ArticlePDF Available

Inter-individual Differences in Heart Rate Variability Are Associated with Inter-individual Differences in Empathy and Alexithymia

Authors:
  • University of Applied Sciences for Sport and Management Potsdam

Abstract and Figures

In the present study, we investigated whether inter-individual differences in vagally mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV) would be associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. To this end, we determined resting state HF-HRV in 90 individuals that also completed questionnaires assessing inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. Our categorical and dimensional analyses revealed that inter-individual differences in HF-HRV were differently associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. We found that individuals with high HF-HRV reported more empathy and less alexithymia than individuals with low HF-HRV. Moreover, we even found that an increase in HF-HRV was associated with an increase in empathy and a decrease in alexithymia across all participants. Taken together, these findings indicate that individuals with high HF-HRV are more empathetic and less alexithymic than individuals with low HF-HRV. These differences in empathy and alexithymia may explain why individuals with high HF-HRV are more successful in sharing and understanding the mental and emotional states of others than individuals with low HF-HRV.
Content may be subject to copyright.
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 27 February 2018
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00229
Edited by:
Katharina Sophia Goerlich,
RWTH Aachen University, Germany
Reviewed by:
Volker Max Perlitz,
Simplana GmbH, Germany
Georgia Panayiotou,
University of Cyprus, Cyprus
*Correspondence:
Alexander Lischke
alexander.lischke@uni-greifswald.de
Matthias Weippert
matthias.weippert@uni-rostock.de
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Clinical and Health Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 26 October 2017
Accepted: 12 February 2018
Published: 27 February 2018
Citation:
Lischke A, Pahnke R,
Mau-Moeller A, Behrens M,
Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Hamm AO
and Weippert M (2018)
Inter-individual Differences in Heart
Rate Variability Are Associated with
Inter-individual Differences in Empathy
and Alexithymia.
Front. Psychol. 9:229.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00229
Inter-individual Differences in Heart
Rate Variability Are Associated with
Inter-individual Differences in
Empathy and Alexithymia
Alexander Lischke1*, Rike Pahnke2, Anett Mau-Moeller3, Martin Behrens2,
Hans J. Grabe4, Harald J. Freyberger4,5 , Alfons O. Hamm1and Matthias Weippert2*
1Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany, 2Institute of Sport Science, University of
Rostock, Rostock, Germany, 3Department of Orthopaedics, University Medicine Rostock, Rostock, Germany, 4Department
of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany, 5HELIOS Klinikum Stralsund, Stralsund,
Germany
In the present study, we investigated whether inter-individual differences in vagally
mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV) would be associated with inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia. To this end, we determined resting state
HF-HRV in 90 individuals that also completed questionnaires assessing inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia. Our categorical and dimensional analyses
revealed that inter-individual differences in HF-HRV were differently associated with
inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. We found that individuals with
high HF-HRV reported more empathy and less alexithymia than individuals with low
HF-HRV. Moreover, we even found that an increase in HF-HRV was associated with
an increase in empathy and a decrease in alexithymia across all participants. Taken
together, these findings indicate that individuals with high HF-HRV are more empathetic
and less alexithymic than individuals with low HF-HRV. These differences in empathy
and alexithymia may explain why individuals with high HF-HRV are more successful in
sharing and understanding the mental and emotional states of others than individuals
with low HF-HRV.
Keywords: social cognition, social interaction, empathy, alexithymia, vagus nerve, high-frequency heart rate
variability
INTRODUCTION
Social relationships have always been of utmost importance for humans. Although the number
and type of relationships may have changed over the course of evolution, the challenges and
opportunities associated with social relationships may have remained the same (Dunbar, 1998).
Evolutionary pressures may, thus, have selected a suite of skills that may have helped us to initiate
or maintain positive relationships and to avoid or terminate negative relationships (De Waal, 2008).
Of these skills, the ability to share and understand others’ emotional and mental states, which
entails a simulation of these states while making a self-other distinction, appears to be of particular
relevance (Preston and De Waal, 2002). Infants are already capable of sharing others’ emotional and
mental states, but a full understanding of these states on basis of a self-other distinction emerges
during late childhood (Frith and Frith, 2003), implying that the ability to share and understand
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 2
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
emotional and mental states comprise various processes that
become more and more complex throughout our development. It
is important to note that these processes involve a simulation of
others’ emotional and mental states on the neural and autonomic
level, an interpretation of the simulated states on basis of the
corresponding neural and autonomic changes, and a distinction
between the simulated and observed states (Preston and De Waal,
2002;De Waal, 2008). These processes have been linked to inter-
individual differences in empathy, a personality trait describing
an individual’s awareness of other’s emotional and mental states
(Deutsch and Madle, 1975), and alexithymia, a personality trait
describing an individual’s awareness of one’s own emotional and
mental states (Nemiah et al., 1976). Individuals with low levels of
empathy and/or high levels of alexithymia are severely impaired
in their ability to share and understand emotional and mental
states of others and the self (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1999;
Parker et al., 2001;Dolan and Fullam, 2004;Moriguchi et al.,
2007;Silani et al., 2008;Decety et al., 2013), which may explain
why these individuals frequently have difficulties to establish and
maintain positive relationships (e.g., Rilling et al., 2007;Chiu
et al., 2008;Mokros et al., 2008;Feldmanhall et al., 2013). It is,
thus, not surprising that the interest for biomarkers indicating
such impairments has steadily been growing over the last decade.
It should be noted, however, that the search for these biomarkers
is more complex than initially thought (Kapur et al., 2012;Davis
et al., 2015).
Vagally mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV), an index
of parasympathetically induced changes in consecutive heart
beats (Berntson et al., 1997), has been suggested to be a
promising biomarker for inter-individual differences in social
behavior and social cognition (Porges, 2007;Thayer and Lane,
2009). Inter-individual differences in vmHRV reflect inter-
individual differences regarding the engagement of prefrontal
and (para-)limbic brain regions during the regulation of
emotional and cognitive processes (Porges, 2007;Thayer and
Lane, 2009;Thayer et al., 2012), indicating that inter-individual
differences in vmHRV may work as biomarker for inter-
individual differences in the social domain. Individuals with
high vmHRV are more efficient in establishing and maintaining
positive relationships than individuals with low vmHRV (e.g.,
Kogan et al., 2014;Beffara et al., 2016;Lischke et al.,
2018), implying that the relationships of individuals with high
vmHRV are more characterized by mutual understanding than
the relationships of individuals with low vmHRV (Kok and
Fredrickson, 2010). Inter-individual differences regarding the
ability to share and understand emotional and mental states
may explain why individuals with high vmHRV are more likely
to achieve a mutual understanding in social relationships than
individuals with low vmHRV. Individuals with high vmHRV
may be more efficient in regulating emotional and cognitive
processes during the simulation of the respective states (e.g.,
Geisler et al., 2010, 2013;Williams et al., 2015) and may be more
efficient in regulating cognitive processes that are necessary for
the interpretation of the respective states (e.g., Hansen et al., 2003;
Segerstrom and Nes, 2007;Luft et al., 2009) than individuals with
low vmHRV, which may result in a more efficient sharing and
understanding of the respective states in individuals with high
as compared to low vmHRV (e.g., Cote et al., 2011;Quintana
et al., 2012a;Lischke et al., 2017). However, individuals with
high and low vmHRV do not only differ from one another
with respect to processes that are relevant for the sharing and
understanding of emotional and mental states, but also with
respect to personality traits that are relevant for the sharing
and understanding of these states. Empathy related personality
traits, like, for example, compassion for other’s emotional and
mental states, are more pronounced in individuals with high
than low vmHRV (e.g., Oveis et al., 2009;Kogan et al., 2014;
Stellar et al., 2015), whereas alexithymia related personality
traits, like, for example, difficulties in identifying or describing
one’s own emotional and mental states, appear to be more
pronounced in individuals with low than high vmHRV (e.g.,
Fukunishi et al., 1999;Panayiotou and Constantinou, 2017). It
should be noted, however, that the association between inter-
individualdifferences in vmHRV and inter-individual differences
in alexithymia related personality traits is less clear than the
association between inter-individual differences in vmHRV and
inter-individual differences in empathy related personality traits
(e.g., Virtanen et al., 2003;Zohar et al., 2013), indicating a
need for further studies investigating this association. Similarly,
there is a need to further study the association between inter-
individual differences in vmHRV and inter-individual differences
in empathy related personality traits because this association has
only been investigated in a few studies (e.g., Oveis et al., 2009;
Kogan et al., 2014;Stellar et al., 2015).
