ArticlePublisher preview available

Remote Microphone System Use at Home: Impact on Caregiver Talk

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Purpose The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of home use of a remote microphone system (RMS) on the spoken language production of caregivers with young children who have hearing loss. Method Language Environment Analysis recorders were used with 10 families during 2 consecutive weekends (RMS weekend and No-RMS weekend). The amount of talk from a single caregiver that could be made accessible to children with hearing loss when using an RMS was estimated using Language Environment Analysis software. The total amount of caregiver talk (close and far talk) was also compared across both weekends. In addition, caregivers' perceptions of RMS use were gathered. Results Children, with the use of RMSs, could potentially have access to approximately 42% more words per day. In addition, although caregivers produced an equivalent number of words on both weekends, they tended to talk more from a distance when using the RMS than when not. Finally, caregivers reported positive perceived communication benefits of RMS use. Conclusions Findings from this investigation suggest that children with hearing loss have increased access to caregiver talk when using an RMS in the home environment. Clinical implications and future directions for research are discussed.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
JSLHR
Research Article
Remote Microphone System Use at Home:
Impact on Caregiver Talk
Carlos R. Benítez-Barrera,
a
Gina P. Angley,
a
and Anne Marie Tharpe
a
Purpose: The purpose of this study was to investigate the
effects of home use of a remote microphone system (RMS)
on the spoken language production of caregivers with young
children who have hearing loss.
Method: Language Environment Analysis recorders were
used with 10 families during 2 consecutive weekends
(RMS weekend and No-RMS weekend). The amount of
talk from a single caregiver that could be made accessible
to children with hearing loss when using an RMS was
estimated using Language Environment Analysis software.
The total amount of caregiver talk (close and far talk) was
also compared across both weekends. In addition, caregivers
perceptions of RMS use were gathered.
Results: Children, with the use of RMSs, could
potentially have access to approximately 42% more
words per day. In addition, although caregivers
produced an equivalent number of words on both
weekends, they tended to talk more from a distance
when using the RMS than when not. Finally, caregivers
reported positive perceived communication benefits of
RMS use.
Conclusions: Findings from this investigation suggest
that children with hearing loss have increased access
to caregiver talk when using an RMS in the home
environment. Clinical implications and future directions
for research are discussed.
Access to linguistic input and social interactions
are essential to the development of childrens
language. There is a positive relationship between
the number of words to which children are exposed and
their subsequent vocabulary (Hart & Risley, 1995). More-
over, children who are exposed to more words have better
processing efficiency than those with less word exposure
(Hurtado, Marchman, & Fernald, 2008). This word learning
process requires access to the acoustic patterns of words,
thus leading to the recognition of conditional probabilities
of phoneme sequences that result in word identification
(Aslin, Saffran, & Newport, 1998).
However, young children with hearing loss might not
have consistent access to this necessary high-quality linguis-
tic input, which is critical for receptive and expressive lan-
guage development (e.g., Hoff & Naigles, 2002; Quittner
et al., 2013). Although multiple factors can affect a childs
access to linguistic input, it is reasonable to conclude that
compromised exposure to high-quality speech input con-
tributes, at least in part, to the well-known deficits in speech,
language, and vocabulary development in children with
hearing loss (Ching & Dillon, 2013; Geers, Strube, Tobey,
& Moog, 2011; Stelmachowicz, Pittman, Hoover, Lewis,
& Moeller, 2004; Tomblin, Oleson, Ambrose, Walker, &
Moeller, 2014). Well-fit hearing aids that are worn consis-
tently (> 10 hours a day), thus providing enhanced audi-
bility, have been shown to help ameliorate some of these
language deficits (Tomblin et al., 2015).
