ArticlePDF Available

Is ‘summer’ reading loss universal? Using ongoing literacy assessment in Malawi to estimate the loss from grade-transition breaks

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Summer learning loss – decreased academic performance following an extended school break, typically during the period after one grade ends and before another grade starts – is a well-documented phenomenon in North America, but poorly described in sub-Saharan African contexts. In this article, we use the term ‘grade-transition break’ loss in lieu of ‘summer’ loss to refer to the period after one grade ends and before another grade starts. This study analyses data from early grade reading assessments in Malawi, estimating statistically significant average reductions of 0.38 standard deviations (SD) across several measures of reading and pre-reading skills during two grade-transition breaks. The data show the loss in reading skills during the extended breaks between grades 1 and 2 and between grades 2 and 3 in two consecutive years. The study found no gender-based differences in loss. The findings suggest a need for early grade reading interventions to develop and evaluate mitigation strategies lest significant proportions of within-year performance gains be lost over the break between academic years.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745499917740657
Research in Comparative &
International Education
2017, Vol. 12(4) 461 –485
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1745499917740657
journals.sagepub.com/home/rci
Is ‘summer’ reading loss
universal? Using ongoing
literacy assessment in Malawi
to estimate the loss from
grade-transition breaks
Timothy S Slade
International Education Division, RTI International, USA
Benjamin Piper
International Education Division, RTI International, USA
Zikani Kaunda
International Education Division, RTI International, USA
Simon King
International Education Division, RTI International, USA
Hibatalla Ibrahim
McKinsey & Company, USA
Abstract
Summer learning loss – decreased academic performance following an extended school break, typically
during the period after one grade ends and before another grade starts – is a well-documented phenomenon
in North America, but poorly described in sub-Saharan African contexts. In this article, we use the term
‘grade-transition break’ loss in lieu of ‘summer’ loss to refer to the period after one grade ends and before
another grade starts. This study analyses data from early grade reading assessments in Malawi, estimating
statistically significant average reductions of 0.38 standard deviations (SD) across several measures of reading
and pre-reading skills during two grade-transition breaks. The data show the loss in reading skills during the
extended breaks between grades 1 and 2 and between grades 2 and 3 in two consecutive years. The study
found no gender-based differences in loss. The findings suggest a need for early grade reading interventions
to develop and evaluate mitigation strategies lest significant proportions of within-year performance gains be
lost over the break between academic years.
Corresponding author:
Timothy S Slade, DCOP–Operations, Tusome; and RTI International, RTI Nairobi Regional Office, The Westwood, 5th
Floor, Vale Close, Westlands, P.O. Box 1181 Village Market, 00621 Nairobi, Kenya.
Email: tslade@rti.org
740657RCI0010.1177/1745499917740657Research in Comparative and International EducationSlade et al.
research-article2017
Article
462 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Keywords
Reading, literacy, Malawi, Africa, reading difficulties, reading acquisition, summer learning loss, grade
transition
Introduction
Remarkable strides have been made towards achieving universal primary education, largely the
result of the enthusiasm brought about by the global Education for All movement and the second
Millennium Development Goal. These accomplishments have been particularly exceptional in
sub-Saharan Africa, where the enrolment rate increased from 52% in 1990 to 80% in 2015
(UNESCO Institute for Statistics (UIS), 2015). In Malawi, for example, primary school enrolment
increased from 1.8 million to 3.2 million soon after the declaration of Free Primary Education in
1994 (National Statistical Office, 2011).
Despite these achievements, significant challenges to the quality of education in sub-Saharan
Africa remain. Recent assessment data suggest that children in the region have alarmingly low
levels of literacy. In Malawi, 98% of grade 2 students and 56% of grade 4 students were unable to
read a single word in English (Dowd et al., 2010; UNESCO, 2012). Results elsewhere on the con-
tinent are equally disheartening, with disturbingly low literacy outcomes identified by many coun-
tries across sub-Saharan Africa (Gove and Cvelich, 2011).
A need to increase emphasis on improving learning outcomes has been identified by many
countries in the region, including Kenya (MoEST, 2015), Ethiopia (MOE, 2013) and Malawi
(Global Partnership for Education, 2016; MoEST, 2008). Recent research has shown a much
stronger link between learning, and particularly literacy, and economic outcomes such as labour
productivity and growth in the gross domestic product (Coulombe and Tremblay, 2006; National
Institute for Literacy, 2010).
The link between education and economic growth, though well-established in the economic
literature, typically has been estimated using quantitative measures of education, such as years
of schooling, rather than cognitive skills or learning outcomes. Hanushek and Woessmann
(2015) argued that tests of cognitive skills, such as international tests of student performance, are
far more important determinants of economic growth than years of schooling. Initial income,
years of schooling and cognitive skills explain 73% of the variation in economic growth, as
opposed to 25% of the variation when cognitive skills are removed from the model (Hanushek
and Woessmann, 2015). Like them, other researchers are now concluding that education quality
as measured by learning is a more robust measure of educational output than the aforementioned
quantity measures.
Thus, focusing on education outcomes in terms of learning rather than years of schooling is
central to policy making (Glewwe and Muralidharan, 2016). Evidence suggests that more school-
ing does not necessarily correlate with education outcomes beyond that predicted by the cross-
country relationship between per-capita income and learning outcomes (Singh, 2016). On the other
hand, interventions that aim to improve pedagogy, such as teaching of literacy skills, have been
found to produce consistently positive effects on learning outcomes, which in turn result in greater
returns on spending (Snilstveit et al., 2015).
Policy makers in sub-Saharan Africa have rapidly mobilized to shift their focus from merely
improving access, to ensuring that schooling translates to increases in learning outcomes. Over the
past several years, real expenditure on education rose by 6% annually (Gove and Cvelich, 2011;
UIS, 2011). Many countries are increasing their investments in evidence-based literacy programs.
Among them are South Africa (Gauteng Primary Language and Mathematics Strategy – Fleisch
and Schöer, 2014), Kenya (the Primary Math and Reading [PRIMR] Initiative and the Tusome
national literacy program – Piper et al., 2015, 2014) and Gambia (Early Literacy in National
Slade et al. 463
Languages pilot program – UNESCO, 2014). Effects from these medium- to large-scale programs
are encouraging, with effect sizes ranging from 0.34 to 0.58 standard deviations (SD).
This emphasis on education outcomes can also help policy makers look beyond school-based
interventions to home- or community-based ones. This is because pedagogical interventions alone
(such as the literacy- and numeracy-focused programs mentioned above) may not be enough to
raise literacy rates to the levels that sub-Saharan African policy makers are targeting. To illustrate,
community-based interventions were found to increase test scores ranging from 0.09 to 0.12 stand-
ardized mean differences, and were classified as having ‘high’ impact on learning outcomes com-
pared to all other interventions in the meta-analysis (Snilstveit et al., 2015). Recent research from
Save the Children showed that the time out of school was where educational inequities expanded
most rapidly, and that children spent more time out of school than in school, when holidays, breaks,
and afternoons and evenings were counted (Friedlander, Guajardo, et al., 2012). Literature from
the US has demonstrated that, with respect to learning outcomes, students’ rate of cognitive gains
decreases during the summer months (Cooper et al., 1996; Heyns, 1978). The summer breaks have
consistently shown an increase in inequitable education growth, especially for students of low
socioeconomic status (SES) (Alexander et al., 2001; Cooper et al., 1996), minorities (Kim, 2004)
and low-SES minorities (O’Brien, 1999), and potentially for boys (Bakle, 2010; Guryan et al.,
2014). As a result, summer loss is a well-established part of the educational literature, with nuanced
understandings of which groups are most adversely affected by summer loss (Cooper et al., 1996).
In order to reduce this learning loss, several recent interventions have attempted to support the
learning of students in the summers (Guryan et al., 2014). This research has increased attention to
cost (McCombs et al., 2014) and to scalability in low-income settings in the US (Kim et al., 2016).
There is little evidence from sub-Saharan Africa regarding ‘summer’ or grade-transition learn-
ing loss. We are unclear as to whether such loss occurs, or whether the sub-Saharan African context
is different from the US literature in ways that make grade-transition loss either not applicable or
negligible in effect. Comparisons are complicated by the fact that countries structure their aca-
demic years differently, with some using terms and others semesters. We have limited literature on
whether grade-transition loss in sub-Saharan Africa would be sensitive to intervention, as it is in
the US, although Save the Children’s work on life-wide learning reported encouraging evidence of
how community activities could support out-of-school learning in general, albeit not yet specific to
grade-transition loss (Friedlander, Dowd, et al., 2012).
In order to examine grade-transition loss in a sub-Saharan African setting, the authors of this
paper utilized the unique Malawi Learner Assessment Tool (LAT) data set. More than 10,000 LAT
assessments, in the Chichewa language, were undertaken across several literacy tasks at 11 data
points over 3 years. The four rounds of data collected before and after the long break between
academic years allowed us to estimate rates of summer loss in Malawi.
Background
Summer loss in the US
Summer learning loss in the US is a measurable problem that has been well documented in aca-
demic literature, and studies examining seasonal learning rates date back over a century (White,
1906). The US summer holiday, with its roots in the needs of an agrarian society to have a long
harvest season, can last up to 12 weeks and represents a substantial interruption to the rhythm of
instruction (Kerry and Davies, 1998). In their meta-analysis of 13 studies on summer loss, Cooper
et al. (1996) found that at best, students achieved no cognitive gains during the summer and on
average, their test scores in reading (particularly spelling) and mathematics declined over the sum-
mer, amounting to nearly one month’s worth of loss on a grade-level scale.
464 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Murnane (1975) suggested that some skills are more likely to deteriorate over the summer
because they are based on conceptual or factual knowledge which requires a significant amount of
practice, as opposed to conceptual knowledge that requires experience rather than practice (Cooper
et al., 1996). Thus, the skills that are most likely to decay during the summer depend on ‘differ-
ences in opportunities to practice and the susceptibility of certain knowledge to be dissipated with-
out practice’ (Cooper et al., 1996: 261). According to the self-teaching hypothesis, procedural
skills such as letter-sound fluency, syllable fluency, fluency in decoding syllables, familiar words
and oral reading fluency of connected text require practice and, specifically, successful practice, to
deepen (Share and Stanovich, 1995).
Summer loss is not evenly distributed across the population and disproportionately affects low-
income students (Allington et al., 2010). Cooper et al. (1996) estimated the annual reading achieve-
ment gap between students from low- and middle-income families to be around three months. This
substantial difference could occur because opportunities to practise reading skills over the summer
are closely linked to students’ socioeconomic background (Lareau, 2011). However, a significant gap
remained between low- and high-SES students when researchers controlled for summer activities,
estimated at 0.15 SD (Gershenson, 2013). Summer learning loss is also cumulative; the compounding
losses between the primary grades were estimated to account for more than half of the achievement
gap between high- and low-SES students in the ninth grade (Alexander et al., 2007).
There is also some evidence that summer break sets back minority students more than their
white peers (Phillips and Chin, 2004). In fact, middle-class students were found to demonstrate
modest gains in reading proficiency during the summer (Cooper et al., 2003). Therefore, a summer
holiday can both cause learning losses generally and exacerbate inequities between students,
mainly along socioeconomic and racial lines (Heyns, 1978).
