Conference PaperPDF Available

Artificial Swarm Intelligence amplifies accuracy when predicting financial markets

Authors:
AUTHOR DRAFT (pre publication)
Artificial Swarm Intelligence Amplifies Accuracy
when Predicting Financial Markets
Louis Rosenberg
Unanimous AI
San Francisco, CA, USA
Louis@Unanimous.ai
Niccolo Pescetelli
University of Oxford
Clarendon, UK
niccolo.pescetelli@chch.ox.ac.uk
Gregg Willcox
Unanimous AI
San Francisco, CA, USA
gregg@Unanimous.ai
Abstract Across the natural world, many species have
evolved methods for amplifying their decision-making accuracy
by thinking together in real-time closed-loop systems. Known as
Swarm Intelligence (SI) in the field of biology, the process has
been deeply studied in schools of fish, flocks of bird, and swarms
of bees. The present research looks at human groups and tests
their ability to make financial predictions by forming online
systems modeled after natural swarms. Specifically, groups of
financial traders were tasked with predicting the weekly trends
of four common market indices (SPX, GLD, GDX, and Crude
Oil) over a period of 14 consecutive weeks. Results showed that
individual participants, who averaged 61% accuracy when
predicting weekly trends on their own, amplified their accuracy
to 77% when predicting together as real-time swarms. These
results reflect a 26% increase in financial prediction accuracy
and show high statistical significance (p=0.001). This suggests
that enabling groups of traders to form real-time systems online,
governed by swarm intelligence algorithms, has the potential to
significantly increase the accuracy of financial forecasts.
Keywords Swarm Intelligence, Artificial Swarm Intelligence,
Collective Intelligence, Human Swarming, Artificial Intelligence.
I. INTRODUCTION
Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) has been shown to
amplify the intelligence of human groups by connecting users
into real-time systems modeled after biological swarms [1, 2].
Prior studies have shown that “human swarms” can produce
more accurate predictions than traditional “Wisdom of Crowd”
methods such as votes, polls, and surveys [3]. For example, a
2015 study tested the ability of human swarms to forecast the
outcome of college football games. A swarm comprised of 75
amateur sports fans was tasked with predicting 10 college bowl
games. As individuals, the participants averaged 50% accuracy
when predicting outcomes against the spread. When thinking
together in real-time swarms, those same participants achieved
70% accuracy against the spread [2]. Similar increases have
been found in other studies, including a long-term test that
required participants to predict a set of 50 soccer matches in
the English Premier League and showed a 31% increase in
accuracy when participants were connected in swarms [4].
While prior studies have documented the ability of artificial
swarms to amplify the predictive ability of human groups when
forecasting sporting events, political races, and media awards
such as the Oscars and Grammys, no formal study has been
performed to assess whether swarm-based predictions of
financial markets can achieve similar improvements. To
address this need, a fourteen-week study was conducted that
tasked human swarms of financial traders with making weekly
predictions regarding the change in four financial indices
(SPX, GLD, GDX, and Crude Oil). The objective was to assess
whether a statistically significant improvement would be
measured when comparing individual predictions to swarm
predictions. In addition, swarm performance was compared
with the traditional “Wisdom of Crowd” method of using the
most popular prediction across the participant pool as the group
forecast. Thus, the present study compared the predictive
abilities of three cases Individuals, Crowds, and Swarms.
II. SWARMS AS INTELLIGENT SYSTEMS
The primary difference between “crowds” and “swarms” is
that in crowd-based methods, individual participants provide
their input in isolation (for statistical aggregation after the fact),
while in swarm-based methods, groups think together as
real-time systems governed by intelligence algorithms and
converge on solutions in synchrony. The swarming process is
generally modeled after biological systems such as schools of
fish and swarms of bees. The present research uses Swarm AI
technology from Unanimous A.I. Inc, which is modeled largely
on honeybee swarms. This model was chosen for the current
study because honeybee swarms are known to significantly
amplify the accuracy of critical decisions by enabling members
to form real-time systems i.e. “hive minds” that can solve
problems as a unified and amplified intelligence.
The decision-making processes that govern the behavior of
honeybee swarms have been studied since the 1950s and have
been shown to be remarkably similar to the decision-making
processes in neurological brains [5,6]. Both employ large
populations of simple excitable units (i.e., bees and neurons)
that work in parallel to integrate noisy evidence, weigh
competing alternatives, and converge on decisions in
synchrony. In both, outcomes are arrived at through a real-time
AUTHOR DRAFT (pre publication)
competition among sub-populations of excitable units. When
one sub-population exceeds a threshold level of support, the
corresponding alternative is chosen. In honeybees, this enables
the group to converge on optimal decisions, picking the best
solution to complex problems (i.e. selecting a new home
location) over 80% of the time [7,8,9].
The similarity between “brains” and “swarms” becomes
even more apparent when comparing decision-making models
that represent each. For example, the decision process in
primate brains is often modeled as mutually inhibitory leaky
integrators that aggregate incoming evidence from competing
neural populations [10]. A common framework for primate
decision is the Usher-McClelland model in Figure 1 below.
