ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Using a single-subject multiple-baseline design, the study examined the efficacy of a flow training program on flow states and performance of four elite level rock-climbers. Having received the intervention that comprised education, goal-setting, self-talk, and mindfulness stages, flow intensity, as measured by the Flow State Scale (FFS-2) increased. In addition, objective performance (time) and self-rated scores improved with, at least, medium effect sizes. Social validation indicated that participants found the training to be rewarding, and in line with the study findings. Such results propose further empirical research on flow training on elite athletes be undertaken to assess the intensity of flow experiences and performance scores. Future research examining retention data and education only interventions are advised. Implications arising from the present data are discussed.
Flow Training, Flow States, and Performance in Elite
Athletes
CAMERON NORSWORTHY*, RICHARD THELWELL*, NEIL WESTON*,
SUSAN A. JACKSON**
(*) Department of Sport & Exercise Science, Spinnaker Building, Cambridge Road, Portsmouth,
UK University of Portsmouth, UK
(**) PO Box 240, Kenmore, QLD Australia
Using a single-subject multiple-baseline design, the study examined the effi-
cacy of a flow training program on flow states and performance of four elite level
rock-climbers. Having received the intervention that comprised education, goal-set-
ting, self-talk, and mindfulness stages, flow intensity, as measured by the Flow
State Scale (FFS-2) increased. In addition, objective performance (time) and self-
rated scores improved with, at least, medium effect sizes. Social validation indicated
that participants found the training to be rewarding, and in line with the study find-
ings. Such results propose further empirical research on flow training on elite ath-
letes be undertaken to assess the intensity of flow experiences and performance
scores. Future research examining retention data and education only interventions
are advised. Implications arising from the present data are discussed.
KEY WORDS: Athletes, Climbing, Intervention, Multiple-baseline training.
Introduction
Positive psychology has increasingly been used in performance arenas
(Swann, Keegan, Piggott, & Crust, 2012), focusing on using an individual’s
strength to increase performance and enjoyment. The flow state has emerged
as a dominant theme in positive psychology (Seligman, 2012) producing a
wealth of qualitative and anecdotal evidence in sport psychology that sug-
gests flow is an optimal psychological state that underpins an athlete’s most
memorable experiences (Jackson, 1996). Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) concep-
tual theory of flow proposes nine components to a flow experience: 1) Chal-
Corresponding Author: Cameron Northworthy, Department of Sport & Exercise Science,
Spinnaker Building, Cambridge Road, Portsmouth, PO1 2ER United Kingdom (e-mail:
Cameron.norsworthy1@myport.ac.uk)
Int. J. Sport Psychol., 2017; 48: 1-00
doi: 10.7352/IJSP 2017.48.000
lenge-skills balance; 2) Merging of action and awareness; 3) Clear goals; 4)
Unambiguous feedback; 5) Concentration on the task at hand; 6) Sense of
control; 7) Loss of self-consciousness; 8) Transformation of time; and 9)
Autotelic experience (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). The flow model
has proven robust across lines of culture, class, gender and sport (Nakamura
& Csikszentmihalyi, 2002). This state differs from an alternative emerging
peak performance state, the clutch state, in which individuals perform under
pressure through similar dimensional experiences but differentiates itself
from flow through the deliberate concentration, intense effort, and height-
ened awareness described by participants (Swann, Crust, Jackman, Vella,
Allen, & Keegan, 2017).
Due to the association between flow and peak performance (Jackson &
Roberts, 1992) and mental toughness (Crust & Swann, 2013), understanding
and attaining flow is of great interest practically to athletes (Pain, Harwood,
& Anderson, 2011). Although flow has been both positively correlated with
performance in tennis players (Flett, 2015), and linked to increases in flow
with performance (e.g., Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2014; Pates, Oliver,
& Maynard, 2001), the flow state is a distinct concept from the outcomes of
peak performance (i.e. winning) and the emotions of peak experiences
(Harmison, 2011). For example, one can perform well but not be in an opti-
mal state of flow (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). Whilst performance
may be expected to increase during flow due to common descriptors, such as
high levels of concentration, clear goals, unambiguous feedback, and a sense
of control (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999), applied performance strate-
gies do not always promote flow descriptors such as loss of self-conscious-
ness, action and awareness merging, transformation of time and autotelic
experience (e.g., Peters, 2013). Thus, indicating what constitutes flow train-
ing may be similar but not identical to training for peak performance or
clutch states. For example, peak performance training is often focused
towards performance goals and achieving outcomes (Filby, Maynard, &
Graydon, 1999), whereas flow training may be more focused towards elicit-
ing intrinsic motivation or flow antecedents (Jackson, Thomas, Marsh, &
Smethurst, 2001).
A multitude of literature exists examining performance enhancement
strategies (Weinberg & Gould, 2014), however, other than Swann et al.’s
(2012) systematic review on flow state research in elite athletes, little infor-
mation collating research surrounding how flow experiences can be manipu-
lated and enhanced is available. Additionally, Swann et al. (2012) found that
the majority of athletes reported flow to be controllable (72%) and restor-
able (81%). Common psychological interventions such as arousal regulation,
2C. Norsworthy et al.
goal setting, self-talk, and imagery on flow have reported mixed results in
enhancing flow states (Nicholls, Polman & Holt; 2005; Pain, Harwood &
Anderson, 2011; Pates, Cowen & Karageorghis, 2012; Koehn, Morris &
Watt, 2014), whilst interventions using hypnosis on cyclists (Lindsay, May-
nard & Thomas, 2005) and golfers (Pates, 2013), and imagery combined with
music on soccer players (Pain et al., 2011), have reported positive results.
However, all these studies were pre-experimental designs not sampling large-
scale randomized controlled trials. Contrarily, mindfulness interventions
(e.g., Bell, 2015; Schwanhausser, 2009) have had perhaps the most influential
training on flow to date, with Aherne, Moran, and Lonsdale (2011) finding a
significant interaction with global scores of flow intensity across a variety of
sports. Recommendations to date, for future research examining flow inter-
ventions, include the use of multi-faceted approaches to match skills to flow
dimension (Swann et al., 2012), and concurrent methods of cognitive
restructuring to ensure the training is both well understood and able to be
applied (Lindsay et al., 2005). Additionally, Lindsay, Maynard, and Thomas
(2005) suggested that future research use multiple control groups in order to
differentiate training towards flow and performance. To the date of publica-
tion, only two studies have examined a multi-skilled approach to flow (Judge,
Bell, Bellar, & Wanless, 2010; Reardon & Gordin, 1999), though, both were
descriptive and did not test participants. This limited research is surprising
considering the mindset accompanying flow pushes athletes to their maxi-
mum performance limits (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). Furthermore,
Williams and Krane (1998) suggest that a psychological profile congruent
with the dimensions of flow correlated with flow, and an autotelic personal-
ity is more amenable to finding flow (Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2014).
Therefore, this study aims to develop a multi-faceted flow intervention and
assess its impact on flow states and performance in elite athletes.
To increase intervention effectiveness and pre-establish potential influ-
encing factors (Swann et al., 2012), the study’s evidence base for intervention
selection and their associated dimensions of flow will now be described.
Goal orientation (Aubé, Brunelle, & Rousseau, 2014) and a process-orien-
tated focus (Jackson & Roberts, 1992) have been stated as important psy-
chological factors to attaining flow. Therefore, the multi-facets of flow train-
ing will directly target the flow dimensions and the process of being in flow
as the primary goal, over and above performance outcomes. Csikszentmiha-
lyi’s (1997) has stated that consciously being aware of flow during the activ-
ity is counterproductive and inhibits one’s ability to merge action and aware-
ness. Thus it is important that the training focuses on one’s intention to find
flow before the activity and not one’s necessity to consciously think about
Flow training 3
finding flow continuously during the performance. In order to meet Lindsay
et al.’s (2005) recommendation of concurrent methods of cognitive restruc-
turing and Holliday, Burton, Sun, Hammermeister, Naylor, and Freigang’s
(2008) proposal that education is an integral part of mental skills training and
necessary for participants to cognitively understand flow, an education on
flow and dimensional importance will initiate. After which, to adhere to
Nakamura and Csikszentmihalyi’s (2014) conditions of flow (i.e. clear goals
and an adequate challenge that matches perceived skill) participants’ goals
will be examined to integrate flow as the primary goal to their performances.
