ChapterPDF Available
Values and Subjective Well-Being
By Shalom H. Schwartz, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem; Florencia M. Sortheix, University of Helsinki
and University of Jyväskylä
Abstract
This chapter summarizes what we know about value—SWB relations and identifies some of
what we do not know but would like to know. We first discuss the nature of values and the
structured system they form. We draw on the ten values in the Schwartz (1992) theory that form
a circular motivational continuum. We note three theoretical perspectives on relations of values
to SWB. The first perspective seeks to explain direct relations between values and SWB. A
second generation of direct relations research explains variation in relations across samples by
pointing to societal-level moderators. The second perspective examines the congruence (or
similarity) between people’s values and the values prevailing in their environment as a
determinant of SWB. The third perspective sees the attainment of valued goals as the source of
SWB. We present these perspectives and the literature based on them, and note ideas for future
research throughout.
Keywords: Values, Subjective Well-being, Cross-Cultural Variations, Value Congruence
Citation Schwartz, S. H., & Sortheix, F. M. (2018). Values and subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, &
L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of Well-Being. Noba Scholar Handbook series: Subjective well-being. Salt Lake City,
UT: DEF publishers. DOI:nobascholar.com
Download
Research on relations of personal values to subjective well-being has begun to flourish
only recently. This is surprising because our values represent what we consider important and
worth pursuing in life, and our subjective well-being (henceforth SWB) represents how happy
and satisfied we are with the life we are leading. This chapter summarizes what we know about
value—SWB relations and identifies some of what we do not know but would like to know. We
first discuss the nature of values and the structured system they form. We then note three
theoretical perspectives on relations of values to SWB (Sagiv & Schwartz, 2000; Sagiv, Roccas
& Hazan, 2004). The first perspective seeks to explain direct relations between values and SWB.
The second examines the congruence (or similarity) between people’s values and the values
prevailing in their environment as a determinant of SWB. The third sees the attainment of valued
goals as the source of SWB. We present these perspectives and the literature based on them.
Throughout, we note ideas for future research.
What Values Are and How They Are Structured
Both psychologists and lay people view values as broad motivational constructs that
express what is important to people (e.g., Rohan, 2000; Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz, 1992). Based
on their values, individuals perceive acts, objects, people, and events to be more or less desirable
(Feather, 1995; Schwartz, 1992). People’s values are central to their identities and concepts of
self (Hitlin, 2003; Rokeach, 1973). Are they also related to our sense of well-being?
Most theorists agree in defining basic values as desirable, trans-situational goals, varying
in importance, that serve as guiding principles in people’s lives (Schwartz, 1992; cf. Kluckhohn,
1951; Rokeach, 1973). Each person holds numerous values (e.g., achievement, security,
benevolence) with varying degrees of importance. Schwartz (1992, 2006, 2015) summarized six
defining features of basic values found in the literature. (1) Values are beliefs, inextricably linked
to affect. (2) Values refer to desirable goals. (3) Values transcend specific actions and situations.
(4) Values serve as standards to guide selection and evaluation of people, events, actions, and
policies. (5) Values are ordered in a hierarchy by their relative importance. (6) It is
the relative importance of the values relevant to any attitude or behavior that motivates and
guides that attitude or behavior.
The above features characterize all values. What distinguishes one value from another is
the type of goal or motivation that the value expresses. Recognizing that every language includes
thousands of value terms, researchers have proposed more inclusive basic values for studying
value origins and consequences (e.g., Rokeach, 1973). The most popular current classification is
from the theory of basic values by Schwartz (1992, 2015). This theory defines ten broad values
according to the motivation that underlies each one. We draw on this theory to frame our
examination of direct relations between values and SWB.
Presumably, the ten values are universal. This is because the values are based on one or
more of three universal requirements of human existence: needs of individuals as biological
organisms, requisites of coordinated social interaction, and survival and welfare needs of groups.
People cannot cope successfully with these requirements of human existence alone. To cope with
them, people must articulate appropriate goals, communicate with others about these goals, and
gain cooperation in pursuing them. Values are the socially desirable concepts used to represent
and communicate these goals.
Figure 1 presents the ten motivationally distinct values in the form of a circular
continuum. There is a wedge for each value. Values in opposing wedges in the circle conflict
with one another motivationally (e.g., stimulation vs. security). Values in adjacent wedges are
compatible (e.g., conformity and tradition). Two bi-polar, higher order dimensions summarize
the relations among the ten values (Figure 1). Self-transcendence versus self-
enhancement describes a conflict between values that express concern for the welfare and
interests of others versus values that express concern for one’s own interests above those of
others. Openness to change versus conservation describes a conflict between values that
emphasize independence of action, thought, and sensations and readiness for change versus
security, predictability, and resistance to change. The theory proposes that this circular structure
describes the pattern of conflict and compatibility among values across cultures. Research in
over 80 countries largely confirms the circular motivational continuum of values (Schwartz,
2017). This suggests a universal organization of human motivations.
Figure 1. The circular structure of ten basic values, four higher order values, and two
underlying motivational sources (adapted from Schwartz, 2015).
The outer two circles in Figure 1 specify two other principles that organize relations
among the values. We discuss these principles later in this chapter. There, we cite literature that
uses them to explain the associations of values to SWB.
Table 1 lists the ten values in their order around the circle. Each is defined in terms of its
motivational goal.1 Conceiving values as organized in a circular motivational continuum has an
important implication: Any variable that is especially congruent with one basic value (e.g.,
supporting immigration with universalism) should also be relatively congruent with the adjacent
values (benevolence and self-direction) but should conflict with the opposing values (power,
security and achievement). As we shall see, this pattern generally holds for associations of values
with SWB.
Table 1. Ten basic values and their motivational goals.
Value Motivational goals
Achievement Personal success, demonstrating competence according to social standards
Power Social status and prestige, control or dominance over people and resources
Security Safety, harmony and stability of society, of relationships, and of self
Conformity Restraint of actions likely to upset others and violate social expectations or
norms
Tradition Respect, commitment and acceptance of the customs and ideas that
traditional culture or religion provide the self
Benevolence Preservation and enhancement of the welfare of people with whom one is
in frequent personal contact
Universalism Understanding, and protection for the welfare of all and the environment
Self-Direction Independent thought and action-choosing, creating, exploring
Stimulation Excitement, novelty and challenge in life
Hedonism Pleasure and sensuous gratification for oneself
Adapted from Schwartz (1992).
There are three main theoretical approaches to understanding relations between values
and SWB. One approach holds that the specific motivational content of a value determines its
influence on SWB. This approach postulates that inherent properties of particular values either
promote or undermine SWB. That is, some values are ‘healthy’ and others ‘unhealthy’ (Sagiv &
Schwartz, 2000; Strupp, 1980). A second approach emphasizes the congruence or similarity
between a person’s values and the values prevailing in the environment. A third approach
emphasizes the extent to which the environment constrains or facilitates people’s attainment of
their valued goals. A third approach emphasizes the effects of successfully attaining valued goals
on SWB. We discuss each approach in turn.
Direct Associations of Values With SWB: Healthy and Unhealthy Values
Theorizing by psychotherapists about value-SWB associations identified values that
emphasize autonomy, responsibility, and fairness as healthy and those based on security,
conformity, or status needs as unhealthy (Bergin, 1991; Strupp, 1980). Bilsky and Schwartz
(1994) were the first social psychologists to identify healthy vs. unhealthy values. Building on
Maslow (1943) and Rokeach (1973), they argued that healthy values are cognitive
transformations of growth needs and unhealthy values are cognitive transformations of
deficiency needs. They classified stimulation, self-direction, universalism, benevolence, and
achievement as transformations of growth needs. Success in attaining the goals of these self-
actualizing values leads people to attribute more importance to them. They classified conformity,
security, and power values as transformations of deficiency needs. Failure to attain the goals of
these self-protective values and thereby overcome deprivation and threat leads people to attribute
more importance to them.
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1995; Ryan & Deci, 2001) also links human
well-being to the satisfaction of innate psychological needs (see Heintzelman, this handbook). It
specifies three needs whose satisfaction promotes well-being: autonomy, competence, and
relatedness. These needs are transformed into intrinsic, self-actualizing goals or values. Parallels
between the three intrinsic goals and the Schwartz (1992) values are autonomy—self-direction,
competence—achievement, and relatedness—both benevolence and universalism (Sagiv &
Schwartz, 2000). Self-determination theory also identifies extrinsic goals that derive from the
need to obtain other people’s approval, admiration, and praise and to avoid social censure (e.g.,
financial success, popularity, social dominance). The Schwartz values parallel to extrinsic goals
include security, conformity, power, and, perhaps, tradition.