Fig. 1. Usher-McClelland model of neurological decision-making
This neurological decision model can be directly compared
to swarm-based decision models, for example the honey-bee
model represented in Figure 2 below. As shown, swarm-based
decisions follow a very similar process, aggregating input from
sub-populations of swarm members through mutual excitation
and inhibition, until a threshold is exceeded.
Fig. 2. Mutually inhibitory decision-making model in bee swarms
When viewed in this context, it becomes apparent that,
while brains are systems of neurons structured so intelligence
emerges, swarms are systems of brains structured so amplified
intelligence emerges. Thus, the objective of the current study
is to connect human financial traders into synchronous systems
that are structured so that an amplified intelligence emerges.
III. ENABLING HUMAN SWARMS
Unlike many other social species, humans have not evolved
the natural ability to form closed-loop systems that enable real-
time swarming. That’s because we lack the subtle connections
that other organisms use to establish high speed feedback-loops
among members. Schooling fish detect vibrations in the water
around them. Flocking birds detect subtle motions propagating
through the population. Swarming bees use complex body
vibrations called a “waggle dance. To enable real-time
swarming among groups of networked humans, specialized
user interfaces, intelligence algorithms, and networking
paradigms are required to close the loop among all members.
To address this need, a technology called Swarm AI was
developed to enable human groups to congregate online as
real-time swarms, connecting synchronously from anywhere in
the world. It was first deployed in 2015 in an online platform
called UNU that allow distributed groups of users to form
closed-loop swarms using standard web-browsers [1]. Modeled
after the decision-making process of honeybee swarms, the
online system allows groups of distributed users to work in
parallel to (a) integrate noisy evidence, (b) weigh competing
alternatives, and (c) converge on decisions in synchrony,
while also allowing all participants to perceive and react to the
changing system in real-time, thereby closing a feedback loop
around the full population of participants.
As shown in Figure 3, swarms answer questions by moving
a graphical puck to select among a set of alternatives. Each
participant provides input by manipulating a graphical magnet
with a mouse or touchscreen. By positioning their magnet with
respect to the moving puck, real-time participants express and
impart their personal intent on the swarm as a whole. The input
from each user is not a discrete vote, but a stream of vectors
that varies freely over time. Because the full population of
users can adjust their intent continuously in real-time, the
swarm moves, not based on the input of any individual, but
based on the dynamics of the full system. This enables a
complex negotiation among all members at once, empowering
the group to collectively explore the decision-space and
converge on the most agreeable solution in synchrony.
Fig. 3. A human swarm answering a question in real-time
AUTHOR DRAFT (pre publication)
It is important to note that participants do not only vary the
direction of their intent, but also modulate the magnitude of
their intent by adjusting the distance between their magnet and
the puck. Because the puck is in continuous motion across the
decision-space, users need to continually move their magnet so
that it stays close to the puck’s outer rim. This is significant,
for it requires participants to be engaged continuously
throughout the decision process, evaluating and re-evaluating
their intent as they convey their contribution. If they stop
adjusting their magnet with respect to the changing position of
the puck, the distance grows and their applied sentiment wanes.
Thus, like bees vibrating their bodies to express sentiment
in a biological swarm, or neurons firing activation signals to
express conviction levels within a biological neural-network,
the participants in an artificial swarm must continuously update
and express their changing preferences during the decision
process, or lose their influence over the collective outcome. In
addition, intelligence algorithms monitor the behaviors of all
swarm members in real-time, inferring their implied conviction
based upon their relative motions over time. This reveals a
range of behavioral characteristics within the swarm population
and weights their contributions accordingly, from entrenched
participants to flexible participants to fickle participants.
IV. FINANCIAL PREDICTION STUDY
To assess the ability of human swarms to amplify their
accuracy in financial predictions, a study was conducted over a
fourteen week period using groups of volunteers who were
unaffiliated with the research team. The participants were all
self-identified as active traders who follow the financial
markets daily and make financial trades regularly. Each
weekly group consisted of between 7 to 36 participants. To
establish a baseline, all participants provided their weekly
forecasts as individuals using a standard online survey. The
group then congregated online as a real-time swarm using the
UNU swarming platform to make synchronous forecasts.
Across the fourteen week period, predictions were made for
the following financial indices: (a) the S&P 500 (SPX), (b) the
gold shares index fund (GLD), (c) the gold miners index fund
(GDX), and (d) the crude oil index (CRUDE). The forecasts
were generated every Tuesday at market close. The participants
were asked to predict if each index would be higher or lower
from the current price at market close on Friday (i.e. 72 hours
later). Predictions were recorded first from individuals on
private surveys, then from swarms working together as a
system. In addition, participants were asked to qualify the
expected change in price by indicating if the predicted move
would be “by a little” or “by a lot. This was included as a
means for evoking participant confidence in their directional
forecast rather than as a true predictor of magnitude.