Differences in summer loss by gender
While the evidence is clear that SES attenuates the magnitude and nature of summer loss, evidence
on the role of gender in summer reading loss is mixed. Cooper et al. (1996) found that gender did
not have a significant moderating effect on cognitive loss during the summer. Other studies, how-
ever, have suggested otherwise. Downey et al. (2004) found that an existing gender gap favouring
girls in literacy rates grew faster when school was not in session. And among low-SES students
with unusually high summer cognitive gains (comparable to their high-SES counterparts), girls had
significantly better outcomes than boys (Slates et al., 2012).
Evidence from the US also suggests that girls may respond more favourably to summer literacy
interventions. In one randomized controlled trial of a home-based reading intervention in which
students received both books that matched their skill and interests and corresponding comprehen-
sion lessons, only third-grade girls showed significant treatment effects (Cohen’s d = 0.073), while
boys had insignificant treatment effects (Cohen’s d = −0.024) (Guryan et al., 2014). Another study
examining the impact of a summer-school program on language usage found no differences by
gender in grade 2 or 3; however, gender had a significant main effect at fifth grade. For fourth-
grade students, a significant interaction effect was identified between attending summer school and
gender among fourth-grade students, where girls outperformed boys (Bakle, 2010).
Some studies were not designed to differentiate between whether the gender gap that existed
during the school year simply carried into the summer months, or whether the magnitude of sum-
mer loss differed by gender (Slates et al., 2012). Basic gender gaps exist in many contexts, with
girls outperforming boys in reading in the US (Willingham and Cole, 1997) and in sub-Saharan
Africa (Gove and Cvelich, 2011). Possible explanations for the gender-differentiated summer loss
results include girls’ more favourable attitudes towards recreational reading (Logan and Johnston,
2009), a higher tendency among girls to take home and read more books in the summer (Heyns,
Slade et al. 465
1978) and evidence that their parents were more likely to take them to the library, although parents
were found to spend more time reading to sons (Willingham and Cole, 1997).
Interventions that address summer loss
Several types of interventions have been devised to ameliorate summer loss. These include extend-
ing the school year or modifying the calendar (Cooper et al., 1996; Von Hippel, 2016). Card and
Krueger (1992) found that when they controlled for other factors, such as teacher wages, term
length had no effect on educational attainment. This finding is relevant for sub-Saharan Africa,
since many countries – such as Malawi – utilize two shorter term breaks and one longer grade-
transition break. Cooper and colleagues (2003) conducted a meta-analysis on 39 US school dis-
tricts with modified calendars, where breaks were shorter than the standard summer vacation and
equally sized, and the number of instructional days did not exceed approximately 180. The analysis
revealed that schools with revised calendars had a small impact on student achievement, with
stronger results among schools serving low-income students. The case for modified calendars was
lessened, however, by the limited and contextual evidence, the high costs and the complex logisti-
cal planning (Cooper et al., 2003).
Other policy alternatives to mitigate summer loss generally fall into two categories: classroom-
based and home-based interventions (Kim and Quinn, 2013). While the evidence suggests compa-
rable impacts on learning outcomes in these interventions, there can be considerable differences in
the costs (Kim and Quinn, 2013; McCombs et al., 2011). Classroom interventions involve instruc-
tional activities such as summer school, carried out by schoolteachers and university researchers,
for example, and remedial or preparatory teacher-led efforts to increase student achievement.
Positive impacts have been recorded for these types of interventions (Cooper et al., 2000), esti-
mated at 0.09 SD (Kim and Quinn, 2013).
Home-based interventions are designed to respond to the literature showing that reading during
the summer months is highly correlated to academic achievement (Chin and Phillips, 2004; Heyns,
1978). Positive impacts have also been recorded for interventions such as increasing access to
(self-selected) high-quality books (Allington et al., 2010) and voluntary summer book reading
programs with scaffolding (Kim and White, 2008), with effect sizes of 0.14 and 0.09 SD respec-
tively. In their meta-analysis, Kim and Quinn (2013) estimated the average effect size of the home-
based interventions at 0.12 SD. Costs for these programs are much lower than for classroom
interventions, such that they have been put forward as scalable and cost-effective policy solutions
to mitigate summer loss (McCombs et al., 2011).
Although many studies have invested in understanding whether literacy outcomes are sensitive
to intervention, we could identify no evidence to help determine whether summer loss was affected
by generic literacy interventions that had no specific components addressing summer loss. It might
be that summer loss in literacy is a significant problem, but that interventions designed to improve
literacy skills during the school year could reduce or remove that summer loss.
‘Summer’ loss in the global South
Research on ‘summer’ (‘grade-transition’ in low-latitude contexts) loss outside of the US and
Canada is scant (Lindahl, 2001), and to our knowledge, there is no literature examining whether
grade-transition loss exists in sub-Saharan Africa. Save the Children has increased its research into
out-of-school factors and their relationship with learning outcomes in sub-Saharan Africa
(Friedlander, Dowd, et al., 2012). Weiseman and Baker (2004) argued that the summer learning
loss phenomenon might be unique to the US because in comparison to other industrial nations, the
US has longer formal breaks (approximately 12 weeks) and shorter overall instructional time.
466 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
However, other studies have shown that summer loss exists in Canada (Davies et al., 2016),
Sweden (Lindahl, 2001), the Netherlands (Verachtert et al., 2009) and Germany (Meyer, 2014). In
their study of summer learning loss in Canada, Davies et al. (2016) concluded that the phenomenon
will be present in any setting where there is socioeconomic stratification.
Context
Malawi is a small, landlocked Anglophone country in southern Africa, with Chichewa as the
national language. Its population density and average class size are among the highest in the world,
with 182 people per square kilometre and nearly 90 students per classroom (UIS, 2015; World
Bank, 2015). It is among the world’s least developed nations with roughly 80% of its 17 million
people living in rural areas (UNDP, 2015). Learning outcomes are low; Malawi’s learning out-
comes rank at the 17th percentile in comparison to other low- and middle-income countries
(Education Policy and Data Center (EPDC), 2014). Recent nationally representative literacy
assessments have shown that 92% of grade 1 and 47% of grade 3 students were unable to read a
single word in English (Nagarajan et al., 2015).
Malawi’s primary education is free but not compulsory, and there are approximately 4 million
students at the primary level. At the end of primary school, students sit for the Primary School Leaving
Certificate Examination (PSLCE) (EPDC, 2014; Kananji, 2014). The school year runs from
September to July, and the country has a trimester system with terms running from September to
December, January to March/April, and April to July. Breaks between terms vary from three to seven
weeks (EPDC, 2014); the 2014 and 2015 grade-transition breaks lasted approximately seven weeks.
The Malawi Early Grade Reading Activity was a USAID-funded intervention designed to sup-
port the Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (MoEST) to improve reading outcomes in
Malawian public primary schools. It was implemented from 2013 to 2016 in 1600 schools across
11 educational districts. The intervention included teacher training, instructional coaching by
MoEST primary education advisers, and provision of scripted lesson plans and learner books. It
was rolled out in a phased approach, creating treatment and control groups. The Activity was
implemented in both English and Chichewa, although this article focuses on the Chichewa results.
The program’s research design is depicted in Table 1. Teachers and students received Chichewa-
specific training, instructional materials and coaching in grade 1 (‘Standard 1’ in Malawi) in the
2013–2014 school year, grade 2 in the 2014–2015 school year, and grade 3 in the 2015–2016
school year.
The intervention’s monitoring and evaluation strategy included periodic collection of student
performance data using the LAT (see Table 2). LAT data were collected in October (near the begin-
ning of the school year), January (the beginning of the second term), March (the end of the second
term), and July (at the end of the school year) for each year of implementation.1 The assessments
were administered to different students for Chichewa and English in grades 1–3 for the first five
Table 1. Implementation design for the Early Grade Reading Activity, by grade.
Grade Sep 2013–
Jul 2014
Sep 2014–
Jul 2015
Sep 2015–
Jul 2016
1
2
3
Slade et al. 467
LAT rounds (until January 2015). Beginning in the sixth LAT round (March 2015), the selected
students were assessed in both Chichewa and English.
The LAT instrument had items measuring skills in letter naming, phonemic awareness, syllable
decoding, familiar word reading, oral reading fluency of connected text and reading comprehen-
sion, which we describe below.
The same LAT form was used for each assessment within a school year. Between the first and
second school years, the grid items (i.e. letters, syllables or familiar words that were presented in
rows and columns on a single sheet) were re-randomized. Between each school year, the reading pas-
sages and associated comprehension questions were modified. An equating study was conducted to
assess the comparability of the reading passages, and a mean equating factor was applied so that oral
reading fluency scores would be comparable (Kolen and Brennan, 2004).
Research questions
The literature reviewed above shows that summer loss in literacy exists in several Northern con-
texts. Summer loss is particularly large for the poor and for ethnic minorities, but the evidence
remains mixed as to whether summer loss differs by gender, and whether summer loss is a phenom-
enon experienced only in the absence of instructional interventions. Our review of the research
suggests that while this body of research is growing in the North, with more written on the US than
elsewhere, it is a gap in the literature with respect to sub-Saharan Africa in general, and Malawi in
particular. The increased emphasis on literacy outcomes in sub-Saharan Africa seems to not have
resulted in increased emphasis on grade-transition loss. Therefore, in this study, we address the
following research questions:
RQ1: Did students have lower literacy outcomes after the grade-transition break than before?
RQ2: Did the magnitude of grade-transition loss differ by gender?
RQ3: Did the magnitude of grade-transition loss differ by assignment to a generic literacy treat-
ment group?
Table 2. Students assessed in Chichewa for LAT 1 through LAT 11, by grade and gender.
LAT round Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 3 Total
Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls
LAT 1 – Jan 14 155 157 145 157 143 130 887
LAT 2 – Mar 14 168 163 150 163 160 160 964
LAT 3 – Jul 14 166 157 146 164 141 153 927
LAT 4 – Oct 14 170 169 166 171 165 165 1006
LAT 5 – Jan 15 170 171 151 159 164 180 995
LAT 6 – Mar 15 159 157 164 164 156 157 957
LAT 7 – Jul 15 169 165 165 165 167 161 992
LAT 8 – Oct 15 165 185 157 167 185 171 1030
LAT 9 – Jan 16 165 156 164 166 161 168 980
LAT 10 – Mar 16 163 159 163 165 162 162 974
LAT 11 – Jul 16 166 166 165 165 165 165 992
Total 1816 1805 1736 1806 1769 1772 10,704
468 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Methodology
Measures
The LAT measured a set of items that we describe below. The fluency scores were a function of
both accuracy and automaticity (speed), as described.
Letter naming: A timed task in which the learner identified the names of letters presented in
a grid consisting of 10 rows and 10 columns (10 × 10). Fluency scores were reported in cor-
rect letters per minute (clpm); there was no theoretical maximum.
Syllable reading: A timed task in which the learner read aloud the sounds represented by
syllables presented in a 10 × 10 grid. Fluency scores were reported in correct syllable sounds
per minute (csspm); there was no theoretical maximum.
Familiar word reading: A timed task in which the learner read aloud familiar words from a
5 × 10 grid. Fluency scores were reported in correct words per minute (cwpm); there was no
theoretical maximum.
Oral reading fluency: A timed task in which the learner read aloud a simple passage of
approximately 50–70 words in length. Fluency scores were reported in cwpm (orf).2 There
was no theoretical maximum.
Sampling
The Malawi LAT sampling frame used multi-stage random sampling. Three stages of sampling
were executed at the zonal, school and grade levels. At the first stage, three intervention zones
were selected at random from within districts. (There are between 9 and 15 zones per district.)
