ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Anticipation for future confers great benefits to human well-being and mental health. However, previous work focus on how people’s well-being correlate with brain activities during perception of emotional stimuli, rather than anticipation for the future events. Here, the current study investigated how well-being relates to neural circuitry underlying the anticipating process of future desired events. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging, 40 participants were scanned while they were performing an emotion anticipation task, in which they were instructed to anticipate the positive or neutral events. The results showed that bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) were activated during anticipation for positive events relative to neutral events, and the enhanced brain activation in MPFC was associated with higher level of well-being. The findings suggest a neural mechanism by which the anticipation process to future desired events correlates to human well-being, which provide a future-oriented view on the neural sources of well-being.
This content is subject to copyright.
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 09 January 2018
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02199
Edited by:
Marco Tamietto,
Tilburg University, Netherlands
Reviewed by:
Matteo Diano,
Tilburg University, Netherlands
Jan Van den Stock,
KU Leuven, Belgium
*Correspondence:
Xiting Huang
xthuang@swu.edu.cn
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Emotion Science,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 01 June 2017
Accepted: 04 December 2017
Published: 09 January 2018
Citation:
Luo Y, Chen X, Qi S, You X and
Huang X (2018) Well-being
and Anticipation for Future Positive
Events: Evidences from an fMRI
Study. Front. Psychol. 8:2199.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02199
Well-being and Anticipation for
Future Positive Events: Evidences
from an fMRI Study
Yangmei Luo1,2, Xuhai Chen1, Senqing Qi3, Xuqun You1and Xiting Huang2*
1Shaanxi Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi’an,
China, 2Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality of Ministry of Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University,
Chongqing, China, 3Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Modern Teaching Technology, Shaanxi Normal University,
Xi’an, China
Anticipation for future confers great benefits to human well-being and mental health.
However, previous work focus on how people’s well-being correlate with brain activities
during perception of emotional stimuli, rather than anticipation for the future events.
Here, the current study investigated how well-being relates to neural circuitry underlying
the anticipating process of future desired events. Using event-related functional
magnetic resonance imaging, 40 participants were scanned while they were performing
an emotion anticipation task, in which they were instructed to anticipate the positive
or neutral events. The results showed that bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC)
were activated during anticipation for positive events relative to neutral events, and
the enhanced brain activation in MPFC was associated with higher level of well-being.
The findings suggest a neural mechanism by which the anticipation process to future
desired events correlates to human well-being, which provide a future-oriented view on
the neural sources of well-being.
Keywords: well-being, anticipation, positive affect, fMRI, medial prefrontal cortex
INTRODUCTION
As well-being is the central construct in positive psychology (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi,
2000), substantial interest has been directed at delineating the sources of well-being. However,
studies of well-being have been limited by the static, single point-in-time view (Gallagher et al.,
2009). In particular, most of previous work focus on how peoples well-being correlate with
brain activities during the perception of emotional events (e.g., van Reekum et al., 2007;Heller
et al., 2013;Cunningham and Kirkland, 2014), rather than the anticipation for the upcoming
events. For example, when emotional events were showed, happy people relative to their unhappy
peers showed greater amygdala responses to positive stimuli (Cunningham and Kirkland, 2014),
greater ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) responses to negative stimuli (van Reekum et al.,
2007). However, given that how people construct their future is a central organizing feature of
perception, cognition, affect, memory, motivation, and action (Seligman et al., 2013), it is important
to elucidate how anticipation for future contribute to people’s well-being, which can provide a
dynamic, future-oriented view on the sources of well-being.
Anticipation, paying attention to the upcoming stimulus predicted by a contextual cue
(Bermpohl et al., 2006), has important implications in human well-being and mental health.
Anticipation confers important evolutionary benefits to human beings. Specifically, expecting
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 2
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
the forthcoming events allow active preparations in cognitive,
affective, and behavioral strategies (Grupe et al., 2013), which
ensure survival in the changing and potential challenging
environment (Gilbert and Wilson, 2007). Furthermore, the
deficits of anticipation of future experience have been associated
with extreme low levels of well-being, such as depression
(MacLeod and Byrne, 1996;Abler et al., 2007) and anxiety
(Nitschke et al., 2009;Boehme et al., 2014;Heitmann et al.,
2014). Nitschke et al. (2009) found patients with generalized
anxiety disorder differed from healthy control by showing
hyperactivity in the amygdala when anticipating future aversive
events. Together, it is necessary to investigate how neural
circuitry underlying the anticipation for future events related to
well-being.
Due to the fact that exaggerated negative anticipation
contributes to the development and maintenance of emotional-
related disorders (e.g., Abler et al., 2007;Nitschke et al.,
2009;Aupperle et al., 2012;Boehme et al., 2014), most of
previous studies focus on the neural circuitry during the
anticipation of negative events (Nitschke et al., 2006;Herwig
et al., 2007, 2010;Sarinopoulos et al., 2010;Yang et al., 2012;
Grupe et al., 2013). However, it is argued that the anticipation
of positive events is a key element of well-being (MacLeod
and Conway, 2005), and thus deserving greater attention. On
the one hand, the clinical literature shows there are distinct
relationships between emotional disorders and positive or
negative anticipation. For example, relative to healthy people,
anxious people anticipated more negative future experiences,
whereas depressed or parasuicidal people anticipated less positive
future experiences, but did not anticipate more future negative
events (MacLeod and Byrne, 1996;MacLeod et al., 1997). On
the other hand, the studies in healthy samples demonstrate
anticipating the positive events increases reward sensitivity
(Gable et al., 2003), enhances the memory of positive stimuli
(Crowell and Schmeichel, 2016), induces positive affect (Monfort
et al., 2015), and relates to higher levels of well-being (MacLeod
and Conway, 2005). A recent study found anticipating positive
events (e.g., a funny cartoon) were a convenient and powerful
way to induce positive emotion, which in turn improving
stress coping (e.g., coping to a public speech) (Monfort et al.,
2015). Taken together, these findings underline the distinctions
between positive and negative anticipation and emphasize the
contribution of the positive anticipation of future in people’s
well-being (MacLeod and Conway, 2005).
Therefore, the current study focused on neural correlates of
the relationship between well-being and anticipation for future
desirable events. We intended to address this issue by employing
the emotional anticipation task when participants were scanned
by a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Given that
the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) involve in the
anticipating process (Nitschke et al., 2006;Bermpohl et al., 2008;
Scherpiet et al., 2014), we hypothesized that the amygdala and
MPFC would be activated during anticipating positive stimuli.