Figure 4 shows a snapshot of a human swarm comprised of
24 participants in the process of predicting a weekly change in
GDX price. As shown in the figure, the swarm is given four
options to choose from, enabling the swarm to identify which
direction the index will move, as well as express a general
sense of magnitude. The magnitude indicator is helpful as it
causes the swarm to split into multiple different factions and
then converge over time on a solution that maximizes their
collective confidence and conviction. It’s important to note
that Figure 4 shows an instantaneous snapshot of the swarm as
it moves over time towards a final answer. The full process of
converging upon a solution generally required between 10 and
30 seconds of real-time interactions within the swarm.
Fig. 4. Snapshot of a human swarm predicting GDX in real-time
A process called Faction Analysis has been developed to
help researchers visualize how the participants in a human
swarm adjust their support over time and converge upon the
answer they can best agree upon. Figure 5 shows a faction
analysis plot for the swarm above, the colors indicating how
the swarm was initially split (at t=0.0) and then converged over
the time upon the down a little” option (at t=12.5), which in
this case was an accurate prediction.
Fig. 5. Faction Analysis for human swarm predicting GDX
V. ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
For each of the fourteen weeks in the testing period, a set of
predictions were made for each of the four market indices
(SPX, GLD, GDX, CRUDE), providing 56 sets of predictions.
Results were generated indicating: (a) Individual Accuracy
computed as the average performance across the pool of human
subjects, (b) Crowd Accuracy computed by taking the most
popular prediction from the participant pool and using that to
compute accuracy over time, and (c) Swarm Accuracy
computed by assessing the accuracy of the predictions made by
the swarms each week.
AUTHOR DRAFT (pre publication)
To assess whether the human swarms predicted the
directional change in market indices more accurately than
individuals, we compared swarm performance with individual
performance using a bootstrapping procedure. For each of the
four investment categories (SPX, GLD, GDX, CRUDE) and
each prediction week, we selected the answer provided by an
individual sampled at random among the individuals who
provided a response for that particular week and investment
type. Answers were averaged across the four investment types
and the 14 weeks to obtain a percentage accuracy measure. The
procedure was repeated 10,000 times in order to obtain a
distribution of probabilities for making a correct prediction.
The distribution, shown in Figure 6 as a probability density
function, represents the probability of an individual making a
correct prediction when responses are randomly sampled from
the individual answers provided. With a median accuracy of
61%, the individuals were moderately better than random
guessing when predicting the directional change in these
market indicators. The red line in Figure 6 shows the empirical
performance of the swarms, which at 77% accuracy was
significantly higher performing as compared to individuals.
The probability of individuals scoring better than the swarm,
using a random sampling procedure, was extremely low
(p=0.002) indicating a highly significant result.
Fig. 6. Individual Accuracy vs Swarm Accuracy when predicting the
directional change in all four indices in the subsequent 72-hour period.
A similar analysis was done using the more traditional
“Wisdom of Crowd” method of taking the most popular
predictions across the pool of individuals as the forecast. This
is shown as the blue line in the figure above. At 66% accurate,
the crowd was significantly lower performing than the swarm.
In addition, the probability of an individual scoring better than
the crowd was less conclusive (p=0.164). For these reasons,
the results suggest that real-time swarming is a significantly
more accurate and more reliable method for amplifying the
intelligence of a human population when making financial
forecasts. Looking at the results as a percentage increase, the
swarms, on average, were 26% more accurate when predicting
the directional movement in the financial indices than the
individual financial traders who comprised those swarms.
In addition to analyzing the predictive accuracy across all
four indices in aggregate (as shown in Figure 6 above), it is
also instructive to assess performance with respect to each of
the four financial categories in isolation. This is shown in
Figure 7 below. Across 14 weeks, the swarm outperformed the
individual traders and the crowd-based forecasts in all four
instances. Of particular interest is the performance of the
swarm when predicting GDX, as we see the crowd
underperformed the individual traders while the swarm
significantly over-performed the individual traders. This
suggests that while crowds can amplify errors, swarms are
more resistant, converging on correct results in instances when
the majority of participants selected an incorrect prediction.
Fig. 7. Individual Accuracy vs Swarm Accuracy when predicting the
directional change in each individual index in the subsequent 72-hour period.
Focusing specifically on the ability of swarms to amplify
the accuracy of human forecasters and thereby enable more
accurate financial predictions, the accuracy improvements for
each of the four indices above are summarized in Table 1
below. As shown, the swarm amplified the accuracy of the
participants by differing amounts across the four financial
categories. The largest accuracy increase was recorded in crude
oil predictions, which registered an impressive 28% point net
gain, corresponding to a 43% amplification in total accuracy.
