ArticlePDF Available

Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic: Implications for formal representations

Authors:
  • Université Paris Cité/Laboratoire de Linguistique Formelle

Abstract and Figures

This exploratory study of Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic (JA and MA) aims to evaluate whether pharyngealization is associated with an epilaryngeal constriction which causes 'retraction' and tense voice quality in surrounding vowels, following the Laryngeal Articulator Model (LAM) (Esling, 2005). Twenty male speakers (10 per dialect) produced vowels preceded by /d or dˤ/. Thirteen acoustic correlates obtained at the onset and midpoint were used to assess this type of constriction. A predictive modeling approach was used; starting with Bayesian Generalized Linear Mixed Effects modeling followed by Conditional Random Forest for classification. Vowels in the pharyngealized context were more open (higher F1, Z1-Z0), more back (lower F2, higher Z3-Z2), more compact (lower Z2-Z1), and showed spectral divergence (higher Z3-Z2). Voice quality results showed these vowels to be produced with a tense voice. High classification rates of 93.5% for JA and 91.1% for MA were obtained and variable importance score showed formant-based measures outperform voice quality ones. This suggests pharyngealization has 'retraction,' with a back and down gesture, as a primary correlate followed by [+constricted glottis]. The implications of these results provide strong support for LAM, the feature [+cet], and the use of the epilarynx to describe pharyngealization.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Al-Tamimi, J. 2017 Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic: Implications for formal representations.
Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology
8(1): 28, pp. 1–40, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.19
lab
la
phon
Journal of the Association for
Laboratory Phonology
Laboratory Phonology
hon
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization
in Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic: Implications for
formal representations
Jalal Al-Tamimi
Speech and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE1 7RU, UK
Jalal.Al-Tamimi@newcastle.ac.uk
This exploratory study of Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic (JA and MA) aims to evaluate whether
pharyngealization is associated with an epilaryngeal constriction which causes ‘retraction’
and tense voice quality in surrounding vowels, following the Laryngeal Articulator Model (LAM)
(Esling, 2005). Twenty male speakers (10 per dialect) produced vowels preceded by /d or d/.
Thirteen acoustic correlates obtained at the onset and midpoint were used to assess this type of
constriction. A predictive modeling approach was used; starting with Bayesian Generalized Linear
Mixed Effects modeling followed by Conditional Random Forest for classification. Vowels in the
pharyngealized context were more open (higher F1, Z1-Z0), more back (lower F2, higher Z3-Z2),
more compact (lower Z2-Z1), and showed spectral divergence (higher Z3-Z2). Voice quality results
showed these vowels to be produced with a tense voice. High classification rates of 93.5% for JA
and 91.1% for MA were obtained and variable importance score showed formant-based measures
outperform voice quality ones. This suggests pharyngealization has ‘retraction,’ with a back and
down gesture, as a primary correlate followed by [+ ]. The implications of
these results provide strong support for LAM, the feature [+], and the use of the epilarynx to
describe pharyngealization.
Keywords: acoustics of pharyngealization; tense/pressed voice; constricted epilarynx; Bayesian
GLMM; Random Forests; Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
1 Introduction
Pharyngealization (or emphasis) in Arabic is generally assumed to involve retraction of
the tongue dorsum towards the upper pharyngeal area, which leads to a lowering of the
second formant in the surrounding vowels (e.g., Bin-Muqbil, 2006; Ghazeli, 1977; Watson,
2007; Zawaydeh, 1999; Zawaydeh & de Jong, 2011). Although these characteristics are
mostly agreed upon, pharyngealization is also associated with a retracted epiglottis, a
raised larynx, a pressed/tense voice quality, and/or a protruded lip posture (see e.g.,
Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood, 2011; Cantineau, 1960; Hess, 1998; Laufer & Baer, 1988;
Lehn, 1963; Zeroual & Clements, 2015; Zeroual et al., 2011, among others). Although
located near the constriction observed for ‘true’ pharyngeals, authors claimed that the two,
i.e., ‘true’ pharyngeals and pharyngealization, share the same place but vary in degree of
constriction (e.g., Laufer & Baer, 1988). Hence, and following the Laryngeal Articulator
Model (Esling, 2005), both will share an epilaryngeal constriction that may be exhibited
      
back and down in a one combined gesture and causes the vowels to be produced with the
‘retracted’ quality (Esling, 2005; Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b). A secondary
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 2 of 40
consequence of an epilaryngeal constriction causes a change in the voice source with an
increase in harmonic amplitude especially in high frequencies (Halle & Stevens, 1969;
Laver, 1980; Moisik, 2013b; Moisik & Esling, 2010; Stevens, 1977, 1998; Story, 2016).
This increase is generally associated with a tense/creaky/harsh voice quality (Edmondson
& Esling, 2006; Kuang & Keating, 2012, 2014; Moisik, 2013b). Our aim in this study is to
provide a complete analysis of the acoustics of Arabic pharyngealized plosives in order
to evaluate the presence of acoustic evidence for epilaryngeal constriction. In addition to
   
metrics as an alternative correlate to the articulatory ‘retracted’ vowel quality that is
associated with an epilaryngeal constriction (Esling, 2005). Acoustic correlates of voice

voice quality as a consequence of an epilaryngeal constriction.
The paper is organized as follows. The sections following this introduction provide an
overview of the literature on the phonetics of pharyngealization, with special attention on
the consequences of epilaryngeal constrictions, before outlining the goals of the current
study. Section 2 presents the method used in this study including speakers, dialects, and
the corpus from which they were drawn, as well as the acoustic analyses and statistical
design. Section 3 presents the results, starting with the most typically used acoustic

and acoustic correlates of voice quality. Finally, this section presents an exploratory

both dialects. The last section ends with a discussion of the results and their implications
for the current descriptions of pharyngealization. It is hoped that these accounts will open
the door to further research into the role of the laryngeal activity that is associated with
pharyngealization and pharyngeals in general.
1.1 Correlates of pharyngealization
Pharyngealization is a secondary articulation that involves a constriction located in the
pharyngeal area that causes retraction of the body and root of the tongue towards the

linguo-pharyngeal whereby the root of the tongue, including the epiglottis, moves backwards
to narrow the pharynx in the front-back dimension. In many, if not all, languages, a
narrowing of the pharyngeal passage near the tip of the epiglottis, and a raised larynx are
the direct consequences of this constriction (see e.g., Catford, 1977, p. 193; Ladefoged
& Maddieson, 1996, p. 307). Although this secondary articulation is pertinent to vowels
(Ladefoged & Maddieson, 1996, p. 306), in Arabic and many Semitic languages, however,
    
(Laver, 1994).
In Arabic, pharyngealization is associated with consonants that have a primary


           muṭbaqa
 mustaliya  mufaḫḫama
(Bin-Muqbil, 2006; Cantineau, 1960; Jakobson, 1957/1962; Khattab et al., 2006; Lehn,
   muṭbaqa 
which describes their articulation as being ‘covered’ or ‘lidded’ (Khattab et al., 2006);
‘spread and with a raised tongue’ (Lehn, 1963) and/or with a ‘pressed voice’ (Cantineau,
1960, p. 23). The second term, mustaliya


Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 3 of 40

grammarians as consonants with a ‘pressed voice’ while the others are not (Cantineau, 1960,
pg. 23–24). The last term used by Arab grammarians is mufaḫḫama
‘thick, heavy’ consonants, which describes all the consonants with an ‘emphatic’ acoustic
impression that seem to block Imāla
             
         
  

articulation, expressed as ‘pressed voice,’ as they were described separately from other
consonants that may share some of their articulatory features. As will be seen in the next
section, none of the studies reported below have attempted to explore the nature of this
‘pressed voice’ quality, hence our aim is to evaluate its acoustic consequences.
1.1.1 Articulatory-acoustic correlates
Based on perturbation theory and the acoustic theory of speech production (Carré &
           
Johnson, 2012; Mrayati et al., 1988; Stevens, 1989), articulatory to acoustic mapping
can be used to evaluate the acoustic consequences of constriction location. A pharyngeal
constriction causes a combined high F1 and low F2 due to the constriction being close
to a node for F1 and an antinode for F2, leading to respectively a rising and a lowering
of their natural frequencies. This correlates well with the quantal region located in the
pharynx (Stevens, 1989). When a pharyngeal constriction is associated with roundness/
lip protrusion, as correlates of pharyngealization in general, one would expect a closer
proximity between F1 and F2 and a lower F3. This is especially pertinent for front vowels

(Fant, 1960/1971; Lindblom & Sundberg, 1971; Stevens, 1989; Wood, 1986). Back vowels


with the back cavity; an increased constriction in the pharynx leads to an increase in
the length of the front cavity, and subsequently an increase in F3 (Stevens, 1989). This
increase in F3 can be caused by either a narrower constriction in the upper pharynx
constriction (Stevens & Keyser, 1989, p. 101) or due to a tighter tongue constriction in the
pharyngealized context (see e.g., Fant, 2004, p. 43; Lindblom & Sundberg, 1971, p. 1175).



