- Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
- Learn more
Download available
Content available from European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4
REVIEW
Sustained eects ofneurofeedback inADHD: asystematic review
andmeta‑analysis
JessicaVanDoren1 · MartijnArns2,3,4 · HartmutHeinrich1,5 · MadelonA.Vollebregt4,6· UteStrehl7·
SandraK.Loo8
Received: 5 October 2017 / Accepted: 5 February 2018 / Published online: 14 February 2018
© The Author(s) 2018. This article is an open access publication
Abstract
Neurofeedback (NF) has gained increasing interest in the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Given
learning principles underlie NF, lasting clinical treatment effects may be expected. This systematic review and meta-analysis
addresses the sustainability of neurofeedback and control treatment effects by considering randomized controlled studies
that conducted follow-up (FU; 2–12months) assessments among children with ADHD. PubMed and Scopus databases
were searched through November 2017. Within-group and between-group standardized mean differences (SMD) of parent
behavior ratings were calculated and analyzed. Ten studies met inclusion criteria (NF: ten studies, N=256; control: nine
studies, N=250). Within-group NF effects on inattention were of medium effect size (ES) (SMD=0.64) at post-treatment
and increased to a large ES (SMD=0.80) at FU. Regarding hyperactivity/impulsivity, NF ES were medium at post-treatment
(SMD=0.50) and FU (SMD=0.61). Non-active control conditions yielded a small significant ES on inattention at post-
treatment (SMD=0.28) but no significant ES at FU. Active treatments (mainly methylphenidate), had large ES for inattention
(post: SMD=1.08; FU: SMD=1.06) and medium ES for hyperactivity/impulsivity (post: SMD=0.74; FU: SMD=0.67).
Between-group analyses also revealed an advantage of NF over non-active controls [inattention (post: SMD=0.38; FU:
SMD=0.57); hyperactivity–impulsivity (post: SMD=0.25; FU: SMD=0.39)], and favored active controls for inattention
only at pre-post (SMD=− 0.44). Compared to non-active control treatments, NF appears to have more durable treatment
effects, for at least 6months following treatment. More studies are needed for a properly powered comparison of follow-up
effects between NF and active treatments and to further control for non-specific effects.
Keywords Neurofeedback· EEG biofeedback· ADHD· Meta-analysis· Sustainability· Follow-up
Introduction
Clinical guidelines for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
der (ADHD) recommend multimodal treatment approaches,
with current evidence suggesting that medication, including
methylphenidate and various amphetamine formulations, in
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this
article (https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0078 7-018-1121-4) contains
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
* Martijn Arns
martijn@brainclinics.com
1 Department ofChild andAdolescent Mental Health,
University Hospital Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany
2 Department ofExperimental Psychology, Utrecht University,
Utrecht, TheNetherlands
3 neuroCare Group, Munich, Germany
4 Research Institute Brainclinics, Bijleveldsingel 34,
6524ADNijmegen, TheNetherlands
5 kbo-Heckscher-Klinikum, Munich, Germany
6 Department ofCognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute
forBrain, Cognition andBehaviour, Radboud University
Medical Centre, Nijmegen, TheNetherlands
7 Institute forMedical Psychology, University ofTuebingen,
Tuebingen, Germany
8 Department ofPsychiatry andBiobehavioral Science,
David Geffen School ofMedicine, University ofCalifornia,
LosAngeles, USA
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
294 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
conjunction with psychosocial treatment are most effec-
tive in the short-term [1]. Medication treatments have large
effect size in the acute treatment of ADHD [2] and, when
combined with psychosocial treatments, large effects up to
2years of treatment were observed [3, 4]. Nevertheless, it
is widely accepted that further treatments with long-lasting
effects have to be developed and evaluated.
Over the last decade, an increasing number of studies
investigating non-pharmacological treatments have been
published. Neurofeedback (NF), which aims at improving
self-regulation of brain activity (most often the electroen-
cephalogram, EEG) using a brain–computer interface, has
gained popularity [5]. A promising aspect of neurofeedback
is that it may rely on procedural learning, thereby poten-
tially allowing lasting effects and thus longer clinical benefit
after completion of neurofeedback treatment. In their review,
Arns and Kenemans [6] found that the clinical effects of
neurofeedback were maintained across 6 and 24-month fol-
low-up periods, with a trend for larger symptom decreases
for hyperactivity/impulsivity after 24months than after
6months, albeit only based on two randomized studies at
6months and only one at the 24-month follow-up, thus limit-
ing the generalizability of the findings. A systematic review
and meta-analysis that assess the sustainability of clinical
effects of NF studies is, therefore, desirable.
In recent years, several randomized control studies
(RCTs) and meta-analyses have been published on the effi-
cacy of neurofeedback for children with ADHD, overall
with mixed results and interpretations [7–11]. Regarding
RCTs published over the last decade, one major issue is
the lack of standardization of neurofeedback protocols and
implementations. Neurofeedback treatments using theta/
beta, slow cortical potential (SCP), or sensorimotor-rhythm
(SMR) protocols have been well studied and can be seen as
‘standard’ neurofeedback treatments (for review and discus-
sion see: Arns etal. [5] and Figure S-2 in Supplementary
Material). These ‘standard’ protocols have been selected
as the primary protocols for NF research based on findings
that children with ADHD have specific deficits in compari-
son with healthy controls: e.g. increased theta/beta ratios
in subgroups of ADHD patients hypothesized to be related
to inattention [12]; decreased contingent negative varia-
tion amplitudes (targeted by SCP training) [13]; addressing
hyperkinetic behavior by means of training sensorimotor
rhythm [14]. Application of these standard neurofeedback
protocols in ADHD have most consistently resulted in clini-
cal benefit in children with ADHD, whereas application of
other neurofeedback protocols has yielded more variable and
mixed results [5, 8]. A second issue with currently available
neurofeedback RCTs is that several studies deviated from
their initial clinical trials register, with samples ranging from
34% [15] to 60% [16, 17] smaller than their preregistered
sample size. This raises the likelihood of over-interpreting
results from these studies, which are insufficiently powered.
This issue is addressed using meta-analyses since effects are
combined across studies, resulting in increased statistical
power.
A third issue concerns the specificity of NF treatment
effects. While NF has been shown to be beneficial for the
treatment of ADHD symptoms, it remains a debate whether
behavioral improvements are the result of specific aspects
of the NF treatment such as the style of training, or active
learning of control over their brain state that is then gen-
eralized to daily life or whether non-specific treatment
effects such as unconditional positive regard of the thera-
pist, positive expectation of change, or repeated practice of
sitting at a computer for increasing lengths of time leads
to behavioral change. To control these non-specific or pla-
cebo effects, double-blind placebo-controlled studies are
often requested—comparable to what is considered as gold
standard in drug research. However, in regard to NF, there
are methodical and ethical issues to consider which have led
to the development of control conditions such as cognitive
training or EMG biofeedback and assessments for placebo
factors via evaluation scales [18, 19]. Though it is known
that placebo effects may last over longer periods [20], it
seems unlikely that they grow larger over time. Hence focus-
ing on longer-term outcomes of NF may also help to clarify
the placebo/specific vs. non-specific issue.
