ArticlePDF Available

Using Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate with Hypocrites About Water Conservation and Climate Change

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Applied Communications
%52;3+ A99;+ 8:/)2+
Using Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate with
Hypocrites About Water Conservation and
Climate Change
Melissa R. Taylor
University of Florida
Alexa J. Lamm
University of Florida
Lisa K. Lundy
University of Florida
5225=:./9'4*'**/:/54'2=5819': .G64+=68'/8/+68+9958-0')
'8:5,:.+ -8/);2:;8+5335494</8543+4:'2":;*/+9533549'4*:.+ "5)/'24F;+4)+
'4* 52/:/)'2533;4/)':/54533549
D/9!+9+'8)./9(85;-.::5?5;,58,8++'4*56+4'))+99(?+= 8'/8/+ 8+99:.'9(++4'))+6:+*,58/4)2;9/54/45;84'25,662/+*533;4/)':/549
(?'4';:.58/@+*'*3/4/9:8':585,+= 8'/8/+ 8+9958358+/4,583':/5462+'9+)54:'): )'*919:':++*;
!+)533+4*+*/:':/54
#'?258+2/99'!'332+>''4*;4*?/9'$9/4-5-4/:/<+/9954'4)+:5533;4/)':+=/:.?65)8/:+9(5;:
&':+8549+8<':/54'4*2/3':+.'4-+ Journal of Applied Communications%5299 .G69*5/58-

Using Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate with Hypocrites About
Water Conservation and Climate Change
Abstract
D++C+):95,)2/3':+).'4-+)'4(+3/:/-':+*(?'2:+8/4-.;3'4(+.'</588+2':+*:5=':+8)549+8<':/54
.5=+<+83'4?=.5'8+'='8+5,)2/3':+).'4-+'8+45:'2/-4/4-:.+/8(+.'</58:5);8(:.+/36'):D/9
8+9+'8).95;-.::5+>6258+:.+8+2':/549./6(+:=++4)/:/@+49B(+2/+,9'G/:;*+9'4*(+.'</5898+-'8*/4-=':+8
)549+8<':/54'4*:.+/8145=2+*-+'4*(+2/+,98+-'8*/4-)2/3':+).'4-+:5-;/*+:.+*+<+2563+4:5,+C+):/<+
)533;4/)':/54)'36'/-49,5);9+*54=':+8)549+8<':/54$9/4-)5-4/:/<+*/9954'4)+:.+58?'4*'4'*'6:+*
+4</8543+4:'2'G/:;*+9'4*(+.'</587;'8:+::./98+9+'8).,5);9+*54/4*/</*;'29=.5*+3549:8':+*./-.
2+<+295,)2/3':+).'4-+145=2+*-+(;:*/*45:+4-'-+/4659/:/<+=':+8)549+8<':/54(+.'</5898+,+88+*:5'9
?65)8/:+9D+E4*/4-98+<+'2+*?65)8/:+9.+2**/C+8+4:6+896+):/<+954)2/3':+).'4-+:.'4:.+-+4+8'2
6;(2/)D+?(+2/+<+*)2/3':+).'4-+='98+'2'4*)';9+*(?.;3'49(;:'8+*5/4-2/G2+:5);8(:.+/86+8954'2
=':+8;9+'4*'8+45::'1/4-6+8954'2'):/54:53/:/-':+:.++C+):95,)2/3':+).'4-+D+(+9:)533;4/)':/54
95;8)+9:5;9+/48+')./4-:.+9+/4*/</*;'29='9+>'3/4+*'4**/9);99+*=/:.8+)533+4*':/5495C+8+*,58
.5=:5(+9:+4-'-+:.+.?65)8/:/)'2-85;6=.59.5;2*(+359:2/1+2?:5).'4-+:.+/8=':+8)549+8<':/54
(+.'</589
Keywords
)2/3':+).'4-+)5-4/:/<+*/9954'4)++4</8543+4:'2)533;4/)':/546;(2/)56/4/54=':+8)549+8<':/54
Creative Commons License
D/9=581/92/)+49+*;4*+8' 8+':/<+533549G8/(;:/5454)533+8)/'2".'8+2/1+/)+49+
Cover Page Footnote/Acknowledgements
D/98+9+'8)./9,;4*+*(?:.+$"+4:+8,58 ;(2/)99;+9*;)':/54/4-8/);2:;8+'4*':;8'2
!+95;8)+9'4*$"!"6850+):D+8+9+'8).='968+9+4:+*'::.+"5;:.+84
995)/':/545,-8/);2:;8'2")/+4:/9:9-8/);2:;8'2533;4/)':/54"+):/54/4+(8;'8?/45(/2+
2'('3'
D/98+9+'8)./9'<'/2'(2+/45;84'25,662/+*533;4/)':/549 .G64+=68'/8/+68+9958-0')<52/99
Using Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate with Hypocrites
About Water Conservation and Climate Change
Introduction
Despite near unanimity about the existence of climate change from the science community,
the United State’s public opinion varies between those who believe to those who deny its
existence (Donner & McDaniels, 2013). Scientists have confirmed that climate change is real, it
is happening now, and humans are primarily to blame (Liu, Vedlitz, Stoutenborough, &
Robinson, 2015). Research has shown individual attitudes about climate change are influenced
by many factors including personal values, political ideology, current events, media coverage
and risk perception (Donner & McDaniels, 2013). The discrepancy between public opinion and
scientific evidence has generated concern given the public makes everyday choices about their
use of natural resources including water, which is affected a great deal by climate change (Guy,
Kashima, Walker, & O’Neill, 2014). While the planet warms, the hydrological cycle will
intensify causing wet regions to get even wetter and dry regions drier (Famiglietti, 2016). The
Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (2013) is highly confident that the contrast between
wet and dry regions and wet and dry seasons will increase over most of the world.
Areas affected by drought have become the most concerning. In the last decade, there has
been an increased interest and attention towards water security, reflected in the numerous
publications, research, and funding initiatives focused on the topic (Bakker, 2012; Cook &
Bakker, 2012; Pahl-Wstol, Gupta, & Bhaduri, 2016; UNESCO-IHE, 2009; World Economic
Forum, 2011). It has been predicted that by 2050 one-third of all U.S. counties will face water
scarcity (Spencer & Altman, 2010). The length and location of droughts have increased due to
climate change and this trend is projected to continue into the future (Burke, Brown, &
Christidis, 2006). In addition, global demands on water continue to rise due to population
increases, agricultural needs, and industrial demands (Kingsolver, 2010). Water supports human
life, sustains the ecological balance, and supports economic activities around the world
(Hurlimann, Dolnicar, & Meyer, 2009), therefore it must be protected. People can witness the
direct effect climate change has on water, therefore water issues associated with climate change
are garnering a great deal of public attention.
Unfortunately, the scientific community has not fully and effectively communicated the
science behind climate change and its link to water resources to the general public (Liu, Smith, &
Safi, 2014). When scientists discuss climate change they are often referring to a set of complex
variables and topical areas which may include ocean levels, temperature, annual rainfall, and
atmospheric pressure (Werndl, 2015) that seem ambiguous to the general public and not
something they directly impact with their behaviors. However, Martinsson and Lundqvist (2010)
found that knowledge levels of climate change did impact their respondents’ attitudes towards
water conservation and guided individual water conservation behavior engagement.