These predicted consequences are evaluated empirically. The exact location of the
constriction responsible for producing pharyngealization in Arabic varies from a (post-)
velar to a (mid-)low pharyngeal (Khattab et al., 2006). Many researchers estimate this
constriction to be located towards the posterior pharyngeal wall in the vicinity of the upper
pharynx near the uvula and designate it only with tongue dorsum retraction (e.g., Bin-
Muqbil, 2006; Ghazeli, 1977; Hassan & Esling, 2011; Zawaydeh, 1999; Zawaydeh & de Jong,
2011). This retraction causes lowering of the second formant in the vowels surrounding
pharyngealized consonants and this is believed to be the main acoustic correlate to the
contrast (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Al-Masri & Jongman, 2004; Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood,
2011; Al-Tamimi, J. & Barkat-Defradas, 2003; Barkat-Defradas et al., 2003; Ghazeli, 1977;
Jongman et al., 2011; Khattab et al., 2006; Laufer & Baer, 1988; Obrecht, 1968; Shahin,
1996, 1997; Zawaydeh, 1999; Zawaydeh & de Jong, 2011; Zeroual et al., 2011, inter
alia). Given the ‘pharyngeal’ component of pharyngealization, a few studies reported a
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 4 of 40

Heselwood, 2011; Al-Tamimi, J. & Barkat-Defradas, 2003; Barkat-Defradas et al., 2003;
Ghazeli, 1977; Jongman et al., 2011; Khattab et al., 2006; Laufer & Baer, 1988; Shahin,
   
for F2, and this led many researchers to consider only F2 as the main acoustic correlate
to pharyngealization in Arabic (e.g., McCarthy, 1994; Watson, 2007). The frequency of
the third formant (F3) was reported to be overall higher in the pharyngealized context
and sometimes dependent on the vowel quality; a higher F3 was reported with back
      


      

by the slight lip-rounding/protrusion and/or sulcalization of the tongue body reported in
the literature (Khattab et al., 2006).
Given the proximity between F1 and F2 observed in ‘true’ pharyngeals and
pharyngealization, it was used as an acoustic correlate of the auditory impression of
‘darkness’ or ‘heaviness’ reported in the literature, with pharyngeals obtaining Z2-Z1
(or F2-F1 in Bark) distance below the 3.5 Z threshold for formant merging, whereas
pharyngealized consonants were slightly above this threshold (close to 4.5 Z) (e.g.,
Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood, 2011; Heselwood & Al-Tamimi, F., 2011). This proximity

correlate to the separation between pharyngealized and non-pharyngealized consonants
in Arabic, either at the midpoint in Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic (Al-Tamimi, J., 2002;
Al-Tamimi, J. & Barkat-Defradas, 2003; Barkat-Defradas et al., 2003) or at the onset in
Moroccan Arabic (Yeou, 2001). We will be using this (and other) formant proximity
measures as correlates of pharyngealization.

investigated the acoustic and perceptual correlates responsible for distinguishing between
uvular and pharyngeal consonants. At the onset of the vowel, pharyngeals were associated
with a higher F1 and lower F2 frequencies, an increased bandwidth of the F2 (through
an estimated A2-H1 value), and a smaller distance between F2-F1, whereas uvulars had
a relatively stable F1 and slight raising of F2, a larger distance between F2-F1 and an
increased bandwidth of F1 (through an estimated A1-H1 value). These same results were
  

            
separation between pharyngeals and uvulars and thus we will be using it as a potential
correlate for pharyngealization.
1.1.2 Epiglotto-laryngeal articulation
Although it is assumed that pharyngealization is accompanied by retraction of the
tongue body, a few studies reported a constriction much lower in the vowel tract. Using
various articulatory techniques, researchers have shown pharyngealized consonants to
be produced with the epiglottis forming a constriction with the pharyngeal wall in the
vicinity of the middle or lower pharynx that is also accompanied by tongue root/epiglottis
retraction and larynx raising (e.g., Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood, 2011; Laufer & Baer,
1988; Zeroual & Clements, 2015; Zeroual et al., 2011). It is not clear then whether
    
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 5 of 40
pharyngealized consonants in Arabic have tongue root retraction and larynx raising that
leads to a tense/pressed voice quality. In her comparative study of tongue root activity in
various languages including Arabic using factor analyses of Ghazeli’s (1977) x-ray data,
Hess (1998) showed that the pharyngealized set in Arabic is better characterized by an
upper pharyngeal constriction. However, her results showed that the pharyngealized set
(both consonants and surrounding vowels) is also accompanied by tongue root retraction
(through the Laryngopharyngeal Constriction Factor), partial constriction at the pocket of
the epiglottis (through the Radical Constriction Factor), raising of the larynx, and widening
of the pharyngeal wall (through the Pharynx Shifting Factor) (Hess, 1998, p. 233). In fact,

explained that in addition to the “slight retraction, lateral spreading, and concavity of the
tongue and raising of its back (what has been called velarization),” emphatic consonants
are associated with “faucal and pharyngeal constriction (pharyngealization),” “slight lip
protrusion or rounding (labialization),” and/or “increased tension of the entire oral and
pharyngeal musculature resulting in the emphatics being noticeably more fortis than the
plain segments.”
More recently, the same patterns of tongue root/epiglottis retraction and larynx raising
of pharyngealized consonants were observed by Al-Tamimi, F. and Heselwood (2011)

acoustic data and by Zeroual et al. (2011) and Zeroual and Clements (2015) on 2–3
speakers of Moroccan Arabic using nasoendoscopic, ultrasound, EMA, and acoustic data.
         
and pharyngealized consonants while examining emphasis spread in the productions of 1

pharyngeal consonants showed aryepiglottic sphinctering, tongue root retraction, and
larynx raising as a direct consequence of a laryngeal constriction, while pharyngealized
consonants only showed tongue body retraction and raising accompanied by larynx lowering.

these discrepancies. Another possibility is related to the fact that the pharyngealized set
is produced with retraction of the tongue dorsum and/or root, whereas pharyngeal sets
have a retraction of the tongue root as an enhancement of the epilaryngeal constriction

Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014b). A slight larynx raising and/or constriction in the
pharyngealized vowels and consonants in Arabic compared to pharyngeals may still be

degree of larynx raising and/or constriction in pharyngeal and pharyngealized sets is an
important point to investigate further both articulatorily and acoustically in order to shed
light to the exact nature of the production of these consonants.
Table 1 provides a summary of the main articulatory and acoustics correlates as observed
in the literature, with the additional acoustic correlates as investigated in this study. It
aims at providing a correlation between articulatory and acoustic correlates in order to
highlight the missing acoustic correlates of the laryngeal activity.

show that its articulatory correlates form a complex picture with retraction of part(s) of
the tongue (dorsum and/or root), retraction of the epiglottis, raising and/or constriction of
the larynx leading to a more tense or pressed voice quality, and lip rounding/protrusion.
      
             
             
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 6 of 40
there do not seem to be any accounts of the acoustic correlates of the tense or pressed
articulation, and/or raised larynx, which are usually measured in terms of articulation
(although see our own research Al-Tamimi, J., 2014, 2015). The next section introduces
more in-depth account of the consequences of an epilaryngeal constriction following the
Laryngeal Articulator Model.
1.2 Epilaryngeal constriction
Both classic and more recent articulatory reports of pharyngealization and ‘true’
pharyngeals sharing the same constriction location lead to the conclusion that articulatorily
speaking, pharyngealization in Arabic is produced by constricting the epilaryngeal tube.
This, in turn, has a direct consequence on retracting the tongue body due to tongue
root retraction, with a concomitant larynx raising and/or constriction. This is the view
developed in the Laryngeal Articulator Model on the type of constriction seen in the
epilarynx (Esling, 2005). This model was extensively modeled by Moisik (2013a) and
typologically and formally evaluated in Sylak-Glassman (2014a). According to Moisik
(2013a, p. 84), the lower vocal tract “is bounded inferiorly by the glottis and superiorly
by the oropharyngeal isthmus and velo-pharyngeal port.” The epilarynx is located within
the lower vocal tract above the larynx and has the ventricular folds as its lower part and
the rim formed by the epiglottis and aryepiglottic folds as its upper part (Moisik et al.,
2012). Pharyngeals in general (and potentially pharyngealized consonants in particular)
are produced by sphincterally constricting the epilarynx through constricting the
intrinsic or the extrinsic laryngeal muscles (Esling, 2005; Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman,
2014a, b); tongue retraction is caused by this constriction and is seen as a facilitator and
an enhancer of the pharyngeal articulation (Esling, 2005; Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman,
2014a, b). Constriction of the hyoglossus muscle draws the tongue as a whole backward
and downward and leads to retraction of the tongue root and dorsum (Moisik, 2013a,