Table 1. Individual Accuracy vs Swarm Accuracy across each index
In addition to assessing the ability to predict the directional
trend of each financial index, it is also instructive to assess the
ability of individuals, crowds, and swarms to qualify their
predictions further by indicating if the weekly change would be
AUTHOR DRAFT (pre publication)
“by a little” or “by a lot.” While these are loose metrics, each
was tied to a specific threshold. For example, when predicting
GDX, the threshold was defined for the participants as a 4%
change, meaning if the index changed by less than 4% it was
classified as “by a little” and if the index changed by more than
4% it was classified as “by a lot. Because all participants
were required to provide this additional qualifier when
predicting on the survey and in the swarm, the same analysis
can be performed for the primary results above.
Figure 8 below shows a probability density function that
represents the probability of an individual making a correct
prediction (in both direction and value) across all fourteen
weeks and all four market indices. With a median accuracy of
44%, the individuals were less accurate with this additional
qualifier added. The red line in Figure 8 shows the empirical
accuracy of the swarms, which at 57% accuracy, was still
significantly higher performing as compared to individuals.
The probability of individuals scoring better than the swarm,
using a random sampling procedure, was extremely low
(p=0.01) indicating a highly significant result.
Fig. 8. Individual Accuracy vs Swarm Accuracy when predicting the
direction and value change in all four indices in the subsequent 72-hour period.
As also shown in Figure 8 above, the swarm predictions
were more accurate than the crowd predictions, which at 46%
accurate was not statistically better than the individual
forecasts (p=0.32). Thus, even with the added constraint of
predicting if the weekly move would be “by a little” or “by a
lot” (which increased difficulty), the swarm demonstrated
significant benefits over both individual and the crowd-based
forecasts. This suggests that swarm-based forecasting is not
only a benefit when the individuals are most often correct (as
in Figure 6), but also a benefit when the individuals are most
often incorrect (as in Figure 8), further supporting robustness.
VI. CONCLUSIONS
Can real-time swarms of financial traders outperform the
predictive accuracy of individual traders? The results of the
current study suggest that swarms can significantly increase
prediction accuracy when forecasting the directional movement
of certain financial metrics. The results also show that the
swarming process is more accurate and more repeatable than
traditional crowd-based forecasting. Additional research is
warranted to further validate the benefits of Artificial Swarm
Intelligence for financial forecasting applications. Of particular
interest is the ability of swarms to amplify prediction accuracy
in longer term predictions, as the current study used a relatively
short 72-hour forecasting window. Other topics recommended
for ongoing research include exploring swarms of larger sizes,
comparing participants of varying expertise levels, and testing
improved swarming algorithms.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Thanks to Chris Hornbostel and David Baltaxe for their
efforts in coordinating the weekly swarms of financial traders
that generated predictions. Also, thanks to Unanimous A.I.
for the use of the unu.ai platform for this ongoing work.
REFERENCES
[1] Rosenberg, L.B., “Human Swarms, a real-time method for collective
intelligence.” Proceedings of the European Conference on Artificial Life
2015, pp. 658-659
[2] Rosenberg, Louis. Artificial Swarm Intelligence vs Human Experts,
Neural Networks (IJCNN) 2016
[3] Rosenberg, Louis. Baltaxe, David and Pescetelli, Nicollo. "Crowds vs
Swarms, a Comparison of Intelligence," IEEE 2016 Swarm/Human
Blended Intelligence (SHBI), Cleveland, OH, 2016, pp. 1-4.
[4] Baltaxe, David, Rosenberg, Louis and N. Pescetelli, “Amplifying
Prediction Accuracy using Human Swarms”, Collective Intelligence
2017. New York, NY ; 2017.
[5] Seeley T.D, Buhrman S.C 2001 Nest-site selection in honey bees: how
well do swarms implement the ‘best-of-N’ decision rule?” Behav. Ecol.
Sociobiol. 49, 416427
[6] Marshall, James. Bogacz, Rafal. Dornhaus, Anna. Planqué, Robert.
Kovacs, Tim. Franks, Nigel. On optimal decision-making in brains and
social insect colonies. Soc. Interface 2009.
[7] Seeley, Thomas D., et al. "Stop signals provide cross inhibition in
collective decision-making by honeybee swarms." Science 335.6064
(2012): 108-111.
[8] Seeley, Thomas D. Honeybee Democracy. Princeton Univ. Press, 2010.
[9] Seeley, Thomas D., Visscher, P. Kirk. Choosing a home: How the
scouts in a honey bee swarm perceive the completion of their group
decision making. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 54 (5) 511-
520.
[10] Usher, M. McClelland J.L 2001 The time course of perceptual choice:
the leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychol. Rev. 108, 550592
... Researchers at California Polytechnic published a study showing that networked business teams could increase the accuracy of subjective judgement by over 25% when deliberating as real-time swarms [9][10][11]. Researchers at Unanimous AI, Oxford University, and MIT showed that small groups of financial traders, when forecasting the price of oil, gold, and stocks, increased their accuracy by over 25% when using ASI method [12][13][14]. ...