Agricultural communicators have encouraged communities to implement water conservation
solutions for the sake of saving water for the future (Gorham, Lamm, & Rumble, 2014; Lenton
& Muller, 2009; Warner, Rumble, Martin, Lamm, & Cantrell, 2015). However, most water
conservation practices occur at the individual level and understanding factors that lead to
positive water conservation attitudes have proven to be difficult to measure (Lamm, Lamm, &
Carter, 2015; Leal, Rumble, & Lamm, 2015). Communication campaigns focused on future
water supply levels with an emphasis on climate change may assist in the promotion of water
1
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
conservation behaviors necessary to ensure communities have enough water to meet future needs
(Evans et al., 2015). Agricultural communicators may be able to increase engagement in water
conservation behavior engagement by focusing on communicating about climate change, a topic
often overlooked or avoided and not included in water discussions. Therefore, this study sought
to explore the connection (or disconnect) between public beliefs and attitudes about water
conservation and knowledge and beliefs regarding climate change to guide the development of
effective communication campaigns focused on water conservation.
Conceptual Framework and Literature Review
The theory of cognitive dissonance suggests individuals tend to feel uncomfortable when
their behavior and beliefs contradict one and another (Festinger, 1957). Cognitive dissonance
(Festinger, 1957) guided the development of a conceptual framework for this study, which
sought to understand the disconnect between individuals holding a high level of climate change
knowledge and yet not engaging in positive water conservation practices. Martinsson and
Lundqvist (2010) stated ‘the importance of consistency in the environmental field and the
amount of dissonance produced by behaving inconsistently has been found to depend on the
person’s moral standards for environmentally responsible behavior’ (p. 522). Furthermore,
Thogersen (2004) found individuals often self-report dissonant environmental behaviors because
they fail to perceive the relevant similarity between the behaviors (example: buying organic and
recycling). He advocated for communicating to citizens the environmental significance of daily
individual behaviors.
When it comes to climate change people typically begin in a state of disinterest about the
climate and exhibit little or no interest in changing their behavior (Markowitz & Doppelt, 2009).
This tends to be caused by a lack of information and the idea that individual behaviors will do
little to mitigate the global situation (Markowitz & Doppelt, 2009). Providing individuals with
knowledge about climate change, and emphasizing their personal role, may enable them to make
the decision most suitable to their beliefs and behaviors. Based on previous research
‘environmental choices are not reflective of a general conservation stance, but are instead made
on an activity-to-activity basis’ (Picket, Kangun, & Gorve, 1993, p. 240). Additionally, studies
have shown cognitive dissonance can produce behavior that is environmentally friendly (Aitken,
McMahon, Wearing, & Finlayson, 1994).
A conceptual framework was introduced by Martinsson and Lundqvist (2010) that identified
individuals who practiced green habits and whether or not those practices correlated with their
attitudes toward the environment. Green habits are defined as behaviors that seek to limit an
individuals’ ecological footprint (Dobson, 2007). Martinsson and Lundqvist (2010) created an
environmental attitudes and behavior quartet. Using this quartet there are four possible
combinations of attitudes and behaviors that can be identified. Two of these groups show
consistent attitudes and behaviors while the remaining exhibit inconsistent patterns leading to
cognitive dissonance. The conceptual framework was adapted to address climate change for this
study and can be seen in Figure 1.
Combinations of climate change knowledge and conservation behavior engagement within
the conceptual framework lead to four theoretical categories of individuals in terms of
environmental attitudes and conservation behaviors: Believers, Diehards, Hypocrites and Coverts
(Martinsson & Lundqvist, 2010). Believers are identified as those who possess high levels of
climate change knowledge and exhibit positive water conservation behaviors. Believers reflect
2
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
consistency when it comes to their attitudes and behaviors (Martinsson & Lundqvist, 2010). In
this case, Believers trust climate change is happening and is influenced by humans. They also
practice positive water conservation behaviors and actions. Diehards also exhibit consistency
between knowledge and behaviors, however, these individuals hold low levels of knowledge of
climate change and do not exhibit positive water conservation behaviors (Martinsson &
Lundqvist, 2010). Diehards typically act with a disregard towards climate change and water
conservation behaviors.
Conservation Behaviors
Coverts
Individuals holding low levels of climate
change knowledge but exhibit positive
conservation practices
Believers
Individuals holding high levels of
climate change knowledge and exhibit
positive conservation practices
Diehards
Individuals holding low levels of climate
change knowledge and do not exhibit
positive conservation practices
Hypocrites
Individuals holding high levels of
climate change knowledge but do not
exhibit positive conservation practices
Climate Change Knowledge
Figure 1. Climate Change Quartet Conceptual Framework (adapted from Martinsson and
Lundqvist, 2010)
On the other side of the model, there are two categories with inconsistencies between beliefs
and behaviors. One is the Hypocrites. Hypocrites express high levels of climate change
knowledge but do not engage in positive water conservation behaviors. Based on Festinger’s
(1957) theory this group exhibits the highest level of cognitive dissonance. Hypocrisy is not
uncommon in the realm of conservation behavior. This discrepancy can produce behavioral
changes, especially when the relationship between knowledge and behavioral actions is deemed
hypercritical (Dickerson, Thibodeau, Aronson, & Miller, 1992; Rubens, Gosling, Bonaiuto,
Brisbois, & Moch, 2015). Researchers have even purposefully induced hypocrisy in order to
examine its ability to change intentions and behaviors (Aronson, Fried, & Stone, 1991; Priolo et
al., 2016).
The last group is the Coverts that engage in water conservation behaviors but have low levels
of climate change knowledge (Martinsson & Lundqvist, 2010). This last group also exhibits a
high level of cognitive dissonance in relation to their knowledge and behaviors and are most
likely engaging in water conservation behaviors for reasons other than climate change.
Purpose and Research Objectives
The purpose of this study was to explain cognitive dissonance in persons from the general
public who display characteristics and traits of Hypocrites. This knowledge will then be used to
develop agricultural communication initiatives targeted at Hypocrites in order to alter their water
conservation behavior. The research objectives were as follows
3
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
Research Objective 1:
Describe Hypocrites knowledge of climate change.
Research Objective 2:
Describe Hypocrites perception of climate change.
Research Objective 3:
Describe Hypocrites level of engagement in water conservation behaviors.
Research Objective 4:
Identify the sources Hypocrites use to get information about water issues.
Methods
The research presented here was part of a larger research project with four sections germane
to the objectives of the study. The researchers used a web-based survey to collect data that
included several elements from already existing, reliable instruments including the Canadian
water attitudes survey (Patterson, 2012) and the American Knowledge of Climate Change survey
(Leiserowitz, Smith, & Marlon, 2010). The latter survey was used in Yale’s 2010 Climate
Change Communication report: Americans’ Knowledge of Climate Change (Leiserowitz et al.,
2010).
