ArticlePDF Available

DHEAS Patterning across Childhood in Three Sub-Saharan Populations: Associations with Age, Sex, Ethnicity, and Cortisol.

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objectives: Hormones have many roles in human ontogeny, including timing of life history ‘switch points’ across development. Limited hormonal data exist from non-Western children, leaving a significant gap in our understanding of the diversity of life history patterning. This cross-sectional study examines dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) production in relation to age, sex, ethnicity, and cortisol concentrations, as well as average age of adrenarche, among Aka and Ngandu children of the Central African Republic and Sidama children of Ethiopia. Methods: Hair was collected from 480 children (160 per population) aged 3-18 years old. These samples were analyzed for DHEAS and cortisol concentrations using ELISAs. A generalized additive model was used to examine DHEAS patterning in relation to age, sex, cortisol, and ethnicity. The derivative of DHEAS as a function of age was used to identify average age of adrenarche in each population. Results: DHEAS patterning in these three populations is distinct from Euro-American patterns of production. In all three groups, the population-level age at adrenarche onset occurs slightly later than Euro-American averages, with both Central African populations experiencing a later onset than the Ethiopian population. Conclusions: DHEAS patterns and age at adrenarche vary across cultures, perhaps indicating adaptive life history responses in diverse eco-cultural environments. Delayed involution of the fetal zone and DHEAS patterning may offer both cognitive protection and immune defense in high-risk, nutritionally-poor environments. Additional research in the majority world is essential to improving our understanding of the diversity of hormonal development and timing of ‘switch points’ in life history trajectories.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE
DHEAS patterning across childhood in three sub-Saharan
populations: Associations with age, sex, ethnicity, and cortisol
Courtney Helfrecht
|
Edward H. Hagen
|
David DeAvila
|
Robin M. Bernstein
|
Samuel J. Dira
|
Courtney L. Meehan
Department of Anthropology, Washington
State University, Pullman, Washington
P.O. Box 4910 99164-4910
Correspondence
Courtney Helfrecht, Department of
Anthropology, Washington State
University, P.O. Box 644910,
Pullman, WA.
Email: chelfrecht@wsu.edu
Funding information
Funded by the National Science
Foundation, Grant Numbers: BCS-
1260428 and BCS-0955213
Abstract
Objectives: Hormones have many roles in human ontogeny, including the timing of
life history switch pointsacross development. Limited hormonal data exist from
non-Western children, leaving a significant gap in our understanding of the diversity
of life history patterning. This cross-sectional study examines dehydroepiandrosterone
sulfate (DHEAS) production in relation to age, sex, ethnicity, and cortisol concentra-
tions, as well as average age of adrenarche, among Aka and Ngandu children of the
Central African Republic and Sidama children of Ethiopia.
Methods: Hair was collected from 480 children (160 per population) aged 3-
18 years old. These samples were analyzed for DHEAS and cortisol concentrations
using ELISAs. A generalized additive model was used to examine DHEAS patterning
in relation to age, sex, cortisol, and ethnicity. The derivative of DHEAS as a function
of age was used to identify average age of adrenarche in each population.
Results: DHEAS patterning in these three populations is distinct from Euro-
American patterns of production. In all three groups, the population-level age at adre-
narche onset occurs slightly later than Euro-American averages, with both Central
African populations experiencing a later onset than the Ethiopian population.
Conclusions: DHEAS patterns and age at adrenarche vary across cultures, perhaps
indicating adaptive life history responses in diverse eco-cultural environments.
Delayed involution of the fetal zone and DHEAS patterning may offer both cognitive
protection and immune defense in high-risk, nutritionally-poor environments. Addi-
tional research in the majority world is essential to improving our understanding of
the diversity of hormonal development and timing of switch pointsin life history
trajectories.
KEYWORDS
adrenarche, cortisol, DHEAS, life history theory, sub-Saharan Africa
1
|
INTRODUCTION
As mediators between biology, environment, and behavior,
hormones have received significant attention in anthropo-
logical research (e.g., Bartz et al., 2010; Bribiescas, 1996;
Burnham et al., 2003; Ellis & Essex, 2007; Ellison, 2001;
Flinn, 1999, 2006; Flinn & Ward, 2005; Gettler, McDade,
Agustin, Feranil, & Kuzawa, 2013; Gray & Campbell, 2009;
Lee, Macbeth, Pagani, & Young, 2009; Nepomnaschy &
Flinn, 2009; Worthman, 1999; Worthman & Konner, 1987).
Hormones are essential to a wide range of biological proc-
essesranging from regulation of fetal development to the
triggering of life history events to immune system mainte-
nanceand are crucial to uncovering our plasticity in
Am J Hum Biol. 2017;e23090.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23090
wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ajhb V
C2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
|
1of17
Received: 31 May 2017
|
Revised: 5 October 2017
|
Accepted: 22 November 2017
DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23090
American Journal of Human Biology
ontogeny and life history (e.g., West-Eberhard, 2003;
Worthman, 1993, 1999). It can often be a challenge, how-
ever, to collect these data from children or in diverse cultures
and remote locations. Traditional methods (i.e., collection of
saliva, urine, or serum) are not feasible in many geographies
due to storage needs, collection requirements, or cultural res-
trictions. As a result, we largely lack data on the wide range
of human diversity in hormones (Worthman, 1999), particu-
larly among preadolescent children.
This cross-sectional study has two goals. The first is to
examine the developmental trajectory of dehydroepian-
drosterone sulfate (DHEAS) production and its relationship
with age, sex, and cortisol among children aged 318 years
in three sub-Saharan populationsAka and Ngandu of
the Central African Republic, and Sidama of southwestern
Ethiopia. Although DHEAS has known correlations with age
(e.g., de Peretti & Forest 1978; Orentreich, Brind, Rizer, &
Vogelman, 1984; Sulcova, Hill, Hampl, & Starka, 1997) and
cortisol (e.g., Goodyer, Herbert, & Altham, 1998, 2001;
Phillips et al., 2010), we hypothesize that its patterning may
manifest differently across populations, reflecting adaptive
life history strategies within diverse ecocultural contexts. Our
second goal is to estimate the average age of onset for
adrenarche, the biological event underpinning postnatal
DHEAS production. Adrenarche has known variation in tim-
ing of onset (Del Giudice, Ellis, & Shirtcliff, 2011; Pratt,
Manatunga, & Li, 1994; Rotter, Wong, Lifrak, & Parker,
1985), yet little empirical research has been conducted out-
side of Euro-American populations (but see Worthman,
1993). We hypothesize that onset may be later in comparison
to Euro-American populations, as found in other biological
events such as menarche (Parent et al., 2003; Worthman,
1999). To our knowledge, this is the largest sample to date
both among children and in sub-Saharan populationsto use
hair hormone analysis to these ends.
1.1
|
Life history theory
Life history theory (LHT) is an evolutionary theoretical
framework that centers on energy allocation (Charnov, 1993;
Hill & Kaplan, 1999; Kaplan, Lancaster, & Robson, 2003;
Stearns, 1976, 1992). The primary expectation of LHT is
that a species(or an individuals) strategy should reflect the
best possible allocation of energy across the lifespan to maxi-
mize reproductive success. The two main categories of ener-
getic expenditure are somatic and reproductive. Somatic
effort broadly refers to growth and survival/maintenance,
while reproductive effort includes mating, parenting, and
inclusive fitness (Hill & Kaplan, 1999; Kaplan et al., 2003).
Energy or effort allocated to one of these categories cannot
be used for another. Thus, there are competing demands that,
along with natural selection, extrinsic risk (e.g., morbidity/
mortality rates), and the limiting factor of time, shape the life
course of a species. This framework allows for predictions of
the timing of important events across the lifespan, including:
length of gestation; age at weaning; age at menarche; age at
first reproduction; number of offspring; age at menopause;
senescence; and the duration of the lifespan.
Developmental trajectories may be set early in life, dur-
ing critical periods when experience with the environment,
particularly harshness or unpredictability, provides cues as to
the optimal reproductive strategy (e.g., Belsky, Steinberg, &
Draper, 1991; Draper & Harpending, 1982, 1988; Quinlan,
2007). Recent research characterizes human extended child-
hood as a phase of assessment, where children use their
experience with risk and unpredictability, in both the social
and physical environments, to shape their life history strat-
egies (Del Giudice, Angeleri, & Manera, 2009; see also
Belsky et al., 1991; Bogin, 2002). This framework suggests
that life history stages allow an individual to evaluate local
conditions and vary timing of transitions in order to generate
optimal developmental and reproductive strategies. These
transitions are coordinated by hormones, which trigger gene
expression. As parents alone may not be capable of pro-
viding an accurate prediction of future conditions, adaptive
plasticity is invaluable to human development (e.g., West-
Eberhard, 2003). Thus, onset of adrenarche, the developmen-
tal event underpinning human juvenility, can also be thought
of as a switch pointfollowing the evaluative phase of early
childhooda transition point in human life history where
hormones (in response to environmental cues) turn onthe
genes that lead to phenotypic variation (Del Giudice et al.,
2009).
1.2
|
DHEAS production and adrenarche
The adrenarche event is marked by rising post-natal pro-
duction of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulfate
(DHEAS; hereafter DHEA/S when referring to both), typi-
cally co-occurring with onset of middle childhood (generally
between 6 and 8 years old; Campbell, 2006, 2011; Del
Giudice et al., 2009; Parker, 1991). DHEAS, which is the
most abundant steroid hormone in circulation, has several
hypothesized functions relating to neurocognitive function,
social and physical development, and protection against
excessive stress (discussed below). Based on these functions,
it appears that our adrenarcheal patterning, in comparison to
other primates, reflects an adaptive extension of juvenility,
one that allows for additional cognitive and behavioral devel-
opment (Bernstein, 2016).
Onset of DHEAS production during childhood, an event
known as adrenarche, is due to the development of the zona
reticularis (ZR) in the adrenal cortex (Dohm, 1973; Parker,
1991). The fetal zone produces DHEA after birth but its sub-
sequent postnatal involution is typically rapid (within
6 months); it is not until around 34 years of age that islands
2of17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
of ZR start to become evident in children and low levels of
DHEA can sometimes be detected in circulation (Dohm,
1973; Hui et al., 2009). As these islands mature into the ZR
layer, DHEA/S becomes more evident in circulation, and is
almost always detectable by age 8 (Dohm, 1973; Parker,
1991). The layer continues to increase in thickness during
development, reflected by increasing levels of DHEA/S
(Dohm, 1973; Parker, 1991), reaching its terminal thickness
around age 13 (Nakamura, Rainey, Kurotaki, Hui, & Sasano,
2010). Because it typically precedes puberty, adrenarche was
formerly believed to be linked with gonadarche. It is now
clear that these are not directly connected (although there is
some evidence that the same genes control expression, with
estimated heritability between 57% and 65%; see Parker,
1995), as one can proceed in the absence of the other
(Campbell, 2006; Parker, 1991). DHEA/S levels appear to be
associated with age, and continue to rise through the mid-20s
(Worthman, 1999) and in some places as late as the mid-30s
(e.g., Turkana men of northern Kenya; Campbell, Leslie, and
Campbell, 2007), before starting to decline.
The adrenarche event encompasses physical, cognitive,
and social changes that mark a visible transition to middle
childhood (Campbell, 2011; Del Giudice et al., 2009). Physi-
cally, onset of adrenarche triggers: increasing production of
axillary hair, emergence of pubic hair, and rising oil produc-
tion from sebaceous glands (Parker, 1991); growth (Parker,
1991; Zemel & Katz, 1986; but see Campbell, 2011); and
development of the immune system, particularly as an antago-
nist against cortisol (Hechter, Grossman, & Chatterton, 1997).
