single application of rTMS, each meaningful action was
presented six times, or each meaningless action was presented three times, giving a total of 60 trials
per condition and TMS site. Imitation was performed in same-sex dyads, with either a male
confederate or one of two female confederates. The same confederate was used as actor for every
condition of a given participant.
Following the third rTMS session, participants were presented with a questionnaire featuring
the meaningful and meaningless images in pseudorandom order. They were asked to state whether
they thought each gesture had a meaning and, if it did, to provide an explanation. This was done
with the aim of excluding participants if they failed to meet an arbitrary 60% agreement with our
meaningful and meaningless action categorisation, but no participants failed this criterion.
Mean±SE agreement on the meaningful gestures was 75.8±7.83%, and the mean percentage of
meaningless gestures described as meaningful was 5.83±1.83%.
9
Data analysis
An automated script was used for pre-processing and extraction of variables. The analysis
routines processed the position data from each trial of each participant and rejected artefacts (e.g.,
trials with missing samples or spikes resulting from electromagnetic interference) before filtering
with a bidirectional low-pass 4th order Butterworth filter (cutoff frequency 15Hz). 5.3% of trials
were removed due to incorrect start times or artefacts.
The imitator's and actor's 3D velocity over their primary movement (movement onset to
gesture completion, mean±SE duration=1021±34.3ms) were resampled to 240 samples then
correlated in order to provide a measure of imitation accuracy (Reader & Holmes, 2015). The 3D
velocity profiles for each of the imitator's trackers were correlated with each of the actor's
corresponding trackers: shoulder (SH), elbow (EL), wrist (WR), thumb (TH), index finger (IN),
middle finger (MI), ring finger (RI), and little finger (LI). 3D velocity profiles were used since they
provide a measure of the change in the 3D position of the trackers over time, and thus the formation
of the final hand posture over time. This was considered preferable to using the x, y, and z position
values since it allowed us to reduce the number of statistical comparisons, and the likelihood of
false positives.
In order to use parametric statistics on the resulting r-values they were converted into Z-values
using Fisher's transformation (Z=0.5*ln((1+r)/(1-r))), where ln is the natural logarithm. JASP
(version 0.8.0.0, JASP Team) was used to perform two-way repeated measures ANOVAs on the
means of all relevant trials for each of these variables across each crossed condition (Table 1).
Preliminary analysis indicated that hand gestures were biasing the results (i.e., the mean Z-values
for all digits were similar since the digits generally moved together). Because of this we split the
hand and finger gestures before examining accuracy, then corrected for multiple comparisons using
10
Bonferroni correction, reducing our alpha used to determine a statistically significant result to .025.
We then performed the following analysis on the hand and finger data separately.
In order to assess whether there were time-dependent significant differences in the
stimulation-site related main effects or interaction, t-statistic plots were created to examine changes
in the imitator (i.e., regardless of the actor) 3D velocity for each instance (Figure 2). We took this
time-series-driven approach in order to inform us of possible differences in peak kinematic values
in separate trackers, without the inflated type 1 error that would occur were we to examine multiple
kinematic parameters in multiple trackers. In cases where the t-value was at a significant level for
any sequence of samples in the time-series, we performed permutation testing on the relevant data.
Permutation testing was performed over 10,000 iterations to create a custom empirical null
distribution of the length of samples with significant t-statistics, which was then used to decide
whether an observed sequence was significantly long. This is similar to the use of cluster based
statistics in fMRI, where a fixed, arbitrary threshold is used for creating clusters, then a second
threshold is calculated for determining how large a cluster needs to be before it is statistically
significant. On each iteration, the condition labels for each participant's data were
pseudorandomised, and the original analyses were then repeated exactly, in order to obtain t-
statistics, and sequences of significant t-statistics for the difference between 'SMG' and 'AG'
conditions, under the null hypothesis. From this we were able to assign a p-value to our actual
results by seeing what proportion of the tail of the distribution was greater (or lesser) than or equal
to the actual result. We examined the minimum length of sequences of continuous values in which
|t|>2.201 (i.e., statistically significant at a samplewise p<.05), and also the p-values associated with
the sequences of timepoints in our recorded data where |t|>2.201.
11
Where significantly long sequences were found, we examined the standard kinematic
parameters that occurred during those periods to confirm whether the differences were derived from
the SMG-AG comparison, or if there was further information to be gained from the no-rTMS
baseline. Imitator peak kinematic parameters were examined using one-tailed post-hoc paired t-
tests. To check that any differences were derived from imitator rather than actor performance, we
ran the same analysis on the actor peak values using two-tailed paired t-tests. For all post-hoc paired
t-tests Hedges’ grm was chosen to report effect sizes in repeated measures comparisons (Lakens,
2013).
Results
No significant effects of stimulation site, nor interaction between stimulation site and
meaning were observed in imitation accuracy (Table 1). In hand gestures, the shoulder and elbow
positions were significantly more correlated for meaningless than for meaningful actions. This
effect was also significant in the same direction for the shoulder in finger gestures, and, only when
uncorrected for multiple comparisons, in the elbow.
[TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE]
[FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE]
Figure 2A shows the t-statistic plots for the main effect of SMG versus AG in hand gestures.
Figure 2B shows the t-statistic plots for the interaction between stimulation site and meaning in
hand gestures. Figure 2D shows the t-statistic plots for the interaction between stimulation site and
meaning in finger gestures. No significantly long sequences were observed in these data.
12
Figure 2C shows the t-statistic plots for SMG versus AG in meaningful and meaningless
actions for finger gestures. Permutation testing for the thumb revealed a significant sequence
between 59 and 108 samples (p=.035). The index finger showed a significant sequence between 65
and 119 samples (p=.022). The ring finger showed a significant sequence between 72 and 116
samples (p=.041). The little finger showed a significant sequence between 67 and 114 samples
(p=.036). The middle finger sequence between 70 and 106 samples was not significantly long
(p=.054).
[FIGURE 3 ABOUT HERE]
The mean peak velocity (PV) for the digits in finger gestures was examined post-hoc since
the above significant sequences overlapped with this kinematic parameter. Figure 3 emphasises this
difference in the original data for the thumb. One-tailed t-tests for imitator mean digit PV (Figure 4)
indicated a Bonferroni-corrected significant (p<.025) difference, where stimulation over AG
resulted in a greater mean digit PV than over SMG (t(11)=2.23, p=.024, grm=0.207), and a similar
difference where stimulation over AG resulted in a greater mean digit PV than the sham baseline
(t(11)=2.10, p=.030, grm=0.182). There was no significant difference in mean digit PV between
stimulation over SMG and the sham baseline t(11)=-0.503, p=.303, grm=0.0465).
We then used two-tailed t-tests to perform the same analysis on the PV of the actor in their
finger gestures (Figure 4), which revealed a Bonferroni-corrected significant (p<.025) difference
between mean digit PV in the AG condition and sham baseline (t(11)=2.91, p=.014, grm=0.529).
There was no significant difference in mean digit PV following stimulation over SMG and the sham
baseline (t(11)=1.89, p=.086, grm=0.331), or between SMG and AG conditions (t(11)=-1.71,
p=.115, grm=0.222). This suggested that actor behaviour was biased, so we also decided post-hoc to
examine the imitator PV relative to the actor PV to try and control for the effects of this bias.
13
[FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE]
We examined the imitator mean digit PV relative to the actor mean digit PV in finger
gestures using two-tailed t-tests and a Bonferroni-corrected alpha criterion of .025 (Figure 5). There
were no significant differences using this corrected criterion. However, mean digit relative PV was
reduced following SMG stimulation compared to the sham baseline (t(11)=-2.37, p=.037,
grm=0.335), and also following AG stimulation compared to the sham baseline (t(11)=-2.31, p=.041,
grm=0.281). There was also no significant difference in mean digit relative PV between SMG and
AG stimulation (t(11)=-0.316, p=.758, grm=0.0424).
[FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE]
Discussion
We tested participants’ ability to imitate meaningful and meaningless hand and finger
gestures following rTMS over the left SMG, left AG, or after a no-rTMS baseline. Whilst there
were no differences in imitation accuracy between stimulation sites, we observed that participants'
digit peak velocity was lower relative to the actor in finger gestures following left SMG or left AG
stimulation, though with a larger effect size in the SMG condition. These results provide the first
causal evidence, using brain stimulation in healthy individuals, for a role of the left IPL in
processing the kinematics of finger movements during gesture imitation.