To measure levels of climate change knowledge test consisting of eleven statements where
respondents were asked to indicate whether each statement was true or false was utilized. This
scale originated from the American Knowledge of Climate Change survey (Leiserowitz et al.,
2010) found to be reliable in the literature with coefficients ranging from .72 to .86. For every
correct answer, the respondents were given a score of one and an incorrect answer was given a
zero. The responses were summed to create an overall climate change knowledge score ranging
from zero to 11 (M = 7.30 SD = 2.43). Reliability was calculated ex post facto using a Guttman
split-half test resulting in a reliability coefficient of .70.
To identify perspectives on climate change, respondents were asked to select which of the
three statements they personally believed: (a) climate change is happening now, caused mainly
by human activities, (b) climate change is happening now, caused mainly by natural forces, and
(c) climate change is NOT happening. This question was adapted from the American Knowledge
of Climate Change survey by Leiserowitz et al. (2010) and reported descriptively.
To measure water conservation behavior engagement, respondents were asked to respond to
two sets of statements. These statements originated from the Canadian water attitudes survey
(Patterson, 2012). The first set contained 10 statements pertaining to water conservation
activities where respondents were asked to indicate how often they engaged in each behavior on
a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = Never, 2 = Almost never, 3 = Sometimes, 4 = Almost
every time, and 5 = Every time. Example statements included: ‘I shower for no more than five
minutes each time I bathe,’ ‘I let my sprinklers run when it has rained or is raining,’ and ‘I allow
used motor oil to run down a storm drain.’ The second set contained six statements asking
respondents to indicate if they engaged in water conservation behaviors by answering ‘yes’or
‘no’ to each statement. Example statements included ‘I have low-flow shower heads installed in
my home,’ ‘I have water-efficient toilets installed in my home,’ and ‘I have low-water
consuming plant materials in my yard.’
4
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
A total engagement score for water conservation behaviors was assigned to each respondent
by adding up the number of positive behavior experiences they reported. For the first set of
statements those who answered almost every time or every time received one point. Three of
these statements were reverse coded to reflect a positive answer: I turn off the water every time
I brush my teeth,’ I avoid watering my lawn in the summer,’ and I shower for no more than
five minutes each time I bathe. From the second set, each yes response was given one point as
well. The responses were summed to create an overall score ranging from zero to 16 (M = 7.48,
SD = 3.31). Reliability was calculated ex post facto using a Guttman split-half test resulting in a
reliability coefficient of .68.
Finally, respondents were asked to identify where they received information about water
issues in the U.S. Respondents were given a list of 13 possible information sources and allowed
to check all that applied. Those sources included newspaper, social media, Internet, magazine,
farming organizations, family/friends, attending events/activities, governmental websites, self-
observation, television, radio, other, or none of the above. Prior to distribution, a panel of experts
reviewed the survey instrument for internal validity. The panel included an Assistant Professor
and Extension Specialist in Water Economics and Policy, the Director of the UF/IFAS Center for
Public Issues Education in Agriculture and Natural Resources, and an Assistant Professor
specializing in survey methodology.
The population of interest was U.S. residents aged 18 or older. Non-probability opt-in
sampling techniques were used. A third party public opinion research company, Qualtrics,
distributed the survey by sending a link to 2,703 U.S. residents. Respondents had to meet certain
criteria based on the sampling procedure to enter the survey and pass a series of quality checks to
complete the survey to ensure cognitively responsive results. After criteria-based selection and
quality assurance, a 39% participation rate was obtained (N = 1,050). Demographic questions
were included in the survey instrument to ensure the collected sample reflected the U.S. adult
population and were geographically representative of the nation. In addition, the data were
weighted using the 2010 U.S. Census for age, gender, and race/ethnicity to ensure the
respondents were representative of the population of interest (Kalton & Flores-Cervantes, 2003).
This is a common procedure when using non-probability sampling to ensure accuracy and
alleviate the impacts of selection, exclusion, and bias (Baker et al., 2013). The results were
analyzed descriptively using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 23
and Excel.
Results
Hypocrites Knowledge of Climate Change
Scores for the climate change knowledge questions were averaged to create an overall
climate change knowledge index that could range from zero to 11. Based on the climate change
index mean score of 7.30 a response of seven or higher indicated a high level of climate change
knowledge (Table 1). Next, an overall water conservation index was created ranging from zero to
16. Based on the water conservation behavior index mean score of 7.48 a response of six or
lower indicated the respondent exhibited negative conservation behaviors. Respondents with
high levels of climate change knowledge and poor water conservation behaviors were labeled
Hypocrites (Figure 2). The 233 respondents that fell into this category were used for further
analysis.
5
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
Table 1
Climate Change Knowledge Level and Level of Water Conservation Behavior Engagement
Knowledge Quiza
M (SD)
Water Conservation
Behaviorsb
M (SD)
8.78 (1.27)
9.84 (2.03)
9.01 (1.28)
3.98 (1.72)
4.59 (1.43)
9.26 (1.87)
4.67 (1.25)
4.15 (1.65)
Note. aScale ranged from 0 = no knowledge to 16 = complete knowledge; bScale ranged from
0 = no engagement to 11 = complete engagement.
Figure 2. Climate Change Quartet
Demographically, the Hypocrites were slightly more female (52.9%) than male. In addition,
Hypocrites were well educated with 46.9% having at least a 4-year college degree or a
Graduate/Professional degree. Hypocrites tended to report being liberal or very liberal (34.8%)
and were young (Table 2).
6
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
Table 2
Demographics of Overall Respondents and Hypocrites
Overall
Hypocrites
(N = 1,050)
(n = 233)
%
%
Sex
Female
51.2
52.9
Male
48.8
47.1
Education
Less than 12th grade
1.7
.7
High School/GED
21.6
19.7
Some college, no degree
24.9
20.8
2-year college degree
13.3
11.9
4-year college
26.2
31.1
Graduate/Professional degree
12.4
15.9
Race
White
66.9
64.5
Black
11.6
11.1
Asian or Pacific Islander
5.0
6.3
Multiracial
1.4
3.3
Native American
0.7
0.7
Other
0.2
.3
Hispanic Ethnicity
14.2
13.8
Political Beliefs
Very Liberal
10.1
9.0
Liberal
18.5
25.8
Moderate
45.4
52.1
Conservative
17.8
10.9
Very Conservative
8.3
2.2
Political Affiliation
Republican
26.1
20.2
Democrat
38.1
41.9
Independent
25.3
25.4
Non-Affiliated
9.9
12.2
Other
.5
.2
Age
20-29
18.2
28.9
30-39
17.1
21.3
40-49
18.6
16.5
50-59
17.9
11.5
60-69
12.5
8.6
70-79
7.1
5.5
80+
8.7
7.8
7
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
Hypocrites Perception of Climate Change
In addition to knowledge level being measured quantitatively using a test, perceptions of
climate change were also examined descriptively using a categorical question. Hypocrites were
likely to believe climate change was happening now and caused mainly by humans. There were a
relatively low number of Hypocrites that believed climate change was caused by natural forces
(18.6%) or not happening at all (Table 3).