Also, given the key role of amygdala in emotion processing
(Davis and Whalen, 2001;Phelps and LeDoux, 2005;Sergerie
et al., 2008) and different functional coupling between amygdala
and prefrontal areas in various emotion processing (Lee et al.,
2012;Diano et al., 2016, 2017;Di et al., 2017), we conducted
psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis using left and
right amygdala seeds. We hypothesized that positive anticipation
would modulate functional coupling between amygdala and
prefrontal cortex. Moreover, based on the close relationship
between anticipation of future positive events and well-being
(MacLeod and Conway, 2005;Monfort et al., 2015), we also
hypothesized that the activation of these regions would correlate
with the people’s well-being.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Forty right-handed participants (males/females, 15/25; mean
age =21.45 ±1.74), with no history of neurological disorders and
psychiatric disorders, were recruited among undergraduate and
postgraduates populations. In accordance with the Declaration
of Helsinki, written informed consent was obtained from all
participants. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics
Committee of the Southwest University. All participants were
paid for their participation.
Experimental Protocol and Stimuli
The stimuli were carefully selected from the Chinese Affective
Picture System (CAPS) (Bai et al., 2005). We selected the
stimuli from the CAPS instead of international affective picture
system (Lang et al., 1997), because the CAPS is a collection of
standardized photographic materials in the context of eastern
culture to avoid cultural bias in emotional studies. However,
the developing procedure of CAPS is identical to that of the
international affective picture system (Bai et al., 2005). The
stimuli from CAPS were successfully used to investigate the
neural correlates of anticipation and perception of emotional
events in previous studies (e.g., Yuan et al., 2007;Lin et al.,
2012). Of these pictures, 52 depicted positive scenes (e.g., smiling
kids, hugging, celebration, wedding, etc.), and 52 depicted neutral
scenes (house appliances, pedestrians, a working man, women
doing handwork, etc.). There were significant differences in
valence (positive pictures: M=6.97, SD =0.21, max =7.54,
min =6.50; neutral pictures: M=5.39, SD =0.25, max =5.88,
min =4.68) (t=36.47, df =51, p<0.001) and arousal (positive
pictures: M=6.07, SD =0.42, max =7.22, min =5.29; neutral
pictures: mean =4.15, SD =0.32, max =4.80, min =3.54)
between positive pictures and neutral pictures (t=28.09, df =51,
p<0.001). There were no significant differences in valence and
arousal among four fMRI runs for positive and neutral images,
respectively (ps>0.05). All the images were identical in size
and resolution (433 pixel ×325 pixel, 72 pixels per inch). Mean
luminance and contrast were balanced between two conditions.
Participants were scanned while they completed an emotion
anticipation task (Figure 1). Each trial started with a visual cue
that signaled the following picture would be positive (“a square”)
or neutral (“a circle”). The cue was presented for 2 s, followed
by a 4 s/6 s/8 s jittered inter-stimulus interval (ISI), and a 1 s
emotional image presentation. Then, participants were instructed
to make a response to indicate whether the cue was congruent
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 3
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
FIGURE 1 | The fMRI experimental protocol. (A) The cue–picture pairings. The square signaled the following picture is positive, while the circle signaled that is
neutral. (B) Example trials. Participants viewed a visual cue for 2 s, followed by a 4 s/6 s/8 s ISI, and a 1 s emotional image presentation. Then, participants made a
congruent (incongruent) response depending on whether the cue was congruent with the valence of the picture. Lastly, another 3 s/5 s/7 s ISI were presented.
with the valence of the picture. If the cue and the valence of the
picture were congruent (incongruent), participants responded
with the number “1” (“2”) within 2-s. Participants were instructed
about all cue–picture pairings prior to scanning. Lastly, another
3 s/5 s/7 s ISI were presented. Most of the cue–picture pairings
were congruent, but we added two filler trials in each run (each
condition has one, total eight trials), in which the cue and the
valence of the images were incongruent, to keep the participants’
attention on the task. The filler trials were dropped due to their
infrequency. There were 26 trials (12 positive, 12 neutral, and 2
filler trials presented in a pseudorandom order) in each of four
functional runs lasting 6 min and 56 s. The total time of the
experiment was about half an hour.
Following the scan, participants’ perceived well-being was
assessed with 48-item Chinese Happiness Inventory (CHI) (Lu
and Shih, 1997), composed of positive affect, negative affect, and
life satisfaction. The CHI is composed of 20 “eastern” items
deriving from Chinese culture and 28 “western” items from the
Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (Argyle et al., 1989). It is a
reliable measure of well-being in Chinese culture (e.g., Luo et al.,
2014). Each item was presented in four incremental levels from
unhappy to happy, numbered from 0 to 3. For example: I do not
feel interested in being with family members (0); I seldom feel
interested in being with family members (1); I often feel interested
in being with family members (2); I always feel interested in being
with family members (3). The higher score indicated a higher
level of overall well-being. In our sample, a good reliability was
observed (Cronbach’s α=0.95).
fMRI Data Acquisition
All images were collected on a 3.0-T scanner (Magnetom Trio,
Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Functional images were acquired
using a single-shot, gradient-recalled echo planar imaging
sequence (TR =2000 ms, TE =30 ms, flip angle =90,
32 axial slices, FOV =192 cm ×192 cm, acquisition
matrix =64 ×64, slice thickness =3 mm, without gap, voxel
size =3 mm ×3 mm ×4 mm). To minimize head motion,
participants’ head were restricted with foam cushions. High-
resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were also acquired
in sagittal orientation using a 3D magnetization prepared rapid
gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (176 slices, TR =1900 ms,
TE =2.53 ms, flip angle =9, resolution =256 ×256, and voxel
size =1mm×1mm×1 mm) on each participant.
fMRI Data Analysis
Data analysis was performed using FSL (FMRIB’s Software
Library1) (Jenkinson et al., 2002;Smith et al., 2004). Pre-statistics
processing consisted of motion correction using MCFLIRT
(Jenkinson et al., 2002), slice-timing correction, non-brain
removal using Brain Extraction Tool (BET; Smith, 2002),
spatial smoothing (5 mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian
kernel), and high-pass temporal filtering (Gaussian-weighted
least-squares straight line fitting, with σ=30.0 s).
For group analysis, we used the three-level approach in FEAT
(FMRI Expert Analysis Tool, version 6.00), part of FSL. Firstly,
a fixed-effects analysis modeled event-related responses for each
run was computed, with the four explanatory variables (i.e.,
positive anticipation, neutral anticipation, positive perception,
and neutral perception), and the six motion estimates as
covariates of no-interest. The first two variables (i.e., positive
anticipation and neutral anticipation) modeled the anticipation
phase (cues onset to offset, 2 s duration, one regressor for
1www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 4
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
positive cues, one regressor for neutral cues). Another two
regressors (i.e., positive perception, neutral perception) modeled
the perception phase (emotional images onset to offset, 1 s
duration, one regressor for positive images, one regressor
for neutral images). Each explanatory variable was convolved
with a double gamma hemodynamic response function with a
temporal derivative. According to our aim of this study, one
contrast of interest was defined (positive anticipation >neutral
anticipation). Functional volumes and first-level contrast images
from this analysis were registered to corresponding structural
volumes using boundary-based registration (BBR; Greve and
Fischl, 2009), and then spatially registered to the standard space
[Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)] using 12 degrees of
freedom with FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT;
Jenkinson et al., 2002).