‘retracted’; they are produced by a back and down gesture, which partially matches the
consequences we see with pharyngealization.
The constriction as seen in the laryngeal area, and particularly in the epilarynx, has
direct consequences on the quality of the sounds produced; ventricular folds couple with
the vocal folds and are brought down to the glottis to allow for creaky phonation to occur
(Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b) and when constricted they are associated with
laryngealization, tense and harsh voice quality (Edmondson & Esling, 2006; Stevens,
1977). Aryepiglottic fold constriction is associated with an enhanced and clearer voice
quality especially in singing due to an increased energy in the higher frequencies as well as
Table 1: List of articulatory and acoustic correlates of the effects of pharyngealization on the
surrounding vowels as highlighted in the literature and the supplementary correlates used in
the current study.
Articulatory Acoustic Additional correlates
Retraction F2 F3-F2
Open F1 F1-F0
Narrow/compact F2-F1, F3 / F3-F2
Roundness/lip-protrusion F1 & F2 & F3 F2-F1, / F3-F2
Raised larynx F1, F2 & F3 Spectral slope
Pressed/tense voice Spectral slope
Epilaryngeal Constriction Amplitude upper harmonics
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 7 of 40

fold constriction is observed in the production of ‘true’ pharyngeal consonants (Esling,
2005) and a few studies reported ‘trilling’ as a direct consequence of ‘true’ pharyngeal


and pharyngealization; the former is produced by constricting the aryepiglottic folds as
a primary feature whereas the latter by constricting the ventricular folds as a secondary
feature. Pharyngeals were described as having tongue root retraction and lowering,
epiglottal retraction, in addition to larynx raising as one component (Esling, 2005;
Hassan & Esling, 2011; Moisik, 2013a)—descriptions that seem to match the few reports
of pharyngealization in Arabic (see Section 1.1.2). This seems to suggest that the whole
guttural class, including pharyngeal, pharyngealized, and uvular consonants, is produced
with an epilaryngeal constriction, albeit to varying degrees of stricture and phonation
(Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b).
1.2.1 Acoustic consequences
From an acoustic point of view, and given that the vowels in the vicinity of pharyngealized
consonants are better described as ‘retracted’ (Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b), we will
advocate the use of the proximity between formants as an (psycho-)acoustic correlate.

perception, and normalization (e.g., Syrdal & Gopal, 1986; Thomas & Kendall, 2007,
inter alia). The proximity between Z1-Z0 (that is F1-f0 in Bark) correlates well with the
openness dimension (i.e., [±
& Gopal, 1986; Traunmüller, 1981), with more close ([+
lower than 3 Bark, and more open ([–
p. 1090). Z2-Z1 correlates well with compactness of the specturm (Sylak, 2011; Syrdal
& Gopal, 1986) and is highest for front vowels, and lowest for (mid-)open back vowels

open back and back vowels and the smallest for front vowels and correlates well with
the backness/retraction of vowels (Syrdal & Gopal, 1986, p. 1090). Z3-Z2 correlates well

/i/ to distinguish it from /y/ (Wood, 1986). Thus we also hypothesize that a large Z3-Z2
  
formant merging observed in Z2-Z1 due to the pharyngeal constriction. Hence we expect
the vowels in the vicinity of pharyngealized consonants to show a higher Z1-Z0, a lower
Z2-Z1, and a higher Z3-Z2.
Voice source changes occur with an epilaryngeal constriction. Constricting the
ventricular folds leads to a tense, pressed, or laryngealized voice quality with an overall

& Stevens, 1969; Hanson et al., 2001; Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Laver, 1980, 1994; Moisik,
2013b; Moisik & Esling, 2010; Stevens, 1977, 1998; Sundberg & Askenfelt, 1981). When
an epilaryngeal constriction is caused by an aryepiglottic fold constriction an ‘enhanced’
voice quality, especially in singing, can be seen (Moisik & Esling, 2010; Story, 2016;
Titze, 2008; Titze & Story, 1997). Samlan and Kreiman (2014) described the acoustic and
perceptual consequences of constricting the epilarynx at either the aryepiglottic or the


constrictions. We will be investigating spectral slope measures that are widely used as an
acoustic evaluation of phonation and voice qualities which have been successfully used to
distinguish non-modal from modal phonation (e.g., Garellek, 2012; Hanson et al., 2001;
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 8 of 40
Keating et al., 2015; Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Kuang & Keating, 2012; Ladefoged & Maddieson,
1996, among others).
f0,
2*f0, F1, F2, and F3 and are expressed as H1, H2, A1, A2, and A3 respectively. Garellek
(2012) and Kuang and Keating (2012) provide a comprehensive summary of the various
acoustic correlates of phonation and spectral slope. Given that the tense/pressed voice
quality was reported in the context of pharyngealization, we will restrict the explanations
below to the creaky/tense/pressed voice quality correlates as compared to breathy voice
quality. A tense/pressed voice tends to show a lower H1-H2 as the main acoustic correlate
(e.g., Garellek, 2012; Hanson et al., 2001; Keating et al., 2015; Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Kuang
& Keating, 2012; Ladefoged & Maddieson, 1996, among others). Spectral tilt measures,
i.e., H1-A1, H1-A2, and H1-A3 seem to be directly correlated with the abruptness of
vocal fold closure (Garellek, 2012; Hanson et al., 2001). H1-A1 correlates well with the
bandwidth of F1; as the bandwidth of F1 increases, the amplitude of the harmonic closest
to F1 decreases and thus a lower H1-A1 indicates a creaky/tense voice (Garellek, 2012;
Hanson et al., 2001; Kuang & Keating, 2012). H1-A2 and H1-A3 are also correlated with
creaky/tense phonation, showing lower values (Garellek, 2012; Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Kuang
& Keating, 2012). Hanson & Chuang (1999) and Hanson et al. (2001) described how an
abrupt closure of the glottis can yield a change in the source spectrum; a decrease in
spectral tilt around F3 is observed, and hence a lowered H1-A3 would be expected. If the

folds, then an increase in spectral tilt would be seen and a higher H1-A3 is obtained. This
should not be seen as an indication of an increased noise, as an increased noise as seen in
breathy voice and/or glottal opening will increase spectral tilt further and H1-A3 will be
much higher (for more detail, see e.g., Hanson & Chuang, 1999; Hanson et al., 2001; Klatt

A1-A2 is directly related to phonation types, and thus a creaky/tense voice tends to have
lower A1-A2 (Aralova et al., 2011; Fulop et al., 1998; Guion et al., 2004; Kang & Ko,
        
tongue root leads to an increase in the energy of the harmonics above F1. This increase
can either be extensive in that the energy in A2 and A3 are higher than that of A1;
consequences of an extreme epilaryngeal constriction that leads to an ‘enhanced’ voice
(Story, 2016). In this case, a lowered or even negative A1-A3 and A2-A3 is obtained due to
the concentration of energy around F3, F4, and F5 as seen in the ‘singer’s’ formant (Story,
2016). However, when the epilaryngeal constriction is minimal and/or is associated with
a primary pharyngeal constriction, we would expect a change in the pattens. This would
A1-A3 and A2-A3, due to the decrease in
energy around F3. This again correlates well with the acoustic consequences of an abrupt
closure of the vocal folds (Hanson et al., 2001).
1.2.2 Formal representation
As we saw above, an epilaryngeal constriction yields both a ‘lingual’ and a ‘laryngeal’
            
shift from considering lingual or laryngeal constrictions separately and can be used to
describe the post-velars as one set of combined articulations (for more detail on the
glottocentric vs linguocentric view, see Moisik, 2013a; Moisik et al., 2012; Moisik & Esling,
2011; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b). This allowed for the introduction of a new set of
          

