ResearchPDF Available

Employment Rates for 11 Country of Origin Groups in Scandinavia

Authors:
  • Ulster Institute for Social Research

Abstract and Figures

Employment rates for 11 country of origin groups living in the three Scandinavian countries are presented. Analysis of variance showed that differences in employment rates are highly predictable (adjusted multiple R = .93). This predictability was mostly due to origin countries (eta = .89), not sex (eta = .25) and host country (eta = .20). Furthermore, national IQs of the origin countries predicted employment rates well across all host countries (r's = 0.74 [95%CI: 0.30, 0.92], 0.75 [0.30, 0.92], 0.66 [0.14, 0.89] for Denmark, Norway and Sweden, respectively), and so did Muslim % of the origin countries (r's =-0.80 [-0.94,-0.43],-0.78 [-0.94,-0.37],-0.58 [-0.87,-0.01]).
Content may be subject to copyright.
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2 312-323
312
Employment Rates for 11 Country of Origin Groups in
Scandinavia
Emil O. W. Kirkegaard*
Ulster Institute for Social Research, London, UK
*Email: emil@emilkirkegaard.dk
Employment rates for 11 country of origin groups living in the
three Scandinavian countries are presented. Analysis of variance
showed that differences in employment rates are highly predictable
(adjusted multiple R = .93). This predictability was mostly due to
origin countries (eta = .89), not sex (eta = .25) and host country
(eta = .20). Furthermore, national IQs of the origin countries
predicted employment rates well across all host countries (r’s = 0.74
[95%CI: 0.30, 0.92], 0.75 [0.30, 0.92], 0.66 [0.14, 0.89] for Denmark,
Norway and Sweden, respectively), and so did Muslim % of the
origin countries (r’s = -0.80 [-0.94, -0.43], -0.78 [-0.94, -0.37], -0.58 [-
0.87, -0.01]).
Key Words: Immigration, Employment, Scandinavia, Denmark,
Norway, Sweden, Country of origin, National IQ, Islam, Muslim
Numerous studies have reported very large differences in social status and
related outcomes for different country of origin groups in Western countries
(Fuerst, 2012; Fuerst & Kirkegaard, 2014; Jones & Schneider, 2010; Kirkegaard,
2014a, 2015; Kirkegaard & Becker, 2017; Kirkegaard & Fuerst, 2014). Detailed
data has previously been reported for Denmark and Norway (Kirkegaard, 2014a;
Kirkegaard & Fuerst, 2014). However, no data has yet been published (in English)
for Sweden to the author’s knowledge. Recently, a new edition of the annual
publication Indvandrere i Danmark (Immigrants in Denmark) (Danmarks Statistik,
2016) was published. Each edition of the series has a particular focus, and the
focus of the 2016 report is comparative analysis of immigrant outcomes in the
Scandinavian countries. The purpose of this paper is to present the main data in
English as well as some analyses of them.
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
313
Data and analyses
Danmarks Statistik (the Danish statistics bureau) collaborated with their
Swedish and Norwegian analogues (Statistiska centralbyrån and Statistisk
sentralbyrå), to compile matching data for employment rates for 2014. The data
concern only foreign born (1st generation) immigrants aged 20-64, and cover a
diverse set of 11 countries which they deemed to have sufficient numbers for
comparison purposes.1 The data only cover persons with residence and thus
work permits, not illegal immigrants. The employment rates are shown in Figure
1.2
Figure 1. Employment rates by country of origin and host country. The thick
blocks for the Scandinavian countries are for natives in their own countries.
Employment rates were not available for Scandinavians living in other
Scandinavian countries.
1 I asked DST if they could provide results for additional countries, which they
declined.
2 The numbers were given by sex, but they have been averaged here for
simplicity of presentation. The full data are given in the supplementary
materials.
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2
314
The employment rates are similar across host countries with a mean
correlation of .86. The rate for Somalis in Denmark is lower than those reported
in Kirkegaard and Fuerst (2014). This probably has to do with the fact that the
present numbers only concern foreign born persons, while the previously reported
numbers concerned all persons from Somalia, including later generations who
have higher employment rates.