Two recent meta-analyses on the acute efficacy of neu-
rofeedback for children with ADHD published by the Euro-
pean ADHD Guidelines Group (EAGG) have used the inter-
esting concept of most-proximal (e.g. least blinded, often
parent ratings) versus probably blinded measures (most often
teacher ratings), assuming that the probably blinded meas-
ures (i.e., teacher ratings) are less susceptible to expecta-
tion/non-specific effects and, therefore, more valid [8, 11].
However, this approach has limitations. For example, par-
ent–teacher correlations on behavior rating scales are only
modestly correlated (ranging from 0.23 to 0.49) [21, 22],
suggesting different aspects of the disorder may be detected
by different raters or in different settings. Furthermore, in a
large candidate gene study, parent-rated hyperactive–impul-
sive behaviors were significantly associated with candidate
gene pathways whereas teacher ratings were not [23]. Addi-
tionally, teacher-ratings are sensitive to effects of methylphe-
nidate [24], which could possibly skew the interpretation of
studies that randomize ADHD treatments against a methyl-
phenidate control, when primarily relying on teacher reports.
Finally, for investigating long-term effects, it may not be
advisable to rely on teacher ratings, since the child may have
more than one teacher over time, potentially compromising
the reliability of the rating.
A second limitation of the aforementioned meta-analyses
is the use of between-group effect sizes, which is a good
practice for compiling results from studies that used similar
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
295European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
designs and control groups; comparing for example, psycho-
stimulants to placebo. However, the utility of this method for
studying results across neurofeedback studies, with various
kinds of control groups (ranging from waiting lists, cogni-
tive training to medication), is more challenging. Therefore,
while between-group effect sizes are useful for controlling
for non-specific effects of treatment, they can miss clinical
effects of neurofeedback that are masked by the active or
semi-active control conditions, thus warranting the use of a
within-group effect-size approach, for example as used by
Arns etal. [7], or separately analyze the results for active vs.
semi-active control groups.
To address the above concerns, we have conducted a
systematic review and meta-analysis of the post-treatment
follow-up period of randomized EEG NF studies among
children and adolescents with ADHD. Methodologically,
we (1) used within-group effect sizes to address the issue
of different control groups; (2) used between-group effect
sizes to control for non-specific effects of treatment; (3)
applied a meta-analytical approach to address the issue of
underpowered studies; and (4) focused on the sustainability
of treatment effects by looking specifically at the follow-up
(FU) period (i.e., pre-FU, post-FU time points), which will
provide information regarding the plausibility of sustainable
NF effects, relative to other treatments, in ADHD.
Method
Study selection
The protocol of this meta-analysis was not preregistered.
A literature search was conducted up to 29th of Novem-
ber 2017 via PubMed and Scopus by author JVD, look-
ing for studies investigating Neurofeedback or EEG Bio-
feedback in ADHD using combinations of the following
keywords: ‘Neurofeedback’, ‘EEG Biofeedback’, ‘Neu-
rotherapy’, ‘SCP’ OR ‘Slow Cortical Potentials’ AND
‘ADHD’, ‘ADD’, ‘Attention Deficit’ OR ‘Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder’. Furthermore, prior meta-analyses
and systematic review reference lists were inspected for
potentially missed studies [7, 8, 10, 11]. After exclusion of
duplicate publications, abstracts were screened for inclu-
sion criteria first by author HH and then by a research
assistant to prevent missing studies. Studies that remained
of interest were then screened based on their full text by
JVD. Inclusion criteria were: (1) randomized controlled
EEG neurofeedback trials published in peer-reviewed
journals; (2) primary diagnosis of ADHD; (3) mean child
age<18years old; (4) available data at a follow-up (FU)
time point for 2 to 12months post-treatment; (5) stand-
ardized mean and standard deviations (SD) for all three
assessments (pre, post, and FU) for at least one of the
following domains had to be available: inattention, hyper-
activity, or hyperactivity/impulsivity ratings from a DSM-
IV/5-based rating scale (these values were taken based on
availability with parental ratings taking priority, then self-
ratings and lastly teacher ratings); (6) publication avail-
able in English; (7) total study sample larger than N=10;
8) less than 50% of participants began or stopped taking
medication between post and FU assessments.
In most of the studies, ‘standard’ neurofeedback protocols
[5] were used, i.e., theta/beta and theta/SMR (sensorimotor
rhythm) training (defined as a down-training of theta and
up-training of beta and SMR, respectively) and slow corti-
cal potential training (SCP) training (addressing modulation
of positive and negative SCPs), for details see Table1 and
Supplementary Figure S-2. Exceptions were the studies of
Arnold etal. [25] and Bink etal. [26] which targeted more
EEG frequency bands (theta, alpha, SMR, and beta), and a
sensitivity analysis was conducted to separately assess the
effects for standard protocols. When the means and SDs
from a given study were not available, or it was unclear if
planned follow-up measurements were published, this infor-
mation was requested via email from the authors. If authors
did not respond or did not provide the missing information,
and if there was not sufficient information available based
on the publication, the study was excluded from the meta-
analysis. Studies were additionally screened for duplicate
data based on author, publication year, participant numbers
and trial registration number (if available). When possible
duplicate data was found, the authors were contacted to
clarify whether the data sets were independent.
Data extraction/outcome measures
Data were first extracted by JVD and checked by HH. The
following pre-, post- and FU-assessment measures were
extracted from the included studies:
1. Demographic and clinical data: age (mean and standard
deviation), medication use, ADHD subtype.
2. Experimental procedure: NF method, control method,
feedback electrode, average number of sessions, session
length.