Sagiv and Schwartz (2000) noted three mechanisms through which values and SWB may
be associated. First, pursuing healthy values may lead to perceptions (e.g. people are kind),
attitudes (e.g., tolerance), or behaviors (e.g., helping) that, in turn, increase well-being.
Conversely, pursuing unhealthy values may lead to perceptions (people are threatening), attitudes
(intolerance), or behaviors (shunning others) that, in turn, undermine well-being. Second,
pursuing healthy values may lead directly to well-being because it satisfies intrinsic, self-
actualizing needs. In contrast, pursuing unhealthy values does not satisfy intrinsic needs and may
require stressful ego-protective activity.
These first two mechanisms posit that pursuing particular values causally influences
SWB. A third mechanism posits the reverse causal direction, the level of SWB influences value
priorities. People who feel safe and satisfied with life have the emotional and cognitive resources
to pursue autonomy (self-direction) and excitement (stimulation) and to care for the welfare of
close others (benevolence) or society and nature (universalism). People who feel unsafe and
threatened, in contrast, are preoccupied with their own problems and lack the resources to pursue
these values. They emphasize values whose realization promises greater safety, certainty, and
relief from anxiety (security, conformity, tradition, and power).
First Generation Studies
Sagiv and Schwartz (2000) published the first empirical study of associations between the
ten values and SWB. They drew upon the above ideas to hypothesize that self-direction,
benevolence, universalism, stimulation and achievement values promote SWB and that power,
conformity, security, and tradition values to undermine SWB. As the value theory implies, the
five values they expected to promote SWB are adjacent in the value circle as are the four values
they expected to undermine SWB. They had no expectation for hedonism values, which lie on
the boundary between these two sets. Subsequent researchers proposed similar or no hypotheses,
simply examining the observed correlations.
We found eight studies of direct value-SWB associations in non-representative student or
adult samples from seven countries (see Table 2). The SWB index was life satisfaction in most
studies and positive affect in a few. Table 2 reveals a consistent, though not perfect, tendency
across studies for power values to relate negatively and for benevolence, stimulation, and self-
direction values to relate positively to SWB, as hypothesized.2 Hedonism also related positively.
Considering only significant findings, there was also a tendency for universalism values to relate
positively and conformity values to relate negatively to SWB. Findings for achievement,
tradition, and security were inconsistent. These studies offer only partial support for the initial
theorizing. Although small sample sizes and cultural differences might account for some
inconsistencies, improved theorizing and more rigorous research were clearly needed.
Schwartz (2012) and Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) suggested a new theoretical model of
direct value-SWB associations. They proposed that relations of values to SWB depend upon the
interplay between two motivational underpinnings of values. Every value has a social or a person
focus, but it also expresses a self-expansive growth orientation or a self-protective, anxiety-based
orientation (Schwartz, 2006, 2015). The self-protection concept improves on the earlier
deficiency concept. Self-transcendence and openness to change values express self-expansive,
growth orientations. They oppose conservation and power values, which express self-protective,
anxiety-control orientations (the top vs. bottom values in the outer circle of Figure 1).
Achievement values can be both self-expansive (expressing competence) and self-protective
(meeting social standards) as Figure 1 shows.
Table 2. Summary of Studies Examining the Relationships between Ten Basic Personal
Values and Well-being (Pearson Correlations)
Study Sample Measur
es
PO AC
H
HE
D
ST SD UN BE
N
TR
A
CO SE
Sagiv &
Schwartz
(2000)
Israeli &
German
Adults &
Students
N=1261
SWLS -.02 .04 .00 .03 -.01 -.05 .05 .02 .02 -.01
PA -.06 .10*.05 .13*.12*-.03 .05 -.08*-.12*-.13*
Israeli
Business
Students
N=40
SWLS .27*.05 -.11 -.10
PA .17 .41* -.22 -.11
Israeli
Psychology
Students
N=42
SWLS -.26*-.07 .03 .18
PA -.34*-.07 .03 .07
Oishi,
Diener, Suh,
& Lucas
US Students
N=256-271
SWLS -.07 .13*.06 .05 -.03 -.02 .03 -.01 -.04 -.06
PA .00 .24** -.03 -.03 .01 -.05 .04 -.07 -.01 -.05
(1999)
Joshanloo &
Ghaedi
(2009)
Iranian
Students
N=200
LS .04 .34** .12 .01 .06 -.15 .08 -.09 -.01 .02
Affect
balance -.05 .18*.08 -.02 .02 -.03 .04 -.22*
*.06 -.02
Haslam,
Whelan, &
Bastion
(2009)
Australian
Students
N=180
SWB -.04 .12 .13 .29** .18*.17*.16*-.10 .08 .00
Karabati, &
Cemalcilar
(2010)
Turkish
Students
N=1120
SWLS -.05 -.07*-.03 -.12*
*
-.22*
*-.10*.13** .
12*** .24** .16**
Bilbao,
Techio, &
Paez (2007)
Samples of
Spanish and
immigrant
adults and
students
(average r)
N=3664
SWLS -.09*
*.01 .16** .04*.14** .07** .15** .05*.00 .07*
Happin
ess -.05*.03 .16** .11** .17** .12** .14** .04*.02 .15**
Bobowik et
al. (2010)
Representati
ve samples
from 22
ESS
countries +
3 from
Spain
N=26778/28
042
LS
-.09
**
-.06
**
.
15*
*
.
07*
*
.
12*
*
.
03*
*
.
12*
*
-.08
**
-.06
**
-.19
**
PANA
-.07
**
-.04
**
.
11*
*
.
07*
*
.
12*
*
.
03*
*
.
06*
*
-.08
**
-.05
**
-.14
**
Sortheix &
Lönnqvist
(2014)
Representati
ve samples
from 25
ESS
countries
N=44106
LS
-.11
**
-.07
**
-.07
**
.04* -.02 .
08*
*
.
08*
*
.
12*
*
.02 -.06
**
Sortheix &
Schwartz
(2017)
Representati
ve samples
from 35
ESS
samples
LS -.09
**
-.06
**
-.07
**
.03* -.02 .
06*
*
.
06*
*
.
11*
*
.02 -.05
**
N=121495
Note. *p <. 05, **p < .01. PO = power, AC = achievement, HE = hedonism, ST = stimulation,
SD = self-direction, UN = universalism, BE = benevolence, TR = tradition, CO = conformity,
SE = security, SWLS = Satisfaction with life scale, PA = Positive affect, LS = Life satisfaction,
PANA = Positive and negative affect, SWB = Subjective well-being (LS and PANA).
These authors argued that the growth orientation and the person focus both promote SWB
(see Table 3). The growth orientation does so because it motivates self-actualizing, free
expression of own ideas, abilities, and feelings (openness to change) and self-expansion to
include concern for the welfare of others (self-transcendence). It satisfies intrinsic needs for
autonomy and relatedness. The person focus does so because it motivates expression of one’s
ideas, abilities, and feelings (openness to change) and the pursuit of personal success and gains
(self-enhancement).
In contrast, the self-protective, anxiety-control orientation and the social focus both
undermine SWB. The self-protective orientation reflects a need to avoid threat and anxiety. It
undermines well-being because it motivates subordinating the self to socially imposed
expectations (conservation) or asserting control and dominance to overcome anxiety (self-
enhancement). It is directed toward extrinsic needs for approval and status. The social focus
undermines well-being because it draws attention to needs and problems of others that may
arouse concern and worry (self-transcendence) and to social expectations and obligations that
limit autonomy (conservation).
Table 3. Theorized associations between values and SWB based on crossing two sets of
motivations underlying values: growth versus self-protection and person versus social focus.
Motivational Bases of Values
and their Effects on SWB Growth—Positive (+) Self-Protection—
Negative (-)
Person Focus—Positive (+)
Hedonism, Stimulation,
Self-Direction
(+ +)
Power
Achievement
(+ -)
Social Focus—Negative (-)
Benevolence
Universalism
(+ -)
Security, Tradition
Conformity
(- -)
Note: (++) = positive associations; (--) = negative associations; (+-) and (-+) = complex
associations (Sortheix & Schwartz, 2017).
Based on this theorizing, Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) hypothesized: (a) Openness to
change values, which combine a growth orientation and person focus, relate positively to SWB.