It is widely recognized that listening in the presence
of background noise is challenging for children with hearing
loss (e.g., Crandell, 1993; Stickney, Zeng, Litovsky, &
Assmann, 2004), thereby potentially limiting acoustic access
to desired speech. Despite the use of well-fit hearing aids,
a signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of at least +15 dB is neces-
sary for optimum speech perception by children with hear-
ing loss (American National Standards Institute/Acoustical
Society of America, 2010). Numerous hearing technologies
have been recommended to improve the ability of children
to hear in adverse listening conditions by enhancing the
SNR, including different microphone types (e.g., fixed direc-
tional, fully adaptive directional), and various signal pro-
cessing approaches (e.g., digital noise reduction). However,
the most effective technology for this purpose used with
a
Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University
School of Medicine, Nashville, TN
Correspondence to Carlos R. Benitez-Barrera:
carlos.r.benitez@vanderbilt.edu
Editor-in-Chief: Frederick (Erick) Gallun
Editor: Steve Aiken
Received May 3, 2017
Revision received August 20, 2017
Accepted September 15, 2017
https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-H-17-0168
Disclosure: This article was funded by Phonak, who provided equipment for use in
this study.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 61 399409 February 2018 Copyright © 2018 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 399
... An alternative approach is the use of remote microphone systems (RMSs) to reduce extraneous noise and bring the parent's voice into the hearing bubble even when at a distance. A study using Language Environment Analysis Software (LENA) showed that with the use of RMS, children could have access to approximately 42% more words per day (Benitez-Barrera et al., 2018), and the amount of child-directed speech increases with use of RMS (Benitez- Barrera et al., 2019). In addition, caregivers reported positive perceived communication benefits of RMS, and they tended to talk more from a distance when using the RMS (Benitez- Barrera et al., 2018). ...
... A study using Language Environment Analysis Software (LENA) showed that with the use of RMS, children could have access to approximately 42% more words per day (Benitez-Barrera et al., 2018), and the amount of child-directed speech increases with use of RMS (Benitez- Barrera et al., 2019). In addition, caregivers reported positive perceived communication benefits of RMS, and they tended to talk more from a distance when using the RMS (Benitez- Barrera et al., 2018). Remote microphone technologies are especially beneficial once children are in daycare, preschool or elementary school, and can provide substantial benefits for children with slight-mild hearing loss. ...
Article
Worldwide, 15 million babies are “born too soon” every year (Blencowe et al., 2012; Howson et al., 2013). Remarkably, prematurity survival rates now exceed 93% across all countries. More than 360,000 preterm babies survived in the United States in 2020; 10% of all births (Blencowe et al., 2012; Howson et al., 2013). While this is an incredible achievement, preterm birth places the developing infant in a particularly vulnerable state for life-long adverse developmental outcomes. Due to improved survival rates, developmental disabilities, including hearing, visual, motor, cognitive, speech-language, and literacy deficits, have also increased (McCormick & Behrman, 2007). In addition to the medical and developmental consequences, the increased cost of raising a preterm child compared to a term-born child exceeds 134,000or>60billionannuallyinU.S.dollars,adjustedfortheyear2023(Manghametal.,2009).Incontrast,thecostofearlyinterventionisarelativebargain,at134,000 or >60 billion annually in U.S. dollars, adjusted for the year 2023 (Mangham et al., 2009). In contrast, the cost of early intervention is a relative bargain, at 1,240 per preterm infant, adjusted for the year 2023 (Clements et al., 2007). Early intervention is highly likely to be effective, based on the remarkable plasticity of the newborn brain (White et al., 2013).
... Atualmente, sabe-se que a indicação do uso do SMR não se restringe ao ambiente escolar. Estudos demonstraram benefícios do uso dessa tecnologia por crianças com deficiência auditiva também no ambiente domiciliar 6,7 LILACS ("Child" OR "Children" OR "Criança" OR "Niño" OR "Enfant") AND ("Autistic Disorder" OR "Autism Spectrum Disorder" OR "Infantile Autism" OR "Autism" OR "Transtorno Autístico" OR "Autismo" OR "Autismo Infantil" OR "Trouble autistique") AND ("Self-help devices" OR "Tecnologia assistiva" OR "Dispositivos de Autoayuda" OR "Dispositifs d'assistance au mouvement") ( db:("LILACS")) Scielo (criança) AND (transtorno do espectro autista) AND (tecnologia assistiva) AND in:("scl") #1 (((TS=(child)) OR TS=(children)) OR TS=(criança)) OR TS=(crianças)) OR TS=(niño) AND #2 ((((TS=(autistic spectrum disorder)) OR TS=(autism spectrum disorder)) OR TS=(autism)) OR TS=(transtorno autístico)) OR TS=(autismo infantil) AND #3 ((((TS=(self-help devices)) OR TS=(tecnologia assistiva)) OR TS=(dispositivos de autoayuda)) Biblioteca Digital Brasileira de Dissertações e Teses "criança" AND "transtorno do espectro autista" AND "tecnologia assistiva" ...