3. Outcome measures:
Symptom domains: Assessed from parent report with
a validated ADHD rating scale (e.g., DSM-IV rating
scale [27], Conners [28], Barkley [29], FBB-HKS [30],
SWAN [31])
(i) Inattention
(ii) Hyperactivity/impulsivity (if no combined
measure was available, the hyperactivity score
was used).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
296 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
Table 1 Characteristics of included studies
Study or subgroup Year N (FU) Age Treatment FU
(months)
Assessment
instrument
Medicated/
total NMedication
dosagea
Neurofeedback
Heinrich etal. [36] 2004 13 11.1±1.6 SCP (Cz); 25 sessions of 50min 3 FBB-HKS 6/13 No change
Gevensleben etal. [52] 2010 38 9.9±1.3 SCP+theta (4–8Hz)/beta (13–20Hz); Cz; 36 sessions of 50min 6 FBB-HKS 0/38 No change
Arnold etal. [25] 2013 25 9.0±1.5 Theta/alpha↓; beta/SMR↑; Cz; 40 sessions of 45min 2 Conners DSM 7/25 Increased
Li etal. [39] 2013 31 10.8±2.6 MPH (pre)+NF; theta (4–8Hz)/SMR (12–15Hz); electrode NR;40 ses-
sions of 25–30min
6 ADHD RS-IV 31/31 Decreased
Meisel etal. [37] 2013 12 9.5±1.8 Theta (4–7Hz)/beta 15–20Hz); Cz or FCz; 40 session of 30min 2 ADHD RS-IV 2/12 Increased
Steiner etal. [34] 2014 34 8.4±1.1 Theta (4–8Hz)/SMR (12–15Hz); electrode NR; 40 sessions of 45min 6 Conners 27/34 Maintained
Christiansenb2014 18 8.7±1.4 SCP; Cz; 30 sessions of 50min 6 Conners DSM 1/18 Decreased
Bink etal. [26] 2016 41 15.8±3.3 TAU+NF; theta/alpha (4–7, 8–11Hz) ↓, SMR (13–15Hz) ↑, beta/
gamma (22–36Hz) ↓; Cz; 40 sessions of 30min
12 ADHD RS-IV
(self-report)
19/41 NR
Duric etal. [38] 2017 24 11.3±2.8 Theta (4–7Hz)/beta (16–20Hz); Cz; 30 sessions of 40min 6 Barkley 0/24 No change
Gelade etal. [35] 2017 20 9.8±1.9 Theta (4–8Hz)/beta (13–20Hz); Cz; 30 sessions of 45min 6 SWA N 0/20 No change
Control conditions
Gevensleben etal. [52] 2010 23 9.4±1.1 Attention training; 36 sessions of 50min 6 FBB-HKS 0/23 No change
Arnold etal. [25] 2013 11 8.7±2.1 Sham neurofeedback; 40 sessions of 45min 2 Conners DSM 0/11 No change
Li etal. [39] 2013 29 10.4±2.9 MPH (pre)+non-feedback attention training 40 sessions of 25–30min 6 ADHD RS-IV 29/29 Maintained
Meisel etal. [37] 2013 11 8.9±1.5 MPH (inferior dosage: 1mg/kg/day) 2 ADHD RS-IV 11/11 No change
Steiner etal. [34] CT 2014 34 8.9±1.0 Cognitive training; 40 sessions of 45min 6 Conners 14/34 Increased
Steiner etal. [34] WL 2014 36 8.4±1.1 Wait list 6 Conners 20/36 Increased
Christiansenb2014 21 8.9±1.2 Self-management; 30 sessions of 50min 6 Conners DSM 6/21 Increased
Bink etal. [26] 2016 19 16.2±3.4 TAU 12 ADHD RS-IV
(self-report)
12/19 NR
Duric etal. [38] 2017 28 10.8±2.4 MPH (1mg/kg/day; range: 20 to 60 mg) 6 Barkley 29/29 No change
Gelade etal. [35] MPH 2017 21 9.0±1.2 MPH (5–20mg daily) 6 SWA N 21/21 NR
Gelade etal. [35] PA 2017 17 9.6±1.8 Physical activity training; 28 sessions of 30min 6 SWA N 0/17 No change
For neurofeedback (NF) protocols, frequency bands and feedback electrodes are listed. Parent ratings were considered (except for Bink etal. [26] which used self-reports)
SCP slow cortical potential, SMR sensorimotor rhythm, MPH methylphenidate, PA physical activity, FBB-HKS German ADHD rating scale for parents, ADHD RS-IV ADHD Rating Scale-IV,
Conners Conners questionnaire subscale, Conners DSM DSM subscale within the Conners questionnaire; SWAN strengths and weaknesses of ADHD symptoms and Normal Behavior Scale, NR
not reported
a For more study details see Table S-4
b Unpublished data from Christiansen etal. [40]
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
297European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
These measures were used as treatment endpoints at post-
treatment and FU relative to baseline values. Additionally,
the change from post-treatment to FU was assessed to deter-
mine if changes occurred after the treatment had stopped.
Meta‑analysis
A random effects model (due to inherent heterogeneity
between studies) and the inverse variance statistical method
was used to calculate the standardized mean difference
(SMD), 95% confidence intervals, and χ2 statistic using
RevMan version 5.3 [32]. Within-group and between-group
analyses were conducted for the following time points using
the means and standard deviations provided in the papers or
by the authors: (1) pre- and post-treatment; (2) pre-treatment
and follow-up; (3) post-treatment and follow-up. Within-
group analyses used the values as presented in the papers (no
additional calculation was necessary). Between-group means
were calculated by subtracting the mean of the second time
point from the mean of the first time point (ex. pre-treatment
minus post-treatment, pre-treatment minus FU-treatment,
post-treatment minus FU-treatment). The standard devia-
tion of the first point was used for analysis.
Although active treatment effects are not the main focus
of this paper, the control conditions were analyzed as a
whole and assessed using two sub-analyses (non-active and
active control conditions) to provide a frame of reference
for the effects of neurofeedback against different type of
controls. Due to the diversity of the control conditions, we
chose to separate them as ‘active’ (proven to have a clinical
effect in the treatment of ADHD: methylphenidate and self-
management training) and ‘non-active’ (all conditions that
do not classify as active). For a more detailed explanation
of these groupings see Table S-2 in the supplement. See
Tables S-5 and S-6 for values used. When the χ2 statistic of
a sample (Qt) was significant (p<0.05)—indicating that
the variance among effect sizes is greater than expected by
sampling error—studies were assessed for possible heteroge-
neity causes and the resulting studies were omitted from the
meta-analysis, for example based on the type of treatment
(separating active and non-active conditions in the control
groups). Active treatments were defined as medication or
psychotherapy (self-management) that was started system-
atically after pre-treatment assessment. Additionally, a sen-
sitivity analysis was conducted including only studies that
used standard NF protocols (theta/beta, theta/SMR, or SCP).
To assess publication bias, MetaWin version 2.1 [33] was
used to calculate the fail-safe number (Rosenthal’s method:
a<0.05).
Results
A total of ten studies met inclusion criteria for at least one
of the parameters and conditions, resulting in ten studies in
the NF arm and nine studies in the control arm (two control
studies had two control groups [34, 35]). See Figure S-1
for the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews
and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) inclusion flow diagram and
Table1 for characteristics of included studies. The PRISMA
checklist is available in the supplementary material (Table
S-1). The supplementary material has a complete list of
inclusions (Table S-4) and exclusions (Table S-3). Included
studies resulted in a total of 506 participants with ADHD
(256 neurofeedback, 250 control). Follow-up time periods
were 2months (K=2), 3months (K=1), 6months (K=6)
and 12months (K=1).