A second-level analysis was performed combining the
parameter estimates of the four runs for each participant, treating
runs as a fixed effect. Then, these were inputs into the group
level and a mixed-effects analysis was used to create group
average maps for contrasts of interest. The Zstatistical parametric
maps were corrected for multiple comparisons using clusters
determined by Z>2.3 and a cluster significance of p=0.05,
based on Gaussian Random Field (GRF) theory (Worsley,
2001).
We conducted PPI analysis to examine whether functional
connectivity between amygdala and prefrontal cortex was
modulated by positive anticipation. MPFC, left and right
amygdala were used as seeds in this analysis, respectively. MPFC
were defined with a 6-mm radius sphere around the group
peak activation voxel identified in the whole-brain analysis, and
amygdala was defined with the Harvard–Oxford cortical atlas2,
with the probability threshold set to 25%. The seeds were first
transformed into functional space of each individual, and the
time-series were extracted using individual mask. The first-
level model included 11 regressors: psychological, physiological,
PPI, two regressors in the perception phase, and six motion
parameters. Positive and neutral trial durations comprised
the psychological regressor, modeled with values 1 and 1,
respectively, and convolved with double-gamma hemodynamic
response function. The physiological regressor comprised the
time-series for MPFC, the left or right amygdala, respectively.
The PPI regressor modeled the interaction of the psychological
regressor and the physiological regressor. Two regressors in
the perception phase included positive perception and neutral
perception. Group-level analysis was conducted using the same
approach as the whole-brain analysis indicated above.
Brain–Behavior Relationship
To assess whether neural regions involving in anticipation for
positive events were correlated with well-being, we further
extracted the percent signal change values from the significant
cluster for positive >neutral contrast as outlined by Mumford
(2007), and estimated a linear regression, with the percent signal
changes values as a predictor, and the individual CHI score as a
dependent variable.
2https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases
RESULTS
Behavioral Data
If the cue–picture pairings in the emotion anticipation task were
congruent and participants were responded with the number “1”
within 2 s, this trial was defined as “correct, and vice versa.
Paired t-tests were conducted on accuracy yielded no significant
differences between the positive condition (M=0.97, SD =0.03)
and neutral condition (M=0.98, SD =0.03) (t= 1.69, df =39,
p>0.05), due to the simplicity of the task. Moreover, paired
t-tests were conducted on reaction times (RTs) showed that
the RTs for the positive condition (M=512, SD =131) were
faster than that for the neutral condition (M=579, SD =146)
(t= 5.90, df =39, p<0.001).
Anticipation Activity for the Comparison
of Positive to Neutral Condition
Whole-brain, cluster wise analysis for the contrast of the
positive >neutral condition revealed that anticipating the
positive stimuli relative to that for the neutral stimuli activated
bilateral MPFC (peak MNI coordinates, 8, 60, 12; Z-score =4.51;
number of voxels =1437, p<0.001, GRF corrected)
(Figures 2A,B).
The results of PPI analyses indicated MPFC (peak MNI
coordinates, 10, 64, 10; Z-score =3.33; number of voxels =515,
p<0.001, GRF corrected) were activated in positive >neutral
contrast. However, there aren’t brain areas that were functionally
coupled with MPFC, left or right amygdala during positive than
neutral condition, respectively.
Brain–Behavior Relationship
The whole-brain analysis revealed that the MPFC were activated
for positive anticipation relative to neutral anticipation. Thus,
positive–neutral percent signal change values were extracted
from the bilateral MPFC. And we then tested the relationship
between the percent signal change of the MPFC on CHI by
estimating a linear regression. The result showed that revealed the
effects of percent signal change of the MPFC on CHI significant
[β=0.33, t(38) =2.12, p<0.05, R2=0.11] (Figure 2C).
DISCUSSION
The present study investigated how brain activities during
anticipation for positive events were associated with individual
difference of well-being. Participants performed an emotion
anticipation task in which they were required to anticipate the
positive events or neutral events and then made judgments
whether the cue and the following affective stimuli was congruent
or not. It was found that people made faster judgments in the
positive condition than that in the neutral condition, which may
indicate people’s greater motivation to positive stimuli relative
to neutral stimuli. More importantly, the bilateral MPFC were
activated in response to the positive cues relative to neutral cues,
and the MPFC activations were positively correlated with people’s
perceived well-being.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 5
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
FIGURE 2 | Correlation between well-being and MPFC activation for positive anticipation >neutral anticipation (thresholded at z>2.3, p= 0.05, whole-brain
corrected). (A) Bilateral MPFC (peak coordinates: X=8,Y= 60, Z= 12) activation responding to positive anticipation >neutral anticipation. Numbers in the upper
left corner of each image refer to the x-plane, y-plane, or z-plane coordinates of the MNI space (R, right and L, left). (B) Percent changes of MPFC in positive and
neutral condition. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. (C) Correlations between scores on the CHI and percent signal change values of the MPFC.
We found that the bilateral MPFC were involved in
anticipating the positive stimuli relative to the neutral stimuli.
MPFC is highly implicated in a variety of cognitions, not only
self-referential processing (Gusnard et al., 2001;Northoff et al.,
2006) and mentalizing (Amodio and Frith, 2006), but also
the emotion and reward processing (Ochsner and Gross, 2005;
Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009;Etkin et al., 2011). In particular,
MPFC is not only involved in emotional perception (Costa et al.,
2010), but also in emotional anticipation (Sharot et al., 2007;
Bermpohl et al., 2008;D’Argembeau et al., 2010). Specifically,
D’Argembeau et al. (2010) found anticipating personal future
goals elicited stronger activation in MPFC relative to non-
personal future events. Therefore, our results were consistent
with previous studies. Taken together, we speculated that the
activation of MPFC might represent the anticipatory pleasures
during expecting the future positive events.
More interestingly, we found that the MPFC activation during
anticipating the positive events were positively correlated with
individual difference of well-being. The results are consistent
with previous neural evidence, which found the cues signaling
emotional events activated MPFC and the activations were
positively correlated with novelty-seeking (Bermpohl et al.,
2008), a personality trait characterized by seeking new and
potential pleasures (Kashdan and Steger, 2007). Previous work
focuses on the neural circuitry underlying hedonic enjoyment
of consumption of positive stimuli associating with well-being
(e.g., Epstein et al., 2006;van Reekum et al., 2007;Heller et al.,
2013;Cunningham and Kirkland, 2014). However, except the
emotional perception, our study found neural activation during
emotional anticipation was related to people’s well-being. The
findings extended past work by providing a dynamic, future-
oriented view on the well-being.
Furthermore, our results were consistent with the goal theory
of well-being (Carver and Scheier, 1990;Diener et al., 1999).