Article
Full-text available
Objetivo: avaliar os benefícios do uso do SMR em crianças ouvintes diagnosticadas com TEA. Métodos: trata-se de revisão integrativa da literatura. A busca foi realizada em diferentes bases de dados e foram incluídos estudos que avaliaram o uso do SMR em crianças ouvintes diagnosticadas com TEA. Resultados: seis estudos foram incluídos. Quatro estudos incluíram também a avaliação com professores e/ou pais. Os estudos demonstraram uma melhora na percepção auditiva da fala em ambientes ruidosos com o uso do SMR, além de uma redução do estresse relacionado à audição. Professores relataram melhorias na compreensão auditiva, no comportamento em sala de aula e na atenção. Pais perceberam melhor desempenho em diferentes ambientes e à distância. Discussão: O SMR pode favorecer a comunicação e interação social de crianças com TEA. Pode haver rejeição ao uso dos receptores pessoais por hiper responsividade tátil. Conclusão: O uso do SMR em crianças ouvintes diagnosticadas com TEA pode oferecer múltiplos benefícios, como uma melhor percepção da fala em ambientes ruidosos, redução do estresse em situações complexas de escuta, aprimoramento na percepção da prosódia emocional, aumento da atenção e da memória auditiva, além da diminuição de comportamentos disruptivos. No entanto, os benefícios variam entre as crianças, não sendo possível garanti-los em toda a população com TEA.
... This creates the impression that the microphone wearer is speaking directly into the ear, which results in a significantly improved SNR. Benítez- Barrera et al. (2018) demonstrated that children using an RMS at home have access to approximately 42% more words throughout the day when using the RMS compared to when they do not use it. They conducted this study by comparing the word count recorded by a LENA device worn by caregivers to the word count recorded by a LENA worn by children with HL. ...
Article
Objectives: One important aspect in facilitating language access for children with hearing loss (HL) is the auditory environment. An optimal auditory environment is characterized by high signal to noise ratios (SNRs), low background noise levels, and low reverberation times. In this study, the authors describe the auditory environment of early intervention groups specifically equipped for young children with HL. Design: Seven early intervention groups for children with HL were included in the study. A total of 26 young children (22 to 46 months) visiting those groups participated. Language Environmental Analysis recorders were used to record all sounds around a child during one group visit. The recordings were analyzed to estimate SNR levels and background noise levels during the intervention groups. The unoccupied noise levels and reverberation times were measured in the unoccupied room either directly before or after the group visit. Results: The average SNR encountered by the children in the intervention groups was +13 dB SNR. The detected speech of the attending professionals achieved the +15 dB SNR recommended by the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association in approximately 42% of the time. The unoccupied noise levels were between 29 and 39 dBA, complying with acoustic norms for classroom environments (≤35 dBA, by ANSI/ASA 12.60-2010 Part 1) for six out of seven groups. Reverberation time was between 0.3 and 0.6 sec for all groups, which complies to the acoustic norms for classroom environments for children without HL (0.6 or 0.7 sec, depending on the room size), while only one group complied to the stricter norm for children with HL (0.3 sec). Conclusions: The current findings show characteristics of the auditory environment of a setting that is specifically equipped and designed for groups of children with HL. Maintaining favorable SNRs seems to be the largest challenge to achieve within the constraints of an environment where young children gather, play, and learn. The results underscore the importance of staying attentive to keep spoken language accessible for children with HL in a group setting.