Whilst stimulation did not influence imitation accuracy, there was some evidence for
differences between accuracy in meaningful and meaningless action performance. Interestingly,
participants matched the confederate's shoulder and elbow movements to a significantly greater
degree in meaningless actions. Meaningless actions may rely more on matching action kinematics
14
(e.g., Rumiati & Tessari, 2002; Tessari & Rumiati, 2004; Wild et al., 2010) than meaningful
actions. The fact that effects were only observed in the shoulder and elbow may reflect the fact that
the differences in accuracy were easier to detect in proximal effectors with lower degrees of
freedom.
Examining t-statistics over time, we found significantly long periods during which imitator
finger velocity was significantly lower following rTMS over left SMG compared to left AG during
imitation of finger gestures, but regardless of action meaning. This effect was also reflected in mean
digit PV. To account for possible differences in actor behaviour, we examined imitator mean digit
PV relative to the actor mean digit PV, and found that participants showed significantly reduced
mean digit PV relative to the actor following SMG and AG stimulation compared to baseline. This
result seems to indicate that during the imitation of finger gestures, rTMS to the left SMG or left
AG reduces digit velocity relative to the observed actor. Further study is necessary to understand
the underlying processes altered in this experiment, but these results have the potential to bring
together both findings from apraxia and discussions of the putative mirror neuron system in healthy
individuals.
As noted in the introduction, some suggest that the putative human mirror neuron system
supports our imitative capacity. The IPL is one area of this proposed frontoparietal system
(Hamilton, 2015), that has been suggested to support imitation through visuomotor matching of
seen actions and those that are already in the motor repertoire. This theory provides one explanation
for our result in the absence of any stimulation-related effects specific to action meaning. That is,
disturbed visuomotor matching following stimulation over left IPL could reduce the velocity with
which the fingers shape complex postures relative to the actor. However, this effect does not
necessarily stem from an rTMS-induced inhibition of direct matching. Rather, by considering
15
claims made in the mirror neuron literature and neuroimaging studies of healthy individuals, recent
discussions in apraxia, and recognising the possibility that different areas of the IPL may subserve
different aspects of imitation, we may be able to provide a more holistic explanation for the
observed data.
Buxbaum et al. (2014) found that damage to the left IPL was associated with deficits in
kinematic (rather than postural) aspects of movement for novel and tool-related actions. Similar
results were found in a more recent voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study by Dressing et al.
(2016). Buxbaum et al. (2014) suggested that the left IPL computes "movement plans [as] dynamic
changes in the relative spatial positions of body parts needed to reach a goal configuration" (p. 13).
If this is the case, changes in effector movement relative to the goal (the actor movement) are a
possible consequence of left IPL stimulation. Our results indicate that this is true for both
intransitive meaningful and meaningless actions. Whilst meaningless action imitation may be more
reliant on kinematic processing (Buxbaum et al., 2014), kinematic information might still be
relevant to the way in which one must replicate a meaningful action. To take an extreme example,
the imitator is not likely to ignore explicit but irrelevant kinematics, such as a particularly slow or
rapid action which does not assist in the development of the final posture (see Forbes & Hamilton,
2017, for evidence that possibly supports this).
Our results seem, then, to confirm the importance of the left IPL in meeting the kinematic
requirements of the observed action, over and above the meaning of that action. The sensitivity of
the IPL to observed movement kinematics (Becchio et al., 2012) might substantiate this claim, and
since movement necessarily contains kinematic features, our results may help to explain why left
IPL activity is frequently reported during imitation in healthy individuals, regardless of the type of
action imitated (e.g., Jack et al., 2011; Molenberghs et al., 2010; Mühlau et al., 2005; Tanaka et al.,
2002). There is also evidence, at least for tool-related actions, that damage to the left IPL is more
16
reliably associated with deficits in action performance, rather than action recognition (Tarhan et al.,
2015), potentially confirming the priority that the left IPL places on processing movement during
imitation, rather than semantic encoding (which may be more reliably served by the temporal lobe
e.g., Dressing et al., 2016; Kalénine et al., 2010).
It is worth stressing here that our assertion is made in the light of our use of intransitive
emblematic gestures, whilst the term ‘meaningful gesture’ in apraxia literature commonly refers to
action pantomimes (i.e., pretending to perform a tool-use hand action without the tool in hand). A
more detailed discussion of the distinctions between object-directed and emblematic gestures is
provided by Buxbaum et al., 2005. However, there is evidence that damage to the IPL can be
associated with deficits in both the imitation of communicative and tool-use gestures (e.g., Dressing
et al., 2016), implying that our findings may be applicable beyond the specific action types used in
this experiment. We do, of course, suggest further testing this hypothesis.
It is also worth noting that the size of the IPL indicates that it is not solely involved in the
kinematic matching process we have proposed. Indeed, this was part of our motivation for using
cytoarchitectonically defined brain regions for stimulation. It is perhaps more feasible that the left
parietal lobe is involved in multiple processes for imitation, and that these processes may be
dependent on the type of action to be imitated. For example, the parietal operculum (POp), anterior
to area PF, or the anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS), superior to area PF, have also been found to be
involved in imitation. Specifically, some have suggested that during imitation the left POp is
involved in comparing information about the imitator’s body with the observed actor’s body
(Krüger et al., 2014; Mengotti et al., 2013), and that activity in this area is correlated with the
subsequent accuracy of the imitative action (Krüger et al., 2014). The left aIPS, however, has been
suggested to guide object-directed hand movements (Tunik et al., 2007) and, importantly, also
appears to represent the goals of observed object-directed actions (Hamilton, 2008, 2014; Hamilton
17
& Grafton, 2006, 2007; Sacheli et al., 2015). As such, the aIPS could support the imitation of
object-directed actions because it ostensibly provides a “common representational system for the
actions of self and other” (p. T84, Tunik et al., 2007). The sensitivity of the aIPS for both observed
and performed action goals (Oosterhof et al., 2010) is in keeping with claims that this area may be
part of the putative human mirror neuron system (Tunik et al., 2007). Lastly, Goldenberg &
Randerath (2015) suggested that the left IPL could have a role in the apprehension of spatial
relationships. They report that damage to this area can result in shared deficits in the imitation of
meaningless hand gestures, which require placing the hand in relation to other parts of the body
whilst the finger positions remain invariant, and in tasks such as the Token Test which require
patients to classify objects based on their spatial and physical properties.
With the above in mind we can distinguish four potential elements of imitation served by the
left parietal lobe. Left area PF and/or area PG - the parts of the SMG and AG stimulated in this
experiment - could be involved in creating movement plans based on the spatiotemporal
requirements of the to-be-performed action (i.e., the kinematics), regardless of whether the action is
imitative. The POp could then ensure that these spatiotemporal requirements are met during
imitative scenarios, by comparing the imitator’s body to that of the actor (Krüger et al., 2014;
Mengotti et al., 2013). This checking process may be essential for meaningless intransitive actions,
in the absence of objects to provide context, and could provide one explanation for the defective
imitation of meaningless gestures frequently associated with left IPL damage (Goldenberg, 2009).
The aIPS, in contrast, could be useful for informing imitation in object-directed scenarios (but see
Martin et al., 2016), where visuomotor matching between the observed and to-be-performed action
can be done at the goal rather than movement level (Hamilton, 2014). There may also be a role for
the left IPL in processing spatial relationships during imitation. This may be particularly important
for the imitation of meaningless hand gestures (Goldenberg & Karnath, 2006; Goldenberg &
Randerath, 2015), since this task could be reliant on the decomposition of visual information about
18
the observed gesture into “simple spatial relationships between a limited number of defined body
parts” (p. 47, Goldenberg & Randerath, 2015). The absence of specific effects for meaningless hand
gesture imitation in our experiment is discussed later.