In the present study, we addressed these issues in a relatively
large and homogenous sample of healthy participants by
measuring inter-individual differences in vmHRV as well as
inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia related
personality traits. On basis of previous studies (e.g., Fukunishi
et al., 1999;Oveis et al., 2009;Kogan et al., 2014;Stellar
et al., 2015;Panayiotou and Constantinou, 2017), we expected
participants with high vmHRV to report more empathy and
less alexithymia than participants with low vmHRV. We also
expected inter-individual differences in vmHRV to be differently
associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and
alexithymia across all participants.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
According to an a priori power analysis with GPower3 (Faul
et al., 2007), we had to recruit 90 participants to be able to detect
medium effect sizes in our categorical (f= 0.30, 1-β= 80, α= 0.05)
and dimensional (f2= 0.15, 1-β= 80, α= 0.05) analyses regarding
the association between inter-individual differences in vmHRV
and inter-individual differences in empathy or alexithymia. In
order to be considered for recruitment, participants had to
pass a screening concerning the presence of current mental
disorders and the use of current psychotropic medication. Female
participants were not considered for recruitment to control sex-
differences in empathy (Christov-Moore et al., 2014), alexithymia
(Levant et al., 2009) and vmHRV (Koenig and Thayer, 2016).
We, thus, recruited 90 male participants at the Institute of Sport
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 3
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
TABLE 1 | Participant characteristics.
M SEM
Age (years) 26.20 0.43
Body mass index (kg/m2) 24.05 0.27
Physical activity (h/w) 7.05 3.70
Respiratory activity
(Log-pHF-HRV, Hz)
0.72 0.01
Heart rate variability
(Log HF-HRV, ms2)
2.69 0.05
Empathy (EQ-15)a16.42 0.01
Alexithymia (TAS-20)b44.13 1.15
Log-pHF-HRV = log-transformed peak of high frequency heart rate variability,
Log-HF-HRV = log-transformed high frequency heart rate variability,
EQ-15 = Empathy Quotient 15 (Allison et al., 2011), TAS-20 = Toronto
Alexithymia Scale 20 (Bagby et al., 1994a,b).
aData was available for 79 participants.
bData was available for 89 participants.
Science of the University of Rostock (see Table 1). All participants
provided written-informed consent to the study protocol that was
approved by the ethics committee of the University of Rostock
and carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Procedure
After arriving at the laboratory, participants were asked to
use the bathroom to control for the effects of bladder filling
and gastric distension on vmHRV (Quintana and Heathers,
2014). Participants were then seated in a comfortable chair
and prepared for a 5 min heart rate (HR) recording. As
recently recommended (Quintana et al., 2016), participants were
instructed to breathe spontaneously and to keep their eyes open
during the recording. After the recording, participants completed
questionnaires assessing inter-individual differences in empathy
(Allison et al., 2011) and alexithymia (Bagby et al., 1994a,b).
Heart Rate Variability
HR was recorded continuously with a chest belt system, the
RS800 HR monitor (Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland),
providing a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. HR monitors like the
RS800 have been shown to record changes in consecutive heart
beats as accurate as conventional electrocardiograms (Weippert
et al., 2010;Quintana et al., 2012b), indicating that the recorded
data were valid and reliable measures of instantaneous HR.
Device specific software (Polar ProTrainer 5; Polar Electro
Oy, Kempele, Finland) was used to transfer the recorded data
to a computer for further data processing with Kubios HRV
2.2 (Tarvainen et al., 2014). Following established guidelines
(Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, 1996),
the recorded data was visually inspected, detrended (smoothn
priors: λ= 500) and, whenever necessary, corrected using
adaptive filtering. Thereafter, the recorded data was subjected to
a spectral analysis to determine HF-HRV, a measure of vagally
mediated cardiac activity (Berntson et al., 1997), and the peak of
HF-HRV (pHF-HRV), a measure of respiratory activity (Berntson
et al., 1997). Besides these measures, no further measures were
determined to avoid interpretational issues arising from the use
of measures that do not clearly reflect vagally mediated cardiac
activity (Berntson et al., 1997).
Questionnaires
The Empathy Quotient (EQ-15; Allison et al., 2011) is a 15
item self-report questionnaire for the assessment of empathy.
The EQ-15 comprises a main scale for the assessment of
global empathy and several subscales for the assessment of
specific aspects of empathy (e.g., emotional reactivity or social
skills). However, the subscales are highly inter-correlated with
one another (Muncer and Ling, 2006;Allison et al., 2011),
implying that the EQ-15 measures empathy as an unidimensional
rather than multidimensional construct. Following previous
recommendations (Muncer and Ling, 2006;Allison et al., 2011),
we only considered the main scale in our analyses. The main
scale had good psychometric properties [α= 0.75], which were
comparable to those that have previously been reported (Muncer
and Ling, 2006;Allison et al., 2011)
The Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20; Bagby et al.,
1994a,b) is a 20 item self-report questionnaire for the assessment
of alexithymia. The TAS-20 consists of a main scale assessing
global differences in alexithymia and of several subscales
assessing specific differences in alexithymia (e.g., difficulties in
identifying or describing feelings). However, the high correlations
between the different subscales and the low reliabilities of some
subscales complicate the interpretation of the respective subscales
(Kooiman et al., 2002;Muller et al., 2003). Accordingly, it has
been suggested that the TAS-20 may be better suited to measure
alexithymia as an unidimensional rather than multidimensional
construct (Vorst and Bermond, 2001). We, therefore, considered
the main scale but not the subscales in our analyses. The
main scale had excellent psychometric properties [α= 0.86],
which were similar to those that have previously been reported
(Bagby et al., 1994a,b).
Statistical Analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS 22 (SPSS
Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). To investigate whether
inter-individual differences in vmHRV would be associated
with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia,
dimensional and categorical analyses were performed. In all
analyses, pre-cautions were taken to control for inter-individual
differences in age (years), body mass index (BMI, kg/m2),
physical activity (h/w) and respiratory activity (pHF-HRV, Hz)
that may contribute to inter-individual differences in vmHRV
(Quintana et al., 2016). Inter-individual differences in physical
and respiratory activity were of particular concern because
participants were recruited at a Sport Science facility, where the
prevalence of athletes that differ from non-athletes in vmHRV
due to inter-individual differences in physical and respiratory
activity is higher than in the general population (Aubert et al.,
2003). For the categorical analyses, analyses of covariance
(ANCOVAs) were computed to determine whether participants
with high and low HF-HRV would show inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia. Assignment of
participants to the high and low HF-HRV group was based on a
median split. For the dimensional analyses, multiple hierarchical
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 4
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
regression analyses were computed to determine whether
inter-individual differences in HF-HRV would be differentially
associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and
alexithymia across all participants. Prior to all analyses, HF-HRV
and pHF-HRV were log transformed (log 10) to account for
deviations from normality distribution. The significance level for
the analyses was set at p0.05 (two-tailed). In addition to the
significance level (p), effect sizes (η2
pand R2) were determined to
facilitate the interpretation of significant findings (Cohen, 1988).
RESULTS
Inter-individual Differences in Heart Rate
Variability and Inter-individual
Differences in Empathy
A one-way ANCOVA showed that participants with high
HF-HRV reported more empathy than participants with low
HF-HRV [F(1,73) = 6.51, p= 0.013, η2
p= 0.08; see Figure 1],
independent of inter-individual differences in age, BMI, physical
or respiratory activity. Across all participants, inter-individual
differences in HF-HRV were positively associated with inter-
individual differences in empathy as indicated by a multiple
regression analysis [t(73) = 2.17, p= 0.033; see Table 2].
The multiple regression analysis also indicated that there was
no association of inter-individual differences in age, BMI,
physical or respiratory activity with inter-individual differences
in empathy [all p>0.339; see Table 2]. Most of the
variance regarding inter-individual differences in empathy was,
thus, explained by inter-individual differences in HF-HRV
[F(1,73) = 4.71, p= 0.033; see Table 2], not by inter-
individual differences in age, BMI, physical or respiratory
activity [F(4,74) = 0.19, p= 0.944; see Table 2]. More
precisely, inter-individual differences in HF-HRV explained 6%
of variance regarding inter-individual differences in empathy in
FIGURE 1 | Barplots demonstrating differences in empathy between
participants with high (white bars) and low (gray bars) high-frequency heart
rate variability (HF-HRV). Bars represent M ±SEM. p0.05.
TABLE 2 | Association between inter-individual differences in heart rate variability
and inter-individual differences in empathy across all participants.
Empathy (EQ-15a)
Predictors R21R2β
Step 1 0.01 0.01
Age (years) 0.08
Body mass index (kg/m2) 0.67
Physical activity (h/w) 0.00
Respiratory activity (Log-pHF-HRV, Hz) 0.01
Step 2 0.07 0.06
Age (years) 0.09
Body mass index (kg/m2) 0.11
Physical activity (h/w) 0.06
Respiratory activity (Log-pHF-HRV, Hz) 0.05
Heart rate variability (Log-HF-HRV, ms2) 0.26
Log-pHF-HRV = log-transformed peak of high frequency heart rate variability,
Log-HF-HRV = log transformed high frequency heart rate variability,
EQ-15 = Empathy Quotient 15 (Allison et al., 2011).
aData was available for 79 participants.
p0.05.
addition to the 2% of variance that tended to be explained by
inter-individual differences in age, BMI, physical or respiratory
activity.