(b) Conservation values, which combine a self-protection orientation and social focus, relate
negatively to SWB.
The combinations of orientation and focus that underlie self-enhancement values and
self-transcendence values have opposing implications for SWB (see Table 3). For achievement
values, Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) expected no direct association with SWB across countries.
They anticipated a trade-off between the self-protective interest in the extrinsic goal of approval
by others and the person focus on the intrinsic goal of developing one’s competence. They
assumed that the opposing implications of the self-protection orientation and the person focus for
SWB would be approximately balanced.
For power values, they expected a negative association with SWB because the negative
implications would outweigh the positive. The self-protection goal of overcoming threats to
one’s status implies a sense of anxiety and need to control that may undermine SWB. Moreover,
the positive implications of the person focus—free self-expression—would be neutralized by
negative implications of pursuing self-interested dominance and wealth. These are extrinsic goals
(Ryan & Deci, 2001) whose pursuit increases anxiety (Kasser & Ryan, 1996) and often elicit
negative reactions from others.
For universalism values, they expected no association with SWB. They assumed that the
negative implications of concern for the often-intractable problems of vulnerable out-group
members and the wider society (social focus) would neutralize the positive implications of the
self-expansive transcendence of personal interests (growth).
For benevolence values, they expected a positive association with SWB. They assumed
that the negative implications of concern for the welfare of close others (social focus) are offset
by its positive outcomes. This concern often motivates actions that promote good relations with
family and friends and satisfy relatedness needs (Deci & Ryan, 1995). The positive implications
of the self-expansive transcendence of personal interests (growth) should outweigh any weak
negative implications of concern for close others (social focus).
Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) analyzed representative data from three rounds of the
European Social Survey. Multilevel analyses across 35 countries confirmed their expectations for
the associations of all ten values with a life-satisfaction index of SWB (Table 2). This supported
their theorizing that what determines a value’s association with SWB is its combination of
growth- versus protection-orientation and person- versus social-focus.
Second Generation Studies: Contextual Moderation
Three recent studies sought to explain variation across samples in the direct relations of
values with SWB. These studies introduced contextual variables as moderators that might
explain this variation. We present each study in turn.
Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2014) analyzed data from 25 representative national samples of
the European Social Survey. They drew upon Schwartz’ (1992) distinction between the social
and person focus of values. Social-focused values (universalism, benevolence, tradition,
conformity and security) primarily regulate how we relate socially to others and affect their
interests (on the right in Figure 1). Person-focused values (self-direction, stimulation, hedonism,
achievement, and power) primarily regulate how we express personal interests and
characteristics (on the left in Figure 1). Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2014) assumed that values’
association to SWB depend on how well values function to help individuals cope with their
environment. Different environmental contexts provide different opportunities and impose
different affordances or constraints on the successful pursuit of valued goals (Gibson, 1977).
They proposed that the level of human development in a country (HDI: affluence, health, and
education) moderates direct value—life satisfaction (LS) relations.
For social-focused values, Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2014) argued that high HDI contexts
(e.g. Norway) provide more resources for and fewer constraints against attaining their goals (e.g.,
equality, harmony). Moreover, these values motivate prosocial behavior and contribute to social
capital and trust (Helliwell & Putnam, 2004; Devos, Spini, & Schwartz, 2002). They further
argued that low HDI contexts (e.g., Ukraine) provide few resources for attaining social harmony
and constrain people to look out for and protect their own interests and the interests of close
others. Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2014) noted two exceptions. They argued that security values are
primarily concerned with self-protection, despite their social focus. Moreover, benevolence
values express concern for close others that motivates people to help family and friends and
promote harmony under all conditions. They hypothesized: (a) In low HDI countries, social
focused-values (except benevolence) relate negatively to SWB. (b) In high HDI countries, social-
focused values (except security) relate positively to SWB.
For person-focused values, Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2014) distinguished between
openness to change and self-enhancement values. They noted that openness to change values
satisfy autonomy needs and consistently related to higher well-being. They argued that openness
to change values motivate behaviors that are especially instrumental for adapting to the difficult
and unpredictable living conditions of low HDI environments. They further argued that power
and achievement values motivate competitive, self-assertive behavior that conflicts with the
values in high HDI contexts but could be especially instrumental for coping with the limited
resources in low HDI contexts. They, therefore, hypothesized: (a) Openness to change values
relate positively to SWB across countries, but more so in low HDI countries. (b) Self-
enhancement values relate negatively to SWB in high HDI countries but positively in low HDI
countries.
Across all countries, without considering moderation by HDI, Sortheix and Lönnqvist
(2014) found that benevolence, self-direction, stimulation, and hedonism related positively to life
satisfaction whereas power, security, and conformity related negatively (Table 2). They tested
their two moderation hypotheses using the cross-level interactions between HDI and values in
predicting SWB. All interactions were significant except for benevolence and hedonism values.
They correlated positively with SWB across countries, regardless of HDI levels. In low HDI
countries, as hypothesized, the social-focused values (except benevolence) related negatively to
SWB. In high HDI countries, as hypothesized, the social-focused values (except security) related
positively to SWB, but the universalism and conformity correlations were not significant.
Openness to change values related positively to SWB across countries, more so in low HDI
countries, as hypothesized. Self-enhancement values related negatively to SWB in high HDI
countries, and achievement related positively in low HDI countries. Contrary to the hypothesis,
power values related negatively to SWB in low HDI countries. This study demonstrated that the
socioeconomic context moderated eight of the ten associations between values and SWB.
However, several findings did not fit the theorizing based on the social versus person focus,
especially in high HDI contexts.
In their study presented above, Sortheix and Schwartz (2017; Schwartz, 2012) also
addressed the variation in direct value-SWB associations across countries. They proposed that
the cultural values that prevail in a society moderate the associations. Cultural values refer to
society’s normative system of value emphases. Specifically, they proposed Cultural
Egalitarianism as the critical moderator. This cultural value emphasizes cooperative behavior out
of personal choice, thereby preserving the social fabric (Schwartz, 2006, 2014). In egalitarian
cultures, people are socialized to engage voluntarily in collaborative, productive work, based on
internalized commitments, and to view all people as morally equal.
Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) generated moderation predictions by combining an
analysis of the cultural context in societies low versus high in Cultural Egalitarianism with the
implications of pursuing person-focused versus social-focused values. They asked whether
pursuing each basic value might help compensate for what the cultural context fails to provide
and whether the cultural context could compensate for what individuals’ values cannot provide.
They argued that person-focused values confer an advantage in low egalitarian societies. Such
societies provide few resources, so, in order to succeed, individuals must exploit their own
capacities and resources and take self-assertive initiatives. This can compensate, at least partially,
for what is lacking in the non-supportive and threatening circumstances of low egalitarian
societies. They, therefore, hypothesized: In low versus high Cultural Egalitarianism societies, (a)
openness to change values relate more positively to SWB and (b) self-enhancement values relate
less negatively to SWB.
In contrast, pursuing social-focused values is less likely to motivate behavior that can
compensate for the instability, uncertainty, and selfishness that characterize low egalitarian
societies. Conservation values, grounded in anxiety, are unlikely to motivate the energy, skill,
and self-assertiveness to cope with the prevailing difficult circumstances. The needs of others,
the concern of self-transcendence values, are liable to be more intense and disturbing in low
egalitarian societies. Moreover, these values motivate harmony rather than the assertiveness and
competitiveness that can overcome a lack of resources. They therefore hypothesized: In low
versus high Cultural Egalitarianism societies, (a) conservation values relate more negatively to
SWB and (b) self-transcendence values relate less positively to SWB.
The cross-level interactions that tested these hypotheses were all significant, and they
confirmed all four hypotheses. This supported the theorizing about how person- versus social-
focused values affect the ability to cope with the difficult conditions in low egalitarian societies.
Moreover, Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) compared the strength of Cultural Egalitarianism
versus HDI as contextual moderators of value-SWB associations. The cultural variable of
Egalitarianism was the stronger moderator for nine values, though not for power.3
Boer (2017) proposed that cultural factors and environmental threats (e.g., disease, wars)
may singly or jointly moderate associations of values with affective well-being. She argued that
economic, climatic, or safety threats strengthen the associations between affective well-being and
the values that protect against threats (e.g., conservation values). Moreover, threat also interacts
with culture. Adequate cultural responses to threat (e.g., cultural embeddedness) can diminish the
impact of threat on value–well-being associations. Boer (2017) obtained some support for her
views, using country as a proxy for the level of threat and of the type of culture. She compared
small student samples from four countries. To assess her theorizing requires research that
measures threat levels and types of culture directly and uses larger, more representative samples.