Table 3
Perceptions of Climate Change
Overall
Hypocrites
(N = 1,050)
(n = 233)
%
%
Climate change is happening now and caused mainly by humans
63.7
79.4
Climate change is happening now, caused mainly by natural forces
29.6
18.6
Climate change is not happening now
6.7
2.0
Hypocrite Engagement in Water Conservation Behaviors
Respondents were asked to identify their level of engagement in water conservation behavior
efforts with a series of 16 statements. The first ten statements represented water conservation
actions. The highest reported negative water conservation actions Hypocrite respondents
reported being engaged in were leaving the water running in the kitchen when washing dishes
(45.0%), showering for longer than five minutes (42.8%), never turning off the water while
brushing teeth (33.6%), and watering the lawn in the summer (32.2%) (Table 4).
Table 4
Hypocrites Engagement in Water Conservation Actions (n = 233)
Never/
Almost
Never
%
Sometimes
%
Almost
Every Time/
Every Time
%
I leave the water running in the kitchen when washing
or rinsing dishes
23.6
31.4
45.0
I shower for no more than five minutes each time I
bathe
42.8
26.8
30.4
I turn off the water while brushing my teeth
33.6
23.1
43.3
I avoid watering my lawn in the summer
32.2
39.3
28.5
I allow soapy water to run down a storm drain
44.0
26.4
29.6
I allow oil from cooking to run down the drain
56.2
26.9
16.9
I let my sprinklers run when rain is predicted in the
forecast
69.1
18.6
12.2
I allow used motor oil to run down a storm drain
85.9
3.8
10.3
I hose down my driveway
65.5
27.0
7.5
I let my sprinklers run when it has rained or is raining
79.6
13.4
7.0
8
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
The second series of questions focused on water conservation behaviors. The highest
reported negative water conservation behaviors included Hypocrite respondents not doing the
following: using recycled wastewater/reclaimed water to irrigate lawns (97.2%), using rain
barrels to collect water for use in garden or lawn (96.8%), donating money to a nonprofit to
provide drinking water to another country (90.7%) and having low-water consuming plant
materials in their yard (88.4%). These results are shown in Table 5.
Table 5
Hypocrites Engagement in Water Conservation Behaviors (n = 233)
Yes
%
No
%
I use recycled wastewater/reclaimed water to irrigate my lawn/landscape
2.8
97.2
I use rain barrels to collect water for use in my garden/lawn
3.2
96.8
I have donated money at least once in the past five years to a nonprofit
that works to provide access to drinking water in another country.
9.3
90.7
I have low-water consuming plant materials in my yard
11.6
88.4
I have low-flow shower heads installed in my home
21.0
78.3
I have water-efficient toilets installed in my home.
26.4
73.6
Sources Hypocrites use to get Information about Water Issues
Respondents were asked where they retrieved their information about water. The results from
the Hypocrites are displayed in Table 6. Hypocrites were most likely to obtain their information
about water issues from the Internet, television, or social media.
Table 6
Sources Hypocrites use to get Information about Water Issues (n = 233)
%
Internet
62.6
Television
56.1
Newspaper
45.0
Social Media
43.4
Family and Friends
21.2
Self-Observation
21.8
Radio
16.7
Governmental Website
10.8
Magazine
8.5
Farming Organization
3.8
Attending Events/Activities
1.5
Conclusions and Recommendations
This study sought to identify the Hypocrites who had a high level of knowledge about
climate change but were not practicing water conservation behaviors so their cognitive
dissonance could be addressed with targeted agricultural communication campaigns.
Demographically, the findings revealed Hypocrites were younger, liberal (possibly Democratic)
9
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
females who are highly educated. These results are comparable to similar studies focused on
environmental conservation; the results also suggested that focusing studies specifically on
climate change does not alter the target Hypocrite audience and that future educational initiatives
can be targeted towards this audience (Liu et al., 2014; McCright & Dunlap, 2011; Milfont,
Milojeve, Greaves, & Sibley, 2015).
The results revealed Hypocrites believed climate change was real and caused by humans but
are doing little to curb their personal water use and are not taking personal action to mitigate the
effects of climate change. For example, Hypocrites are likely to allow cooking oil to run down
the drain, a serious water quality issue, and allow the faucet to run while brushing their teeth.
Both of these behaviors are simple to alter.
In addition, a large percentage are also watering their lawn after it rains/if rain is predicted,
which uses three gallons of water per minute (University of Florida IFAS Extension, 2016), are
not reducing their showering time, which equates to 30-75 gallons per 15 minute shower (United
States Geological Survey, 2016) and continue to leave the faucet running while doing dishes, a
possible usage of 8-27 gallons (United States Geological Survey, 2016). Since these behaviors
are those where the largest impact can be made, agricultural communication materials should
focus on trying to alter these targeted behaviors. Based on cognitive dissonance theory
(Festinger, 1957), Hypocrites should want to adjust their behavior to more closely align with
their beliefs so targeted communication efforts to the younger, liberal, more highly educated
population should have the largest effect.
The results also revealed Hypocrites are getting their information about water issues from the
Internet followed by television. Agricultural communicators should consider utilizing targeted
social media campaigns during times of water restriction. Attention is already paid to water
issues and the media buzz can be leveraged to encourage specific behavior engagement. Social
media outlets, such as Facebook, can target campaigns to specific users. Agricultural
communicators should consider utilizing these avenues to send specific messages about
minimizing shower time, shutting off the faucet when brushing teeth and minimizing water use
when washing dishes. They may want to consider partnering with organizations whose websites
or social media outlets are visited by a younger liberal clientele in an effort to reach Hypocrites
specifically. Hypocrites can become Believers if steps are taken to encourage them to rectify
inconsistencies between their beliefs and actions (Martinsson & Lundqvist, 2010).
In order to reduce dissonance, continued exposure targeted at the Hypocrite demographic
audience may encourage understanding of why particular habits are damaging and should be
addressed. Specifically, targeting some of the behaviors that are simple to alter is suggested. For
example, the social media efforts previously described could address the fact that Hypocrites
may not know the damage their behaviors cause. It is important to inform them of the hazards of
pouring cooking oil down the sink such as the possibility of it clogging household pipes and
potentially leading to sewage overflows, resulting in damaged local waterways (Harris County
Water Protection Group, 2016). The oil can also cause disruption at water treatment facilities,
form toxic products that linger in the environment and cause devastating physical effects to
animal and wildlife areas (Environmental Protection Agency, 2016). Preventing Hypocrites (and
others) from pouring oil down the drain will help minimize negative water quality issues. The
other simple behavior to alter is turning off the water while brushing their teeth. This can save up
to four gallons of water per day, potentially 120 gallons per month (Texas A&M Agrilife
Extension, 2013).
10
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
Agricultural communicators could create infographics to be shared online and in print that
stresses the importance of protecting water resources in terms of quality or quantity.
Communicators can help motivate Hypocrites to promote the adoption of better water habits.
Working directly with this target group may also increase researchers’ understanding of why this
specific group’s actions are incongruent with their beliefs.
Water issues related to climate change are happening across the nation and are becoming a
priority for many states. Future research could be conducted to further understand why the public
does not believe in or understand climate change and how to leverage climate change knowledge
to alter behavior. An in-depth analysis of Hypocrites, specifically, may provide additional data
on how to access and initiate change within this audience. A qualitative approach using focus
groups or in-person interviews with Hypocrites in different regions of the U.S. could be used to
further understand if there are regional differences and how Hypocrites want to be
communicated with about climate change and water conservation behavior engagement. If focus
groups were conducted, a pre/post test could be used to determine if the simple act of meeting in
a group with likeminded individuals altered their perceptions regarding their personal water
behaviors. Communication materials, including the social media messages recommended earlier,
could also be tested using experimental designs with Hypocrites to identify which would be best
to utilize broadly to encourage behavior change.