To ensure a focus towards finding flow is continued during the performance
and facilitates the dimension ‘concentration on task at hand’ as outlined by
Judge et al. (2010), participants’ self-talk during the performance will be
examined and redirected towards finding flow and concentrating on the task
at hand. .Mindfulness interventions have been reported to strongly affect
dimensions ‘concentration’, ‘clear goals’, ‘sense of control’, ‘unambiguous
feedback’, and ‘loss of self-consciousness’ (Aherne et al., 2011; Cathcart et
al., 2014); the explanation being that through increased awareness, we learn
to regulate attentional processes towards more relevant information for the
task at hand (Gardner & Moore, 2007), thus positively impacting one’s deci-
sion-making and perception of the integral ‘challenge-skill balance’ enabling
a higher degree of self-efficacy (Pineau, Glass, Kaufman, & Bernal, 2014).
Therefore, a mindfulness intervention aimed at re-directing attention when
distracting thoughts occur towards finding flow will be deployed. Lastly, to
adhere to Pain et al.’s (2011) recommendations regarding flow interventions,
objective as well as subjective performance measures will be assessed.
In summary, the main purpose of this study is to examine the effects of a
multi-faceted flow training program on flow states and objective and subjec-
tive performance scores in elite athletes. Further, as this study is breaking
new ground by applying a flow training program on participants for the first
time, it will attempt to develop the knowledge and understanding of practi-
cally developing and applying a flow training program.
Method
PARTICIPANTS
Four elite rock-climbers (age range = 38-44 years; mean = 41 years), based within the
United Kingdom took part in the study. Elite status was contextualised by climbers adhering
to Swann, Moran, and Piggott’s (2015) designation of elite athletes and achieving the grade of
6b or above in the British sport climbing grading system.
4C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 5
DEPENDENT VARIABLES
The three dependent variables were flow intensity, self-rated performance, and timed per-
formance. Global and dimensional flow intensity was measured using the Flow State Scale-2
(FFS-2) (Jackson & Eklund, 2002). The FFS-2 measures the intensity of flow experienced
through a 36-item questionnaire self-scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = ‘strongly
disagree’ to 5 = ‘strongly agree’ targeting the nine components of flow (Jackson & Csikszent-
mihalyi, 1999). The FFS-2 has produced acceptable reliability and internal consistency (alphas
0.80) scores (Jackson & Eklund, 2002). Participants scoring above a 3.4 score were deemed in
flow, as suggested by Kawabata and Mallett’s (2016) series of latent class factor analyses. Sub-
jective performance scores were self-scored on a 20-point scale ranging from 1 = ‘terrible’ to 20
= ‘outstanding’. Objective performance scores were assessed using the climb time.
RESEARCH DESIGN
To examine the effects of a multi-skilled flow intervention on flow states and perfor-
mance, a single-subject multiple baseline design (Martin & Pear, 2003; Thelwell & Greenlees,
2003) was adopted. The study had multiple interventions and no reversal, as adopted by Pain
et al. (2011). The participants acted as their own control, and testing took place over a nine-
week period. Since three dependent variables were employed, with flow scores consisting of
nine further dimensions, the team made an ‘a priori’ decision to introduce the first interven-
tion after four data points rather than introducing the interventions after a stable baseline or
scores moving in an opposite direction (Kazdin, 1992). Deciding on a ‘primary’ dependent
variable to determine intervention timing would undermine the other variables and flow
dimensional importance, and was therefore deemed inappropriate (Thelwell, Greenlees, &
Weston, 2006). Interventions followed after every two data points, except intervention 1 (edu-
cation) which was given extra data points for retention testing as the effects of flow education
had not been reported to date. Additionally, two extra data points were undertaken at the end
of the study to examine retention scores.
FLOW TRAINING PROGRAM
A training program including an education, goal-setting, self-talk, and mindfulness inter-
vention to facilitate flow was delivered to each participant individually by the researcher and
overseen by two Health Care and Professions Council (HCPC) registered Sport and Exercise
Psychologists.
The education on flow was delivered in three pre-recorded video presentations, totaling
two hours in length. The video content included descriptions of Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990)
nine flow dimension definitions, Jackson’s (1996) applied dimensions to flow, summaries of
flow research, anecdotal evidence describing the experience of elite athletes in flow, scenario
based examples where an athlete might choose a higher order goal towards flow rather than
performance focused outcomes. The videos were delivered in a lecture format. Within two
days of viewing each of the videos twice, a debrief session was facilitated through online video
software to facilitate questions on flow only, and test whether learning was retained.
The goal-setting intervention to adopt flow as the primary goal was delivered during a
one-to-one session lasting approximately forty minutes over online video software. The con-
tent of the goals was participant-led to ensure autonomy (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Deci &
Ryan, 2000), whilst the goal-setting structure was pre-determined starting with a higher order
goal (flow) that generated multiple process goals (Grant, 2016) aligned with prioritising flow.
The researcher facilitated the session to ensure goals were congruent with other areas of their
lives (Wesson & Boniwell, 2007) and then integrated into their positive pre-performance rit-
ual (Jackson, 1992).
Self-talk was examined to identify negative self-talk and reconstruct functional short
phrases (Ellis, 1999) from self-generated positive keywords (Jackson, 1992) targeted towards
prioritizing flow. Scenarios were then addressed with the client for practical application,
whilst providing an opportunity to fine-tune appropriate self-talk (Thelwell, et al., 2006).
Lastly, participants were asked to listen to three 20-minute mindfulness recordings.
Adherence was managed through text and email communication. The recordings were
scripted on the ‘breath, body and mountain’, ‘body scan’ and ‘standing yoga’ work of Kabat-
Zinn (Teasdale & Segal, 2007) that both Gardner and Moore (2004) and Aherne et al. (2011)
successfully employed during their flow studies. Script adaptation included flow descriptions
from the education on flow and directing the participants’ attention towards flow.
PROCEDURES
Prior to the recruitment of participants, institutional ethical approval was formally
received from the institution of the lead author. Volunteer participants were recruited through
the emailing of various rock-climbing clubs within the UK. All engaged participants claimed
to have minimal prior knowledge on flow when asked to share their existing knowledge of
flow during the recruitment process. One participant was excluded from the study before
commencing as their level of knowledge on flow already included dimensional clarification
and had claimed to read two books on flow. All participants volunteered for the study, signed
informed consent forms prior to commencement, and were informed that all data would
remain confidential. Indoor rock climbing was deemed an appropriate activity as influencing
external factors such as environmental conditions could be controlled, performances could be
easily replicated, and assessments could be immediately conducted post-performance. Inter-
ested participants were sent a participant information sheet and consent form via email, then
followed up via video software to answer questions and ensure participants understood the
study’s practicalities. Participants chose an indoor climbing route that was self-assessed as
challenging but possible, and practiced the route for a week to minimise effects such as famil-
iarity or technical improvements. After which, twice a week, the participant performed a
timed ascent after warming-up. Where possible, participants kept the same belayer (assistant)
who was responsible for measuring time with a stopwatch. Flow and performance scales were
completed independently by the participant immediately after route completion for best prac-
tice (Jackson & Eklund, 2002). Climb time was revealed to the participant by the belayer after
questionnaire completion to ensure objective performance scores did not affect flow scores or
subjective performance scores (Koehn, Morris, & Watt, 2014). Completed scales and forms
were scanned and emailed on the day to the researcher, bar a few exceptions due to limited
internet access. Contact with participants during the study was kept to a minimum and inter-
ventions were delivered after the same number of climbs, as opposed to a fixed time frame to
6C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 7
increase procedural reliability (see Figure 1 for sequencing). Each participant was debriefed
via video software after social validation procedures.
SOCIAL VALIDATION
Social validation questions were collected to assess reactions to treatment procedures
and experimental outcomes (Pates, Maynard, & Westbury, 2001). Questions included: “Do
you feel the training increased your flow experiences (intensity)? Do you feel the training
improved your ability to find flow (frequency)? Do you feel the training increased your
flow experiences (intensity)? Overall how helpful was the training? Do you feel the train-
ing was professional?” Additionally, during the post study debrief informal discussions
were held between the participants and lead researcher to elicit any additional effects of
the training.
Fig. 1. - Flow state scores for participant 1-4. Each vertical line indicates an inter-
vention. M = total mean score for that period.