Value-Environment Congruence and SWB
The second approach to understanding relations of values to SWB emphasizes the extent
to which people’s values are congruent with (i.e., similar to) those in their environment, whatever
the motivational content of their values. Sagiv and Schwartz (2000) proposed three mechanisms
through which person–environment value congruence influences SWB.
The first mechanism refers to the opportunities and constraints that the environment
affords for goal-attainment. Environmental affordances (Gibson, 1977) are likely to be more
favorable to goal-attainment when a person’s values are congruent with those prevailing in the
environment. The second mechanism refers to the positive or negative social sanctions likely to
be experienced in the environment. When people share similar values, these values take on
normative force. Expressing congruent values promotes SWB because others validate them and
provide social support. Expressing non-congruent values undermines SWB because others
disapprove or even punish. The third mechanism refers to internal value conflict, conflict within
one’s own value system that may diminish SWB. This happens if values internalized from the
environment contradict the values a person already espouses.
The studies of context as a moderator of direct value-SWB associations applied two of
these mechanisms to explain moderation effects. These studies hypothesized and tested how
variation in aspects of the context (HDI or Cultural Egalitarianism) affected the environmental
affordances that constrained or facilitated attaining the goals of particular values. The
environmental affordances moderated the associations of those values with SWB. In addition,
Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) referred to the social sanctions mechanism to explain why power
values relate negatively to SWB regardless of cultural values.
Researchers have applied the mechanisms that account for the effects of value-
environment congruence on SWB to many environments (e.g., family, friends, organization,
profession, and community). We discuss a selection of these.
Congruence with Reference Groups
Sagiv and Schwartz (2000) studied congruence between the values of business and of
psychology students with the values prevalent in the departments in which they had chosen to
study. They assumed that power values are more important than benevolence values in Israeli
business schools, but the reverse is true in psychology, an assumption supported in Gandal,
Roccas, Sagiv, and Wrzesniewski (2005). As expected, those who endorsed values congruent
with the environment had higher well-being. Endorsing power values related positively to SWB
among business students, but not among psychology students. Endorsing benevolence values
related positively to SWB among psychology students, but not business students.
Studies of business students in Singapore (Kasser & Ahuvia, 2002) and Belgium
(Vansteenkiste, Duriez, Simons, & Soenens, 2006) did not show the congruence effect, however.
These studies measured extrinsic and materialist values in the self-determination theory tradition,
not the power value. They found that business students had lower well-being the more
importance they ascribed to materialist values. The opposing findings might reflect differences in
the index of values, the actual values prevailing in the various business schools, national
cultures, or something else. Further research is needed to clarify this.
Later studies operationalized value congruence objectively, correlating individuals’
values with their group’s average values. Sortheix and Lönnqvist (2015) found that students in
Argentina, Bulgaria, and Finland whose values were more congruent with students in their
discipline (psychology or business) had higher life satisfaction and positive affect and lower
negative affect. Moreover, value congruence led to better interpersonal relationships, which
partially mediated the value congruence - SWB association. In a longitudinal study, Sortheix,
Olakivi, and Helkama (2013) found that Finns whose values were more congruent with those of
others in their small community had better psychological health.
In a Russian national sample, Khaptsova and Schwartz (2016) found greater life
satisfaction among those whose values were more congruent with others with similar socio-
demographic characteristics (age, gender, education, and ethnicity). Thus, the value congruence
effect extended even to an implicit reference group with which one did not interact directly.
Numerous organizational studies demonstrate beneficial effects on SWB of sharing
values with colleagues or with the organization. For example, perceived compatibility between
individual and organizational values was critical in predicting higher work well-being (Leiter &
Maslach, 1999). Congruence between personal values and the core values of one´s profession
may also protect against poor well-being. Social work is a profession that emphasizes
benevolence and universalism values as opposed to power and face (prestige) values (cf. NASW
Code of Ethics, 2008). Tartakovsky (2016) found that the more social workers endorsed
benevolence and universalism values and the less they endorsed power and face values, the less
exhaustion and burnout they experienced.
Value Congruence Among Marital Couples
Research using difference scores and profile correlations to measure value congruence
found that Israeli couples’ overall value similarity correlated with their relationship satisfaction
(Gaunt, 2006). A study of Finnish couples examined similarity on each of the ten values
separately (Leikas, Verkasalo, Ilmarinen, Vartiainen, & Lönnqvist, 2017). The relationships
between value similarity and relationship satisfaction depended on gender. For men, similarity
with their spouse in self-direction values related positively to satisfaction. For women, similarity
with their spouse in conformity values related negatively to satisfaction. For both men and
women, similarity with their spouse in hedonism values related positively to satisfaction.
However, value similarity mattered less for couples’ satisfaction than similarity in political
attitudes and religiosity, variables that are influenced by underlying values (Leikas et al., 2017).
Value Congruence with the Societal Culture
If individuals strongly identify with the society in which they live, we might expect that
congruence between their values and those in the society would affect SWB. Zilberfeld (2010)
assessed this possibility in a study of ultra-orthodox students in Israel. Ultra-orthodox society
emphasizes the embeddedness of the person in the group and rejects autonomous thought and
action. As expected, SWB correlated positively with endorsing conformity values and negatively
with endorsing self-direction values. This pattern of correlations did not replicate among students
from the wider Israeli society that does not share the same cultural value emphases.
On the broader societal level, however, there is little support for the hypothesis that
similarity between individuals’ values and those of their country promotes SWB. Schwartz
(2012) tested this hypothesis with data from representative samples from 32 European Social
Survey countries. Contrary to the hypothesis, congruence with the average value in the country
increased the negative correlations of conservation values with SWB and decreased the positive
correlations of openness to change values with SWB. Research on cultural estrangement also
questions the positive role of congruence between individuals’ values and those in their society.
Cultural estrangement is the inverse of congruence. It refers to the discrepancy between personal
values and perceived societal values (Bernard, Gebauer, & Maio, 2006). The congruence
hypothesis would predict a negative association between cultural estrangement and mental health
indicators. However, Bernard et al. (2006) found no association between value discrepancies and
psychological outcomes.
Perhaps value congruence with one’s society enhances SWB only when individuals are
aware of the prevailing cultural values and identify strongly with the society. This is a topic for
future research.
Caveat
A problem overlooked in some similarity studies is that apparent similarity effects may be
due to the value scores of the individuals and/or groups compared. To uncover unique effects of
similarity requires first controlling for the actual value scores. Such controls may eliminate
similarity effects (e.g., Dyrenforth, Kashy, Donnellan, & Lucas, 2010) or leave them unchanged
(e.g., Khaptsova & Schwartz, 2016; Sortheix & Lönnqvist, 2015).
Attaining Valued Goals and SWB
This third approach is concerned with the process through which values lead to SWB. It
proposes that the link between a value and SWB depends upon the successful attainment of the
goals the value motivates a person to pursue. Unlike the healthy values approach, it does not
assume that holding particular values (e.g., self-direction) is conducive to SWB. Rather, it
assumes that people experience high SWB to the extent they attain the goals they. Some evidence
suggests that simply attaining a valued goal is sufficient to enhance SWB (e.g., Oishi, Diener,
Suh, & Lucas, 1999). Other evidence suggests that the effect on SWB depends on the nature of
the goal that is attained (e.g., Sheldon & Kasser, 1998).
In a study by Oishi et al. (1999), students reported daily for 23 days how satisfied they
were with their achievements and their social life. They also reported whether it was a good or
bad day. Subsequently, they completed a measure of the importance they ascribe to the ten basic
values. Students’ goal attainment (indicated by their daily satisfaction with their achievements
and social life) predicted their daily well-being. Except for achievement, their values did not
(Table 2). But their values did moderate the effect of goal attainment. Satisfaction with
achievements predicted well-being more strongly for those high in achievement values than for
those low in achievement values. Likewise, satisfaction with their social life predicted well-being
more strongly for those high in benevolence values than for those low in benevolence values.
Another analysis showed that various activities were more or less satisfying to the extent
that they were consistent with individual’s important values (Oishi et al. 1999). For example, the
more people valued power, the more satisfaction they gained from performing power-related
activities (e.g., buying expensive clothes). Findings were similar for the effects of achievement,
universalism, benevolence, and stimulation/hedonism values on the satisfaction gained from their
related activities. Moreover, the more importance individuals attributed to a value, the less
satisfaction they gained from activities related to the values on the opposite side of the value
circle. For example, the more people valued universalism, the less satisfaction they gained from
power-related activities. The third analysis in Oishi et al. (1999) showed that values also
moderated relations between satisfaction in particular life domains and global life satisfaction.