... This creates the impression that the microphone wearer is speaking directly into the ear, which results in a significantly improved SNR. Benítez- Barrera et al. (2018) demonstrated that children using an RMS at home have access to approximately 42% more words throughout the day when using the RMS compared to when they do not use it. They conducted this study by comparing the word count recorded by a LENA device worn by caregivers to the word count recorded by a LENA worn by children with HL. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives One important aspect in facilitating language access for children with hearing loss (HL) is the auditory environment. An optimal auditory environment is characterized by high signal to noise ratios (SNRs), low background noise levels, and low reverberation times. In this study, the authors describe the auditory environment of early intervention groups specifically equipped for young children with HL. Design Seven early intervention groups for children with HL were included in the study. A total of 26 young children (22 to 46 months) visiting those groups participated. Language Environmental Analysis recorders were used to record all sounds around a child during one group visit. The recordings were analyzed to estimate SNR levels and background noise levels during the intervention groups. The unoccupied noise levels and reverberation times were measured in the unoccupied room either directly before or after the group visit. Results The average SNR encountered by the children in the intervention groups was +13 dB SNR. The detected speech of the attending professionals achieved the +15 dB SNR recommended by the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association in approximately 42% of the time. The unoccupied noise levels were between 29 and 39 dBA, complying with acoustic norms for classroom environments (≤35 dBA, by ANSI/ASA 12.60-2010 Part 1) for six out of seven groups. Reverberation time was between 0.3 and 0.6 sec for all groups, which complies to the acoustic norms for classroom environments for children without HL (0.6 or 0.7 sec, depending on the room size), while only one group complied to the stricter norm for children with HL (0.3 sec). Conclusions The current findings show characteristics of the auditory environment of a setting that is specifically equipped and designed for groups of children with HL. Maintaining favorable SNRs seems to be the largest challenge to achieve within the constraints of an environment where young children gather, play, and learn. The results underscore the importance of staying attentive to keep spoken language accessible for children with HL in a group setting.
... XX, 00-00 ratio by increasing the intensity of the speech signal entering the participant's ear relative to background noise (Thibodeau 1990;Thibodeau & Saucedo 1991;Schafer et al. 2013;Keller et al. 2021). A large body of literature has previously reported benefits of RM systems for youth with hearing loss listening in a variety of environments (Anderson & Goldstein 2004;Flynn et al. 2005;Benítez-Barrera et al. 2018Thompson et al. 2020). The benefits of RM systems when listening in noise have also been reported for autistic youth (Schafer et al. 2013(Schafer et al. , 2016(Schafer et al. , 2019Rance et al. 2014Rance et al. , 2017Xu et al. 2023). ...
Article
Objectives A recent study has provided empirical support for the use of remote microphone (RM) systems to improve listening-in-noise performance of autistic youth. It has been proposed that RM system effects might be achieved by boosting engagement in this population. The present study used behavioral coding to test this hypothesis in autistic and nonautistic youth listening in an ecologically valid, noisy environment. Design We drew on extant data from a recent experimental study in which 56 youth (32 autistic, 24 nonautistic) matched at the group level on age and biological sex completed listening-in-noise tasks wherein they reported their perception of audiovisual syllables, words, sentences, and passages with and without an RM system; conditions were counter-balanced across participants. As previously reported, perceptual accuracy varied with stimulus complexity and overall improved with the RM system, with improvements not significantly different between groups. Video recordings of participants completing listening-in-noise tasks in both conditions were coded via a 5-second, partial-interval coding system by naive coders for (a) engagement in the task (indexed via proportion of intervals in which participants displayed on-task behaviors) and (b) verbal, stimulus-specific protesting in the task (indexed via proportion of intervals in which participants displayed verbal, stimulus-specific protesting behaviors). Examples of on-task behaviors included attending to the screen and completing task activities. Examples of protesting behaviors included complaining about stimuli volume or the inability to hear. Chronological age, autism features, language ability, audiovisual speech integration as measured by psychophysical tasks, tactile responsiveness, and nonverbal intelligence quotient were evaluated as putative predictors and/or moderators of effects on behaviors of interest. Results In general, participants were highly engaged in the task, and there were few protests, reflecting