The control group from Heinrich etal. [36] was excluded
because most of the controls began psychotherapy between
post and FU (personal communication with first author). The
2-month FU time point from the Meisel etal. [37] study
was included instead of the 6-month FU because over 50%
of their NF participants began medication between post-
and 6-month FU measurements while only two participants
started taking medication between post and 2-month FU. The
combined NF+MPH arm of Duric etal. [38] was excluded
since the NF treatment was accompanied by another active
treatment, which did not allow differentiation of treatment
effects; this differed from other studies in which NF children
could receive medication if they had been already medicated
prior to study participation, ensuring that the baseline meas-
urements already included medication effects. The supple-
mentary analysis was used from Gelade etal. [35] instead
of the primary analysis to account for participant drop-out
and medications change. For the majority of the studies, data
were available for participants who completed assessments
at all three time points (pre-, post-, FU) with the exception
of Li etal. [39], in which drop-out from baseline to FU were
3.13% (N=1) for the NF group and 9.38% (N=3) for the
control group.
Regarding medication change for included studies, the
number of participants who began or stopped taking med-
ication did not change over time for most of the studies,
with the exception of four [25, 26, 37, 40]. In summary: of
the total NF participants three stopped taking medication
[pre-post (N=2), post-FU (N=1)], while nine began tak-
ing medication [pre-post (N=1), post-FU (N=8)]; for the
control group only one participant stopped taking medica-
tion from post to FU time point. Dosage was allowed to
be changed in five studies (see Table1); however, dosage
change was variable with some groups maintaining (one NF,
one control), decreasing (two NF) or increasing (two NF,
three control) dosage. See Table S-4 for details.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
298 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
Within‑group analyses
Inattention
Forest plots and results for inattention are presented in
Fig.1; Bar plots in Figure S-3. The test for heterogeneity
was not significant for NF at pre-post (χ2=9.69, df=9,
n.s.) or pre-FU (χ2=12.37, df= 9, n.s.), while controls
did display significant heterogeneity for both pre-post
(χ2=27.95, df=10, p<0.05) and pre-FU (χ2=29.60,
df=10, p<0.05) when all controls were included. When
considering non-active controls only, heterogeneity was no
longer significant for the pre-post measurement (χ2=6.72,
df=6, n.s.), but remained significant for the pre-FU meas-
urement (χ2=14.02, df=6, p<0.05). When only active
controls were included, pre-post heterogeneity remained
significant (χ2= 10.12, df = 3, p < 0.05) while pre-FU
(χ2=0.48, df=3, n.s.) was not significant. These results
suggest that when heterogeneity is significant for controls,
it will be useful to examine SMDs for active and non-active
controls separately, which will be done for the remainder
of the paper. This reduces the number of studies examined,
however, and caution will be used when interpreting the
results. Heterogeneity was non-significant for all groups for
the post-FU measurement: NF (χ2=2.29, df=9, n.s.); non-
active (χ2=4.05, df=6, n.s.); active (χ2=7.40, df=3, n.s.).
NF yielded a significant medium effect size (SMD=0.64;
95% CI 0.45, 0.82) for the change from the pre- to post-
treatment measurements and a significant large effect size
(SMD=0.80; 95% CI 0.58, 1.01) for the change from pre-
to FU measurement; however, post-treatment to FU was
not significant (SMD=0.14; 95% CI − 0.03, 0.31). For
non-active controls, a small, significant effect for pre-post
(SMD=0.28; 95% CI 0.05, 0.51) was found but the small
effect at pre-FU was no longer significant (SMD=0.29;
95% CI − 0.04, 0.63). When looking at only active controls,
there were large, significant effect sizes at both pre-post (sig-
nificant heterogeneity) (SMD=1.08; 95% CI 0.45, 1.72)
and pre-FU (non-significant heterogeneity) (SMD=1.06;
95% CI 0.73, 1.39). Post-treatment to FU was not significant
for either control group. The fail-safe numbers for NF were:
pre-post (156.0), pre-FU (190.7). For the control conditions,
the fail-safe numbers were: 1. non-active controls: pre-post
(11.2), pre-FU (1.1); 2. active controls: pre-post (13.5), pre-
FU (55.2).
Hyperactivity/impulsivity
Forest plots and results for hyperactivity/impulsivity are
presented in Fig.2; Bar plots in Figure S-3. The test for
heterogeneity was not significant for any of the hyperactiv-
ity/impulsivity measurements, indicating that the variance
of SMD was not large enough to be attributed to sampling
error only (see Fig.2). For comparability to the inatten-
tion domain, the active and non-active control groups are
reported separately.
For NF, a significant medium effect size (SMD=0.50;
95% CI 0.33, 0.68) was found for the pre-post measurement
and a medium effect size (SMD=0.61; 95% CI 0.43, 0.79)
for the pre-FU measurement; the post-FU measurement was
not significant (SMD=0.11; 95% CI − 0.06, 0.28).
Analysis of non-active control groups indicated that none
of the measurements (pre-post, pre-FU, post-FU) were sig-
nificant. When only active controls were considered, there
were significant medium effect sizes for both pre-post
(SMD=0.74; 95% CI 0.41, 1.06) and pre-FU (SMD=0.67;
95% CI 0.35, 0.99). For both control analyses, post-FU was
not significant. The fail-safe numbers for NF were: pre-post
(107.6), pre-FU (163.4). For the controls, the fail-safe num-
bers were: 1. non-active controls: pre-post (0), pre-FU (0);
2. active controls: pre-post (25), pre-FU (18.4).
Between‑group meta‑analysis
Forest plots and results for inattention and hyperactivity/
impulsivity are presented in Fig.3.
Inattention
The test for heterogeneity was significant when including all
studies at pre-post (χ2=30.60, df=10, p<0.05) and signif-
icant at pre-FU (χ2=33.27, df=10, p<0.001). Considering
only trials with non-active control conditions, heterogeneity
was not significant for the pre-post measurement (χ2=7.83,
df=6, n.s.) or the pre-FU measurement (χ2=7.48, df=6,
n.s.). For active controls, heterogeneity was not significant
for pre-post (χ2=5.00, df=3, n.s.) or for pre-FU (χ2=7.05,
df=3, n.s.).
When including only studies with non-active con-
trol conditions, a significant small effect size for pre-post
(SMD=0.38; 95% CI 0.14, 0.61) and a medium effect size
for pre-FU (SMD=0.57; 95% CI 0.34, 0.81) were observed
favoring neurofeedback. When only active controls were
included, a pre-post effect size favoring active controls was
significant (SMD=− 0.44; 95% CI − 0.86, − 0.02) but at
pre-FU it was no longer significant. Post-training to FU was
not significant for either analysis.
Hyperactivity/impulsivity
The test for heterogeneity was not significant for any of the
hyperactivity/impulsivity measurements, indicating that
the variance of SMD was not large enough to be attributed
to sampling error only. The between-group analysis of all
control groups resulted in a significant small effect size
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
299European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
favoring NF (SMD=0.32; 95% CI 0.15, 0.49) at pre-FU,
pre-post was not significant. When including only non-
active control groups (done for comparison with inatten-
tion, not due to heterogeneity), results favored NF with a
significant small effect for the pre-post (SMD=0.25; 95%
CI 0.05, 0.45) measurement and a small effect for pre-FU
(SMD=0.39; 95% CI 0.19, 0.59). Including only active
controls resulted in no significant findings for either pre-
post or pre-FU. For both control analyses post-FU was not
significant; however, post-FU did show a trend toward sig-
nificance favoring neurofeedback over all control groups
(p=0.08; SMD=0.15; 95% CI − 0.02, 0.32].