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
180
200
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Global Flow Scores
Climbs
Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4
pp
M= 118.12
M= 135.58
M= 147.75
M= 153.12
M= 164
Education
Goal - Setting Self -talk Mindfulness
DATA ANALYSIS
Visual inspection recommendations by Martin and Pear (2003) and Hrycaiko and Mar-
tin (1996) were followed to determine the confidence of the training effect on the dependent
variables. Confidence was enhanced: (a) when baseline performance is stable or in a direction
opposite to that predicted for the treatment, (b) the greater the number of times that an effect
is replicated both within and across subjects; (c) the fewer the number of overlapping data
points between baseline and treatment phase; (d) the sooner the effect occurs following the
introduction of treatment; and, (e) the larger the size of the effect in comparison with baseline
(Pain et al., 2011). Following, effect size calculation (d) of intervention effect (mean baseline
scores minus mean intervention scores, divided by standard deviation of data in baseline) was
conducted, in which < .87 = small effect, .87 to 2.67 = medium effect, and > 2.67 = large effect
(Parker & Vannest, 2009).
Results
FLOW ANALYSIS
Flow scores for the study period are presented in Figure 1 and Table I.
Participant 1 increased mean flow scores by 23% (d= 3.42) post-interven-
tion with four overlapping data points; participant 2 by 47% (d= 6.56) with
one overlapping data point that may have been affected by a reported illness;
participant 3 by 15% (d= 1.3) with four overlapping data points; and partic-
ipant 4 by 29% (d= 1.3) with three overlapping data points. All participants
showed a stable baseline and an immediate improvement after intervention 1
and mean increases on interventions 1 (M = 21.52%), 2 (M = 0.97%) after, 3
(M = 2.95%) after and 4 (M = 3.26%). Across participants mean global flow
scores increased by 28.5%.
Flow dimensional analysis revealed increases for all dimensions post-
training, except for ‘transformation of time’ from participant 2 (Table II).
Specific interventions did not always increase associated dimensional scores
as expected. Interventions resulted in an escalating positive effect on flow
scores (Table III).
8C. Norsworthy et al.
TABLE I
Mean Effect Sizes (d) of Flow State Scores Post-Intervention
Global Flow Participants
12 3 4Total Mean
Effect Size 1.30 6.56 1.30 3.42 3.14
Medium Large Medium Large Large
Flow training 9
It is worth noting that all participants met Kawabata and Mallett’s (2016)
proposal that the scores of the three proximal dimensions (challenge-skills
balance, clear goals, unambiguous feedback) have to be above 3.4 to identify
individuals as experiencing flow.
TIMED PERFORMANCE ANALYSIS
The impact of the intervention on timed performance is shown in Figure
2, and Table IV
. Participant 1 decreased mean climb time by 47% (d= -1.19)
TABLE III
Individual Intervention Mean Increases between Baseline and Intervention Data
Interventions
Education Goal-Setting Self-Talk Mindfulness
%d%d%d%d
15% 0.86 26% 1.67 31% 2.12 75% 5.07
Small Med Med Large
% = % difference from baseline data; d = effect size difference from baseline data
TABLE II
Mean Effect Sizes (d) of Flow State Dimensional Scores Post-Intervention
Flow Dimensions Participants
12 3 4Total Effect Mean
Skill-Challenge 0.58 2.02 0.75 1.99 1.34
Small Medium Small Medium Medium
Action/Awareness 0.70 6.60 1.93 5.92 3.79
Small Large Medium Large Large
Clear Goals 1.09 2.74 0.36 5.77 2.49
Medium Large Small Large Medium
Unambiguous Feedback 9.58 2.00 1.19 5.51 4.57
Large Medium Medium Large Large
Concentration 3.63 18.63 3.27 12.31 9.46
Large Large Large Large Large
Control 1.53 5.64 1.53 1.64 2.59
Medium Large Medium Medium Medium
Self-Consciousness 1.61 2.36 -1.67 0.64 0.73
Medium Medium Medium Small Small
Transformation of Time 0.62 -0.01 24.38 7.11 8.02
Small None Large Large Large
Autotelic 1.48 0.86 1.77 0.85 1.24
Medium Small Medium Small Medium
post-intervention with no overlapping data points; participant 2 by 57% (d=
-2.05) with one overlapping data point that may have been affected by a
reported illness; participant 3 by 69% (d= -2.02) with one overlapping data
point; and participant 4 by 74% (d= -3.42) with no overlapping data points.
Participant 1 and 2 showed stable baselines, whilst 3 and 4 were relatively
stable. All participants showed an immediate improvement after intervention
10 C. Norsworthy et al.
Fig. 2. - Timed scores for participant 1-4. Each vertical line indicates an intervention.
M = mean score for that period.
!
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Climb Time
Climb
M=6.52
M=4.84
M=4.48
M=3.95
M=3.36
Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4
pp
TABLE IV
Mean Effect Sizes Of Timed Performances Post-Intervention
Time Performance Participants
12 3 4Total Mean
Effect Size -1.19 -2.05 -2.02 -3.42 -2.17
Medium Medium Medium Large Medium
Flow training 11
1 and mean increases on interventions 1 (M = 16.83%), 2 (M = 3.35%) after,
3 (M = 12.9%) after and 4 (M = 9.03%). Across participants mean timed per-
formance scores increased by 61.8%.
SELF-RATING PERFORMANCE ANALYSIS
The impact of the intervention on self-rating performance are shown in
Figure 3, and Table V. Participant 1 increased mean self-rated performance
scores by 9% (d= 0.67) post-intervention with eight overlapping data points;
participant 2 by 53% (d= 2.57) with one overlapping data point that may
Fig. 3. - Self-rated performance scores for participant 1. Each vertical line indicates
an intervention. M = total mean score.
0
5
10
15
20
25
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Self-rated Performance
Climb
M=12.87 M=15.5 M=16.5 M=17.3 7 M=17.9
Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4
pp
TABLE V
Mean Effect Sizes Of Self-Rating Performances Post-Intervention
Self-Rating Performance Participants
1234Total Mean
Effect Size 0.67 2.57 1.20 2.14 1.65
Small Large Medium Medium Medium
have been affected by a reported illness; participant 3 by 24% (d= 1.2) with
four overlapping data points; and Participant 4 by 50% (d= 2.14) with two
overlapping data points. Participant 1 and 2 showed stable baselines, whilst
3 and 4 were relatively stable. All participants showed an immediate
improvement after intervention 1 and mean increases on interventions 1 (M
= 43.05%), 2 (M = 0.59%) after, 3 (M = 4.69%) after and 4 (M = 2.3%).
Across participants mean self-rating performance scores increased by 34%.
SOCIAL VALIDATION
Social validation was assessed via brief questionnaires on completion of
the study. All participants rated the study to be professional, helpful and
improved their performance and ability to experience flow, suggesting trust-
worthiness for the study’s findings, and efficacy of the intervention. Each par-
ticipant stated that they consistently adhered to the procedures. Additional
debrief findings included increased motivation, reduced anxiety and an
excitement to apply finding flow under different conditions and in different
activities. E.g. participant 1 stated “I didn’t feel so anxious during the per-
formance…I am excited to try this out in other areas of my life.”
Discussion
The aim of this study was to develop a multi-faceted intervention, specif-
ically designed towards adopting flow as the primary goal, and then assess its
impact on flow states and performance in elite athletes. The findings revealed
that all participants increased flow and performance scores by at least
medium effect sizes, except participant 1 whose self-rating performance
increased by a small effect size. Confidence in the findings was acceptable
using Martin and Pear’s (2003) visual recommendations and Pain et al.’s
(2011) adoption of effect size analysis on SS designs. Thus suggesting a multi-
faceted flow training program should be explored further in future research.
The findings are in line with those reported by Flett (2015) and Jackson and
Csikszentmihalyi (1999) who reported performance to increase with flow
scores. However, although both positive increases were reported for all vari-
ables the magnitude of variance potentially supports Harmison’s (2011)
notion that flow and peak performance are distinct states. Interestingly,
although the training was focused towards increasing flow states over and
above performance, objective performance scores were substantially higher
12 C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 13
than flow increases. A possible explanation may derive from the participant’s
comments regarding reduced anxiety and increased motivation, which may
be linked to the dimensional increases of ‘action and awareness merging’,
‘loss of self-consciousness’ and ‘autotelic experience’ that may not be ordi-
narily associated with performance training and potentially separates flow
training from performance training. However, as all the flow dimensions
increased, this report adds feasibility to the effects of flow training and
echoes Lindsay, Maynard, and Thomas’s (2005) suggestion of deploying
larger sample sizes and multiple control groups to develop more robust
future studies in this domain.