In light of the discussion above, it is also necessary for us to comment on the similar effects
observed for both of our active stimulation sites. Unfortunately, our study was not able to reveal
more about the possible division of labour between the left SMG and AG. The existence of similar
effects in both the left SMG and AG may not necessarily reflect similar roles for each of these
regions, but perhaps the connectivity between them. For example, the left AG stimulation may have
had an effect on information processing between the AG and SMG, with the greater effect size
observed following SMG stimulation (Hedges’ grm = 0.335 versus 0.281) partially supportive of this
claim. Since the AG is anatomically connected to the posterior SMG (Seghier, 2013), rTMS over
both areas might induce a reduction in efficient SMG functioning if information regarding the
kinematic constraints of the observed action is passed in a posterior-anterior (AG to SMG) fashion.
It is also worth noting that a previous study indicated that the AG could be involved in both
meaningful and meaningless action imitation (Vanbellingen et al., 2014). However, the location of
AG in that experiment was more ventral and anterior to ours.
An alternative explanation for the similar effects observed for both stimulation sites is that
our results reflect an influence of unspecified peripheral stimulation of another area of the IPL, for
example, cytoarchitectonic area PFm, which lies between PF (in the SMG) and PGa (AG).
Interestingly, a previous study by Weiss et al. (2013) using transcranial direct current stimulation
(tDCS), found that anodal stimulation over left area PFm improved participants’ gesture matching
ability. However, the authors suggested that their result could have been driven by a combined
effect of SMG and AG stimulation, considering the central position of PFm, and the size of tDCS
19
electrodes. Weiss et al. (2013) stated that both parts of the IPL may have to be stimulated
simultaneously in order to facilitate gesture matching. Considering our results, the same could be
true for influencing gesture imitation, and further highlights the importance of considering in detail
the role of different regions of the IPL, along with their interactions.
One response to this problem is to suggest further neurostimulation research aimed at
disentangling the relative contributions of the seven parcellated regions of the IPL (PFt, PFop [the
POp], PFcm, PF, PFm, PGa, PGp), along with the aIPS. Such an endeavour would be essential in
order to clarify how different areas of the left parietal lobe may or may not interact, and their
relative contributions to different types of action and action imitation. In addition, neuroimaging
approaches that compare brain activity with actual imitative performance (i.e., Frey & Gerry, 2006;
Krüger et al., 2014), across different types of action, are also likely to be invaluable. In general we
suggest that, where possible, researchers try to report in more detail at least the distinction between
the SMG and AG, if not their parcellated subregions, rather than stating a role for the IPL in
general.
A potential limitation of this study, and another possible explanation for the consistent
effects observed in both the SMG and AG, is that these results simply reflect a TMS-general
influence, in the absence of an active control site. TMS can cause changes in behaviour distinctly
unrelated to changes in cortical activity, whereby the clicking sound of the TMS coil alone can
facilitate or inhibit reaction times dependent on the time of stimulation within a trial (Duecker et al.,
2013; Meteyard & Holmes, 2018). However, as far as we are aware this has only been found for
online TMS protocols, and we are not aware of data suggesting that the offline approach we used is
also likely to result in ‘mere-presence’ effects of rTMS. In addition, the no-rTMS session involved
much of the same conditions as in the active stimulation sessions (e.g., 15 minutes of waiting during
TMS application, clicking sound).
20
Finally, it is interesting that our primary result was found specifically for finger gestures,
rather than hand gestures. This is partly at odds with some previous findings in apraxia, though our
stimuli were not modelled on previous distinctions between hand and finger gestures our main
aim was to ensure there was a sufficient number of different emblematic stimuli for participants to
copy. There is some evidence to suggest that defective finger gesture imitation is more associated
with left inferior frontal gyrus damage, compared to defective hand gesture imitation, which is more
associated with left IPL damage (Goldenberg & Karnath, 2006). As stated above, some discuss the
role of the left IPL in hand gesture imitation in terms of ‘body part coding’, where hand gesture
imitation is reliant on the spatial mapping of the hand in relation to other body parts (e.g.,
Goldenberg & Karnath, 2006; Goldenberg & Randerath, 2015). However, some experimenters have
failed to find a dissociation for brain regions related to defective hand and finger imitation (Achilles
et al., 2017). It is also possible that the movement to attain the posture, rather than the final hand
position alone may be an important factor. For example, whilst the Goldenberg (1996) assessment
for meaningless gestures (as used by Goldenberg & Karnath, 2006 and Goldenberg & Randerath,
2015) considers only performance on the final gesture posture, Buxbaum et al. (2014) examined
dynamic movements, hence their suggestion that dynamic change in body part position is important.
It is possible that the difference between kinematic and postural elements of movement may take
precedence over the difference between hand and finger gestures, though further research would be
needed to clarify this. If this is the case, the fact we only observed kinematic effects in the finger
velocities for finger-specific gestures may reflect the fact that the dynamic change of each of the
fingers independently is more complex than the movement of the hand as a whole.
In conclusion, we found that the left IPL is involved in matching observed digit velocity in
action imitation, regardless of action meaning. More work is needed to expand on how imitation
21
kinematics are processed in the left IPL, and the relative contributions of the SMG and AG and their
respective cytoarchitectonic subregions. These nuances can be examined using neuronavigated
TMS. Our results highlight that brain stimulation may help close the gap in understanding imitation
in apraxia and in healthy people, particularly if it is combined with large-scale motion-tracking.
Acknowledgements
Many thanks to Dr. Anastasia Christakou and Dr. Peter Scarfe for their helpful comments on
a draft of this work. This research was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council
(grant number ES/J500148/1).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors report no conflict of interest.
Author contributions
All authors have made significant contributions to this article. In particular, experiment
design, analysis, and manuscript writing by ATR and NPH, proofing by ATR, BPR, JEM, KC, and
NPH, and participant recruitment and data collection by ATR, BPR, JEM, and KC.
Data accessibility statement:
Raw data and analysis scripts are freely available from the corresponding author (ATR).
Abbreviations
AG: angular gyrus
aIPS: anterior intraparietal sulcus
IPL: inferior parietal lobule
MF: meaningful
22
ML: meaningless
POp: parietal operculum
rTMS: repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation
SMG: supramarginal gyrus
References
Achilles, E.I.S., Weiss, P.H., Fink, G.R., Binder, E., Price, C.J., & Hope, T.M.H. (2017). Using
multi-level Bayesian lesion-symptom mapping to probe the body-part-specificity of gesture
imitation skills. NeuroImage, 161, 94-103. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.036
Bien, N., Roebroeck, A., Goebel, R., & Sack, A.T. (2009). The brain’s intention to imitate: the
neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation. Cereb. Cortex, 19, 2338-2351.
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn251
Becchio, C., Cavallo, A., Begliomini, C., Sartori, L., Feltrin, G., & Castiello, U. (2012). Social
grasping: From mirroring to mentalizing. NeuroImage, 61, 240-248.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.013
Braadbaart, L., Waiter, G.D., & Williams, J.H.G. (2012). Neural correlates of individual differences
in manual imitation fidelity. Front. Integr. Neurosci., 6, 91. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00091
Buxbaum, L.J., Kyle, K.M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: internal
representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans.
Cog. Brain Res., 25, 226-239. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.014
23
Buxbaum, L.J., Shapiro, A.D., & Coslett, H.B. (2014). Critical brain regions for tool-related and
imitative actions: a componential analysis. Brain, 137, 1971-1985. doi:10.1093/brain/awu111
Campione, G.C., & Gentilucci, M. (2011). Is the observation of the human kinematics sufficient to
activate automatic imitation of transitive actions? Behav. Brain Res., 225, 201-208.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.025
Carmo, J.C., Rumiati, R.I., & Vallesi, A. (2012). Understanding and imitating unfamiliar actions:
distinct underlying mechanisms. PLOS ONE, 7, e46939. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046939
Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human
inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage,
33, 430-448. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.054
Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S.B., Geyer, S., Scheperjams, F., Mohlberg, H., Zilles, K., & Amunts, K.
(2008). The human inferior parietal lobule in stereotaxic space. Brain Struct. Func., 212, 481-495.
doi:10.1007/s00429-008-0195-z
Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A.R., & Eickhoff, S.B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action
observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage, 50, 1148-1167.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112
Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F.
(1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: influence of action content and subject’s
strategy. Brain, 120, 1763-1777.
24
Dressing, A., Nitschke, K., Kümmerer, D., Bormann, T., Beume, L., Schmidt, C.S.M., Ludwig,
V.M., Mader, I., Willmes, K., Rijntjes, M., Kaller, C.P., Weiller, C., & Martin, M. (2016). Distinct
contributions of dorsal and ventral streams to imitation of tool-use and communicative gestures.