Sensitivity meta‑analysis: ‘standard’ NF training
See Tables S-7 and S-8 for details regarding this analysis.
Inattention
The tests for heterogeneity as well as effect sizes were simi-
lar to those seen in the within-group and between-group
analyses including all studies, with slightly stronger effect
sizes seen for the sensitivity analysis regarding pre-post and
pre-FU time points (increase in SMD ranging from 0.01 to
0.14), however, with small changes in both directions for
post-FU (change ranging from − 0.02 decrease to 0.02
increase).
Hyperactivity/impulsivity
The tests for heterogeneity as well as effect sizes were simi-
lar to those seen in the within-group and between-group
analyses including all participants (decrease of 0.01 SMD
to increases in SMD up to 0.02).
Discussion
This meta-analysis investigated the effects of neurofeedback
and control conditions directly after treatment and during a
follow-up period (2–12months post-treatment), in which
Fig. 1 Forest Plot of within-group analysis for inattention param-
eter. Total standardized mean difference (SMD) with 95% confidence
interval, overall effect, and heterogeneity are reported. Due to signifi-
cant heterogeneity in the initial control analysis, additional analyses
examining non-active and active controls separately were included.
Pre-Post refers to the difference in means at pre- and post-measure-
ment, and similarly for pre-FU and post-FU
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
300 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
no additional neurofeedback sessions or booster sessions
were performed. For neurofeedback, a medium SMD for
inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity were found post-
treatment, which changed to a large SMD for inattention and
remained a medium SMD for hyperactivity/impulsivity at
follow-up (relative to baseline). Non-active control groups
yielded a significant small effect size at pre-post that was
no longer significant at FU for inattention, and there were
no significant effects found for the hyperactivity/impulsivity
domain. Active controls had significant large effect sizes for
inattention and medium effect sizes for hyperactivity at both
pre-post and pre-FU. The between-group analysis was found
to significantly favor NF over non-active control groups for
both inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity at pre-post
(small effect sizes) and pre-FU (small to medium effect
sizes). Active controls were found to be significantly supe-
rior regarding inattention at pre-post but no longer at follow-
up. In summary, focusing on the pre-treatment to follow-up
results, neurofeedback was superior to non-active control
groups and similarly effective for inattention and hyper-
activity/impulsivity compared to active treatments. These
findings provide evidence that there are sustained clinical
benefits after neurofeedback and active treatments over an
average 6–12month follow-up period, whereas effects of
non-active control groups are no longer significant at FU.
The significant improvement in symptoms at FU for both
inattention and hyperactivity–impulsivity in the neurofeed-
back conditions indicates that NF results in lasting effects for
approximately 6months and potentially up to 1year. Com-
parison of effect-sizes between neurofeedback and active
control groups showed overlapping confidence intervals
(also visualized in Figure S-3), and no significant difference
in the between-group analysis, suggesting NF and active
controls having similar effects in the respective FU period;
however, this finding needs to be viewed with caution due
to the small number of studies in the active controls groups
(K=4). The tendency in the within-group analyses for a
small further improvement in the NF group (inattention:
Fig. 2 Forest Plot of within-group analysis for hyperactivity/
impulsivity parameter. Total standardized mean difference (SMD)
with 95% confidence interval, overall effect, and heterogeneity are
reported. Analysis of the control condition separately for non-active
and active controls was conducted for comparability to the inattention
parameter analysis. Pre-Post refers to the difference in means at pre-
and post-measurement, and similarly for pre-FU and post-FU
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
301European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
SMD=0.14; hyperactivity/impulsivity: SMD=0.11) from
post-treatment to FU, albeit not significant, is in line with the
further improvement effects seen after two-year follow-up
[6, 41]. This tendency was non-existent and moves primarily
in the opposite direction (SMD=− 0.1 to 0) for the active
and non-active controls (see Figure S-3). Accordingly, post-
FU results of the between-group analyses were slightly in
favor of NF (SMDs between 0.1 and 0.2, with a statistical
trend for hyperactivity/impulsivity) suggesting that effects
may become significant with further studies available. In any
case, current results do support the sustainability of the clin-
ical benefits of neurofeedback after cessation of treatment.
Regarding the use of medication, we only focused on
follow-up periods of 2–12months, and effects for active
treatments demonstrated sustained clinical benefit for these
periods (in which children were actively taking medication).
This is line with previous studies which demonstrated clini-
cal benefits of psychostimulant medication at 12months [42]
and 24months [3, 4]; however, the clinical benefits of psy-
chostimulant medication (when naturalistically assessed) are
not empirically supported for longer follow-up periods of
2–8years [43–45]. Despite this, recent epidemiological stud-
ies have found that continued controlled medication intake
can have positive benefits for patients with ADHD [46–48].
However, Swanson etal. [49] reported that at 12–16-year
follow-up of long-term medication use (both consistent and
inconsistent use over this time period) was not associated
with reduced symptom severity, but it was associated with
decreased adult height. These contrasting findings suggest
that while medication may have some long-term benefits,
the adherence of medication intake may be problematic and
long-term medication exposure may be related to potential
Fig. 3 Forest Plot of between-group analysis for inattention and
hyperactivity/impulsivity parameter. Total standardized mean differ-
ence (SMD) with 95% confidence interval, overall effect, and hetero-
geneity are reported. Analysis of the studies separately for non-active
and active controls was conducted for comparability to the inattention
parameter analysis. Pre-Post refers to the difference in means at pre-
and post-measurement, and similarly for pre-FU and post-FU
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
302 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
physical side effects. The follow-up periods used in our
study were probably not long enough to demonstrate the
decreased efficacy of medication as reported in the above
studies [43–45], and since these studies were controlled dur-
ing the FU periods and not naturalistic, it is likely that medi-
cation adherence was high. While NF follow-up treatment
effects have not been studied for such long-time intervals,
the short-term clinical effects of NF appear to be sustained
(without continued training) for an average 2–12month FU,
suggesting potential promise of this approach for sustained
clinical benefit in ADHD.
Interestingly, a strong point of our findings is that, despite
past heterogeneity in the application of neurofeedback pro-
tocols [5], most of the studies included in this paper used
‘standard NF protocols’, the exceptions being Arnold etal.