Analysis 1 ― Analysis of variance
Two regressions were run to see how host, origin and sex predicted the
employment rate. The first model (n = 36) did not include sex and had an adjusted
(multiple) R of .93 (R2 = 87%).3 Analysis of variance showed that origin was the
most important predictor with an eta of .88 while host had an eta of .05. In the
second model (n = 72), sex-disaggregated data were used and sex was added
as a predictor. This model had an adjusted R of .93 (R2 = 86%). Analysis of
variance etas were: origin .89, host .20 and sex .25. Thus, origin was still by far
the best predictor, and the more complex model with disaggregated data was not
superior to the simpler model. However, because sex itself was non-redundant
and because it was based on more data points, the betas for the complex model
are reported in Table 1. The output from the first model can be found in the
supplementary materials.
The beta for each origin country can be regarded as an estimate of that
country’s human capital taken as composite variable consisting of cognitive,
personality, interest, temperament, knowledge and skills (Jones & Potrafke,
2014). Such estimates will only be precise under strict assumptions, which are
that migrants are equally representative of their country of origin with respect to
human capital (no differences in migrant selectivity), and lack of host x origin
interactions (equal opportunity for each immigrant group).
The host country predictor can be interpreted in multiple ways. One might
regard it as a measure of integrational success. This could be, but then it should
not affect the natives. Figure 1 shows that native Danes have lower employment
rates than native Swedes and Norwegians. The employment rate of Danes in
Denmark cannot easily be ascribed to integration efforts. For that reason, the host
effects are more parsimoniously explained by system-wide effects such as slightly
different ways of counting employment across countries or generosity of
unemployment benefits.
3 The design was nearly but not fully crossed because there was no cross-
Scandinavian data (e.g. no data for Norwegians in Denmark).
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
315
Table 1. Regression standardized betas. Outcome: employment rate. N = 72.
Predictor
Beta
CI lower
CI upper
Origin: Afghanistan
0.00
Origin: Bosnia & Herzogovina
1.14
0.71
1.57
Origin: China
0.73
0.29
1.16
Origin: Denmark
1.99
1.37
2.62
Origin: Germany
1.40
0.97
1.83
Origin: Iran
0.69
0.26
1.12
Origin: Iraq
-0.05
-0.49
0.38
Origin: Norway
1.76
1.14
2.38
Origin: Poland
1.48
1.05
1.91
Origin: Somalia
-0.74
-1.17
-0.31
Origin: Sweden
2.10
1.47
2.72
Origin: Syria
-1.11
-1.54
-0.68
Origin: Thailand
1.12
0.68
1.55
Origin: Turkey
0.63
0.20
1.07
Host: Denmark
0.00
Host: Norway
0.50
0.28
0.73
Host: Sweden
0.25
0.03
0.48
Sex: men
0.00
Sex: women
-0.50
-0.68
-0.32
An alternative analytic approach here is to recode employment rates as
fractions of the native employment rate in that country. This is based on the
assumption that human capital of the natives is equal, which seems a fair
assumption for the Scandinavian countries. Table 2 shows the modeling results
from the recoded data.
The model fit using relative rates had similar fit to the original data: adjusted
R = .92 vs. original coding .93. Using relative rates coding, Norway still did seem
to have better immigrant outcomes, which could be interpreted as better
integrational success or stronger immigrant selection across all origin countries.
Still, the effect size was quite small, etas: origin = .91, host = .14, and sex = .20.
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2
316
Table 2. Regression standardized betas with relative rate of natives coding.
Outcome: employment rate. N = 72.
Predictor Beta SE CI lower CI upper
Origin: Afghanistan 0
Origin: Bosnia & Herzogovina 1.16 0.22 0.71 1.61
Origin: China 0.75 0.22 0.30 1.20
Origin: Denmark 2.11 0.33 1.46 2.76
Origin: Germany 1.43 0.22 0.98 1.88
Origin: Iran 0.70 0.22 0.25 1.15
Origin: Iraq -0.06 0.22 -0.51 0.39
Origin: Norway 1.78 0.33 1.13 2.43
Origin: Poland 1.51 0.22 1.06 1.96
Origin: Somalia -0.76 0.22 -1.21 -0.30
Origin: Sweden 2.08 0.33 1.43 2.73
Origin: Syria -1.13 0.22 -1.58 -0.68
Origin: Thailand 1.15 0.22 0.70 1.61
Origin: Turkey 064 0.22 0.19 1.09
Host: Denmark 0
Host: Norway 0.33 0.12 0.10 0.57
Host: Sweden 0.03 0.12 -0.20 0.27
Sex: men 0
Sex: women -0.39 0.09 0.58 -0.21
Analysis 2 ― Analysis of variance with PISA data
Dronkers et al. (2014) also analyzed origin effects on immigrant outcomes.