For example, satisfaction with grades related more strongly to life satisfaction the more
important achievement values.
In sum, values led to increased SWB when the goals they motivated were successfully
attained. This process applied to all values in Oishi et al. (1999). However, other research
suggests that not all goal attainment is beneficial for SWB. Most of this research is grounded in
self-determination theory and does not measure the ten basic values explicitly, but we can
nonetheless extrapolate from it.
In three studies of adolescents and young adults, Kasser and Ryan (1993) examined
associations of individuals’ aspirations with various indexes of well-being. Aspirations for
financial success (i.e., power values) related negatively to well-being, fitting the healthy values
expectation. Significantly for the goal attainment approach, anticipating attainment of the desired
financial success also led to poorer well-being. In a diary study of students, Sheldon and Kasser
(1998) examined whether students’ goal progress during a semester promoted their well-being.
Those who made more progress toward their goals experienced increased SWB over time.
However, this occurred only if the goals they pursued were high in self-determination and/or in
intrinsic orientation. Such goals largely express growth values. Attaining extrinsic (power-
related) goals did not affect SWB. Kasser and Ryan (2001) reported similar findings. They found
that attainment of intrinsic goals related to increased well-being, but the attainment of extrinsic
goals did not.
The different outcomes of attaining achievement and power-related goals in Oishi et al.
(1999) and the research based on the self-determination theory discussed above (see also, Kasser
& Ahuvia, 2002; Vansteenkiste et al., 2006) points to the need for further research. Such research
could measure both the ten basic values and the intrinsic/extrinsic values explicitly. It could
identify the circumstances under which each value motivates goals whose attainment does or
does not promote SWB. It would also be instructive to examine relations between the ten basic
values and people’s intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations.
Future Research
Moderation and Mediation Studies
A clear message of this chapter is that value-SWB associations vary across contexts. We
saw that both a socioeconomic (HDI) and a cultural (Cultural Egalitarianism) characteristic of
societies moderated these associations. Other potential societal moderators also merit
investigation. For example, researchers might consider levels of democracy, social expenditure,
corruption, and rural/urban differences, on the one hand, and cultural values like embeddedness,
hierarchy, and harmony, on the other. Studies in non-Western societies might reveal other
patterns of moderation. Theoretical analyses could suggest how espousing each value might
enhance or undermine successful coping with the environmental demands, constraints,
opportunities, and sanctions associated with variation on potential moderators.
Less research attention has been paid to characteristics of individuals that may moderate
value-SWB associations. One potential moderator for which there is some support is peoples
social identities (Sagiv, Roccas, & Oppenheim-Weller, 2015). Identities provide different
opportunities and constraints for attaining valued goals. Whether particular values are likely to
promote SWB may depend, therefore, on which of a person’s social identities he or she is
enacting. For example, endorsing power values may enhance SWB for a person when enacting
her manager identity but undermine SWB when enacting her friend identity. Identities may also
mediate relations of values to SWB. Espousing values consonant with a social identity (e.g., self-
direction values, but not conformity values, with an artist identity) may enhance SWB by
validating the identity. Links between values, identities, and SWB are a fruitful domain for future
research. Studies of self-esteem suggest that internal conflict or confusion between the values
one espouses in different contexts may also damage SWB (e.g., Daniel, Boehnke, & Knafo-
Noam 2016).
No research we found treated age or gender as possible moderators of value-SWB
associations. We analyzed data from the representative samples from 27 countries in the first
three rounds of the European Social Survey to assess whether considering these ubiquitous
individual characteristics might be worthwhile. We categorized age into four groups: 15-29, 30-
45, 45-65, and 65+ years. Correlations of values and SWB revealed monotonic trends across age
groups for eight of the ten values. With increasing age, openness to change values correlated
more positively with SWB, whereas conservation and self-enhancement values correlated more
negatively. Although these findings are based on cross-sectional data, they suggest that
theorizing about why age acts as a moderator assessing possible explanations is worthwhile.
Findings for gender also revealed significant moderation. Openness to change and self-
transcendence values correlated more positively with SWB among women than among men,
whereas conservation and power values correlated more negatively. So, moderation by gender
also deserves further study.
Taken together, two cross-national studies suggest that emotions may mediate relations of
values to SWB. One study revealed that people’s values influence the emotions they desire to
feel (Tamir, et al. 2016). For example, people who endorse benevolence values want to feel more
empathy or love and people who endorse power values want to feel more anger or hatred. The
second study (Tamir, Schwartz, Oishi, & Kim, 2017) found that people who experience the
emotions they desire to experience, whether pleasant (e.g., empathy or love) emotions or
unpleasant (e.g., anger or hatred) emotions, are happier. Thus, people are happier the more they
experience the emotions that their own values promote, whatever the content of their values.
We found only one study that examined both values and personality traits together as
predictors of SWB. Haslam, Whelan, and Bastian (2009) found that one or more Big 5 traits
fully mediated relations of four values to SWB in a sample of Australian students. For example,
extraversion mediated relations of stimulation and self-direction with SWB. Further studies
should examine joint effects of values and traits in larger and more representative samples. Might
there be additive effects? Or might they interact in their effects on SWB?
Causality in Value-SWB Relations
Throughout this chapter, we have spoken of relations or associations between values and
SWB. Long-term longitudinal studies are required to establish whether value change affects
subsequent change in SWB and/or vice versa. Because values change quite slowly and only in
response to major changes in life conditions whereas SWB is more volatile, it seems more
plausible to expect causality to flow primarily from values to SWB. Three studies by Kasser et
al. (2014) examined change in SWB and materialism values (related to power value) in different
age groups and cultures. In all three studies, decreases in materialism values led to increased
SWB whereas increases in materialism lead to decreased SWB. Moreover, changes in well-being
predicted changes in materialism. The SWB to materialism effect was weaker, as our reasoning
suggests, but it does indicate some reciprocal causality.
Experimental studies have examined causality in short-term relations between various
types of well-being and materialism values. Some primed materialist values and observed the
impact on well-being (e.g., study 4 in Kasser et al., 2014), others manipulated well-being and
observed the impact on state materialism (e.g., Solberg, Diener, & Robinson, 2004). The findings
support reciprocal causality between values and SWB.
These studies all examined materialism values. It is important to study value-SWB
causality for other values both longitudinally and experimentally. The strength and direction of
causality between values and SWB may depend upon the specific values in question.
Different Aspects of SWB
In reviewing the various studies, we have not distinguished among different aspects of
SWB. Although values usually relate to most aspects of SWB similarly, there may be some
systematic differences worth studying. For example, social well-being, defined as a sense of
integration, cohesion, acceptance, and contribution to society (Keyes, 1998), correlated
positively with conformity and negatively with self-direction among Iranian students (Joshanloo
& Ghaedi, 2009). These findings may reflect unique characteristics of this sample or of Iran.
However, it also possible that the relationships of conformity and self-direction values to the
social aspects of SWB differ from relations to other aspects of SWB in some conditions.
The studies of value-SWB associations we reviewed investigate satisfaction with life,
happiness, and positive and negative affect. These SWB indicators correlate positively with
meaning in life, but the association is far from perfect (Baumeister, 1991; Oishi & Diener, 2014).
Meaning is an important aspect of well-being. Research on relations of the basic values to
meaning in life and on the environmental and individual characteristics that moderate them
would provide an important addition to our understanding of how values affect the quality of life.
Recent Value Developments
We have focused on the ten basic values identified by Schwartz (1992). Recent theorizing
and research (Schwartz et al., 2012; Schwartz, 2017) has refined these values and introduced two
others (humility and face) that are recognized across cultures that may yield worthwhile new
insights.
Implications for National SWB
National levels of SWB have become a tool to evaluate the progress of countries and to
inform public policy (Diener, Lucas, Schimmack, & Helliwell, 2009). Most countries with the
highest levels of SWB are in Scandinavia and West-Europe (Helliwell, Layard, & Sachs, 2012).