Interestingly, self-rating performance (subjective) and timed perfor-
mance (objective) scores increased at varying rates, consequently resulting in
varying relationships with flow. Closer analysis revealed that a plateauing
effect for self-ratings resulted in only marginal increases appearing towards
the later stages when subjects neared their ceiling, even when performance
times improved. Thus, the participant’s self-perception of performance levels
may have changed throughout the study and may explain the differentiation
between subjective and objective performance scores. Any conclusion of the
relationship between flow and performance is out of the scope of this study,
however, this study does urge researchers looking to further this field (see
Flett, 2015) that it would be advisable to distinguish between subjective and
objective performance measures.
Unexpectedly, specific dimensions associated with specific interven-
tions, such as ‘clear goals’ and goal-setting’ (see introduction), did not
always increase directly after the associated intervention was employed.
However, global flow scores did increase for each intervention, thus suggest-
ing improvements in flow were more likely related to the adoption of flow as
a primary goal rather than the intervention methodology (e.g. generic goal
setting or mindfulness interventions) that may have otherwise skewed related
flow dimensional scores. Flow dimensional analysis was also inconsistent
with previous research suggesting that dimensions ‘loss of self-consciousness’
and ‘transformation of time’ ordinarily report smaller variations (Jackson &
Marsh, 1996).
The findings, combined with the participant’s social validation feedback,
support the notion that the mindset accompanying flow pushes athletes to
their maximum performance limits (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999) and
flow states may be controllable (Swann et al., 2012). Additionally, this study
has added data to the void of cognitive restructuring (Lindsay et al., 2005)
and multi-skilled intervention research on flow (Swann et al., 2012). Fur-
thermore, since flow is a state of consciousness (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi,
1999), a process that occurs during an activity, the positive results support
wider literature that associate both flow (Jackson & Roberts, 1992) and high
performance (Weinberg & Butt, 2014) states with a process-orientated focus
as opposed to an outcome-orientated focus.
This study was designed to examine the collective effects of a multi-
skilled program, however, individual intervention analysis revealed an esca-
lating positive mean increase for interventions. Also, each intervention across
all participants in global flow scores (range = 15-75%). Therefore, it is rec-
ommended that future research compare the effects of single-skilled flow
worthy of note is that all participants, except participant 3 for timed perfor-
mance, increased their mean scores for all dependent variables immediately
after the educational intervention, then decreased scores on the next data
point and subsequently increased scores to the initial level or higher. Conse-
quently, it is recommended future research investigate the effects of educa-
tion only interventions to further understand this pattern and assess whether
flow education alone has a positive impact practically on flow and perfor-
mance within athletes and other performance arenas. Assessing education
only interventions more rigorously would help to determine whether the pos-
itive effects where due to cognitive restructuring or a participant’s inclination
to score differently on the flow scales after being exposed to them. Indeed,
regarding the practical application of sport psychology, disseminating an
educational program may prove far more efficient both financially and in
terms of time management than the continual involvement of sport psychol-
ogists; although customized support will always be applicable in certain cir-
cumstances.
Since the study directed each intervention towards finding flow and col-
lectively adopted a multi-skilled approach, it is difficult to conclusively sup-
port previous research that found positive effects on individual skills, such as
Wessen and Boniwell’s (2007) congruent goal-setting and Aherne et al.’s
(2011) mindfulness intervention. Therefore, in order to further develop flow
training that is effective and practically applicable to athletes, it is advised
future research compare the flow training employed in this study to inter-
ventions that have previously reported positive results (see Swann et al.,
2012).
A weakness of this study is the use of limited data points during baseline
and between interventions. Future research may address this by reducing the
number of dependent variables and interventions or increasing the study
duration, thus creating capacity for further data points. Another weakness is
14 C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 15
the use of retrospective self-reports to measure flow as previously questioned
by Jackson (2000), however, no non-retrospective alternative exists to date.
Additionally, as the researcher was not present during the moment of data
collection a level of trust towards procedural compliance was employed,
which could be scrutinised; measures for procedural clarity such as clear
briefs, text reminders and requests for participant assurances were deployed,
thus participants were deemed to have abided by the procedures. A further
weakness was evident from the social validation procedures when participant
3 stated that in hindsight the chosen climb could have been more challenging
and thus limited his intensity of flow. Participant 3 reported the lowest
increase in flow scores (15%), therefore, it is recommended future research
confirm whether the activity is sufficiently challenging after initial practice.
A limitation of this study is the transferability of findings to other
domains. Indoor rock-climbing was chosen to reduce the effect of external
variables, and although ecological to indoor rock climbers, participant 2, 3
and 4 stated their excitement of transferring these skills to climbing out-
doors. In line with this participant feedback, it is recommended future stud-
ies assess larger samples in a multitude of settings including other domains,
to determine causality and generalizability. Furthermore, it is suggested
future studies employ a control group and a performance training group, to
add greater rigor in the assessment of flow training over peak performance
training. A final limitation to the intervention’s validity is a lack of retention
data. The participants’ schedules and local gyms did not facilitate follow-up
data on various time points post intervention however this would clearly be
beneficial.
The study’s findings and positive participant feedback on professional-
ism and methodology suggest the study’s adopted interventions are a positive
starting point to developing a flow training program. The study adds detailed
information to an otherwise scant area of research. In an applied setting these
findings may seem exciting, however, the training employed is elementary
thus it is important some guidelines are outlined for interested practitioners.
Flow is an easily misunderstood concept (Hytönen-Ng, 2016) therefore
it is advised that the educational intervention on flow is a) based on reputable
flow research and b) embedded within the participants on completion. It is
recommended that tests and sessions to assess learning quality be carried out
to ensure participants are not focused on similar but varying concepts such
as peak performance or peak experience. For example, participant 3 high-
lights the benefits of an education on flow “I made gains in how to attain flow
state, if for no other reason than I understand it better now and can recognise
it better.” Equally, it is important that subsequent interventions, such as goal-
setting or self-talk, are focused towards attaining flow states and not peak
performance. Furthermore, and congruent with Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990)
notion that if the subject is consciously thinking about flow it is contradictory
to the flow experience of action and awareness merging and loss of self-con-
sciousness, flow training must direct focus and preparation towards flow and
not be a necessary consumption of conscious thought throughout the perfor-
mance. This confusion occurred for 3 out of 4 participants and may have lim-
ited initial positive results. Also, during this study participants found it help-
ful to integrate their flow-orientated goals and self-talk into their rituals,
thereby ensuring finding flow was reinforced at multiple stages before their
performance. Finally, it must be noted that the current study was applied to
individuals as opposed to teams. Personal goals towards flow may conflict to
that of differing team goals and be detrimental to a collective flow or perfor-
mance, thus team application requires further research.
To conclude, the findings indicated that flow training warrants further
research. Future research is advised to examine education only interventions,
include retention data, and report further development and refinement on
the evidence-based implications for professional practice of flow training
presented in this study.
REFERENCES
Aherne, C., Moran, A. P., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). The effect of mindfulness training on ath-
letes’ flow: An initial investigation. Sport Psychologist, 25, 177-198.
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation follow-
ing a focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1849-1858.
Aubé, C., Brunelle, E., & Rousseau, V. (2014). Flow experience and team performance: The
role of team goal commitment and information exchange. Motivation and Emotion, 38,
120-130.
Bell, K. A. (2015). Mindfulness Skills Training for Elite Adolescent Athletes (Doctoral Thesis).
British Columbia, Canada: Simon Fraser University
Burton, D., Naylor S., & Holliday B. (2001). Goal setting in sport: Investigating the goal effec-
tiveness paradox. In: Singer R, Hausenblas H.A, & Janelle C.M. (Eds.), Handbook of
research on sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 497-528). New York: Wiley.
Cathcart, S., McGregor, M., & Groundwater, E. (2014). Mindfulness and Flow in Elite Ath-
letes. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 8, 119-141
Crust, L., & Swann, C. (2013). The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional
flow. European Journal of Sport Science, 13, 215-220.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal performance. NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding Flow. NY: Basic Books.
16 C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 17
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The” what” and” why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and
the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Dweck, C. (2007). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.
Ellis, A. (1999). Early theories and practices of rational emotive behavior therapy and how
they have been augmented and revised during the last three decades. Journal of Rational-
Rmotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 17, 69-93.
Filby, W. C., Maynard, I. W., & Graydon, J. K. (1999). The effect of multiple-goal strategies
on performance outcomes in training and competition. Journal of Applied Sport Psychol-
ogy, 11, 230-246.
Flett, M. R. (2015). Is Flow Related to Positive Feelings or Optimal Performance? Path Analy-
sis of Challenge-Skill Balance and Feelings. Sport Science Review, 24, 5-26.
Gardner, F. L., & Moore, Z. E. (2004). A mindfulness-acceptance-commitment-based
approach to athletic performance enhancement: Theoretical considerations. Behavior
Therapy, 35, 707-723.