Inter-individual Differences in Heart Rate
Variability and Inter-individual
Differences in Alexithymia
A one-way ANCOVA revealed that participants with high
HF-HRV reported less alexithymia than participants with
low HF-HRV [F(1,83) = 3.99, p= 0.049, η2
p= 0.05, see
Figure 2], irrespective of inter-individual differences in age, BMI,
physical or respiratory activity. Across all participants, inter-
individual differences in HF-HRV were negatively associated
FIGURE 2 | Barplots demonstrating differences in alexithymia between
participants with high (white bars) and low (gray bars) high-frequency heart
rate variability (HF-HRV). Bars represent M ±SEM. p0.05.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 5
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
with inter-individual differences in alexithymia as indicated by
a multiple regression analysis [t(83) = 2.02, p= 0.047; see
Table 3]. The multiple regression analysis further indicated that
inter-individual differences in age were also negatively associated
with inter-individual differences in alexithymia [t(83) = 2.81,
p= 0.006; see Table 3] and that inter-individual differences in
BMI, physical or respiratory activity were not associated with
inter-individual differences in alexithymia [all p>0.416; see
Table 3]. However, inter-individual differences in alexithymia
were more relevant for explaining inter-individual differences
in HF-HRV [F(1,83) = 4.07, p= 0.047; see Table 3] than
inter-individual differences in age, BMI, physical or respiratory
activity [F(4,84) = 1.98, p= 0.106; see Table 3]. Inter-
individual differences in HF-HRV explained 4% of variance
regarding inter-individual differences in alexithymia in addition
to the 9% of variance that tended to be explained by inter-
individual differences in age, BMI, physical or respiratory
activity.
DISCUSSION
In the present study, we investigated the association of
inter-individual differences in vmHRV with inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia related personality traits.
Inter-individual differences in vmHRV were determined on
basis of inter-individual differences in resting state HF-HRV
and inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia
were determined on basis of inter-individual differences in
questionnaire scores. In line with our expectations, we found
a positive association between inter-individual differences in
vmHRV and inter-individual differences in empathy. Our
categorical analyses revealed that participants with high vmHRV
were more empathetic than participants with low vmHRV
TABLE 3 | Association between inter-individual differences in heart rate variability
and inter-individual differences in alexithymia across all participants.
Alexithymia (TAS-20a)
Predictors R21R2β
Step 1 0.09 0.09
Age (years) 0.29
Body mass index (kg/m2) 0.08
Physical activity (h/w) 0.04
Respiratory activity (Log-pHF-HRV, Hz) 0.10
Step 2 0.13 0.04
Age (years) 0.29∗∗
Body mass index (kg/m2) 0.04
Physical activity (h/w) 0.09
Respiratory activity (Log-pHF-HRV, Hz) 0.60
Heart rate variability (Log-HF-HRV, ms2)0.22
Log-pHF-HRV = log-transformed peak of high frequency heart rate variability,
Log-HF-HRV = log transformed high frequency heart rate variability,
TAS-20 = Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (Bagby et al., 1994a,b).
aData was available for 89 participants.
p0.05, ∗∗p<0.01.
and our dimensional analyses indicated that an increase in
vmHRV was associated with an increase in empathy across
all participants. Also as expected, we found a negative
association between inter-individual differences in vmHRV
and inter-individual differences in alexithymia. Our categorical
analyses showed that participants with high vmHRV were
less alexithymic than participants with low vmHRV and our
dimensional analyses indicated that an increase in vmHRV
was associated with a decrease in alexithymia across all
participants.
Previous studies revealed a similar association of
inter-individual differences in vmHRV with inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia related personality
traits (e.g., Fukunishi et al., 1999;Oveis et al., 2009;Kogan
et al., 2014;Stellar et al., 2015;Panayiotou and Constantinou,
2017). With respect to empathy it is noteworthy that individuals
with high vmHRV show more agreeableness with others
and more compassion for others’ emotional or mental states
than individuals with low vmHRV (e.g., Oveis et al., 2009;
Kogan et al., 2014;Stellar et al., 2015). Agreeableness is a
personality trait that is closely related to compassion and
compassion is a personality trait that is closely related to
empathetic concern (Goetz et al., 2010), a distinct dimension
of empathy that has been regarded as an important precursor
of prosocial behavior (Batson and Shaw, 1991). Inter-individual
differences in vmHRV may, thus, be differentially associated
with distinct empathy dimensions, implying the possibility of
positive associations with empathy dimensions that facilitate
prosocial behavior, such as empathetic concern (e.g., Toi and
Batson, 1982;Batson et al., 1983), and negative associations
with empathy dimensions that impair prosocial behavior, such
as empathetic distress (e.g., Toi and Batson, 1982;Batson
et al., 1983). In the present study, we were unable to test this
possibility because the psychometric properties of our empathy
questionnaire argued against the use of the questionnaire’s
subscales in the respective analyses. Future studies should,
thus, employ empathy questionnaires with psychometrically
sound subscales to further elucidate the association between
inter-individual differences in vmHRV and inter-individual
differences in empathy. With respect to alexithymia it is
noteworthy that individuals with high vmHRV report fewer
difficulties in identifying or describing their own emotional
and mental states than individuals with low vmHRV (e.g.,
Fukunishi et al., 1999;Panayiotou and Constantinou, 2017).
However, the association between inter-individual differences
in vmHRV and inter-individual differences in alexithymia
seem to be more pronounced among younger (e.g., Fukunishi
et al., 1999;Panayiotou and Constantinou, 2017) than older
(e.g., Virtanen et al., 2003;Zohar et al., 2013) individuals.
Future studies should, therefore, investigate this association
among individuals showing a wider age range than those
individuals that have been included in the present study. These
studies should also employ alexithymia questionnaires with
psychometrically sound subscales to explore whether inter-
individual differences in vmHRV are differentially associated
with distinct dimensions of alexithymia as suggested by
previous studies (e.g., Fukunishi et al., 1999;Panayiotou and
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 6
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
Constantinou, 2017). In the present study, we were unable to
perform the respective analyses because of the problematic
subscale structure of our alexithymia questionnaire. Taken
together, the findings of the present and previous studies suggest
that inter-individual differences in vmHRV are associated
with inter-individual differences regarding the ability to share
and understand emotional and mental states of others and
the self.
Assuming an association of inter-individual differences
in vmHRV with inter-individual differences in empathy
and alexithymia may help to explain why individuals
with high vmHRV are more successful in establishing and
maintaining positive relationships than individuals with
low vmHRV (e.g., Kok and Fredrickson, 2010;Beffara
et al., 2016;Lischke et al., 2018). Individuals with high
vmHRV may be more efficient in simulating and interpreting
emotional and mental states under a self-other awareness
than individuals with low vmHRV, which may increase the
likelihood of mutual understanding that is necessary for the
establishment and maintenance of positive relationships.
Inter-individual differences regarding the regulation of cognitive
and emotional processes that are relevant for the simulation
and interpretation of emotional and mental states, like, for
example, the control of emotions (e.g., Geisler et al., 2010,
2013;Williams et al., 2015) or the allocation of attention
(e.g., Hansen et al., 2003;Segerstrom and Nes, 2007;Luft
et al., 2009), may contribute to these differences. In this
respect it is noteworthy that individuals with high vmHRV
outperform individuals with low vmHRV on tasks that require
the inference of others’ states on basis of facial and/or vocal
cues (e.g., Cote et al., 2011;Quintana et al., 2012a;Lischke
et al., 2017), indicating the plausibility of the aforementioned
assumptions.
With respect to the neurobiological mechanisms mediating
the association of inter-individual differences in vmHRV with
inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia, it
is important to note that a similar set of prefrontal and
(para-)limbic brain regions is engaged during the simulation
and interpretation of emotional and mental states as during
the regulation of cardiac activity (Bernhardt and Singer, 2012;
Thayer et al., 2012;Wingbermuhle et al., 2012). Of these
brain regions, the anterior cingulate cortex, the insula and
amygdala are of particular relevance because functional and
structural changes in these brain regions are associated with
changes in empathy and alexithymia (e.g., Singer et al., 2004;
Moriguchi et al., 2007;Reker et al., 2010;Banissy et al., 2012;
Klimecki et al., 2012;Bernhardt et al., 2014;Grabe et al.,
2014;Goerlich-Dobre et al., 2015) as well as with changes in
vmHRV (e.g., Gianaros et al., 2004;Lane et al., 2009;Sakaki
et al., 2016;Winkelmann et al., 2017). Following previous
suggestions that changes in vmHRV serve as a proxy for changes
in prefrontal activity and prefrontal-(para-)limbic connectivity
(Porges, 2007;Thayer and Lane, 2009;Thayer et al., 2012),
we assume that inter-individual differences in vmHRV reflect
inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia that are
due to inter-individual differences in prefrontal activity and
prefrontal-(para-)limbic connectivity. More precisely, we assume
that individuals with high vmHRV are more empathetic and less
alexithymic than individual with low vmHRV because individuals
with high vmHRV are more efficient in recruiting prefrontal
and (para-)limbic brain regions implicated in the simulation and
interpretation of emotional and mental states than individuals
with low vmHRV. In this respect it is noteworthy that individuals
with autism, a disorder that is characterized by alterations in
empathy and alexithymia (Hill et al., 2004;Dziobek et al.,
2008), show alterations in a prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain
regions (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1999;Dziobek et al., 2006;
Silani et al., 2008;Wicker et al., 2008;Ecker et al., 2012) as
well as alterations in vmHRV (e.g., Mathewson et al., 2011;
Kuiper et al., 2017). Inter-individual differences in vmHRV
may, thus, indicate inter-individual differences regarding the
recruitment of prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain regions during
the simulation and interpretation of emotional and mental states
in healthy as well as in mentally disordered individuals, implying
that inter-individual differences in vmHRV may indeed serve
as biomarker for inter-individual differences in empathy and
alexithymia.