They did not use analysis of variance so their results are not directly comparable
to those reported in this study. However, they give the PISA 2006 sciences scores
in a table for each host x origin cell with available data (immigrants only). To
expand the dataset and make it more comparable to the present study’s, the
scores for the native populations were added. Unfortunately, the PISA 2006
reports do not contain scores for natives and immigrants separately as the later
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
317
reports do, so the total country scores were used as a proxy. For European
countries, this results in a slight underestimate of the scores in most cases
because immigrants on average perform worse than natives in most European
countries (Rindermann & Thompson, 2016). These data were then analyzed
using the same method as before, with the PISA scores as the outcome and the
origin country as the predictor (n = 147, 72 origin countries, 58 host countries).
The adjusted R was .89 (R2 = 78%). Analysis of variance showed that the most
important predictor was origin country, eta = .64, while host country also had
sizable validity, eta = .33. Interpretation of these results is complicated by the fact
that there were many missing cells: 4,176 host x origin combinations, but only
147 were present, or 3.5%. These cells are unlikely to be randomly missing and
will thus likely cause misestimation of betas, especially because immigrant
selection effects will be partially included in the host term. In contrast, for the
employment data, there were 42 host x origin combinations of which 36, or 86%,
were present. The results nevertheless illustrate that country of origin is more
important than host country for two measures of human capital: employment
rates, and PISA scores.
Analysis 3 ― Country of origin characteristics regression
As in previous studies (cited in the introduction), the outcome (employment
rates) was correlated with national IQ (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2012) and Muslim%
(Pew Research Center, 2011) of the origin countries. National IQs reflect the
populations’ average levels of cognitive ability. Cognitive ability has been found
to positively predict almost all socially valued outcomes, including employment
rates, at the individual-differences level (Fergusson, John Horwood & Ridder,
2005; Gottfredson, 1997; Herrnstein & Murray, 1994; Lynn, Hampson & Magee,
1984). This is also true at the aggregate level, in that groups with higher cognitive
ability have better outcomes (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994; Kirkegaard & Fuerst,
2016), and countries or areas within a country with lower average cognitive ability
have worse outcomes (Fuerst & Kirkegaard, 2016; Jones, 2016; Jones &
Potrafke, 2014; Kirkegaard, 2014b; Kirkegaard & Fuerst, 2017; Lynn, Cheng &
Grigoriev, 2017; Lynn & Vanhanen, 2012). Less work has been done on Islamic
beliefs and Muslims at the individual level (but see Dronkers, van der Velden &
Dunne, 2011), however, a number of previous studies showed that this is a strong
negative predictor of immigrant group-level outcomes (see for review Kirkegaard,
2017), and probably has incremental validity over national IQ (Kirkegaard &
Fuerst, 2014), though this has not yet been thoroughly examined.
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2
318
Figure 2. Scatterplot of national IQs in the home country and employment rates
in Scandinavian countries.
Figure 3. Scatterplot of Muslim% in the home country and employment rates in
Scandinavian countries.
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
319
Figures 2 and 3 show the correlations across all three countries and for both
predictors. Both plots show strong relationships between the country-of-origin
predictors and the outcome that are similar across host countries. Table 3 gives
the numerical relationships. Correlations were similar in size, but somewhat
weaker for Sweden and for women. Not much can be made of this because of
the small sample sizes.
Table 3. Correlations between predictors and employment rates by sex.
Numbers in brackets are 95% analytic confidence intervals.