more than 90% and fewer than 0.5% of coded intervals, respectively. We did not detect any statistically significant effects of group or RM system use on task engagement. Nonautistic youth were engaged in the listening-in-noise task for an average of 97.45% of intervals, whereas autistic youth were engaged in the listening-in-noise task for an average of 94.25% of intervals. In contrast, verbal, stimulus-specific protesting in the listening-in-noise task was significantly reduced, on average, in the RM (0.04% of intervals) versus the No RM (0.2% of intervals) conditions. There were no effects related to group for this behaviorally coded outcome. In addition, select participant characteristics predicted engagement within conditions across participants. Greater language ability and nonverbal intelligence quotient predicted increased engagement when not using an RM system. Increased features of autism and wider temporal binding windows for audiovisual speech predicted reduced engagement while using an RM system, and greater audiovisual integration predicted increased engagement while using an RM system. Conclusions The results of this study suggest that RM system use reduces verbal, stimulus-specific protesting, which likely reflects difficulty engaging when listening in noise. The present study extends our previous study to provide additional empirical support for RM system use in autistic and nonautistic youth.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To map the literature on the use of the Remote Microphone System (RMS) in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Methods Scoping Review following the Joanna Briggs Institute recommendations and PRISMA-ScR checklist. Search was carried out in the databases: PubMed, Embase, Scopus, Web of Science, Lilacs, and gray literature, including Google Scholar and ProQuest, as well as reference lists of included studies and expert consultations. Intervention studies with children and adolescents with ASD using RMS were included, without gender, language, age, publication time, ethnicity, or geographical location restrictions. Results 709 studies were identified in phase 1. After reviewing 14 full texts with eligibility, eight studies were eligible. Studies were heterogeneous in the RMS model (personal or free field), applied tests, intervention period, and location. Improvement in speech perception, social interaction, behavior, attention, auditory memory, noise tolerance, stress reduction, and modification in neural activity through electrophysiological evaluation were observed. Conclusion Using RMS demonstrated benefits in speech perception, social interaction, and behavior in adolescents and children with ASD. Further studies are needed to define protocols and indication parameters in this population. Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder; Remote Microphone; Auditory Perception; Children; Teens
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To map the literature on the use of the Remote Microphone System (RMS) in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Methods Scoping Review following the Joanna Briggs Institute recommendations and PRISMA-ScR checklist. Search was carried out in the databases: PubMed, Embase, Scopus, Web of Science, Lilacs, and gray literature, including Google Scholar and ProQuest, as well as reference lists of included studies and expert consultations. Intervention studies with children and adolescents with ASD using RMS were included, without gender, language, age, publication time, ethnicity, or geographical location restrictions. Results 709 studies were identified in phase 1. After reviewing 14 full texts with eligibility, eight studies were eligible. Studies were heterogeneous in the RMS model (personal or free field), applied tests, intervention period, and location. Improvement in speech perception, social interaction, behavior, attention, auditory memory, noise tolerance, stress reduction, and modification in neural activity through electrophysiological evaluation were observed. Conclusion Using RMS demonstrated benefits in speech perception, social interaction, and behavior in adolescents and children with ASD. Further studies are needed to define protocols and indication parameters in this population. Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder; Remote Microphone; Auditory Perception; Children; Teens
Article
Full-text available
RESUMO Objetivo: analisar criticamente se a legislação garantidora do dispositivo Sistema de Frequência Modulada Pessoal abrange a população adequada de escolares com deficiência auditiva no critério idade, de forma a favorecer o desenvolvimento das habilidades necessárias para a comunicação, a alfabetização e a aprendizagem dessas crianças. Métodos: trata-se de uma análise legislativa das normas referentes ao uso do dispositivo Sistema de Frequência Modulada Pessoal. Para a busca da legislação pertinente, foram utilizados bancos de dados públicos, tais como: o portal do Planalto e o portal do Ministério da Saúde. Foram consultadas as leis, portarias e diretrizes Brasileiras no que se refere ao uso do Dispositivo Sistema de Frequência Modulada Pessoal. Revisão da Literatura: foram identificadas na Revisão de Literatura a Portaria n. 1.274 de 25 de junho de 2013, o Relatório do CONITEC sobre o FM no ano de 2020 e a Portaria GM/MS Nº 2.465, de 27 de setembro de 2021 que regulamentam o dispositivo Sistema de Frequência Modulada Pessoal. Conclusão: é evidente que a portaria inicial de 2013, a qual regulamentava o fornecimento do Sistema FM, foi revista e atualizada, uma vez que não contemplava crianças menores de 6 anos, em fase de máxima aprendizagem da linguagem oral, pré-alfabetização e letramento, mesmo que estivessem inseridas em instituições escolares.