This framework proposed that having and progressing goals
confer benefits to well-being (Carver and Scheier, 1990;Diener
et al., 1999). The benefits to well-being are not only from
goal achievement, but also from the strong positive anticipation
element in goal progress (MacLeod and Conway, 2005). Goals
and goal progress is possible to enhance people’s well-being
through positive anticipation (MacLeod and Conway, 2005). The
results were consistent with our previous resting state work,
which indicated the happy people relative to their unhappy
peers showed increased local functional connectivity within
MPFC (Luo et al., 2014). Combining the resting and task-based
neuroimaging results, we speculated that people with higher level
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 6
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
of well-being may derive more pleasure from anticipating future
positive events, which may relative to the desirable goals. In this
regard, our results may provide the neural evidence for the goal
theory of well-being.
Anticipation for future is a process of emotion regulation
(Erk et al., 2006;Grupe et al., 2013). Thus, the neuroimaging
results can also be interpreted from the perspective of emotion
regulation. Anticipating the future allows allocating cognitive and
emotional resources and planning behavior strategies to cope
the upcoming events (Erk et al., 2006;Grupe et al., 2013). For
example, anticipating positive events can induce positive emotion
to cope the social stress (e.g., Monfort et al., 2015). Previous
findings suggest that MPFC and ACC are highly implicated in
emotion regulation (Phillips et al., 2008;Etkin et al., 2015).
Specifically, MPFC and ACC are thought to be part of model-
free emotion regulation system, in which MPFC and ACC
encoding the experience-dependent value of regulatory actions,
which modulate activity in emotional-reactivity regions, such
as the amygdala, insula, dorsal ACC, and periaqueductal gray
(Etkin et al., 2015). The MPFC activation during the anticipating
future positive events in our study may indicate people felt more
positive emotion when anticipating positive future, which in turn
enhanced people’s well-being. In this perspective, our results were
consistent with Etkin et al.’s (2015) framework.
However, we did not observe the functional connectivity
between MPFC and other emotion-related areas, such as the
amygdala. Also, we did not observe amygdala activation during
positive anticipation relative to neutral anticipation. Our results
were inconsistent with prior studies which show that amygdala is
highly involved in emotional perception (e.g., Sergerie et al., 2008;
Cunningham and Kirkland, 2014) and has different coupling
pattern with other areas during different tasks of emotion
processing (Lee et al., 2012;Diano et al., 2016, 2017;Di et al.,
2017). Reviewing the literature of neural correlates of emotion
anticipation, we found amygdala was consistently activated
during anticipation of aversive events, such as negative picture
(e.g., Ueda et al., 2003;Erk et al., 2006;Nitschke et al., 2006)
and pain (e.g., Wager et al., 2004), but there is little evidence
that amygdala was involved in positive anticipation (e.g., Ueda
et al., 2003;Scherpiet et al., 2014). One reason that the lack
of significant results in the amygdala in our study could be
that amygdala was more involved in negative anticipation, but
the task we employed didn’t include anticipation of negative
events. Our results might indicate that amygdala was involved in
perception phase of positive emotion events (e.g., Cunningham
and Kirkland, 2014), but it didn’t be involved in the anticipation
phase. However, the role of amygdala in the emotion anticipation
needs further investigation.
Although we provide evidence that MPFC activation during
positive anticipation can predict the levels of well-being, several
limits and future directions of this study need to be considered.
Firstly, the cross-section design hinders the conclusion of the
causal relationships between the brain activity and well-being. It
will be of merit for future work using neuroimaging modulation
technique, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), to alter
brain activity and find the causal relationship. Secondly, we
did not find the amygdala activation in positive anticipation,
but the previous study found amygdala was activated when
positive stimuli were presented (Cunningham and Kirkland,
2014). This may indicate the distinct brain regions involved in
emotional perception and anticipation. Nevertheless, whether
the amygdala is activated in positive anticipation needs further
investigation. Thirdly, we speculated the psychological processes
during anticipation in this study. However, future studies may
be beneficial to collect the online self-report of the motivation
to the hedonic cue when people were anticipating. Fourthly,
the contents of pictures used in positive and neutral condition
weren’t rigidly matched in the current study. Although we tried
to select pictures depicted people’s activities in both conditions,
the content of the stimuli in both conditions were not rigidly
matched. The content difference of the pictures between positive
and neutral condition may confound the neural activation
results. For example, amygdala may respond to human faces
or bodies (Van den Stock et al., 2014;Wang et al., 2014) but
not to household appliances. However, this may not a fatal
factor that confounded our results, because we are interest in
the anticipation phase of emotion instead of the perception
phase. We rigidly matched the physical properties of the cues
between two conditions. But the contents of pictures from both
conditions should be rigidly matched in future studies. Lastly,
our study did not include the anticipation for negative events
to control for potentially confounding factors. However, given
the negative psychopathological consequences resulting from
exaggerated negative anticipation (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013;
Heitmann et al., 2014), future studies examining the association
between well-being and neural underpinning of anticipation to
the negative emotion may be interesting.
CONCLUSION
In this study, we used emotional anticipation task to investigate
how the neural underpinning during anticipating to future
positive events was associated with individual difference of well-
being. The results showed that bilateral MPFC was activated
during anticipation for positive events relative to neutral events,
and MPFC activities were positively correlated the levels of well-
being. The findings may be consistent with the goal theory of
well-being and provide the dynamic, future-oriented view on
well-being.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
YL and XH conceived and designed the experiments. YL
performed the experiments. YL and XC analyzed the data. YL,
XC, SQ, XY, and XH wrote the paper. All authors contributed to
and have approved the final manuscript.
FUNDING
This work was supported by the National Natural
Science Foundation of China (grant number 31600913);
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 7
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
the Youth foundation for Humanities and Social Science
Research of Ministry of Education (grant number 15XJC190001);
China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (grant numbers
2016M590918 and 2017T100724); Shaanxi Postdoctoral Science
Foundation (grant number 2016BSHTDZZ10); Young Talent
Fund of University Association for Science and Technology
in Shaanxi, China (grant number 20160210); the Ministry
of Education (MOE) of China, Key Project of Philosophy
and Social Science (grant number 11JZD044); and the
Research Team’s Construction Project from the Faculty of
Psychology in Southwest University (2012) (grant number
TR201201-1).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to thank the two reviewers who provided
helpful comments on a previous version of this article. The
authors also would like to thank all the participants who took part
in the project.
REFERENCES
Abler, B., Erk, S., Herwig, U., and Walter, H. (2007). Anticipation of aversive
stimuli activates extended amygdala in unipolar depression. J. Psychiatr. Res.
41, 511–522. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.020
Amodio, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex
and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277. doi: 10.1038/Nrn1884
Argyle, M., Martin, M., and Crossland, J. (1989). “Happiness as a function of
personality and social encounters, in Happiness as a Function of Personality and
Social Encounters, eds J. P. Forgas and J. M. Innes (North Holland: Elsevier),
189–203.
Aupperle, R. L., Allard, C. B., Grimes, E. M., Simmons, A. N., Flagan, T.,
Behrooznia, M., et al. (2012). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation
during emotional anticipation and neuropsychological performance in
posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 360–371. doi: 10.1001/
archgenpsychiatry.2011.1539
Bai, L., Ma, H., Huang, Y., and Luo, Y. (2005). The development of native chinese
affective picture system—a pretest in 46 college students. Chin. Ment. Health J.