Aitken, C. K., McMahon, T. A., Wearing, A. J., & Finlayson, B. L. (1994). Residential water
use: Predicting and reducing consumption. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 136
158.
Aronson, E., Fried, C. B., & Stone, J. (1991). Overcoming denial and increasing the intention to
use condoms through the induction of hypocrisy. American Journal of Public Health, 81,
1636-1638.
Baker, R., Brick, J. M., Bates, N. A., Battaglia, M., Couper, M. P., Dever, J. A., … &
Tourangeau, R. (2013). Report of the AAPOR task force on non-probability sampling.
American Association for Public Opinion Research.
Bakker, K. (2012). Water security: research challenges and opportunities. Science, 337, 914-915.
Burke, E. J., Brown, S.J., & Christidis, N. (2006). Modeling the recent evolution of global
drought, and projects for the twenty-first century with the Hadley centre climate model. J.
Hydrometeor Journal of Hydrometeorology, 7(5), 1113-1125.
Cook, C., & Bakker, K. (2012). Water security: debating an emerging paradigm. Global
Environmental Change, 22(1), 94-102.
Dickerson, C. A., Thibodeau, R., Aronson, E., & Miller, D. (1992). Using cognitive-dissonance
to encourage water conservation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 22(11), 841854.
Dobson, A. (2007). Environmental citizenship: towards sustainable development. Sustainable
Development, 15: 276285.
Dobson, A., & Valencia Sais, A. (2005). Introduction. Environmental Politics, 14, 157 162.
Donner, S. D., & McDaniels, J. (2013). The influence of national temperature fluctuations on
opinions about climate change in the U.S. since 1990. Climatic Change, 118, 537-550.
Environmental Protection Agency. (2016). Vegetable oils and animal fats. Retrieved from
https://www.epa.gov/emergency-response/vegetable-oils-and-animal-fats
Evans, J. M, Calabria, J., Borisova, T., Boellstorf, D. E., Sochacka, N., Smolen, M. D., Mahler,
R. L., & Risse, L. M. (2015). Effects of local drought condition on public opinions about
water supply and future climate change. Climate Change, 132, 193-207.
11
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
Famiglietti, J. (2016, February). 21st Century global freshwater security: Can it exist and can
scientists communicate the challenges? Paper presented at the 2016 UF Water Institute
Symposium, Gainesville, Florida.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Guy, S., Kashima, Y., Walker, I., & O’Neill, S. (2014). Investigating the effects of knowledge
and ideology on climate change beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(5), 421-
429. DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2039
Gorham, L., Lamm, A. J., & Rumble, J. (2014). The critical target audience: Communicating
water conservation behaviors to critical thinking styles. Journal of Applied Communications,
98(4), 42-55.
Harris County Water Protection Group. (2016). Residents: Request educational materials.
Retrieved from http://www.cleanwaterways.org/residents/educational_materials.asp
Hurlimann, A., Dolnicar, S., & Meyer, P. (2009). Understanding behavior to inform water supply
management in developed nationsA review of literature, conceptual model, and research
agenda. Journal of Environmental Management, 91(1), 47-56.
Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. (2013). The physical science basis. Contribution of
working group I to the fifth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate
change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 33115.
Kalton, G., & Flores-Cervantes, I. (2003). Weighting methods. Journal of Official Statistics,
19(2), 81-97.
Kingsolver, B. (2010). Water is life. National Geographic Water Issue, 217(4), 36-59.
Lamm, K. W., Lamm, A. J., & Carter, H. (2015). Bridging water issue knowledge gaps between
the general public and opinion leaders. Journal of Agricultural Education, 56(3), 146-161.
DOI: 10.5032/jae.2015.03146
Leal, A., Rumble, J., & Lamm, A. J. (2015). Setting the agenda: Exploring Floridian's
perceptions of water quality and quantity issues. Journal of Applied Communications, 99(3),
53-67. Retrieved from
http://journalofappliedcommunications.org/images/stories/issues/2015/jac_v99_n3_article4.p
df
Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. R. (2010) Americans’ knowledge of climate change.
Yale University. New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.
Retrieved from http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/ClimateChangeKnowledge2010.pdf
Lenton, R., & Muller, M. (2009). Integrated water resources management in practice. London:
Dunstan House.
Liu, X., Vedlitz, A., Stoutenborough, J., & Robinson, S. (2015). Scientists’ views and positions
on global warming and climate change: A content analysis of congressional
testimonies.Climatic Change, 131(4), 487-503. DOI:10.1007/s10584-015-1390-6
Liu, Z., Smith, W. J., & Safi, A. (2014). Rancher and farmer perceptions of climate change in
Nevada, USA. Climatic Change, 122, 313-327.
Markowitz, E., & Doppelt, B. (2009). Reducing greenhouse gas emissions through behavioral
change: An assessment of past research on energy use, transportation, and water
consumption. Climate Leadership Initiative Institute for a Sustainable Environment.
Martinsson, J., & Lundqvist, L. (2010). Ecological citizenship: coming out ‘clean’ without
turning ‘green’? Environmental Politics, 19(4), 518-537, DOI:
10.1080/09644016.2010.489709
12
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). Cool dudes: The denial of climate change among
conservative white males in the United States. Global Environmental Change, 21, 1163
1172.
Milfont, T. L., Milojeve, P., Greaves, L. M., & Sibley, C. G. (2015). Socio-structural and
psychological foundations of climate change beliefs. New Zealand Journal of Psychology,
44(1), 17-30.
Pahl-Wostl, C., Gupta, J., & Bhaduri, A. (2016). Water security: A popular but contested
concept. Handbook on Water Security, 1-16.
Patterson, L. (2012). 2012 RBC Canadian water attitudes study. RBC Blue Water Project.
Retrieved from http://www.rbc.com/community-sustainability/environment/rbc- blue-
water/index.html
Pickett, G. M., Kangun, N., & Gorve, S. J. (1993). Is there a general conserving consumer? A
public policy concern. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 12, 234-243.
Priolo, D., Milhabet, I., Codou, O., Fointiat, V., Lebarbenchon, E., & Gabarrot, F. (2016).
Encouraging ecological behavior through induced hypocrisy and inconsistency. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 47, 166-180.
Rubens, L., Gosling, P., Bonaiuto, M., Brisbois, X., & Moch, A. (2015). Being a hypocrite or
committed while I am shopping? A comparison of the impact of two interventions on
environmentally friendly behavior. Environment and Behavior, 4, 3-16.
Spencer, T., & Altman, P. (2010). Climate change, water, and risk: Current water demands are
not sustainable. Natural Resources Defense Council, Washington, DC. Retrieved from
http://www.nrdc.org/globalWarming/watersustainability/files/WaterRisk.pdf.
Texas A&M Agrilife Extension. (2013). Save water at home. Water Education Network.