The findings of the present study suggest that inter-individual
differences in vmHRV are associated with inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia, presumably because
of inter-individual differences in prefrontal activity and
prefrontal-(para-)limbic connectivity during the simulation
and interpretation of emotional and mental states. The present
findings are not only consistent with findings of previous
studies revealing an association between inter-individual
differences in vmHRV and inter-individual differences in
the regulation of emotional and cognitive processes that are
necessary for the simulation and interpretation of emotional
and mental states (e.g., Hansen et al., 2003;Segerstrom and
Nes, 2007;Luft et al., 2009;Geisler et al., 2010;Geisler et al.,
2013;Williams et al., 2015), but also with findings of previous
studies suggesting an association between inter-individual
differences in vmHRV and inter-individual differences in
prefrontal activity and prefrontal-(para-)limbic connectivity
during the regulation of emotional and cognitive processes
that are necessary for the simulation and interpretation of
emotional and mental states (e.g., Gianaros et al., 2004;
Lane et al., 2009;Sakaki et al., 2016). Taken together, these
findings corroborate our assumption that inter-individual
differences in vmHRV are associated with inter-individual
differences regarding the ability to share and understand
emotional and mental states of others and the self. However,
whether inter-individual differences in vmHRV really have
the potential to work as a biomarker for inter-individual
differences in empathy and alexithymia remains to be
determined in future studies that are explicitly designed
for these types of investigations (Kapur et al., 2012;Davis
et al., 2015). These studies should employ correlational
and experimental study designs in a cross-sectional
or longitudinal way to investigate the aforementioned
associations on the behavioral and neural level in healthy
and mentally disordered individuals with performance and
questionnaire based measures of empathy, alexithymia and social
behavior. Otherwise it will be difficult to determine whether
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 7
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
inter-individual differences in vmHRV qualify as a biomarker for
inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
AL, AM-M, and RP designed the study. AM-M and MW collected
the data. AL and RP analyzed the data. AL wrote the manuscript.
AH, AM-M, HF, HG, MB, MW, and RP contributed to writing,
reviewing and editing of the manuscript. All authors approved
the final version of the manuscript.
FUNDING
AL was supported by a grant from the German Research
Foundation (DFG; LI 2517/2-1).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Thomas Dreyer, Robert Jacksteit,
Valentin Propp, Michel Rickler, and Nils Seitz for research
assistance.
REFERENCES
Allison, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, M. H., and Muncer, S. J.
(2011). Psychometric analysis of the empathy quotient (EQ). Pers. Individ. Dif.
51, 829–835. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.005
Aubert, A. E., Seps, B., and Beckers, F. (2003). Heart rate variability in athletes.
Sports Med. 33, 889–919. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333120- 00003
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., and Taylor, G. J. (1994a). The twenty-item Toronto
Alexithymia Scale–I, Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure.
J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1
Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., and Parker, J. D. (1994b). The twenty-item Toronto
Alexithymia scale–II, convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity.
J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-X
Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., and Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual
differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuroimage 62,
2034–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.081
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J.,
Simmons, A., and Williams, S. C. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and
autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1891–1898. doi: 10.1046/j.
1460-9568.1999.00621.x
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., and Isen, A. M. (1983).
Influence of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic
motivation to help. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 706–718. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.
45.3.706
Batson, C. D., and Shaw, L. L. (1991). Evidence for altruism: toward a pluralism of
prosocial motives. Psychol. Inq. 2, 107–122. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0202_1
Beffara, B., Bret, A. G., Vermeulen, N., and Mermillod, M. (2016). Resting
high frequency heart rate variability selectively predicts cooperative
behavior. Physiol. Behav. 164(Pt A), 417–428. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.
06.011
Bernhardt, B. C., Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., and Singer, T. (2014). Structural
covariance networks of the dorsal anterior insula predict females’ individual
differences in empathic responding. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2189–2198. doi: 10.1093/
cercor/bht072
Bernhardt, B. C., and Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annu. Rev.
Neurosci. 35, 1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro- 062111-150536
Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T. Jr., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G.,
Malik, M., and van der Molen, M. W. (1997). Heart rate variability: origins,
methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 34, 623–648. doi: 10.1111/
j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x
Chiu, P. H., Kayali, M. A., Kishida, K. T., Tomlin, D., Klinger, L. G., Klinger,
M. R., and Montague, P. R. (2008). Self responses along cingulate cortex
reveal quantitative neural phenotype for high-functioning autism. Neuron 57,
463–473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.020
Christov-Moore, L., Simpson, E. A., Coude, G., Grigaityte, K., Iacoboni, M., and
Ferrari, P. F. (2014). Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci.
Biobehav. Rev. 46(Pt 4), 604–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn.
Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Cote, S., Kraus, M. W., Cheng, B. H., Oveis, C., van der Lowe, I., Lian, H., and
Keltner, D. (2011). Social power facilitates the effect of prosocial orientation
on empathic accuracy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 217–232. doi: 10.1037/
a0023171
Davis, J., Maes, M., Andreazza, A., McGrath, J. J., Tye, S. J., and Berk, M.
(2015). Towards a classification of biomarkers of neuropsychiatric disease: from
encompass to compass. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 152–153. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.139
De Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution
of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.
103006.093625
Decety, J., Skelly, L. R., and Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Brain response to empathy-eliciting
scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA
Psychiatry 70, 638–645. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27
Deutsch, F., and Madle, R. A. (1975). Empathy: historic and current
conceptualizations, measurement, and a cognitive theoretical perspective. Hum.
Dev. 18, 267–287. doi: 10.1159/000271488
Dolan, M., and Fullam, R. (2004). Theory of mind and mentalizing ability in
antisocial personality disorders with and without psychopathy. Psychol. Med.
34, 1093–1102. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002028
Dunbar, R. I. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evol. Anthropol. 6, 178–190.
doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5< 178::AID-EVAN5> 3.0.CO;2-8
Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Rogers, K., Wolf, O. T., and Convit, A. (2006). The ’amygdala
theory of autism’ revisited: linking structure to behavior. Neuropsychologia 44,
1891–1899. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.005
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., and
Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults
with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 38, 464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007- 0486-x
Ecker, C., Suckling, J., Deoni, S. C., Lombardo, M. V., Bullmore, E. T.,
Baron-Cohen, S., and Consortium, M. A. (2012). Brain anatomy and its
relationship to behavior in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a multicenter
magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 195–209. doi:
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1251
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible
statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
Feldmanhall, O., Dalgleish, T., and Mobbs, D. (2013). Alexithymia decreases
altruism in real social decisions. Cortex 49, 899–904. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.
10.015
Frith, U., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of
mentalizing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 459–473. doi: 10.1098/
rstb.2002.1218
Fukunishi, I., Sei, H., Morita, Y., and Rahe, R. H. (1999). Sympathetic activity
in alexithymics with mother’s low care. J. Psychosom. Res. 46, 579–589.
doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(98)00083- X
Geisler, F. C. M., Kubiak, T., Siewert, K., and Weber, H. (2013). Cardiac vagal
tone is associated with social engagement and self-regulation. Biol. Psychol. 93,
279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.013
Geisler, F. C. M., Vennewald, N., Kubiak, T., and Weber, H. (2010). The
impact of heart rate variability on subjective well-being is mediated by
emotion regulation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 49, 723–728. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.
06.015
Gianaros, P. J., Van Der Veen, F. M., and Jennings, J. R. (2004). Regional
cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart
period variability during working-memory tasks: Implications for the cortical
and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology 41,
521–530. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00179.x
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 8
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
Goerlich-Dobre, K. S., Lamm, C., Pripfl, J., Habel, U., and Votinov, M. (2015). The
left amygdala: a shared substrate of Alexithymia and empathy. Neuroimage 122,
20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.014
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., and Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: an
evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 351–374.
doi: 10.1037/a0018807
Grabe, H. J., Wittfeld, K., Hegenscheid, K., Hosten, N., Lotze, M., Janowitz, D., and
Freyberger, H. J. (2014). Alexithymia and brain gray matter volumes in a general
population sample. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5932–5945. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22595
Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., and Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working
memory and attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 48, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/S0167-
8760(03)00073-4
Hill, E., Berthoz, S., and Frith, U. (2004). Brief report: cognitive processing of own
emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 229–235. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022613.41399.14
Kapur, S., Phillips, A. G., and Insel, T. R. (2012). Why has it taken so long for
biological psychiatry to develop clinical tests and what to do about it? Mol.