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: to critically analyze whether the legislation regarding the Personal Frequency-Modulated (FM) System device encompasses most of the students with hearing impairment to facilitate the development of skills required for communication, literacy, and learning. Methods: a legislative analysis of the norms regarding the use of the Personal Frequency-Modulated (FM) System device. Relevant legislations were searched on public databases such as the Planalto and the Ministry of Health portals. Brazilian laws, ordinances, and relevant guidelines were consulted as well. Literature Review: Ordinance n. 1,274 of June 25, 2013, the CONITEC Report on FM in 2020 and GM/MS Ordinance No. 2,465, of September 27, 2021, which regulate the Personal Modulated Frequency System device, were identified. Conclusion: the initial ordinance of 2013, which regulated the Personal Frequency-Modulated (FM) System, was revised and updated, because it did not include children under six years of age, who are in the peak phase of oral language learning.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This article describes the major findings of the Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment (LOCHI) study. Design: A population-based prospective cohort study; with randomized controlled trials of hearing aid prescription and non-linear frequency compression. Sample: 451 children in New South Wales, Victoria and Southern Queensland. Results: Significant predictors of language outcomes at 3 years of age included severity of hearing loss, gender, presence of additional disabilities, maternal education, and age at cochlear implantation. Although prescription did not have a significant effect on outcomes, its influence on loudness and hearing aid safety has implications for management. After controlling for a range of predictor variables, nonlinear frequency compression did not have a significant effect on outcomes. For the same hearing sensitivity, the presence of auditory neuropathy did not have a significant effect on outcomes. Conclusions: These findings form the basis for evidence-based guidelines for management of children with hearing loss.
Article
Full-text available
Infants differ substantially in their rates of language growth, and slow growth predicts later academic difficulties. In this study, we explored how the amount of speech directed to infants in Spanish-speaking families low in socioeconomic status influenced the development of children's skill in real-time language processing and vocabulary learning. All-day recordings of parent-infant interactions at home revealed striking variability among families in how much speech caregivers addressed to their child. Infants who experienced more child-directed speech became more efficient in processing familiar words in real time and had larger expressive vocabularies by the age of 24 months, although speech simply overheard by the child was unrelated to vocabulary outcomes. Mediation analyses showed that the effect of child-directed speech on expressive vocabulary was explained by infants' language-processing efficiency, which suggests that richer language experience strengthens processing skills that facilitate language growth.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examines the effect of parents’ language input on the linguistic progress of children with cochlear implants. Participants were 21 children with cochlear implants and their mothers. Age at implantation ranged between 14 and 46 months. The study was longitudinal with data collections every 4½ months for a period of 27 months. Spontaneous speech in a free play situation with a parent was recorded at each data point. Children's grammar was measured in terms of Men Length of Utterance (MLU) and the use of noun plurals, verb markings, and case and gender markings on articles. Mothers’ child-directed speech was analysed in terms of MLU, self-repetitions and expansions. Time-lagged correlational analyses were performed relating properties of maternal speech at an earlier data point —while controlling for the child's language level at this data point— to child language at subsequent data points. The results showed that maternal MLU and expansions are positively related to child linguistic progress. Higher maternal MLU and more expansions are related to higher child MLU subsequently. More specifically, expansions of specific grammatical structures are related to an increased correct use of these structures by the child subsequently. This was the case particularly for case and gender marking on articles, but also for noun plurals and verb markings. Maternal self-repetitions were negatively related to child progress in grammar. The results demonstrate an effect on mothers’ language input on the linguistic progress of young children with cochlear implants. Rich language input leads to better language growth.