       
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 9 of 40
Lower-Vocal-Tract Phonological Potentials (Moisik, 2013a), [+
sounds to be retracted, constricted and to have raised larynx voice and tense phonation;
all as a combined unit (Moisik, 2013a; Moisik et al., 2012; Moisik & Esling, 2011).
‘Retracted’ is due to tongue and epiglottis retraction with a back and down gesture;
‘constricted’ of certain intralaryngeal muscles and ‘raised larynx’ leading to tense voice
quality (Moisik et al., 2012, p. 5). The feature [+  
pharyngeals/epiglottal and pharyngealized/epiglottalized categories with the former
being assigned this feature as a primary place feature, whereas the latter receives it as a
secondary feature (Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, p. 136–138). Following Moisik et al. (2012)
and Moisik and Esling (2011), ‘true’ pharyngeals (and other categories) are assigned
this feature as an indication of the primary constriction of the epilarynx; pharyngealized
consonants are assigned [+ 
p. 138). Whether all or parts of these combined articulatory consequences are to be used
depends on the category of sounds (Moisik et al., 2012, p. 5–6), hence pharyngealized
consonants may show tongue root retraction and/or intralaryngeal muscle constriction
but not larynx raising, etc. (for more detail, see e.g., Moisik & Esling, 2011, table 2,
p. 1407 on pharyngeal and pharyngealized categories being producing by a glottal source,
yielding a raised larynx and a [+
where pharyngeal and pharyngealized receive a [+

1.3 Aims of the current study
This exploratory study aims at acoustically investigating whether an epilaryngeal
constriction is associated with pharyngealization in Arabic. We take the views developed
in the Laryngeal Articulator Model that an epilaryngeal constriction leads to a ‘retracted’
vowel in the pharyngeal area with both tongue backing and lowering that causes a
tense/pressed/laryngealized voice quality due to constricting the larynx. Acoustically
speaking, we expect the vowels in the pharyngealized context to show a raised F1 and

Laryngeal Articulator Model, as well as a raised Z3-Z2 signaling both backness and spectral
divergence as an enhancing correlate to the already lowered Z2-Z1. F3 can potentially
show a lowered value if pharyngealization is produced in the mid-low pharynx for front
vowels or a raised F3 for back vowels due to the tighter constriction. Spectral slope and
voice quality correlates are expected to correlate well with the tense/pressed voice quality
with an overall lowered H1-H2, H1-A1, H1-A2, H1-A3, A1-A2, and with an increased
             
lowered A1-A3 and A2-A3.
2 Method
2.1 Material
2.1.1 Speakers
Twenty Jordanian and Moroccan male speakers (10 of each dialect), aged 20 to 30,
participated in this experiment. Jordanian Arabic speakers originated from Irbid in
the north of Jordan whereas Moroccan Arabic speakers come from Mohammedia (near
Casablanca). They all reported no history of articulatory or hearing disorders, and shared
            
university, and all lived in the city (i.e., spoke an urban variety). Some Jordanian Arabic
speakers had some knowledge of French and/or English (beginner to advanced levels),
while Moroccan Arabic speakers were non-Berber, and had knowledge of French. Both

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 10 of 40
2.1.2 Dialects
Although Jordanian and Moroccan are dialects of the same language, some important

They belong to the eastern and western zones respectively, and some researchers have
  
Vowel inventories are reduced in western dialects compared to the eastern ones (Marçais,

(Al-Tamimi, J., 2007a; Al-Tamimi, J. & Ferragne, 2005; Barkat, 2000). A more complex
syllable structure seems to operate in western dialects (Cohen, 1962), which has a direct
        
(or more halting) rhythmic structure than eastern dialects (Barkat, 2000; Ghazali et al.,

in placement of stress, although their patterns in producing the statements that are used

the two zones are important enough for the dialects to be mutually unintelligible, which

A key aspect of the current study is to evaluate whether the phonetic implementation
            
perspective, Bellem (2007) suggested that pharyngealization is implemented in the same
manner, although some dialects can show more of a ‘guttural’ quality than others. It is
not clear however, what is meant by a more ‘guttural,’ whether it is more mid-to-low
        

of pharyngealization with Moroccan Arabic speakers having the mostly distinctive locus
equation slopes, followed by Jordanians (and then the two other dialects, Kuwaiti and

steeper formant slopes of F1, F2, and F3 were obtained in JA (Al-Tamimi, J., 2007a, b).
              
between these two dialects in how pharyngealization is implemented.
2.1.3 Material and recordings
The material used in this exploratory study comes from a larger corpus on bilabial, alveolar,
and velar stops that was used to investigate the role of dynamic correlates (i.e., formant
slopes) in production and perception (for more details, see Al-Tamimi, J., 2007a, b). The
real words used in this study are listed in Table 2. Voiced alveolar pharyngealized and
 1V11V11V1CVC, or
1V1C syllable structures, where C1 =1 =

used, it was not possible to obtain minimal sets for the two varieties nor comparable
   
sometimes either in initial or medial positions, or with other ‘guttural’ sounds present
  
with cross-dialectal variations, emphasis spread is greater from coronal pharyngealized
consonants, i.e., ‘true’ emphatics compared to other ‘guttural’ sounds (Hellmuth, 2013;
Watson, 2007). In addition, rightward emphasis spread is more common than leftward
(Hellmuth, 2013), although Bellem (2007) seems to suggest small variations between
dialects in how this aspect is implemented. Given these restrictions, our aim is to evaluate
how these additional acoustic correlates can be used on such a corpus.
The speakers were seated in front of a computer in a sound attenuated room (for
Jordanian Arabic speakers) or in a very quiet room (for Moroccan Arabic speakers), and
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 11 of 40

for this task. After a training phase, and for purposes of other experiments using this
dataset, speakers were asked to produce each item as realized in the target word, in
the target CV syllable and then in the target isolated vowel, while trying to keep the
production of each vowel constant across realizations and having at least 0.5 sec gap
between each sequence. This particular task is aimed at evaluating the role of contextual
information on the degree of vowel hypo- vs hyper-articulation and in vowel perception
(for more details see Al-Tamimi, J., 2007a). The words were randomly presented with
           
       
and to obtain the real dialectal realization of each word. The carrier sentence had an
important role here as it was used as a way to convey the meaning of the unvocalized
   
some speakers produced a non-dialectal form; in these cases, the experimenter clicked

speaker was asked to reproduce it. In this second round of production, most speakers
reproduced the words in a dialectal form. The quality of the productions was assessed
by the author and in the case of Moroccan Arabic by a native speaker of the variety.
Speakers were asked to produce each word, while not moving or modifying the distance
 
then digitized directly on the same computer, with a sampling frequency of 22.05 kHz,
16-bit quantization, in mono channel using a Sony MS 907 microphone (distance 15–20
cm from the speakers’ mouths). Given that the corpus used here is part of a larger study
           
vowels, the length of all the experiments (production and perception) was about 2 hours
per speaker (for more details see Al-Tamimi, J., 2007a). For the current study, vowels

realizations as a function of the pharyngealized vs non-pharyngealized environments;
the total number of words produced by the speakers for this study was 700 for Jordanian
and 500 for Moroccan Arabic (henceforth JA and MA).
2.2 Data processing and acoustic analyses



Table 2: List of items used in the current study for both JA and MA.
/iː/ /ɪ/ /eː/ /aː/ /ɐ/ /ʊ/ /uː/
JA
/d/ ˈdiːnak ˈdɪjja ˈdeːr ˈdaːr ˈdɐm ˈdʊbb ˈduːd
(your) religion compensation monastery home blood bear worms
/dˤ/ maˈdˤiːq ˈdˤɪdˤdˤ ˈdˤeːf ˈdˤaːq ˈdˤɐbtˤ ˈdˤʊħa maʕˈdˤuːdˤ
strait against guest shrunk exactly before noon bitten
/iː/ /aː/ /ə/ /ʊ/ /uː/
MA
/d/ ˈdiːb ˈdaːb dəmˈliːʒ ˈdʊll ˈduːda
wolf melted bracelet humiliation worm
/dˤ/ ˈdˤiːf ˈdˤaːq ˈdˤəbtˤ ˈdˤʊlma ˈdˤuːsˤ
guest shrunk exactly darkness the second
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 12 of 40
wide-band spectrogram. In the cases where a sonorant followed the vowel, intensity drop
and visual inspections were used to determine the boundary position of the vowels.
2.2.1 Acoustic analyses
Acoustic analyses were performed automatically using a Praat script designed by the
author and adapted from Al-Tamimi, J. and Khattab (2015). Before performing the
analyses, measurement frame positions of the onset and midpoint of a vowel were
estimated following Al-Tamimi, J. (2004, 2007b) and Al-Tamimi, J. and Khattab (2015)
in order to obtain accurate measurements and reduce errors from the automatic analyses

by computing f    
PointProcess (cross-correlation) analysis. Following this, the average length of a complete


times as obtained from the TextGrids (following the segmentation as described above),

length of an average complete glottal cycle. Following this, they were left-aligned to the
original onset estimate, and centered at the original midpoint estimate. The intensity
values, computed every 5 ms, were interpolated before computing the maximum; the
             
positions. All the reported measurements are obtained at the estimated positions.
Formant frequencies
onset and midpoint of each vowel. These were obtained from a 25-ms Kaiser2 (Gaussian-

requested in the formant analysis using the default Burg algorithm for formant estimation
with a maximum frequency of 5 kHz for male speakers. Following formant estimation,
Praat’s Formant track function was used to reduce the errors in automatic formant

obtained from automatic extraction. When formant tracks obtained through the initial

an LPC smoothed curve obtained from a 256-point zero-padded DFT spectrum computed
from a 10-ms Kaiser2 window left-aligned at the onset or centered at the midpoint of