Psychiatry 17, 1174–1179. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.105
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2012). Functional neural
plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training.
Cereb. Cortex 23, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142
Koenig, J., and Thayer, J. F. (2016). Sex differences in healthy human heart rate
variability: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 64, 288–310. doi: 10.1016/
j.neubiorev.2016.03.007
Kogan, A., Oveis, C., Carr, E. W., Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., Shallcross, A., and
Keltner, D. (2014). Vagal activity is quadratically related to prosocial traits,
prosocial emotions, and observer perceptions of prosociality. J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 107, 1051–1063. doi: 10.1037/a0037509
Kok, B. E., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic
flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts
positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol. Psychol. 85, 432–436.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005
Kooiman, C. G., Spinhoven, P., and Trijsburg, R. W. (2002). The assessment of
Alexithymia: a critical review of the literature and a psychometric study of the
Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. J. Psychosom. Res. 53, 1083–1090. doi: 10.1016/
S0022-3999(02)00348- 3
Kuiper, M. W. M., Verhoeven, E. W. M., and Geurts, H. M. (2017). Heart rate
variability predicts inhibitory control in adults with autism spectrum disorders.
Biol. Psychol. 128, 141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.006
Lane, R. D., McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Ahern, G. L., and Thayer, J. F.
(2009). Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage
44, 213–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.056
Levant, R. F., Hall, R. J., Williams, C. M., and Hasan, N. T. (2009). Gender
differences in alexithymia. Psychol. Men Masc. 10, 190–203. doi: 10.1037/
a0015652
Lischke, A., Jacksteit, R., Mau-Moeller, A., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., and
Weippert, M. (2018). Heart rate variability is associated with psychosocial
stress in distinct social domains. J. Psychosom. Res. 106, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/
j.jpsychores.2018.01.005
Lischke, A., Lemke, D., Neubert, J., Hamm, A. O., and Lotze, M. (2017).
Inter-individual differences in heart rate variability are associated with
inter-individual differences in mind-reading. Sci. Rep. 7:11557. doi: 10.1038/
s41598-017- 11290-1
Luft, C. D., Takase, E., and Darby, D. (2009). Heart rate variability and cognitive
function: effects of physical effort. Biol. Psychol. 82, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.
biopsycho.2009.07.007
Mathewson, K. J., Drmic, I. E., Jetha, M. K., Bryson, S. E., Goldberg, J. O., Hall,
G. B., and Schmidt, L. A. (2011). Behavioral and cardiac responses to emotional
stroop in adults with autism spectrum disorders: influence of medication.
Autism Res. 4, 98–108. doi: 10.1002/aur.176
Mokros, A., Menner, B., Eisenbarth, H., Alpers, G. W., Lange, K. W., and
Osterheider, M. (2008). Diminished cooperativeness of psychopaths in a
prisoner’s dilemma game yields higher rewards. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117,
406–413. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.406
Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K.,
and Komaki, G. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI
study of alexithymia. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2223–2234. doi: 10.1093/cercor/
bhl130
Muller, J., Buhner, M., and Ellgring, H. (2003). Is there a reliable factorial structure
in the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale? A comparison of factor models in
clinical and normal adult samples. J. Psychosom. Res. 55, 561–568. doi: 10.1016/
S0022-3999(03)00033- 3
Muncer, S. J., and Ling, J. (2006). Psychometric analysis of the empathy quotient
(EQ) scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 40, 1111–1119. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.020
Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., and Sifneos, P. E. (1976). “Alexithymia: a view of the
psychosomatic process,” in Modern Trends in Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol. 3,
ed. O. W. Hill (London: Butterworths), 430–439.
Oveis, C., Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., Shiota, M. N., Haidt, J., and Keltner, D.
(2009). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic positive
emotionality. Emotion 9, 265–270. doi: 10.1037/a0015383
Panayiotou, G., and Constantinou, E. (2017). Emotion dysregulation in
alexithymia: startle reactivity to fearful affective imagery and its relation to heart
rate variability. Psychophysiology 54, 1323–1334. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12887
Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G. J., and Bagby, R. M. (2001). The relationship
between emotional intelligence and alexithymia. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30, 107–115.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00014- 3
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74, 116–143.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
Preston, S. D., and De Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate
bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1–20.
Quintana, D. S., Alvares, G. A., and Heathers, J. A. (2016). Guidelines for reporting
articles on psychiatry and heart rate variability (GRAPH): recommendations
to advance research communication. Transl. Psychiatry 6:e803. doi: 10.1038/tp.
2016.73
Quintana, D. S., Guastella, A. J., Outhred, T., Hickie, I. B., and Kemp, A. H. (2012a).
Heart rate variability is associated with emotion recognition: direct evidence for
a relationship between the autonomic nervous system and social cognition. Int.
J. Psychophysiol. 86, 168–172. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.012
Quintana, D. S., and Heathers, J. A. (2014). Considerations in the assessment
of heart rate variability in biobehavioral research. Front. Psychol. 5:805.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00805
Quintana, D. S., Heathers, J. A., and Kemp, A. H. (2012b). On the validity of using
the Polar RS800 heart rate monitor for heart rate variability research. Eur. J.
Appl. Physiol. 112, 4179–4180. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2453- 2
Reker, M., Ohrmann, P., Rauch, A. V., Kugel, H., Bauer, J., Dannlowski, U., and
Suslow, T. (2010). Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to
masked emotion faces. Cortex 46, 658–667. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.008
Rilling, J. K., Glenn, A. L., Jairam, M. R., Pagnoni, G., Goldsmith, D. R., Elfenbein,
H. A., and Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Neural correlates of social cooperation and
non-cooperation as a function of psychopathy. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 1260–1271.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.021
Sakaki, M., Yoo, H. J., Nga, L., Lee, T., Thayer, J. F., and Mather, M. (2016).
Heart rate variability is associated with amygdala functional connectivity with
MPFC across younger and older adults. Neuroimage 139, 44–52. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2016.05.076
Segerstrom, S. C., and Nes, L. S. (2007). Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory
strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychol. Sci. 18, 275–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.
2007.01888.x
Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., and Frith, U. (2008). Levels
of emotional awareness and autism: an fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 97–112.
doi: 10.1080/17470910701577020
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D.
(2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of
pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535
Stellar, J. E., Cohen, A., Oveis, C., and Keltner, D. (2015). Affective and
physiological responses to the suffering of others: compassion and vagal activity.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 572–585. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000010
Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J. P., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-Aho, P. O., and
Karjalainen, P. A. (2014). Kubios HRV–heart rate variability analysis software.
Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 113, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.
07.024
Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (1996). Heart rate variability
standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur.
Heart J. 17, 354–381. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868
Thayer, J. F., Ahs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J., III, and Wager, T. D. (2012).
A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
fpsyg-09-00229 February 26, 2018 Time: 16:47 # 9
Lischke et al. HRV Is Associated with Empathy and Alexithymia
for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 36, 747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009
Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain
connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004
Toi, M., and Batson, C. D. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of
altruistic motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 43, 281–292. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.
43.2.281
Virtanen, R., Jula, A., Salminen, J. K., Voipio-Pulkki, L. M., Helenius, H.,
Kuusela, T., and Airaksinen, J. (2003). Anxiety and hostility are associated
with reduced baroreflex sensitivity and increased beat-to-beat blood pressure
variability. Psychosom. Med. 65, 751–756. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000088760.
65046.CF
Vorst, H. C. M., and Bermond, B. (2001). Validity and reliability of the bermond–
vorst alexithymia questionnaire. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30, 413–434. doi: 10.1016/
S0191-8869(00)00033- 7
Weippert, M., Kumar, M., Kreuzfeld, S., Arndt, D., Rieger, A., and Stoll, R. (2010).
Comparison of three mobile devices for measuring R-R intervals and heart rate
variability: polar S810i, Suunto t6 and an ambulatory ECG system. Eur. J. Appl.
Physiol. 109, 779–786. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1415-9
Wicker, B., Fonlupt, P., Hubert, B., Tardif, C., Gepner, B., and Deruelle, C. (2008).
Abnormal cerebral effective connectivity during explicit emotional processing
in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 135–143.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn007
Williams, D. P., Cash, C., Rankin, C., Bernardi, A., Koenig, J., and Thayer,
J. F. (2015). Resting heart rate variability predicts self-reported difficulties in
emotion regulation: a focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Front.
Psychol. 6:261. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00261
Wingbermuhle, E., Theunissen, H., Verhoeven, W. M., Kessels, R. P., and Egger, J. I.
(2012). The neurocognition of alexithymia: evidence from neuropsychological
and neuroimaging studies. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 24, 67–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-
5215.2011.00613.x
Winkelmann, T., Thayer, J. F., Pohlack, S., Nees, F., Grimm, O., and Flor, H.