These countries provide a favorable context of high Cultural Egalitarianism and socio-economic
level. Countries with low levels of SWB tend to be low in Cultural Egalitarianism and
socioeconomic level. Our findings imply that change in basic values would have different
consequences for national SWB in more versus less favorable contexts. Increased emphases on
self-transcendence and conservation values may enhance SWB in favorable contexts but may
weaken SWB in unfavorable contexts. Increased emphases on self-enhancement values may
weaken SWB in favorable contexts, but in less favorable contexts, achievement values could
even enhance SWB. It is, therefore, necessary to consider the context when anticipating the kinds
of value change that may help to improve people’s well-being.
Conclusion
The literature reviewed here suggests that some values are healthier than others, though
not under all circumstances. Theorizing based on combining the social- versus person-focus and
the growth versus self-protection orientation of values explains direct value-SWB associations
well. However, these associations vary across context. The context may refer to family, friends,
neighborhood, organization, demographic peers, society, and more. Sharing values (congruence)
with those in close social contexts enhances SWB. In both close and broad societal contexts, the
opportunities and constraints the environment provides for attaining valued goals moderate
value-SWB associations. We can understand variation across cultures and countries in the
associations of particular values with SWB by identifying characteristics of the context that
promote or block attainment of these values. With the recent flourishing of research on value-
SWB relations, we anticipate many new insights.
Footnotes
1Recently, Schwartz and colleagues (Schwartz, et al. 2012; Schwartz, 2017) split the same circle
into 19 more narrowly defined values in their refined value theory.
2We discuss the exceptions for business and psychology students in Sagiv and Schwartz (2000)
below, under ‘value-environment congruence’.
3This study also examined associations between values and depressive affect. Hypotheses were
the mirror opposite of those for life satisfaction. All main effect hypotheses were confirmed.
Cultural Egalitarianism moderated the associations with depressive affect for only four values in
the predicted manner, however.
References
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York: Guilford Press.
Bergin, A. E. (1991). Values and religious issues in psychotherapy and mental health. American
Psychologist, 46, 394-403.
Bernard, M. M., Gebauer, J. E., & Maio, G. R. (2006). Cultural estrangement: The role of
personal and societal value discrepancies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 78-92.
Bilbao, M. A., Techio, E. M., & Paez, D. (2007). Felicidad, cultura y valores personales: Estado
de la cuestión y síntesis meta-analítica. [Happiness, culture and personal values: Current
situation and meta-analytic synthesis]. Revista de Psicología –Lima, 25, 223–276.
Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Values and personality. European Journal of Personality, 8,
163-181.
Bobowik, M., Basabe, N., Páez, D., Jiménez, A., & Bilbao, M. Á. (2011). Personal values and
well-being among Europeans, Spanish natives and immigrants to Spain: Does the culture
matter? Journal of Happiness Studies, 12, 401-419.
Boer, D. (2017). Values and affective well-being: How culture and environmental threat
influence their association. In S. Roccas & L. Sagiv (Eds.), Values and behavior: Taking a cross-
cultural perspective.
Daniel, E., Boehnke, K., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2016). Value-differentiation and self-esteem among
majority and immigrant youth. Journal of Moral Education, 45, 338-353.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy. In Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp.
31-49). US: Springer.
Devos, T., Spini, D., & Schwartz, S. H. (2002). Conflicts among human values and trust in
institutions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 481-494.
Diener, E., Lucas, R., Schimmack, U., & Helliwell, J. (2009). Well-being for public policy. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Dyrenforth, P. S., Kashy, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Lucas, R. E. (2010). Predicting relationship
and life satisfaction from personality in nationally representative samples from three countries:
The relative importance of actor, partner, and similarity effects. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 99, 690-702.
Feather, N. T. (1995). Values, valences, and choice: The influence of values on the perceived
attractiveness and choice of alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
1135-1151.
Gandal, N., Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., & Wrzesniewski, A. (2005). Personal value priorities of
economists. Human Relations, 58, 1227-1252.
Gaunt, R. (2006). Couple similarity and marital satisfaction: Are similar spouses
happier?. Journal of Personality, 74, 1401-1420.
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving,
acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 67-82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Haslam, N., Whelan, J., & Bastian, B. (2009). Big Five traits mediate associations between
values and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 40-42.
Helliwell, J., Layard, R., & Sachs, J. (2012). World happiness report.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well-being. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1435.
Hitlin, S. (2003).Values as the core of personal identity: Drawing links between two theories of
self. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 118–137.
Joshanloo, M., & Ghaedi, G. (2009). Value priorities as predictors of hedonic and eudaimonic
aspects of well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 294-298.
Karabati, S., & Cemalcilar, Z. (2010). Values, materialism, and well-being: A study with Turkish
university students. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 624-633.
Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well‐being in business
students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 137-146.
Kasser, T., Rosenblum, K. L., Sameroff, A. J., Deci, E. L.,….Hawks, S. (2014). Changes in
materialism, changes in psychological well-being: Evidence from three longitudinal studies and
an intervention experiment. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 1-22.
Kasser, T. , & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial
success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410-422.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates
of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280-287.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Be careful what you wish for: Optimal functioning and the
relative attainment of intrinsic and extrinsic goals (116-131). In P. Schmuck and K. M. Sheldon
(Eds.), Life goals and well-being: Towards a positive psychology of human striving. Lengerich,
Germany: Pabst Science.
Keyes, C. L. M. (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61 (2), 121-140.
Khaptsova, A., & Schwartz, S. H. (2016). Life satisfaction and value congruence: Moderators
and extension to constructed socio-demographic groups in a Russian national sample. Social
Psychology, 47, 163-173.
Kluckhohn, C. (1951). Values and valueorientations in the theory of action: An exploration in
definition and classification. In T. Parsons & E. Shils (Eds.), Toward a general theory of
action (pp.388433). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Leikas, S., Verkasalo, M., Ilmarinen, V.-J., Vartiainen, H.L. and Lönnqvist J.-E. (2017). Couple
satisfaction and similarity of personality traits, personal values, and attitudes. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Leiter, M. P., & Maslach, C. (1999). Six areas of work life: A model of the organizational context
of burnout. Journal of Health and Human Services Administration, 21, 472-489.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370–396.
NASW Code of Ethics (2008). Retrieved from:
https://www.socialworkers.org/pubs/code/code.asp.
Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2014). Residents of poor nations have a greater sense of meaning in life
than residents of wealthy nations. Psychological Science, 25, 422–430.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Suh, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Value as a moderator in subjective well-
being. Journal of Personality, 24, 1319–1331.
Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1998). The measurement of values and
individualism-collectivism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1177–1189.
Rohan, M. J. (2000). A rose by any name? The values construct. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 4, 255–277.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free press.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
Sagiv, L., Roccas, S., & Hazan, O. (2004). Value pathways to well‐being: Healthy values, valued
goal attainment, and environmental congruence. In P.A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive
psychology in practice (pp. 68–85). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Sagiv, L., Roccas, S., & Oppenheim‐Weller, S. (2015). Values and well-being. In S. Joseph
(Ed.), Positive psychology in practice: Promoting human flourishing in work, health, education,
and everyday life, second edition (pp. 103–120). New Jersey: Wiley.
Sagiv, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (2000). Value priorities and subjective well-being: Direct relations
and congruity effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 177-198.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical
tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
25) (pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (2006). Les valeurs de base de la personne: Théorie, mesures et applications
[Basic human values: Theory, measurement, and applications]. Revue Française de Sociologie,
47, 249-288.
Schwartz, S. H. (2012, November). Do personal values underlie or undermine happiness?
Cultural effects. Paper presented at the European Social Survey International Conference,
Nicosia, Cyprus.
Schwartz, S. H. (2014). National culture as value orientations: Consequences of value
differences and cultural distance. In V. Ginsburgh & D. Throsby (Eds.), Handbook of the
economics of art and culture, vol.2 (pp. 547-586). Elsevier/North Holland.
Schwartz, S. H. (2015). Basic individual values: Sources and consequences. In D. Sander and T.
Brosch (Eds.), Handbook of value (pp.63-84). Oxford: UK, Oxford University Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (2017). The refined theory of basic values. In S. Roccas & L. Sagiv
(Eds.), Values and behavior: Taking a cross-cultural perspective (pp. 51-72). Cham,
Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Vecchione, M., Davidov, E., Fischer, R., Beierlein, C., ... &
Dirilen-Gumus, O. (2012). Refining the theory of basic individual values. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 103(4), 663.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress, but not all
progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319-1331.
Solberg, E. G., Diener, E., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why are materialists less satisfied?. In T.