Cereb. Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw383
Duecker, F., de Graaf, T.A., Jacobs, C., & Sack, A.T. (2013). Time- and task-dependent non-neural
effects of real and sham TMS. PLOS ONE, 8, e73813. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073813
Forbes, P.A.G., & Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2017). Moving higher and higher: imitators’ movements
are sensitive to observed trajectories regardless of action rationality. Exp. Brain Res.
doi:10.1007/s00221-017-5006-4
Frey, S.H., & Gerry, V.E. (2006). Modulation of neural activity during observational learning of
actions and their sequential orders. J. Neurosci., 26, 13194-13201. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3914-
06.2006
Gold, B.J., Pomplun, M., Rice, N.J., & Sekuler, R. (2008). A new way to quantify the fidelity of
imitation: preliminary results with gesture sequences. Exp. Brain Res., 187, 139-152.
doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1291-2
Goldenberg, G. (1996). Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right
hemispheres. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 61, 176-180.
Goldenberg, G. (2009). Apraxia and the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1449-1459.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.014
25
Goldenberg, G., & Karnath, H. (2006). The neural basis of imitation is body part specific. J.
Neurosci., 26, 6282-6287. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-06.2006
Goldenberg, G., & Randerath, J. (2015). Shared neural substrates of apraxia and aphasia.
Neuropsychologia, 75, 40-49. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.017
Grèzes, J. (1998). Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet
investigation. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 15, 553-582. doi:10.1080/026432998381023
Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2008). Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: A theoretical approach
to imitation in autism. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 61, 101-115. doi:10.1080/17470210701508798
Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2014). Cognitive underpinnings of social interaction. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 68,
417-412. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.973424
Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2015). The neurocognitive mechanisms of imitation. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.,
3, 63-67. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.011
Hamilton, A.F. de C., & Grafton, S.T. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal
sulcus. J. Neurosci., 26, 1133-1137. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006
Hamilton, A.F. de C., & Grafton, S.T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and
intentions. In Rosetti, Y., Kawato, M., & Haggard, P. (eds), Attention & Performance 22. Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
26
Hayes, S.J., Dutoy, C.A., Elliott, D., Gowen, E., & Bennett, S.J. (2016). Atypical biological motion
kinematics are represented by complementary lower-level and top-down processes during imitation
learning. Acta Psychol., 163, 10-16. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.10.005
Hermsdörfer, J., Mai, N., Spatt, J., Marquardt, C., Veltkamp, R., & Goldenberg, G. (1996).
Kinematic analysis of movement imitation in apraxia. Brain, 119, 1575-1586.
Higuchi, S., Holle, H., Roberts, N., Eickhoff, S.B., & Vogt, S. (2012). Imitation and observational
learning of hand actions: prefrontal involvement and connectivity. NeuroImage, 59, 1668-1683.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.021
Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 60, 653-670.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604
Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its
dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 7, 942-951. doi:10.1038/nrn2024
Jack, A., Englander, Z.A., & Morris, J.P. (2011). Subcortical contributions to effective connectivity
in brain networks supporting imitation. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3689-3698.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.024
Kalénine, S., Buxbaum, L.J., & Coslett, H.B. (2010). Critical brain regions for action recognition:
lesion symptom mapping in left hemisphere stroke. Brain, 133, 3269-3280.
doi:10.1093/brain/awq210
27
Koski, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M., Woods, R.P., & Mazziotta, J.C. (2003). Modulation of
cortical activity during different imitative behaviors. J. Neurophysiol., 89, 460-471.
doi:10.1152/jn.00248.2002
Krishnan-Barman, S., Forbes, P.A.G., & Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2017). How can the study of action
kinematics inform our understanding of human social interaction? Neuropsychologia, 105, 101-110.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.018
Króliczak, G., Piper, B.J., & Frey, S.H. (2016). Specialization of the left supramarginal gyrus for
hand-independent praxis representation is not related to hand dominance. Neuropsychologia, 93,
501-512. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.023.
Krüger, B., Bischoff, M., Blecker, C., Langhanns, C., Kindermann, S., Sauerbier, I., Reiser, M.,
Stark, R., Munzert, J., & Pilgramm, S. (2014). Parietal and premotor cortices: Activation reflects
imitation accuracy during observation, delayed imitation and concurrent imitation. NeuroImage,
100, 39-50. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.074
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical
primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol., 4, 863. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
Martin, M., Nitschke, K., Beume, L., Dressing, A., Bühler, L.E., Ludwig, V.M., Mader, I., Rijntjes,
M., Kaller, C.P., & Weiller, C. Brain activity underlying tool-related and imitative skills after major
left hemisphere stroke. Brain, 139, 1497-1516. doi:10.1093/brain/aww035
28
Menz, M.M., McNamara, A., Klemen, J., & Binkofski, F. (2009). Dissociating networks of
imitation. Hum. Brain Mapp., 30, 3339-3350. doi:10.1002/hbm.20756
Mengotti, P., Ticini, L.F., Waszak, F., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Rumiati, R.I. (2013). Imitating others'
actions: transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal opercula reveals the processes underlying
automatic imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 316322. doi:10.1111/ejn.12019
Meteyard, L., & Holmes, N.P. (under review) TMS SMART scalp mapping of annoyance ratings
and twitches caused by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. Meth.
Molenberghs, P., Brander, C., Mattingley, J.B., & Cunnington, R. (2010). The role of the superior
remporal sulcus and the mirror neuron system in imitation. Hum. Brain Mapp., 31, 1316-1326.
doi:10.1002/hbm.20938
Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J.B. (2009). Is the mirror neuron system involved
in imitation? A short review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 33, 975-980.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.03.010
Mühlau, M., Hermsdörfer, J., Goldenberg, G., Wohlschläger, A.M., Castrop, F., Stahl, R.,
Röttinger, M., Erhard, P., Haslinger, B., Ceballos-Baumann, A.O., Conrad, B., & Boecker, H.
(2005). Left inferior parietal dominance in gesture imitation: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 43,
1086-1098. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.004
Oosterhof, N.N., Wiggett, A.J., Diedrichsen, J., Tipper, S.P., & Downing, P.E. (2010). Surface-
based information mapping reveals crossmodal visionaction representations in human parietal and
occipitotemporal cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 104, 1077-1089. doi:10.1152/jn.00326.2010
29
Pan, X., & Hamilton, A.F. de C. (2015). Automatic imitation in a rich social context with virtual
characters. Front. Psychol., 6, 790. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00790
Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2008). Stimulus-driven selection of routes to imitation. Exp. Brain Res.,
188, 147-152. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1422-9
Reader, A.T., & Holmes, N.P. (2015). Video stimuli reduce object-directed imitation accuracy: a
novel two-person motion-tracking approach. Front. Psychol., 6, 644. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00644
Reader, A.T., & Holmes, N.P. (2016). Examining ecological validity in social interaction: problems
of visual fidelity, gaze, and social potential. Culture and Brain, 4, 134-146. doi:10.1007/s40167-
016-0041-8
Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms underlying
the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. Physiol.
Rev., 94, 655-706. doi:10.1152/physrev.00009.2013.
Rossini, P.M., Barker, A.T., Berardelli, A., Caramia, M.D., Caruso, G., Cracco, R.Q., Dimitrijević,
M.R., Hallett, M., Katayama, Y., Lücking, C.H., et al. (1994). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic
stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical
application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 91, 79-92.
Rumiati, R.I., & Tessari, A. (2002). Imitation of novel and well-known actions: the role of short
term memory. Exp. Brain Res., 142, 425-433. doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0956-x
30
Rumiati, R.I., Weiss, P.H., Tessari, A., Assmus, A., Zilles, K., Herzog, H., & Fink, G.R. (2005).
Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless
actions. J. Cognitive Neurosci., 17, 1420-1431.