One school was then selected at random from within each sampled zone. At each school, stu-
dents were stratified by gender within grade, and equal numbers of girls and boys were ran-
domly selected from each grade. For LAT rounds 1–5 (in which students were assessed
separately for Chichewa and English), the target sample size was 1980 students (20 per grade
per school); for LAT rounds 6–11, the target sample size was 990 students (10 per grade per
school). The LAT sample of schools was maintained for one full school year, with a fresh sam-
ple drawn for the new school year. The sampling of LAT schools was done in early October (at
the beginning of each school year).
Analysis
This section presents the design of the LAT study. Different students were sampled at each LAT
round; thus, while individual students may have repeated a grade, at an aggregate level, cohorts of
students progressed from one grade to another across years.
In the Malawi LAT data, students were grouped into cohorts. Cohort 1 students were in grade 1
during the 2013–2014 school year, grade 2 in 2014–2015, and grade 3 in 2015–2016. Cohort 2
students followed one year behind, and were in grade 1 during the 2014–2015 school year and
grade 2 in 2015–2016. Cohort 3 students started in grade 2 during the 2013–2014 school year,
moving to grade 3 in 2014–2015. Cohort 1 students faced two grade transitions, while Cohorts 2
and 3 each faced one grade transition.
We were interested in knowing whether grade-transition loss would differ by whether the school
was included in an intervention, such as the Early Grade Reading Activity. We compared grade 2
and 3 Cohort 1 and 2 children with grade 2 and 3 Cohort 3 children, since teachers and learners in
Cohort 3 were not exposed to the intervention.3
Slade et al. 469
We estimated grade-transition loss in the following way. The delta (Δ) for the grade-transition
loss estimate is simply the difference between the sample means pre- and post-grade-transition.
∆=
XX
pos
tp
re
In order to compare the magnitude of grade-transition loss, we calculated the Cohen’s d effect
size (Cohen, 1988). This was estimated by dividing the absolute mean difference by the pooled SD
of the two estimates, as
abs
S
()
where S is the pooled SD.
To determine whether there were differences in summer loss by gender, we fit a differences-in-
differences (DiD) model comparing the fluency rates pre- and post-grade-transition for boys and
girls, as per the following equation:
XX
XX
boys post boyspre girls postgirls pre,,
,,
()
−−
()
The gender difference is expressed in the DiD mean, and we present the Cohen’s d effect size
(Cohen, 1988) in the Findings section below.
Findings
RQ1: Did students have lower literacy outcomes after the grade-transition break
than before?
The first research question reflects our interest in determining whether there were differences in learn-
ing outcomes for literacy before and after Malawi’s long break between grades. The three cohorts of
children assessed in LAT faced a grade-transition break four times. There were two transitions between
grade 1 and 2, in both 2014 and 2015; and two transitions between grade 2 and 3, in both 2014 and
2015. Table 3 presents our findings. The table shows the mean scores before and after the four transi-
tions for the four measures (letter sound fluency, syllable fluency, familiar word fluency and oral read-
ing fluency in connected text). The table also shows the grade-transition loss, the results of the statistical
test determining whether the loss was statistically significant and the Cohen’s d effect size of the loss.
Our results show statistically significant grade-transition loss for all four measures at each of the
four transitions. For example, children transitioning from grade 2 to 3 in 2015 lost 12.7 letters per
minute (p-value < .001), 10.2 syllables per minute (p-value < .001), 5.7 familiar words per minute
(p-value < .001), and 5.4 words per minute on connected text (p-value < .001) in the 2015 grade
transition. The absolute magnitude of the grade-transition loss was somewhat smaller for the grade
1 to 2 transition than for the grade 2 to 3 transition in many of the analyses; however, the relative
loss was greater for the grade 1 to 2 transition than for the grade 2 to 3 transition. In any case, the
results show statistically significant evidence for grade-transition loss in all 16 cases – that is, for
both the grade 1 and 2 transition, in both 2014 and 2015, and for letter naming fluency, syllable
fluency, familiar word fluency and oral reading fluency in connected text.
The magnitude of the grade-transition loss is presented in Table 3’s effect size column. Loss
effect sizes are substantial in some of the comparisons. The results showed an average effect size
of 0.58 SD for letter naming fluency, 0.35 SD for syllable fluency, 0.32 SD for familiar word flu-
ency and 0.29 SD for oral reading fluency. Our results showed an average effect size of 0.38 SD
for both the grade 1 to 2 and the grade 2 to 3 transition periods.
470 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
To better explain the nature of grade-transition loss, we present Figure 1. Figure 1 shows the
mean rates of oral reading fluency of connected text for each of the three cohorts of children in
each of the four LAT rounds per academic year (October, January, March and July). While the dif-
ferences in mean oral reading fluency varied between LAT rounds, Figure 1 shows that all of the
other, non-grade-transition breaks corresponded with positive changes in performance. Only at the
grade-transition break, between July and October, was there a decline in learning outcomes. The
figure also presents key information to understand grade-transition loss: the cwpm in July (before
the break) and October (after the break), when the transition between the grades occurred. Asterisks
show when each of these four comparisons was statistically significant. The figure also shows that
modest but positive growth occurred for each cohort between each LAT round, with only the grade-
transition comparison presenting as negative. Note that 27 of the 28 LAT round comparisons were
statistically significant and positive, and only one comparison was not statistically significant.4
RQ2: Did the magnitude of grade-transition loss differ by gender?
Given that the literature on summer loss in North American and European contexts was not in
agreement on whether summer loss results differed by gender, we present the results of our DiD
models that determined whether the declines between the July and October time periods were
larger for boys or girls.
Table 4 shows the magnitude of grade-transition loss for both boys and girls and the DiD com-
parison, with associated p-values and effect sizes. The table presents the same 16 comparisons as
discussed above, with four comparisons before and after the grade-transition break for each of the
four measures (letter naming fluency, syllable fluency, familiar word fluency and oral reading flu-
ency on connected text). None of the 16 comparisons showed statistically significant differences
between boys and girls. In fact, the effect size of the comparisons was small, with a statistically
insignificant 0.17 SD identified for a 2.3 letters per minute difference (p-value .29), and 0.12 SD
for a difference of 1.9 syllables per minute (p-value .46). All of the comparisons for familiar word
Table 3. Grade-transition loss estimates by LAT task. Standard errors in parentheses.
Grade
transition
Year Before
break
After
break
Grade-
transition loss
T score
(p-value)
Effect
size
Average
effect size
Letter fluency 1 to 2 2015 23.5 12.3 11.2 (1.35) −8.32*** 0.65
Letter fluency 1 to 2 2014 15.4 6.3 9.1 (1.09) −8.34*** 0.66
Letter fluency 2 to 3 2015 37.6 24.9 12.7 (1.83) −6.92*** 0.54
Letter fluency 2 to 3 2014 21.2 12.5 8.7 (1.54) −5.66*** 0.45 0.58
Syllable fluency 1 to 2 2015 13.8 8.7 5.1 (1.31) −3.89*** 0.31
Syllable fluency 1 to 2 2014 6.5 3.1 3.4 (0.72) −4.80*** 0.38
Syllable fluency 2 to 3 2015 31.6 21.4 10.2 (1.94) −5.24*** 0.41
Syllable fluency 2 to 3 2014 13.9 8.9 5.0 (1.42) −3.56*** 0.28 0.35
Familiar word fluency 1 to 2 2015 5.5 3.8 1.73 (0.7) −2.46* 0.19
Familiar word fluency 1 to 2 2014 3.9 1.4 2.4 (0.56) −4.31*** 0.34
Familiar word fluency 2 to 3 2015 16.9 11.2 5.7 (1.18) −4.80*** 0.37
Familiar word fluency 2 to 3 2014 9.7 5.0 4.7 (0.98) −4.79*** 0.38 0.32
Oral reading fluency 1 to 2 2015 5.0 3.4 1.6 (0.68) −2.39* 0.19
Oral reading fluency 1 to 2 2014 4.1 1.2 2.9 (0.63) −4.57*** 0.36
Oral reading fluency 2 to 3 2015 17.6 12.2 5.4 (1.3) −4.18*** 0.33
Oral reading fluency 2 to 3 2014 9.6 5.6 4.0 (1.09) −3.70*** 0.29 0.29
Slade et al. 471
and oral reading fluency were both statistically and substantively insignificant. This suggests no
difference by gender in the magnitude of grade-transition loss.
RQ3: Did the magnitude of grade-transition loss differ by assignment to a generic
literacy treatment group?
While the North American and European literature was largely silent as to whether summer loss
would differ for children who were involved in an instructional improvement program similar to
the ones implemented in Malawi and in several other sub-Saharan African countries, we fit models
that determined whether there was a difference in grade-transition loss for children in a literacy
intervention treatment program, implemented during the school year. Our DiD model compared
the loss for children whose teachers were receiving the Early Grade Reading Activity treatment
with another group of children whose teachers were not receiving the treatment. Our results showed
no differences (p-value .98). In other words, whether one’s teachers were in a research-based
instructional improvement program that provided books, training and support had no relationship
with the magnitude of grade-transition loss.
Discussion
Our first research question addressed whether a grade-transition loss phenomenon analogous to
North American ‘summer loss’ applied to Malawi. We found grade-transition loss in Malawi to be
substantial, with an average effect size of 0.38 SD. The rich LAT data set allowed us to determine
whether this identified loss was specific to the gap between grades, or was within the range of vari-
ation between two different LAT rounds. We found that all of the other (non-grade-transition)
comparisons between LAT rounds showed either statistically significant improvements (27
Figure 1. Chichewa oral reading fluency, by student cohort and assessment time.
472 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Table 4. Grade-transition loss DiD estimates by gender. Standard errors in parentheses.
LAT task Grade
transition
Year Boys’ grade-
transition loss
Girls’ grade-
transition loss
DiD p-value T score DiD effect
size
Letter fluency 1 to 2 2015 10.9 (1.9) 11.6 (1.9) 0.7 (2.7) 0.81 −0.24 0.04
Letter fluency 1 to 2 2014 10.3 (1.6) 7.9 (1.5) 2.3 (2.2) 0.29 1.07 0.17
Letter fluency 2 to 3 2015 12.4 (2.5) 13.0 (2.6) 0.6 (3.7) 0.87 −0.16 0.02
Letter fluency 2 to 3 2014 9.3 (2.1) 8.0 (2.2) 1.3 (3.1) 0.67 0.43 0.07
Syllable fluency 1 to 2 2015 6.1 (1.7) 4.2 (2.0) 1.9 (2.0) 0.46 0.74 0.12
Syllable fluency 1 to 2 2014 3.6 (1.1) 3.2 (0.9) 0.4 (1.4) 0.78 0.28 0.04
Syllable fluency 2 to 3 2015 10.3 (2.6) 10.0 (2.8) 0.4 (3.9) 0.92 0.10 0.01
Syllable fluency 2 to 3 2014 5.1 (2.0) 4.9 (2.0) 0.2 (2.8) 0.93 0.09 0.01
Familiar word fluency 1 to 2 2015 2.0 (0.9) 1.5 (1.1) 0.5 (1.4) 0.74 0.33 0.05
Familiar word fluency 1 to 2 2014 2.6 (0.9) 2.3 (0.7) 0.3 (1.1) 0.81 0.24 0.04
Familiar word fluency 2 to 3 2015 5.8 (1.6) 5.6 (1.7) 0.3 (2.4) 0.92 0.11 0.02
Familiar word fluency 2 to 3 2014 5.3 (1.5) 4.1 (1.3) 1.2 (1.9) 0.54 0.62 0.10
Oral reading fluency 1 to 2 2015 1.4 (0.9) 1.9 (1.0) 0.5 (1.4) 0.71 −0.38 0.04
Oral reading fluency 1 to 2 2014 3.2 (1.0) 2.5 (0.8) 0.7 (1.3) 0.57 0.57 0.09
Oral reading fluency 2 to 3 2015 5.8 (1.8) 5.1 (1.9) 0.7 (2.6) 0.79 0.27 0.04
Oral reading fluency 2 to 3 2014 4.1 (1.7) 3.9 (1.4) 0.2 (2.2) 0.93 0.03 0.01
Slade et al. 473
transitions) or non-significant changes (one transition). The only negative comparisons were the
four grade-transition comparisons that we presented in Figure 1 above.