... Based on the structure shown in Figure 1 above, we hypothesize that for any population P, a hyperswarm can be defined with Hn subgroups, each of size S (where S<P), such that a confident minority can sway a less confident majority so long as most subgroups have a membership that is substantially independent of most other subgroups. For example, in the figure above with P=20 and S=7, each subgroup Hn has at least one user in common with 12 We further hypothesize that under the right conditions, this hyperswarm structure will allow a minority of users to sway a majority of lower confidence users, and that this effect will occur more often in a hyperswarm with local subgroups than standard artificial swarms with global connectivity (i.e. all members directly exposed to all other members). ...
Chapter
Swarm Intelligence (SI) is a biological phenomenon that enables social species to reach group decisions by converging as real-time systems. Studied in fish schools, bird flocks, and bee swarms, SI has been shown to greatly amplify group intelligence. Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is recently developed method that enables networked human groups, moderated by real-time swarming algorithms to achieve similar benefits. While ASI can significantly boost the accuracy of group forecasts, estimation, and decisions, the process can be brittle if a large majority supports an inaccurate view, even if their average confidence is low. Thus, a major focus of ASI research has been to increase resilience to low-confidence majorities. This paper introduces a new structure called a hyperswarm that enables a confident minority to more easily sway an unsure majority, enabling convergence on solutions that would otherwise be inaccessible. The approach involves dividing a population P into a set of overlapping subgroups (H1, H2 … HP) such that each member is only exposed to the real-time behaviors of their subgroup. And because each subgroup overlaps multiple other subgroups, interactions can quickly propagate across the full population. In this paper we simulate hyperswarm dynamics, showing that a confident minority can sway a less confident majority in real time, even when more than 65% of the participants initially harbor the majority view, so long as the minority has higher confidence. In addition, we derive theoretical guidelines to predict hyperswarm success based on population size, subgroup size, and confidence differential.
... Unlike votes, polls, or surveys, which treat participants as separable datapoints for post-hoc statistical aggregation, the "swarming" process treats each individual as an active member of a synchronous system, enabling the full group to converge on solutions as a unified intelligence. This is achieved using a unique combination of human-computer interaction (HCI) methods and intelligence algorithms (Rosenberg et al., 2017;Rosenberg and Willcox, 2020). A snapshot of a real-time "human swarm" is shown in Fig. 1 below as it would be seen simultaneously by all participants. ...
Conference Paper
The biological phenomenon of Swarm Intelligence (SI) enables social species to converge on group decisions by interacting in real-time systems. Studied in schools of fish, bee swarms, and bird flocks, biologists have shown for decades that SI can greatly amplify group intelligence in natural systems. Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is a computer-mediated technique developed in 2015 to enable networked human groups to form real-time systems that can deliberate and converge on decisions, predictions, estimations, and prioritizations. A unique combination of real-time HCI methods and AI algorithms, ASI technology (also called “Human Swarming” or “Swarm AI”) has been shown in many studies to amplify group intelligence in forecasting tasks, often enabling small groups of non-professionals to exceed expert level performance. In the current study, small groups of approximately 24 amateur sports fans used an online platform called Swarm to collaboratively make weekly predictions (against the spread) of every football game in four consecutive NFL seasons (2019 - 2022) for a total of 1027 forecasted games. Approximately 5 games per week (as forecast by the human swarm) were identified as “predictable” using statistical heuristics. Performance was compared against the Vegas betting markets and measured against accepted performance benchmarks for professional handicappers. It is well known that professional bettors rarely achieve more than 55% accuracy against the Vegas spread and that top experts in the world rarely exceed 58% accuracy. In this study the amateur sports fans achieved 62.5% accuracy against the spread when connected as real-time “swarms.” A statistical analysis of this result (across 4 NFL seasons) found that swarms outperformed the 55% accuracy benchmark for human experts with significance (p=0.002). These results confirmed for the first time that groups of amateurs, when connected in real-time using ASI, can consistently generate forecasts that exceeded expert level performance with a high degree of statistical certainty.Keywords: Swarm Intelligence, Artificial Swarm Intelligence, Collective Intelligence, Wisdom of Crowds, Hyperswarms
... These configurations include relatively simple systems that aggregate human and machine judgments (Fildes & Petropoulos, 2015;Yaniv & Hogarth, 1993), and systems that allow for human decision-makers to adjust the data fed into a machine model or the model outputs themselves (Grønsund & Aanestad, 2020;Sanders & Manrodt, 2003;Wolfe & Flores, 1990). They also include more complex systems that use machines to mediate collective decision-making processes (Burton et al., 2021a, b;Rosenberg et al., 2017). While each of these configurations aims to increase decision accuracy, no single configuration suits all contexts. ...
Chapter
A longstanding finding in the judgment and decision-making literature is that human decision performance can be improved with the help of a mechanical aid. Despite this observation and celebrated advances in computing technologies, recently presented evidence of algorithm aversion raises concerns about whether the potential of human-machine decision-making is undermined by a human tendency to discount algorithmic outputs. In this chapter, we examine the algorithm aversion phenomenon and what it means for judgment in predictive analytics. We contextualize algorithm aversion in the broader human vs. machine debate and the augmented decision-making literature before defining algorithm aversion, its implications, and its antecedents. Finally, we conclude with proposals to improve methods and metrics to help guide the development of human-machine decision-making.