Retrieved from http://water.tamu.edu/save-water-at-home/
Thogersen, T. (2004). A cognitive dissonance interpretation of consistencies and inconsistencies
in environmentally responsible behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(1), 93-
103.
UNESCO-IHE. (2009). Research themes. Water Security. Retrieved from http://www.unesco-
ihe.org/node/5659
United States Census Bureau. (2010). The white population: 2010. Retrieved from
http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-05.pdf
United States Geological Survey. (2016). Per capita water use: How much water do you use in
your home? Retrieved from https://water.usgs.gov/edu/activity-percapita.php
University of Florida IFAS Extension. (2016). Calculate your water ways. Retrieved from
http://citrus.ifas.ufl.edu/sustainable_liv/calculate.shtml
Warner, L. A., Rumble, J. N., Martin, E., Lamm, A. J., & Cantrell, R. A. (2015). The effect of
strategic message selection on residents’ intent to conserve water in the landscape. Journal of
Agricultural Education, 56(3). 59-74. Doi: 10.5032/jae.2015.04059.
Werndl, C. (2015). On defining climate and climate change. The British Journal for The
Philosophy of Science, 67(2), 337-364.
World Economic Forum. (2011). Water Security: the water-food-energy-climate nexus. Island
Press, Washington, D.C.
13
Taylor et al.: Cognitive Dissonance to Communicate About Water and Climate Change
Published by New Prairie Press, 2017
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Melissa Taylor was a Research Assistant in the UF/IFAS Center for Public Issues Education
when this research was conducted. She is currently a research consultant based in Southern
California.
Alexa Lamm is an associate professor in the Department of Agricultural Education and
Communication at University of Florida.
Lisa Lundy is an associate professor in the Department of Agricultural Education and
Communication at University of Florida.
14
Journal of Applied Communications, Vol. 101 [2017], Iss. 3, Art. 5
http://newprairiepress.org/jac/vol101/iss3/5
DOI: 10.4148/1051-0834.1843
... However, it is important to emphasize that the surveys cited above found that of the individuals that believed in climate change a minority expressed concern for its effects. This represents a cognitive dissonance in the public, by which we mean that an individual can hold two contradictory cognitions simultaneously (Whitmarsh 2011;Taylor, Lamm, and Lundy 2017). This can manifest itself by an individual that says they believe in the science of climate change, yet their behaviors do not match their reported beliefs. ...
... The theoretical foundation for this study suggests that individuals do not exist in a simple binary set of mental states. Rather, when presented with a number of issues related to climate change, an individual will not necessarily answer in a consistent fashion (see stoll-Kleemann and schmidt 2017; Taylor, Lamm, and Lundy 2017;Cohen, Higham, and Cavaliere 2011 for examples). This results in some individuals that uniformly accept all aspects of climate change (i.e. it is human caused and impacts their personal lives), some individuals with a mix of attitudes (i.e. it is human caused, but will not impact their personal lives), and some that deny all aspects (it is not caused by humans, and will not impact their personal lives). ...
Article
Full-text available
Climate change denial is often treated as a binary opinion. However, an individual can express acceptance of climate change, while still denying other aspects of the field such as its causation by humans, impacts, or our ability to mitigate these impacts. Here, we conduct a semester long survey and discourse analysis of a class of first-year undergraduates as they complete a course on climate change to assess changes in their attitudes on climate denial verse acceptance across these more specific factors. Our results suggest that acceptance of the scientific facts about climate change is higher than acceptance of its impacts and solutions. However, acceptance that personal and societal changes can mitigate climate change increased throughout the term. These results can help create course curriculum with more effectively targeted content to assist in shifting perspectives on climate change in young undergraduates.
... It means that as CCK increases, the probability of adopting WC as a measure decreases. For instance, Taylor et al. (2017) found that some farmers demonstrated a high level of CCK but did not engage in positive water conservation behaviors. This might be due to economic reasons as most farmers in Punjab are poor. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Adaptation to climate change (ACC) is imperative to avoid deleterious consequences of climate change in agriculture. However, the uptake of adaptation measures has been slow among farmers because of low adaptive capacity (AC) in developing countries, particularly in Pakistan. Farmers and their supporting institutions have been successful in introducing technological innovations to respond and adapt to environmental challenges. The present study intended to determine the impact of farming technologies, along with human, financial, social, physical, natural, and climate information resources that support AC and hence ACC. Methods The study collected data from 360 farmers in Punjab through a multi-stage random sampling technique. A binary logit model and odds ratio were used to identify the factors affecting ACC. The study also utilized correlation tests to show the correlation between each pair of variables included in the analysis. Results The results indicated that physical capital such as ownership of tube wells, transportation, and sowing and harvesting tools by the farmers builds farmers’ AC and consequently determines the ACC such as change crop variety (CCV), change crop type (CCT), change planting date (CPD), soil conservation (SC), water conservation (WC), and diversification strategies (DSs). The findings also revealed that human capital (age, education, family size, and labor), financial capital (off-farm employment, access to the marketing of produce, and agricultural credit), social capital (farmers-to-farmers extensions, access to extension services, and the farm association membership), and natural capital (land ownership, tenancy status, and the location of the farm) were importantly related to farm households’ ACC strategies. The odds (likelihood) of adaptation were higher for the users of farm technology as compared to non-users. Discussion The analysis conducted in this study showed that climate information resources amplify the adaptation to climate change: technology allows farming to be much more efficient, while climate change knowledge (CCK) self-motivates farmers to adopt more ACC measures. Our findings provide evidence that suggests the need to provide credits and financial support for farming technologies that speed up the ACC in the long run, while in the short run, climate information should be spread among farming communities.
... A person in this situation can resolve the dissonance, and the associated discomfort, by changing their water use practices. In fact, pointing out these discrepancies can be an effective strategy for promoting water conservation (Dickerson et al., 1992;Taylor et al., 2017). But instead of changing their behavior, people can also resolve their dissonance and discomfort by refusing to believe that their behaviors cause environmental degradation (Festinger, 1962). ...
... The other-ignorance variable focused on ignorance about the impact of their decisions on others, which might be a useful way to manage the cognitive dissonance associated with rejecting public flood mapping that might be of benefit to future property buyers. This individualistic approach is again reflected in the literature of climate change adaptation, where individuals have been found to recognize the need for climate adaptation, but refuse to change their own behaviours due to a lack of perceived personal impact (Taylor et al., 2017). Understanding that the dimensions of climax thinking most predictive of resistance are impacted by lack of understanding or awareness of personal adaptative capacity and collective impact suggests a need for public preparedness measures and tools (including flood risk maps highlighting, for instance, environmental justice implications) to inform preparation measures, while also sharing stories of the efficacy of similar tools in other jurisdictions. ...