Further, the magnitude of grade-transition loss in Malawi appears to have been substantial.
Several countries in sub-Saharan Africa have undertaken literacy improvement programs, which
have had varied effects. A recent article from New Directions for Child and Adolescent
Development presented the effect sizes of programs in Egypt, Guatemala, Jordan, Kenya, Liberia,
Mozambique, Philippines, Rwanda and Senegal (Moore et al., 2017). The effect sizes for oral
reading fluency ranged from 0.24 to 0.80. The average effect size identified in our analysis was a
loss equivalent to 0.38 SD for both the grade 1 to 2 and the grade 2 to 3 transitions. It appears that
grade-transition loss – a relatively ignored phenomenon in the recent increased investments in
literacy – has as substantial a relationship to learning outcomes as do well-designed, well-imple-
mented, well-funded and targeted interventions in literacy.
Our second research question asked whether the magnitude of grade-transition loss differed by
gender. Studies have shown substantial gender differences in literacy outcomes, typically benefiting
girls (Gove and Cvelich, 2011; Willingham and Cole, 1997). Recent investments have focused on
girls’ education in general and literacy for girls specifically, such as the UK Department for International
Development’s (DFID’s) Girls’ Education Challenge and USAID’s Let Girls Learn. Our analysis used
a DiD estimate to determine whether there was an interaction between gender and grade-transition
loss. We found that none of the 16 comparisons for grade-transition loss differed by gender. This
means that girls’ results dropped by as much as boys’ did. This finding might seem somewhat surpris-
ing given previous research showing assigned tasks and duties differing by gender (Immajati, 2016;
Jha and Pouezevara, 2016), and in ways that one could hypothesize would allow boys more time to
practise the educational skills that otherwise would atrophy during the summer. However, features of
the Malawian economic, cultural and social context may explain that finding, as indicated next.
The majority of Malawians live in rural areas and engage in subsistence farming (World Bank,
2010). As a result, typical Malawian families rely heavily upon their children’s labour for assis-
tance with farming and for remuneration through ganyu (casual wage labour) arrangements.
Younger children are more likely to be allocated unpaid work in the household, freeing up adults
and older children to pursue income-generating activities outside the home (Colclough et al., 2000;
Sunny et al., 2017; Westberg, 2010).
Discussions of how child labour supports Malawian households often draw connections to edu-
cational outcomes. For instance, Taniguchi (2015) found that the greater the burden of household
chores, the greater the likelihood of grade repetition or dropout; however, this was significant for
children in grade 7, but not in grade 5. Furthermore, it is not necessarily clear whether the relation-
ship between household labour and schooling holds for children in grades 1–3. Among school
heads queried for Malawi’s 2007 education management information system, 44% asserted that
‘family responsibilities’ were a main reason for boys dropping out of school, with 41% reporting it
as a main reason for girls dropping out. However, when the pupils themselves were queried, only
1.3% to 1.8% of grade 1–3 respondents gave ‘had to work or help at home’ as a main reason for
dropping out of school (World Bank, 2010).
Researchers have found gendered elements as to how children spend their out-of-school time.
They established that girls were assigned the majority of household chores, whether one or both
parents had died or under normal circumstances (Chimombo et al., 2000; Kadzamira and Rose,
2003; Munthali, 2002). Boys, on the other hand, were more likely to engage in ganyu (Kadzamira
and Rose, 2003). In either case, a clear finding was that Malawian children were committing a very
significant portion of their waking hours to supporting their households, and as such could be
expected to have limited opportunities to engage in out-of-school academic activities. As Kadzamira
and Rose (2003: 507–508) asserted:
474 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Evidence also suggests that, although children in school spend approximately two to three hours per day
working for the household, on average, children out of school spend an additional four and a half hours per
day than a child in school working either for the household or in income-generating activities in both rural
and urban areas. This suggests that children out of school provide an important contribution to the
household which poorest households are unable to sacrifice even in the context of FPE [free primary
education]. Furthermore, girls, both in and out of school, spend approximately one hour per day more than
boys working for the household. (Rose, 2002)
We did not have data to examine whether children had particular resilience strategies that they could
employ to reduce the grade-transition loss that one might expect given the extra household duties
assigned to girls (Kadzamira and Rose, 2003) or the extra income-generating activities assigned to boys.
Further research would also be necessary to determine whether the daily extra hour of household-sup-
porting labour provided by girls results in gender-based differences in how much out-of-school time
children devote to academic pursuits. In any case, given the magnitude of the general grade-transition
loss, effective girl-focused educational interventions should not ignore the phenomenon.
Contextual data collected alongside a nationally representative National Reading Assessment
(NRA) commissioned by USAID and the MoEST in June–July 2014 showed that a higher percent-
age of boys took books home with them, although the overall percentage was low (Nagarajan et al.,
2015).5 This finding matters because regression results from those same data showed that taking
books home was associated with higher literacy outcomes, except for grade 1 boys (Nagarajan
et al., 2015). However, the NRA was intended to give ‘an overview of education across Malawi
rather than just of education in the districts affected by the USAID interventions’ (Nagarajan et al.,
2015: vii). In other words, all the assessments described in this article (LATs and NRA) took place
at school, at times when school was in session; and only a portion of them were administered
within the context of a literacy intervention. Thus, the causal link between learning outcomes and
books available at home might differ in the school year and in the break between grades.
Our final research question examined whether literacy interventions reduced grade-transition
loss. The Early Grade Reading Activity intervention that we evaluated focused on improving the
literacy environment, with additional literacy materials given to pupils, training on improved lit-
eracy methods provided to teachers, and support given to teachers on utilizing those methods and
materials. The Activity intervention did not have a grade-transition loss-reduction component, but
we hypothesized that the increased interest in literacy and more frequent access to materials within
the program might either create greater resistance to grade-transition loss, or change behaviours in
the break between grades to allow higher access to materials and training. Data from a community
mobilization study conducted internally in 2015 for the Activity found that 83% of parents sur-
veyed while school was in session said that their children brought books home in the previous
week; however, more work may need to be done to increase children’s access to materials outside
of normal academic interventions.
Our results show that the magnitude of grade-transition loss was the same regardless of whether
a child’s teacher was receiving a literacy intervention. If one assumes that the Malawi Early Grade
Reading Activity intervention was somewhat similar to other large-scale interventions currently in
place across sub-Saharan Africa (Moore et al., 2017), it begs the question whether children enjoy-
ing these programs would similarly have no resiliency against loss. More research is necessary to
understand this, but at least in Malawi we note that as designed, this literacy intervention was
unable to reduce grade-transition loss.
Given that the literacy intervention in Malawi had no impact on grade-transition loss, whereas
several US-based interventions specifically targeting summer loss have been effective, it might be
possible to conceive a grade-transition loss-reduction intervention for Malawi. More research is
Slade et al. 475
necessary to determine whether the design of the grade-transition loss-reduction interventions for a
Malawi-type context would be similar to or different from the US-based ones, and whether school-
based or community-based grade-transition loss-reduction interventions would be more effective.
Limitations
This section presents the threats to validity for our analyses, and our attempts to address them. One
possible threat to validity relates to the potential for students to be assessed more than once, given
the random sampling at the school level during each LAT round. This means it is possible that
individual students were assessed more than once, and that a learning effect could have occurred.
A student tested during the LAT data collection immediately before the break between grades (July
2014 or July 2015) who had been tested at least once before might exhibit stronger performance
because of increased familiarity with the assessment, particularly because the same assessment
was used for every round within an academic year. If many children were assessed repeatedly, and
a learning effect occurred, this in turn would bias estimates of the magnitude of learning loss
between that round and the first round of the subsequent academic year.
To address this threat to validity, we used a binomial probability analysis that considered the
schools’ enrolment by grade and gender, and the number of students sampled each round, to estimate
the likelihood that a student assessed during the July round was seeing the test for the first time. For
the 2013–2014 school year, the average probability across schools was 88%; for the 2014–2015 school
year, the average probability across schools was 75%.6 Put another way, less than an eighth of the
assessed students in 2014, or less than a quarter in 2015, were likely to have been assessed in previous
rounds. Given that literacy assessment results from the same children were higher by 20% at the sec-
ond round of data collection, and operating under the assumption that approximately 25% of children
were assessed more than once, our estimates might have overestimated the size of summer loss by up
to 1 cwpm (for an analysis of similar repeated test-takers in Nepal, see RTI International, 2016b).
A second limitation relates to the timing of assessment. Our research questions focused on esti-
mating the magnitude of grade-transition loss; ideally, data collection would have been done at the
very end of the instructional time in July and at the start of the academic year in October. The July
data collections occurred within a couple of weeks of the end of the term, and in Malawi, much of
that time is spent on examinations, so little learning occurs after the assessment. However, there is
a larger time difference at the beginning of the academic year. The school years began on September
8 in 2014 and September 7 in 2015, while LAT data collection began on October 6 in 2014 and
October 27 in 2015. Given that data collection extended over two weeks in 2014 and one week in
2015, this means that students were assessed between four and eight weeks after the term began.
This suggests that the magnitude of the grade-transition loss might be somewhat underestimated,
since students would have learned (or relearned) material in that first month or two of school.
A third limitation relates to grade repetition, where children fail to advance to the next grade. A
substantial number of children in grades 1–3 in Malawi repeat grades. This inefficiency is well
attested in the Malawian education literature (Hewett et al., 2009; Khasu et al., 2014; Sabates
et al., 2010). Our analyses were unable to account for the influence of this repetition on the magni-
tude of grade-transition loss, since we did not follow an actual cohort of children from grade to
grade. We are also unable to know whether being unable to identify repeaters biased the results
negatively or positively, although each sample was likely to have included some pupils who had
previously repeated.
Finally, a fourth threat to validity relates to whether the inclusion or exclusion of non-readers
(children who read zero words per minute on the oral reading fluency task) influenced grade-
transition loss. Because the July (pre-break) and October (post-break) assessments were
476 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
administered to different children, the ‘loss’ cannot be assumed to have been driven entirely by a
decrease in mean scores. An alternative explanation might be that the loss was due to imbalances
in the proportion of readers and non-readers. The section below considers some possible implica-
tions of this threat.
Table 5 presents one approach to understanding how grade-transition loss might have been
affected by the proportion of non-readers. The entire sample is presented first. This shows that, for
all four grade-transition comparisons, there were more non-readers in the post-break assessment
than in the pre-break assessment. These differences ranged from 8 percentage points more in the
grade 2 to 3 transition for Cohort 1 in the 2015 break (30% to 38%) to 31 percentage points more
in the grade 2 to 3 transition for Cohort 3 in the 2014 break (55% to 86%).