Article
Objectives: This study examined the language outcomes of children with mild to severe hearing loss during the preschool years. The longitudinal design was leveraged to test whether language growth trajectories were associated with degree of hearing loss and whether aided hearing influenced language growth in a systematic manner. The study also explored the influence of the timing of hearing aid fitting and extent of use on children's language growth. Finally, the study tested the hypothesis that morphosyntax may be at particular risk due to the demands it places on the processing of fine details in the linguistic input. Design: The full cohort of children in this study comprised 290 children who were hard of hearing (CHH) and 112 children with normal hearing who participated in the Outcomes of Children with Hearing Loss (OCHL) study between the ages of 2 and 6 years. CHH had a mean better-ear pure-tone average of 47.66 dB HL (SD = 13.35). All children received a comprehensive battery of language measures at annual intervals, including standardized tests, parent-report measures, and spontaneous and elicited language samples. Principal components analysis supported the use of a single composite language score for each of the age levels (2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 years). Measures of unaided (better-ear pure-tone average, speech intelligibility index) and aided (residualized speech intelligibility index) hearing were collected, along with parent-report measures of daily hearing aid use time. Mixed modeling procedures were applied to examine the rate of change (227 CHH; 94 children with normal hearing) in language ability over time in relation to (1) degree of hearing loss, (2) aided hearing, (3) age of hearing aid fit and duration of use, and (4) daily hearing aid use. Principal components analysis was also employed to examine factor loadings from spontaneous language samples and to test their correspondence with standardized measures. Multiple regression analysis was used to test for differential effects of hearing loss on morphosyntax and lexical development. Results: Children with mild to severe hearing loss, on average, showed depressed language levels compared with peers with normal hearing who were matched on age and socioeconomic status. The degree to which CHH fell behind increased with greater severity of hearing loss. The amount of improved audibility with hearing aids was associated with differential rates of language growth; better audibility was associated with faster rates of language growth in the preschool years. Children fit early with hearing aids had better early language achievement than children fit later. However, children who were fit after 18 months of age improved in their language abilities as a function of the duration of hearing aid use. These results suggest that the language learning system remains open to experience provided by improved access to linguistic input. Performance in the domain of morphosyntax was found to be more delayed in CHH than their semantic abilities. Conclusion: The data obtained in this study largely support the predictions, suggesting that mild to severe hearing loss places children at risk for delays in language development. Risks are moderated by the provision of early and consistent access to well-fit hearing aids that provide optimized audibility.
Article
Objectives: The primary objective of this study was to examine the quantity and quality of caregiver talk directed to children who are hard of hearing (CHH) compared with children with normal hearing (CNH). For the CHH only, the study explored how caregiver input changed as a function of child age (18 months versus 3 years), which child and family factors contributed to variance in caregiver linguistic input at 18 months and 3 years, and how caregiver talk at 18 months related to child language outcomes at 3 years. Design: Participants were 59 CNH and 156 children with bilateral, mild-to-severe hearing loss. When children were approximately 18 months and/or 3 years of age, caregivers and children participated in a 5-min semistructured, conversational interaction. Interactions were transcribed and coded for two features of caregiver input representing quantity (number of total utterances and number of total words) and four features representing quality (number of different words, mean length of utterance in morphemes, proportion of utterances that were high level, and proportion of utterances that were directing). In addition, at the 18-month visit, parents completed a standardized questionnaire regarding their child's communication development. At the 3-year visit, a clinician administered a standardized language measure. Results: At the 18-month visit, the CHH were exposed to a greater proportion of directing utterances than the CNH. At the 3-year visit, there were significant differences between the CNH and CHH for number of total words and all four of the quality variables, with the CHH being exposed to fewer words and lower quality input. Caregivers generally provided higher quality input to CHH at the 3-year visit compared with the 18-month visit. At the 18-month visit, quantity variables, but not quality variables, were related to several child and family factors. At the 3-year visit, the variable most strongly related to caregiver input was child language. Longitudinal analyses indicated that quality, but not quantity, of caregiver linguistic input at 18 months was related to child language abilities at 3 years, with directing utterances accounting for significant unique variance in child language outcomes. Conclusions: Although caregivers of CHH increased their use of quality features of linguistic input over time, the differences when compared with CNH suggest that some caregivers may need additional support to provide their children with optimal language learning environments. This is particularly important given the relationships that were identified between quality features of caregivers' linguistic input and children's language abilities. Family supports should include a focus on developing a style that is conversational eliciting as opposed to directive.