Gardner, F. L., & Moore, Z. E. (2007). The psychology of enhancing human performance: The
mindfulness-acceptance-commitment (MAC) approach. Springer Publishing Company.
Grant, A. (2016). New Perspectives on Goal Setting in Coaching Practice: An Integrated
Model of Goal-focused Coaching. In S. David (Ed.), Beyond goals: effective strategies for
coaching and mentoring (pp. 55-84). London: Routledge.
Harmison, R. J. (2011). Peak performance in sport: Identifying ideal performance states and
developing athletes’ psychological skills. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
37, 233-243.
Holliday, B., Burton, D., Sun, G., Hammermeister, J., Naylor, S., & Freigang, D. (2008).
Building the better mental training mousetrap: Is periodization a more systematic
approach to promoting performance excellence? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20,
199-219.
Hrycaiko, D.W.. & Martin, G.L. (1996). Applied research studies with single-subject designs:
Why so few? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 8, 183-199.
Hytönen-Ng, E. (2016). Experiencing ‘flow’ in jazz performance. New York: Routledge.
Jackson, S. A. (1992). Athletes in flow: A qualitative investigation of flow states in elite figure
skaters. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 4, 161-180.
Jackson, S. A. (1996). Toward a conceptual understanding of the flow experience in elite ath-
letes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 76-90.
Jackson, S. A. (2000). Joy, fun, and flow state in sport. In Y. L. Hanin (Ed.), Emotions in sport
(p.135-155). Leeds: Human Kinetics.
Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports. Champaign; IL: Human Kinet-
ics.
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: the flow state scale-
2 and dispositional flow scale-2. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 133-150.
Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure opti-
mal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 17-
35.
Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J. (2001). Relationships between
flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. Journal of applied sport psy-
chology, 13, 129-153.
Jackson, S. A., & Roberts, G. C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward a con-
ceptual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6(2), 156-171.
Judge, L. W., Bell, R., Bellar, D., & Wanless, E. Developing a mental game plan: Mental peri-
odization for achieving a “flow” state for the track and field athlete. The Sport Journal,
13, 1.
Kawabata, M., & Mallett, C. J. (2016). Reconsideration of the state of optimal functioning
beyond flow theory. Routledge International Handbook of Sport Psychology, 369-377.
Kazdin, A. E. (1992). Research design in clinical psychology. NY: Macmillan.
Koehn, S., Morris, T., & Watt, A. P. (2014). Imagery Intervention to Increase Flow State and
Performance in Competition. Sport Psychologist, 28, 48-59.
Lindsay, P., Maynard, I., & Thomas, O. (2005). Effects of hypnosis on flow states and cycling
performance. The Sport Psychologist, 19, 164-177.
Martin, G. L., & Pear, J. J. (2003). Behaviour modification: What’s it is and how to do it (7thed.,
pp. 281-294). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. Handbook of Positive Psy-
chology, 89-105.
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). The concept of flow. In M. Csikszentmihalyi
(Ed.), Flow and the foundations of positive psychology (pp. 239-263). Springer Nether-
lands.
Nicholls, A. R., Polman, R. C., & Holt, N. L. (2005). The effects of an individualized imagery
interventions on flow states and golf performance. Athletic Insight, 7(1).
Pain, M. A., Harwood, C., & Anderson, R. (2011). Pre-competition imagery and music: The
impact on flow and performance in competitive soccer. The Sport Psychologist, 25, 212-
232.
Parker, R. I., & Vannest, K. (2009). An improved effect size for single-case research: Nonover-
lap of all pairs. Behavior Therapy, 40, 357-367.
Pates, J. (2013). The Effects of Hypnosis On An Elite Senior European Tour Golfer: A Single-
Subject Design. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 61, 193-
204.
Pates, J., & Cowen, A. P. (2013). The effect of a hypnosis intervention on performance and
flow state of an elite golfer: A single subject design. International Journal of Golf Science,
2, 43-53.
Pates, J., Cowen, A. P., & Karageorghis, C. I. (2012). The Effect of a Client-Centered
Approach on Flow States and the Performance of Three Elite Golfers. International
Journal of Golf Science, 1(2).
Pates, J., Maynar, I., & Westbury, T. (2001). An investigation into the effects of hypnosis on
basketball performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 84-102.
Pates, J., Oliver, R., & Maynard, I. (2001). The effects of hypnosis on flow states and golf-
putting performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 341-354.
Peters, S. (2013). The Chimp Paradox: The Mind Management Program to Help You Achieve
Success, Confidence, and Happiness. Tarcher.
Pineau, T. R., Glass, C. R., Kaufman, K. A., & Bernal, D. R. (2014). Self-and Team-Efficacy
Beliefs of Rowers and Their Relation to Mindfulness and Flow. Journal of Clinical Sport
Psychology, 8, 142-158.
Reardon, J., & Gordin, R. (1999). Psychological skill development leading to a peak perfor-
mance “flow state”. Track and Field Coaches Review, 3, 22-25.
Schwanhausser, L. (2009). Application of the mindfulness acceptance-commitment (MAC)
protocol with an adolescent springboard diver. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 4,
377-395.
18 C. Norsworthy et al.
Flow training 19
Seligman, M. E. (2012). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being.
New York: Simon and Schuster.
Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D., & Crust, L. (2012). A systematic review of the experi-
ence, occurrence, and controllability of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise, 13, 807-819.
Swann, C., Moran, A., & Piggott, D. (2015). Defining elite athletes: Issues in the study of
expert performance in sport psychology. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 3-14.
Swann, C., Crust, L., Jackman, P., Vella, S. A., Allen, M. S., & Keegan, R. (2017). Performing
under pressure: Exploring the psychological state underlying clutch performance in
sport. Journal of Sports Sciences, 35(23), 2272-2280.
Teasdale, J. D., & Segal, Z. V. (2007). The mindful way through depression: Freeing yourself
from chronic unhappiness. New York: Guilford Press.
Thelwell, R. C., & Greenlees, I. A. (2003). Developing competitive endurance performance
using mental skills training. Sport Psychologist, 17, 318-337.
Thelwell, R. C., Greenlees, I. A., & Weston, N. J. (2006). Using psychological skills training to
develop soccer performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18, 254-270.
Trei, L. (2007). New study yields instructive results on how mindset affects learning. Retrieved
from http://news.stanford.edu/news/2007/february7/dweck-020707.html
Weinberg, R., & Butt, J. (2014). Goal-setting and sport performance. Routledge In A. G.
Papaioannou, & D. Hackfort (Eds.), Companion to Sport and Exercise Psychology: Global
Perspectives and Fundamental Concepts (p. 343). New York: Routledge
Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D. (2014). Foundations of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 6E. Cham-
paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Wesson, K., & Boniwell, I. (2007). Flow theory-its application to coaching psychology. Inter-
national Coaching Psychology Review, 2, 33-43.
Williams, J. M., & Krane, V. (1998). Psychological charactersitics of peak performance. In,
J.M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology personal growth to peak performance (pp.
158-170). Mountain View: Manuscript submitted July 2017.Accepted for publication Octo-
ber 2017Mayfield Publishing Co.
... The third movement arouse from the criticism to flow state and its confrontation to other possible mental states as experienced by athletes: clutch state (Swann, Crust, Jackman et al., 2017b, 2017a, mental toughness (Ajilchi et al., 2019;Jackman et al., 2017) and the very unfolding of flow (telic / paratelic flow, Mackenzie et al., 2011). This confrontation, besides bringing news categories to analyze flow state, offered advancements in the way it is inducedflow training (Norsworthy et al., 2018). ...
... Some gaps are notorious among these movements, particularly when it comes to relate flow state to team sports and to its impact on sports performance (Norsworthy et al., 2018;Swann et al., 2018;Stoll, Ufer, 2012). More precisely, these gaps concern invasion team sports once their essence lies on the presence of opponents who constantly confront the team/player goals (Afonso, Garganta & Mesquita, 2012;Martiny, 2012;Gonzaléz, 2004). ...