To illustrate how this significant number of non-readers might have affected the magnitude of
grade-transition loss, the ‘readers only’ columns of Table 5 present the fluency rates for the sub-
population of the July and October samples that read at least one word correctly (i.e., had an
orf >= 1). The ‘readers only’ calculations indicate that the number of non-zero readers was lower
in the post-break assessment for all four comparisons. It also shows that fluency rates for those
non-zero readers were lower after the break for three of the four comparisons. (The fourth compari-
son group—the Cohort 2 children transitioning from grade 1 to 2—recorded an orf 2.9 correct
words per minute higher in the post-break period than the pre-break period.)
The problem with an approach that ignores children who recorded a fluency rate of zero is that
it ignores the possibility that children who were reading before the break had forgotten how to read
after the break. The final columns in Table 5 (‘hypothetical sample’) illustrate what grade-transi-
tion loss might be if the sample sizes before and after each break were the same, assigning an orf
of 0 to each hypothetical child added to balance the samples.
Figure 2 provides a graphic comparison of these calculations. First, our interpretation of these
data is that the high number of non-readers in Malawi somewhat reduces apparent grade-transition
loss, since there is nothing lower than zero. The ‘hypothetical sample’ calculations illustrate a case
where grade-transition loss could range from 6.2 cwpm (for the grade 2 to 3 transition in 2015) to
10.7 cwpm (for the grade 1 to 2 transition in 2014). The inability to detect grade-transition loss due
to non-readers indicates that the analysis of the full sample presented in this article might underes-
timate grade-transition loss by 38% or more (derived from the difference between the loss observed
in the entire sample and that stipulated in the hypothetical sample population).
Conclusion
This study shows that grade-transition loss exists in Malawi and suggests that policy makers inter-
ested in improving learning outcomes should actively address this phenomenon. In Malawi, the
grade-transition-loss effect was impervious to an intervention that did not directly target it; how-
ever, the US-based research shows that out-of-school interventions can improve learning out-
comes. Unfortunately, there is little rigorous evidence as to whether reading camps or literacy
interventions that organizations like Pratham and Save the Children do during term breaks would
be more or less effective than the targeted, home-based summer-loss interventions that have been
shown to be successful in lower-income contexts in the US.
In Malawi, interventions that take a community-focused approach to strengthening learning out-
comes have yet to produce rigorous, actionable evidence of program effects at scale. Save the
Children has been implementing various small-scale iterations of the Literacy Boost intervention
throughout southern Malawi since 2009.7 In the most recent version documented to date, the
Tiwerenge Ndi Ana Athu (TiANA) program provided community-produced reading materials, used
high school graduates as volunteers to run reading camps, and broadcast recordings developed at the
Slade et al. 477
Table 5. Grade-transition changes in fluency scores and proportion of non-readers for the entire LAT sample, for a sample restricted to readers at 1
cwpm or more, and for a hypothetical sample of readers in the pre-break assessment.
Entire sample Readers only Hypothetical sample
Jul Oct Grade-transition
change
Jul Oct Grade-transition
change
Jul Oct Grade-transition
change
Break 2014
(Grade
12)
cwpm 4.11 1.15 −2.97 15.1 8.4 −6.7 15.10 4.39 −10.71
% zeros 73% 86% 13% 0 0 0 48%
N323 337 14 88 46 88 46 + (42 zeroes)
Break 2014
(Grade
23)
Cwpm 9.62 5.26 −4.36 21.3 15.1 −6.2 21.30 12.40 −8.90
% zeros 55% 86% 31% 0 0 0 18%
N310 330 20 140 115 140 115 + (25 zeroes)
Break 2015
(Grade
12)
Cwpm 4.75 3.46 −1.29 11.2 14.1 2.9 11.20 7.81 −3.39
% zeros 57% 75% 18% 0 0 0 45%
N328 314 −14 139 77 139 77 + (62 zeroes)
Break 2015
(Grade
23)
Cwpm 16.66 12.16 −4.50 23.9 19.6 −4.3 23.90 17.71 −6.19
% zeros 30% 38% 8% 0 0 0 10%
N327 332 5 228 206 228 206 + (22 zeroes)
478 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
reading camps over a radio station serving the area. Impact was modest, however, and the endline
report did not present the program’s learning gains net of the within-year learning effects of the
school-based teaching, so it is difficult to gauge its success (Save the Children International, 2014).
One add-on element of the Malawi Early Grade Reading Activity was a small-scale study (31
schools) of a roughly six-month social and behaviour-change communication (SBCC) pilot inter-
vention in two zones of Ntcheu district. The multichannel approach used a radio program, radio
spots, listening groups, posters hung in public areas, community meetings and community theatre
performances to inform parents about how they could support their children’s reading acquisition.
While results of the evaluation were positive, they were not linked to children’s learning outcomes
(Schmidt et al., 2016).
A lamentably under-documented and underreported aspect of the pilot was a book-lending pro-
gram conducted during the grade-transition break. Head teachers made arrangements for school
staff to be available at the school to lend books to children from the schools’ Reading Tools in a
Box8 mini libraries. Participating children took the books home, read them (sometimes with their
parents) and then exchanged them for others. Unfortunately, rigorous data on the frequency and
duration of the book borrowing were not collected (Dr. Elizabeth Randolph, personal communica-
tion, 12 March 2017). In communities whose schools have been provided with large quantities of
books by donor-funded interventions, such borrowing programs would have promise and merit
further investigation.
A major limiting factor to any intervention aimed at children when school is not in session is the
economic reality of the average Malawian family. As quoted above from Kadzamira and Rose (2003),
‘children out of school provide an important contribution to the household which poorest households
are unable to sacrifice even in the context of FPE’ (p. 507). When poor rural families rely heavily
upon even young children for farm-based labour and income-generation activities – for up to seven
hours a day, when school is not in session – significant sensitization efforts would likely be needed to
convince parents that the potential long-term benefit of any educational activity would outweigh the
short-term opportunity cost in lost wages or harvest productivity (Kadzamira and Rose, 2003).
Figure 2. Grade-transition loss in the entire sample, the readers-only subsample and the hypothetical
sample.
Slade et al. 479
Large-scale literacy interventions, spurred on by increased evidence of low learning achieve-
ment, have been burgeoning across the developed world. The designs of those interventions have
focused on classroom-based, within-school interventions. The magnitude of grade-transition loss
in Malawi should give pause to those supervising, designing and implementing these large-scale
interventions, and prompt them to consider including investments to reduce grade-transition loss.
An examination of the within-year rate of reading acquisition suggests that, on average, between
one and two months of instruction may be required to regain pre-break levels of performance. To
combat these steps backward, we recommend that targeted interventions be undertaken that are
both low cost and potentially scalable, to determine whether school-based, home-based or commu-
nity-based interventions are more promising in these contexts. Policy makers should pay careful
attention to those results so that the improvements identified during the school year will not be lost
just as quickly during the break between grades.
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank the United States Agency for International Development (USAID) for funding and
direction in the implementation of the Early Grade Reading Activity, and for approving the release of these
data; Malawi’s Ministry of Education, Science, and Technology (MoEST) and the staff of the Early Grade
Reading Activity for their support in the implementation of the Activity; and RTI International (especially
Erin Newton) for analytical, writing and editorial support.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
One co-author served as the Chief of Party for the Malawi Early Grade Reading Activity and another co-
author served as the Deputy Chief of Party for the same Activity. The authors declare no potential conflicts of
interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publica-
tion of this article: These analyses are based on data generated through and collected by the Malawi Early
Grade Reading Activity, USAID Contract AID-612-C-13-00002.
Notes
1. No data were collected in October 2013 because the project had just started.
2. The abbreviation ‘orf’ (for ‘oral reading fluency’) is used to distinguish performance on this task from
performance on the familiar word reading task.
3. Cohort 1 was the leading edge of the intervention; Cohort 3 was the group of children one grade above
them. As a result, Cohort 3 teachers and students never received the Activity treatment.
4. Results not presented, for parsimony.
5. The 2014 NRA was carried out by the nongovernmental organization Social Impact as part of an external
impact evaluation of the Early Grade Reading Activity; results are presented in Nagarajan et al. (2015).
6. Wide variations in enrolment produce wide-ranging estimates; however, for three-quarters of 2013–2014
LAT schools, the average likelihood that a student sampled in July was being assessed for the first time
was at least 84%, and for 2014–2015 it was 69%.
7. The 2009 program implemented by Save the Children intervened in 24 schools (12 treatment, 12 control).
The 2012 program jointly implemented by Save the Children and World Vision intervened in 30 schools
(15 treatment, 15 control). The 2014 program, named Tiwerenge Ndi Ana Athu, intervened in 10 schools
(Dowd and Mabeti, 2011; Friedlander, Guajardo, et al., 2012; Save the Children International, 2014).
8. The ‘Reading Tools in a Box’ were large plastic containers filled with 700 books (18 titles, including
decodable and levelled readers), with one tub each provided for grades 1–3 in every Activity school (RTI
International, 2016a).
480 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
References
Alexander KL, Entwisle DR and Olson LS (2001) Schools, achievement, and inequality: A seasonal perspec-
tive. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis 23(2): 171–191.
Alexander KL, Entwisle DR and Olson LS (2007) Lasting consequences of the summer learning gap.
American Sociological Review 72(2): 167–180.
Allington RL, McGill-Franzen A, Camilli G, Williams L, Graff J, Zeig J, Zmach C and Nowak R (2010).
Addressing summer reading setback among economically disadvantaged elementary students. Reading
Psychology 31(5): 411–427.
Bakle BR (2010) Summer learning loss: The influence of summer school programs on student achievement
in language usage, math, and reading. PhD dissertation, Indiana State University, USA. ERIC Number:
ED517121.
Card D and Krueger AB (1992) Does school quality matter? Returns to education and the characteristics of
public schools in the United States. Journal of Political Economy 100(1): 1–40.
Chimombo J, Chibwanna M, Dzimadzi C, Kadzamira E, Kunkwenzu E, Kunje D and Namphota D (2000)
Classroom, school and home factors that negatively affect girls education in Malawi. Report prepared
for UNICEF. Lilongwe: Centre for Educational Research and Training (CERT), University of Malawi.
Available at: www.unicef.org/spanish/evaldatabase/files/MLW_2000_005.pdf (accessed 25 February
2017).
Chin T and Phillips M (2004) school reproduction and child-rearing practices: Social class, children’s agency,
and the summer activity gap. Sociology of Education 77(3): 185–210.
Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Colclough C, Rose P and Tembon M (2000) Gender inequalities in primary schooling: The roles of poverty
and adverse cultural practice. IDS Working Paper No. 78. Brighton, UK: Institute of Development
Studies, University of Sussex. Available at: http://pt.ircwash.org/sites/default/files/Colclough-2000-
Gender.pdf (accessed 12 March 2017).
Cooper H, Charlton K, Valentine JC and Muhlenbruck L (2000) Making The Most Of Summer School: A
Meta-Analytic And Narrative Review. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
65(1): i-vi+1–127.
Cooper H, Nye B, Charlton K, Lindsay J and Greathouse S (1996) The effects of summer vacation on achieve-
ment test scores: A narrative and meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research 66(3): 227–268.
Cooper H, Valentine JC, Charlton K and Melson A (2003) The effects of modified school calendars on
student achievement and school community attitudes: A research synthesis. Review of Educational
Research 73(1): 1–52.
Coulombe S and Tremblay J-F (2006) Literacy and growth. Topics in Macroeconomics 6(2): 1–34.
Davies S, Aurini J and Milne E (2016) Contextualizing summer learning inequality: The view from Canada.