19, 719–722.
Bermpohl, F., Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Fregni, F., Gaab, N.,
et al. (2006). Dissociable networks for the expectancy and perception of
emotional stimuli in the human brain. Neuroimage 30, 588–600. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.09.040
Bermpohl, F., Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Fregni, F., Wrase, J.,
et al. (2008). Novelty seeking modulates medial prefrontal activity during the
anticipation of emotional stimuli. Psychiatry Res. 164, 81–85. doi: 10.1016/j.
pscychresns.2007.12.019
Boehme, S., Ritter, V., Tefikow, S., Stangier, U., Strauss, B., Miltner, W. H., et al.
(2014). Brain activation during anticipatory anxiety in social anxiety disorder.
Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1413–1418. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst129
Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and
negative affect: a control-process view. Psychol. Rev. 97, 19–35. doi: 10.1037/
0033-295X.97.1.19
Costa, V. D., Lang, P. J., Sabatinelli, D., Versace, F., and Bradley, M. M. (2010).
Emotional imagery: assessing pleasure and arousal in the brain’s reward
circuitry. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1446–1457. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20948
Crowell, A., and Schmeichel, B. J. (2016). Approach motivation and cognitive
resources combine to influence memory for positive emotional stimuli. Cogn.
Emot. 30, 389–397. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.1000829
Cunningham, W. A., and Kirkland, T. (2014). The joyful, yet balanced, amygdala:
moderated responses to positive but not negative stimuli in trait happiness. Soc.
Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 760–766. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst045
D’Argembeau, A., Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M.,
Feyers, D., et al. (2010). The neural basis of personal goal processing when
envisioning future events. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1701–1713. doi: 10.1162/jocn.
2009.21314
Davis, M., and Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol.
Psychiatry 6, 13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812
Di, X., Huang, J., and Biswal, B. B. (2017). Task modulated brain connectivity of
the amygdala: a meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Brain Struct.
Funct. 222, 619–634. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016- 1239-4
Diano, M., Celeghin, A., Bagnis, A., and Tamietto,M. (2016). Amygdala response to
emotional stimuli without awareness: facts and interpretations. Front. Psychol.
7:2029. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02029
Diano, M., Tamietto, M., Celeghin, A., Weiskrantz, L., Tatu, M. K., Bagnis, A.,
et al. (2017). Dynamic changes in amygdala psychophysiological connectivity
reveal distinct neural networks for facial expressions of basic emotions. Sci. Rep.
7:45260. doi: 10.1038/srep45260
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., and Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being:
three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 125, 276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.
125.2.276
Epstein, J., Pan, H., Kocsis, J. H., Yang, Y., Butler,T., C husid,J., et al. (2006). L ack of
ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects.
Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1784–1790. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1784
Erk, S., Abler, B., and Walter, H. (2006). Cognitive modulation of emotion
anticipation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1227–1236. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.
04976.x
Etkin, A., Buchel, C., and Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044
Etkin, A., Egner, T., and Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior
cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 85–93.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., and Elliot, A. J. (2003). Evidence for bivariate systems:
an empirical test of appetition and aversion across domains. J. Res. Pers. 37,
349–372. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00580- 9
Gallagher, M. W., Lopez, S. J., and Preacher, K. J. (2009). The hierarchical structure
of well-being. J. Pers. 77, 1025–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00573.x
Gilbert, D. T., and Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: experiencing the future.
Science 317, 1351–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1144161
Greve, D. N., and Fischl, B. (2009). Accurate and robust brain image alignment
using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2009.06.060
Grupe, D. W., and Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety:
an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
14, 488–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524
Grupe, D. W., Oathes, D. J., and Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Dissecting the anticipation
of aversion reveals dissociable neural networks. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1874–1883.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs175
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial
prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode
of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 4259–4264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
071043098
Heitmann, C. Y., Peterburs, J., Mothes-Lasch, M., Hallfarth, M. C., Bohme, S.,
Miltner, W. H., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of anticipation and processing
of performance feedback in social anxiety. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 6023–6031.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.22602
Heller, A. S., van Reekum, C. M., Schaefer, S. M., Lapate, R. C., Radler, B. T.,
Ryff, C. D., et al. (2013). Sustained striatal activity predicts eudaimonic
well-being and cortisol output. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2191–2200. doi: 10.1177/
0956797613490744
Herwig, U., Abler, B., Walter, H., and Erk, S. (2007). Expecting unpleasant stimuli–
an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 154, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.
02.007
Herwig, U., Bruhl, A. B., Kaffenberger, T., Baumgartner, T., Boeker, H., and
Jancke, L. (2010). Neural correlates of ‘pessimistic’ attitude in depression.
Psychol. Med. 40, 789–800. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991073
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved
optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
fpsyg-08-02199 January 6, 2018 Time: 16:23 # 8
Luo et al. Well-being and Positive Anticipation
correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.
1132
Kashdan, T. B., and Steger, M. F. (2007). Curiosity and pathways to well-being and
meaning in life: traits, states, and everyday behaviors. Motiv. Emot. 31, 159–173.
doi: 10.1007/s11031-007- 9068-7
Kringelbach, M. L., and Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy
of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.
2009.08.006
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective
Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. Gainesville, FL:
University of Florida.
Lee, H., Heller, A. S., van Reekum, C. M., Nelson, B., and Davidson, R. J. (2012).
Amygdala-prefrontal coupling underlies individual differences in emotion
regulation. Neuroimage 62, 1575–1581. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.044
Lin, H., Gao, H., Ye, Z., Wang, P., Tao,L., Ke, X., et al. (2012). Expect ation enhances
event-related responses to affective stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 522, 123–127.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.022
Lu, L., and Shih, J. B. (1997). Personality and happiness: Is mental health a
mediator? Pers. Individ. Dif. 22, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00187- 0
Luo, Y., Huang, X., Yang, Z., Li, B., Liu, J., and Wei, D. (2014). Regional
homogeneity of intrinsic brain activity in happy and unhappy individuals. PLOS
ONE 9:e85181. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085181
MacLeod, A. K., and Byrne, A. (1996). Anxiety, depression, and the anticipation
of future positive and negative experiences. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 286–289.
doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.286
MacLeod, A. K., and Conway, C. (2005). Well-being and the anticipation of future
positive experiences: the role of income, social networks, and planning ability.
Cogn. Emot. 19, 357–374. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000247
MacLeod, A. K., Pankhania, B., Lee, M., and Mitchell, D. (1997). Depression,
hopelessness and future-directed thinking in parasuicide. Psychol. Med. 27,
973–977. doi: 10.1017/S003329179600459X
Monfort, S. S., Stroup, H. E., and Waugh, C. E. (2015). The impact of anticipating
positive events on responses to stress. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 58, 11–22.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.12.003
Mumford, J. (2007). A Guide to Calculating Percent Change with Featquery.