... The studies included in this systematic review followed some criteria: i) only original studies carried out with athletes or student-athletes over 14 years old (Swan, 2016); ii) only studies carried out with team sports and flow (a weakness indicated in studies cf Norsworthy et al., 2017Norsworthy et al., , 2018; iii) studies published in Portuguese, English and Spanish; iv) studies using qualitative, quantitative or mixed methods; v) studies published between 2015-2020 (appropriate search time reference cf Sampaio & Mancini, 2007); vi) studies that used quasi experimental, experimental groups and/or control groups, with preference for studies that included pre-and post-tests; vii) studies that presented methodologies to improve, induce, facilitate, achieve flow (e.g., mindfulness, imagery meditation, flow training, Integral Yoga, Self-talk); viii) studies that addressed different types of methodology for teaching-learning-training team sports (e.g., global method; situational-active method; traditional method) relating them to the flow state and; ix) types of tests used to measure personality traits, nomothetic profile [emotions, energy, mood] associated -[correlation and causality]with the flow state (e.g., Big five personality test; Individual Zone of Luis Eugênio Martiny, Larissa Zanetti Theil, Eloy Maciel Neto, Gonçalo Dias, José Pedro Ferreira, Rui Mendes ____________________________________________________________________________ Optimal Functions (IZOF); Hedonic orientation of experienced emoticons (Hedonic tone); Self-determination theory; Dispositional flow state scale; Satisfaction perception). ...
Article
Full-text available
The flow state has materialized as a condition of possibility for the search for the best performance in sports, both individual and collective. The objective of this systematic review of studies was to identify which form the “state of flow” influenced the sports performance of team sports athletes. Up from references to selected databases (for example, Web of Science Core Collection; SPORTDiscus (EBSCO-Host); PsycINFO and Pubmed), the results showed that 1946 articles were obtained, with the exclusion of duplicate studies (n = 407). Among the 46 articles that emerged from the analysis, the data excluded 37 studies, and considering an amount of 9 articles which were included in the final section of the systematic review and carried out in the period from 2015 to 2020. As a result, the flow state enhances a full absorption involvement of team-sport athletes. Emotional states tend to be part of an athlete's success. Knowing the three essential fields that comprise this scope (flow-mindfulness-performance) allows us to raise questions for the future, inherent to the effects of performance and the magnitude of the athlete's response depending on their individual characteristics, action possibilities, task constraints, and how much this influences their state of flow at the individual and collective levels.
... Flow experience was generally conceptualized in nine key dimensions, three describing preconditions for flow occurrence (e.g., challenge-skill balance, clear goals, and action-awareness) and six reflecting characteristics of the experience (e.g., transformation of sense of time and sense of control) [25,27]. Recent research in sport has mainly focused on the antecedents of flow experience [28], the role of flow experience on performance [29], and interventions to increase flow [30]. Through structural equation modeling, scholars have verified that satisfaction with event levels fully mediated the role of flow experience in people's overall life satisfaction [31]. ...
... When people are highly involved in long-distance running, they tend to engage in regular daily exercise, make significant efforts to improve their knowledge and skills, and show a strong willingness to continue running [39]. As specialization progresses, such individuals will come to exhibit a high level of flow experience, reap various durable benefits, and report a more satisfying life [29,31]. Extant literature suggests that a higher degree of self-efficacy is more likely to correlate with people's life satisfaction [23,24]. ...
... The results of this study support earlier findings of other investigators [28][29][30] who found that flow experience positively predicts life satisfaction. In a qualitative investigation [51], participants reported many positive outcomes after engaging in rewarding physical activities, including the building of confidence and the experiencing of optimal arousal, self-recovery, and intrinsic motivation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies confirmed that leisure sport participation could contribute to people’s life satisfaction. However, little is known about the predictors of life satisfaction in the context of long-distance running. A model was proposed in this study to examine the relationship between recreation specialization, self-efficacy, flow experience, and life satisfaction. An online questionnaire was distributed to long-distance runners in China, and a total of 404 valid questionnaires were obtained for data analysis in this study. Results indicated that recreation specialization and self-efficacy had a direct and positive effect on runners’ flow experience; recreation specialization, self-efficacy, and flow experience were positively associated with runners’ life satisfaction. Furthermore, flow experience partially mediated the relationship between self-efficacy and life satisfaction, while it fully mediated the role of recreation specialization in life satisfaction. The findings shed some new insights for understanding the influence of leisure sport engagement on people’s life satisfaction.
... The experimental groups (male adult, female adult, male youth, and female youth) participated individually in the nine-week mindfulness intervention program, with the first week providing clarification on how the program worked and education about flow (adapted) [47] and about mindfulness [48,49]. The following eight weeks consisted of the application of the training program itself. ...
... Schutte and Malouff [15] indicated not only a strong association between mindfulness and flow but also a connection with a variety of beneficial results for practitioners. Therefore, the improvement of mental well-being can not only be accompanied by an excellent disposition for flow and an adequate level of mindfulness but can also be an excellent indicator of improved athlete performance [9,47,60]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This study aimed to analyze the effect of a flow training program based on mindfulness applied to young and adult elite handball athletes. A quantitative, quasi-experimental, descriptive data analysis approach was carried out. The sample included 105 athletes (51 female and 54 male). The athletes were divided into two groups: i) experimental (n = 53) and ii) control (n = 52). The results of the repeated ANOVA indicated that the experimental group achieved significant improvements compared to the control group in decision making (n2p=.086), mental well-being (n2p=.045), dispositional flow state (n2p=.103), non-judgment (n2p=.040), refocusing (n2p=.052) and mindful trait in daily life (n2p=.058). In the linear regression test, the factor non-judgment was identified as a predictor of decision making (F(103)=7.99, p=.006) and the predictor for the mental well-being were dispositional flow state (F(102)=29.2, p
... The experimental groups (male adult, female adult, male youth, and female youth) participated individually in the nine-week mindfulness intervention program ( Figure 1), with the first week providing clarification on how the program worked and education about flow (adapted) [47] and mindfulness [48,49]. The following eight weeks consisted of the application of the training program itself. ...
... Schutte and Malouff [15] indicated not only a strong association between mindfulness and flow, but also a connection with a variety of beneficial results for practitioners. Therefore, the improvement in mental well-being may not only be accompanied by an excellent disposition for flow and an adequate level of mindfulness, but it may also be an excellent indicator of improved athlete performance [9,47,60]. Positive correlations also indicated that the program may offer the athletes the ability to experiment and increase their dispositional flow state and mindfulness in activities other than those related to sports contexts. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to analyze the effect of a flow training program based on mindfulness applied to young and adult elite handball athletes. A quantitative, quasi-experimental, descriptive data analysis approach was carried out. The sample included 105 athletes (51 female and 54 male). The athletes were divided into two groups: (i) experimental (n = 53) and (ii) control (n = 52). The results of the repeated ANOVA indicated that the experimental group achieved significant improvements compared to the control group in decision making (n2p = 0.086), mental well-being (n2p = 0.045), dispositional flow state (n2p = 0.103), non-judgment (n2p = 0.040), refocusing (n2p = 0.052), and mindful traits in daily life (n2p = 0.058). A Pearson analysis demonstrated a positive correlation between dispositional flow state and mindfulness in sport and mental well-being. The correlation analysis also showed an inverse correlation between decision making and sense of control and mindful traits in daily life. The findings revealed that the program can be effective in decision making, mental well-being, dispositional flow state, mindfulness in sport, and mindful traits in daily lives of athletes.
... A growing amount of research has been conducted on the existence of flow in a recreational and tourism context. For example, sport psychology and leisure scholars have made numerous attempts at defining flow (e.g., Jackson, 1995;Sinnamon, Moran, & O'Connell, 2012), and several recent systematic reviews have been conducted on flow states in sport (Goddard, Stevens, Jackman, & Swann, 2021;Harris, Allen, Vine, & Wilson, 2021;Jackman, Dargue, Johnston, & Hawkins, 2021;Norsworthy, Thelwell, Weston, & Jackson, 2018;Stamatelopoulou, Pezirkianidis, Karakasidou, Lakioti, & Stalikas, 2018), exercise (Jackman, Hawkins, Crust, & Swann, 2019), and adventure recreation (Boudreau, Mackenzie, & Hodge, 2020). Flow states have been commonly explored in relation to mountain biking and mountain bike tourism (Amatulli, Peluso, Sestino, Petruzzellis, & Guido, 2021;Taylor, 2011;Taylor & Carr, 2021). ...
... Therefore, being asked about one's state of mind during an activity is thought to distract participants from the optimal psychological states that they are potentially experiencing (Harris et al., 2021). Second, optimal psychological states are relatively elusive and difficult to successfully control, even during interventions specifically designed to facilitate optimal psychological states (e.g., Norsworthy et al., 2018). Concerns have also been raised about studies that ask participants to recall memories (Yarrow et al.,1970), particularly those related to flow, due to poor recall (Kimiecik & Stein, 1992) and recall biases (Brewer, Van Raalte, Linder, & Van Raalte, 1991) and the fleeting nature of the flow state. ...