[25] and Bink etal. [26]. Additionally, Arnold etal. [25]
used an ‘entertaining’ NF protocol that may not be compat-
ible with principles of learning theory. However, the use of
uniform protocols by most of the papers is also evidenced by
the absence of significant heterogeneity. Considering only
standard protocols, we found that the results supporting NF
over non-active controls are slightly strengthened when only
including standard NF protocols. This finding supports the
continued use of these protocols for future NF studies. Addi-
tionally, our significant findings are in line with those of
Cortese etal. [8] who demonstrated significant between-
group effects even for ‘probably blinded’ ratings (teacher
ratings) at post-treatment when only standard NF protocols
for total ADHD and inattention symptoms were considered.
When considering placebo effects of NF training, they
may still operate at follow-up but we are not aware of ten-
dencies for further improvements of placebo effects for
other treatments of other disorders. NF treatment is found
to be superior to non-active controls in this analysis, and
the effects of non-active controls were not significant at FU,
neither for inattention nor hyperactivity/impulsivity. These
findings support the idea that NF does indeed have a differ-
ent, specific effect due to its actual training and not simply
due to the non-specific or placebo effects related to the set-
ting, the therapist–patient relationship or expectations. To
verify this more RCT’s using controls that closely mimic
NF training are required.
Possible limitations andopen questions
Results of the current meta-analysis should be interpreted in
line with its limitations.
While some of the studies included here do simultane-
ously use medication and NF (which may influence the
results), the number of participants taking medication did
not change for most studies, (only in two studies the NF
participants began taking medication between pre- and FU
measurement [25, 37]), suggesting that medication changes
are not an explanation for the effects found here. Addition-
ally, while dosage was allowed to be changed in five of these
studies, two NF groups decreased dosage and two increased
dosage while three control groups increased dosage. These
changes should not bias the data in favor of the NF, but
rather suggest that the NF effects may be slightly masked by
the dosage increase seen in the control groups.
When comparing the between-group effect sizes for pre-
to post-treatment between this study (Fig.3) and the latest
EAGG meta-analysis by Cortese etal. [8], a small effect
for inattention (SMD=0.36) and hyperactivity/impulsivity
(SMD=0.26) was found. In the current study, SMD’s are
lower when including all controls (inattention: SMD=0.09;
hyperactivity/impulsivity: SMD=0.16) and nearly identi-
cal when considering only non-active controls (inattention:
SMD=0.38; hyperactivity/impulsivity SMD=0.25). This
indicates that the studies we have included are representa-
tive and not biased towards more effective studies (opposite
file-drawer problem, i.e. higher likelihood that positive stud-
ies are more often published). But, while we attempted to
address the file drawer problem by assessing the fail-safe
numbers for our analysis, a potential reporting bias cannot be
definitively excluded. However, our finding of much larger
fail-safe numbers for NF (generally≥100) than for control
conditions (active<56, non-active<12) do suggest that
the NF condition results are probably not influenced by a
reporting bias.
Inherently when investigating FU periods, there are addi-
tional limitations involved including completer bias (a bias
is introduced because of the factors that cause a person to be
involved during a FU time-point) and the lack of an intention
to treat analysis (ITT) available for studies using a FU time
point. This type of analysis is considered more conservative
than the per protocol analyses found in the majority of the
papers included here. We chose specifically to not run a risk
of bias assessment because this model does not work well for
NF studies since it relies heavily on blinding, which poses a
problem since most NF studies were not blinded.
As already mentioned, more carefully designed RCTs
with longer follow-up time periods are needed before defi-
nite conclusions can be drawn. However, the meta-analysis
of Cortese etal. [8] on the acute effects of NF was comprised
of studies with a comparable design (“well-controlled”) and
about the same number of participants (ca. 500). Therefore,
this meta-analysis on follow-up effects at the present time
may also allow us to derive the first relevant conclusions
about the lasting effects of NF treatment.
Future research should focus on addressing both post- and
FU-effects of NF and other non-pharmacological treatments
for ADHD. Additionally, based on the current findings of
within-group effects, placebo or non-specific treatment
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
303European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
effects in NF cannot be ruled out and better controls for these
effects should continue to be investigated. Finally, it should
be noted that the specificity of neurofeedback effects cannot
only be derived from RCTs and a meta-analysis of RCTs
investigating behavioral outcome. Associations between the
behavioral and neurophysiological level (e.g., neuroregula-
tion skills) have already been documented with respect to the
post-treatment outcome [17, 50, 51], but are largely missing
from the literature that has conducted FU measurements.
These parameters provide an additional method to evaluate
treatment effects and should be included in future research
of long-term follow-ups.
Conclusion
Our meta-analytic results of NF treatment follow-up sug-
gest that there are sustained symptom reductions over time
in comparison with non-active control conditions. The
improvements seen here are comparable to active treat-
ments (including methylphenidate) at a short-term FU of
2–12months. As such, NF can be considered a non-phar-
macological treatment option for ADHD with evidence of
treatment effects that are sustained when treatment is com-
pleted and withdrawn. Future research should focus on the
comparison of standardized NF treatments with standard-
ized control treatments, controlling for unspecific effects
and changes in additional treatments (medication). Given
the need for additional treatments for ADHD with long-term
outcomes, clinical trials of NF should aim for primary out-
come measures that compare pre-treatment with systematic
long-term follow-up behavioral ratings, to address sustain-
ability of effects.
Acknowledgements The authors recognize Dr. Nezla Duric, Dr. Hanna
Christiansen, Dr. Naomi Steiner and Dr. L. Eugene Arnold for provid-
ing additional information that made inclusion of their data in this
meta-analysis possible.
Funding No funding was provided for this project.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest MA reports options from Brain Resource (Sydney,
Australia); is director and owner of Research Institute Brainclinics, a
minority shareholder in neuroCare Group (Munich, Germany), and a
co-inventor on 4 patent applications (A61B5/0402; US2007/0299323,
A1; WO2010/139361 A1; WO2017/099603 A1) related to EEG,
neuromodulation and psychophysiology, but does not own these nor
receives any proceeds related to these patents; Research Institute
Brainclinics received research funding from Brain Resource (Sydney,
Australia) and neuroCare Group (Munich, Germany), and equipment
support from Deymed, neuroConn and Magventure; however, data
analyses and writing of this manuscript were unconstrained. US has
been paid for public speaking by Novartis, Medice, neuroCare, the
German Society for Biofeedback, and Akademie König und Müller.