Available at: http://mumford.bol.ucla.edu/perchange_guide.pdf
Nitschke, J. B., Sarinopoulos, I., Mackiewicz, K. L., Schaefer, H. S., and Davidson,
R. J. (2006). Functional neuroanatomy of aversion and its anticipation.
Neuroimage 29, 106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.068
Nitschke, J. B., Sarinopoulos, I., Oathes, D. J., Johnstone, T., Whalen, P. J.,
Davidson, R. J., et al. (2009). Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and
anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment
response. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 302–310. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101682
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., Greck, M., Bennpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., and
Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain - a meta-analysis
of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.12.002
Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010
Phelps, E. A., and LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion
processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48, 175–187.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025
Phillips, M. L., Ladouceur, C. D., and Drevets, W. C. (2008). A neural model of
voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding
the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry
13, 833–857. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.65
Sarinopoulos, I., Grupe, D. W., Mackiewicz, K. L., Herrington, J. D., Lor,M., Stee ge,
E. E., et al. (2010). Uncertainty during anticipation modulates neural responses
to aversion in human insula and amygdala. Cereb. Cortex 20, 929–940.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp155
Scherpiet, S., Bruhl, A., Opialla, S., Roth, L., Jancke, L., and Herwig, U. (2014).
Altered emotion processing circuits during the anticipation of emotional
stimuli in women with borderline personality disorder. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry
Clin. Neurosci. 264, 45–60. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013- 0444-x
Seligman, M. E., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: an
introduction. Am. Psychol. 55, 5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.1.89
Seligman, M. E., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., and Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating
into the future or driven by the past. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 119–141.
doi: 10.1177/1745691612474317
Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., and Armony, J. L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in
emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging
studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 811–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.
12.002
Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural
mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105. doi: 10.1038/
nature06280
Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17,
143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens,
T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural
MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1),
S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051
Ueda, K., Okamoto, Y., Okada, G., Yamashita, H., Hori, T., and Yamawaki, S.
(2003). Brain activity during expectancy of emotional stimuli: an fMRI study.
NeuroReport 14, 51–55. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000050712.17082.1c
Van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Zhan, M., Heinecke, A., Hervais-Adelman, A.,
Legrand, L. B., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of body and face perception
following bilateral destruction of the primary visual cortices. Front. Behav.
Neurosci. 8:30. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00030
van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Johnstone, T., Thurow, M. E., Frye, C. J., Jackson,
C. A., et al. (2007). Individual differences in amygdala and ventromedial
prefrontal cortex activity are associated with evaluation speed and psychological
well-being. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 237–248. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.
19.2.237
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokolik, A., Casey, K. L., Davidson,
R. J., et al. (2004). Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and
experience of pain. Science 303, 1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065
Wang, S., Tudusciuc, O., Mamelak, A. N., Ross, I. B., Adolphs, R., and
Rutishauser, U. (2014). Neurons in the human amygdala selective for perceived
emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, E3110–E3119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.
1323342111
Worsley, K. J. (2001). “Statistical analysis of activation images, in Functional MRI:
An Introduction to Methods, eds P. Jezzard, P. M. Matthews, and S. M. Smith
(Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Yang, J., Yuan, J., and Li, H. (2012). Expectation decreases brain susceptibility
to fearful stimuli: ERP evidence from a modified emotion evaluation task.
Neurosci. Lett. 514, 198–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.094
Yuan, J., Zhang, Q., Chen, A., Li, H., Wang, Q., Zhuang, Z., et al. (2007).
Are we sensitive to valence differences in emotionally negative stimuli?
Electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 45,
2764–2771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.018
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer MD and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.
Copyright © 2018 Luo, Chen, Qi, You and Huang. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution
or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8January 2018 | Volume 8 | Article 2199
... It is also imperative to include that, incorporating such positive and future-oriented meanings is supported by other psychological and neurological documents. Current research in psychology and neuroscience suggests advantages associated with anticipating positive future events (Bermpohl et al., 2008;Carver & Scheier, 2014;Erk et al., 2006;Grupe et al., 2013;MacLeod & Conway, 2005;Monfort et al., 2015;Yangmei et al., 2018), while evidence also indicates the negative consequences of lacking positive expectations about future good occurrences (Carver & Scheier, 2014;Chang, 1998;Scheier et al., 1994). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: This study explores the effectiveness of Religious Savior-Centered Psychotherapy (RSCP) in treating depression in religious women. RSCP is a therapeutic approach that integrates Islamic teachings, particularly the concept of a Savior, with cognitive and behavioral strategies to address mental health challenges. Unlike traditional therapy models, RSCP emphasizes spiritual healing by fostering a strong connection to faith and a hopeful outlook for the future. Aims: The study aims to assess RSCP's impact on depression in women by aligning with Islamic teachings and emphasizing positive future expectations. Methods: A pretest-posttest design with a two-month follow-up was employed to evaluate the effects of RSCP. The study included 70 women aged 18-60 from five counseling clinics in Tehran, all diagnosed with mild to moderate depression, and having expressed a belief in a Savior. MANCOVA was used to analyze the data, ensuring assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were met. Results: MANCOVA results indicated a significant linear correlation between the treatment and depression levels (p < 0.05), with a large effect size (partial η² = .61 for post-test and .54 for follow-up). Initial findings suggest RSCP's potential benefit on mental well-being. Conclusions: RSCP has the potential to be integrated into existing mental health frameworks as a culturally sensitive option, particularly for Muslim women seeking faith-based approaches to mental health care. Future research with larger samples, randomized controlled trials, and longitudinal studies is needed to explore the effectiveness of this new positive psychotherapy approach. Keywords: Spiritual Psychotherapy, Religious Psychotherapy, Faith-Focused Psychotherapy, Depression, Well-being
... Therefore, the anticipation of engaging in the campaign serves as a motivating factor and a positive anchor, irrespective of the challenges he may be facing. Past research has shown that anticipating something can be a positive and powerful emotion (Luo et al. 2018;Monfort et al. 2015). Specifically, looking forward to something has a more positive impact on one's mental health than looking back on it (Van Boven and Ashworth 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Sarah Lynne Bowman, William J. White, Evan Torner -- "Editorial: The Increasing Specificity and Maturity of Role-playing Game Studies" Kjell Hedgard Hugaas -- “Bleed and Identity: A Conceptual Model of Bleed and How Bleed-out from Role-playing Games Can Affect a Player’s Sense of Self” Orla Walsh and Conor Linehan -- “Roll for Insight: Understanding How the Experience of Playing Dungeons & Dragons Impacts the Mental Health of a Frequent Player” Emry Sottile -- “‘It Might Have a Little to Do with Wish Fulfillment’: The Life-Giving Force of Queer Performance in TTRPG Spaces” Kerttu Lehto -- “Nordic Larp as a Method in Mental Health Care and Substance Abuse Work: Case SÄRÖT” Lennart Bartenstein -- “Live Action Role-playing (Larp) in Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapy: A Case Study” Maryanne Cullinan -- “Surveying the Perspectives of Middle and High School Educators Who Use Role-playing Games as Pedagogy” Mátyás Hartyándi and Gijs van Bilsen -- “Playing With Leadership: A Multiple-Case Study of Leadership Development Larps” Jeremy Riel and Rob Monahan -- “Learning from Ludemes: An Inventory of Common Player Actions within Tabletop Role-Playing Games (TTRPGs) to Inform Principled Design of Game-Based Learning Experiences”
... Therefore, the anticipation of engaging in the campaign serves as a motivating factor and a positive anchor, irrespective of the challenges he may be facing. Past research has shown that anticipating something can be a positive and powerful emotion (Luo et al. 2018;Monfort et al. 2015). Specifically, looking forward to something has a more positive impact on one's mental health than looking back on it (Van Boven and Ashworth 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Dungeons & Dragons (D&D) is a fantasy tabletop role-playing game published by Wizards of the Coast. Evidence shows that D&D is being used as therapeutic practice by clinical psychologists, and the reported success of these interventions indicate that it may have positive outcomes for its players. However, there is little research that establishes, from a psychosocial perspective, how playing D&D provides opportunities for supporting frequent player’s mental health. Using semi-structured interviews and a reflexive thematic analysis, we aimed to understand better how playing D&D interacted with players’ well-being psychologically and socially, as well as exploring factors that may prove challenging or problematic in the therapeutic context. 6 men, 2 women, and 2 nonbinary people participated in the study. Analysis illustrated several processes that are supportive for players’ mental health. These features were identified as five key themes: escapism, exploration of self, creative expression, social support, and routine. These findings highlight the transformative potential of D&D in a therapeutic setting, offering insights into its positive impact.