Sacheli, L.M., Candidi, M., Pavone, E.F., Tidoni, E., & Aglioti, S.M. (2012). And yet they act
together: interpersonal perception modulates visuo-motor interference and mutual adjustments
during a joint-grasping task. PLOS ONE, 7, e50223. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050223
Sacheli, L.M., Tidoni, E., Pavone, E.F., Aglioti, S.M., & Candidi, M. (2013). Kinematics
fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions. Exp. Brain Res.,
226, 473-486. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3459-7
Sacheli, L.M., Candidi, M., Era, V., & Aglioti, S.M. (2015). Causative role of left aIPS in coding
shared goals during humanavatar complementary joint actions. Nat. Commun., 6, 7544.
doi:10.1038/ncomms8544
Sacheli, L.M., Christensen, A.., Giese, M.A., Taubert, N., Pavone, E.F., Aglioti, S.M., & Candidi,
M. (2015b). Prejudiced interactions: implicit racial bias reduces predictive simulation during joint
action with an out-group avatar. Sci. Rep., 5, 8507. doi:10.1038/srep08507
Seghier, M.L. (2013). The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. The
Neuroscientist, 19, 43-61. doi:10.1177/1073858412440596
31
Stokes, M.G., Chambers, C.D., Gould, I.C., English, T., McNaught, E., McDonald, O., &
Mattingley, J.B. (2007). Distance-adjusted motor threshold for transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Clin. Neurophysiol., 118, 1617-1625. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.004
Tanaka, S., Inui, T., Iwaki, S., Konishi, J., & Nakai, T. (2001). Neural substrates involved in
imitating finger configurations: an fMRI study. NeuroReport, 12, 1171-1174.
Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2002). Cortical involvement for action imitation of hand/arm postures versus
finger configurations: an fMRI study. NeuroReport, 13, 1599-1602.
Tarhan, L.Y., Watson, C.E., & Buxbaum, L.J. (2015). Shared and distinct neuroanatomic regions
critical for tool-related action production and recognition: evidence from 131 left-hemisphere stroke
patients. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 27, 2491-2511. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00876
Tessari, A., & Rumiati, R.I. (2004). The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation of actions.
J. Exp. Psychol.-Hum. Percept. Perform., 30, 1107-1116. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.6.1107
Tunik, E., Rice, N.J., Hamilton, A., & Grafton, S.T. (2007). Beyond grasping: representation of
action in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage, 36, T77-T86.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.026
Vanbellingen, T., Berschi, M., Nyffler, T., Cazzoli, D., Wiest, R., Bassetti, C., Kaelin-Lang, A.,
Müri, R., & Bohlhalter, S. (2014). Left posterior parietal theta burst stimulation affects gestural
imitation regardless of semantic content. Clin. Neurophysiol., 125, 457-462.
doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2013.07.024
32
Weiss, P.H., Achilles, E.I.S., Moos, K., Hesse, M.D., Sparing, R., & Fink, G.R. (2013).
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of left parietal cortex facilitates gesture processing in
healthy subjects. J. Neurosci., 33, 19205-19211. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4714-12.2013
Wild, K.S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2010). The influence of goals on movement
kinematics during imitation. Exp. Brain Res., 204, 353-360. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2034-8
Williams, J.H.G., Casey, J.M., Braadbaart, L., Culmer, P.R., & Mon-Williams, M. (2013).
Kinematic measures of imitation fidelity in primary school children. J. Cogn. Dev., 15, 345-362.
doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.771265
33
Table 1: Two-way ANOVA main effects and interactions for actor-imitator correlations (Z-
values) in hand and finger gestures; significant p-values (<.025) are in bold; SH: shoulder,
EL: elbow, WR: wrist, TH: thumb, IN: index finger, MI: middle finger, RI: ring finger, LI:
little finger
Gesture
Tracker
Mean(±SE) Z-value
Main effect
Site*meaning
interaction
Site
Meaning
AG
No-
rTMS
MF
ML
Site
Meaning
F
(2,22)
p
ƞp2
F
(2,22)
p
ƞp2
F (1,11)
p
ƞp2
Hand
SH
0.485
(0.0687)
0.525
(0.0534)
0.388
(0.0514)
0.622
(0.0669)
0.243
.787
.022
39.2
<.001
.781
2.84
.080
.205
EL
0.860
(0.0989)
0.918
(0.0708)
0.753
(0.0862)
1.04
(0.0679)
0.253
.779
.022
28.8
<.001
.723
1.48
.250
.118
WR
1.57
(0.0740)
1.50
(0.0673)
1.54
(0.0658)
1.53
(0.0822)
1.07
.359
.089
0.051
.826
.005
0.256
.777
.023
TH
1.53
(0.0835)
1.52
(0.0646)
1.53
(0.0730)
1.53
(0.0708)
0.076
.927
.007
0.002
.966
<.001
0.905
.419
.076
IN
1.50
(0.0772)
1.49
(0.0650)
1.48
(0.0766)
1.50
(0.0686)
0.045
.956
.004
0.263
.618
.023
0.696
.509
.060
MI
1.52
(0.0744)
1.52
(0.0615)
1.51
(0.0750)
1.54
(0.0638)
0.004
.997
<.001
0.322
.582
.028
0.698
.508
.060
RI
1.52
(0.0738)
1.52
(0.0616)
1.51
(0.0743)
1.53
(0.0621)
0.027
.974
.002
0.173
.685
.015
0.694
.510
.059
LI
1.52
(0.0749)
1.52
(0.0624)
1.52
(0.0769)
1.52
(0.0648)
0.011
.989
.001
0.014
.909
.001
0.620
.547
.053
Finger
SH
0.119
(0.0398)
0.112
(0.0280)
0.0486
(0.0309)
0.187
(0.0277)
0.040
.961
.004
37.1
<.001
.771
0.099
.906
.009
EL
0.492
(0.112)
0.359
(0.0683)
0.386
(0.0791)
0.488
(0.0710)
1.14
.337
.094
6.54
.027
.373
2.03
.155
.156
WR
1.34
(0.0603)
1.21
(0.0633)
1.31
(0.0594)
1.25
(0.0694)
3.19
.061
.225
1.95
.190
.151
1.33
.285
.108
TH
1.31
(0.0671)
1.20
(0.0553)
1.30
(0.0614)
1.23
(0.0469)
2.18
.137
.165
2.27
.160
.171
0.045
.956
.004
IN
1.24
(0.0701)
1.14
(0.0610)
1.20
(0.0654)
1.17
(0.0495)
1.53
.239
.122
.579
.463
.050
0.739
.489
.063
MI
1.31
(0.0516)
1.19
(0.0513)
1.28
(0.0507)
1.20
(0.0403)
2.85
.079
.206
3.43
.091
.238
.088
.916
.008
RI
1.31
(0.0511)
1.19
(0.0506)
1.27
(0.0540)
1.23
(0.0414)
2.77
.085
.201
1.17
.303
.096
0.594
.561
.051
LI
1.31
(0.0595)
1.18
(0.0487)
1.28
(0.0536)
1.20
(0.0421)
2.90
.076
.208
6.45
.028
.370
0.772
.474
.066
34
Figure 1: A) Example meaningful finger and hand gestures alongside their matched
meaningless counterparts. B) Experimental set-up. Dots indicate the location of motion
trackers. The tracking box was placed under the table, and the actor's actions were cued
through images displayed on a computer screen that was not observable to the imitator. C)
95% confidence ellipsoids for the TMS target sites shown on a representative participant’s
brain. Mean±95% CI MNI coordinates for the SMG: x=-57±2.81 y=-44±4.42 z=44±3.12, and
AG: x=-37±4.37 y=-77±3.10 z=40±6.64
35
Figure 2: t-statistic plots for resampled comparisons in all trackers; A) SMG versus AG in
hand gestures B) stimulation site* meaning interaction in hand gestures C) SMG versus AG
in finger gestures D) site*meaning interaction in finger gestures; In all plots the black
horizontal lines indicate positive and negative critical t-values. Shoulder in dashed magenta,
elbow in dashed cyan, wrist in dashed grey, thumb in solid light green, index finger in solid
blue, middle finger in solid orange, ring finger in solid purple, little finger in solid dark green
(in colour online).
36
Figure 3: Original mean velocity curve for the thumb comparing SMG and AG in finger
gestures. SMG in blue, AG in red (in colour online), dashed lines indicate SE.
37
Figure 4: Mean digit peak velocity for finger gestures in imitator and actor data; Error bars
indicate standard error, whilst single points show individual participant values. * = p<.05
corrected, † = p<.05 uncorrected.