... Swarm Intelligence has gained many applications in business, especially as financial activities are conducted increasingly online. There is now a wide range of applications, such as sentiment analysis (Yildirim 2022), security (Mishra et al. 2021), competitive bidding (Bajpai et al. 2008), and predicting financial markets (Rosenberg et al. 2017) (see Sect. 5.6). ...
Article
Full-text available
Patterns and processes emerge unbidden in complex systems when many simple entities interact. This overview emphasizes the role of networks in emergence, notably network topology, modules, motifs, critical phase changes, networks of networks and dual-phase evolution. Several driving mechanisms are examined, including percolation, entrainment, and feedback. The account also outlines some of the modelling paradigms and methods used to study emergence, and presents cases to show how emergence occurs, and its implications in economics and other real-world contexts.
... Opinion aggregation in WoC systems is an open problem [18]. Previous work has shown promise in aggregating human opinions [19,20], and the work presented here continues with the honey-bee-derived "swarm intelligence" aggregation mechanism published in [8]. Bees and other simple organisms are able to forage large areas, finding near optimal food sources and new nesting locations by sending a subset of bees, the scouts, to search the environment around the colony [21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Wisdom-of-Crowds-Bots (WoC-Bots) are simple, modular agents working together in a multi-agent environment to collectively make binary predictions. The agents represent a knowledge-diverse crowd, with each agent trained on a subset of available information. A honey-bee-derived swarm aggregation mechanism is used to elicit a collective prediction with an associated confidence value from the agents. Due to their multi-agent design, WoC-Bots can be distributed across multiple hardware nodes, include new features without re-training existing agents, and the aggregation mechanism can be used to incorporate predictions from other sources, thus improving overall predictive accuracy of the system. In addition to these advantages, we demonstrate that WoC-Bots are competitive with other top classification methods on three datasets and apply our system to a real-world sports betting problem, producing a consistent return on investment from 1 January 2021 through 15 November 2022 on most major sports.
... Researchers at Boeing and the U.S. Army recently showed that small groups of military pilots, when using ASI technology, could more effectively generate subjective insights about the design of cockpits than traditional methods [8]. Also, researchers at Unanimous AI, Oxford University, and MIT showed that small groups of financial traders, when forecasting the price of oil, gold, and public equities, increased their predictive accuracy by over 25% when using ASI method [12][13][14]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is a powerful method for harnessing the collective intelligence of decentralized human teams and amplifying their decision-making accuracy. Previous studies have shown that ASI collaboration environments, such as the Swarm® software platform, can significantly increase the collaborative accuracy of decentralized groups across a wide range of tasks from forecasting and prioritization to estimation and evaluation. In this paper, we introduce a new ASI method for amplifying group intelligence called the “slider-swarm” and show that networked human groups using this method were 11% more accurate in generating collaborative forecasts as compared to traditional poll-based Wisdom of Crowds (WoC) aggregation methods (p<0.001). These results were generated by groups tasked with forecasting three different real-world events. These results suggest slider-swarms can amplify group forecasting accuracy across a range of authentic forecasting applications.
... Researchers at California Polytechnic published a study showing that networked business teams increased their accuracy on a standard subjective judgment test by over 25% when deliberating as real-time ASI swarms [9][10][11]. Also, researchers at Unanimous AI, Oxford University, and MIT showed that small groups of financial traders, when forecasting the price of oil, gold, and stocks, increased their predictive accuracy by over 25% when using ASI method [12][13][14]. And researchers at Unanimous AI showed that networked human teams collaboratively responding to standard IQ question tests could increase their collective IQ score by 14 points when working together using the Swarm ® software platform vs WoC voting. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is a powerful method for amplifying the collective intelligence of decentralized human teams and quickly improving their decision-making accuracy. Previous studies have shown that ASI tools, such as the Swarm ® software platform, can significantly amplify the collaborative accuracy of decentralized groups across a wide range of tasks from forecasting and prioritization to estimation and evaluation. In this paper, we introduce a new ASI method for amplifying group intelligence called the "slider-swarm" and show that networked human groups using this method were 11% more accurate in generating collaborative forecasts as compared to traditional polling-based Wisdom of Crowds (WoC) aggregation methods (p<0.001). Finally, we show that groups using slider-swarm on three real-world forecasting tasks, including forecasting the winners of the 2022 Academy Awards, produce collective forecasts that are 11% more accurate than a WoC aggregation. These results suggest slider-swarms amplify group forecasting accuracy across a range of real-world forecasting applications.