Country
Employment rate
IQ
Muslim%
Denmark
Men
0.71 [0.24 0.91]
-0.73 [-0.92 – -0.28]
Women
0.77 [0.35 0.93]
-0.86 [-0.96 – -0.55]
Total
0.74 [0.30 0.92]
-0.80 [-0.94 – -0.43]
Norway
Men
0.67 [0.15 0.90]
-0.69 [-0.90 – -0.18]
Women
0.82 [0.46 0.95]
-0.85 [-0.96 – -0.53]
Total
0.75 [0.30 0.92]
-0.78 [-0.94 – -0.37]
Sweden
Men
0.59 [0.03 0.87]
-0.50 [-0.83 0.10]
Women
0.72 [0.24 0.91]
-0.67 [-0.90 – -0.15]
Total
0.66 [0.14 0.89]
-0.58 [-0.87 – -0.01]
Finally, the country of origin human capital estimates from the analysis of
variance (Analysis 1 and 2) were correlated with the predictors. For the
employment data, national IQ and Muslim% correlations were .78 [.43 to .93, n =
14]4 and -.81 [-.94 to -.48, n = 14], and for the PISA data, they were .62 [.45 to
.74, n = 71] and -.35 [-.59 to -.05, n = 71]. The PISA results were weaker. This is
probably in part because of oddities in the data presumably related to sampling
error. For instance, Denmark’s beta was estimated at .57, which is lower than
expected. If one traces the origin of the result, it is found to be based on only two
datapoints. Danes in Denmark who obtained a score of 494 and Danes in Norway
who obtained the implausibly low PISA score of 411 (compare: Romania 418,
Mexico 410, Indonesia 393). It seems likely that if a comprehensive PISA dataset
were constructed and analyzed in the same way, many of these irregularities
would vanish (for a comprehensive, though different, analysis, see Rindermann
& Thompson, 2016).
4 The increase to n = 14 from n = 11 is due to the addition of the values from the
three Scandinavian countries.
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2
320
Analysis 4 Sex interaction
During review, it was noted that the sex difference in employment rates is
higher in lower IQ and higher Muslim% countries, while the effect of sex was
modeled only as a constant (main) effect. To investigate this more formally, the
data were recoded to be relative rates of men’s employment rate from that
country.5 After this, the data were analyzed as before using analysis of variance
except with the addition of a sex x origin term. This model had an adjusted R of
.94 vs. .92 for the same model without the interaction term but with relative rates.
Thus, the effect size was quite small, even if it was perhaps not a fluke (p = .038).6
Discussion and conclusion
The present study found that a socially important outcome, employment
rates, is predicted quite well by average IQ and Muslim% in the country of origin.
This finding is congruent with nearly all previous studies on immigrant groups
(Jones & Schneider, 2010; Kirkegaard, 2014a, 2015; Kirkegaard & Becker, 2017;
Kirkegaard & Fuerst, 2014). In terms of comparing host and origin country
predictive validity, origin was found to dominate effects from the host countries
quite strongly. This conclusion is similar to those reached by other researchers
who have examined e.g. PISA data (Dronkers et al., 2014; Rindermann &
Thompson, 2016). When data from PISA 2006 were examined using the same
methods as those used elsewhere in this study, they were fairly similar but with
somewhat stronger host country effects. Interpretation of this is not easy due to
very large amounts of probably non-randomly missing data in the PISA dataset
and well as substantial sampling error.
In general, the relative importance of the predictors in analysis of variance
as quantified by etas should be interpreted with caution. Etas are non-negative
metrics7 and thus sampling error necessitates validities above zero for any
5 For example, if the male employment rate for a given origin country was 70%
and women’s was 60%, the relative rate would be .86.
6 Using the original data instead of the relativized data gave near identical results:
adjusted R .94 vs. .93, for model with and without interaction term, respectively
(p = .041).
7 Eta is based on eta2, which are variance-type metrics (0-1 range). Because
variances are based on squared deviations from the mean, they cannot be
negative, and the derived eta values likewise cannot. I report eta instead of the
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
321
predictor with zero true validity. This is especially important for small sample
models such as those reported in this study.
Supplementary material
Supplementary materials including code, high quality figures and data can
be found at https://osf.io/4c62n/files/.