38
Figure 5: Mean digit peak velocity for finger gestures in imitator relative to actor data; Error
bars indicate standard error, whilst single points show individual participant values. † = p<.05
uncorrected.
... Thakral et al. 2020), (2) individual MRI-guided neuronavigation based on macro-anatomical landmarks (a priori, or post hoc, if combined with an a priori functional localizer; e.g. Reader et al. 2018;Rushworth et al. 2001), 3) MRI-guided neuronavigation based on average coordinates in stereotaxic space (from meta-analyses or single studies), projected on the normalized brain of a participant and then back-transformed to coordinates in native space for site identification (e.g. Varnava et al. 2013). ...
... The TMS protocols used in these studies (see Supplementary Table 1, Motor planning worksheet) included inhibitory cTBS (50 Hz) delivered off-line for a 40-s window (Adam et al. 2016) and off-line 1-Hz monophasic rTMS delivered for 15 min (Reader et al. 2018). One study tested 16 righthanded participants (Adam et al. 2016), while the other tested 12 participants (10 right-handed and 2 left-handed) (Reader et al. 2018). ...
... The TMS protocols used in these studies (see Supplementary Table 1, Motor planning worksheet) included inhibitory cTBS (50 Hz) delivered off-line for a 40-s window (Adam et al. 2016) and off-line 1-Hz monophasic rTMS delivered for 15 min (Reader et al. 2018). One study tested 16 righthanded participants (Adam et al. 2016), while the other tested 12 participants (10 right-handed and 2 left-handed) (Reader et al. 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a non-invasive technique that can transiently interfere with local cortical functioning, thus enabling inferences of causal left AG involvement in higher functions from experimentation with healthy participants. Here, we examine 35 studies that measure behavioural outcomes soon after or during targeting TMS to the left AG, by design and as documented by individual magnetic resonance images, in healthy adult participants. The reviewed evidence suggests a specific causal involvement of the left AG in a wide range of tasks involving language, memory, number processing, visuospatial attention, body awareness and motor planning functions. These core findings are particularly valuable to inform theoretical models of the left AG role(s) in higher functions, due to the anatomical specificity afforded by the selected studies and the complementarity of TMS to different methods of investigation. In particular, the variety of the operations within and between functions in which the left AG appears to be causally involved poses a formidable challenge to any attempts to identify a single computational process subserved by the left AG (as opposed to just outlining a broad type of functional contribution) that could apply across thematic areas. We conclude by highlighting directions for improvement in future experimentation with TMS, in order to strengthen the available evidence, while taking into account the anatomical heterogeneity of this brain region.
... In a previous experiment (Reader et al. 2018a), in which we motion-tracked participants' wrist movements during meaningless and emblematic meaningful gesture imitation, we observed that the imitation of meaningless gestures was associated with a longer correction period than the imitation of meaningful gestures. That is, the deceleration phase of their wrist movements was longer, as reflected by a relatively earlier time to peak velocity (TPV/MT) and time to peak deceleration (TPD/MT), despite a longer overall movement time (MT). ...
... The block order was counterbalanced across sessions and participants. Meaningless and meaningful actions were presented in separate blocks, since there is some evidence to suggest that performing novel and known actions in a sequence recruits a single processing route, whilst presenting them separately recruits separate routes (Tessari and Cubelli 2014;Tessari and Rumiati 2004, but see; Press and Heyes 2008;Reader et al. 2018a). Hand and finger gestures were pseudorandomly interleaved within each separate block of action meaning. ...
... In keeping with Reader et al. (2018a), four imitator wrist kinematic variables were extracted: MT, PV, TPV/MT, and TPD/MT. The mean values of these variables across every trial for each condition were analysed using repeated-measures ANOVAs with three levels: stimulation site (pMTG, vertex), action meaning (meaningful, meaningless), and action effector (hand, finger). ...
Article
Full-text available
Results from neuropsychological studies, and neuroimaging and behavioural experiments with healthy individuals, suggest that the imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions may be reliant on different processing routes. The left posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) is one area that might be important for the recognition and imitation of meaningful actions. We studied the role of the left pMTG in imitation using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and two-person motion-tracking. Participants imitated meaningless and emblematic meaningful hand and finger gestures performed by a confederate actor whilst both individuals were motion-tracked. rTMS was applied during action observation (before imitation) over the left pMTG or a vertex control site. Since meaningless action imitation has been previously associated with a greater wrist velocity and longer correction period at the end of the movement, we hypothesised that stimulation over the left pMTG would increase wrist velocity and extend the correction period of meaningful actions (i.e., due to interference with action recognition). We also hypothesised that imitator accuracy (actor-imitator correspondence) would be reduced following stimulation over the left pMTG. Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that stimulation over the pMTG, but not the vertex, during action observation reduced wrist velocity when participants later imitated meaningful, but not meaningless, hand gestures. These results provide causal evidence for a role of the left pMTG in the imitation of meaningful gestures, and may also be in keeping with proposals that left posterior temporal regions play a role in the production of postural components of gesture.
... Causal methodologies, including brain stimulation and patient studies, have continued to support the consensus that mirror neuron brain areas contribute to imitation. Two studies using facilitatory brain stimulation to inferior frontal gyrus demonstrated improvements in vocal imitation and naturalistic mimicry (Restle et al., 2012;Hogeveen et al., 2015), while inhibitory stimulation to the inferior parietal lobule slowed participants in an instructed imitation task (Reader et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Ten years ago, Perspectives in Psychological Science published the Mirror Neuron Forum, in which authors debated the role of mirror neurons in action understanding, speech, imitation, and autism and asked whether mirror neurons are acquired through visual-motor learning. Subsequent research on these themes has made significant advances, which should encourage further, more systematic research. For action understanding, multivoxel pattern analysis, patient studies, and brain stimulation suggest that mirror-neuron brain areas contribute to low-level processing of observed actions (e.g., distinguishing types of grip) but not to high-level action interpretation (e.g., inferring actors’ intentions). In the area of speech perception, although it remains unclear whether mirror neurons play a specific, causal role in speech perception, there is compelling evidence for the involvement of the motor system in the discrimination of speech in perceptually noisy conditions. For imitation, there is strong evidence from patient, brain-stimulation, and brain-imaging studies that mirror-neuron brain areas play a causal role in copying of body movement topography. In the area of autism, studies using behavioral and neurological measures have tried and failed to find evidence supporting the “broken-mirror theory” of autism. Furthermore, research on the origin of mirror neurons has confirmed the importance of domain-general visual-motor associative learning rather than canalized visual-motor learning, or motor learning alone.
... [23] Moreover, through the transcranial magnetic stimulation, the functional connectivity in the DMN was also altered. [25][26][27][28] Via a meta-analysis and systematic review, it was found that electroconvulsive therapy could be related to the depression. [29] Consequently, our investigation on first-episode treatment-naive people with MDD may yield the benefit of reducing these confoundings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and objective: Previous studies have shown that the default-mode network (DMN) has a substantial role in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD). However, there is a shortage of information regarding variations in the functional connectivity (FC) of the DMN of treatment-naive patients with first-episode MDD. The present study aims to explore the FC of the DMN in such patients. Methods: The study population consisted of 33 patients and 35 controls, paired regarding age, gender, education level, and health condition. Depression severity was assessed through the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAM-D), and subjects underwent evaluation during the resting-state through functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). To assess the result, we used FC and ICA. We used Spearman's correlation test to detect potential correlations between anomalous FC and severity of HAM-D scores. Results: We have found a decreased FC in the left medial orbitofrontal gyrus (MOFG) and right marginal gyrus (SMG) in depressive patients compared to controls. There was a negative correlation between abnormal FC in the right SMG and HAM-D scores. We have not found any increase in FC of the DMN in treatment-naive, first-episode of MDD patients. Conclusions: Our study provided evidence of a negative correlation between abnormal FC in the DMN and severity of depression symptoms measured by HAM-D in treatment-naive MDD patients. This finding could shed some light on the relevance of DMN for understanding the pathophysiology of cognitive impairment in MDD.