In: Alexander K, Pitcock S and Boulay M (eds) The Summer Slide: What We Know and Can Do About
Summer Learning Loss. New York: Teachers College Press.
Dowd AJ and Mabeti F (2011) Literacy Boost Malawi: Year 2 report. Washington, DC: Save the Children.
Available at: https://globalreadingnetwork.net/eddata/literacy-boost-malawi-year-2-report (accessed 5
March 2017).
Dowd AJ, Wiener K and Mabeti F (2010) Malawi Literacy Boost 2009: Year 1 report. Washington, DC: Save
the Children. Available at: https://globalreadingnetwork.net/eddata/literacy-boost-malawi-2009-year-
1-report (accessed 5 March 2017).
Downey DB, Von Hippel PT and Broh BA (2004) Are schools the great equalizer? Cognitive inequality dur-
ing the summer months and the school year. American Sociological Review 69(5): 613–635.
Education Policy and Data Center (EPDC) (2014) Malawi: National Education Profile. Washington, DC:
FHI 360. Available at: www.epdc.org/sites/default/files/documents/EPDC%20NEP_Malawi.pdf
(accessed 1 November 2017).
Fleisch B and Schöer V (2014) Large-scale instructional reform in the global South: Insights from the mid-
point evaluation of the Gauteng primary language and mathematics strategy. South African Journal of
Education 34(3): 1–12.
Slade et al. 481
Friedlander E, Dowd AJ, Borisova I and Guajardo J (2012) Life-Wide Learning: Supporting All Children
to Enjoy Quality Education. Prepared for the United Nations Global Thematic Consultation
‘Addressing Inequalities: The Heart of the Post-2015 Development Agenda and the Future
We Want for All’. Washington, DC: Save the Children. Available at: https://static1.squares-
pace.com/static/57ffc29c414fb543385340da/t/580b998d1b631bb287e9333a/1477155217838/
Friedlander+Dowd+etal_2012_Life-wide+learning-+Supporting+all+children+to+enjoy+quality+e
ducation+-+Friedlander.pdf (accessed 4 November 2017).
Friedlander E, Guajardo J, Williams Vasulu L, Joaki Z, Chasanga K and Polepole M (2012) Malawi Literacy
Boost: Baseline report. Washington, DC: Save the Children and World Vision. Available at: https://
resourcecentre.savethechildren.net/sites/default/files/documents/literacy_boost_world_vision_malawi_
baseline_report-_september_2012.pdf (accessed 12 March 2017).
Gershenson S (2013) Do summer time-use gaps vary by socioeconomic status? American Educational
Research Journal 50(6): 1219–1248.
Glewwe P and Muralidharan K (2016). Chapter 10. Improving education outcomes in developing countries:
Evidence, knowledge gaps, and policy implications. In: Hanushek EA, Machin S and Woessmann L
(eds) Handbook of the Economics of Education, Vol. 5. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp.543–743.
Global Partnership for Education (2016) Education Sector Plan 2008–2017: Malawi. A Statement. Lilongwe,
Malawi: Ministry of Education, Science and Technology. Available at: www.globalpartnership.org/con-
tent/malawi-education-sector-plan (accessed 1 November 2017).
Gove A and Cvelich P (2011) Early Reading: Igniting Education for All. A Report by the Early Learning
Community of Practice. Revised Edition. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI International. Available at:
http://www.rti.org/pubs/early-reading-report-revised.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Guryan J, Kim JS and Quinn DM (2014) Does Reading During the Summer Build Reading Skills? Evidence
from a Randomized Experiment in 463 Classrooms. Working Paper 20689. Cambridge, MA: National
Bureau of Economic Research. Available at: www.nber.org/papers/w20689 (accessed 1 November
2017).
Hanushek EA and Woessmann L (2015) The economic impact of educational quality. In: Dixon P, Humble
S and Counihan C (eds) Handbook of International Development and Education. Cheltenham, UK:
Edward Elgar Publishing, pp.6–19.
Hewett PC, Mensch BS, Lloyd CB, Chimombo J and Rankin J (2009) Assessing the Impact of Primary School
Quality on Adolescent Educational Outcomes in Rural Malawi. Presentation given at 24th International
Population Conference of the International Union for the Scientific Study of Population, Marrakech,
Morocco, 27 September–2 October. Available at: www.popcouncil.org/pdfs/events/2009IUSSP_Hewett.
pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Heyns B (1978) Summer Learning and the Effects of Schooling. New York: Academic.
Immajati Y (2016) Follow-On Gender Study from USAID PRIORITAS Mid-Term Evaluation. Prepared for
USAID under the Prioritizing Reform, Innovation, and Opportunities for Reaching Indonesia’s Teachers,
Administrators, and Students (PRIORITAS) project, Contract No. AID-497-C-12–00003. Research
Triangle Park, NC: RTI International. Available at: http://pdf.usaid.gov/pdf_docs/PA00MG4H.pdf
(accessed 1 November 2017).
Jha J and Pouezevara S (2016) Measurement and Research Support to Education Strategy Goal 1: Boys’
underachievement in education: A review of the literature with a focus on reading in the early years.
Prepared for USAID under the Education Data for Decision Making (EdData II) project, Task Order No.
AID-OAA-12-BC-00003 (RTI Task 20, Activity 13). Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI International.
Available at: http://pdf.usaid.gov/pdf_docs/PBAAF451.pdf.
Kadzamira E and Rose P (2003) Can free primary education meet the needs of the poor? Evidence from
Malawi. International Journal of Educational Development 23: 501–516.
Kananji C (2014) School Examinations and National Assessment in Malawi. Presentation given at
UNESCO Institute for Statistics Regional Workshop on Measurement of Learning Outcomes, Dakar,
Senegal. Available at: www.uis.unesco.org/StatisticalCapacityBuilding/Workshop%20Documents/
Education%20workshop%20dox/Dakar%202014/05_Malawi_EN.pdf.
Kerry T and Davies B (1998) Summer learning loss: The evidence and a possible solution. Support for
Learning 13(3): 118–122.
482 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
Khasu D, Mapondera A, Shawa N and Chiyani D (2014) Report for Study on Student Repetition and Attrition
in Primary Education in Malawi. Prepared by D R Consulting Services (DRCS) for USAID/Malawi.
Available at: http://pdf.usaid.gov/pdf_docs/PA00K2HF.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Kim J (2004) Summer reading and the ethnic achievement gap. Journal of Education for Students Placed at
Risk 9(2): 169–188.
Kim JS and Quinn D (2013) The effects of summer reading on low-income children’s literacy achieve-
ment from kindergarten to grade 8: A meta-analysis of classroom and home interventions. Review of
Educational Research 83(3): 386–431.
Kim JS and White TG (2008) Scaffolding voluntary summer reading for children in grades 3 to 5: An experi-
mental study. Scientific Studies of Reading 12(1): 1–23.
Kim JS, Guryan J, White TG, Quinn DM, Capotosto L and Kingston HC (2016) Delayed effects of a low-cost
and large-scale summer reading intervention on elementary school children’s reading comprehension.
Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness 9(suppl 1): 1–22.
Kolen MJ and Brennan RL (2004) Test Equating, Linking, and Scaling: Methods and Practices (2nd ed.).
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Lareau A (2011) Unequal Childhoods: Class, Race, and Family Life. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Lindahl M (2001) Summer Learning and the Effect of Schooling: Evidence from Sweden. IZA Discussion
Paper Series, No. 262. Bonn, Germany: Forschungsinstitut zur Zukunft der Arbeit [Institute for the Study
of Labor]. Available at: http://ftp.iza.org/dp262.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Logan S and Johnston R (2009) Gender differences in reading ability and attitudes: Examining where these
differences lie. Journal of Research in Reading 32(2): 199–214.
McCombs JS, Augustine CH, Schwartz HL, Bodilly SJ, McInnis B, Lichter DS and Cross AB (2011) Making
Summer Count: How Summer Programs Can Boost Children’s Learning. Santa Monica, CA: RAND
Corporation. Available at: www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/monographs/2011/RAND_MG1120.
pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
McCombs JS, Pane JF, Augustine CH, Schwartz HL, Martorell P and Zakaras L (2014) Ready for Fall?
Near-Term Effects of Voluntary Summer Learning Programs on Low-Income Students’ Learning
Opportunities and Outcomes. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation. Available at: www.rand.org/
pubs/research_reports/RR815.html (accessed 1 November 2017).
Meyer F (2014) The Summer Learning Effect in Germany: Home and School Contributions to Summer
Learning Patterns in the Communities of Two Primary Schools. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Auckland, New Zealand.
Ministry of Education (MOE) [Ethiopia] (2013) Mother Tongue Syllabus, Grades 1–4. Addis Ababa: Ministry
of Education.
Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (MoEST) [Kenya] (2015) National Education Sector Plan
(NESP). Volume One: Basic Education Programme Rationale and Approach (2013–2018). Nairobi:
Ministry of Education, Science and Technology.
Ministry of Education, Science and Technology (MoEST) [Malawi] (2008) National Education Strategic
Plan (NESP) 2008–2017. Lilongwe: Ministry of Education, Science and Technology.
Moore AM, Gove A and Tietjen K (2017) Great expectations: A framework for assessing and understanding
key factors affecting student learning of foundational reading skills. In: McCardle P, Mora A and Gove
A (eds) Progress Toward a Literate World: Early Reading Interventions in Low-Income Countries. New
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 155: 13–30.
Munthali AC (2002) Adaptive Strategies and Coping Mechanisms of Families and Communities Affected by
HIV/AIDS in Malawi. Report prepared for the United Nations Research Institute for Social Development’s
(UNRISD’s) HIV/AIDS and Development project. Grahamstown, South Africa: Department of
Anthropology, Rhodes University. Available at: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.
1.1.511.918&rep=rep1&type=pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Murnane RJ (1975) The Impact of School Resources on the Learning of Inner City Children. Cambridge, MA:
Ballinger.
Nagarajan G, Carneiro P, Chiappetta M, Fuller B, Gonzales E, Sinclair B and Mapondera A (2015)
Assessment: 2014 National Reading Assessment. Prepared for USAID/Malawi under Contract No.
Slade et al. 483
AID-612-C-13-00001. Washington, DC: Social Impact. Available at: http://pdf.usaid.gov/pdf_docs/
PA00KHCK.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
National Institute for Literacy (NIL) [USA] (2010) Adult Literacy Development and Economic Growth.
Washington, DC: NIL. Available at: https://lincs.ed.gov/publications/pdf/AdultLiteracyDevEcoGrowth.
pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
National Statistical Office [Malawi] (2011) Malawi Demographic and Health Survey 2010. Zomba, Malawi:
National Statistical Office; and Calverton, MD: ICF Macro. Available at: www.dhsprogram.com/pubs/
pdf/FR247/FR247.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
O’Brien DM (1999) Three essays on early academic achievement of minority and disadvantaged students.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Dallas.
Phillips M and Chin T (2004) How families, children, and teachers contribute to summer learning and loss.
In: Borman GD and Boulay M (eds) Summer Learning: Research, Policies and Programs. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp.53–69.
Piper B, Jepkemei E and Kibukho K (2015) Pro-poor PRIMR: Improving early literacy skills for children
from low-income families in Kenya. Africa Educational Review 12(1): 67–87.
Piper B, Zuilkowski SS and Mugenda A (2014) Improving reading outcomes in Kenya: First-year effects of
the PRIMR Initiative. International Journal of Educational Development 37: 11–21.
Rose P (2002). Cost-Sharing in Malawian Primary Schooling: From the Washington to the Post-Washington
Consensus. Unpublished PhD thesis, Institute of Education, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.