References
Danmarks Statistik (2016). Indvandrere i Danmark 2016. Retrieved from
http://www.dst.dk/da/Statistik/Publikationer/VisPub?cid=20704
Dronkers, J., van der Velden, R. & Dunne, A. (2011). The effects of educational systems,
school-composition, track-level, parental background and immigrants’ origin on the
achievement of 15-years-old native and immigrant students: A reanalysis of PISA 2006.
ROA Research Memorandum.
Dronkers, J., Levels, M. & de Heus, M. (2014). Migrant pupils’ scientific performance: The
influence of educational system features of origin and destination countries. Large-Scale
Assessments in Education 2(1): 3. https://doi.org/10.1186/2196-0739-2-3
Fergusson, D.M., John Horwood, L. & Ridder, E.M. (2005). Show me the child at seven
II: Childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 46(8): 850-858.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x
Fuerst, J. (2012). L&V’s (2012) National IQs predict 2011-2012 GRE scores for 114
citizenship groups, 2010 + 2012 TOEFL scores for 157 citizenship groups, PISA scores
of migrants from 62 nations of origin across 17 destination nations, .... Retrieved January
3, 2016, from http://humanvarieties.org/2014/02/11/lvs-2012-national-iqs-predict-2011-
2012-gre-scores-for-114-citizenship-groups-2010-2012-toefl-scores-for-157-citizenship-
groups-pisa-scores-of-migrants-from-62-nations-of-origin-across-1/
Fuerst, J. & Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2014). Do national IQs predict U.S. immigrant cognitive
ability and outcomes? An analysis of the National Longitudinal Survey of Freshmen. Open
Differential Psychology. Retrieved from https://openpsych.net/paper/20
Fuerst, J. & Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2016). Admixture in the Americas: Regional and national
differences. Mankind Quarterly 56: 256-374.
more common eta² for the reasons discussed by, among others, Hunter and
Schmidt (2004, p. 189ff).
MANKIND QUARTERLY 2017 58:2
322
Gottfredson, L.S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24:
79-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
Herrnstein, R.J. & Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in
American Life. New York: Free Press.
Hunter, J.E. & Schmidt, F.L. (2004). Methods of Meta-Analysis. Correcting Error and Bias
in Research Findings. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Retrieved from
http://site.ebrary.com/id/10387875
Jones, G. (2016). Hive Mind: How Your Nation’s IQ Matters so Much More than Your
Own. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.
Jones, G. & Potrafke, N. (2014). Human capital and national institutional quality: Are
TIMSS, PISA, and national average IQ robust predictors? Intelligence 46: 148-155.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.011
Jones, G. & Schneider, W.J. (2010). IQ in the production function: Evidence from
immigrant earnings. Economic Inquiry 48: 743-755. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-
7295.2008.00206.x
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2014a). Crime, income, educational attainment and employment
among immigrant groups in Norway and Finland. Open Differential Psychology. Retrieved
from http://openpsych.net/ODP/2014/10/crime-income-educational-attainment-and-
employment-among-immigrant-groups-in-norway-and-finland/
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2014b). The international general socioeconomic factor: Factor
analyzing international rankings. Open Differential Psychology. Retrieved from
http://openpsych.net/ODP/2014/09/the-international-general-socioeconomic-factor-
factor-analyzing-international-rankings/
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2015). Crime among Dutch immigrant groups is predictable from
country-level variables. Open Differential Psychology.
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. (2017, May). Differential immigrant group performance: A matter of
intelligence? Presented at the London Conference on Intelligence 2017. Retrieved from
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=jMSRZQ1usDc
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. & Becker, D. (2017). Immigrant crime in Germany 2012-2015. Open
Quantitative Sociology & Political Science 1(1). Retrieved from https://openpsych.net/
paper/50
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. & Fuerst, J. (2014). Educational attainment, income, use of social
benefits, crime rate and the general socioeconomic factor among 70 immigrant groups in
Denmark. Open Differential Psychology. Retrieved from https://openpsych.net/paper/21
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. & Fuerst, J. (2016). Inequality in the US: Ethnicity, racial admixture
and environmental causes. Mankind Quarterly 56: 580-606.