In addition, DHEAS can act not only as a DHEA reservoir, as
it can be back-converted into DHEA [which in turn can be
converted to testosterone and dihydrotestosterone or estradiol
via steroidogenic enzymes (White & Porterfield, 2013)], but
DHEAS can also be converted directly into other androgens/
estrogens from reservoirs in peripheral tissues (Labrie et al.,
2005). Cognitively, DHEA/S is associated with neurological
development, by facilitating learning and enhancing memory
(Majewska, 1995). Given these neurological aspects of
DHEA/S production, it is perhaps not surprising that the adre-
narche event maps neatly onto the 5-to-7-year-old transition
(White, 1996), a time when, cross-culturally, children start to
make sense(Lancy & Grove, 2011; Rogoff, Sellers,
Pirrotta, Fox, & White, 1975) due to their increased reasoning
abilities. Socially, onset of DHEA/S production appears to
lead to increased social interactions and reduced fearfulness
(Campbell, 2006). Studies undertaken in western Kenya also
indicate that elevated DHEAS concentrations are associated
with decreased malaria parasite density among pubertal
females (Leenstra et al., 2003) and increased malaria resist-
ance, as well as reduced parasite density, among pubertal
males (Kurtis, Mtalib, Onyango, & Duffy, 2001).
There is individual variation in the timing of the adre-
narche event (Del Giudice et al., 2011; Pratt et al., 1994;
Rotter et al., 1985), and onset can be somewhat earlier
among girls than boys (Sulcova et al., 1997). It is hypothe-
sized that perhaps environmental cues, like increases in body
mass index (Remer & Manz, 1999) or prenatal programming
(Ong et al., 2004), set the timing of adrenarche but it may be
that the onset of DHEA/S production is due solely to the
maturation of the adrenal glands. However, both physical
and social environments have known effects on the timing of
hormonal events; it has been well-established, for example,
that both nutritional (e.g., Kulin, Bwibo, Mutie, & Saniner,
1982) and social (e.g., Ellis & Garber, 2000) stress can affect
pubertal timing. It has also been demonstrated among rhesus
monkeys that calorie restriction can inhibit age-related
declines in DHEAS (Lane, Ingram, Ball, & Roth, 1997), sug-
gesting that environmental inputs may have important effects
on DHEAS production. Ellis and Essex (2007), for example,
found that low parental support is correlated with earlier
onset of adrenarche. As a result, it appears likely that the tim-
ing of adrenarche, within the context of life history pattern-
ing, is tied to early social and environmental inputs (Del
Giudice et al., 2011).
1.3
|
Cortisol production
Cortisol is a steroid hormone produced in the zona fascicu-
lata of the adrenal glands. It is the key glucocorticoid pro-
duced by humans, playing many important roles in both
normal maintenance of the body via its metabolic function as
well as in development, immune function, and maintenance
of homeostasis. Perturbations to homeostasis can be cha-
racterized as stressors, and stress, or allostasis, as the process
by which an organism attempts to return to homeostasis
(Chrousos, 1998; Sapolsky, 1994). These perturbations are
often caused by risk or unpredictability in the physical or
social environment (e.g., Charnov, 1993; Chisholm, Bur-
bank, Coall, & Gemmiti, 2005; Ellis & Essex, 2007; Moore
et al., 1997; Quinlan, 2007).
Effects of allostasis can vary depending on the timing,
patterning, duration, and individual experience of stressors.
For males, prenatal stress can lead to higher disease risk,
whereas the greatest risk for females is associated with stress
during peripubertal or pubertal maturation (Bale & Epperson,
2015). Allostatic load represents the cumulative effects of
extended or repeated stress (McEwen, 1998; McEwen &
Steller, 1993) and is tied to a wide range of negative health
impacts including, among others, immune deficiency, cogni-
tive impairment, inhibited growth, delayed sexual maturity,
damage to the hippocampus, sensitivity of amygdala fear cir-
cuits, and psychological maladjustments (e.g., Flinn, 2006;
McEwen, 1998; McEwen & Steller, 1993; Nepomnaschy &
Flinn, 2009; Sapolsky, 1999). Individuals also vary in both
responsiveness to (Ellis & Boyce, 2008; Ellis, Jackson, &
Boyce, 2006; McEwen, 1998; McEwen & Steller, 1993) and
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
3of17
perceptions of (McEwen, 1998; McEwen & Steller, 1993;
Piko, 2002) stress and this, coupled with genetic differences,
can result in distinct outcomes despite similar experiences.
An alternative perspective on the allostatic load suggests that
chronic stress provides children information during develop-
ment that, in the context of adaptive plasticity, may inform
appropriate life history strategies (Ellis & Del Giudice, 2014;
Ellis et al., 2006). Regardless, stress clearly has significant
impacts on life history trajectories, and cortisol is one mea-
sure of its effects.
1.4
|
DHEA/S as a cortisol antagonist
In addition to its role in childrens social and physical devel-
opment, DHEAS may also act as an antagonist to cortisol,
mitigating some of the effects of allostatic load. High corti-
sol:DHEAS ratios are linked to persistent major depression
among children 816 years old (Goodyer et al., 1998). Ade-
quate DHEAS concentrations have been hypothesized to mit-
igate the detrimental effects of cortisol hypersecretion on the
brain (Goodyer, Park, Netherton, & Herbert, 2001). Higher
cortisol:DHEAS ratios are also associated with increased risk
of metabolic syndrome (at least among adult males), while
higher DHEAS appears protective (Phillips et al., 2010).
Increased DHEAS in response to elevated cortisol thus app-
ears to have protective qualities, and likely plays a role
in allostasis. This may be of particular importance during
sensitive developmental periods and in high-stress/high-risk
environments.
1.5
|
Hair hormone analysis
Cortisol and, to a lesser extent, other steroids have been well-
demonstrated to be detectable in hair, both human and nonhu-
man, using commercially available enzyme-linked immu-
nosorbent assay (ELISA) kits (e.g., Davenport, Tiefenbacher,
Lutz, Novak, & Meyer, 2006; Fourie & Bernstein, 2011;
Gow, Thomson, Rieder, Van Uum, & Koren, 2010; Laudensl-
ager, Jorgensen, & Fairbanks, 2012; Sauv
e, Koren, Walsh,
Tokmakejian, & van Uum, 2007). Most prior research quanti-
fying adrenal androgens in hair has focused on DHEA (Kintz,
Cirimele, & Ludes, 1999; Shen, Xiang, Shen, Bu, & Wang,
2009), with only one study including analysis of DHEAS
(Chen et al., 2013). These studies demonstrate that these hor-
mones are present and measurable in hair. None of these stud-
ies used ELISAs for analysis, making this project the first to
do so.
Passive diffusion from blood is considered the most
likely way that hormone is incorporated into hair (Russell,
Koren, Rieder, & Van Uum, 2012; Stalder & Kirschbaum,
2012), but external sources may also contribute to hair corti-
sol concentrations. While topical steroid application is an
obvious confound, sweat (Russell, Koren, Rieder, & Van
Uum, 2014; cf. Noppe et al., 2014) and relative humidity
(Boesch et al., 2015) may increase cortisol concentrations.
The hair follicle itself may also be capable of locally produc-
ing cortisol (Ito et al., 2005). Neither hair color nor hair type
appears to influence cortisol content (Noppe et al., 2014; cf.
Rippe et al., 2016). There is disagreement regarding the
effects of ethnicity on hair cortisol concentrations; some sug-
gest that ethnicity is not correlated with hair cortisol concen-
trations (Vaghri et al., 2013) while others consider it to be an
important control variable (e.g., Rippe et al., 2016).
Hair collection eliminates the need for and stress of hav-
ing multiple spit, blood, or urine collections within and
across several days and weeks (Russell et al., 2012; Van
Uum et al., 2008). Additionally, it does not require any spe-
cial training or context for collection or storage (e.g., Gow
et al., 2010), and the sample remains stable until processing
(even for thousands of years; Webb et al., 2010). In many
populations, but especially among children, use of a noninva-
sive methodology may be preferable when collecting bio-
marker data, as it minimizes the potential for psychological
harm or physical risk.
A single hair sample can be indicative of an individuals
hormone phenotypeand concentration changes can reflect,
for example, developmental inflection points or the experi-
ence of long term stress (Davenport et al., 2006; Russell
et al., 2012). Hair does not reflect circulating hormone con-
centrations at the time of collection, but provides a picture of
an individuals physiological state during hair growth. This
allows for examination of a time-averaged signal generated
over the time of hair growth (especially cortisolsee Meyer
& Novak, 2012; Sauv
e et al., 2007; Stalder & Kirschbaum,
2012), in contrast to highly variable point samples (e.g.,
saliva or blood collection). By providing both a picture of an
individuals hormonal phenotype as well as a window into
their experience over several months, hair hormone analysis
extends the range of questions that can be examined within
the constraints of anthropological fieldwork.
1.6
|
Study populations
Research was undertaken among three sub-Saharan popula-
tions: Aka forest foragers, Ngandu horticulturalists, and
Sidama agropastoralists. Aka forest foragers and Ngandu
horticulturalists reside in the southwestern portion of the
Central African Republic within the tropical rainforest of the
Congo Basin. Aka live in association with Ngandu horticul-
turalists. This relationship is one of both economic exchange
(with Aka exchanging forest goods for agricultural foods)
and social and spiritual ties (Bahuchet & Guillaume, 1982;
Hewlett, 1991; Takeuchi, 2005). Although the Aka remain
mobile, time spent living in close proximity to the Ngandu
village has increased in recent years (Meehan, Hagen, &
Hewlett, 2017).
4of17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
The Aka social environment is highly supportive. Aka
practice demand sharing; this, along with cooperation and
egalitarianism, is a core value (Bahuchet, 1990; Hewlett,
1992). Aka are indulgent and affectionate parents, instilling
autonomy and independence at an early age (Hewlett, 1991).
Infants are held most of the day by their mothers or another
caregiver, and allomothers (nonmaternal caregivers) consti-
tute an essential component to childrens social environments
(Meehan, 2005). Aka children as young as 3 years old often
act as caregivers, although infants would not be left for
extensive periods with other children until the caregiver
reached approximately 5 years of age. By the age of 10, chil-
dren have already acquired many of the skills necessary to
forest life (Hewlett & Cavalli-Sforza, 1986).
In contrast to their intimate rearing environment, the physical
environment can be considered fairly harsh. Among the Aka,
there are high rates of infant and childhood mortality (Hewlett,
1991). The leading causes of death at all ages are infectious and
parasitic diseases (Hewlett, van de Koppel, & van de Koppel,
1986). Most Aka children have weight-for-age (60.63%) and
height-for-age (80.95%) z-scores 2 SD below WHO reference
means, suggesting that the population is severely underweight
and stunted (Meehan, Helfrecht, & Quinlan, 2014). While nutri-
tional stress is certainly an important issue in this population, it
must also be noted that Aka are genetically distinct from other
Central African peoples (Bozzola et al., 2009; Dietz, Marino,
Peacock, & Bailey, 1989; Jarvis et al., 2012) and are considered
apygmypopulation, indicating that they will almost always
fall below reference means (Meehan et al., 2014). Despite these
environmental hardships, Aka view their environmentspecifi-
cally, the forestas giving and supportive (Hewlett, Lamb,
Leyendecker, & Scholmerick, 2000).
As noted above, the Ngandu live in the same geographic
region and frequently in close proximity to the Aka but differ
ethnically, linguistically, and culturally. Ngandu parenting
styles have been characterized as more authoritative than
among the Aka (Hewlett, 1991). Infants are typically held
less often than Aka infants and care is less intimate (Hewlett
& Lamb, 2002; Meehan, 2008). Men and women work inde-
pendently and in strongly sex-prescribed activities (Hewlett,
1991; Meehan, 2008). Although the Ngandu practice shar-
ing, it occurs less frequently and is not as widespread
(Hewlett, 1991; Hewlett & Lamb, 2002).