RTI International (2016a) [Malawi] Early Grade Reading Activity, Quarterly Report, Fiscal Year 2016,
Period of Performance: April 1, 2016 – June 30, 2016. Prepared for USAID under Contract No. AID-
612-C-13-00002. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI.
RTI International (2016b) USAID’s Early Grade Reading Program (EGRP) in Nepal: Baseline report.
Prepared for USAID/Nepal, Task Order No. AID-367-TO-15-00002 of the Assistance to Basic
Education–All Children Reading (ABE-ACR) program. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI.
Sabates R, Akyeampong K, Westbrook J and Hunt F (2010) School Dropout: Patterns, Causes, Changes
and Policies. Background paper prepared for the Education for All Global Monitoring Report 2011,
The Hidden Crisis: Armed Conflict and Education. Report No. 2011/ED/EFA/MRT/PI/08. Available at:
http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0019/001907/190771e.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Save the Children International (2014) Basic education program: TiANA final evaluation report for St. Anthony
and Namadidi Education Zones, Zomba Rural, Malawi. Prepared for USAID under the ‘Tiwerenge Ndi
Ana Athu’ (TiANA) project. Washington, DC: Save the Children.
Schmidt K, DeStefano J and Cummings S (2016) Malawi social and behavior change communications
pilot: Endline report. Prepared for USAID under the Education Data for Decision Making (EdData
II) project, Measurement and Research Support to Education Strategy Goal 1, Task Order No. AID-
OAA-BC-12-00003 (RTI Task 20, Activity 11). Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI International.
Available at: http://pdf.usaid.gov/pdf_docs/PBAAF442.pdf (accessed 12 March 2017).
Share DL and Stanovich KE (1995) Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating
individual differences in a model of acquisition. Issues in Education 1(1): 1–57.
Singh A (2016) Learning More with Every Year: School Year Productivity and International Learning
Divergence. Job Market Paper. London: Department of Economics, University College. Available at:
https://weblearn.ox.ac.uk/access/content/group/34d393d2-bc7a-46b7-8fa9-bf2c2ce8f65e/2016_17/
Job%20Market/Job%20Market%20Folders/Singh_Abhijeet_JMP.pdf (accessed 12 March 2017).
Slates SL, Alexander KL, Entwisle DR and Olson LS (2012) Counteracting summer slide: Social capital
resources within socioeconomically disadvantaged families. Journal of Education for Students Placed
at Risk 17(3): 165–185.
Snilstveit B, Stevenson J, Phillips D, Vojtkova M, Gallagher E, Schmidt T, Jobse H, Geelen M, Grazia
Pastorello M and Eyers J (2015) Interventions for Improving Learning Outcomes and Access to Education
in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: A Systematic Review. 3ie Final Review. London: International
Initiative for Impact Evaluation (3ie). Available at: www.3ieimpact.org/media/filer_public/2015/12/31/
education-review-finalreport.pdf (accessed 12 March 2017).
Sunny BS, Elze M, Chihana M, Gondwe L, Crampin AC, Munkhondya M, Kondowe S and Glynn JR (2017)
Failing to progress or progressing to fail? Age-for-grade heterogeneity and grade repetition in primary
484 Research in Comparative & International Education 12(4)
schools in Karonga district, northern Malawi. International Journal of Educational Development 52:
68–80.
Taniguchi K (2015) Determinants of grade repetition in primary school in sub-Saharan Africa: An event his-
tory analysis for rural Malawi. International Journal of Educational Development 45: 98–111.
UNDP (2015) Briefing Note for Countries on the 2015 Human Development Report: Work for Human
Development, Malawi. Available at: http://hdr.undp.org/sites/all/themes/hdr_theme/country-notes/
MWI.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
UNESCO (2012) At Least 250 Million Children of Primary School-Age Are Failing to Learn the Basics.
Technical note prepared for the Education for All Global Monitoring Report 2012, Youth and Skills:
Putting Education to Work. Report No. 2012/ED/EFA/MRT/PI/49/REV. Paris: UNESCO. Available at:
http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0021/002193/219349E.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
UNESCO (2014) Gambia: Education for All 2015 National Review. Prepared by the Ministry of Basic and
Secondary Education in collaboration with the Gambia National Commission for UNESCO, for use at
the World Education Forum, Incheon, Republic of Korea, 19–22 May 2015. Available at: http://unesdoc.
unesco.org/images/0023/002314/231425e.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
UNESCO Institute for Statistics (UIS) (2011) Financing Education in Sub-Saharan Africa: Meeting the
Challenges of Expansion, Equity and Quality. Montreal: UNESCO-UIS. Available at: http://unesdoc.
unesco.org/images/0019/001921/192186e.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
UNESCO Institute for Statistics (UIS) (2015) School Resources and Learning Environments in Africa: Key
Results from a Regional Survey on Factors Affecting Quality of Education (July 2015). Presentation.
Montreal: UNESCO-UIS. Available at: http://uis.unesco.org/sites/default/files/school-resources-and-
learning-environment-in-africa-2016-en/school-resources-and-learning-environment-in-africa-2016-en.
pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
Verachtert P, Van Damme J, Onghena P and Ghesquiere P (2009) A seasonal perspective on school effective-
ness: Evidence from a Flemish longitudinal study in kindergarten and first grade. School Effectiveness
and School Improvement 20(2): 215–233.
Von Hippel P (2016) Year-round school calendars: Effects on summer learning, achievement, parents, teach-
ers, and property values. In: Alexander K, Pitcock S and Boulay M (eds) Summer Learning and Summer
Learning Loss. New York: Teachers College Press.
Weiseman AW and Baker DP (2004) The American summer learning gap from an international perspective.
In: Borman GD and Boulay M (eds) Summer Learning: Research, Policies and Programs. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp.53–69.
Westberg NB (2010) Girls Versus Boys? Factors Associated with Children’s Schooling in Rural Malawi.
Master’s thesis, Department of Economics and Resource Management, Norwegian University of Life
Sciences, Ås, Norway. Available at: http://www.sv.uio.no/esop/english/research/news-and-events/
news/2014/westberg-thesis.pdf (accessed 1 November 2017).
White WF (1906) Reviews before and after vacation. American Education 19: 185–188.
Willingham WW and Cole NS (1997) Gender and Fair Assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
World Bank (2010) The Education System in Malawi. Working Paper No. 182. Washington, DC: International
Bank for Reconstruction and Development/World Bank. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1596/978-0-
8213-8198-4 (accessed 25 February 2017).
World Bank (2015) World Data Bank: World Development Indicators, Malawi. Available at: http://data.
worldbank.org/country/malawi (accessed 1 November 2017).
Author biography
Timothy S Slade is the Deputy Chief of Party for the Kenya Tusome Early Grade Reading Activity imple-
mented by RTI International, having previously served as DCOP for the Malawi Early Grade Reading
Activity. Since joining RTI his work has focused on the design, delivery, and evaluation of reading interven-
tions in various African contexts. He graduated from Alma College with a B.S. in Exercise & Health Sciences
and a B.A. in French Language, and earned a Master’s degree in International Studies from North Carolina
State University.
Slade et al. 485
Dr. Benjamin Piper is the Senior Director, Africa Education for RTI International based in Nairobi. Dr. Piper
provides technical support to RTI’s work across Sub-Saharan Africa and supervises Tusome, the national
literacy program of Kenya, funded by USAID and DFID, and the Tayari early childhood development (ECD)
program, sponsored by CIFF. Tusome is being implemented in all public primary schools across Kenya, and
Dr. Piper served as the Chief of Party from Tusome’s inception in 2014 to 2016. Dr. Piper led the PRIMR
Initiative, the National Tablet Programme, and the PRIMR Rural Expansion Programme from 2011-2015.
PRIMR tested low-cost, scalable approaches to improving literacy and numeracy. His dissertation research
investigated the impact of Ethiopian in-service programs on teacher and student outcomes. He has worked
with RTI, World Bank, DFID, UNICEF, and Save the Children.
Dr. Zikani Kaunda was the Chief of Party for the Malawi Early Grade Reading Activity implemented by RTI
International. Dr. Kaunda’s education-sector expertise lies in leading programs focused on early grade read-
ing, girls’ education, orphans and other vulnerable children (OVCs), expansion of education access, and
improvement of education quality. Outside the field of education, he has expertise in HIV/AIDs prevention
and mitigation; promotion of gender equality; elimination of gender-based violence; elimination of child
labor; agriculture; and community-based natural resource management. He earned a Bachelor’s degree in
agriculture from the University of Malawi, a Master’s degree in Public Development and Management from
Witwatersrand University, and a Doctor of Philosophy degree in Comparative International Development and
Education at the University of Minnesota.
Simon King is a Research Statistician with interests in complex survey methodology and analysis. Mr. King
has been lead statistician on many early grade reading projects, conducted capacity-building activities and led
four technical workshops on sampling, data management, weighting, and analysis. Prior to working for RTI,
Mr. King was involved in K–12 education in the UK, Switzerland, USA, Zambia and Channel Islands, nota-
bly as a principal of a charter school and as a volunteer teacher working for DFID in rural Zambia. He
received his B.S. in Mathematics from Royal Holloway College, University of London, and his M.S. in
Statistics from Texas A&M University.
Hibatalla Ibrahim is an Ed.M. candidate in International Education Policy at Harvard Graduate School of
Education. She is interested in literacy, mother-tongue instruction, gender and education in conflict settings
with particular emphasis on disadvantaged populations in the Middle East and Africa. She obtained her under-
graduate degree from Monash University.
... The Education Endowment Foundation (2020) indicates that any gap in schooling, even routinely scheduled holidays, can cause a significant learning loss. Slade et al. (2017) According to Baker (2011), during a 20-day school closure due to teachers' strikes in Ontario, Canada, a learning loss occurred in the Mathematics test scores, equal to half (0.5) of a standard deviation. Wills (2019) discovered that, in South Africa, students' performance in subjects taught by a teacher who did not engage in strikes was approximately 0.1 standard deviations higher than the subjects taught by a teacher who striked. ...
... Another popular definition of Learning Loss by Ari (2005) is the "inability to retrieve acquired information from memory". Slade et al. (2017) define Learning Loss as "going beyond the determined planning, not gaining or missing the desired competencies, and naturally experiencing problems in reaching educational goals". ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
A good tertiary education is linked to broad socio-economic advantages after students with disabilities enroll in higher education. Their overall academic performance, retention rates, and graduation rates are more similar than ever to those of their non-disabled classmates. A student can be considered as having special education needs if he or she is suffering from learning difficulties or a disability and cannot work equally as peers. The broad problem of the study was what are existing barriers that hinder educational inclusion for students with disabilities in higher education sector in Sri Lanka. Hence the key objective of the research was to identify the barriers that hinder educational inclusion for students with disabilities through the perceptions of undergraduates in relation to peers with disabilities. The study is a non-experimental, descriptive, and association design between variables using multivariate statistical techniques using 184 sample units from Humanities and Social Science Faculty, University of Ruhuna. It was found by exploratory factor analysis that the elements had the most impact on the situation were to accessibility and resources that universities must facilitate inclusion, academic staff’ willingness to meet the needs of students with disabilities, real implementation of the curricular adjustments and relationships and participation of students with disabilities and peers. The findings of the study suggest that universities should execute targeted programs to address the knowledge gap about the students with disabilities and the operation of care and support services for them. Keywords: factor analysis, higher education; inclusive education; perceptions, students with disabilities
... Similarly, during school closures, many school holidays pose risks, especially for disadvantaged students (Entwistle & Alexander, 1997). Jahan, Zviedrite, Gao, Ahmed, and Uzicanin (2022) point out that school closures which are unplanned or outside the determined academic calendar cause undesirable consequences for rural students from families in low socioeconomic levels (Slade, Piper, Kaunda, King, & Ibrahim, 2017;Stewart, Watson, & Campbell, 2018). Likewise, the distance from rural areas to places with learning opportunities negatively affects families (Oncescu & Giles, 2013). ...