Kasser, and A. Kanner (Eds.). Psychology and consumer culture: The struggle for a good life in
a materialistic world (pp. 29—48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sortheix, F. M., & Lönnqvist, J. E. (2014). Personal value priorities and life satisfaction in
Europe: The moderating role of socioeconomic development. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 45(2), 282-299.
Sortheix, F. M., & Lönnqvist, J. E. (2015). Person‐group value congruence and subjective well‐
being in students from Argentina, Bulgaria and Finland: The role of interpersonal
relationships. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 25(1), 34-48.
Sortheix, F. M., Olakivi, A., & Helkama, K. (2013). Values, life events, and health: A study in a
Finnish rural community. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 23(4), 331-346.
Sortheix, F. M., & Schwartz, S. H. (2017). Values that underlie and undermine well‐being:
Variability across countries. European Journal of Personality, 31(2), 187-201.
Strupp, H. H. (1980). Humanism and psychotherapy: A personal statement of the therapist's
essential values. Psychotherapy, 17, 396-400.
Tamir, M., Schwartz, S. H., Cieciuch, J., Riediger, M., Torres, C., Scollon, C., Dzokoto, V., Zhou,
X., & Vishkin, A. (2016). Desired emotions across cultures: A value-based account. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 67-82.
Tamir, M., Schwartz, S. H., Oishi, S., Kim, M. Y. (2017). The secret to happiness: Feeling good
or feeling right? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(10), 1448-1459.
Tartakovsky, E. (2016). Personal value preferences and burnout of social workers. Journal of
Social Work, 16(6), 657-673.
Vansteenkiste, M., Duriez, B., Simons, J., & Soenens, B. (2006). Materialistic values and well‐
being among business students: Further evidence of their detrimental effect. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 36(12), 2892-2908.
Zilberfeld, T. (2010). Person-culture fit and subjective well-being among ultra-orthodox and
secular Israelis. Unpublished master’s thesis, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel.
2018 Ed Diener. Copyright Creative Commons: Attribution,
noncommercial, no derivatives
... -объяснение прямой взаимосвязи между ценностями и субъективным благополучием [13]; ...
... -соответствие ценностей конкретной личности с ценностями социального окружения (различного порядка) как фактор субъективного благополучия [13]; ...
... -достижение значимых целей в качестве источника субъективного благополучия [13]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction. The well-being of student youth is in the focus of the tasks of state programs of educational and youth policy. The international socio-political situation poses the tasks of actualization and formation of value orientations of the young generation. In modern psychological and pedagogical science, a significant number of studies are devoted to the problem of well-being, its correlates and risks. However, relatively few works are devoted to the topic of correlation between self-esteem as a component of psychological well-being and value orientations. The aim of the research is to study the values of modern Russian students in the context of the specifics of self-esteem. Materials and Methods. The study involved 2,315 university students from 10 cities of the Russian Federation. Self-esteem was analyzed using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in the adaptation of A. A. Zolotareva, and the Schwartz Value Questionnaire (PVQ-R2) was used to study students’ axiological orientations. The correlations between self-esteem and value orientations among students were determined using by correlation analysis using the Spearman method. In order to identify differences between groups with low and high levels of self-esteem (self-esteem), Student’s t-test for independent samples and Mann-Whitney U-criterion was used to test the consistency of the results. Results. Data analysis has shown that most values, except for “Power – Dominance”, “Power – Resources” and “Face”, have a significant correlation with self-esteem. The majority of these values have a positive correlation, while a negative correlation was found between “Humility”, “Conformity – interpersonal”, and self-esteem. In other words, in most cases, it can be said that the higher a student’s self-esteem, the more prominent their value orientations are. These correlations correspond to significant differences in the expression of values among students with relatively high and low levels of self-esteem. Discussion and Conclusion. The conducted research deepens our understanding not only of the specifics of the modern Russian students’ axiological positions, but also of how the indicator of subjective well-being, measured by the level of self-esteem, relates to them. The results obtained can be used as part of the development of educational concepts, value-based motivational strategies, for working with at-risk students, and designing adaptation and development programs.
... Growth-oriented values -such as self-direction, stimulation, benevolence, and universalism -enhance well-being by fostering intrinsic motivation and personal development. Conversely, self-protective values -including tradition, conformity, security, and power -are often associated with lower well-being (Bilsky and Schwartz 1994;Sagiv and Schwartz 2000;Schwartz and Sortheix 2018). Sortheix and Schwartz (2017) found that only personal-focused growth values (self-direction, stimulation) enhanced SWB, whereas social-focused and self-protection values (tradition, conformity, security) diminished it. ...
Article
Full-text available
Human values (HV) and subjective well-being (SWB) are critical factors influencing firm performance and entrepreneurial success. While prior research has examined HV and SWB independently among entrepreneurs, the relationship between these factors remains underexplored, particularly across sectors with varying levels of technological intensity. Drawing on Schwartz’s Theory of Basic Human Values, this study investigates how specific value configurations relate to entrepreneurs’ SWB in low-, medium-, and high-technology sectors. Using data from 527 entrepreneurs across 27 European countries and applying fuzzy Qualitative Comparative Analysis (fsQCA), we identify distinct, sector-specific patterns in the relationship between HV configurations and high levels of SWB. Our findings reveal that entrepreneurs in high-technology sectors exhibit a stronger association between self-focused values (e.g., hedonism) and SWB, whereas those in low-technology sectors show a stronger link between growth-oriented values (e.g., self-transcendence) and SWB. Additionally, entrepreneurs in low- and medium-technology sectors achieve high SWB through more diverse value combinations than their high-technology counterparts. These findings advance Schwartz’s theory by highlighting the importance of aligning values with sectoral contexts to enhance entrepreneurial well-being. The results provide a foundation for developing targeted, sector-specific interventions to improve entrepreneurial satisfaction and foster success.
... Furthermore, this study addresses two more issues. It points to the regime effect as a possible contextual mechanism linking values and SWB and thus contributes to the discussion in Schwartz and Sortheix (2018). It also continues the debate about the universality of SWB predictors across countries (Gardiner et al., 2020;Zagrebina et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has often demonstrated that liberal values and democratic regimes are associated with higher levels of subjective well-being (SWB). However, consistent results are only found at the country level, while at the individual level the relationship between values, democracy, and SWB is not as clear. This article analyzes recent data from 86 countries and shows that individual SWB depends on whether one’s values match the country’s predominant values and political system. In authoritarian countries, those with more conservative values tend to exhibit higher levels of SWB, reaching a level of happiness comparable to that of an average person in a typical democracy. Conversely, their liberal-minded compatriots often report significantly lower levels of SWB. In democracies, people with more liberal values tend to have higher SWB than do conservatives, although this difference is not as robust as in autocracies. This study emphasizes the importance of political context in the relationship between liberal values and SWB.
... Consumption among Muslim consumer is not only influenced by religiosity and spirituality, but also individual characteristics also determine the values people give importance and consider them worth pursuing in their life to achieve satisfaction. (Diener et al., 2018;Schwartz and Sortheix, 2018). The consideration of halal and haram as key Islamic value differentiate the Islamic consumption, which promotes larger interest and well-being of the society. ...
... Though conservation and self-transcendence values have a common core, they are essentially different. Conservation is much more centred on self-protection, guaranteed by the preservation of the status quo, while self-transcendence aligns more with the existential need for relatedness and is centred on motivations for self-expansion and growth (Schwartz & Sortheix, 2018). In other words, conservation values are self-protective because they comply with the need to avoid conflicts, unpredictability, and changes; on the other side, self-transcendence values adhere to the need for relatedness, emphasizing the concern for the welfare of others . ...