Article
Full-text available
Flow, a state of mind that occurs when persons are deeply immersed in an activity, plays an important role in enhancing the tourist experience through a variety of activities. Historically, flow is explored from the perspective of how tourists' experience the state, while little attention has been given to how tourism businesses can facilitate flow states to improve their product offerings. This preliminary study explores a tourism business in Tasmania, Australia, whose ethos is centred around a desire to facilitate a flow state for their guests, including the design of infrastructure, guide training and the design of itineraries. The impact of these mechanisms upon guests' experiences is assessed to understand whether and how these design elements influence the attainment of flow.
... En el ámbito del deporte, un programa de entrenamiento (con contenidos educativos, establecimiento de objetivos, diálogo interno y atención plena) mostró su eficacia para promover el estado de Fluidez y aumentar el rendimiento (medido en tiempo) de cuatro escaladores de élite (Norsworthy et al., 2018). También se ha encontrado una relación pequeña pero estadísticamente significativa entre las experiencias de Fluidez de los atletas (calificadas justo después de una competición) y la posición final (Jackson et al., 2001). ...
Thesis
La interpretación musical es una actividad compleja a nivel motor, cognitivo y emocional que depende de una variedad de factores, no solo relacionados con la competencia musical. Los estados psicológicos pueden influir en el nivel de competencia artística, ya que pueden facilitar o impedir que los músicos intérpretes muestren su verdadero potencial musical. El desarrollo de habilidades de autorregulación dirigidas a suscitar la respuesta de Fluidez puede contribuir a mejorar la calidad de su experiencia durante la interpretación, aumentar la motivación intrínseca, y facilitar el compromiso con la actividad durante largos períodos de tiempo (importante para los logros creativos y artísticos). En las enseñanzas musicales parece fundamental el desarrollo de habilidades psicológicas que preparen adecuadamente a los músicos y estudiantes de música a afrontar los estresores específicos vinculados a las demandas de sus estudios y de su futura profesión. Pero, más allá de los beneficios específicos en la actividad profesional, el entrenamiento en habilidades psicológicas de autorregulación podría influir en su salud y bienestar general. La teoría de la Fluidez se enmarca en la corriente de la Psicología Positiva, cuyo cuerpo de conocimiento ha ido en aumento desde el inicio del S. XXI. Desde esta corriente, la investigación científica se ha dirigido a comprender y construir aquellos factores que permiten que las personas, las comunidades y las sociedades prosperen. Los resultados del creciente número de investigaciones que han estudiado los efectos de aplicar la Psicología Positiva en diferentes ámbitos, muestran que la Psicología Positiva tiene una amplia base de evidencia que respalda su eficacia. Específicamente, la investigación sobre la experiencia de Fluidez ha aumentado durante los últimos años. La Fluidez “es un estado gratificante de profunda implicación y absorción que las personas experimentan cuando afrontan una actividad desafiante y perciben habilidades adecuadas para involucrarse” (EFRN, 2014)1. El fenómeno fue descrito por Csikszentmihalyi (1975)2 para explicar por qué las personas realizan actividades sin más motivo que la actividad en sí misma, sin recompensas extrínsecas, y, además, persisten en esas actividades. La experiencia de Fluidez es una experiencia reconocida como una realidad fenomenológica por personas de todas las edades, género, estatus socioeconómico y muy diversas culturas; y se considera como un estado positivo de conciencia por todas ellas. La evidencia que se ha obtenido a través de décadas de investigación ha mostrado que la experiencia de Fluidez, entendida como una experiencia óptima, sucede cuando los desafíos que una persona afronta, así como las habilidades que tiene para involucrarse están en equilibrio y a partir de un cierto nivel (superior a lo que uno realiza de forma más cotidiana en la vida diaria). Aunque estas relaciones están en parte moderadas por otros factores, tanto situacionales, como personales. La evidencia empírica también muestra que la Fluidez se asocia al afecto positivo. Cuando las personas experimentan Fluidez en una situación, también tienden a ser felices después. En el contexto de actividades que ofrecen desafíos óptimos para las habilidades que posee una persona, es un estado que se ha asociado de forma positiva con el rendimiento. En parte, porque el estado de Fluidez, como estado intrínsecamente gratificante, conduciría a un mayor compromiso con la actividad a lo largo del tiempo. En el ámbito de la música, existe una acumulación creciente de trabajos de investigación que han estudiado la Fluidez desde diferentes perspectivas. En el contexto de la interpretación musical, uno de los temas de mayor interés está relacionado con la contribución de la experiencia de Fluidez a la mejora de los síntomas de la Ansiedad Escénica Musical y a la mejora del rendimiento o calidad interpretativa. Para poder evaluar el estado de Fluidez en personas que interpretan música, pero también, para poder evaluar la eficacia de intervenciones dirigidas a desencadenar la respuesta de Fluidez, es necesario contar con un instrumento de medida del estado de Fluidez, validado en una muestra representativa de músicos intérpretes del Estado Español. El hilo conductor de la presente tesis ha sido la medición del estado de Fluidez en el contexto de la música. En primer lugar, se realizó la adaptación al español y la validación del instrumento de medida del estado de Fluidez, cuyas propiedades psicométricas se analizaron con una amplia muestra de 486 músicos del Estado Español que tenían una relación consolidada con la actividad musical (tanto estudiantes, profesionales, como aficionados). En segundo lugar, se utilizó el instrumento para evaluar el estado de Fluidez en personas con Altas Capacidades Intelectuales cuando interpretan música. Este estudio se realizó como un estudio piloto, dado que no existe en la literatura un trabajo previo en el que se haya medido el estado de Fluidez en estas personas. En tercer lugar, se utilizó el instrumento para evaluar un programa de intervención específico de entrenamiento de habilidades de autorregulación psicológica diseñado para músicos intérpretes. El objetivo principal fue desencadenar la respuesta de Fluidez y el afrontamiento de la Ansiedad Escénica Musical durante la interpretación. De los resultados obtenidos en los tres estudios se puede concluir, en primer lugar, que se dispone de una herramienta validada para evaluar el estado de Fluidez en músicos intérpretes. La validación de este instrumento puede tener implicaciones clínicas y educativas, ya que el uso del cuestionario permite identificar aspectos importantes de lo que facilita o inhibe una actuación musical o del mismo aprendizaje. También puede utilizarse para futuras investigaciones donde se desee medir la variable estado de Fluidez. En segundo lugar, los resultados del segundo estudio sugieren una relación entre las personas con altas capacidades, la experiencia de Fluidez, concretamente en la experiencia de la pérdida de la autoconciencia, y aspectos de la personalidad creativa. Los resultados también sugieren que las personas con altas capacidades podrían controlar mejor su atención, disfrutar más durante el aprendizaje y, por tanto, aprender mejor. Para finalizar, los resultados del programa de intervención mostraron que los músicos intérpretes que participaron en el programa aumentaron los niveles del estado de Fluidez y disminuyeron los niveles de Ansiedad Escénica Musical de forma estadísticamente significativa. Ello sugiere que los programas que contemplen en su diseño una combinación de todas las técnicas y métodos que se utilizaron en el programa y que provienen de la Psicología científica podrían ser útiles para tratar la problemática de la Ansiedad Escénica Musical o prevenirla; y, además, podrían facilitar el estado de Fluidez, un mayor disfrute durante la interpretación y potencialmente una mejor calidad interpretativa. Se exponen las limitaciones y se señalan direcciones futuras de investigación. 1 EFRN, 2014: Red Europea de Investigadores de Fluidez (European Flow-Researchers’ Network) 2 Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-bass.