All other authors have indicated no potential conflicts of interest rel-
evant to this article to disclose.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Crea-
tive Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creat iveco
mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribu-
tion, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
References
1. Daly BP, Creed T, Xanthopoulos M, Brown RT (2007) Psycho-
social treatments for children with attention deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Neuropsychol Rev 17(1):73–89. https ://doi.org/10.1007/
s1106 5-006-9018-2
2. Faraone SV, Buitelaar J (2010) Comparing the efficacy of stimu-
lants for ADHD in children and adolescents using meta-analy-
sis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19(4):353–364. https ://doi.
org/10.1007/s0078 7-009-0054-3
3. MTA Cooperative Group (2014) National Institute of Men-
tal Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow-up:
24-month outcomes of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 113(4):754–761
4. Abikoff H, Hechtman L, Klein RG, Weiss G, Fleiss K, Etcovitch
J, Cousins L, Greenfield B, Martin D, Pollack S (2004) Sympto-
matic improvement in children with ADHD treated with long-
term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. J
Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 43(7):802–811. https ://doi.
org/10.1097/01.chi.00001 28791 .10014 .ac
5. Arns M, Heinrich H, Strehl U (2014) Evaluation of neurofeedback
in ADHD: the long and winding road. Biol Psychol 95:108–115.
https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops ycho.2013.11.013
6. Arns M, Kenemans JL (2014) Neurofeedback in ADHD and
insomnia: vigilance stabilization through sleep spindles and cir-
cadian networks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 44:183–194. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neubi orev.2012.10.006
7. Arns M, de Ridder S, Strehl U, Breteler M, Coenen A (2009)
Efficacy of neurofeedback treatment in ADHD: the effects on inat-
tention, impulsivity and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Clin EEG
Neurosci 40(3):180–189. https ://doi.org/10.1177/15500 59409
04000 311
8. Cortese S, Ferrin M, Brandeis D, Holtmann M, Aggensteiner
P, Daley D, Santosh P, Simonoff E, Stevenson J, Stringaris A,
Sonuga-Barke EJ (2016) Neurofeedback for attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsy-
chological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 55(6):444–455. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.03.007
9. Hodgson K, Hutchinson AD, Denson L (2014) Nonpharmacologi-
cal treatments for ADHD: a meta-analytic review. J Atten Disord
18(4):275–282. https ://doi.org/10.1177/10870 54712 44473 2
10. Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Geoffroy PA, Fond G, Lopez R, Biou-
lac S, Philip P (2014) EEG neurofeedback treatments in children
with ADHD: an updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled
trials. Front Hum Neurosci 8:906. https ://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum
.2014.00906
11. Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Cortese S, Daley D, Ferrin M,
Holtmann M, Stevenson J, Danckaerts M, van der Oord S, Dop-
fner M, Dittmann RW, Simonoff E, Zuddas A, Banaschewski
T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Hollis C, Konofal E, Lecendreux M,
Wong IC, Sergeant J (2013) Nonpharmacological interventions
for ADHD: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
304 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am
J Psychiatry 170(3):275–289. https ://doi.org/10.1176/appi.
ajp.2012.12070 991
12. Monastra VJ, Monastra DM, George S (2002) The effects of
stimulant therapy, EEG biofeedback, and parenting style on the
primary symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 27(4):231–249
13. Albrecht B, Brandeis D, Uebel H, Valko L, Heinrich H, Drechsler
R, Heise A, Muller UC, Steinhausen HC, Rothenberger A,
Banaschewski T (2013) Familiality of neural preparation and
response control in childhood attention deficit-hyperactivity dis-
order. Psychol Med 43(9):1997–2011. https ://doi.org/10.1017/
s0033 29171 20027 0x
14. Lubar JF, Shouse MN (1976) EEG and behavioral changes in
a hyperkinetic child concurrent with training of the sensorimo-
tor rhythm (SMR): a preliminary report. Biofeedback Self Regul
1(3):293–306
15. van Dongen-Boomsma M, Vollebregt MA, Slaats-Willemse
D, Buitelaar JK (2013) A randomized placebo-controlled trial
of electroencephalographic (EEG) neurofeedback in children
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry
74(8):821–827. https ://doi.org/10.4088/jcp.12m08 321
16. Bink M, van Nieuwenhuizen C, Popma A, Bongers IL, van Boxtel
GJ (2015) Behavioral effects of neurofeedback in adolescents with
ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychia-
try 24(9):1035–1048. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0078 7-014-0655-3
17. Janssen TW, Bink M, Gelade K, van Mourik R, Maras A, Oos-
terlaan J (2016) A randomized controlled trial investigating the
effects of neurofeedback, methylphenidate, and physical activity
on event-related potentials in children with attention-deficit/hyper-
activity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 26(4):344–
353. https ://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0144
18. Gevensleben H, Rothenberger A, Moll GH, Heinrich H (2012)
Neurofeedback in children with ADHD: validation and challenges.
Expert Rev Neurother 12(4):447–460. https ://doi.org/10.1586/
ern.12.22
19. Loo SK, Makeig S (2012) Clinical utility of EEG in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a research update. Neurotherapeu-
tics 9(3):569–587. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1331 1-012-0131-z
20. Ashar YK, Chang LJ, Wager TD (2017) Brain mechanisms of the
placebo effect: an affective appraisal account. Annu Rev Clin Psy-
chol 13(1):73–98. https ://doi.org/10.1146/annur ev-clinp sy-02181
5-09301 5
21. Sollie H, Larsson B, Morch WT (2013) Comparison of mother,
father, and teacher reports of ADHD core symptoms in a sample
of child psychiatric outpatients. J Atten Disord 17(8):699–710.
https ://doi.org/10.1177/10870 54711 43601 0
22. Willcutt EG, Hartung CM, Lahey BB, Loney J, Pelham WE
(1999) Utility of behavior ratings by examiners during assess-
ments of preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 27(6):463–472
23. Bralten J, Franke B, Waldman I, Rommelse N, Hartman C,
Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Ebstein RP, Gill M, Miranda A,
Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Sergeant JA, Oosterlaan
J, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Faraone SV, Buitelaar JK,
Arias-Vasquez A (2013) Candidate genetic pathways for attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) show association to hyper-
active/impulsive symptoms in children with ADHD. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 52(11):1204–1212.e1201. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.08.020
24. Wang L-J, Chen C-K, Huang Y-S (2013) Changes in behaviour
symptoms of patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
during treatment: observation from different informants. Psychia-
try Investig 10(1):1–7. https ://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2013.10.1.1
25. Arnold LE, Lofthouse N, Hersch S, Pan X, Hurt E, Bates B,
Kassouf K, Moone S, Grantier C (2013) EEG neurofeedback
for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: double-blind sham-
controlled randomized pilot feasibility trial. J Atten Disord
17(5):410–419. https ://doi.org/10.1177/10870 54712 44617 3
26. Bink M, Bongers IL, Popma A, Janssen TW, van Nieuwenhui-
zen C (2016) 1-year follow-up of neurofeedback treatment in
adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: ran-
domised controlled trial. BJPsych Open 2(2):107–115. https ://
doi.org/10.1192/bjpo.bp.115.00016 6
27. DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, Reid R (1998) ADHD
rating scale-IV: checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation.