... Khan et al. [22] find correlations between Big5 personality traits and LIWC scores. The relevant LIWC categories are: work [67], tentative [68], positive emotion [48], and negative emotion [69]. Similarly, in our study, we also find a few traits have also correlations between self-efficacy and LIWC scores (Section IV) which include work [70], tentative [71], time [72], positive emotion [55], negative emotion [69] and affect [73]. ...
Article
Full-text available
General Self-Efficacy (GSE) is a vital attribute of human psychology that describes one’s belief about his own ability to succeed in specific situations. GSE is composed of cognitive, social, and behavioral skills of an individual. In this research, we first develop a GSE classification model by using Facebook content (i.e., profile photos and statuses). We collect data from a total of 435 Facebook users in an ethical data collection manner. Two hybrid machine learning methods are applied based on distinct feature extraction approaches: tool-based and deep learning-based. In our tool-based approach, we employ Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) and Bidirectional Encoder Representations from Transformers (BERT) for text and Mediapipe and DeepFace for image feature extraction. We apply Particle Swarm Optimization (PSO) for feature selection, resulting in a robust tabular dataset with high predictive performance for GSE scores. In the deep learning-based approach, we apply BERT and 1-dimensional convolutional neural network (1D-CNN) for text feature extraction, while UNet++ handles image segmentation, and VGG16 and ResNet-152 contribute image features, fused via Canonical Correlation Analysis (CCA). We also integrate a co-attention model for image and text features. Traditional machine learning models, including Random Forest (RF), Xgboost, AdaBoost, and Stacking, are then trained on the feature set to predict GSE scores. This comprehensive model showcases a multifaceted approach to GSE prediction, combining tool-based and deep learning methodologies for enhanced accuracy and insights. Then, we develop a GSE prediction model by using the mentioned tool-based (i.e., LIWC, BERT, Mediapipe, and DeepFace) and deep learning-based feature extraction methods from both image and text datasets. The tool-based model achieves remarkable accuracy percentages of 85.80% (text), 91.06% (image), and an outstanding 93.25% for the hybrid model. The deep learning-based model exhibits competitive results, with accuracies of 64.80% (text), 73.06% (image), and 81.87% for the hybrid model.
... Participants simply knowing that they will be receiving treatment to reduce their loneliness could have led them to implement prosocial behaviors preemptively, such as growing or maintaining their existing social networks or engaging in social activities. Past research has also found anticipation of positive events to be related to well-being, implying that when participants look forward to receiving the intervention, it may result in an increase in their subjective well-being, reducing loneliness in the process (Luo et al., 2017;Macleod & Conway, 2005). Table 2. Comparison of scores before control period (week 0), before intervention (week 6), and after intervention (week 12). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Loneliness and chronic stress are prevalent issues for older adults that have been linked to adverse health outcomes. We conducted a remote resilience and self-compassion intervention targeting loneliness and chronic stress. Methods: This study utilized a multiple-phase-change single-case experimental design with three consecutive 6-week phases: control, intervention, follow-up. Assessments and biomarker collection (blood pressure, inflammation, sleep actigraphy) were conducted at each phase. Participants completed a 6-week remotely-administered resilience and self-compassion intervention using techniques from cognitive behavioral therapy and resilience training. Repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted over the 12-week period from control (week 0) to intervention completion (week 12) and over the 18-week period from control (week 0) to follow-up (week 18) in supplemental analyses. Results: Participants reported a reduction in stress (p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.15), depression (p = 0.02; ηp2 = 0.08), and loneliness (p = 0.003; ηp2 = 0.18), and an increase in self-compassion (p = 0.01; ηp2 = 0.13) from control to intervention completion (weeks 0-12). Post-hoc tests revealed that stress reduced significantly during the intervention phase (weeks 6-12) and loneliness reduced significantly during the control phase (weeks 0-6). Some improvements in blood pressure, inflammation, and sleep quality were noted in a subsample of participants. Conclusion: Findings indicate that our remote resilience and self-compassion intervention for older adults targeting loneliness and chronic stress was efficacious.
Article
This research addresses the role of different phases of consumption—anticipation, acquisition, and usage—in the relationship between clothing consumption curtailment (CCC) and increased consumer subjective well-being (CSWB). Building on past research, we theorize and empirically explore whether increased CSWB is explained by a change in focus from acquisition to usage. Through a content analysis of 140 blog posts from clothes shopping detoxers, we unearth how reduced acquisition and intensive and extended usage manifest in CCC practices. Furthermore, we apply structural equation modeling (SEM) to representative survey data (N = 661) to show that focusing on acquisition reduction is not associated with CSWB, while intensive and extended usage are positively associated with CSWB. In addition, we establish that this relationship is partially mediated by improved body image. Our results open a path for further research, and can be utilized in social marketing to promote the intensive usage rather than acquisition of clothing.
Article
This perspective article discusses the physical, mental and sociocultural benefits of international travel and considers the case for prescribing travel for therapeutic reasons. It also identifies priorities for further research in this neglected area. ‘To move, to breathe, to fly, to float, To gain all while you give, To roam the roads of lands remote, To travel is to live.’. Hans Christian Andersen, The Fairy Tale of My Life: An Autobiography.