KIRKEGAARD, E.O.W. EMPLOYMENT RATES IN SCANDINAVIA
323
Kirkegaard, E.O.W. & Fuerst, J. (2017). Admixture in Argentina. Mankind Quarterly 57:
542-580. Retrieved from http://mankindquarterly.org/archive/issue/57-4/4
Lynn, R., Cheng, H. & Grigoriev, A. (2017). Differences in the intelligence of 15 year olds
in 42 provinces and cities of the Russian Federation and their economic, social and
geographical correlates. Mankind Quarterly 57: 659-668. Retrieved from
http://mankindquarterly.org/archive/issue/57-4/9
Lynn, R., Hampson, S. & Magee, M. (1984). Home background, intelligence, personality
and education as predictors of unemployment in young people. Personality and Individual
Differences 5: 549-557. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(84)90029-1
Lynn, R. & Vanhanen, T. (2012). Intelligence: A Unifying Construct for the Social
Sciences. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.
Pew Research Center (2011, January 27). Table: Muslim Population by Country.
Retrieved from http://www.pewforum.org/2011/01/27/table-muslim-population-by-
country/
Rindermann, H. & Thompson, J. (2016). The cognitive competences of immigrant and
native students across the world: An analysis of gaps, possible causes and impact.
Journal of Biosocial Science 48: 66-93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021932014000480
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
This study reports data for the intelligence of 15 year olds in 42 provinces and cities of the Russian Federation assessed in the 2015 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) and their economic, social and geographical correlates. It was found that the average PISA scores of the provinces and cities were significantly correlated with the scores on the Unified State Examination in 2014 (r =.53, p<.001) reported by Grigoriev et al. (2016), with literacy rates in 1897 (r =.50, p<.01), with the percentage of ethnic Russians in the population, and with latitude and longitude showing that PISA scores were higher in the more northerly and westerly provinces.
Article
Full-text available
Analyses of the relationships between cognitive ability, socioeconomic outcomes, and European ancestry were carried out at multiple levels in Argentina: individual (max. n = 5,920), district (n = 437), municipal (n = 299), and provincial (n = 24). Socioeconomic outcomes correlated in expected ways such that there was a general socioeconomic factor (S factor). The structure of this factor replicated across four levels of analysis, with a mean congruence coefficient of .96. Cognitive ability and S were moderately to strongly correlated at the four levels of analyses: individual r=.55 (.44 before disattenuation), district r=.52, municipal r=.66, and provincial r=.88. European biogeographic ancestry (BGA) for the provinces was estimated from 25 genomics papers. These estimates were validated against European ancestry estimated from self-identified race/ethnicity (SIRE; r=.67) and interviewer-rated skin brightness (r=.33). On the provincial level, European BGA correlated strongly with scholastic achievement-based cognitive ability and composite S-factor scores (r's .48 and .54, respectively). These relationships were not due to confounding with latitude or mean temperature when analyzed in multivariate analyses. There were no BGA data for the other levels, so we relied on %White, skin brightness, and SIRE-based ancestry estimates instead, all of which were related to cognitive ability and S at all levels of analysis. At the individual level, skin brightness was related to both cognitive ability and S. Regression analyses showed that SIRE had little detectable predictive validity when skin brightness was included in models. Similarly, the correlations between skin brightness, cognitive ability, and S were also found inside SIRE groups. The results were similar when analyzed within provinces. In general, results were congruent with a familial model of individual and regional outcome differences.
Article
Full-text available
Number of suspects per capita were estimated for immigrants in Germany grouped by citizenship (n=83). These were correlated with national IQs (r=-.53) and Islam prevalence in the home countries (r=.49). Multivariate analyses revealed that the mean age and sex distribution of the groups in Germany were confounds. The German data lacked age and sex information for the crime data and so it was not possible to adjust for age and sex using subgroup analyses. For this reason, an alternative adjustment method was developed. This method was tested on the detailed Danish data which does have the necessary information to carry out subgroup analyses. The new method was found to give highly congruent results with the subgrouping method. The German crime data were then adjusted for age and sex using the developed method and the resulting values were analyzed with respect to the predictors. They were moderately to strongly correlated with national IQs (.46) and Islam prevalence in the home country (.35). Combining national IQ, Islam% and distance to Germany resulted in a model with a cross-validated r 2 of 20%, equivalent to a correlation of .45. If two strong outliers were removed, this rose to 25%, equivalent to a correlation of .50.