Ngandu are subject to similar disease risks as Aka, partic-
ularly malaria and other parasites. Additional diseases known
to be problematic include bronchitis, sexually-transmitted dis-
eases, and high blood pressure (Hewlett, 1991). Helfrecht and
Meehan (2016) found that fully one-third of Ngandu children
had weight-for-age (33.04%) and height-for-age (34.51%)
z-scores 2 SD below CDC reference means, indicating that
nutritional stress is relatively high in this population.
Sidama agropastoralists reside in the Sidama Zone of the
Southern Nations, Nationalities, and PeoplesRegion of
southwestern Ethiopia. Sidamaland, part of the Great Rift
Valley, lies in the area between Lake Awassa and Lake
Abaya, the northern and southern boundaries, respectively
(Br
ogger, 1986; Hamer, 1987). Both agriculture and cattle
herding are important components of Sidama life but concen-
tration may vary depending on residence elevation (i.e.,
low-, mid-, or high-land environments; Hamer, 1987). The
data here come from Sidama residing in a lowland, peri-
urban environment.
Similar to the Ngandu, the Sidama practice a strict sexual
division of labor (Asefach & Nigatu, 2008; Hamer, 1987).
Children assist their parents in all tasks and, as they mature,
they segregate into sex-proscribed activities (Hamer, 1987).
Due to increased school attendance, however, it was noted
that many children are spending less time obtaining these
skills from their parents. The gerontocracy is male-
dominated (although women gain influence when they
become mothers and particularly when their sons attain eld-
erhood; Br
ogger, 1986) and infiltrates parenting stylemen
are considered responsible for the actions of their wives and
children (Hamer, 1987). Participation in community life,
while generally desirable and pleasant, is obligate; coopera-
tion is essential to social and economic success (Br
ogger,
1986).
In a survey of disease risks that affect child health,
parents frequently noted malaria, bacteria, parasites (particu-
larly helminths; Ashenafi, Techalew, Mulugeta, Asrat, &
Berhanu, 2011), and pneumonia, as well as influenza and
typhoid (Helfrecht, 2016). They additionally stated that the
prevalence of these is worsened by food shortages. Although
the majority of Sidama children fall within normal ranges for
height- and weight-for-age, 26.8% had height-for-age and
36% had weight-for-age z-scores 2 SD below CDC reference
means (Helfrecht, 2016; see also Yewelsew, Kennedy, Gates,
& Stoecker, 2008). These scores, like those among Aka and
Ngandu, clearly indicate serious potential for increased mor-
bidity and mortality (Martorell & Ho, 1984; Pelletier & Fron-
gillo, 2003; Scrimshaw, Taylor, & Gordon, 1968).
Below, we present the methods and results of our exami-
nation of DHEAS patterning and onset of adrenarche among
these three sub-Saharan populations. Based on the diverse
ecocultural contexts, we hypothesize that DHEAS patterning
will manifest differently across populations, reflecting
adaptive plasticity in human life history trajectories. We
additionally hypothesize that onset of adrenarche, like other
biological events, will occur later than among Euro-
American populations.
2
|
METHODS
The protocol and procedures presented below were reviewed
and approved by Washington State Universitys Institutional
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
5of17
Review Board. Following explanation of the proposed re-
search to participants and prior to hair collection, informed
assent was obtained from child participants and parent per-
mission was obtained from all adults.
2.1
|
Hair collection
Hair was collected from 480 children (80 male, 80 female in
each population), between the ages of approximately 3 and
18 years. Hair samples were obtained by shaving or snipping
a small portion (3 cm diameter) from the posterior vertex
of the participants head (left side). Consistency in where
hair is sampled is important as there is research indicating
variation in hormone concentrations in different areas of the
scalp, with the posterior vertex demonstrating the least intra-
sample variation (Sauv
e et al., 2007). If a sample was col-
lected by shaving, a new razor blade was used for each par-
ticipant; scissors, when used, were thoroughly cleaned with
alcohol swabs and allowed to dry between collections. Hair
was collected directly into a labeled paper envelope and
sealed.
As this study was noninvasive, it was not possible to
obtain blood in order to compare circulating hormone to hair
hormone. In addition, blood (or urine/saliva) would have
required several repeated samples to make such a compari-
son. It should be noted, however, that such studies have been
previously done with cortisol and found that hair cortisol
concentrations (HCC) are significantly correlated with
those found in saliva (DAnna-Hernandez, Ross, Natvig, &
Laudenslager, 2011; Sauv
e et al., 2007), urine (Sauv
e et al.,
2007), and serum (Sauv
e et al., 2007; Yang, Lan, Meng,
Wan, & Han, 1998).
2.2
|
Age
Aka parents do not keep track of childrens birthdates, but
ages of children were accurately calculated by relative aging
to other children in camp and by using seasonal or local
events to determine the childs birth month and year (see
Helfrecht & Meehan, 2016; Meehan et al., 2014). In addi-
tion, two of the authors (CH & CM) spent 5 and 13 years,
respectively, working with this population, allowing for a
cumulative acquisition of birth dates and demographic data;
several, but not all, of the children participating in this study
have been involved in previous research so their birth month
and year were previously known. As research among this
population of Aka has been ongoing for 301years, both par-
ticipants and researchers are aware of significant events that
have occurred in the village. This allows for year of birth to
be determined with relative ease. From there, seasonal events
such as when caterpillar season occurred or corn was
plantedcan be used to determine the month of birth.
Among the Ngandu and the Sidama, families typically know
the age and date of birth for their children and, if not, birth
certificates are usually available. When this information was
not available, ages were determined to month and year of
birth using relative aging based on season of birth, other fam-
ily members whose birth date was known, and known birth
dates of nearby neighbors. Age is reported here in years.
2.3
|
Hair hormone analysis
Prior to starting extraction, it was necessary to remove for-
eign matter from the hair samples. When working with popu-
lations who live, for example, in mobile camps or mudbrick
homes, such as forest foragers, horticulturalists, and agropas-
toralists, hair collections often include extraneous material.
Some samples had significant quantities of such matter (e.g.,
lice/lice eggs, dirt, or leaf debris) that could affect weights or
interfere with hormone analysis (Cooper, Kronstrand, &
Kintz, 2012) if not inspected and removed prior to the extrac-
tion process. While some researchers are concerned that
washing the hair (especially with water) may remove cortisol
(Hamel et al., 2011), others note that the exterior of the hair
shaft could have been exposed to exogenous sources of hor-
mone and that all samples should undergo decontamination
procedures (Society of Hair Testing, 1997).
After visual examination and debris removal, approxi-
mately 30 mg of hair was weighed into a glass vial. Hair was
then washed twice in 3 ml of isopropanol for 3 min. Isopro-
pranol has been previously determined to be the best choice
of wash medium as it does not extract cortisol from the inte-
rior of the hair shaft (e.g., Davenport et al., 2006) and addi-
tionally serves to remove any remaining debris. Hair was
dried completely (35 days minimum) under a fume hood.
More hormone can be recovered from ground than
minced hair (Davenport et al., 2006). Thus, the complete
sample of dry hair was next placed into a stainless steel
microvial (1.8 ml) along with 4 chrome steel beads (size
3.2 mm) and sealed with a silicon rubber cap. These vials
were placed in liquid nitrogen for 1 min and then pulverized
for 1 min at a high speed using a BioSpec (Bartlesville, OK)
mini-beadbeater-16 (set between 3.0 and 3.5). The liquid
nitrogen-pulverization cycle was repeated two additional
times (3 cycles total). The use of liquid nitrogen is not essen-
tial, but freezing the hair led to the greatest consistency in
the processed samples. Following the third cycle, hair sam-
ples had a uniform powder-like consistency.
Ten milligrams (or less) of powdered hair was weighed
into a plastic tube and extracted in 4 ml of methanol. Quanti-
ties greater than 10 mg did not serially dilute consistently,
but dilutions of samples 10 mg were proportional. Samples
were next sonicated at 45 8C for 30 min at 50 KHz using a
Branson 3800 sonicator. Sonication, like milling, can inc-
rease the amount of hormone that can be extracted from hair
(Fourie et al., 2016). Samples were then placed on a plate
6of17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
shaker at room temperature for 23.5 h at 470 RPM (started at
550 RPM to get the powder into solution, then reduced to
470). At the end of the incubation period, samples were cen-
trifuged at 2620G/3000 RPM for 10 min at 4 8C to separate
the powder from the methanol, and 3.5 ml of the extract was
aliquotted into a polypropylene tube. Samples were dried in
a vacuum concentrator at 37 8C until completely evaporated
(5 h), and then stored at 4 8C if not immediately removed.
The dry extracts were reconstituted in 500 ml of assay buffer
and sonicated for 20 min at 37 8C.
To determine hair hormone concentrations, the samples
were assayed using commercially-available ELISA kits (for
development of this procedure and subsequent analyses,
ALPCO [Salem, NH] Cortisol ELISA [Catalog no. 11-
CORHU-E01-SLV] and Salimetrics [State College, PA] Sali-
vary DHEA-S [Catalog No. 11252] kits were used). Cross-
reactivity for the ALPCO cortisol ELISA is 13.6% for pred-
nisolone, 7.6% for corticosterone, 7.2% for deoxycorticoste-
rone, 7.2% for progesterone, 6.2% for cortisone, 5.6% for
deoxycortisol, 5.6% for prednisone, and 1.6% for dexametha-
sone. Crossreactivity for the Salimetrics DHEA-S ELISA is
0.0844% for androsterone and 0.0268% for transandrosterone.
Results for cortisol are returned as ng/ml, while DHEAS
results are returned in pg/ml. These were converted to pg/mg
by dividing the result in half (as only 500 ml of buffer is used
to reconstitute the sample) and then dividing by 0.875
(because only 3.5 ml of the original 4 ml extraction was
dried down). This was then divided by the initial weight of
the powdered hair to determine ng/mg for cortisol and pg/mg
for DHEAS. For cortisol, this was then multiplied by 1000 in
order to determine pg/mg. Both cortisol and DHEAS results
are presented here in pg/mg.
2.4
|
Statistical methods
The hair hormone concentrations for DHEAS were strongly
skewed and were therefore log-transformed to improve nor-
mality; while both transformed and untransformed data are
presented below, transformed data were used for analyses.
Generalized additive models (GAM) were used to evaluate
the associations of age, sex, cortisol, and population with
DHEAS hair hormone concentrations, as well as in examin-
ing the relationship between DHEAS and age by population
to estimate derivatives. GAMs can be a preferable alternative
to linear analyses as they employ a local scoring algorithm
that allows smoothing of predictor variables and identifica-
tion of nonlinear covariate effects (Hastie & Tibshirani,
1986). A one-way ANOVA was used to examine mean pop-
ulation differences in cortisol concentrations and a t-test was
used to evaluate mean differences between the sexes. The
GAM analyses and plots were generated using R 3.3.2 and
the Mixed GAM Computation Vehicle with GCV/AIC/
REML Smoothness Estimation package v. 1.8-1.5; all other
analyses were conducted in Stata 10.1.
3
|
RESULTS
Of the initial 480 participants, we were able to successfully
analyze 449 (221 male; 228 female) for hair DHEAS concen-
trations (Table 1). Across the entire sample, hair DHEAS
concentrations ranged from a minimum of 11.46 pg/mg to a
maximum of 1372.57 pg/mg, with a mean of 231.34 pg/mg
(SD 5231.34) and a median of 163.98 pg/mg. Log
10
-trans-
formation of the data reduces the skew.
Of the 480 participants, 479 (240 male, 239 female) were
included in the cortisol analyses (Table 2). One Sidama girl
had an extremely high HCC (>7000 pg/mg). At the time of
collection, this almost 11-year-old had a skin infection on her
head. Because her HCC well-exceeds the range identified
within the rest of the sample and is likely due to something
external (e.g., the skin infection itself increasing local cortisol
and/or a topical treatment that was not fully eliminated during
the two wash cycles), she was removed from the analyses.