   
0.98. Both the FFT and the smoothed LPC displays were used to estimate the position of
a particularly weakened formant.
Bark-dierence formant frequencies
estimated, these were converted to the psychoacoustic Bark scale following Traunmüller’s
(1990) formula 1, where Zn is the Z value (i.e., critical bandwidth) of the formantn, and fn
is the frequency in Hz of the formantn (including f0 in both cases), and any Z values lower
Zc

()
{}
26.81/ 1 1960 / 0.53
nn
Zf
=+−
(1)
()
2.0 : 0.15 2
c
Z Bark Z Z Z
< =+−
(2)
The fundamental frequency f

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 13 of 40
quality; there was however a pattern of lower f0 in JA and higher in MA. The estimation
followed the procedure in Al-Tamimi, J. and Khattab (2015) and used the two-pass





pass and the actual f

and 150–200 Hz respectively. The frequencies obtained in Hz were converted to the Bark
scale using formulas 1 and 2. Once the Bark transformation applied on F1, F2, F3, and f0

Z1-Z0, Z2-Z1, and Z3-Z2 at the onset and at the midpoint of the vowel.
Voice quality     
       
 

      
was left-aligned at the onset and a second centered at the midpoint of the vowel; these
were then windowed using a Kaiser2 window function. From each windowed interval, a
256-point zero-padded DFT spectrum was computed and the logarithmic power spectral
density, with a bin size of 19 Hz, was computed. Following Al-Tamimi, J. and Khattab
    
formants were automatically obtained by detecting the highest peaks for a particular
harmonic; maximum amplitude was obtained from f0*0.9 to f0*1.1 and from 2*f0*0.95
to 2*f0*1.05 for H1 and H2, respectively. For the amplitude of the harmonics closest

proposed by Hawks and Miller (1995) instead of using the automatically estimated ones
in Praat due to many errors that prevented manual correction. Then maximum amplitudes
were obtained in the region from F1 – 0.5*Bandwidth1 to F1 + 0.5*Bandwidth1 for
A1. The same procedure was applied for A2 and A3 (and using Bandwidths 2 and 3
respectively). The automatic detection of formant frequencies, and highest peaks were

harmonics, we relied on the normalization procedure as developed by Iseli et al. (2007)
and implemented in our Praat script to obtain the ‘corrected’ versions of these harmonics
   H1, H2, A1, and A2 were
 A3 was normalized by

H1*-H2*, H1*-A1*, H1*-A2*, H1*-A3*, A1*-A2*, A1*-A3*, and A2*-A3* at both onset
and midpoint of the vowel.
2.2.2 Statistical analyses
A total of 30,966 measurements (17,992 in JA and 12,974 in MA) were obtained from

onset and midpoint (i.e., a total of 26 measures). Our main aim in this study is to evaluate
the degree to which a particular acoustic correlate can be used to successfully predict the
    
adopted a predictive approach (Baguley, 2012; Hastie et al., 2009; Kuhn & Johnson, 2013).
          
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 14 of 40
              

which acoustic correlate(s) are the most predictive of the two consonant categories. All
analyses were run using the statistical software (version 3.3.1) (R Core Team, 2016).
Generalized Linear Mixed-Effects Modeling (GLMM)
Before running the GLMMs, we started by examining the correlation levels in our data. We
used correlation matrices ordered by hierarchical clustering obtained with the function
hclust from the package reshape2R
script (Melike, 2016). These correlation matrices are presented in Appendix 1 for JA and
in Appendix 2 for MA. The correlation matrices showed that out of the total combinations

p < 0.01, i.e., any absolute
r2p values obtained with the function rcorr
from the package Hmisc (Harrell Jr, 2016)). In both dialects, voice quality and formant-
based measures are negatively correlated with each other, and all formant measures
  
JA, where Z3-Z2 is negatively correlated with Z2 and Z2-Z1). Given the recommendations
in Baayen (2008, p. 181–183), it was not advisable to use all these measures together
in a regression analysis as they will be giving either the same outcome (in the case of
positively correlated ones) or cancel each other out (for negatively correlated cases).
Hence we decided to use separate regression analyses on each of the individual acoustic
correlates and compared these via predicted probabilities. In addition, due to the high
predictive outcome of some acoustic correlates (e.g., F2, Z3-Z2, see below), it was not
possible to use one GLMM model combining all the acoustic correlates, as these were
canceling each other out, resulting in model non-convergence.
Due to these constraints (multicollinearity and high predictive power), we used an

      
measures are already on logarithmic scales). We obtained the descriptive statistics using
the package psycholing (Fraundorf, 2015), with the means and standard deviations
for the original and z-scored values. The z-scored values were computed to a mean of 0
and a standard deviation of 1, separately for each dialect (using the means and standard
deviations presented in column “All” in Table A3 in Appendix 3 that provides a summary

mean and standard deviations in the original and the z-scored scale. We also included the

of the outcome with the results of the GLMMs).
Following this, and to avoid multicollinearity, we ran separate GLMMs on the individual
z-scored acoustic correlates following Schielzeth (2010) as a simple and meaningful
             
running GLMMs with the ‘consonant’ as a binomial response category (dummy coded
with /d/ =  = 1), and the separate acoustic correlates as predictors (i.e., a
  
the GLMM and allows for a meaningful interpretation of the results with the
β
of the
Intercept representing the average in the /d/ environment whereas the predictor’s
β
            

   
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 15 of 40
For the random part of the model, we used speakers and items as crossed random factors
(Baayen et al., 2008). Within item variation with respect to repetitions was included to
allow for this random variation to be taken into account. Both by-speaker and by-item
     
These allowed both speakers and words to vary with respect to the within variation that is
due to the acoustic correlates. By-item random slopes were necessary given that we only
had one item per consonant and vowel combination; this allowed the acoustic correlates
to vary within each word, otherwise the model would inaccurately overestimate this
variation. In many occasions, random intercepts for words were not successful at providing
a clear picture of the results, and hence were dropped, i.e., only a by-item random slope
was used.
To prevent quasi- and complete separation of the data that was obtained with lme4

as implemented in the package blme (Chung et al., 2013). The function bglmer requires

      
xef.prior = normal(cov = diag(2.5,2)), with 2    
parameters and 2.5 representing a variance of 2.5 which is equivalent to a 1.58 SD that is
close to our actual SD for the z-scored data (being 1SD) (following the recommendations

kept at its default, (i.e., cov.prior = wishart, for more detail, see Chung et al., 2013).
Random Forests via Conditional Inference Trees
            
predictive model (Hastie et al., 2009; Kuhn & Johnson, 2013). Random Forests are one
of the most versatile machine learning algorithms as they do not require many tunings of
their settings. They have been applied to sociolinguistic data (for a detailed description
of these methods, see Tagliamonte & Baayen, 2012), and also to acoustic cue weighting
in perception (Brown et al., 2014). Random Forests were originally proposed by Breiman
(2001) as an ensemble learning algorithm that uses independent
trees in growing a forest. The independence stems from the randomness of the selection

a decision tree is constructed. This is then repeated several times (for the total number