... Causal methodologies, including brain stimulation and patient studies, have continued to support the consensus that mirror neuron brain areas contribute to imitation. Two studies using facilitatory brain stimulation to inferior frontal gyrus demonstrated improvements in vocal imitation and naturalistic mimicry (Restle et al., 2012;Hogeveen et al., 2015), while inhibitory stimulation to the inferior parietal lobule slowed participants in an instructed imitation task (Reader et al., 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Ten years ago, Perspectives in Psychological Science published the Mirror Neuron Forum, debating the role of mirror neurons in action understanding, speech, imitation and autism, and asking whether mirror neurons are acquired through visual-motor learning. Subsequent research on these themes has made significant advances, which should encourage further system-level research: Action understanding - Multivoxel pattern analysis, patient studies, and brain stimulation suggest that mirror neuron brain areas contribute to low-level processing of observed actions (e.g. distinguishing types of grip), but not to high-level action interpretation (e.g. inferring actors’ intentions). Speech perception – Although it remains unclear whether mirror neurons play a specific, causal role in speech perception, there is compelling evidence for the involvement of the motor system in the discrimination of speech in perceptually noisy conditions. Imitation – There is strong evidence from patient, brain stimulation and brain imaging studies that mirror neuron brain areas play a causal role in copying of body movement topography. Autism – Studies using behavioural and neurological measures have tried and failed to find evidence supporting the “broken mirror” theory of autism. Furthermore, research on the origin of mirror neurons has confirmed the importance of domain-general visual-motor learning, rather than canalised visual-motor learning, or motor learning alone.
... Some patients show disturbed trajectories but achieve a proper end-position of the tested effector (e.g., hand), while other patients exhibit a smooth (movement) trajectory to a wrong end-position (Hermsdörfer et al., 1996). Thus, BSD deficits may contribute to apraxic end-position errors during imitation rather than to deficits in (movement) trajectories (Reader et al., 2018). ...
Article
Apraxia is a common cognitive deficit after left hemisphere (LH) stroke. It has been suggested that a disturbed representation of the human body underlies apraxic imitation deficits. Thus, we here tested the hypothesis that a deficient body structural description (BSD), i.e., a deficient representation of a body part's position (relative to a standard human body), contributes to apraxic end-position errors in imitation, while controlling for deficits in the semantic representation of the human body (body image, BI) and naming deficits. A quantitative pointing task to assess putative BSD deficits and an apraxia assessment, including imitation and pantomime tasks, were applied to 27 patients with LH stroke and 19 healthy subjects. While LH stroke patients without apraxia (n=15) did not differ from control subjects in their pointing performance, patients suffering from imitation apraxia (n=10) showed a differential deficit when pointing to body parts of other humans compared to object parts. Voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) revealed an association of these differential pointing deficits (indicating a deficient BSD) with lesions in the angular gyrus of the left inferior parietal cortex. This first quantitative group study of BSD deficits in LH stroke patients supports the notion that apraxic end-position errors in imitation are – at least in part – due to a deficient coding of the position of human body parts.
Article
Full-text available
While influential works since the 1970s have widely assumed that imitation is an innate skill in both human and non-human primate neonates, recent empirical studies and meta-analyses have challenged this view, indicating other forms of reward-based learning as relevant factors in the development of social behavior. The visual input translation into matching motor output that underlies imitation abilities instead seems to develop along with social interactions and sensorimotor experience during infancy and childhood. Recently, a new visual stream has been identified in both human and non-human primate brains, updating the dual visual stream model. This third pathway is thought to be specialized for dynamics aspects of social perceptions such as eye-gaze, facial expression and crucially for audiovisual integration of speech. Here, we review empirical studies addressing an understudied but crucial aspect of speech and communication, namely the processing of visual orofacial cues (i.e., the perception of a speaker's lips and tongue movements) and its integration with vocal auditory cues. Along this review, we offer new insights from our understanding of speech as the product of evolution and development of a rhythmic and multimodal organization of sensorimotor brain networks, supporting volitional motor control of the upper vocal tract and audiovisual voices-faces integration.
Article
Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques are widely used in research settings to investigate brain mechanisms and increasingly being used for treatment purposes. The aim of this study was to systematically identify and review the current literature on NIBS studies of limb praxis and apraxia in healthy subjects and stroke patients with a scoping review using PRISMA-ScR guidelines. MEDLINE-PubMed, EMBASE and PsycINFO were searched. Inclusion criteria were English peer-reviewed studies focusing on the investigation of limb praxis/apraxia using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), or transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Fourteen out of 139 records met the inclusion criteria, including thirteen studies with healthy subjects and one with stroke patients. The results of our systematic review suggest that in healthy subjects NIBS over left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) mainly interfered with gesture processing, by either affecting reaction times in judgment tasks or real gesturing. First promising results suggest that inhibitory continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) over right IPL may enhance gesturing in healthy subjects, explained by transcallosal facilitation of left IPL. In stroke patients, excitatory anodal tDCS over left IPL may improve limb apraxia. However, larger well powered and sham-controlled clinical trials are needed to expand on these proof-of concept results, before NIBS could be a treatment option to improve limb apraxia in stroke patients.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Imaging studies point to a posture (finger vs. hand) and domain-specific neural basis of gestures. Furthermore, modulation of gestures by theta burst stimulation (TBS) may depend on interhemispheric disinhibition. Objective/hypothesis: In this randomized sham-controlled study, we hypothesized that dual site continuous TBS over left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG-L) and right inferior parietal gyrus (IPL-R) predominantly affects pantomime of finger postures. Furthermore, we predicted that dual cTBS improves imitation of hand gestures if the effect correlates with measures of callosal connectivity. Methods: Forty-six healthy subjects participated in this study and were targeted with one train of TBS in different experimental sessions: baseline, sham, single site IFG-L, dual IFG-L/IPL-R, single site IPL-R. Gestures were evaluated by blinded raters using the Test for Upper Limb Apraxia (TULIA) and Postural Imitation Test (PIT). Callosal connectivity was analyzed by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Results: Dual cTBS significantly improved TULIAtotal (F [3, 28] = 4.118, p = .009), but did not affect TULIApantomime. The beneficial effect was driven by the cTBS over IPL-R, which improved TULIAimitation (p = .038). Furthermore, TULIAimitation significantly correlated with the microstructure (fractional anisotropy) of the splenium (r = 0.420, p = .026), corrected for age and whole brain volume. Conclusions: The study suggests that inhibition of IPL-R largely accounted for improved gesturing, possibly through transcallosal facilitation of IPL-L. Therefore, the findings may be relevant for the treatment of apraxic stroke patients. Gesture pantomime and postural gestures escaped the modulation by dual cTBS, suggesting a more widespread and/or variable neural representation.
Article
Full-text available
In a social world, observing the actions of others is fundamental to understanding what they are doing, as well as their intentions and feelings. Studies of the neural basis and decoding of action observation are important for understanding action-related processes, and have implications for cognitive, social neuroscience, and human-machine interaction. In the current study, we first investigated temporal-spatial dynamics during action observation using a combined 64-channel electroencephalography (EEG) and 48-channel functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) system. We measured brain activation while 16 healthy participants observed three action tasks: 1) grasping a cup with the intention of drinking; 2) grasping a cup with the intention of moving it; and 3) touching a cup with an unclear intention. The EEG and fNIRS source analysis results revealed the dynamic involvement of both the mirror neuron system (MNS) and the theory of mind (ToM)/mentalizing network during action observation. The source analysis results suggested that the extent to which these two systems were engaged was determined by the clarity of the intention of the observed action. Based on the difference in neural activity observed among different action-observation tasks in the first experiment, we conducted a second experiment to classify the neural processes underlying action observation using a feature classification method. We constructed complex brain networks based on the EEG and fNIRS data. Fusing features from both EEG and fNIRS complex brain networks resulted in a classification accuracy of 72.7% for the three action observation tasks. This study provides a theoretical and empirical basis for elucidating the neural mechanisms of action observation and intention understanding, and a feasible method for decoding the underlying neural processes.