Article
Flood risk mapping allows for informed decision making regarding personal and community planning. Resistance to flood risk mapping can be driven by potential decline of property values. This paper explores resistance to flood risk mapping through the lens of climax thinking. Climax thinking is a novel theory guiding explorations of resistance to proposed land use changes. The aim of this study was to understand flood experiences, the presence of resistance to flood risk mapping, and whether climax thinking could help explain this resistance. To address this, surveys were administered to residents in the Nova Scotian towns of Liverpool and Bridgewater. We found that one third of respondents have experienced flooding, yet the majority have not seen a flood risk map, nor were they concerned about the potential impacts of flooding. Only one sixth of respondents exhibited resistance to flood risk mapping because of potential loss to property value. Dimensions of climax thinking were predictive of this resistance, specifically ignorance of an individual's own ability to adapt and inability to recognize the impact of their adaptation decisions on others, which together quadrupled the predictive power of the ordinal regression model. These insights can be applied to support the acceptance of flood risk mapping
... One potential method is the 'hypocrisy paradigm' that seeks to induce feelings of hypocrisy and associated negative emotions [69]. Dickerson et al. [70] and Taylor et al. [71], for example, highlights the potential to exploit cognitive dissonance to encourage water conservation by making people feel hypocritical where their underlying beliefs are inconsistent with their behaviours. Nevertheless, we found that most participants acknowledged that they did not proactively think about the connection between their health and their well, or conciously think about the connection between flooding and their supply. ...
Article
Full-text available
Flooding events can inflict major disruption on society and cause significant infrastructural and environmental damage. However, the adverse health impacts of flooding, particularly as they pertain to private groundwater resources used for consumption, are frequently overlooked. Whilst the literature has previously found a lack of well stewardship among private well owners under ‘normal’ conditions, our understanding of private well owners’ perceptions of and preparedness for the risks posed by flooding to their domestic well-water supply is limited. This study advances the qualitative literature on this subject. It is amongst the first qualitative studies employing focus groups to examine private well owners, and the first in an Irish context. Six focus groups were conducted in four counties in Ireland, with the themes emerging from the focus groups refined, organised, and interpreted in the context of the Health Belief Model. Most focus group participants expressed awareness of the potential severity of well contamination following flooding, but many did not consider their local area “at risk” of it, notwithstanding the occurrence of previous local flooding events. All focus group participants shared the view that owners were primarily responsible for their own wells. However, their capacity to undertake appropriate actions was reduced by reliance on visual and olfactory evidence to assess water quality, and concerns regarding the financial cost and accessibility of water testing facilities. The phenomenon of misperception was also evident among participants. In light of the increasing frequency and severity of extreme weather events including flooding, these findings provide direction for future socio-hydrogeological interventions. Targeted communication strategies highlighting the risks posed by flooding, mitigation measures that promote well stewardship, and protective behaviours are required. The provision of access to free well water testing would also promote protective actions.
... In the EB literature, cognitive dissonance has been explored in various contexts, including climate change, water conservation, sustainable tourism and sustainable consumption (Dolnicar et al., 2017;Gadeikien_ e et al., 2019;Taylor et al., 2017). In a recent study, the authors reported certain levels of cognitive dissonance among sustainability scientists and their flight behaviors (Schrems and Upham, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This study aims to examine the organizational citizenship behaviors toward the environment among US-based faculty members, in social sciences, natural sciences, the humanities and engineering. Potential barriers that may stop academia from being more sustainable are examined but also the opportunities for academics to be involved in environmental sustainability are explored. Design/methodology/approach The authors followed a sequential explanatory mixed methods approach using online surveys (n = 633) followed by open-ended phone interviews (n = 28). Findings Results suggest that there are certain power dynamics that force young faculty members in anti-environmental behaviors, although they are cognizant of the negative impacts on the environment. Many faculty members engage in environmental-related actions outside their service requirements, but this is mostly the case for tenured faculty members. Originality/value The originality of this study lies in the fact that it explores environmental behaviors from an academic ranking perspective and expands on pertinent barriers to promote sustainable behaviors in academia.
... Generally, management by values attempts to identify the gap between what you say you believe and what you do practically. Festinger (1957) introduced the concept of cognitive dissonance in theory for the first time by publishing a book entitled "theory of cognitive dissonance" (To get acquainted with the examples of employing cognitive dissonance for conservation purposes, see Taylor et al. 2017;Kantola et al. 1984;Dickerson et al. 1992). According to Fig. 4 It is possible to attribute geotourism attractions to the two concepts of "intrinsic value" and "instrumental value" Festinger (1957), the basis of this theory is the belief that human always attempts to maintain internal harmony and integrity between values, attitudes, and beliefs. ...
Article
Water safety refers to the quality of one's drinking water and whether it lacks dangerous contaminants. Limited access to safe water is projected to impact approximately 5 billion people worldwide by 2050. Climate change and worsening severe weather events pose increasing threats to global water safety. However, people may not perceive links between climate change and water safety, potentially undermining their willingness to implement behaviors that improve water safety. Existing studies on water safety risk perceptions have mostly been conducted in single-country contexts, which limits researchers' ability to make cross-national comparisons. Here, we assessed the extent to which people's severe weather concern and climate change concern predict their water safety concern. Our analyses used survey data from the 142-country 2019 Lloyd's Register Foundation World Risk Poll, including 21 low-income and 34 lower-middle-income countries. In mixed-effects models, severe weather concern was significantly more predictive of water safety concern than was climate change concern, although both resulted in positive associations. Worldwide, this finding was robust, insensitive to key model specifications and countries' varying protection against unsafe drinking water. We suggest communicators and policymakers improve messaging about water safety and other environmental threats by explaining how they are impacted by worsening severe weather.
Article
The ambitious carbon-reduction targets set out by the Paris Agreement, or by recent Green Deals such as the one launched by European Commission President Ursula von der Leyen, will require a high degree of engagement with the public. This is true not only as important changes in lifestyle are going to be needed from citizens, especially in developed economies, but also because top-down measures will need public approval to be put in place. This paper reviews literature from behavioural and environmental economics, cognitive sciences, (social) psychology, health policy, and marketing to condense key insights on how to make communication on the green transition more effective in terms of citizens’ engagement. In doing so, it distils six policy recommendations that can serve as building blocks for an impactful narrative accompanying decarbonisation strategies, in Europe and beyond. If used skilfully, an effective communication and consequent behavioural change holds the promise of complementing top-down financial and regulatory tools, accelerating the green transition.
Article
Full-text available
Remembering one's past transgressions of a social norm is known as an effective paradigm for enhancing pro-social and ecological behaviours. Our study aimed to show that reminding norm transgressions can arise cognitive dissonance and can lead to behavioural change as induced hypocrisy does. In particular, we tested whether inconsistency between the self-concept and the remembered past transgressions is or is not likely to encourage behavioural change. To reach this goal, we conducted an experiment comparing induced hypocrisy, injunctive inconsistency and descriptive inconsistency with five comparison conditions. The results showed that, as observed with the induced hypocrisy paradigm, presenting a salient injunctive norm and its past transgressions enhances psychological discomfort, actual donation and donation amounts for an ecological association. The discussion addresses applied perspectives and theoretical implications.