... Research has shown that students often experience a decline in academic skills and knowledge after a long summer break if they do not engage in learning activities [4,5]. This is particularly problematic for university students who have a longer summer break, as the extended period of inactivity can exacerbate the potential for learning loss. ...
Article
Summer classes have become increasingly prevalent in higher education institutions worldwide, offering students opportunities to accelerate their learning, reduce course loads, and maintain academic momentum. This qualitative study explores the perspectives of lecturers and undergraduate students on the benefits, drawbacks, and strategies for success of summer classes. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with ten students and two lecturers from a university in Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam. The findings reveal that the primary perceived benefits of summer classes include the ability to complete courses more quickly, have a lighter subject load during the regular year, and improve academic performance. However, drawbacks cited were the sacrifice of leisure time or income from part-time work and the added pressure of condensed timelines. Recommended strategies comprise proactive promotion of summer class advantages to increase student awareness, careful selection and compensation of skilled instructors, and incorporation of recreational activities to support student well-being. This study contributes qualitative insights to inform the development of effective, student-centered summer programs in higher education.
... Also in Africa, intervention programs show mixed results. A five-month mediation for proficiency educators in Malawi resulted in changes in attitude and beliefs about their showing capacities however there were no movements in instructional practices watched (Slade et al., 2017). In Kenya, Tusome which is the national proficiency program of the Kenya Ministry of Education, was planned and is being executed in a setting of developing enthusiasm for huge-scale program usage . ...
Article
The purpose of the study was to establish the teachers’ perception on the influence of Tusome instructional intervention strategies on reading levels in public primary schools in central zone, Kilifi North Sub-County, Kenya. The study employed a descriptive design to carry out the study. A pilot study was conducted in two schools. The sample size comprised 60 grade two children, 18 teachers, 5 headteachers, ICSO and 1 sub-county director of education. Data collection techniques involved the use of observations and questionnaires to collect both qualitative and quantitative data. The collected data obtained from observations and respondents were grouped together, coded, transformed and analyzed through the use of Statistical Packages for Social Scientists (SPSS) software version 21. Then the analyzed data was presented in form of frequencies and percentages while figures and tables will be used to present the result where necessary. The findings revealed that teaching strategies have an influence on the implementation of Tusome in lower primary schools. Pupil’s textbooks, teacher’s guides and storybooks were adequately provided. There were no other ICT resources provided for the learners. It was concluded that indeed, the Tusome literacy intervention program was strongly related to the improved learner achievement in other tasks that require reading, in the improvement of the overall learner achievement at grade 2. The ANOVA statistics were used to test the fitness of the regression model. The significance F value of 47.345 (p<0.001) was obtained implying that the regression model was fit and statistically significant and can be deemed fit for prediction. The study recommends that the Ministry of Education implements literacy interventions as a means of improving learner achievement in other subjects. The instructional support included should take into consideration the scope (number of teachers supported), as well as the logistics of reaching schools, teachers and head teachers. Article visualizations: </p
... Learning loss during school closures is already a well-documented challenge in high-and low-income countries (Slade et al., 2017). The pandemic poses additional challenges because it is unpredictable and combines school closures with additional shocks, such as adverse health impacts on households, and economic impacts through the lost of parents' jobs (Jain et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Online learning conducted during the pandemic is a solution to this global disaster dilemma. Students must follow government policies to carry out online learning amid their limitations. Even though technology is currently developing rapidly, there are still difficulties that cause learning at home to be not optimal. The existence of difficulties in the learning process will result in learning loss. This study aims to see the impact of learning loss experienced by students. This study uses a literature study method by describing the findings from various literature sources from previous research. The results of this study indicate that the form of learning loss experienced by students will cause lost for years and have an impact on academic performance until they become adults cumulatively. Students feel a decrease in interest in learning compared to face-to-face learning. To address learning loss, policymakers should consider targeted programs, use teaching methods, improve teacher-student communication and involve parents to accelerate recovery and minimize the impact of learning loss.
... Learning loss is the deterioration of learned skills and missed learning opportunities that students would have benefitted from without interruptions to schooling (Angrist et al., 2021;Slade et al., 2017). The Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) and Harvard Graduate School of Education's (2020) survey of 98 countries reveals salient education challenges in mitigating learning loss during COVID-19 converge to, among others: (1) ensuring the continuity of academic learning; (2) supporting students who lack skills for independent or online study; (3) ensuring continuity and integrity of the learning assessments; and (4) defining new priorities. ...
Chapter
The COVID-19 pandemic has wreaked havoc across all sectors, with education being no exception. India's education system is one of the world's largest, serving 265 million students from diverse socio-economic backgrounds. India is also known for the heterogeneity and diversity in its education system. Nearly 50% of the total students in schools are enrolled in private schools of many hues and color. The rest study in government schools, again of many different kind and types. The present chapter seeks to analyse the effect of the pandemic specifically on the socially and economically disadvantaged groups, which mostly rely on the government for schooling. The chapter uses a counter-factual approach to analyse the data culled out from UDISE, UDISE+, and ASER. It seeks to analyse the impact and emergent trends. The chapter intends to identify the probable causes of massive learning losses amongst children in government schools and comment on the impact of lockdown and loss of lives and livelihood. The significance of this chapter lies in quantifying and highlighting the extent of the pandemic's impact on the education sector. Finally, the chapter is expected to help policymakers address critical issues, reallocate budgets, and make necessary operational and strategic decisions.
Chapter
The sole reason behind this chapter was to focus on the traditional pedagogical practices implemented prior to the pandemic. In addition to this, the unprecedented and daunting issues experienced for home-learning which forced to a more personal learning can be seen as an outcome. Although humanity has witnessed as many pandemics as war, readiness and decision-making on the part of educational policymakers, administrators, and teachers was the need of the hour. The traditional forte failed to relieve the knowledge sector in availing and utilizing the technological innovation. Thus, how unprepared we were for the pandemic and how a recovery plan was required to balance out the loss of time can also be read.
Article
Full-text available
The study assessed the implementation of the Malawi nation reading Programme's theoretical grounding and its localisation based on context. The methodology included a standardised reading assessment that tested the ability of children to read and regressed contextual factors to examine how they contributed to the reading skills of pupils. Results showed that the programme was well grounded in theory but lacked awareness of the implementation context. Therefore, the study recommends a balance of effort and investment in the implementation of reading programmes for optimal and sustained reading skill gains by pupils.
Article
Full-text available
Timely progression through school is an important measure for school performance, completion and the onset of other life transitions for adolescents. This study examines the risk factors for grade repetition and establishes the extent to which age-for-grade heterogeneity contributes to subsequent grade repetition at early and later stages of school. Using data from a demographic surveillance site in Karonga district, northern Malawi, a cohort of 8174 respondents (ages 5–24 years) in primary school was followed in 2010 and subsequent grade repetition observed in 2011. Grade repetition was more common among those at early (grades 1–3) and later (grades 7–8) stages of school, with little variation by sex. Being under-age or over-age in school has different implications on schooling outcomes, depending on the stage of schooling. After adjusting for other risk factors, boys and girls who were under-age at early stages were at least twice as likely to repeat a grade as those at the official age-for-grade (girls: adjusted OR 2.06 p<0.01; boys: adjusted OR 2.37 p<0.01); while those over-age at early stages were about 30% less likely to repeat (girls: adjusted OR 0.65 p<0.01; boys: adjusted OR 0.72 p<0.01). Being under/over-age at later grades (4–8) was not associated with subsequent repetition but being over-age was associated with dropout. Other risk factors identified that were associated with repetition included both family-level factors (living away from their mother, having young children in the household, lower paternal education) and school-level factors (higher student-teacher ratio, proportion of female teachers and schools without access to water). Reducing direct and indirect costs of schooling for households; and improving school quality and resources at early stages of school may enable timely progression at early stages for greater retention at later stages.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports on a mid-point evaluation of the Gauteng Primary Language and Mathematics Strategy (GPLMS), an innovative large-scale reform designed to improve learning outcomes. Using data from universal testing of all learners in 2008 on a provincial systemic evaluation, and data from the 2011 and 2012 Annual National Assessment (ANA) test, this paper addresses the key research question, namely whether the GPLMS is effective in closing the gap between performing and underperforming schools. Given the evidence we have presented of an instrument effect, namely that various versions of the ANA may not be strictly comparable, no definitive conclusions can be drawn about the effectiveness of the GPLMS.
Article
I use unique child-level panel data from Ethiopia, India, Peru, and Vietnam, four developing countries with widely differing levels of student achievement, to study the extent to which differences in the productivity of primary schooling can explain international differences in human capital. I document, using identical tests of quantitative skills across countries, that although some cross-sectional gaps in test scores between these countries are evident at preschool ages, these grow substantially in the first 2–3 years of schooling. By the age of 8 years, differences are particularly stark between Vietnam and the other three countries. Using value-added models, and a regression-discontinuity design based on enrolment guidelines, I show that the causal effect of an extra grade of schooling on test scores is substantially higher in Vietnam by 0.25–0.4 standard deviations compared to the other countries. This differential productivity of a school year accounts for most of the cross-country achievement gap at 8 years of age. Equalizing the exposure to and the productivity of schooling closes the gap with Vietnam almost entirely for Peru and India and by ∼60% for Ethiopian students enrolled.
This article addresses the evolution of the underlying theories of change in global education reform efforts between 1990 and 2015, informed by the shift in focus from access to quality and learning. We review recent data regarding how different types of donor interventions (i.e., structural or pedagogical) have contributed to improved reading outcomes and compare effect sizes over a series of intervention studies conducted from 2003 to 2015. Against this background, we present a framework for understanding how the intensity, frequency, and fidelity of the interventions as well as the enabling environments of reform affect the magnitude and rates at which reading and learning outcomes can be expected to improve. In this, we present the context for the articles that follow, identifying the program design characteristics and types of interventions that increase the likelihood of successful expansion of the interventions commonly referred to as “scaling-up,” the ability to sustain interventions, and the value (cost effectiveness) of reading programs in low- and middle-income countries.
Article
To improve the reading comprehension outcomes of children in high poverty schools, policymakers need to identify reading interventions that show promise of effectiveness at scale. This study evaluated the effectiveness of a low-cost and large-scale summer reading intervention that provided comprehension lessons at the end of the school year and stimulated home-based summer reading routines with narrative and informational books. We conducted a randomized controlled trial involving 59 elementary schools, 463 classrooms, and 6,383 second and third graders and examined outcomes on the North Carolina End-of-Grade (EOG) reading comprehension test administered nine months after the intervention, in the children's third- or fourth-grade year. We found that on this delayed outcome, the treatment had a statistically significant impact on children's reading comprehension, improving performance by .04 SD (standard deviation) overall and .05 SD in high poverty schools. We also found, in estimates from an instrumental variables analysis, that children's participation in home-based summer book reading routines improved reading comprehension. The cost-effectiveness ratio for the intervention compared favorably to existing compensatory education programs that target high poverty schools.