Hair cortisol concentrations across the entire sample ranged
from a minimum of 24.82 pg/mg to a maximum of 3405.42
pg/mg, with a mean of 535.27 pg/mg (SD 5440.01) and a
median of 451.94 pg/mg. Log
10
-transformation of the data
reduces the skew.
Based on loess regression plots, each population
appears to have its own DHEAS and cortisol patterning
(Figure 1). Surprisingly, very high DHEAS levels were
found among the youngest in all three samples, but espe-
cially in the Aka and Sidama populations. In general,
DHEAS patterning does not reflect expectations based on
the literature cited above; the high concentrations among
young children coupled with the relatively gradual rise
after the adrenarche event deviate from previously identi-
fied trajectories. For the most part, cortisol concentrations
mirrored the DHEAS trajectory, albeit with visible varia-
tion at the tails of the age distribution.
Results from a one-way ANOVA indicated that there are
significant differences in cortisol concentrations between
populations [F(2,236)58.52, P<.001]. Post hoc compari-
sons using the Scheffe test indicate that there is no significant
difference between Aka and Ngandu, but both of these popu-
lations have significantly (P<.001) higher HCC than
Sidama children (see Table 2 for means and SD). Sex differ-
ences among children were not expected but it has been
noted in some other studies that males have higher cortisol
responses to stress than females (see Kudielka & Kirsch-
baum, 2005 for a review) so this was tested here. Among the
Aka and the Sidama, but not the Ngandu, males have higher
mean cortisol concentrations. This only reaches significance
for the Sidama (t51.88, P5.031).
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
7of17
A GAM was used to evaluate the effects of age, sex, cor-
tisol, and population on DHEAS concentrations; based on
the loess regression plots, an interaction term for sex and
population was also included (Table 3). The intercept for this
model represents Aka females. Controlling for age within
each population, results indicate that Aka have significantly
higher DHEAS concentrations than Sidama, but not Ngandu
(although there does appear to be a trend in this direction as
well). In general, there is no difference in DHEAS concentra-
tions between males and females. Sidama children have the
lowest hair DHEAS concentrations, controlling for age, sex,
cortisol, and population. Within populations, there is no dif-
ference between Aka males and females or between Sidama
males and females, but Ngandu males have significantly
lower DHEAS concentrations than females. For both Aka
and Sidama children, age is a significant predictor of
DHEAS hair concentrations. Because GAMs allow the data
to determine the fit of the curve, the estimated degrees of
freedom (edf) allow interpretation of the variation that exists
across the sample. Here, the relatively high edf indicate that
there are evident changes across the age range of the sample.
Age is not a significant predictor for Ngandu children, and
the regression line is relatively flat in comparison to Aka and
Sidama, albeit somewhat curvilinear (Figure 2). HCC also
has a significant (though relatively minor) effect on DHEAS
concentrations, with higher cortisol being associated with
higher DHEAS concentrations. The model accounts for 47%
of the variance in DHEAS hair concentrations.
TABLE 1 Summary statistics for age, DHEAS, and log
10
-transformed DHEAS by population and sex
Aka Ngandu Sidama
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
N71 63 77 79 80 79
Age (years) Mean 9.82 8.69 7.89 7.99 8.18 7.94
Range 2.99-18 3.0617.42 3.0218.26 3.1317.10 2.76-18.12 2.96-15.14
SD 3.88 4.02 3.48 3.38 3.63 3.06
DHEAS (pg/mg) Mean 359.65 356.03 293.56 184.54 109.11 126.55
Range 78.19853.93 89.94905.26 43.801372.57 33.08736.52 24.07598.46 11.46585.97
SD 226.26 209.99 222.64 131.07 108.74 114.57
Log
10
DHEAS Mean 2.46 2.48 2.37 2.18 1.90 1.97
Range 1.89-2.93 1.95-2.96 1.64-3.14 1.52-2.87 1.38-2.78 1.06-2.77
SD 0.30 0.25 0.29 0.27 0.32 0.34
TABLE 2 Summary statistics for age, cortisol, and log
10
-transformed cortisol by population and sex
Aka Ngandu Sidama
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
N80 80 80 80 79 80
Age (years) Mean 9.83 9.41 7.94 8.05 8.14 7.88
Range 2.99-18.00 3.0618.00 3.0218.26 3.1317.1 2.76-18.12 2.96-15.14
SD 4.25 4.33 3.53 3.40 3.64 3.09
Cortisol (pg/mg) Mean 577.65 624.07 681.91 641.59 279.03 404.17
Range 112.301488.00 97.322728.16 97.671957.35 34.233072.48 24.821921.89 29.103405.42
SD 342.02 447.36 383.86 451.96 313.14 528.99
Log
10
cortisol Mean 2.68 2.70 2.76 2.72 2.25 2.37
Range 2.05-3.17 1.99-3.44 1.99-3.29 1.53-3.49 1.39-3.28 1.46-3.53
SD 0.28 0.29 0.26 0.30 0.40 0.43
8of17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
Average age of adrenarcheal onset was determined by
taking the derivative of the curve for age regressed on log
10
-
transformed DHEAS concentrations using a GAM. The zero
point of the curve is the age at which DHEAS production
changes from declining concentrations to rising production
and is illustrated for each population in Figure 3. Based on
these samples, the age at which the early childhood decline
in DHEAS transitions to an increase in DHEAS concentra-
tionsrepresenting onset of adrenarcheis approximately
9 years among Aka and Ngandu children, and around 8 years
for Sidama children.
4
|
DISCUSSION
We were successfully able to use hair hormone analysis to
examine patterns of DHEAS and cortisol production among
children in three sub-Saharan populations, providing an alter-
nate methodology for life history research in areas where tra-
ditional methods are challenging. We additionally provide
baseline data for these two biomarkers, useful to both intra-
and cross-cultural analyses. Most importantly, the DHEAS
patterns presented here reflect distinct variation from known
patterns of development, with implications for studies of
human ontogeny and life history trajectories. That differences
exist was expected, but the high levels observed across early
childhood and the comparatively flat pattern observed among
Ngandu children were both surprising. Although all three
populations appear to have delayed involution of the fetal
zone in comparison to Euro-American populations, the
Sidama and Aka both reflect a decline across early childhood
followed by a rise across middle childhood and adolescence,
whereas Ngandu only trend towards a positive association
between DHEAS and age. We also found that while there
was, in general, no sex difference in DHEAS concentrations,
Ngandu males had significantly lower DHEAS than Ngandu
females.
Two factors of the physical environment, malnutrition
and disease burden, may be of relevance here in relation to
both DHEAS and cortisol patterning. Although previous
sample sizes for hair cortisol analyses using ELISAs and
conducted among children are smaller, potentially obscuring
FIGURE 1 Scatter plot and loess regression of age (in years) on cortisol and DHEAS hair concentrations (pg/mg) by population and sex. Each dot
represents an individual data point
TABLE 3 Significance values of the GAM predictors (population,
sex, log
10
-transformed cortisol, population 3sex, and age by popula-
tion) for log
10
-transformed DHEAS concentrations (n5449; adj. R-
sq 50.471)
Parametric coefficients Estimate P-value
(Intercept) 2.191 .000
Ngandu 20.083 .075
Sidama 20.511 .000
Male 0.009 .845
Log
10
-transformed cortisol 0.096 .016
Ngandu 3male 20.198 .002
Sidama 3male 0.055 .395
Smooth terms edf P-value
Age 3Aka 2.606 .000
Age 3Ngandu 1.863 .258
Age 3Sidama 3.632 .000
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
9of17
extant variation, the concentrations reported are also gener-
ally much lower than those found here (see Boesch et al.,
2015; Groenveld et al., 2013; Grunau et al., 2013; Karl
en,
Frostell, Theodorsson, Faresj
o, & Ludvigsson, 2013; Karl
en,
Ludvigsson, Frostell, Theodorsson, & Faresj
o, 2011; Noppe
et al., 2014; Steudte et al., 2011; Vaghri et al., 2013). It is
possible that our methodology is more effective at extracting
hair cortisol, as we both milled and sonicated our samples,
methods known to improve extraction (see Fourie et al.,
2016). It is also probable that our results are a reflection of
the greater environmental stress in these geographies. In all
three populations, the majority of children fall below interna-
tional reference means for height- and weight-for age, sug-
gesting extensive experience with both acute and chronic
nutritional stress (Helfrecht, 2016; Helfrecht & Meehan,
2016; Meehan et al., 2014). Malnutrition is associated with
elevated cortisol (e.g., Alleyne & Young, 1967; Fernald &
Grantham-McGregor, 1998; Jaya Rao, Srikantia, & Gopalan,
1968; Smith et al., 1981), and this is a likely explanation for
the high levels of cortisol seen across all three populations.
Given the comparatively lower rates of malnutrition among
the Sidama in this study (Helfrecht, 2016), it is not surprising
that their cortisol concentrations are significantly lower than
among Aka and Ngandu children.
Disease burden is also high in both these geographies and
some risks, such as malaria and other parasitic diseases, are
shared. This also likely contributes to the high HCC observed,
but may also be a factor in DHEAS patterning. As noted
above, higher concentrations of DHEAS are associated with
reduced parasite density and increased resistance to malaria
among adolescents in Kenya (Kurtis et al., 2001; Leenstra
et al., 2003). The coupled impact of disease and malnutrition
has potentially led to an allostatic load requiring modification
of DHEAS production, particularly among Aka and Ngandu
FIGURE 2 GAM plots by population (A, Aka; B, Ngandu; C, Sidama). Each tick on the x-axis represents an individual data point
10 of 17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
children, who face greater risk of death from malaria than
Sidama children (WHO, 2010). The loess regression plots
suggest that this is a feasible explanation, as the DHEAS tra-
jectories largely mirror cortisol concentrations.
Little data on DHEAS patterning currently exist for
majority world populations (but see Worthman, 1993), but
fairly extensive data exist on puberty. Although puberty and
adrenarche are not related events, these data may be informa-
tive for interpreting the timing and patterning of DHEAS
production observed here. For example, examination of age
at menarche in nonindustrial populations allowed for an
understanding of the historically-novel trend toward earlier
puberty (e.g., Parent et al., 2003). It is possible that the
delayedadrenarcheal onset observed in these three popula-
tions is actually more reflective of timing throughout our
evolutionary history, and earlier onset in minority world pop-
ulations is due to greater environmental stability/less nutri-
tional stress, biasing our previous interpretations.
FIGURE 3 Derivatives (dotted lines) of the GAM for DHEAS by age (years)
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
11 of 17
This lens may also aid in our understanding of the pat-
terning of DHEAS production. Worthman (1999) found that
the amount of time between onset of puberty (as marked hor-
monally) and the menarche event is greater among Kikuyu
girls than among British girls; Kikuyu girls exhibited onset
of puberty an average of 2.9 years prior to reaching men-
arche, whereas this difference was only 2.3 years among
British girls. The slow rise of DHEAS production following
adrenarche observed here may reflect a comparableyet
independentbiological process, demonstrating a LH event
adaptively modified to address the harshness of the environ-
ment by ensuring survival prior to reproduction.
The relatively flat pattern observed among Ngandu chil-
dren is challenging to address, as is the higher DHEAS
concentrations among females. One potential explanation
may lie in the pattern of nutritional decline experienced by
Ngandu boys and girls across childhood (Helfrecht &
Meehan, 2016). The stress of malnutrition may be impacting
DHEAS production, as nutritional status is a hypothesized
factor influencing DHEAS production (Ong et al., 2004;
Remer & Manz, 1999; Shi, Wudy, Buyken, Hartmann, &
Remer, 2009). Although Ngandu girls experience significant
declines in their HAZ and WAZ scores between certain
developmental phases, Ngandu boys start and remain lower
across development (Helfrecht & Meehan, 2016). This may,
in part, explain girlshigher DHEAS concentrations, as their
HAZ scores (a measure of chronic nutritional stress) average
higher than those of boys. In contrast, however, Ngandu girls
have higher HCC, which would suggest the potential for
greater nutritional stress. It is possible that their HCC reflects
more acute nutritional stress, as their WAZ scores are more
comparable to those of boys, and girls often do more allocare
and resource acquisition while boys are in school. Additional
research including anthropometric and health measures, as
well as allostatic load, is necessary to better evaluate this
outcome.