Article
Full-text available
Water quantity and quality are among the top issues currently facing Florida. To understand residents' perceptions of these issues as well as understand how agenda-setting may be used to influence residents' behaviors and opinions surrounding water issues, this study explored Florida residents' opinions of water. Agenda-setting served as the conceptual framework to aid in understanding where water quality and quantity emerge on the public's agenda. Responses were obtained from 469 Florida residents via an online survey. The results showed respondents believed water quality had not changed, with the exception of the quality of bays, which they believed was getting worse. Water quality was found to be an issue of high importance among respondents, especially in regard to the quality of drinking water. Respondents believed water quantity was highly important; however, more importance was associated with water quality issues. The results of this study identified the current disconnect that exists among residents concerning water issues. This study also established the salience of water issues on the public's agenda and how Florida residents could be better informed. A statewide communication campaign focused on both water quality and quantity issues was recommended to decrease the disconnect that currently exists between residents' perceptions and the reality of water issues. This campaign should utilize the technology-based outlets to stay informed with the public's agenda to personalize communication efforts. These efforts would increase the public's interest concerning water issues by reducing redundant information and diluting important issues.
Article
Full-text available
Although water covers approximately 70% of the planet, only a fraction is fresh water, and even less is used as a major source of drinking water. With the continuous increase in the amount of water used in modern standards of living, the quantity of water available is decreasing. The public is beginning to understand water needs to be conserved and they must play a role in water conservation. While previous literature examined how the majority of messages were catered toward the cost-effectiveness of conserving water, this study proposed how using a specific audience attribute could affect behaviors. The purpose of the study was to determine if critical thinking style can be used in the development of future communication strategies to improve water conservation behaviors. The findings of this study provided evidence of a relationship between critical thinking style and the level of engagement in water conservation behaviors. Recommendations suggested targeting the two constructs of critical thinking style, information seekers and engagers, in two different ways. Since the seekers prefer to gather information by seeking the sources themselves, communicators should focus on developing quality information about water conservation and placing it in easily accessible communications channels for the information seeker. On the other hand, a different communications approach should be taken with the engagers, who prefer to learn through their environment. Communicators should focus on communicating to the engager through the environment in word-of-mouth situations using traditional means such as opinion leaders as well as social media.
Article
Full-text available
Changing individuals' behaviors is a critical challenge for Extension professionals who encourage good irrigation practices and technologies for landscape water conservation. Multiple messages were used to influence two predictors of behavioral intent informed by the theory of planned behavior, Florida residents' (N = 1,063) attitude and perceived behavioral control. Individuals were randomly assigned to one of four treatment groups, each receiving a different strategically framed message. This article contributes to the literature on landscape water consumption behaviors by (a) demonstrating that messages can be used to positively affect both attitude and perceived behavioral control and (b) identifying specific message frames that may be used to realize greater impact. Two gain-framed message treatments both significantly increased participants' attitude and perceived behavioral control, and one of the loss-framed messages significantly increased participants' attitude. Results are discussed with practical application to promotion of landscape water-conservation behaviors and implications for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Global conflicts have rapidly made water the most contentious issue in the world today. Considering water drives health, industry, recreation, and the agricultural food system it is no surprise that it has become such a hot topic. As a result, the general public has an increased interest in water-focused policy; policy that can have a large impact on the agriculture industry. Agricultural and natural resource opinion leaders may differ or agree with the general public on water related issues, as well as perceptions of government influence. Identifying similarities and differences will allow agricultural educators to identify ways to resolve disagreements through improved education, communication and messaging, and leadership development of agricultural and natural resource opinion leaders. The findings indicated there are significant differences between agricultural and natural resource opinion leaders and the general public as it related to their knowledge of water issues and perceptions of government support for individual decision-making regarding environmental issues. Based on these findings it is suggested agricultural educators establish a common language that can be used with all audiences to discuss water issues, develop educational programming to assist opinion leaders when addressing observed knowledge gaps, and create educational coalitions to provide source credibility across audiences.
Article
Full-text available
Using a national probability sample of over 6,000 New Zealanders, this study examines socio-structural and psychological variables underpinning core climate change beliefs— " climate change is real " and " climate change is caused by humans ". Analyses focused on four belief profiles: those who believe in the reality of climate change and its human cause (53%), those undecided (30%), the complete skeptics (10%) and those who believe the climate is changing but is not caused by human activity (7%). Results support and extend a " conservative white male " effect in doubts concerning the science of human-caused climate change. Uniformly high beliefs in climate change reality and human cause was observed among respondents who were younger, female, educated, politically liberal, belonged to minority groups and who perceived that they were able to influence environmental outcomes. Belief in climate change was also stronger for those who endorse altruistic and openness values and who were high in personality trait levels of Agreeableness and Openness to Experience. Theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Among many potential causes for policymakers’ contention over whether there is a largely unified scientific agreement on global warming and climate change (GWCC), one possible factor, according to the information deficit theory, is that the scientists who testified in congressional hearings might be substantially divided in their views and positions associated with GWCC. To clarify this, we perform content analysis of 1350 testimonies from congressional GWCC hearings over a period of 39 years from 1969 to 2007 and use the data derived from this content analysis to provide an overview of scientist witnesses’ stances on GWCC. The key findings include: (1) among the scientists’ testimonies with an expressed view on whether GWCC is real, a vast majority (86 %) indicates that it is happening; (2) among the scientists’ testimonies with an identified stance on whether GWCC is anthropogenic, a great majority of them (78 %) indicates that GWCC is caused, at least to some degree, by human activity; (3) even under Republican controlled congresses, there is still a supermajority (75 %) - among the scientists’ testimonies with an expressed position on GWCC existence or GWCC cause - that believes that GWCC is real and that GWCC is anthropogenic; (4) most scientists’ testimonies (95 %) endorse pro-action policy to combat GWCC; and (5) the percentages of scientists’ views and positions are consistent across different types of scientist testimony groups. Our findings suggest that the scientific information transmitted to Congress is not substantially different from the general agreement in the climate science community.
Article
Full-text available
A growing body of research indicates that opinions about long-term climate change and other natural resource issues can be significantly affected by current weather conditions (e.g., outside air temperature) and other highly contingent environmental cues. Although increased severity and frequency of droughts is regarded as a likely consequence of anthropogenic climate change, little previous research has attempted to relate the experience of drought with public attitudes about water supply or water-related climate change issues. For this study, a large set (n = 3,163) of public survey data collected across nine states of the southern United States was spatio-temporally linked with records of short-term (~12 weeks) and long-term (~5 years) drought condition at the level of each respondent’s zip code. Multivariate ordinal logistic regression models that included numerous other independent variables (environmental ideology, age, gender, education, community size, residency duration, and local annual precipitation) indicated highly significant interactions with long-term drought condition, but showed no significant effect from short-term drought condition. Conversely, attitudes about water-related climate change showed highly significant interactions with short-term drought, with weaker to no effects from long-term drought. While the finding of significant effects from short-term drought condition on opinions about future drought is broadly consistent with previous public opinion research on climate change, the finding of water supply attitudes being more responsive to longer term drought condition is, to our knowledge, a novel result. This study more generally demonstrates the methodological feasibility and applied importance of accounting for local drought condition when public opinion information is used to evaluate outreach programs for water conservation and climate change.
Article
The authors conducted a survey to identify conservation activity among the general populace of a midsize Southwestern community to increase our understanding of those who do and do not engage in a broad section of environmentally friendly activities. Numerous self-reported behaviors were gauged and combined to form a composite measure representing the conserving consumer. They then explored both demographic and psychosocial variables as predictors of this self-reported composite scale of conservation. The results and their implications are discussed for researchers and public policy officials.