Article
Energetic demand and nutrient supply fluctuate as a function of time-of-day, in alignment with sleep-wake and fasting-feeding cycles. These daily rhythms are mirrored by 24-hour oscillations in numerous cardiovascular functional parameters, including blood pressure, heart rate, and myocardial contractility. It is, therefore, not surprising that metabolic processes also fluctuate over the course of the day, to ensure temporal needs for ATP, building blocks, and metabolism-based signaling molecules are met. What has become increasingly clear is that in addition to classic signal-response coupling (termed reactionary mechanisms), cardiovascular-relevant cells use autonomous circadian clocks to temporally orchestrate metabolic pathways in preparation for predicted stimuli/stresses (termed anticipatory mechanisms). Here, we review current knowledge regarding circadian regulation of metabolism, how metabolic rhythms are synchronized with cardiovascular function, and whether circadian misalignment/disruption of metabolic processes contribute toward the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease.
Article
Full-text available
The quest to characterize the neural signature distinctive of different basic emotions has recently come under renewed scrutiny. Here we investigated whether facial expressions of different basic emotions modulate the functional connectivity of the amygdala with the rest of the brain. To this end, we presented seventeen healthy participants (8 females) with facial expressions of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and emotional neutrality and analyzed amygdala?s psychophysiological interaction (PPI). In fact, PPI can reveal how inter-regional amygdala communications change dynamically depending on perception of various emotional expressions to recruit different brain networks, compared to the functional interactions it entertains during perception of neutral expressions. We found that for each emotion the amygdala recruited a distinctive and spatially distributed set of structures to interact with. These changes in amygdala connectional patters characterize the dynamic signature prototypical of individual emotion processing, and seemingly represent a neural mechanism that serves to implement the distinctive influence that each emotion exerts on perceptual, cognitive, and motor responses. Besides these differences, all emotions enhanced amygdala functional integration with premotor cortices compared to neutral faces. The present findings thus concur to reconceptualise the structure-function relation between brain-emotion from the traditional one-to-one mapping toward a network-based and dynamic perspective.
Article
Full-text available
Over the past two decades, evidence has accumulated that the human amygdala exerts some of its functions also when the observer is not aware of the content, or even presence, of the triggering emotional stimulus. Nevertheless, there is as of yet no consensus on the limits and conditions that affect the extent of amygdala’s response without focused attention or awareness. Here we review past and recent studies on this subject, examining neuroimaging literature on healthy participants as well as brain-damaged patients, and we comment on their strengths and limits. We propose a theoretical distinction between processes involved in attentional unawareness, wherein the stimulus is potentially accessible to enter visual awareness but fails to do so because attention is diverted, and in sensory unawareness, wherein the stimulus fails to enter awareness because its normal processing in the visual cortex is suppressed. We argue this distinction, along with data sampling amygdala responses with high temporal resolution, helps to appreciate the multiplicity of functional and anatomical mechanisms centered on the amygdala and supporting its role in non-conscious emotion processing. Separate, but interacting, networks relay visual information to the amygdala exploiting different computational properties of subcortical and cortical routes, thereby supporting amygdala functions at different stages of emotion processing. This view reconciles some apparent contradictions in the literature, as well as seemingly contrasting proposals, such as the dual stage and the dual route model. We conclude that evidence in favor of the amygdala response without awareness is solid, albeit this response originates from different functional mechanisms and is driven by more complex neural networks than commonly assumed. Acknowledging the complexity of such mechanisms can foster new insights on the varieties of amygdala functions without awareness and their impact on human behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding functional connectivity of the amygdala with other brain regions, especially task modulated connectivity, is a critical step toward understanding the role of the amygdala in emotional processes and the interactions between emotion and cognition. The present study performed coordinate-based meta-analysis on studies of task modulated connectivity of the amygdala which used psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis. We first analyzed 49 PPI studies on different types of tasks using activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Widespread cortical and subcortical regions showed consistent task modulated connectivity with the amygdala, including the medial frontal cortex, bilateral insula, anterior cingulate, fusiform gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, thalamus, and basal ganglia. These regions were in general overlapped with those showed coactivations with the amygdala, suggesting that these regions and amygdala are not only activated together, but also show different levels of interactions during tasks. Further analyses with subsets of PPI studies revealed task specific functional connectivities with the amygdala that were modulated by fear processing, face processing, and emotion regulation. These results suggest a dynamic modulation of connectivity upon task demands, and provide new insights on the functions of the amygdala in different affective and cognitive processes. The meta-analytic approach on PPI studies may offer a framework toward systematical examinations of task modulated connectivity.
Article
Emotions are powerful determinants of behaviour, thought and experience, and they may be regulated in various ways. Neuroimaging studies have implicated several brain regions in emotion regulation, including the ventral anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal cortices, as well as the lateral prefrontal and parietal cortices. Drawing on computational approaches to value-based decision-making and reinforcement learning, we propose a unifying conceptual framework for understanding the neural bases of diverse forms of emotion regulation.
Article
The question of how affect arises and what affect indicates is examined from a feedback-based viewpoint on self-regulation. Using the analogy of action control as the attempt to diminish distance to a goal, a second feedback system is postulated that senses and regulates the rate at which the action-guiding system is functioning. This second system is seen as responsible for affect. Implications of these assertions and issues that arise from them are addressed in the remainder of the article. Several issues relate to the emotion model itself; others concern the relation between negative emotion and disengagement from goals. Relations to 3 other emotion theories are also addressed. The authors conclude that this view on affect is a useful supplement to other theories and that the concept of emotion is easily assimilated to feedback models of self-regulation.
Article
Inspired by the elaborated intrusion theory of desire, the current research tested the hypothesis that persons higher in trait approach motivation process positive stimuli deeply, which enhances memory for them. Ninety-four undergraduates completed a measure of trait approach motivation, viewed positive or negative image slideshows in the presence or absence of a cognitive load, and one week later completed an image memory test. Higher trait approach motivation predicted better memory for the positive slideshow, but this memory boost disappeared under cognitive load. Approach motivation did not influence memory for the negative slideshow. The current findings support the idea that individuals higher in approach motivation spontaneously devote limited resources to processing positive stimuli.
Article
The few studies examining the impact of positive emotions on discrete stressors suggest that positive emotions improve stress responding. We hypothesized that merely anticipating a positive event would be sufficient to harness these benefits. In Study 1, we found that the anticipation of funny (relative to unfunny) cartoons increased positive emotions immediately following the offset of a social stressor. In Study 2, we found that the post-stress mood elevation was greater when anticipating a positive event than when having experienced the same positive event prior to the stressor, but that both positive emotion groups reported more adaptive thoughts during the stressor itself compared to participants receiving a neutral emotion induction. In Study 3, we found that this boost in post-stress positive emotion predicts decreases in concurrent negative emotion. In sum, these findings suggest that anticipating a positive event is uniquely able to induce positive emotions both during and after stress, and that this boost subserves improved coping and recovery.