Although this is the largest sample to date making use of
hair hormone analysis among children and in sub-Saharan
populations, the age range of the sample is not evenly distrib-
uted, which necessitates some caution when drawing conclu-
sions surrounding the youngest (<5 years old; n592 for
DHEAS) and oldest (12 and above; n575 for DHEAS) par-
ticipants (see also Figures 1 and 2). A wider age range is
needed to fully understand why DHEAS concentrations are so
high in young children, as well as to complete the picture of
DHEAS patterning in non-Western populations. Future
research should be longitudinal, commencing data collection
in infancy and extending into early adulthood in order to bet-
ter evaluate adrenal maturation and DHEAS patterning across
development. In addition, anthropometrics were not collected
in all three populations, preventing a deeper exploration into
the relationship between environment and genetics at this
time. Explorations of HAZ, WAZ, and BMIZ in relation to
hair DHEAS concentrations among Sidama children, how-
ever, indicated that these measures may be less predictive
than HCC (Helfrecht, 2016). We have here provided evidence
of the usefulness of this non-invasive method to explore ques-
tions related to biomarkers and life history, but it should be
noted that cultural or religious restrictions might make this
approach challenging in some regions, particularly in areas
where hair is associated with sorcery (such as in the Central
African Republic). Despite their beliefs in sorcery, Aka and
Ngandu parents and children were willing to participate and
no concerns were expressed by the communities.
This study offers a significant contribution towards un-
covering the full range of normalDHEAS and cortisol pat-
terning among populations where it has traditionally been
difficult to collect biomarkers. The variation from expected
trajectories of DHEAS production provides some of the first
empirical data on adaptive responses to the local environ-
ments during early and middle childhood. These data further
demonstrate the importance of situating development within
local ecocultural contexts and the need for continued
research in majority world populations.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
CH designed the study and collected the data with input
from CM, RB, EH, and SD. CM and SD provided logisti-
cal support. DDA and CH conducted the hair hormone
analyses. EH and CH conducted the statistical analyses.
CH authored the manuscript. CM, RB, EH, SD, and DDA
edited for intellectual content and provided critical feed-
back on the manuscript.
ORCID
Courtney L. Meehan http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2034-652X
REFERENCES
Alleyne, G. A. O., & Young, V. H. (1967). Adrenocortical function
in children with severe protein-calorie malnutrition. Clinical Sci-
ence,33, 189200.
Asefach, H., & Nigatu, R. (2008). Characterization of women under
polygamous marital relationship (an examination of socio-cultural
contexts of gender disparities through qualitative approach study
in Sidama Zone of Southern Ethiopia). The Oriental Anthropolo-
gist,8, 169179.
Ashenafi, T., Techalew, S., Mulugeta, M., Asrat, H., & Berhanu, E.
(2011). Schistosomiasis mansoni and soil-transmitted helminthia-
sis in Bushulo village, Southern Ethiopia. The Ethiopian Journal
of Health Development,25,4650.
Bahuchet, S. (1990). Food sharing among the pygmies of Central
Africa. African Study Monographs,11,2753.
Bahuchet, S., & Guillaume, H. (1982). Aka-farmer relations in the
northwest Congo Basin. In E. Leacock & R. B. Lee (Eds.),
12 of 17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
Politics and history in band societies (pp. 189211). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Bale, T. L., & Epperson, C. N. (2015). Sex differences and stress
across the lifespan. Nature Neuroscience,18, 14131420.
Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Ochsner, K. N., Bolder, M., Kolevzon, A., Lud-
wig, N., & Lydon, J. E. (2010). Effects of oxytocin on recollec-
tions of maternal care and closeness. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences,107, 2137121375.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience,
interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolu-
tionary theory of socialization. Child Development,62, 647670.
Bernstein, R. (2016). Hormones and the evolution of childhood in
human and nonhuman primates. In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crit-
tenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications
(pp. 103119). Santa Fe, NM: School of Advanced Research
(SAR)/University of New Mexico Press.
Boesch, M., Sefidan, S., Annen, H., Ehlert, U., Roos, L., Van Uum,
S., ... La Marca, R. (2015). Hair cortisol concentration is unaf-
fected by basic military training but related to sociodemographic
and environmental factors. Stress,18,3541.
Bogin, B. (2002). The evolution of human growth. In N. Cameron
(Ed.), Human growth and development (pp. 295320). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Bozzola, M., Travaglino, P., Marziliano, N., Meazza, C., Pagani, S.,
Grasso, M., ... Arbustini, E. (2009). The shortness of pygmies is
associated with severe under-expression of the growth hormone
receptor. Molecular Genetics & Metabolism,98, 310313.
Bribiescas, R. G. (1996). Testosterone levels among Ache hunter-
gatherer men: A functional interpretation of population variation
among adult males. Human Nature,7, 163188.
Br
ogger, J. (1986). Belief and experience among the Sidamo: A case
study towards an anthropology of knowledge. Oslo: Norwegian
University Press.
Burnham, T. C., Chapman, J. F., Gray, P. B., McIntyre, M. H., Lipson,
S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (2003). Men in committed, romantic relation-
ships have lower testosterone. Hormones & Behavior,44, 119122.
Campbell, B. C. (2006). Adrenarche and the evolution of human life
history. American Journal of Human Biology,18, 569589.
Campbell, B. C. (2011). Adrenarche and middle childhood. Human
Nature,22, 327349.
Campbell, B. C., Leslie, P. W., & Campbell, K. L. (2007). DHEAS among
Turkana men of Northern Kenya. The Aging Male,10, 203209.
Charnov, E. (1993). Life history invariants. Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Chen, Z., Li, J., Zhang, J., Xing, X., Gao, W., Lu, Z., & Deng, H.
(2013). Simultaneous determination of hair cortisol, cortisone and
DHEAS with liquid chromatography-electrospray ionization-tan-
dem mass spectrometry in negative mode. Journal of Chromatog-
raphy B,929, 187194.
Chisholm, J. S., Burbank, V. K., Coall, D. A., & Gemmiti, F. (2005).
Early stress: Perspectives from developmental evolutionary ecol-
ogy. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social
mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 76
107). New York: The Guilford Press.
Chrousos, G. P. (1998). Stressors, stress, and neuroendocrine integra-
tion of the adaptive response: The 1997 Hans Selye Memorial
Lecture. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,851, 311
335.
Cooper, G. A. A., Kronstrand, R., & Kintz, P. (2012). Society of hair
testing guidelines for drug testing in hair. Forensic Science Inter-
national,218,2024.
DAnna-Hernandez, K. L., Ross, R. G., Natvig, C. L., & Laudensl-
ager, M. L. (2011). Hair cortisol levels as a retrospective marker
of hypothalamic-pituitary axis activity throughout pregnancy:
Comparison to salivary cortisol. Physiology & Behavior,104,
348353.
Davenport, M. D., Tiefenbacher, S., Lutz, C. K., Novak, M. A., &
Meyer, J. S. (2006). Analysis of endogenous cortisol concentra-
tions in the hair of rhesus macaques. General & Comparative
Endocrinology,147, 255261.
De Peretti, E., & Forest, M. G. (1978). Pattern of plasma dehydroe-
piandrosterone sulfate levels in humans from birth to adulthood:
Evidence for testicular production. Journal of Clinical Endocri-
nology & Metabolism,47, 572577.
Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., & Manera, V. (2009). The juvenile
transition: A developmental switch point in human life history.
Developmental Review,29,131.
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive
calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobeha-
vioral Reviews,35, 15621592.
Dietz, W. H., Marino, B., Peacock, N. R., & Bailey, R. C. (1989).
Nutritional status of Efe pygmies and Lese horticulturalists. Amer-
ican Journal of Physical Anthropology,78, 509518.
Dohm, G. L. (1973). The prepuberal and puberal growth of the adre-
nal (adrenarche). Bietrage Zur Pathologie,150, 357377.
Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1982). Father absence and reproduc-
tive strategy: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Anthropo-
logical Research,38, 255273.
Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1988). A sociobiological perspective
on the development of human reproductive strategies. In K. B.
McDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human develop-
ment (pp. 340372). New York: Springer.
Ellis, B. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context.
Current Directions in Psychological Science,17, 183187.
Ellis, B. J., & Del Giudice, M. (2014). Beyond allostatic load:
Rethinking the role of stress in regulating human development.
Development & Psychopathology,26,120.
Ellis, B. J., & Essex, M. (2007). Family environments, adrenarche,
and sexual maturation: A longitudinal test of a life history model.
Child Development,78, 17991817.
Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation
in girlspubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence,
and marital and family stress. Child Development,71, 485501.
Ellis, B. J., Jackson, J., & Boyce, W. T. (2006). The stress response
systems: Universality and adaptive individual differences. Devel-
opmental Review,26, 175212.
Ellison, P. (2001). On fertile ground. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
Fernald, L. C., & Grantham-McGregor, S. M. (1998). Stress response
in school-age children who have been growth-retarded since early
childhood. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,68, 691698.
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
13 of 17
Flinn, M. (1999). Family environment, stress, and health during childhood.
In C. Panter-Brick & C. Worthman (Eds.), Hormones, health, and
behavior (pp. 105138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Flinn, M. (2006). Evolution and ontogeny of stress response to social
challenges in the human child. Developmental Review,26, 138
174.
Flinn, M., & Ward, C. (2005). Ontogeny and evolution of the social
child. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social
mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 19
44). New York: The Guilford Press.
Fourie, N. H., & Bernstein, R. M. (2011). Hair cortisol levels track
phylogenetic and age related differences in hypothalamic-
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity in non-human primates. Gen-
eral & Comparative Endocrinology,174, 150155.
Fourie, N. H., Brown, J. L., Jolly, C. J., Phillips-Conroy, J. E., Rog-
ers, J., & Bernstein, R. M. (2016). Sources of variation in hair
cortisol in wild and captive non-human primates. Zoology,119,
119125.
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Agustin, S. S., Feranil, A. B., &
Kuzawa, C. W. (2013). Do testosterone declines during the transi-
tion to marriage and fatherhood relate to mens sexual behavior?
Evidence from the Philippines. Hormones & Behavior,64, 755
763.
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., & Altham, P. M. E. (1998). Adrenal ste-
roid secretion and major depression in 8- to 16-year olds, III.
Influence of cortisol/DHEA ratio at presentation on subsequent
rates of disappointing life events and persistent major depression.
Psychological Medicine,28, 265273.
Goodyer, I. M., Park, R. J., Netherton, C. M., & Herbert, J. (2001).
Possible role of cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone in human
development and psychopathology. British Journal of Psychiatry,
179, 243249.
Gow, R., Thomson, S., Rieder, M., Van Uum, S., & Koren, G.
(2010). An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical
applications. Forensic Science International,196,3237.
Gray, P. B., & Campbell, B. C. (2009). Human male testosterone,
pair bonding and fatherhood. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray
(Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 270293). Cam-
bridge: Harvard University Press.
Groenveld, M. G., Vermeer, H. J., Linting, M., Noppe, G., van Ros-
sum, E. F. C., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2013). Childrens hair
cortisol as a biomarker of stress at school entry. Stress,16, 711
715.
Grunau, R. E., Cepeda, I. L., Chau, C. M. Y., Brummelte, S., Wein-
berg, J., Lavoie, P. M., ... Turvey, S. E. (2013). Neonatal pain-
related stress and NFKBIA genotype are associated with altered
cortisol levels in preterm boys at school age. PLOS One,8(9), 1
10.
Hamel, A. F., Meyer, J. S., Henchey, E., Dettmer, A. M., Suomi, S.