... There are some common features between mindfulness and flow in that both emphasize focus on the present moment (Wright et al., 2006). Norsworthy et al. (2017) found that flow requires unconscious attention to specific tasks in the present moment. Some scholars have suggested that being present is an effective strategy for achieving flow (Rogatko, 2009;Weinstein et al., 2009;Landhäußer and Keller, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Creativity is so important for social and technological development that people are eager to find an easy way to enhance it. Previous studies have shown that mindfulness has significant effects on positive affect (PA), working memory capacity, cognitive flexibility and many other aspects, which are the key to promoting creativity. However, there are few studies on the relationship between mindfulness and creativity. The mechanism between mindfulness and creativity is still uncertain. Meditation is an important method of mindfulness training, but for most people who do not have the basic training, it’s difficult to master how to get into a state of mindfulness. Animation has been shown by many studies to help improve cognition and is often used as a guiding tool. Using animation as the guiding carrier of meditation is more convenient and easier to accept. Therefore, this study adopted the intervention method of animation-guided meditation, aiming to explore: (1) the effect of animation-guided meditation on enhancing creativity; (2) the role of flow and emotion in the influence of mindfulness on creativity. We advertised recruitment through the internal network of a creative industrial park, and the final 95 eligible participants were divided into two groups: animation (n = 48) and audio (n = 47) guided meditation. The animation group was given an animated meditation intervention, and the audio group was given an audio meditation intervention, both interventions were performed 3 times a week and last for 8 weeks. Results: (1) Animation-guided meditation significantly increased participants’ mindfulness and creativity levels; Significantly reduced their cognitive load compared to audio-guided meditation. (2) Mindfulness has a significant direct effect on creativity, and significant indirect effects on creativity; Flow and PA act as the mediating variable. Conclusion: (1) Mindfulness, flow, and PA all helped to improve the subjects’ work creativity. In addition to the direct positive impact of mindfulness on creativity, mindfulness can also have an indirect positive impact on creativity through flow and PA. (2) Compared with audio, animation can significantly reduce cognitive load and help improve users’ cognitive ability, which is more suitable for the guidance materials of mindfulness meditation to enhance the effect of meditation.
... In the sport literature, flow is routinely described as underlying peak or enhanced performances (Jackson et al., 2001;Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999;Norsworthy et al., 2017;Swann et al., 2017). Given that flow is associated with functional characteristics like feeling focused, motivated and confident, the link to performance is appealing (Landhäußer & Keller, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Flow is an optimal experience that has received particular interest within sport because of a possible relationship with enhanced athletic performances. Yet, the strength and direction of the putative flow–performance relationship remain unclear. Consequently, a PRISMA guided systematic review was conducted in May 2020 to examine the empirical evidence for a flow–performance relationship, to examine potential mechanisms, and to assess the quality of current evidence. Peer-reviewed articles that examined the relationship between flow and performance in sport or computer gaming tasks were searched for using five online databases. The results were collated into a narrative synthesis and a meta-analysis. Twenty articles met the inclusion criteria, featuring 22 studies that were appropriate for meta-analysis. The overall quality of the studies was fairly good, with a mean quality assessment score of 76.5% (SD = 9.7). The pooled effect size (r = 0.31, 95% CI [0.24; 0.38]) indicated that across a range of sport and gaming tasks there was a medium-sized flow–performance relationship. However, current evidence is unable to determine the causal direction of this relationship or the mechanisms that mediate it. A number of conceptual and methodological challenges facing the study of flow are discussed and recommendations for future work are outlined.
Article
Full-text available
Research on psychological flow is well established, although criticisms remain regarding conceptual and measurement issues associated with the construct. This scoping review maps flow-related research across scientific disciplines, examining the conceptualization, measurement instruments, and outcomes of flow between 2012 and 2019. Across 236 sources that met the review criteria, 108 different flow-related constructs were measured by 141 instruments, and 84 possible antecedents were identified. Despite the varied approaches, a common set of overarching antecedent constructs included "optimal challenge" and "high motivation," and recurring characteristics of the flow experience itself included "absorption," "effort-less control," and "intrinsic reward." Applied studies-albeit inconsistent in approach and largely correlational in nature-predominantly linked flow to "positive development" (i.e., well-being and health), "high functioning," and "further engagement." We contextualize the findings of the review relative to important work on flow that has recently emerged (following the review period)-in doing so, we hope this review offers a contemporary framework that can be used for the study of flow across scientific disciplines. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Uspešen nastop je rezultat premagovanja številnih izzivov, tako fizičnih kot psihičnih, s katerimi se glasbene in športne skupine srečujejo na svoji poti. Na prvi pogled se mogoče zdi, da teme z glasbenega področja nimajo veliko skupnega s temami s športnega področja, vendar pa obstaja kar veliko podobnosti. Namen naše raziskave je bil ugotoviti, kakšne so podobnosti priprave in izvedbe glasbenega nastopa v manjših skupinah v primerjavi s pripravo in izvedbo tekmovanja v ekipnih športih. V raziskavi je sodelovalo 51 študentov Akademije za glasbo in 54 študentov Fakultete za šport, ki so člani manjših skupin (do 12 članov). Rezultati so pokazali, da med skupino glasbenikov in skupino športnikov obstajajo podobnosti pri dejavnikih, ki najpomembneje vplivajo na delovanje manjše skupine, podobnosti pri dejavnikih, ki vplivajo na uspešnost izvedbe nastopa, ter podobnosti pri psihični pripravi na glasbeno oz. športno izvedbo v manjših skupinah. Nekatere razlike so se pokazale v zaznavanju prednosti in slabosti skupinske izvedbe glede na individualno izvedbo. Prav tako so bile potrjene razlike v raznolikosti vrst izvedbe pri glasbenikih kot pri športnikih v manjših skupinah.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the effects of hypnosis on flow states and golf-putting per- formance in 5 competitive players. The investigation utilized an ideographic single- subject multiple baseline across subjects design combined with a procedure that monitors the internal experience of the participants (Wollman, 1986). The method of intervention utilized in this study involved hypnotic induction, hypnotic re- gression, and trigger control procedures. The results indicated that all 5 partici- pants increased both their mean golf putting performance and their mean flow scores from baseline to intervention. There were no overlapping data points be- tween the baseline and intervention for either performance or flow state. Addi- tionally, each participant indicated that they had felt the intervention was useful in keeping them relaxed, confident, and focused. Three of the golfers also re- ported experiencing reduced concerns about performing and more control over their putting stroke.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of hypnosis on set- and jump-shooting performance among male collegiate basketball players. A single-subject ABA research design combined with a procedure that monitors the internal experience of the participants (Wollman, 1986) was implemented. The results indicated that all three participants increased their mean jump- and set- shooting performance from baseline to intervention, with all three participants returning to baseline levels of performance postintervention phase. Finally, each participant reported they had felt the intervention had increased sensations they associated with peak performance. These results support the hypothesis that a hypnosis intervention can improve jump- and set-shooting performance and increase feelings and cognitions that are associated with peak performance.
Article
Full-text available
Clutch performance is improved performance under pressure. However, little research has examined the psychological state experienced by athletes in these situations. Therefore, this study qualitatively examined the subjective experience underlying clutch performance across a range of sports (e.g., team, individual) and standards (Olympic to recreational athletes). Sixteen athletes (Mage = 27.08 years; SD = 6.48) took part in in-depth, semi-structured interviews primarily after an exceptional performance (M = 4.38 days later; SD = 3.14). Data were analysed inductively and thematically. Clutch states involved 12 characteristics, including heightened and deliberate concentration, intense effort, and heightened awareness, which distinguished the experience of clutch from other optimal psychological states such as flow. Other characteristics, such as perceptions of control, were also reported and supported previous experimental research on clutch. These findings present in-depth qualitative insights into the psychological state underlying clutch performance, and are discussed in relation to the existing literature on optimal psychological states in sport.
Article
Purpose: Being ‘in flow’ or ‘in the zone’ enables individuals to focus on tasks more fully and to maximise performance. As this phenomenon results in pleasure being experienced whilst mastery is gained, it can be a natural aid to goal-oriented activities such as coaching. This paper explores the applications of flow theory to coaching psychology. Method: Various conditions are thought to influence the acquisition and maintenance of psychological flow. These are reviewed and linked to the work of coaches and coaching psychologists. Results: The literature and models reviewed indicate that flow theory has a number of applications to coaching psychology, and that it can provide a useful framework for coaching psychology practice. Conclusions: This paper suggests how these factors may be captured by coaching methodology thereby: (i) helping the client and coach to find focus and fulfilment during sessions; and (ii) encouraging the client to remain ‘on task’ whilst engaging in goal actioning activities afterwards.
Article
This study presents the case of Steve, an adolescent competitive springboard diver. This diver, referred by his coach, received the Mindfulness-Acceptance-Commitment (MAC) approach for performance enhancement. The MAC protocol, originally written for an adult population, was used in modified form (under consultation from the authors) to ensure appropriateness for an adolescent population. Conducted in nine individual sessions, the intervention targeted abilities in attention and value-driven behavior to enhance focus, poise, and overall diving performance. Self-report measures of mindfulness and flow, along with objective measures of diving performance were collected pre- and postintervention. Results indicated increases in mindful awareness, mindful attention, experiential acceptance, flow, and diving performance from pre- to postintervention. This case supports the applicability of the MAC protocol with an adolescent athlete population.