2002). After the forest is grown, one can estimate the prediction accuracy as well as the
ranking of the most important predictors. This model can also be used to predict the
outcome on new unseen data, e.g., newly collected production/perception data or on the
testing set (out-of-bag set). The algorithm splits the data into learning and testing sets; the
in-bag set, while the latter
out-of-bag set. Then subsampling
without replacement is used in growing the forest (for more details, see Strobl et al., 2009).
Instead of using the original implementation of Random Forests available in the
R package randomForest (Liaw & Wiener, 2002), we used Random Forests grown
from Conditional Inference Trees as implemented in the package party (Hothorn et
al., 2006; Strobl et al., 2008, 2007). Strobl et al. (2008, 2007) found that the original
randomForest provided biased estimates of Random Forests’ variable importance (see
below) as it was biased towards variables with multiple categories and multiple cut-points,
and also overestimated variable importance measures when correlated data is used (as is
the case in our study). To guard against this bias, they developed an unbiased selection
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 16 of 40
process (subsampling without replacement). This form of random forests, i.e., based on
conditional inference trees, are well suited to deal with collinear variables (Strobl et al.,
2008, 2007; Tagliamonte & Baayen, 2012), with “‘small n large p’ case, where the number
of predictor variables p   n” (Strobl et al., 2009,

by ranking them after controlling for interactions and collinearity.
Random Forests were run using the party package with the function cforest on the
     
recommended cforest_unbiased control with mtry = 5 (rounded square root of 26
ntree can be tuned to allow for less computation time, and
we followed the density-based metrics developed by (Oshiro et al., 2012) to estimate the
density of our dataset using the formula 3, with an
of observations and c
{}
log /
a
Density n c
= (3)
For JA and MA, the density-based metrics were equal to 1.79 and 1.69 respectively.
According to Oshiro et al. (2012), these values constitute a low density database. The
authors found that a large number of trees close to and above 2000 is only needed with
high density datasets that exceed a density-based value of 3. A low number of trees
(ntree         
predictive accuracy. Hence, we implemented the same procedure as that suggested by

by using ntree from 100 to 1500 in a 100 trees increment. Then for all generated random
forests, we checked their predictive power, by using the function predict using the
out-of-bag set as a cross-validation (using OOB = TRUE). Then we used an AUC (for Area
Under the Curve) based comparison using the package pROC (Robin et al., 2011), by
generating an ROC curve (for Receiver Operating Characteristics) and then by performing

roc.test, following DeLong et al. (1988). The results of this comparison showed that for
JA, 400 trees were enough to reach the highest predictive accuracy, whereas for MA, 300
trees were enough. Hence we ran random forests with these ntree values.
Then we used an AUC-based estimation of the variable importance, varimpAUC
as it takes into account both accuracy and error of estimation and used conditional
permutation tests with conditional = T
et al., 2008, 2009). We then evaluated how well correlated are the random forest results
            out-of-bag
cross-validation.
After this initial random forest using all of the acoustic correlates, we ran six additional
exploratory random forest analyses that will be used for their predictive accuracy. These
    
the type of acoustic correlates used. We compared formant-based metrics only, i.e.,

The aim here is to evaluate the strength of these metrics at separating the two categories.

in addition to voice quality, in order to assess whether there is an increase/decrease in

of the voice quality metrics on their own, in order to evaluate whether any observed

categories. In each case, we used the above procedure to estimate the optimal number of
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 17 of 40
trees needed to obtain the highest predictive accuracy, and adapted mtry to each case.
The next section presents the results of this study.
3 Results
3.1 GLMM

acoustic correlate as a predictor (i.e., a Full model) and a second without the acoustic

ratio tests to derive all p values (Barr et al., 2013) and to test whether a particular acoustic


Table 3
(as a reference, complete descriptive statistics of the results are presented in the Table A3
in Appendix 3). Figures 1, 2, 5, and 6 were generated using the predicted probabilities
of the individual acoustic correlates as obtained from the Full GLMM. These predicted
probabilities were obtained from the predict function in blme based on a new dataset
with the range between –3 and 3 z
an R
lattice (Sarkar, 2008), latticeExtra (Sarkar & Andrews, 2016) and gridExtra
(Auguie, 2016). Using this range allows for a meaningful comparison between all the
measures. When looking at the predicted probability curves, we will refer to a sigmoidal
  
linear shape. A sigmoidal curve indicates a high level of separation between the groups

each acoustic correlate, the absolute
β

size; the negative or positive signs are indicative of falling or raising probability curves
Figure 1 shows the predicted probabilities for Z1, Z2,
and Z3 at both onset and midpoint of the vowel in JA (blue solid) and MA (red dashed). In
both dialects, Z1 at the onset of the vowel shows raising curves from /d/ (values starting
from –3 zz-score); Z2 shows the reverse pattern,
z-score) to /d/ (values ending with
+3 z
–3 zz-score), although the shape is not as sigmoidal

GLMM based on the
β

β

β

3.1.1 Absolute Formants
From model comparison results (see Table 3), it can be seen that F1 and F2 at the onset and

and non-pharyngealized consonants. In MA, F3 at the midpoint only showed a tendency

in the literature; F1 shows an increased
β


as displayed in Figure 1 show a clear pattern of rise, fall, rise patterns for F1, F2, and F3;

z
size measures (Schielzeth, 2010). In JA, F2 at the onset has the highest absolute
β
value and
shows more of a sigmoidal curve (see Z2 Onset, Figure 1). In MA, however, F1 mid and F2 at
both onset and mid have the highest absolute
β
values and show a near complete sigmoidal
curve. When converting the
β
values to percent correct, all four acoustic correlates (in JA
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 18 of 40
Table 3: Summary of statistical results, with model comparison between Null and Full models, with χ2 (1 df), and p values, followed by the GLMM results
of the Full model with the estimates of the Fixed effect of each acoustic measure (with
β
, SE (Standard Error), Wald’s z value, associated p value, and %
correct classification based on
β
values); significant results are in bold.
JA MA
Model comparison GLMM results Model comparison GLMM results
χ2 (1) p β SE z Pr (>|z|) % χ2 (1) p β SE z Pr (>|z|) %
Z1 Onset 8.07 <0.005 1.28 0.47 2.71 <0.007 78.26 24.2 <0.00001 2.05 0.32 6.51 <0.00001 88.63
Mid 8.18 <0.005 2.39 0.97 2.46 <0.02 91.62 25.51 <0.00001 4.54 1.04 4.38 <0.00001 98.95
Z2 Onset 27.38 <0.00001 –6.92 0.92 –7.51 <0.00001 99.90 30.88 <0.00001 –4.59 0.64 –7.23 <0.00001 99.00
Mid 10.303 <0.001 –1.55 0.77 –2.00 <0.05 82.43 2.9403 =0.086 –4.79 0.67 –7.17 <0.00001 99.17
Z3 Onset 0.27 =0.61 0.12 0.29 0.40 =0.70 52.91 0.75 =0.39 0.16 0.22 0.74 =0.47 53.96
Mid 0.46 =0.50 0.17 0.24 0.69 =0.49 54.19 3.26 =0.071 0.56 0.32 1.74 =0.082 63.66
Z1-Z0 Onset 12.38 <0.0001 1.36 0.38 3.59 <0.0001 79.55 20.58 <0.00001 1.85 0.31 6.03 <0.00001 86.38
Mid 6.65 <0.01 2.07 0.94 2.20 <0.05 88.78 27.61 <0.00001 4.50 0.97 4.63 <0.00001 98.90
Z2-Z1 Onset 26.15 <0.00001 –6.09 0.90 –6.74 <0.00001 99.77 30.1 <0.00001 –4.65 0.56 –8.23 <0.00001 99.05
Mid 39.622 <0.00001 3.77 1.70 –2.22 <0.05 97.75 13.327 <0.0005 –2.58 0.85 –3.04 <0.002 92.95
Z3-Z2 Onset 22.94 <0.00001 6.35 1.03 6.15 <0.00001 99.83 28.37 <0.00001 4.38 0.58 7.58 <0.00001 98.76
Mid 8.5688 <0.005 1.55 0.56 2.76 <0.006 82.46 22.109 <0.00001 4.75 1.07 4.45 <0.00001 99.15
H1*-H2* Onset 0.29 =0.60 0.09 0.19 0.49 =0.62 52.32 0.06 =0.81 0.05 0.18 0.26 =0.42 51.15
Mid 0.03 =0.87 0.05 0.19 0.28 =0.78 51.34 0.88 =0.35 0.14 0.17 0.81 =0.80 53.52
H1*-A1* Onset 2.56 =0.11 –0.41 0.27 –1.49 =0.14 60.02 22.46 <0.00001 –1.51 0.23 –6.41 <0.00001 81.83
Mid 8.27 <0.005 –0.58 0.21 –2.74 <0.006 64.07 13.68 <0.0001 –1.33 0.34 –3.93 <0.0001 7 9 .1 0
H1*-A2* Onset 10.35 <0.001 –0.76 0.23 –3.34 <0.001 68.24 19.74 <0.00001 –1.98 0.33 –6.05 <0.00001 87.82
Mid 5.76 <0.02 –0.61 0.26 –2.29 <0.05 64.73 17.63 <0.00001 –1.46 0.32 –4.60 <0.00001 81.14
H1*-A3* Onset 3.18 =0.074 0.25 0.15 1.65 =0.098 56.31 4.03 <0.05 –0.32 0.18 –1.82 =0.068 58.00
Mid 2.07 =0.15 0.05 0.19 0.28 =0.17 51.34 1.93 =0.17 –0.27 0.22 –1.25 =0.21 56.69
A1*-A2* Onset 2.54 =0.11 –0.42 0.28 –1.51 =0.13 60.32 5.74 <0.02 –0.69 0.28 –2.44 <0.02 66.56
Mid 0.79 =0.37 –0.16 0.19 –0.80 =0.42 53.88 13.18 <0.0001 –0.80 0.21 –3.77 <0.0002 68.97
A1*-A3* Onset 11.04 <0.001 0.77 0.22 3.55 <0.0005 68.36 2.23 =0.14 0.33 0.24 1.39 =0.17 58.24
Mid 0.17 =0.68 0.09 0.21 0.43 =0.67 52.29 1.16 =0.28 0.30 0.29 1.04 =0.30 57.37
A2*-A3* Onset 16.66 <0.00001 1.23 0.24 5.07 <0.00001 77.40 11.39 <0.001 1.01 0.27 3.74 <0.0002 73.37
Mid 1.72 =0.19 0.41 0.34 1.20 =0.23 60.17 4.01 <0.05 0.96 0.52 1.85 =0.065 72.28
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 19 of 40