J., & Novak, M. A. (2011). Effects of shampoo and water wash-
ing on hair cortisol concentrations. Clinica Chimica Acta,412,
382385.
Hamer, J. H. (1987). Humane development: Participation and change
among the sad
ama of Ethiopia. Tuscaloosa, AL: The University
of Alabama Press.
Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (1986). Generalized additive models. Sta-
tistical Science,1, 297318.
Hechter, O., Grossman, A., & Chatterton, R. T. Jr. (1997). Relation-
ship of dehydroepiandrosterone and cortisol in disease. Medical
Hypotheses,49,8591.
Helfrecht, C. (2016). Effects of stress on onset of Middle childhood
among sidama agropastoralists [dissertation]. Pullman, WA:
Washington State University.
Helfrecht, C., & Meehan, C. L. (2016). Sibling effects on nutritional
status: Intersections of cooperation and competition across devel-
opment. American Journal of Human Biology,28, 159170.
Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers: The nature and context of
aka pygmy paternal infant care. Ann Arbor: University of Michi-
gan Press.
Hewlett, B. S. (1992). Husbandwife reciprocity and the fatherinfant
relationship among Aka pygmies. In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.), Father
child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 153176).
New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission
among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist,88, 922934.
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2002). Integrating evolution, culture,
and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant prox-
imity and responsiveness in Central Africa and the USA. In Y.
Keller, H. Poortinga, & A. Scholmerich (Eds.), Between culture
and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 241
269). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hewlett, B. S., Lamb, M. E., Leyendecker, B., & Scholmerick, A.
(2000). Internal working models, trust, and sharing among forag-
ers. Current Anthropology,41, 287297.
Hewlett, B. S., van de Koppel, J. M. H., & van de Koppel, M.
(1986). Causes of death among Aka pygmies of the Central Afri-
can Republic. In L. L. Cavalli Sforza (Ed.), African pygmies (pp.
4563). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory
and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology,28, 297
430.
Hui, X.-G., Akahira, J.-I., Suzuki, T., Nio, M., Nakamura, Y., Suzuki,
H., ... Sasano, H. (2009). Development of the human adrenal
zona reticularis: Morphometric and immunohistochemical studies
from birth to adolescence. Journal of Endocrinology,203, 241
252.
Ito, N., Ito, T., Kromminga, A., Bettermann, A., Takigawa, M., Kees,
F., ... Paus, R. (2015). Human hair follicles display a functional
equivalent of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and synthe-
size cortisol. The FASEB Journal,19, 13321334.
Jarvis, J. P., Scheinfeldt, L. B., Soi, S., Lambert, C., Omberg, L., Fer-
werda, B., ... Tishkoff, S. A. (2012). Patterns of ancestry, signa-
tures of natural selection, and genetic association with stature in
Western African pygmies. PLOS Genetics,8,115.
Jaya Rao, K. S., Srikantia, S. G., & Gopalan, C. (1968). Plasma corti-
sol levels in proteincalorie malnutrition. Archives of Disease in
Childhood,43, 365367.
Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J., & Robson, A. (2003). Embodied capital
and the evolutionary economics of the human life span. Popula-
tion & Development Review,29S, 152182.
14 of 17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
Karl
en, J., Ludvigsson, J., Frostell, A., Theodorsson, E., & Faresj
o,
T. (2011). Cortisol in hair measured in young adultsA bio-
marker of major life stressors? BMC Clinical Pathology,11, 12.
Karl
en, J., Frostell, A., Theodorsson, E., Faresj
o, T., & Ludvigsson,
J. (2013). Maternal influence on child HPA axis: A prospective
study of cortisol levels in hair. Pediatrics,132, e1333e1340.
Kintz, P., Cirimele, V., & Ludes, B. (1999). Physiological concentra-
tions of DHEA in human hair. Journal of Analytical Toxicology,
23, 424428.
Kudielka, B. M., & Kirschbaum, C. (2005). Sex differences in HPA
axis responses to stress: A review. Biological Psychology,69,
113132.
Kulin, H. E., Bwibo, N., Mutie, D., & Saniner, S. J. (1982). The
effect of chronic childhood malnutrition on pubertal growth and
development. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition,36,
527536.
Kurtis, J. D., Mtalib, R., Onyango, F. K., & Duffy, P. E. (2001).
Human resistance to Plasmodium falciparum increases during
puberty and is predicted by dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate levels.
Infection & Immunity,69, 123128.
Labrie, F., Luu-The, V., B
elanger, A., Lin, S.-X., Simard, J., Pelletier,
G., & Labrie, C. (2005). Is dehydroepiandrosterone a hormone?
Journal of Endocrinology,187, 169196.
Lancy, D., & Grove, M. A. (2011). Getting noticed: Middle child-
hood in cross-cultural perspective. Human Nature,22, 281302.
Lane, M. A., Ingram, D. K., Ball, S. S., & Roth, G. S. (1997). Dehy-
droepiandrosterone sulfate: A biomarker of primate aging slowed
by calorie restriction. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism,82, 20932096.
Laudenslager, M., Jorgensen, M. J., & Fairbanks, L. A. (2012).
Developmental patterns of hair cortisol in male and female nonhu-
man primates: Lower hair cortisol levels in vervet males emerge
at puberty. Psychoneuroendocrinology,10, 17361739.
Lee, H.-J., Macbeth, A. H., Pagani, J., & Young, W. S. (2009). Oxy-
tocin the great facilitator of life. Progress in Neurobiology,88,
127151.
Leenstra, T., ter Kuile, F. O., Kariuki, S. K., Nixon, C. P., Oloo, A.
J., Kager, P. A., & Kurtis, J. D. (2003). Dehydroepiandrosterone
sulfate levels associated with decreased malaria parasite density
and increased hemoglobin concentration in pubertal girls from
Western Kenya. Journal of Infectious Disease,188, 297304.
Majewska, M. D. (1995). Neuronal actions of dehydroepiandros-
terone: Possible roles in brain development, aging, memory,
and affect. New York Academy of Sciences Annals,774,111
120.
Martorell, R., & Ho, T. J. (1984). Malnutrition, morbidity, and mor-
tality. Population & Development Review,10(Suppl), 4968.
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: allostasis and
allostatic load. Annals New York Academy of Sciences,840,33
44.
McEwen, B. S., & Steller, E. (1993). Stress and the individual: Mech-
anisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine,153,
20932101.
Meehan, C. L. (2005). The effects of residential locality on parental
and alloparental investment among the Aka foragers of the Cen-
tral African Republic. Human Nature,6,6284.
Meehan, C. L. (2008). Allomaternal investment and relational uncer-
tainty among the Ngandu farmers of the Central African Republic.
Human Nature,19, 211226.
Meehan, C. L., Hagen, E. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (2017). Persistence in
infant care patterns among Aka foragers. In V. Reyes-García &
A. A. Pyhälä (Eds.), Hunter-gatherers in a changing world (pp.
213232). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
Meehan, C. L., Helfrecht, C., & Quinlan, R. (2014). Allomaternal
networks and childrens nutritional status: Is flexibility key?
American Journal of Physical Anthropology,153, 513525.
Meyer, J. S., & Novak, M. A. (2012). Minireview: Hair cortisol: A
novel biomarker of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical activity.
Endocrinology,153, 41204127.
Moore, S. E., Cole, T. J., Poskitt, E. M. E., Sonko, B. J., Whitehead,
R. G., McGregor, I. A., & Prentice, A. M. (1997). Season of birth
predicts mortality in rural Gambia. Nature,388, 434.
Nakamura, Y., Rainey, W. E., Kurotaki, Y., Hui, X.-G., & Sasano,
H. (2010). Development of the human adrenal zona reticularis
during adrenarche. Asia-Pacific Journal of Endocrinology,000,
15.
Nepomnaschy, P., & Flinn, M. V. (2009). Early life influences on the
ontogeny of neuroendocrine stress response in the human child. In
P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of social relation-
ships (pp. 364382). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Noppe, G., Van Rossum, E. F. C., Koper, J. W., Manenschijn, L.,
Bruining, G. J., de Rijke, Y. B., & van den Akker, E. L. T.
(2014). Validation and reference ranges of hair cortisol measure-
ment in healthy children. Hormone Research in Paediatrics,82,
97102.
Ong, K. K., Potau, N., Petry, C. J., Jones, R., Ness, A. R., Honour, J.
W., ... Dunger, D. B. (2004). The Avon Longitudinal Study of
Parents and Children (ALSPAC) Study Team. Opposing influen-
ces of prenatal and postnatal weight gain on adrenarche in normal
boys and girls. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism,
89, 26472651.
Orentreich, N., Brind, J. L., Rizer, R. L., & Vogelman, J. H. (1984).
Age changes and sex differences in serum dehydroepiandrosterone
sulfate concentrations throughout adulthood. Journal of Clinical
Endocrinology & Metabolism,59, 551555.
Parent, A.-S., Teilmann, G., Juul, A., Skakkebaek, N. E., Toppari, J.,
& Bourguignon, J.-P. (2003). The timing of normal puberty and
the age limits of sexual precocity: Variations around the world,
secular trends, and changes after migration. Endocrine Reviews,
24, 668693.
Parker, L. N. (1991). Adrenarche. Endocrinology & Metabolism Clin-
ics of North America,20,7183.
Parker, L. N. (1995). Control of DHEAS secretion. Seminars in
Reproductive Endocrinology,13, 275281.
Pelletier, D., & Frongillo, E. (2003). Changes in child survival are
strongly associated with changes in malnutrition in developing
countries. Journal of Nutrition,133, 107119.
Phillips, A. C., Carroll, D., Gale, C. R., Lord, J. M., Arlt, W., &
Batty, G. D. (2010). Cortisol, DHEAS, their ratio, and the meta-
bolic syndrome: Evidence from the Vietnam Experience Study.
European Journal of Endocrinology,162, 919923.
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
15 of 17
Piko, B. (2002). Socio-cultural stress in modern societies and the
myth of anxiety in Eastern Europe. Administration & Policy in
Mental Health,29, 275280.
Pratt, J. H., Manatunga, A. K., & Li, W. (1994). Familial influences
on the adrenal androgen excretion rate during the adrenarche.
Metabolism: Clinical & Experimental,43, 186189.
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and environmental risk.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,274
(1606), 121125.
Remer, T., & Manz, F. (1999). Role of nutritional status in the regu-
lation of adrenarche. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metab-
olism,84, 39363944.
Rippe, R. C. A., Noppe, G., Windhorst, D. A., Tiemeier, H., van
Rossum, E. F. C., Jaddoe, V. W. V., ... van den Akker, E. L. T.
(2016). Splitting hair for cortisol? Associations of socio-economic
status, ethnicity, hair color, gender and other child characteristics
with hair cortisol and cortisone. Psychoneuroendocrinology,66,
5664.
Rogoff, B., Sellers, M. J., Pirrotta, S., Fox, N., & White, S. H.
(1975). Age of assignment of roles and responsibilities to chil-
dren: A cross-cultural survey. Human Development,18, 353369.
Rotter, J. I., Wong, F. L., Lifrak, E. T., & Parker, L. N. (1985). A
genetic component to the variation of dehydroepiandrosterone sul-
fate. Metabolism: Clinical & Experimental,34, 731736.
Russell, E., Koren, G., Rieder, M., & Van Uum, S. (2012). Hair cor-
tisol as a biological marker of chronic stress: Current status,
future directions, and unanswered questions. Psychoneuroendocri-
nology,37, 589601.
Russell, E., Koren, G., Rieder, M., & Van Uum, S. H. M. (2014).
The detection of cortisol in human sweat: Implications for mea-
surement of cortisol in hair. Therapeutic Drug Monitoring,36,
3034.
Sapolsky, R. (1994). Why zebras dont get ulcers. New York: W.H.
Freeman and Company.
Sapolsky, R. (1999). Hormonal correlates of personality and social
contexts: From non-human to human primates. In C. Panter-Brick
& C. M. Worthman (Eds.), Hormones, health, and behavior (pp.