Article
Full-text available
Past attempts to identify the neural substrates of hand and finger imitation skills in the left hemisphere of the brain have yielded inconsistent results. Here, we analyse those associations in a large sample of 257 left hemisphere stroke patients. By introducing novel Bayesian methods, we characterise lesion symptom associations at three levels: the voxel-level, the single-region level (using anatomically defined regions), and the region-pair level. The results are inconsistent across those three levels and we argue that each level of analysis makes assumptions which constrain the results it can produce. Regardless of the inconsistencies across levels, and contrary to past studies which implicated differential neural substrates for hand and finger imitation, we find no consistent voxels or regions, where damage affects one imitation skill and not the other, at any of the three analysis levels. Our novel Bayesian approach indicates that any apparent differences appear to be driven by an increased sensitivity of hand imitation skills to lesions that also impair finger imitation. In our analyses, the results of the highest level of analysis (region-pairs) emphasise a role of the primary somatosensory and motor cortices, and the occipital lobe in imitation. We argue that this emphasis supports an account of both imitation tasks based on direct sensor-motor connections, which throws doubt on past accounts which imply the need for an intermediate (e.g. body-part-coding) system of representation.
Article
Full-text available
Humans sometimes perform actions which, at least superficially, appear suboptimal to the goal they are trying to achieve. Despite being able to identify these irrational actions from an early age, humans display a curious tendency to copy them. The current study recorded participants' movements during an established imitation task and manipulated the rationality of the observed action in two ways. Participants observed videos of a model point to a series of targets with either a low, high or 'superhigh' trajectory either in the presence or absence of obstacles between her targets. The participants' task was to watch which targets the model pointed to and then point to the same targets on the table in front of them. There were no obstacles between the participants' targets. Firstly, we found that the peak height of participants' movements between their targets was sensitive to the height of the model's movements, even in the 'superhigh' condition where the model's action was rated as irrational. Secondly, participants showed obstacle priming-the peak height of participants' movements was higher after having observed the model move over obstacles to reach her targets, compared to when there were no obstacles between her targets. This suggests that participants code the environment of co-actors into their own motor programs, even when this compromises the efficiency of their own movements. We discuss the implications of our findings in terms of theories of imitation and obstacle priming.
Article
Full-text available
Imitation is an important form of social behavior, and research has aimed to discover and explain the neural and kinematic aspects of imitation. However, much of this research has featured single participants imitating in response to pre-recorded video stimuli. This is in spite of findings that show reduced neural activation to video vs. real life movement stimuli, particularly in the motor cortex. We investigated the degree to which video stimuli may affect the imitation process using a novel motion tracking paradigm with high spatial and temporal resolution. We recorded 14 positions on the hands, arms, and heads of two individuals in an imitation experiment. One individual freely moved within given parameters (moving balls across a series of pegs) and a second participant imitated. This task was performed with either simple (one ball) or complex (three balls) movement difficulty, and either face-to-face or via a live video projection. After an exploratory analysis, three dependent variables were chosen for examination: 3D grip position, joint angles in the arm, and grip aperture. A cross-correlation and multivariate analysis revealed that object-directed imitation task accuracy (as represented by grip position) was reduced in video compared to face-to-face feedback, and in complex compared to simple difficulty. This was most prevalent in the left-right and forward-back motions, relevant to the imitator sitting face-to-face with the actor or with a live projected video of the same actor. The results suggest that for tasks which require object-directed imitation, video stimuli may not be an ecologically valid way to present task materials. However, no similar effects were found in the joint angle and grip aperture variables, suggesting that there are limits to the influence of video stimuli on imitation. The implications of these results are discussed with regards to previous findings, and with suggestions for future experimentation.
Article
Full-text available
Social interaction is an essential part of the human experience, and much work has been done to study it. However, several common approaches to examining social interactions in psychological research may inadvertently either unnaturally constrain the observed behaviour by causing it to deviate from naturalistic performance, or introduce unwanted sources of variance. In particular, these sources are the differences between naturalistic and experimental behaviour that occur from changes in visual fidelity (quality of the observed stimuli), gaze (whether it is controlled for in the stimuli), and social potential (potential for the stimuli to provide actual interaction). We expand on these possible sources of extraneous variance and why they may be important. We review the ways in which experimenters have developed novel designs to remove these sources of extraneous variance. New experimental designs using a ‘two-person’ approach are argued to be one of the most effective ways to develop more ecologically valid measures of social interaction, and we suggest that future work on social interaction should use these designs wherever possible.
Article
Full-text available
View largeDownload slide The severity of apraxia varies between patients with similar lesions. Using functional MRI in 36 chronic left-hemisphere stroke patients, Martin et al . reveal that tool-related and imitative skills depend not only on activation of spared left-hemisphere regions associated with cognitive motor functions, but also on activity within other mainly contralesional areas. View largeDownload slide The severity of apraxia varies between patients with similar lesions. Using functional MRI in 36 chronic left-hemisphere stroke patients, Martin et al . reveal that tool-related and imitative skills depend not only on activation of spared left-hemisphere regions associated with cognitive motor functions, but also on activity within other mainly contralesional areas.
Article
Background: The magnetic pulse generated during transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) also stimulates cutaneous nerves and muscle fibres, with the most commonly reported side effect being muscle twitches and sometimes painful sensations. These sensations affect behaviour during experimental tasks, presenting a potential confound for 'online' TMS studies. New method: Our objective was to systematically map the degree of disturbance (ratings of annoyance, pain, and muscle twitches) caused by TMS at 43 locations across the scalp. Ten participants provided ratings whilst completing a choice reaction time task, and ten different participants provided ratings whilst completing a 'flanker' reaction time task. Results: TMS over frontal and inferior regions resulted in the highest ratings of annoyance, pain, and muscle twitches caused by TMS. We predicted the difference in reaction times (RT) under TMS by scalp location and subjective ratings. Frontal and inferior scalp locations showed the greatest cost to RTs under TMS (i.e., slowing), with midline sites showing no or minimal slowing. Increases in subjective ratings of disturbance predicted longer RTs under TMS. Critically, ratings were a better predictor of the cost of TMS than scalp location or scalp-to-cortex distance. The more difficult 'flanker' task showed a greater effect of subjective disturbance. Comparison with existing methods: We provide the data as an online resource (www.tms-smart.info) so that researchers can select control sites that account for the level of general interference in task performance caused by online single-pulse TMS. Conclusions: The peripheral sensations and discomfort caused by TMS pulses significantly and systematically influence RTs during single-pulse, online TMS experiments.
Article
The kinematics of human actions are influenced by the social context in which they are performed. Motion-capture technology has allowed researchers to build up a detailed and complex picture of how action kinematics vary across different social contexts. Here we review three task domains—point-to-point imitation tasks, motor interference tasks and reach-to-grasp tasks—to critically evaluate how these tasks can inform our understanding of social interactions. First, we consider how actions within these task domains are performed in a non-social context, before highlighting how a plethora of social cues can perturb the baseline kinematics. We show that there is considerable overlap in the findings from these different tasks domains but also highlight the inconsistencies in the literature and the possible reasons for this. Specifically, we draw attention to the pitfalls of dealing with rich, kinematic data. As a way to avoid these pitfalls, we call for greater standardisation and clarity in the reporting of kinematic measures and suggest the field would benefit from a move towards more naturalistic tasks.
Article
Imitation of tool-use gestures (transitive; e.g., hammering) and communicative emblems (intransitive; e.g., waving goodbye) is frequently impaired after left-hemispheric lesions. We aimed 1) to identify lesions related to deficient transitive or intransitive gestures, 2) to delineate regions associated with distinct error types (e.g., hand configuration, kinematics), and 3) to compare imitation to previous data on pantomimed and actual tool use. Of note, 156 patients (64.3 ± 14.6 years; 56 female) with first-ever left-hemispheric ischemic stroke were prospectively examined 4.8 ± 2.0 days after symptom onset. Lesions were delineated on magnetic resonance imaging scans for voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. First, while inferior-parietal lesions affected both gesture types, specific associations emerged between intransitive gesture deficits and anterior temporal damage and between transitive gesture deficits and premotor and occipito-parietal lesions. Second, impaired hand configurations were related to anterior intraparietal damage, hand/wrist-orientation errors to premotor lesions, and kinematic errors to inferior-parietal/occipito-temporal lesions. Third, premotor lesions impacted more on transitive imitation compared with actual tool use, pantomimed and actual tool use were more susceptible to lesioned insular cortex and subjacent white matter. In summary, transitive and intransitive gestures differentially rely on ventro-dorsal and ventral streams due to higher demands on temporo-spatial processing (transitive) or stronger reliance on semantic information (intransitive), respectively.