Article
Full-text available
Previously, we looked at the association between overall state-level biogeographic ancestry (BGA) and overall state-level outcomes. It was found that European BGA relative to African and Amerindian BGA was associated with better outcomes. In this paper, the analysis is extended by looking at the state-level ancestry-outcome associations individually for black and Hispanic self-identified race-ethnicity (SIRE) groups. General socioeconomic factor (S) scores were calculated for US states by SIRE groups based on three indicators. The S factor loadings were generally stable across subgroup analyses and the factor scores were stable across factor analytic extraction methods (for the latter, almost all r's ≈ 1). For Whites, Blacks and Hispanics, there were strong correlations between cognitive ability scores and S factor scores across states (r = .55 to .78; N = 28-50). This pattern also held when all data were analyzed together (r = .86, N = 115). Furthermore, the size of the Hispanic-White and Black-White S and cognitive ability gaps strongly correlated across states (r = .62 to .69; N = 36-37). Lastly, parasite prevalence did not plausibly explain SIRE gaps in cognitive ability because gaps were smaller in more parasite-rich states (combined analysis r = -.17, N = 91). We found that climatic and geospatial variables did not correlate strongly with cognitive ability and S scores when scores were decomposed by SIRE group, but did so at the total state level, even after statistically controlling for SIRE composition. Key words: Inequality, General socioeconomic factor, S factor, USA, States, Cognitive ability, Intelligence, NAEP, Race, SIRE, Biogeographic ancestry, Ecology
Article
Full-text available
We conducted novel analyses regarding the association between continental racial ancestry, cognitive ability and socioeconomic outcomes across 6 datasets: states of Mexico, states of the United States, states of Brazil, departments of Colombia, sovereign nations and all units together. We find that European ancestry is consistently and usually strongly positively correlated with cognitive ability and socioeconomic outcomes (mean r for cognitive ability = .708; for socioeconomic well-being = .643) (Sections 3-8). In most cases, including another ancestry component, in addition to European ancestry, did not increase predictive power (Section 9). At the national level, the association between European ancestry and outcomes was robust to controls for natural-environmental factors (Section 10). This was not always the case at the regional level (Section 18). It was found that genetic distance did not have predictive power independent of European ancestry (Section 10). Automatic modeling using best subset selection and lasso regression agreed in most cases that European ancestry was a non-redundant predictor (Section 11). Results were robust across 4 different ways of weighting the analyses (Section 12). It was found that the effect of European ancestry on socioeconomic outcomes was mostly mediated by cognitive ability (Section 13). We failed to find evidence of international colorism or culturalism (i.e., neither skin reflectance nor self-reported race/ethnicity showed incremental predictive ability once genomic ancestry had been taken into account) (Section 14). The association between European ancestry and cognitive outcomes was robust across a number of alternative measures of cognitive ability (Section 15). It was found that the general socioeconomic factor was not structurally different in the American sample as compared to the worldwide sample, thus justifying the use of that measure. Using Jensen's method of correlated vectors, it was found that the association between European ancestry and socioeconomic outcomes was stronger on more S factor loaded outcomes, r = .75 (Section 16). There was some evidence that tourist expenditure helped explain the relatively high socioeconomic performance of Caribbean states (Section 17).
Article
Full-text available
Immigrants can be classified into groups based on their country of origin. Group-level data concerning immigrant crime by country of origin was obtained from a 2005 Dutch-language report and were from 2002. There are data for 57 countries of origin. The crime rates were correlated with country of origin predictor variables: national IQ, prevalence of Islam and general socioeconomic factor (S). For males aged 12-17 and 18-24, the mean correlation with IQ, Islam, and S was, respectively, -.51, .37, and -.42. When subsamples split into 1st and 2nd generations were used, the mean correlation was -.74, .34, and -.40. A general crime factor among young persons was extracted. The correlations with the predictors for this variable were -.80, .34, and -.43. The results were similar when weighing the observations by the population of each immigrant group in the Netherlands. The results were also similar when using crime rates controlled for differences in household income. Some groups increased their crime rates from the 1st to 2nd generation, while for others the reverse happened.