obtained for the absolute formant frequencies are comparable in direction and range to the
previous literature (see Table A3 in Appendix 3 and Section 1.1.1).
Following perturbation theory and the acoustic theory of speech production (Carré
    
Johnson, 2012; Mrayati et al., 1988; Stevens, 1989), the location of the constriction
responsible for producing pharyngealization is predicted to be located in the pharyngeal
area, potentially between the upper to mid pharynx, as is generally reported in the


vowels (by around 70 Hz) and a raised F3 in back vowels (by around 200 Hz) from

2009) (see also Section 2.2.1).
3.1.2 Bark-difference formants
  

divergence of vowels (Fahey et al., 1996; Hoemeke & Diehl, 1994; Sylak, 2011; Syrdal
& Gopal, 1986; Traunmüller, 1981; Wood, 1986). Model comparison results summarized
in Table 3          

show increased
β
values, whereas Z2-Z1 shows lowered ones. The predicted probabilities
presented in Figure 2
with variable degrees of separation. Generally, MA shows higher
β
values for Z1-Z0 and

(see absolute formant frequencies results in Figure 1 above).

being associated with a lowered Z2-Z1. A low pharyngeal constriction will show Z2-Z1
values below 3 Bark values whereas a mid to upper pharynegal constriction will show
higher Z2-Z1 values around 4.6 Bark (Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood, 2011; Heselwood &
Al-Tamimi, F., 2011). Our results show a lowering of Z2-Z1 values that is close to the

Figure 1: Predicted probability curves spanning –3 to +3 z-score for absolute formant frequencies
in both JA (blue solid) and MA (red dashed). Falling curves indicate a lowering in the predicted
probabilities from /d/ to /d/ (as in Z2) and vice versa. The reference line (in gray) indicates the
non difference line.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 20 of 40

β
values at the Onset
for JA and at both positions in MA. They show a more retracted vowel quality that is
      

        

           
correlate to the already lowered Z2-Z1. The predictive strength of these correlates is
Table 3 for more detail).
3.1.3 Vowel spaces
We used relational formant frequencies to display vowel spaces; using the traditional
F2*F1 (in Bark) vowel spaces and that of Z3-Z2*Z2-Z1 at both onset and midpoint of the

Figures 3 and 4 show both Z2*Z1 (a) and Z3-Z2*Z2-Z1 (b) at onset (top) and midpoint
(bottom) in both JA and MA respectively (charts generated with the package phonR,
  


Glassman, 2014b).
Vowel spaces at the onset provide the clearest separation between the two categories
compared with that at the midpoint, although in MA, the midpoint results lend support
Figure 4). At the onset, JA shows a clear back vowel
quality as represented with absolute F2, whereas MA shows a clear open and back vowel
articulation through absolute F1 and F2 respectively (see Figures 3a and 4a). Moving on
Figures 3b and 4b), and particularly at the onset (top),
both JA and MA show a clear separation between the two categories, with vowels in the
pharyngealized context showing a ‘compact’ (with lower Z2-Z1) and ‘backed’ (with higher
Z3-Z2) production; they are ‘retracted’ in the sense of the Laryngeal Articulator Model
(Esling, 2005; Moisik et al., 2012; Sylak-Glassman, 2014b). By using absolute formants
only (Figures 3a and 4a
all vowels seem to range around 4 Bark. Our claim is then if one is to acoustically evaluate
Figure 2: Predicted probability curves spanning –3 to +3 z-score for Bark-difference formant
frequencies in both JA (blue solid) and MA (red dashed). Falling curves indicate a lowering in
the predicted probabilities from /d/ to /d/ (as in Z2-Z1) and vice versa. The reference line (in
gray) indicates the non difference line.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 21 of 40
the predictions of the Laryngeal Articulator Model, absolute formant frequencies alone
are not useful at showing the ‘retraction’ that has a combined back and down gesture;

Figure 4: Absolute formant (Z1*Z2; a) Versus Bark-difference (Z2-Z1*Z3-Z2; b) Vowel spaces at
onset (top) and midpoint (bottom) of /d/ (red solid) and /d/ (blue dashed) in MA.
Figure 3: Absolute formant (Z1*Z2; a) Versus Bark-difference (Z2-Z1*Z3-Z2; b) Vowel spaces at
onset (top) and midpoint (bottom) of /d/ (red solid) and /d/ (blue dashed) in JA.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 22 of 40
3.1.4 Voice quality
   
associated with pharyngealization in Arabic (although see Alwan, 1986, 1989, on the
estimated bandwidth in pharyngeals). Our voice quality results are separated into spectral
slope and high frequency energy (Figures 5 and 6 respectively). Starting with spectral
slope, model comparison results suggest that H1*-A1*, H1*-A2*, and H1*-A3*, in either
            
consonants in both dialects (Table 3). GLMM results show an overall decrease in
β
values
H1*-A3* Onset
in JA). MA results show a stronger contribution of these metrics to the distinction between
the consonants compared to JA. This is shown further in the curve shape in Figure 5 where

H1*-A1*, H1*-A2* at both onset and midpoint show a near sigmoidal curve, although this


β
and a predictive
Table 3). These spectral slope measures seem to act as
secondary correlates to pharyngealization as they do not show the same predictive power
as formant frequencies.

voice quality that is related to a greater glottal constriction (Keating et al., 2015). This leads
to variation in the bandwidths of F1 and F2 (through H1*-A1* and H1*-A2* respectively)
that accompanies tense/pressed voice quality; an increased bandwidth of F1 and F2
causes a decrease in the amplitude of the harmonic closest to F1 and F2, respectively
  H1*-A3* is indicative

Keating, 2012). This seems to be the case with MA as a more abruptly constricted glottis

H1*-A3* (Hanson & Chuang, 1999; Hanson et al., 2001). For JA however, the increased
Figure 5: Predicted probability curves spanning –3 to +3 z-score for Spectral slope results in
both JA (blue solid) and MA (red dashed). Falling curves indicate a lowering in the predicted
probabilities from /d/ to /d/ (as in H1*-A1*) and vice versa. The reference line (in gray) indicates
the non difference line.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 23 of 40
H1*-A3* is indicative of a lowered energy around F3 in the pharyngealized context, which
seems to correlate well with the predictions of a constricted glottis with a simultaneous
front to back closure along the length of the vocal folds (for more detail, see e.g., Hanson
& Chuang, 1999; Hanson et al., 2001; Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Stevens, 1998). Both dialects
display tense voice quality that is associated with pharyngealization though spectral slope

is; abrupt in MA and with a simultaneous front to back closure of the vocal folds in JA.
Moving on to high frequency components, and through model comparison (Table 3),

two consonants. In JA, only A1*-A3* and A2*-A
in MA, it is A1*-A2* and A2*-A3* at the onset and midpoint. This can also be seen from
the predicted probabilities as shown in Figure 6 where some curves are near sigmoidal,