... Researchers at California Polytechnic published a study showing that networked business teams increased their accuracy on a standard subjective judgement test by over 25% when deliberating as real-time ASI swarms [9,10]. And researchers at Unanimous AI, Oxford University, and MIT showed that small groups of financial traders, when forecasting the price of oil, gold, and stocks, increased their predictive accuracy by over 25% when using ASI method [11,12]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is a recently developed method that enables networked human groups to converge on more accurate group forecasts, estimations, and decisions. While ASI can significantly amplify collective intelligence, the process struggles when too large of a majority supports an inaccurate view, even if their average confidence is low. Thus, a major goal of ASI research is to increase resilience to low-confidence majorities. This paper introduces a new ASI structure called a Hyperswarm that enables a confident minority to more readily sway an unsure majority. The approach involves dividing a population P into a set of overlapping sub-groups (H 1 , H 2 … H P ) such that each member only interacts with members of their subgroup. And because each subgroup overlaps multiple other subgroups, the local interactions quickly propagate throughout the full population. In this paper we simulate hyperswarms, showing that a confident minority can intelligently overcome a less confident majority, even when 70% of participants initially harbor the majority view. In addition, we explore a variety of hyperswarm design parameters and derive guidelines for future development. Keywords: Collective Intelligence, Swarm Intelligence, Hyperswarm
Chapter
Artificial Swarm Intelligence (ASI) is a powerful method for amplifying the collective intelligence of decentralized human teams and quickly improving their decision-making accuracy. Previous studies have shown that ASI tools, such as the Swarm® software platform, can significantly amplify the collaborative accuracy of decentralized groups across a wide range of tasks from forecasting and prioritization to estimation and evaluation. In this paper, we introduce a new ASI method for amplifying group intelligence called the “slider-swarm” and show that networked human groups using this method were 11% more accurate in generating collaborative forecasts as compared to traditional polling-based Wisdom of Crowds (WoC) aggregation methods (p < 0.001). Finally, we show that groups using slider-swarm on three real-world forecasting tasks, including forecasting the winners of the 2022 Academy Awards, produce collective forecasts that are 11% more accurate than a WoC aggregation. These results suggest slider-swarms amplify group forecasting accuracy across a range of real-world forecasting applications.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
For well over a century, researchers in the field of Collective Intelligence have shown that groups can outperform individuals when making decisions, predictions, and forecasts. The most common methods for harnessing the intelligence of groups treats the population as a “crowd” of independent agents that provide input in isolation in the form of polls, surveys, and market transactions. While such crowd-based methods can be effective, they are markedly different from how natural systems harness group intelligence. In the natural world, groups commonly form real-time closed-loop systems (i.e. “swarms”) that converge on solutions in synchrony. The present study compares the predictive ability of crowds and swarms when tapping the intelligence of human groups. More specifically, the present study tasked a crowd of 469 football fans and a swarm of 29 football fans in a challenge to predict 20 Prop Bets during the 2016 Super Bowl. Results revealed that the crowd, although 16 times larger in size, was significantly less accurate (at 47% correct) than the swarm (at 68% correct). Further, the swarm outperformed 98% of the individuals in the full study. These results suggest that swarming, with closed-loop feedback, is potentially a more effective method for tapping the insights of groups than traditional polling.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
"Artificial Swarm Intelligence" (ASI) strives to amplify the combined intelligence of networked human groups by enabling populations of participants to form real-time closed-loop systems modeled after biological swarms. Prior studies [Rosenberg 2015] have shown that "human swarms" can converge on more accurate decisions and predictions than traditional methods for tapping the wisdom of groups such as votes and polls. To further explore the predictive ability of ASI systems, 75 randomly selected sports fans were assembled into real-time human swarms using the UNU software platform and were tasked with predicting College Bowl football games against the spread. Results show intelligence amplification.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Much research has been done in the field of collective intelligence to aggregate input from human populations with the goal of amplifying the abilities of the groups. Nearly all prior research follows a similar model where input is collected from human participants in isolation and then aggregated statistically after the fact. The paper introduces a radically different approach in which the human participants is not aggregated statistically, but through a real-time dynamic control in which the participants act, react, and interact as a part of a system modeled after swarms in nature. Early testing of these "human swarms" suggest great potential for amplifying the intelligence of human groups, exceeding traditional aggregation methods. on the simulation of collaborative systems as it relates to the emergence of real-time collective intelligence. While theoretical studies are of great research value, there’s a growing need for real-world platforms that test the emergence of collective intelligence among human users. This short paper introduces such a platform. It enables networks of online collaborators to converge on questions, decisions, and dilemmas in real-time, functioning as a unified dynamic system. The dynamic system has been modeled after biological swarms, which is why refer to the process as “social swarming” or "human swarming". Early testing of human swarms suggests a great potential for harnessing collective intelligence.