... Eğer bireyler olumlu ve kişisel gelişeme götüren değerlere sahipse, iyi oluş hallerinin daha yüksek olduğu gösterilmiştir. Fakat olumsuz ve kişisel gelişime mani değerlere sahip olmakta mümkündür ve bu değerler insanların iyi oluş haline olumsuz etkide bulunmaktadır (Schwartz ve Sortheix, 2018). Daha önce yapılmış araştırmalar kadın ve erkeklerin hayata bakışlarında ve değerlerinde önemli farklar olduğunu ve bu farkların da öznel iyi oluş hallerini etkilediğini göstermektedir (Schwartz ve Rubel, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışmada, Türkiye sosyo-ekonomik bağlamında kadınların ve erkeklerin mutluluğu arasında belirgin bir fark olup olmadığı araştırılmaktadır. Bu amaçla ile 2004 ile 2022 yılları arasındaki dönemi kapsayan Yaşam Memnuniyeti Anketi'nden (YMA) birey düzeyinde veriler kullanılmaktadır. YMA, 2004 ile 2022 yılları arasındaki 19 yıllık süreyi kapsayarak 344.000'den fazla gözlem içermekte ve ulusal temsil yeteneğine sahip bir ankettir. Bu kapsamlı veriye dayanan sonuçlarımız, birçok arka plan değişkenini kontrol ettikten sonra, kadınların mutluluğunun belirgin şekilde erkeklerden daha yüksek olduğunu göstermektedir. Cinsiyetler arasındaki mutluluk farkının kaynağını anlamak için, hayatta önem verilen değerler de incelenmiştir. Kadınlar ve erkeklerin kendilerini en mutlu ettiğini belirttiği değer incelendiğinde de belirgin farklar görünmektedir. Erkekler başarı, iş ve paraya kadınlardan daha fazla önem verirken, kadınlar sağlık ve sevgiye erkeklerden daha fazla önem vermektedir. Çalışmanın önemli bulgularından biri, her bir anket yılında kadınların yaklaşık %80'inin cinsiyetinden dolayı üzerinde sosyal baskı hissetmediğini belirtmesidir. Bu sonuç, sanılanın aksine Türkiye'nin çok da cinsiyetçi bir ülke olmadığını göstermektedir. Bununla beraber, kadınların cinsiyetleri nedeniyle baskı hissediyor olma ihtimali erkeklerden daha yüksektir. Bu da kadınların mutluluğunun bir miktar sosyal baskıya rağmen daha yüksek olduğunu göstermektedir.
... Governmental institutions have been asserting a persistent need to address mental health in organizations with employerprovided interventions (Dewa & McDaid, 2010). Organizations have reasons to pursue better employee well-being, as it is associated with better performance (Nielsen et al., 2017), alignment with organizational values and goals (Schwartz & Sortheix, 2018), higher employee engagement, productivity, creativity, and lower employee turnover (Matthews et al., 2022). Organizations are increasingly adopting interventions to enhance employee well-being, mental health, and job performance (Fox et al., 2022) and recently began to explore mobile solutions as an alternative to traditional interventions . ...
Article
Full-text available
Employee well-being has become a significant management concern for employers who seek to improve employee performance and retention. Mobile apps can provide a convenient solution with readily available, adaptable, and employee-driven tools to address employee well-being and mental health issues. This article presents a meta-analysis of 18 empirical studies that examined the effectiveness of mobile apps provided by employers for reducing stress, anxiety, and depression and improving overall well-being among employees. The study compared psychological and physical app orientations. The findings indicate that mobile apps are effective in reducing stress and depression and improving overall well-being. Mobile mental health solutions should be tailored to specific audiences based on variations in effectiveness by gender and age. This article provides recommendations for selecting and adapting mobile solutions to fit the organizational context.
... En los valores, y más concretamente, en la moral de la persona, se observan algunos elementos, como el papel del afecto, y la necesidad de actuar conforme a una serie de criterios, que también están presentes en los modelos de bienestar humano descritos anteriormente. En efecto, se han encontrado evidencias de que los valores de la persona pueden influir en el estado de salud de la persona, tanto física como mental, y en el bienestar (Kristiansen, 1985;Schwartz y Sortheix, 2018), por lo que, especialmente en los últimos años, se han desarrollado terapias donde los valores de la persona juegan un papel muy relevante. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although Moral Foundations Theory is possibly the best-known recent model on human values and morals, the relationship between moral foundations and well-being is a topic that has barely been investigated. This study quantitatively reviews all published information about the relationship between moral foundations and well-being, both hedonic and eudaimonic. The review focused on hedonic well-being has been carried out using a metanalytic approach, while for eudaimonic well-being, a descriptive approach has been assumed. The results show a significant relationship between moral foundations and well-being in most of the well-being subscales included, which depends in general terms on the type of moral foundation considered. While individual foundations (Care/harm and Fairness/cheating) show a significant association with measures of eudaimonic well-being, group foundations (Loyalty/betrayal, Authority/ subversion, and Purity/degradation) show a significant association with both measures of eudaimonic well-being as well as hedonic well-being. However, the results generally depend on the moral foundation and the well-being subscale considered. Finally, the limitations found are discussed, as well as the possible application of the results obtained to future research in a therapy that includes moral foundations to improve people's well-being. ESPAÑOL Pese a que la Teoría de los Fundamentos Morales es, posiblemente, el modelo reciente más conocido sobre valores y moral humana, y sobre el que más se ha investigado, su relación con el bienestar es un tópico que apenas se ha investigado. Este estudio revisa cuantitativamente y exhaustivamente la información publicada acerca de la relación entre fundamentos morales y el bienestar, tanto hedónico como eudaimónico. La revisión centrada en el bienestar hedónico se ha realizado mediante un enfoque metanalítico, mientras que para el bienestar eudaimónico se ha asumido un enfoque descriptivo. Los resultados muestran una relación significativa entre los fundamentos morales y el bienestar, en la mayor parte de las subescalas del bienestar analizadas, que depende, en términos generales, del tipo de fundamento moral considerado. Mientras que los fundamentos individuales (cuidado/daño y justicia/engaño) muestran una asociación significativa con las medidas de bienestar eudaimónico, los fundamentos grupales (lealtad/traición, autoridad/subversión y pureza/degradación) muestran una asociación significativa tanto con las medidas de bienestar eudaimónico como con las de bienestar hedónico. No obstante, los resultados dependen, en general, del fundamento moral y de la subescala del bienestar consideradas. Se discuten las limitaciones encontradas, así como la posible aplicación de los resultados obtenidos a la investigación futura en una terapia que incluya los fundamentos morales para mejorar el bienestar de las personas.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines predictors of life satisfaction in Native Hawaiian and Pacific Islander (NH/PI) college students, who form an underrepresented group in well-being research. In a sample of 128 NH/PI students from a public university in Hawaii, self-report measures of life satisfaction, affect, stress, and mood were analyzed. The results indicate that positive affect and mood are significant positive predictors for life satisfaction, while negative affect predicts lower satisfaction. By contrast with other racial/ethnic groups, perceived stress did not significantly impact life satisfaction in NH/PI students. These findings suggest that emotional well-being is a key determinant for life satisfaction in NH/PI students, and stress is not. Culturally responsive interventions that focus on positive emotions and community support can enhance well-being and academic success in this population. This study contributes to the understanding of unique cultural influences on well-being in NH/PI students and provides implications for targeted interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Background Young adults are in the period of the formation and establishment of values. Even though previous research has revealed that personal values are important factors influencing young adults’ life satisfaction, it is still unknown when and under what circumstances values affect life satisfaction among young adults. Therefore, this study explored the relationship between personal values and life satisfaction among Chinese young adults, focusing on the mediating roles of resilience and emotional intelligence. Methods A sample of N = 635 (271 male) young adults from four universities located in Guangzhou were recruited using a cross-sectional stratified sampling method. All participants completed the Revised Portrait Value Questionnaire, Emotional Intelligence Scale, Satisfaction with Life Scale, and Brief Resilience Scale. Results Results revealed that: (1) self-transcendence (r = 0.29, p < 0.001), self-enhancement (r = 0.27, p < 0.001), openness to change (r = 0.22, p < 0.001), and conservation (r = 0.26, p < 0.001) were differentially positively associated with life satisfaction. Chain mediation analysis found that: there was a significant indirect effect for self-transcendence, via EI (ind = 0.070, p = 0.004, 95% CI = [0.027, 0.124]), and the sequential influence via EI and resilience suggested a moderate effect (ind = 0.024, p = 0.008, 95% CI = [0.009, 0.045]). For self-enhancement values exerted significantly negative indirect effects on LS via resilience (ind = −0.029, p = 0.034, 95% CI = [−0.060, −0.005]). Openness to change had a significant indirect impact on LS via EI (ind = 0.086, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.046, 0.133]), and the sequential influence via resilience and EI was significant (ind = 0.029, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [0.015, 0.050]). For conservation, the chain mediation model did not reveal any significant indirect effects via resilience or EI. Conclusion Our findings extend the current literature on personal values and life satisfaction, highlighting the significant effects of resilience and emotional intelligence on the link between personal values and life satisfaction. Overall, this research helps young adults strengthen their resilience and emotional intelligence to increase the impact of values on life satisfaction.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any references for this publication.