Article
The aim of this research was to evaluate and explore the benefits of frequency-modulated (FM) technology with preschool children with hearing aids. The research was of a longitudinal prospective design, including both quantitative and qualitative analysis. This article will focus on the quantitative results only. Daily diaries, data-logging, and questionnaires were used to collect quantitative data. Five of the seven families were able to establish regular FM use in a range of environments and settings with the most frequent being at home, in the car, at the nursery, during shopping, and when outdoors. Situational analysis of FM use was established and compared over time using the FM Listening Evaluation for Children questionnaire. Improvements in listening in noise, auditory only, and distance were identified over time. The Language Developmental Snapshot was used to monitor language development over the duration of the study. Results indicated that children who were at risk at the start of the study were within normal levels or close by the end of the study. The current study provides a unique contribution to the existing literature and together with future research can be integral to the provision of FM/remote microphone technology as standard for preschool children with hearing aids.
Article
Importance Hearing loss (HL) in children can be deleterious to their speech and language development. The standard of practice has been early provision of hearing aids (HAs) to moderate these effects; however, there have been few empirical studies evaluating the effectiveness of this practice on speech and language development among children with mild-to-severe HL.Objective To investigate the contributions of aided hearing and duration of HA use to speech and language outcomes in children with mild-to-severe HL.Design, Setting, and Participants An observational cross-sectional design was used to examine the association of aided hearing levels and length of HA use with levels of speech and language outcomes. One hundred eighty 3- and 5-year-old children with HL were recruited through records of Universal Newborn Hearing Screening and referrals from clinical service providers in the general community in 6 US states.Interventions All but 4 children had been fitted with HAs, and measures of aided hearing and the duration of HA use were obtained.Main Outcomes and Measures Standardized measures of speech and language ability were obtained.Results Measures of the gain in hearing ability for speech provided by the HA were significantly correlated with levels of speech (ρ179 = 0.20; P = .008) and language: ρ155 = 0.21; P = .01) ability. These correlations were indicative of modest levels of association between aided hearing and speech and language outcomes. These benefits were found for children with mild and moderate-to-severe HL. In addition, the amount of benefit from aided hearing interacted with the duration of HA experience (Speech: F4,161 = 4.98; P < .001; Language: F4,138 = 2.91; P < .02). Longer duration of HA experience was most beneficial for children who had the best aided hearing. Conclusions and Relevance The degree of improved hearing provided by HAs was associated with better speech and language development in children. In addition, the duration of HA experience interacted with the aided hearing to influence outcomes. These results provide support for the provision of well-fitted HAs to children with HL. In particular, the findings support early HA fitting and HA provision to children with mild HL.
Article
Previous research has shown that children as young as 2 can learn words from 3rd-party conversations (Akhtar, Jipson, & Callanan, 2001). The focus of this study was to determine whether younger infants could learn a new word through overhearing. Novel object labels were introduced to 18-month-old infants in 1 of 2 conditions: directly by an experimenter or in the context of overhearing the experimenter use the word while interacting with another adult. The findings suggest that, when memory demands are not too high, 18-month-old infants can learn words through overhearing.
Article
A recent report demonstrated that 8-month-olds can seg- ment a continuous stream of speech syllables, containing no acoustic or prosodic cues to word boundaries, into wordlike units after only 2 min of listening experience (Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996). Thus, a powerful learning mechanism capable of extracting statistical informa- tion from fluent speech is available early in development. The present study extends these results by documenting the particular type of statis- tical computation—transitional (conditional) probability—used by infants to solve this word-segmentation task. An artificial language corpus, consisting of a continuous stream of trisyllabic nonsense words, was presented to 8-month-olds for 3 min. A postfamiliarization test compared the infants' responses to words versus part-words (tri- syllabic sequences spanning word boundaries). The corpus was con- structed so that test words and part-words were matched in frequency, but differed in their transitional probabilities. Infants showed reliable discrimination of words from part-words, thereby demonstrating rapid segmentation of continuous speech into words on the basis of transi- tional probabilities of syllable pairs.