1846). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sauv
e, B., Koren, G., Walsh, G., Tokmakejian, S., & van Uum, S. H.
M. (2007). Measurement of cortisol in human hair as a biomarker
of systemic exposure. Clinical & Investigative Medicine,30(5),
E183E191.
Scrimshaw, N. S., Taylor, C. E., & Gordon, J. E. (1968). Interactions
of nutrition and infection (WHO Monograph Series No. 57).
Geneva: World Health Organization.
Shen, M., Xiang, P., Shen, B., Bu, J., & Wang, M. (2009). Physio-
logical concentrations of anabolic steroids in human hair. Foren-
sic Science International,184,3236.
Shi, L., Wudy, S. A., Buyken, A. E., Hartmann, M. F., & Remer, T.
(2009). Body fat and animal protein intakes are associated with
adrenal androgen secretion in children. American Journal of Clini-
cal Nutrition,90, 13211328.
Smith, I. F., Latham, M. C., Azubuike, J. A., Butler, W. R., Phillips,
L. S., Pond, W. G., & Enwonwu, C. O. (1981). Blood plasma
levels of cortisol, insulin, growth hormone and somatomedin in
children with marasmus, Kwashiorkor, and intermediate forms of
proteinenergy malnutrition. Experimental Biology & Medicine,
167, 607611.
Society of Hair Testing (1997). Statement of the Society of Hair Test-
ing concerning the examination of drugs in human hair. Forensic
Science International,84,36.
Stalder, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012). Analysis of cortisol in hair
State of the art and future directions. Brain, Behavior, & Immu-
nity,26, 10191029.
Stearns, S. (1976). Life history tactics: A review of the ideas. Quar-
terly Review of Biology,51,347.
Stearns, S. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Steudte, S., Kolassa, I.-T., Stalder, T., Pfeiffer, A., Kirschbaum, C.,
& Elbert, T. (2011). Increased cortisol concentrations in hair of
severely traumatized Ugandan Individuals with PTSD. Psycho-
neuroendocrinology,36, 11931200.
Sulcova, J., Hill, M., Hampl, R., & Starka, L. (1997). Age and sex
related differences in serum levels of unconjugated dehydroepian-
drosterone and its sulphate in normal subjects. Journal of Endo-
crinology,154,5762.
Takeuchi, K. (2005). The ambivalent symbiosis between the Aka
hunter-gatherers and neighboring farmers. In K. Takeuchi (Ed.),
Culture Conservation and Development in African Rain Forest
(Research report of MEXT Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research)
(pp. 1128). Toyama, Japan: Toyama University.
Vaghri, Z., Guhn, M., Weinberg, J., Grunau, R. E., Yu, W., & Hertz-
man, C. (2013). Hair cortisol reflects socio-economic factors and
hair zinc in preschoolers. Psychoneuroendocrinology,38, 331
340.
Van Uum, S. H. M., Sauv
e, B., Fraser, L. A., Morely-Forster, P.,
Paul, T. L., & Koren, G. (2008). Elevated content of cortisol in
patients with severe chronic pain: A novel biomarker for stress.
Stress,11, 483488.
Webb, E., Thomson, S., Nelson, A., White, C., Koren, G., Rieder,
M., & Van Uum, S. (2010). Assessing individual systemic stress
through cortisol analysis. Journal of Archaeological Science,37,
807812.
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
White, S. H. (1996). The childs entry into the age of reason.In A.
J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five- to seven-year shift
(pp. 1730). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
White, B. A., & Porterfield, S. P. (2013). Endocrine and reproductive
physiology (4th Ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Mosby.
World Health Organization. (2010). World Malaria Report: 2010.
Geneva: WHO Press.
Worthman, C. 1993. Biocultural interactions in human development.
In M. E. Pereira & L.A. Fairbanks (Eds.), Juvenile primates: Life
history, development, and behavior. (pp. 339358). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Worthman, C. (1999). Epidemiology of human development. In C.
Panter-Brick & C. M. Worthman (Eds.), Hormones, health, and
behavior: A socio-ecological and lifespan perspective (pp. 47
104). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
16 of 17
|
American Journal of Human Biology HELFRECHT ET AL.
Worthman, C. M., & Konner, M. (1987). Testosterone levels change
with subsistence hunting effort in! Kung San men. Psychoneur-
oendocrinology,12, 449458.
Yang, H. Z., Lan, J., Meng, Y. J., Wan, X. J., & Han, D. W. (1998).
A preliminary study of steroid reproductive hormones in human
hair. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry & Molecular Biology,67,
447450.
Yewelsew, A., Kennedy, T. S., Gates, G. E., & Stoecker, B. J. (2008).
Anthropometric assessment and maternal perceptions of infant well-
being in a feeding center in Sidama, Southern Ethiopia. Paper pre-
sented at the 17
th
Annual Conference of The Ethiopian Statistical
Association. Hawassa University, Awassa, Ethiopia. January 12,
2008.
Zemel, B. S., & Katz, S. H. (1986). The contribution of adrenal
and gonadal androgens to the growth in height of adolescent
males. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,71, 459
466.
How to cite this article: Helfrecht C, Hagen EH, DeA-
vila D, Bernstein RM, Dira SJ, Meehan CL. DHEAS
patterning across childhood in three sub-Saharan popu-
lations: Associations with age, sex, ethnicity, and corti-
sol. Am J Hum Biol. 2017;e23090. https://doi.org/10.
1002/ajhb.23090
HELFRECHT ET AL.American Journal of Human Biology
|
17 of 17
... Adrenarche, a biological event with onset around ages 6-8 years old (Campbell, 2006;Del Giudice et al., 2009), is defined by rising production of the steroid hormone dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulfate (DHEAS) in the zona reticularis of the adrenal gland, and may represent the first of these switch points that allow for adaptive modification of life history strategies. Although its timing is largely under genetic control (Del Giudice et al., 2009;Parker, 1995), there is significant intra-and inter-cultural variation (Del Giudice et al., 2011;Helfrecht et al., 2018;Pratt et al., 1994;Rotter et al., 1985). This suggests that, like menarche or spermarche (Gluckman & Hanson, 2006), environmental factors may influence the patterning of DHEA/S production. ...
... DHEA/S may also be tied to sex-specific neurocognitive development across middle childhood, prior to canalization in adolescence. Yet, it is unclear if this connection is demonstrable; data on adrenarche and DHEA/S patterning in non-industrialized populations are sparse, and what we have indicates divergence from known timing and patterning (Helfrecht et al., 2018;Houghton et al., 2014;Keestra et al., 2021). This challenge aligns with research suggesting that our focus on WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) societies has biased our understanding of human biology and its interaction with local ecologies (Gurven & Lieberman, 2020). ...
... In addition, Keestra and colleagues found no correlations between DHEAS concentrations and birth weight, body fat, or fat mass in their sample of 25 Mayan girls aged 7-9 years old. Our previous research among Sidama children in Ethiopia and Aka forager and Ngandu horticulturalist children in CAR suggests slightly later timing for onset of adrenarche, around ages 8-9 years old (Helfrecht et al., 2018); we hypothesized that this may be due to stress from high rates of malnutrition (UNICEF/WHO/World Bank Group, 2021) and/or high disease burden in these regions (Hewlett, 1991;Hewlett et al., 1986;Terefe et al., 2011). Further, production of DHEAS in these three populations differs considerably from known patterns (Helfrecht et al., 2018). ...
Article
Objective: Adrenarche, the biological event marked by rising production of dehydroepiandrosterone and its sulfate (DHEAS), may represent a sensitive period in child development, with important implications for adolescence and beyond. Nutritional status, particularly BMI and/or adiposity, has long been hypothesized as a factor in DHEAS production but findings are inconsistent, and few studies have examined this among non-industrialized societies. In addition, cortisol has not been included in these models. We here evaluate effects of height- (HAZ), weight- (WAZ), and BMI- (BMIZ) for-age on DHEAS concentrations among Sidama agropastoralist, Ngandu horticulturalist, and Aka hunter-gatherer children. Methods: Heights and weights were collected from 206 children aged 2-18 years old. HAZ, WAZ, and BMIZ were calculated using CDC standards. DHEAS and cortisol assays were used to determine biomarker concentrations in hair. Generalized linear modeling was used to examine effects of nutritional status on DHEAS concentrations, as well as cortisol, controlling for age, sex, and population. Results: Despite the prevalence of low HAZ and WAZ scores, the majority (77%) of children had BMI z-scores >-2.0 SD. Nutritional status has no significant effect on DHEAS concentrations, controlling for age, sex, and population. Cortisol, however, is a significant predictor of DHEAS concentrations. Conclusions: Our findings do not support a relationship between nutritional status and DHEAS. Instead, results suggest an important role for stress and ecology in DHEAS concentrations across childhood. Specifically, effects of environment via cortisol may be influential to patterning of DHEAS. Future work should investigate local ecological stressors and their relationship to adrenarche.
... In this study, the social and nutritional environments were not found to impact DHEAS production (Helfrecht et al., 2023), but these comparisons were broad and largely qualitative. Overall, results like these demonstrate the need for more adrenarche research outside of industrialized socioecological environments, in which there is generally less pathogenic and nutritional stress (Helfrecht et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Developmental plasticity is particularly important for humans and other primates because of our extended period of growth and maturation, during which our phenotypes adaptively respond to environmental cues. The hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) and hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axes are likely to be principal targets of developmental "programming" given their roles in coordinating fitness-relevant aspects of the phenotype, including sexual development, adult reproductive and social strategies, and internal responses to the external environment. In social animals, including humans, the social environment is believed to be an important source of cues to which these axes may adaptively respond. The effects of early social environments on the HPA axis have been widely studied in humans, and to some extent, in other primates, but there are still major gaps in knowledge specifically relating to males. There has also been relatively little research examining the role that social environments play in developmental programming of the HPG axis or the HPA/HPG interface, and what does exist disproportionately focuses on females. These topics are likely understudied in males in part due to the difficulty of identifying developmental milestones in males relative to females and the general quiescence of the HPG axis prior to maturation. However, there are clear indicators that early life social environments matter for both sexes. In this review, we examine what is known about the impact of social environments on HPG and HPA axis programming during male development in humans and nonhuman primates, including the role that epigenetic mechanisms may play in this programming. We conclude by highlighting important next steps in this research area. K E Y W O R D S developmental programming, hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis, hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal axis, male primates, social environment
... Было доказано, что кортизол чуть в меньшей степени, чем другие стероиды, обнаруживается в волосах человека или шерсти млекопитающих при использовании наборов для иммуноферментного анализа (ELISA) [46,14,61]. ...
Article
The steroid hormone cortisol is the end product of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Recently, cortisol has been considered as a potential biomarker for identifying stress, because it is directly related to the body›s response. The most common methods for determining endocrine status, including cortisol, are enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) and radioimmunoassay (RIA), as they are the most reliable and have high sensitivity. Cortisol levels are determined mainly in blood, saliva, serum and urine. The concentration of this hormone in these samples is related to the circadian rhythm and fluctuates throughout the day. Therefore, samples of this biomaterial allow the analysis of acute dynamic changes in cortisol. The effects of chronic stress require assessment of long-term stress levels. Hair (fur) cortisol concentrations are increasingly used as a biomarker of long-term stress. Hair cortisol analysis has thus been a major methodological advance because it provides retrospective assessment of cumulative levels over long periods of time (from days to months). Over the past decade, the validity of hair (fur) cortisol levels as an indicator of long-term levels has been established. In addition, this material is the most stable over time and resistant to environmental influences. Measuring hair cortisol levels is an innovative method for determining long-term increases in cortisol levels, independent of many methodological difficulties and not associated with other matrices. Interest in hair as an object of endocrine activity is due to various unique features that provide many advantages over other biomaterials.