Guilford, New York. https ://doi.org/10.1177/07342 82905 28579 2
28. Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN (1998) The
revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor struc-
ture, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol
26(4):257–268
29. Barkley RA (2014) Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a
handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Guilford, New York
30. Döpfner M, Lehmkuhl G (2003) Diagnostik-System für psy-
chische Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter nach ICD-10 und
DSM-IV (DISYPS-KJ). Praxis Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatrie
46:519–547
31. Swanson JM, Schuck S, Porter MM, Carlson C, Hartman CA, Ser-
geant JA, Clevenger W, Wasdell M, McCleary R, Lakes K, Wigal
T (2012) Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations
of symptoms of ADHD: history of the SNAP and the SWAN rat-
ing scales. Int J Educ Psychol Assess 10(1):51–70
32. Review Manager (2014) (RevMan) [Computer program]. Version
5.3. The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration,
Copenhagen
33. Rosenberg MS, Adams DC, Gurevitch J (2000) MetaWin: Statisti-
cal software for meta-analysis: version 2.0. Sinauer Associates,
Sunderland
34. Steiner NJ, Frenette EC, Rene KM, Brennan RT, Perrin EC (2014)
In-school neurofeedback training for ADHD: sustained improve-
ments from a randomized control trial. Pediatrics 133(3):483–492.
https ://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2059
35. Gelade K, Janssen TWP, Bink M, Twisk JWR, van Mourik R,
Maras A, Oosterlaan J (2017) A 6-month follow-up of an RCT
on behavioral and neurocognitive effects of neurofeedback in
children with ADHD. Child Adolesc Psychiatry, Eur. https ://doi.
org/10.1007/s0078 7-017-1072-1
36. Heinrich H, Gevensleben H, Freisleder FJ, Moll GH, Rothen-
berger A (2004) Training of slow cortical potentials in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence for positive behavioral and
neurophysiological effects. Biol Psychiatry 55(7):772–775. https
://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops ych.2003.11.013
37. Meisel V, Servera M, Garcia-Banda G, Cardo E, Moreno I (2013)
Neurofeedback and standard pharmacological intervention in
ADHD: a randomized controlled trial with six-month follow-
up. Biol Psychol 94(1):12–21. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops
ycho.2013.04.015
38. Duric NS, Assmus J, Gundersen D, Duric Golos A, Elgen IB
(2017) Multimodal treatment in children and adolescents with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a 6-month follow-up.
Nord J Psychiatry. https ://doi.org/10.1080/08039 488.2017.13054
46
39. Li L, Yang L, Zhuo CJ, Wang YF (2013) A randomised controlled
trial of combined EEG feedback and methylphenidate therapy for
the treatment of ADHD. Swiss Med Wkly 143:w13838. https ://
doi.org/10.4414/smw.2013.13838
40. Christiansen H, Reh V, Schmidt MH, Rief W (2014) Slow cor-
tical potential neurofeedback and self-management training in
outpatient care for children with ADHD: study protocol and first
preliminary results of a randomized controlled trial. Front Hum
Neurosci. 8:943. https ://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum .2014.00943
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
305European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (2019) 28:293–305
1 3
41. Gani C, Birbaumer N, Strehl U (2008) Long term effects after
feedback of slow cortical potentials and of theta-beta-amplitudes
in children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Int J Bioelectromagn. 10(4):209–232
42. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Wigal T, Kollins SH, Newcorn JH, Tel-
ang F, Logan J, Jayne M, Wong CT, Han H, Fowler JS, Zhu W,
Swanson JM (2013) Long-term stimulant treatment affects brain
dopamine transporter level in patients with attention deficit hyper-
active disorder. PLoS One 8(5):e63023. https ://doi.org/10.1371/
journ al.pone.00630 23
43. Molina BS, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B,
Jensen PS, Epstein JN, Hoza B, Hechtman L, Abikoff HB, Elliott
GR, Greenhill LL, Newcorn JH, Wells KC, Wigal T, Gibbons RD,
Hur K, Houck PR (2009) The MTA at 8years: prospective follow-
up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite
study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48(5):484–500. https
://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013 e3181 9c23d 0
44. Riddle MA, Yershova K, Lazzaretto D, Paykina N, Yenokyan
G, Greenhill L, Abikoff H, Vitiello B, Wigal T, McCracken JT,
Kollins SH, Murray DW, Wigal S, Kastelic E, McGough JJ, dos-
Reis S, Bauzo-Rosario A, Stehli A, Posner K (2013) The pre-
school attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder treatment study
(PATS) 6-year follow-up. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
52(3):264–278.e262. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.007
45. van Lieshout M, Luman M, Twisk JWR, van Ewijk H, Groenman
AP, Thissen AJAM, Faraone SV, Heslenfeld DJ, Hartman CA,
Hoekstra PJ, Franke B, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NNJ, Oosterlaan
J (2016) A 6-year follow-up of a large European cohort of children
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined subtype:
outcomes in late adolescence and young adulthood. Eur Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 25:1007–1017. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0078
7-016-0820-y
46. Lu Y, Sjolander A, Cederlof M, D’Onofrio BM, Almqvist C, Lars-
son H, Lichtenstein P (2017) Association between medication use
and performance on higher education entrance tests in individu-
als with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA psychiatry
74(8):815–822. https ://doi.org/10.1001/jamap sychi atry.2017.1472
47. Chang Z, D’Onofrio BM, Quinn PD, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H
(2016) Medication for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
and risk for depression: a nationwide longitudinal cohort study.
Biol Psychiatry 80(12):916–922. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops
ych.2016.02.018
48. Chang Z, Quinn PD, Hur K, Gibbons RD, Sjolander A, Lars-
son H, D’Onofrio BM (2017) Association between medication
use for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and risk of motor
vehicle crashes. JAMA Psychiatry 74(6):597–603. https ://doi.
org/10.1001/jamap sychi atry.2017.0659
49. Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Molina BSG, Sibley MH, Hechtman
LT, Hinshaw SP, Abikoff HB, Stehli A, Owens EB, Mitchell JT,
Nichols Q, Howard A, Greenhill LL, Hoza B, Newcorn JH, Jensen
PS, Vitiello B, Wigal T, Epstein JN, Tamm L, Lakes KD, Wax-
monsky J, Lerner M, Etcovitch J, Murray DW, Muenke M, Acosta
MT, Arcos-Burgos M, Pelham WE, Kraemer HC (2017) Young
adult outcomes in the follow-up of the multimodal treatment study
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: symptom persistence,
source discrepancy, and height suppression. J Child Psychol Psy-
chiatry 58(6):663–678. https ://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12684
50. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Schlamp D, Kratz O, Studer
P, Wangler S, Rothenberger A, Moll GH, Heinrich H (2009) Dis-
tinct EEG effects related to neurofeedback training in children
with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Psychophysiol
74(2):149–157. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsy cho.2009.08.005
51. Strehl U, Leins U, Goth G, Klinger C, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer
N (2006) Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: a new
treatment for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
order. Pediatrics 118(5):e1530–1540. https ://doi.org/10.1542/
peds.2005-2478
52. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Schlamp D, Kratz O, Studer
P, Rothenberger A, Moll GH, Heinrich H (2010) Neurofeedback
training in children with ADHD: 6-month follow-up of a ran-
domised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19(9):715–
724. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0078 7-010-0109-5
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.