Article
Full-text available
This study views a honey bee swarm as a supraorganismal entity which has been shaped by natural selection to be skilled at choosing a future home site. Prior studies of this decision-making process indicate that swarms attempt to use the best-of-N decision rule: sample some number (N) of alternatives and then select the best one. We tested how well swarms implement this decision rule by presenting them with an array of five nest boxes, only one of which was a high-quality (desirable) nest site; the other four were medium-quality (acceptable) sites. We found that swarms are reasonably good at carrying out the best-of-N decision rule: in four out of five trials, swarms selected the best site. In addition, we gained insights into how a swarm implements this decision rule. We found that when a scout bee returns to the swarm cluster and advertises a potential nest site with a waggle dance, she tunes the strength of her dance in relation to the quality of her site: the better the site, the stronger the dance. A dancing bee tunes her dance strength by adjusting the number of waggle-runs/dance, and she adjusts the number of waggle-runs/dance by changing both the duration and the rate of her waggle-run production. Moreover, we found that a dancing bee changes the rate of her waggle-run production by changing the mean duration of the return-phase portion of her dance circuits. Differences in return-phase duration underlie the impression that dances differ in liveliness. Although a honey bee swarm has bounded rationality (e.g., it lacks complete knowledge of the possible nesting sites), through its capacity for parallel processing it can choose a nest site without greatly reducing either the breadth or depth of its consideration of the alternative sites. Such thoroughness of information gathering and processing no doubt helps a swarm implement the best-of-N decision rule.
Article
Full-text available
This study considers the mystery of how the scout bees in a honey bee swarm know when they have completed their group decision making regarding the swarm's new nest site. More specifically, we investigated how the scouts sense when it is appropriate for them to begin producing the worker piping signals that stimulate their swarm-mates to prepare for the flight to their new home. We tested two hypotheses: "consensus sensing," the scouts noting when all the bees performing waggle dances are advertising just one site; and "quorum sensing," the scouts noting when one site is being visited by a sufficiently large number of scouts. Our test involved monitoring four swarms as they discovered, recruited to, and chose between two nest boxes and their scouts started producing piping signals. We found that a consensus among the dancers was neither necessary nor sufficient for the start of worker piping, which indicates that the consensus sensing hypothesis is false. We also found that a buildup of 10–15 or more bees at one of the nest boxes was consistently associated with the start of worker piping, which indicates that the quorum sensing hypothesis may be true. In considering why the scout bees rely on reaching a quorum rather than a consensus as their cue of when to start preparing for liftoff, we suggest that quorum sensing may provide a better balance between accuracy and speed in decision making. In short, the bees appear to begin preparations for liftoff as soon as enough of the scout bees, but not all of them, have approved of one of the potential nest sites.
Article
Full-text available
Stop, Don't Go There Decision-making in complex brains requires the reaching of a consensus based on information relayed by multiple neurons. A similar situation occurs in the decision-making process of swarming bees, where multiple individuals relay information about suitable hive sites, but a single site is chosen by the swarm. Seeley et al. (p. 108 , published online 8 December; see the Perspective by Niven ) show that consensus in this system is reached as information from scouting bees accumulates within the hive. Stop signals were given by scout bees from a particular site to those scout bees signaling from other sites. As the signals accumulate, scouting ceases and the bees prepare to swarm to the site that was best represented among the scouts. When this process was simulated, the results indicated that cross-inhibition among the bees functions similarly to that which occurs among neurons within complex brains. In both cases, such cross-inhibition prevents the overall system from coming to an impasse.
Article
Full-text available
The problem of how to compromise between speed and accuracy in decision-making faces organisms at many levels of biological complexity. Striking parallels are evident between decision-making in primate brains and collective decision-making in social insect colonies: in both systems, separate populations accumulate evidence for alternative choices; when one population reaches a threshold, a decision is made for the corresponding alternative, and this threshold may be varied to compromise between the speed and the accuracy of decision-making. In primate decision-making, simple models of these processes have been shown, under certain parametrizations, to implement the statistically optimal procedure that minimizes decision time for any given error rate. In this paper, we adapt these same analysis techniques and apply them to new models of collective decision-making in social insect colonies. We show that social insect colonies may also be able to achieve statistically optimal collective decision-making in a very similar way to primate brains, via direct competition between evidence-accumulating populations. This optimality result makes testable predictions for how collective decision-making in social insects should be organized. Our approach also represents the first attempt to identify a common theoretical framework for the study of decision-making in diverse biological systems.
Article
The time course of perceptual choice is discussed in a model of gradual, leaky, stochastic, and competitive information accumulation in nonlinear decision units. Special cases of the model match a classical diffusion process, but leakage and competition work together to address several challenges to existing diffusion, random walk, and accumulator models. The model accounts for data from choice tasks using both time-controlled (e.g., response signal) and standard reaction time paradigms and its adequacy compares favorably with other approaches. A new paradigm that controls the time of arrival of information supporting different choice alternatives provides further support. The model captures choice behavior regardless of the number of alternatives, accounting for the log-linear relation between reaction time and number of alternatives (Hick's law) and explains a complex pattern of visual and contextual priming in visual word identification.
The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model
  • M Usher
  • J L Mc Clelland