(2017). Structural brain correlates of heart rate variability in a healthy young
adult population. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 1061–1068. doi: 10.1007/s00429-
016-1185- 1
Zohar, A. H., Cloninger, C. R., and McCraty, R. (2013). Personality and
heart rate variability: exploring pathways from personality to cardiac
coherence and health. Open J. Soc. Sci. 1, 32–39. doi: 10.4236/jss.2013.
16007
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2018 Lischke, Pahnke, Mau-Moeller, Behrens, Grabe, Freyberger,
Hamm and Weippert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or
reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the
copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited,
in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction
is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9February 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 229
... High-Frequency Heart rate variability (HF-HRV) is a well-established marker of vagal activity (Allen et al., 2014;Thayer et al., 2012). Higher levels of HF-HRV have also been associated with better emotion regulation, empathy, compassion, and physical health (Lischke et al., 2018;Thayer et al., 2012). Most Psychological Flexibility research relies on self-report data which has well known construct validity limitations (Goldberg et al., 2019;Ong et al., 2019;Van Dam et al., 2010). ...
... The association between increases in HF-HRV and empathy, compassion, and prosocial behaviors has been established in several laboratory studies (Lischke et al., 2018;Sassenrath et al., 2021;Stellar et al., 2015). Surgery/injury videos reliably evoke vagal activation and increases in HF-HRV (Shenhav & Mendes, 2014). ...
Article
Psychological Inflexibility (PI) is a key component of the Unified Flexibility and Mindfulness Model. Higher levels of psychological Inflexibility have been associated with poorer wellbeing. High Frequency Heart Rate Variability (HF-HRV) is a reliable and valid index of vagal activation and wellbeing. The association between Psychological Inflexibility and HF-HRV has been examined in the context of laboratory-induced stressors that evoke different patterns of cardiovascular activation. The present sample included 81 US undergraduate students comprised of 44 females and 37 males (4 did not identify their gender) with a mean age of 19.9 (SD = 2.79) Participants were assigned to view a 3-min video that elicited an orienting response and complete a 3-min math task that elicited a defensive response. The order of stimulus exposure was counterbalanced. Psychological Inflexibility was measured using subscales from the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire using the Unified Flexibility and Mindfulness Model as a guide for scoring. Higher Psychological Inflexibility participants, relative to lower Psychological Inflexibility participants, had a significantly smaller increase in HF-HRV during the orienting stressor indicating less vagal activation. Higher Psychological Inflexibility participants also had a larger decrease in HF-HRV in response to the defensive stressor indicating greater vagal withdrawal. These results shed light on the interactions between Psychological Inflexibility, HF-HRV reactivity to laboratory stressors.
... People with lower HRV have a higher negative motivational value when faced with unfavorable images (Katahira et al., 2014). Inter-individual differences in vagally mediated HRV (HF-HRV) and empathy and alexithymia were also studied (Lischke et al., 2018). The category and dimensional analyses revealed that high HRV people had more empathy and lower alexithymia than low HRV people (Lischke et al., 2018). ...
... Inter-individual differences in vagally mediated HRV (HF-HRV) and empathy and alexithymia were also studied (Lischke et al., 2018). The category and dimensional analyses revealed that high HRV people had more empathy and lower alexithymia than low HRV people (Lischke et al., 2018). The Japanese study found higher interindividual than intra-individual variations in healthy males at rest (Kobayashi, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Autonomic modulation is critical during various physiological activities, including orthostatic stimuli and primarily evaluated by heart rate variability (HRV). Orthostatic stress affects people differently suggesting the possibility of identification of predisposed groups to autonomic dysfunction-related disorders in a healthy state. One way to understand this kind of variability is by using Ayurvedic approach that classifies healthy individuals into Prakriti types based on clinical phenotypes. To this end, we explored the differential response to orthostatic stress in different Prakriti types using HRV. HRV was measured in 379 subjects(Vata = 97, Pitta = 68, Kapha = 68, and Mixed Prakriti = 146) from two geographical regions(Vadu and Delhi NCR) for 5 min supine (baseline), 3 min head-up-tilt (HUT) at 60°, and 5 min resupine. We observed that Kapha group had lower baseline HRV than other two groups, although not statistically significant. The relative change (%Δ1&2 ) in various HRV parameters in response to HUT was although minimal in Kapha group. Kapha also had significantly lower change in HR, LF (nu), HF (nu), and LF/HF than Pitta in response to HUT. The relative change (%Δ1 ) in HR and parasympathetic parameters (RMSSD, HF, SD1) was significantly greater in the Vata than in the Kapha. Thus, the low baseline and lower response to HUT in Kapha and the maximum drop in parasympathetic activity of Vata may indicate a predisposition to early autonomic dysfunction and associated conditions. It emphasizes the critical role of Prakriti-based phenotyping in stratifying the differential responses of cardiac autonomic modulation in various postures among healthy individuals across different populations.
... Artificial intelligence can be leveraged to model the interplay between heart rate variability and various elements of psychotherapy. Higher heart rate variability, for instance, is associated with better emotion regulation (Luecken & Appelhans, 2006;Yoo et al., 2018) enhanced metacognitive awareness (Lischke et al., 2017;Meessen et al., 2018), and increased empathy (Lischke et al., 2018). By using artificial intelligence models trained on heart rate and heart rate variability data and corresponding therapy outcomes, we can start to predict how changes in physiological states might influence or signal changes in the therapeutic relationship. ...
Article
Full-text available
The evidence-based treatment (EBT) movement has primarily focused on core intervention content or treatment fidelity and has largely ignored practitioner skills to manage interpersonal process issues that emerge during treatment, especially with difficult-to-treat adolescents (delinquent, substance-using, medical non-adherence) and those of color. A chief complaint of “real world” practitioners about manualized treatments is the lack of correspondence between following a manual and managing microsocial interpersonal processes (e.g. negative affect) that arise in treating “real world clients.” Although family-based EBTs share core similarities (e.g. focus on family interactions, emphasis on practitioner engagement, family involvement), most of these treatments do not have an evidence base regarding common implementation and treatment process problems that practitioners experience in delivering particular models, especially in mid-treatment when demands on families to change their behavior is greatest in treatment – a lack that characterizes the field as a whole. Failure to effectively address common interpersonal processes with difficult-to-treat families likely undermines treatment fidelity and sustained use of EBTs, treatment outcome, and contributes to treatment dropout and treatment nonadherence. Recent advancements in wearables, sensing technologies, multivariate time-series analyses, and machine learning allow scientists to make significant advancements in the study of psychotherapy processes by looking “under the skin” of the provider–client interpersonal interactions that define therapeutic alliance, empathy, and empathic accuracy, along with the predictive validity of these therapy processes (therapeutic alliance, therapist empathy) to treatment outcome. Moreover, assessment of these processes can be extended to develop procedures for training providers to manage difficult interpersonal processes while maintaining a physiological profile that is consistent with astute skills in psychotherapeutic processes. This paper argues for opening the “black box” of therapy to advance the science of evidence-based psychotherapy by examining the clinical interior of evidence-based treatments to develop the next generation of audit- and feedback- (i.e., systemic review of professional performance) supervision systems.
... Taken together, the results of this study suggest that SSD patients' prefrontal cortical network alterations may be related to their IA. In addition, previous studies have reported on the close associations between IA, HRV, empathy and emotion recognition [40][41][42][43]. Therefore, the correlation between IA and HRV speci cally found in SSD patients supports that the pathophysiology of SSD is associated with IA and emotional processing. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Objective: Patients with somatic symptom disorder (SSD) tend to have problems perceiving their bodily signals. We hypothesized that SSD patients would exhibit changes in interoceptive accuracy (IA), particularly when emotional processing is involved. Methods: Twenty-three patients with SSD and 20 healthy controls were recruited. IA was assessed using the heartbeat perception task. The task was performed in the absence of stimuli as well as in the presence of emotional interference, i.e., photographs of faces with an emotional expression. IA were examined for correlation with measures related to their somatic symptoms, including resting-state heart rate variability (HRV). Results: There was no significant difference in the absolute values of IA between patients with SSD and healthy controls, regardless of the condition. However, the degree of difference in IA without emotional interference and with neutral facial interference was greater in patients with SSD than in healthy controls (p=0.039). The IA of patients with SSD also showed a significant correlation with low-frequency HRV (p=0.004) and high-frequency HRV (p=0.007). Conclusion: SSD patients showed more significant changes in IA when neutral facial interference was given. These results suggest that bodily awareness is more affected by emotionally ambiguous stimuli in SSD patients than in healthy controls.