... DHEA has a very specific curvilinear age pattern in adults, peaking in the late 20s and early 30s, before a lifelong decline (Labrie et al., 1997;Nafziger et al., 1998;Orentreich et al., 1984;Šulcová et al., 1997). Differences in the timing of maturational events leading to DHEA/S synthesis have been examined across populations, implicating differences in the timing related to ecology and stress (Helfrecht et al., 2018(Helfrecht et al., , 2023. However, few studies examine age profiles in non-industrialized populations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adrenal androgens like dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) are important to numerous aspects of health and psychosocial stress physiology. DHEA is responsive to stress, and previous studies have shown chronic stress can be associated with a reduction in DHEA. However, the large majority of this work has been conducted in resource-rich, industrialized societies, with few studies examining how adrenal androgens respond to stressors in environments with persistent resource related concerns. Here we examine the relationships between androgens and chronic psychosocial stress in a sample of Himba pastoralists, in order to determine the relationship between DHEA and stress in a resource-limited environment. We assayed DHEA and testosterone in 122 afternoon saliva samples from 46 Himba women aged 18–66, median age 30. Women also completed a chronic psychosocial stress survey, which included social, health, and resource related stressors reported over the past thirty days. DHEA concentrations show a curvilinear relationship with age, peaking in the mid-30s; testosterone was relatively flat across the life course. DHEA, but not testosterone, was negatively associated with chronic stress scores. In a comparison of question types, resource-related stressors showed the strongest relationship with DHEA. Our results support findings from previous studies conducted in industrialized societies, showing that chronic stress is associated with a reduction in DHEA concentrations. In contrast, salivary testosterone appears unrelated to chronic stress. Given the associations between DHEA and other aspects of health, better understanding of drivers of DHEA variability can elucidate linkages between stressors and health outcomes.
... These include capacity for learning and improved memory (Majewska, 1995), greater social orientation (Campbell, 2006), and increased attention to sex roles (Del Giudice et al., 2018). Timing of adrenarche varies within and between cultures, suggesting eco-cultural context has an important influence on its programming (Helfrecht et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Human ontogeny has been shaped through evolution, resulting in markers of physical, cognitive, and social development that are widely shared and often used to demarcate the lifespan. Yet, development is demonstrably biocultural and strongly influenced by context. As a result, emic age categories can vary in duration and composition, constituted by both common physical markers as well as culturally meaningful indicators, with implications for our understanding of the evolution of human life history. Semi-structured group interviews (n = 24) among Sidama adults and children, as well as individual interviews with children (n = 30), were used to identify age categories across the lifespan and to specifically investigate acquisition of sociocultural skills and cognitive development. Ten major age categories were identified, covering birth through death. These largely map onto patterning of human universals, but specific cultural beliefs and behaviors were indicated as important markers of development. Adults and children are oriented toward the dynamic relationships between physical development and acquisition of skills tied to social and cultural success. Culture, ecology, and ontogeny are co-determinants of human development, and the interactions among them should be considered in studies examining human life history and its evolution.
... Saliva Urine Milk Humans NHPs Energy regulation Adrenarche; developmental plasticity (Behringer et al., 2012;Helfrecht et al., 2018;Keestra et al., 2021) Epstein-Barr virus antibody (EBV-Ab) DBS Humans Immunity Energy allocation; cell-mediated immunity; life history evolution Eick et al., 2016;Urlacher, Ellison, et al., 2018) Estradiol DBS Feces Saliva Urine ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The use of minimally invasive biomarkers (MIBs – physiological biomarkers obtained from minimally invasive sample types) has expanded rapidly in science and medicine over the past several decades. The MIB approach is a methodological strength in the field of human and non-human primate evolutionary biology (HEB). Among humans and our closest relatives, MIBs provide unique opportunities to document phenotypic variation and to operationalize evolutionary hypotheses. Aims: This paper overviews the use of MIBs in HEB. Our objectives are to (1) highlight key research topics which successfully implement MIBs, (2) identify promising yet under-investigated areas of MIB application, and (3) discuss current challenges in MIB research, with suggestions for advancing the field. Discussion and Conclusions: A range of MIBs are used to investigate focal topics in HEB, including energetics and life history variation/evolution, developmental plasticity, and social status and dominance relationships. Nonetheless, we identify gaps in existing MIB research on traits such as physical growth and gut function that are central to the field. Several challenges remain for HEB research using MIBs, including the need for additional biomarkers and methods of assessment, robust validations, and approaches that are standardized across labs and research groups. Importantly, researchers must provide better support for adaptation and fitness effects in hypothesis testing (e.g., by obtaining complementary measures of energy expenditure, demonstrating redundancy of function, and performing lifetime/longitudinal analyses). We point to continued progress in the use of MIBs in HEB to better understand the past, present, and future of humans and our closest primate relatives.
Article
Context Adrenarche marks the timepoint of human adrenal development when the cortex starts secreting androgens in increasing amounts, in healthy children at age 8–9 years, with premature adrenarche (PA) earlier. Because the molecular regulation and significance of adrenarche are unknown, this prepubertal event is characterized descriptively, and PA is a diagnosis by exclusion with unclear long-term consequences. Evidence Acquisition We searched the literature of the past 5 years, including original articles, reviews, and meta-analyses from PubMed, ScienceDirect, Web of Science, Embase, and Scopus, using search terms adrenarche, pubarche, DHEAS, steroidogenesis, adrenal, and zona reticularis. Evidence Synthesis Numerous studies addressed different topics of adrenarche and PA. Although basic studies on human adrenal development, zonation, and zona reticularis function enhanced our knowledge, the exact mechanism leading to adrenarche remains unsolved. Many regulators seem involved. A promising marker of adrenarche (11-ketotestosterone) was found in the 11-oxy androgen pathway. By current definition, the prevalence of PA can be as high as 9% to 23% in girls and 2% to 10% in boys, but only a subset of these children might face related adverse health outcomes. Conclusion New criteria for defining adrenarche and PA are needed to identify children at risk for later disease and to spare children with a normal variation. Further research is therefore required to understand adrenarche. Prospective, long-term studies should characterize prenatal or early postnatal developmental pathways that modulate trajectories of birth size, early postnatal growth, childhood overweight/obesity, adrenarche and puberty onset, and lead to abnormal sexual maturation, fertility, and other adverse outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Objective Few large-scale studies have provided population-based estimates of hair cortisol levels and its determinants. Hair cortisol and potential determinants were measured in children and their mothers in a population-based sample in a Brazilian city with large variations in socioeconomic conditions. Methods We used data from the 4-year follow-up of the 2015 Pelotas (Brazil) Birth Cohort Study. Hair samples were collected by trained fieldworkers to analyze average levels of cortisol over a 3-month period. Four groups of variables were tested as potential determinants: hair characteristics (natural color, treatment, type, and frequency of wash), use of corticosteroids and oral contraceptives, sociodemographic factors (sex, age, skin color, socioeconomic level, maternal relationship, pregnancy, daycare enrollment), maternal perceived stress, and substance exposure (smoking and illicit drug use). Linear regression with log transformation was used to test associations. Results 3,235 children and 3,102 mothers were analyzed (80.7% and 77.4% of those interviewed when children were 4 years of age, respectively), for whom sufficient hair was collected for cortisol analysis. The median of hair cortisol concentration was 7.8 pg/mg (IQR = 5.6 – 11.0) for children, and 5.6 pg/mg (IQR = 4.2 – 7.8) for mothers. In adjusted models, sex and socioeconomic level were associated with child cortisol levels. For mothers, hair cortisol levels were associated with socioeconomic level, skin color, age, hair treatment and hair natural color. Conclusion This study provides estimates of hair cortisol levels in a diverse population in a upper-middle income country. Although just a few predictors were associated with maternal/child cortisol levels, socioeconomic level was the key variable that should be incorporated in studies using hair cortisol to measure biological manifestations of stress, but other variables, such as some hair and sociodemographic characteristics are important to consider when using hair cortisol.
Chapter
As widespread social transformations have been paralleled by gains in health and life expectancy through public health and other improvements, a variety of other challenges to health have emerged, particularly in lifestyle related, behaviourally mediated changes in rates of chronic disease. Hormones, Health and Behavior looks at the relationship of human biology and human society at the intersection of behavior, hormones and health. There is both scientific interest and practical urgency behind the ideas and findings presented here, as the need for a socio-ecological view of function and well-being has become more apparent. This book documents an emerging understanding of how hormones create linkage between behavior or social life and health. It will inform graduate students and researchers interested in human sciences, human development, anthropology, epidemiology, public environmental and reproductive health.
Chapter
As widespread social transformations have been paralleled by gains in health and life expectancy through public health and other improvements, a variety of other challenges to health have emerged, particularly in lifestyle related, behaviourally mediated changes in rates of chronic disease. Hormones, Health and Behavior looks at the relationship of human biology and human society at the intersection of behavior, hormones and health. There is both scientific interest and practical urgency behind the ideas and findings presented here, as the need for a socio-ecological view of function and well-being has become more apparent. This book documents an emerging understanding of how hormones create linkage between behavior or social life and health. It will inform graduate students and researchers interested in human sciences, human development, anthropology, epidemiology, public environmental and reproductive health.
Article
Chapter
Early research found commonalities across hunter-gatherer communities in regards to infant care. Infants were held frequently, spent most of the day and night in skin-to-skin contact, breastfed on demand, received intimate caregiving, and experienced low levels of distress. These shared infant care practices are assumed to have deep evolutionary roots and together serve as a defining characteristic of hunter-gatherer life. Yet, the degree to which hunter-gatherers are experiencing social, economic, and environmental change may have created conditions that alter these defining characteristics. In this chapter we explore infant care among the Aka foragers in the Central African Republic. We utilize naturalistic behavioral observations collected over almost 20 years to examine whether care patterns have persisted or changed over the course of a generational timespan. The Aka discussed in this chapter remain a mobile foraging population, but they have also experienced change at multiple levels. Whether or how these changes influence infant care has not yet been examined. Results indicate that Aka infant care patterns have persisted. Although some significant changes were noted, not all caregiving showed a decline, some increased and some changes were only limited to one age category. Additionally, the vast majority of caregiving practices remained constant across the periods. Cross-cultural data presented also support this finding. Aka infant care patterns are comparable to other hunter-gatherer populations and the frequency of care and interactions match or exceed most farming and Euro-American patterns. We argue that Aka infant care patterns have persisted because they are vertically transmitted and highly-conserved. Moreover, Aka infant care is indicative of a much larger cultural pattern of trust, intimacy and sharing, not immediately affected by outside influences.
Article
Background: Hair cortisol analysis has been shown to be an effective measure of chronic stress. Cortisol is assumed to incorporate into hair via serum, sebum, and sweat sources; however, the extent to which sweat contributes to hair cortisol content is unknown. Methods: Sweat and saliva samples were collected from 17 subjects after a period of intensive exercise and analyzed by salivary enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). Subsequently, an in vitro test on exposure of hair to hydrocortisone was conducted. Residual hair samples were immersed in a 50-ng/mL hydrocortisone solution for periods lasting 15 minutes to 24 hours, followed by a wash or no-wash condition. Hair cortisol content was determined using our modified protocol for a salivary ELISA. Results: Postexercise control sweat cortisol concentrations ranged from 8.16 to 141.7 ng/mL and correlated significantly with the log-transformed time of day. Sweat cortisol levels significantly correlated with salivary cortisol concentrations. In vitro hair exposure to a 50-ng/mL hydrocortisone solution (mimicking sweat) for 60 minutes or more resulted in significantly increased hair cortisol concentrations. Washing with isopropanol did not affect immersion-increased hair cortisol concentrations. Conclusions: Human sweat contains cortisol in concentrations comparable with salivary cortisol levels. This study suggests that perfuse sweating after intense exercise may increase cortisol concentrations detected in hair. This increase likely cannot be effectively decreased with conventional washing procedures and should be considered carefully in studies using hair cortisol as a biomarker of chronic stress.