Article
Background Earlier studies using a double perspective (destination & origin) indicate that several macro-characteristics of both destination and origin countries affect the educational performance of migrant children. This paper explores the extent to which educational system features of destination and origin countries can explain these differences in educational achievement of migrant children, next to these macro-characteristics. Methods Using data from the 2006 PISA survey, we performed cross-classified multilevel analysis on the science performance of 9.279 15-year-old migrant children, originating from 35 different countries, living in 16 Western countries of destination. We take into account a number of educational system characteristics of the countries of destination and origin, in order to measure the importance of differentiation, standardization, and the availability of resources. Results We show that differences in educational achievement between migrants cannot be fully attributed to individual characteristics or macro-characteristics. Educational system characteristics of countries of destination and origin are also meaningful. At the origin level, the length of compulsory education positively influences educational performance. This is especially the case for migrant pupils who attended education in their countries of origin. We show also that at the destination level, a high student-teacher ratio in primary education positively affects migrant pupil’s scientific performance. Moreover, migrant children with low educated parents do not perform less in highly stratified systems and even perform better in moderately differentiated systems than they do in comprehensive one. But migrant children with highly educated parents perform worse in highly and moderately stratified systems. Conclusion This study underscores the importance of educational system features as an explanation of differences in educational achievement across different origin groups and across migrants living in different destination countries. Although individual level characteristics account for the largest educational achievement differences, educational system characteristics have an effect on top of these individual level characteristics and the average educational performance in their countries of origin. Differences in educational systems contribute to explaining the effects of economic and political macro-characteristics of the countries of origin on the educational performance of migrant children in destination countries.
Book
Over the last few decades, economists and psychologists have quietly documented the many ways in which a person's IQ matters. But, research suggests that a nation's IQ matters so much more. As Garett Jones argues in Hive Mind, modest differences in national IQ can explain most cross-country inequalities. Whereas IQ scores do a moderately good job of predicting individual wages, information processing power, and brain size, a country's average score is a much stronger bellwether of its overall prosperity. Drawing on an expansive array of research from psychology, economics, management, and political science, Jones argues that intelligence and cognitive skill are significantly more important on a national level than on an individual one because they have "positive spillovers." On average, people who do better on standardized tests are more patient, more cooperative, and have better memories. As a result, these qualities—and others necessary to take on the complexity of a modern economy—become more prevalent in a society as national test scores rise. What's more, when we are surrounded by slightly more patient, informed, and cooperative neighbors we take on these qualities a bit more ourselves. In other words, the worker bees in every nation create a "hive mind" with a power all its own. Once the hive is established, each individual has only a tiny impact on his or her own life. Jones makes the case that, through better nutrition and schooling, we can raise IQ, thereby fostering higher savings rates, more productive teams, and more effective bureaucracies. After demonstrating how test scores that matter little for individuals can mean a world of difference for nations, the book leaves readers with policy-oriented conclusions and hopeful speculation: Whether we lift up the bottom through changing the nature of work, institutional improvements, or freer immigration, it is possible that this period of massive global inequality will be a short season by the standards of human history if we raise our global IQ.
Article
Personnel selection research provides much evidence that intelligence (g) is an important predictor of performance in training and on the job, especially in higher level work. This article provides evidence that g has pervasive utility in work settings because it is essentially the ability to deal with cognitive complexity, in particular, with complex information processing. The more complex a work task, the greater the advantages that higher g confers in performing it well. Everyday tasks, like job duties, also differ in their level of complexity. The importance of intelligence therefore differs systematically across different arenas of social life as well as economic endeavor. Data from the National Adult Literacy Survey are used to show how higher levels of cognitive ability systematically improve individual's odds of dealing successfully with the ordinary demands of modern life (such as banking, using maps and transportation schedules, reading and understanding forms, interpreting news articles). These and other data are summarized to illustrate how the advantages of higher g, even when they are small, cumulate to affect the overall life chances of individuals at different ranges of the IQ bell curve. The article concludes by suggesting ways to reduce the risks for low-IQ individuals of being left behind by an increasingly complex postindustrial economy.