... Slow breathing induces synchronous alpha waves across broad areas of the cerebral cortex, consistent with a state of calm attention and awareness, as indicated on the Subjective Indicators of Psychophysiological States. 6. Slow breathing, such as Coherent Breathing, activates the social engagement system and enhances feelings of safety, trust, friendliness, empathy, connection, and bonding (4,(22)(23)(24), as documented by the Subjective Indicators of Psychophysiological States. 7. Slow gentle breath practices may increase levels of oxytocin, enhancing feelings of closeness, trust, safety, bonding, and love (4,16,53) that are consistent with items on the Subjective Indicators of Psychophysiological States. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background During the COVID-19 pandemic, healthcare workers endured prolonged stress affecting their psychological well-being. Objectives: (1) Evaluate the effects of the Breath-Body-Mind Introductory Course (BBMIC) on COVID-related stress among employees of the Regional Integrated Support for Education, Northern Ireland, (2) Reduce the risk of adverse effects from COVID-related stress, and (3) Evaluate the effects of BBMIC on indicators of psychophysiological states and the consistency with hypothesized mechanisms of action. Methods In this single group study, a convenience sample of 39 female healthcare workers completed informed consent and baseline measures: Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), Stress Overload Scale-Short (SOS-S), and Exercise-Induced Feelings Inventory (EFI). Following the online BBMIC 4 h/day for 3 days and the 6 week solo (20 min/day) and group practice (45 min weekly), repeat testing plus the Indicators of Psychophysiological State (IPSS) and Program Evaluation were obtained. Results Baseline (T1) mean PSS score was significantly elevated compared to a normative sample: PSS = 18.2 vs. 13.7 ( p < 0.001) and improved significantly 11 weeks post-BBMIC (T4). SOS-S mean score declined from 10.7(T1) to 9.7 at 6 week post-test (T3). The SOS-S proportion of High Risk scores found in 22/29 participants (T1), dropped to 7/29 (T3). EFI mean subscale scores improved significantly from T1 to T2 and T3 for Revitalization ( p < 0.001); Exhaustion ( p < 0.002); and Tranquility ( p < 0.001); but not Engagement ( p < 0.289). Conclusion Among RISE NI healthcare workers affected by COVID-related stress, participation in the BBMIC significantly reduced scores for Perceived Stress, Stress Overload, and Exhaustion. EFI Revitalization and Tranquility scores significantly improved. More than 60% of participants reported moderate to very strong improvements in 22 indicators of psychophysiological state, e.g., tension, mood, sleep, mental focus, anger, connectedness, awareness, hopefulness, and empathy. These results are consistent with the hypothesized mechanisms of action whereby voluntarily regulated breathing exercises change interoceptive messaging to brain regulatory networks that shift psychophysiological states of distress and defense to states of calmness and connection. These positive findings warrant validation in larger, controlled studies to extend the understanding of how breath-centered Mind-body Medicine practices could mitigate adverse effects of stress.
... High CU individuals also showed less HR change from baseline than low CU ones (de Wied et al., 2012). Also, when measuring high-frequency HRV (HF-HRV) in healthy male students, greater HF-HRV was associated with increased self-reported empathy (Lischke et al., 2018). Hence, it could be argued that HRV could be used as a proxy for empathic functioning in studying psychopathy. ...
Article
Ho, M.H., Kemp, B.T., Eisenbarth, H. & Rijnders, R.J.P. Designing a neuroclinical assessment of empathy deficits in psychopathy based on the Zipper Model of Empathy. NEUROSCI BIOBEHAV REV YY(Y) XXX-XXX, 2023. The heterogeneity of the literature on empathy highlights its multidimensional and dynamic nature and affects unclear descriptions of empathy in the context of psychopathology. The Zipper Model of Empathy integrates current theories of empathy and proposes that empathy maturity is dependent on whether contextual and personal factors push affective and cognitive processes together or apart. This concept paper therefore proposes a comprehensive battery of physiological and behavioral measures to empirically assess empathy processing according to this model with an application for psychopathic personality. We propose using the following measures to assess each component of this model: (1) facial electromyography; (2) the Emotion Recognition Task; (3) the Empathy Accuracy task and physiological measures (e.g., heart rate); (4) a selection of Theory of Mind tasks and an adapted Dot Perspective Task, and; (5) an adjusted Charity Task. Ultimately, we hope this paper serves as a starting point for discussion and debate on defining and assessing empathy processing, to encourage research to falsify and update this model to improve our understanding of empathy.
... Obtaining psychophysiological measurements would also provide novel data about the relationships between empathy, alexithymia, and SPS. Alexithymia has been associated with differences in heart rate variability, skin conductance response, and electromyography during empathy tasks (Sonnby-Borgström, 2009;Bogdanov et al., 2013;Cecchetto et al., 2018;Lischke et al., 2018;Härtwig et al., 2020). Conducting both neuroimaging and psychophysiological investigations would serve as a catalyst for future research into individual differences in processes like emotional contagion, which support empathy and prosocial behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Empathy—the ability to identify and share another person’s emotional state—is an important socio-emotional process arising, in part, from emotional contagion. In the current study, we assessed unique variance in emotional contagion and other empathy-related constructs accounted for by two personality traits, alexithymia and sensory processing sensitivity (SPS), when controlling for childhood emotional abuse and current depressed mood. Methods A sample of 305 adults ( M age = 20.1 years) watched brief film clips chosen to induce various emotional states. After each film, the participants rated how strongly they experienced each of nine different emotions. They then completed self-report measures of alexithymia, SPS, empathy-related constructs, childhood emotional abuse, and current mood. Results Those scoring high (vs. low) on SPS reported stronger primary emotions and a larger range of emotions when watching the films and were more apt to believe that their emotions matched those of the individuals featured in the films. They also scored higher on both self-oriented processes (such as the tendency to feel personal distress in tense situations) and other-oriented processes (such as perspective taking and empathic concern) related to empathy. Individuals scoring high (vs. low) on alexithymia reported feeling a larger range of emotions while watching the films but scored lower on other-oriented processes related to empathy. After controlling for SPS and alexithymia, current depressed mood predicted experiencing less varied reactions to mixed valence films that elicited strong feelings of embarrassment/humiliation, and less amusement when watching positive films. Childhood emotional abuse did not emerge as a predictor of emotional contagion or empathy. Discussion We propose that the strong and nuanced feelings elicited in those scoring high on SPS by observing others support their personal view that they are highly empathic. In contrast, by failing to closely examine their own mixed reactions to others, individuals with alexithymia may find it difficult to connect with, understand, and respond to others’ feelings.
... Within this perspective, recent meta-analytic evidence has found compassion to be positively associated with the fluctuation of instantaneous periodicity of the heart over time, namely heart rate variability (HRV), which is considered a proxy of how top-down appraisal shapes the autonomic response in the body (Di Bello et al., 2020). In the context of interpersonal motivations, higher tonic HRV has been positively associated with mind reading and empathy (e.g., Lischke et al., 2017;2018), theory of mind (Zammuto et al., 2021), social engagement (e.g., Shahrestani et al., 2015), social connectedness (e.g., Kok & Fredrickson, 2010), prosociality (e.g., Kogan et al., 2014), and cooperative behavior (e.g., Beffara et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Compassion motivation is associated with increased heart rate variability (HRV), reflecting a calm and self-soothing physiological state. Recent work, however, suggests that this association is dynamic for the specific components of compassion. Objectives: The present study adopted anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) targeting the right insula to see whether this would modulate the sensitivity to suffering and the commitment to engage in helpful actions (i.e., the components of compassion motivation). Method: Ninety-seven healthy individuals underwent 15-min anodal or sham tDCS over the frontotemporal lobe, while watching a video inducing empathic sensitivity and performing a Redistribution Game. Tonic and phasic HRV, dispositional traits, and momentary affects were assessed. Results: Compared to sham condition, anodal stimulation favored significant i) HRV reductions during the video and HRV increases during the Redistribution Game; ii) decreases in self-reported levels of negative affect and increases in positive affect during task when the latter was preceded by the video, without influencing altruistic behavior. Conclusions: Anodal tDCS over the right insula may modulate the engagement phase of compassion by intensifying the psychophysiological sensitivity to signals of distress and protecting from being subjectively overwhelmed by it.
... In the last decades, vagally-mediated HRV has been robustly employed to study brain-heart interactions in the regulation of negative emotions in clinical conditions such as depression and anxiety (Heiss et al., 2021). Accumulating evidence is also showing that vagal tone as indexed by HRV, may be correlated with indices of wellbeing (Balzarotti et al., 2017;Di Bello et al., 2020;Geisler et al., 2010;Lischke et al., 2018Lischke et al., , 2019. Furthermore, some longitudinal research reported on the role of resting HRV in increasing the experience of positive emotional experience (Duarte & Pinto-Gouveia, 2017;Kok & Fredrickson, 2010;Oveis et al., 2009). ...
Article
Sleep quality is considered a basic dimension of emotional health. The psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the associations between sleep quality and positive emotions are still largely unknown, yet autonomic regulation may play a role. This study employed a two-day ecological momentary assessment methodology in a sample of young adults to investigate whether subjective sleep quality reported in the morning was associated with daily positive emotional experience and whether this association was mediated by heart rate variability (HRV), a measure of cardiac vagal tone. Sleep quality was assessed using an electronic sleep diary upon awakening, while resting HRV and positive emotions were inspected at random times throughout the day using photoplethysmography and an electronic diary, respectively. Relevant confounding variables such as smoking, alcohol intake, and physical exercise between each measurement were also assessed. The sample included 121 participants (64.8% females, Mage = 25.97 ± 5.32 years). After controlling for relevant confounders including health behaviors and psychiatric comorbidities, mediation analysis revealed that greater sleep quality positively predicted daily HRV (β = .289, p < .001) which, in turn, had a direct influence on positive emotions (β = .244, p = .006). Also, sleep quality directly predicted positive emotional experience (β = .272, p = .001). Lastly, the model showed an indirect effect between sleep quality and positive emotions via HRV (β = .071, 95% BCI [.011, .146]). Results support the view of HRV as a process variable linking sleep to positive emotions. Experimental data is needed to consolidate the present findings.
Preprint
Full-text available
Intransitive gestures are expressive and symbolic, whereas pantomimes are object-related actions. These gestures convey different meanings depending on whether they are directed toward (TB) or away from the body (AB). TB gestures express mental states (intransitive) or hygiene/nutritional activities (pantomime), while AB gestures modify the behavior of the observer (intransitive) or demonstrate tool use with an object (pantomime). A substantial body of literature suggests that females exhibit stronger social cue processing compared to males. Considering the social significance of gestures, this study aims to explore the physiological gender differences in the observation of AB and TB gestures. Pupil dilation and High Frequency Heart Rate Variability (HF-HRV) were measured in 54 participants (27 female) while observing TB and AB gestures. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and the Vicarious Distress Questionnaire (VDQ) were used to assess social-emotional processes. Results showed greater pupil dilation in females for TB gestures, but no significant gender differences for HF-HRV. Males showed a significant correlation between increased pupil dilation to both TB and AB gestures and empathy levels (IRI). The support scale of the VDQ correlated significantly with TB gestures in males. These findings provide insight into the neurobiological basis of gender differences in perceiving social gestures.
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, we investigated whether inter-individual differences in vagally-mediated cardiac activity (high frequency heart rate variability, HF-HRV) would be associated with inter-individual differences in mind-reading, a specific aspect of social cognition. To this end, we recorded resting state HF-HRV in 49 individuals before they completed the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, a test that required the identification of mental states on basis of subtle facial cues. As expected, inter-individual differences in HF-HRV were associated with inter-individual differences in mental state identification: Individuals with high HF-HRV were more accurate in the identification of positive but not negative states than individuals with low HF-HRV. Individuals with high HF-HRV may, thus, be more sensitive to positive states of others, which may increase the likelihood to detect cues that encourage approach and affiliative behavior in social contexts. Inter-individual differences in mental state identification may, thus, explain why individuals with high HF-HRV have been shown to be more successful in initiating and maintaining social relationships than individuals with low HF-HRV.
Article
Full-text available
The number of publications investigating heart rate variability (HRV) in psychiatry and the behavioral sciences has increased markedly in the last decade. In addition to the significant debates surrounding ideal methods to collect and interpret measures of HRV, standardized reporting of methodology in this field is lacking. Commonly cited recommendations were designed well before recent calls to improve research communication and reproducibility across disciplines. In an effort to standardize reporting, we propose the Guidelines for Reporting Articles on Psychiatry and Heart rate variability (GRAPH), a checklist with four domains:participant selection, interbeat interval collection, data preparation and HRV calculation. This paper provides an overview of these four domains and why their standardized reporting is necessary to suitably evaluate HRV research in psychiatry and related disciplines. Adherence to these communication guidelines will help expedite the translation of HRV research into a potential psychiatric biomarker by improving interpretation, reproducibility and future meta-analyses.
Article
Objective Psychosocial stress is associated with substantial morbidity and mortality. Accordingly, there is a growing interest in biomarkers that indicate whether individuals show adaptive (i.e., stress-buffering and health-promoting) or maladaptive (i.e., stress-escalating and health-impairing) stress reactions in social contexts. As heart rate variability (HRV) has been suggested to be a biomarker of adaptive behavior during social encounters, it may be possible that inter-individual differences in HRV are associated with inter-individual differences regarding stress in distinct social domains. Methods To test this hypothesis, resting state HRV and psychosocial stress was assessed in 83 healthy community-dwelling individuals (age: 18–35 years). HRV was derived from heart rate recordings during spontaneous and instructed breathing to assess the robustness of possible associations between inter-individual differences in HRV and inter-individual differences in psychosocial stress. Psychosocial stress was determined with a self-report questionnaire assessing stress in distinct social domains. Results A series of categorical and dimensional analyses revealed an association between inter-individual differences in HRV and inter-individual differences in psychosocial stress: Individuals with high HRV reported less stress in social life, but not in family life, work life or everyday life, than individuals with low HRV. Conclusions On basis of these findings, it may be assumed that individuals with high HRV experience less psychosocial stress than individuals with low HRV. Although such an assumption needs to be corroborated by further findings, it seems to be consistent with previous findings showing that individuals with high HRV suffer less from stress and stress-related disorders than individuals with low HRV.
Article
Several studies suggest that inhibition difficulties among people with ASD might be related to atypical cardiac vagal control. We examined how low versus high baseline heart rate variability (HRV) influences prepotent response inhibition in 31 males with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; mean age: 32.2; mean IQ: 107.8) compared to 39 typically developing (TD) males (mean age: 30.5; mean IQ: 102.0) by administering a stop signal task. Moreover, we examined whether adding an affective manipulation would alter findings and whether this manipulation affected HRV. Findings indicated that baseline HRV influenced inhibition in ASD males. Specifically, an ASD subgroup with low baseline HRV performed significantly worse compared to an ASD subgroup with high baseline HRV. No influence of baseline HRV was found in TD males. The affective manipulation did negatively influence performance and also altered HRV. Although replication is required, these first findings indicate that baseline cardiac vagal control seems to affect inhibitory control in males with ASD.
Article
Alexithymia is associated with deficiencies in recognizing and expressing emotions and impaired emotion regulation, though few studies have verified the latter assertion using objective measures. This study examined startle reflex modulation by fearful imagery and its associations with heart rate variability in alexithymia. Fifty-four adults (27 alexithymic) imagined previously normed fear scripts. Startle responses were assessed during baseline, first exposure, and reexposure. During first exposure, participants, in separate trials, engaged in either shallow or deep emotion processing, giving emphasis on descriptive or affective aspects of imagery, respectively. Resting heart rate variability was assessed during 2 min of rest prior to the experiment, with high alexithymic participants demonstrating significantly higher LF/HF (low frequency/high frequency) ratio than controls. Deep processing was associated with nonsignificantly larger and faster startle responses at first exposure for alexithymic participants. Lower LF/HF ratio, reflecting higher parasympathetic cardiac activity, predicted greater startle amplitude habituation for alexithymia but lower habituation for controls. Results suggest that, when exposed to prolonged threat, alexithymics may adjust poorly, showing a smaller initial defensive response but slower habituation. This pattern seems related to their low emotion regulation ability as indexed by heart rate variability.
Article
This study explores whether the vagal connection between the heart and the brain is involved in prosocial behaviors. The Polyvagal Theory postulates that vagal activity underlies prosocial tendencies. Even if several results suggest that vagal activity is associated with prosocial behaviors, none of them used behavioral measures of prosociality to establish this relationship. We recorded the resting state vagal activity (reflected by High Frequency Heart Rate Variability, HF-HRV) of 48 (42 suitale for analysis) healthy human adults and measured their level of cooperation during a hawk-dove game. We also manipulated the consequence of mutual defection in the hawk-dove game (severe vs. moderate). Results show that HF-HRV is positively and linearly related to cooperation level, but only when the consequence of mutual defection is severe (compared to moderate). This supports that i) prosocial behaviors are likely to be underpinned by vagal functioning ii) physiological disposition to cooperate interacts with environmental context. We discuss these results within the theoretical framework of the Polyvagal Theory.
Article
The ability to regulate emotion is crucial to promote well-being. Evidence suggests that the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and adjacent anterior cingulate (ACC) modulate amygdala activity during emotion regulation. Yet less is known about whether the amygdala–mPFC circuit is linked with regulation of the autonomic nervous system and whether the relationship differs across the adult lifespan. The current study tested the hypothesis that heart rate variability (HRV) reflects the strength of mPFC–amygdala interaction across younger and older adults. We recorded participants' heart rates at baseline and examined whether baseline HRV was associated with amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity during rest. We found that higher HRV was associated with stronger functional connectivity between the amygdala and the mPFC during rest across younger and older adults. In addition to this age-invariant pattern, there was an age-related change, such that greater HRV was linked with stronger functional connectivity between amygdala and ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC) in younger than in older adults. These results are in line with past evidence that vlPFC is involved in emotion regulation especially in younger adults. Taken together, our results support the neurovisceral integration model and suggest that higher heart rate variability is associated with neural mechanisms that support successful emotional regulation across the adult lifespan.