β
and percent correct values)
are small compared to formant based measures, and these high frequency components
          A1*-A2* is correlated
with a constriction closer to the tongue root (i.e., mid to lower pharynx) as is the case
in languages with [–
1998; Guion et al., 2004; Kang & Ko, 2012, among others). This suggests that in MA, a
lower constriction location for pharyngealization may be in operation, although a non-
 A1*-A2* at the onset in JA is obtained. With respect to the two
other metrics, an increase in A1*-A3* and A2*-A3* is observed in both JA and MA with
raised predicted probabilities (see Figure 6). This increase is indicative of a relatively
decreased energy around F3 with respect to that of F1 or F2. In fact, A1 and A2 are
already high in energy due to the pharyngeal constriction, and the change observed in
A3 energy would be indicative of a change due to the epilaryngeal constriction that leads

and the high frequency components are indicative of a constricted epilarynx; the former
shows acoustic correlates of a tense voice quality, and the latter a constricted epilarynx
with a relative increase in A1*-A3* and A2*-A3* due to decreased energy around F3.
This combination suggests that pharyngealization in Arabic is associated with constricted
ventricular folds (Moisik et al., 2012; Moisik & Esling, 2011; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a). This
constriction of the glottis leads to a tense voice quality that is potentially associated with
Figure 6: Predicted probability curves spanning –3 to +3 z-score for the High frequency
components in both JA (blue solid) and MA (red dashed). Falling curves indicate a lowering in
the predicted probabilities from /d/ to /d/ (as in A1*-A2*) and vice versa. The reference line
(in gray) indicates the non difference line.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 24 of 40
a constricted and/or raised larynx posture (Klatt & Klatt, 1990; Laver, 1994; Sundberg &
Askenfelt, 1981). These novel acoustic consequences require further investigation from an
articulatory point of view to shed light into this secondary correlate of pharyngealization
in Arabic.
3.1.5 Summary of results
The results presented above showed the vowels in the vicinity of pharyngealized consonants
in Arabic to be ‘retracted’ (Esling, 2005; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b). They are produced as
more open (higher F1, Z1-Z0) and more back (lower F2, higher Z3-Z2), with a constriction
in the pharyngeal area that causes compaction of the spectrum (lower Z2-Z1), and spectral
divergence as an enhancing correlate to the compacted spectrum (higher Z3-Z2). Z2-Z1
seems to provide a better combined correlate to the compaction of the spectrum rather
than the separate F1 and F2, although both are related to each other (see Section 3.1.3).
Our novel results with respect to spectral slope and the high frequency components seem
to suggest that pharyngealization in Arabic is associated with a secondary constricted
ventricular folds and hence a constricted epilarynx; this induces a tense voice quality with
lower H1*-A1*, H1*-A2*, H1*-A3*, and A1*-A2*, and an increased A1*-A3* and A2*-A3*
due to the decreased energy around F3. This is caused by a secondary constriction of the

Given that we used individual GLMM analyses due to the constraints of our data (high
collinearity), the individual
β
and percent correct values provided some insights into the
discriminatory power of each of these acoustic correlates. However, it is not clear how
these behave together and which acoustic correlates are more informative than the others.

that will shed light into the discriminatory power of the combined acoustic correlates.
3.2 Random Forests
The results presented in the previous section showed that none of the acoustic correlates

β
= 
all 26 acoustic correlates (the 13 acoustic correlates at both onset and midpoint) in the


actual data). Overall, the random forest analysis correlated well with our actual data
22
After running the random forest analysis, we used the predict function from the party
package to provide predictions based on the out-of-bag    
specifying OOB=TRUE. This allows the algorithm to train itself on two-thirds of the data
(the in-bag set), and then to use the remaining third of the data (the out-of-bag set) for

  out-of-bag    

Following this, we ran conditional permutations variable importance to measure
the strength of each of the acoustic correlates conditional on each other and by using
varimpAUC and conditional=TRUE. Figure 7    

a particular acoustic correlate is removed.
The results show a clear separation between formant-based and voice quality-based
measures with the former being highly predictive of the two consonant category
(Figure 7). Looking at the results in detail, the ranking of the predictors in JA (Figure 7a)
shows that F2 Onset, Z3-Z2 Onset, followed by Z2-Z1 Onset, are the most important
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 25 of 40
correlates, whereas in MA (Figure 7b), Z2-Z1 Onset followed by F2 Onset, and Z3-Z2
Onset are the most important. All the other correlates have lower values and thus can be


when compared with the GLMM results above, MA potentially shows more mid-lower
pharyngeal constriction, while JA shows mid-upper pharyngeal constriction. More
  

JA showing a score of 0.027 of the conditional mean decrease in accuracy when F2 Onset
is removed, whereas in MA it is a 0.015 decrease for F2 Onset. These results correlate well
with the
β
values presented in the GLMM results (see Section 3.1).

are only used for their predictive accuracy. Table 4 provides a summary of the predictive
accuracy of each of these additional random forests and as a comparison, the results of the


between the two contexts. This is to be expected as pharyngealization is primarily
         
          
            
formant-based metrics have the most explanatory power, and may be suggestive of a
Figure 7: Mean decrease in accuracy importance scores in JA (a) and MA (b).
Table 4: Summary of predictive accuracy as classification rate for each of the random forests in
JA and MA.
Form + BkDiff + VQ Form + BkDiff Form BkDiff Form + VQ BkDiff + VQ VQ
JA 93.5% 93.2% 92.1% 92.9% 92.2% 93.1% 70.2%
MA 91.1% 91.0% 90.5% 90.8% 90.6% 91.0% 75.8%
Form = Absolute Formants; BkDiff = Bark-Difference formants; VQ = Voice Quality.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 26 of 40

metrics to absolute ones (either on their own or combined with voice quality), the results
          
      
metrics as showing the same patterns; they are both indicative of [+ 
[+
pharyngealization, as the GLMM results summarized above (see Figures 3b and 4b)

quality of the vowels in the pharyngealized context, following LAM (Esling, 2005;
Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b). Overall, the ranking of the various metrics was comparable
to those reported in the full model (see Figure 7). However when absolute formants
were used on their own, F2 at the Onset was the main correlate in both dialects. As
expected, these results point to the fact that formant-based metrics are the primary
correlates to pharyngealization in JA and MA.
Finally, the predictive accuracy of voice quality metrics on their own was assessed. The
results presented in the last column of Table 4 suggest that in both JA and MA, voice


metrics, but provide support for voice quality metrics to act as a secondary correlates
to pharyngealization. The ranking of correlates (not presented here) matches what we
already saw in Figure 7, in that A1*-A3*, A2*-A3*, H1*-A1*, H1*-A2* are the most
predictive acoustic correlates in both dialects. However, in MA, spectral slope correlates
are used more than in JA, and JA tends to use more the high-frequency components more.
In both dialects, voice quality measures are indicative of an epilaryngeal constriction used
as a secondary articulatory setting with formant-based metrics being the best predictors.
4 Discussion and Conclusion
This exploratory study is aimed at investigating whether pharyngealization in Arabic is
associated with an epilaryngeal constriction from an acoustic perspective. Traditionally,
pharyngealization in Arabic is generally assumed to involve tongue body (dorsum)
retraction towards the upper-pharyngeal areas that leads to a lowering of the second
formant in the surrounding vowels (e.g., Bin-Muqbil, 2006; Ghazeli, 1977; Watson, 2007;
Zawaydeh, 1999; Zawaydeh & de Jong, 2011). However, both classic and more recent
articulatory evidence show this constriction to be located much lower in the pharynx,
with epiglottis retraction, and raising of the larynx that leads to a pressed/tense voice
quality (see e.g., Al-Tamimi, F. & Heselwood, 2011; Cantineau, 1960; Hess, 1998; Laufer
& Baer, 1988; Lehn, 1963; Zeroual & Clements, 2015; Zeroual et al., 2011, among others).
Following the Laryngeal Articulator Model (Esling, 2005), ‘true’ pharyngeals seem
    
the aryepiglottic folds. Pharyngealization on the other hand seems to show this type of
constriction albeit as a secondary one with a primary pharyngeal constriction (Esling,

are usually investigated (see Section 1.1.1 above). However, these are only indicative of
a pharyngeal constriction and do not directly explain any voice quality correlates that are
a by-product of an epilaryngeal constriction. An epilaryngeal constriction is expected to
back and down movement of the tongue as well as laryngeal
muscles constriction (Esling, 2005; Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b).
This study investigates the absolute formant frequencies, which is typical of such

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 27 of 40

pharyngealization in Arabic. Our results with respect to absolute formants are in agreement
with previous literature, with vowels in the pharyngealized context showing decrease in
F2 and an increase in F1 regardless of the vowel quality. In addition, F3 shows an overall
increase in both dialects, but seems to show lowering when the vowel is front, and rising
when the vowel is back (see Section 3.1.1 and Figure 1). Using the Distinctive Regions
Model (DRM) (Carré & Mrayati, 1992; Mrayati et al., 1988) that is based on the principles

location based on the acoustic output. When F1 is rising, F2 is falling, and F3 is rising as
in back and central vowels, the location of the constriction is close to the DRM region R4
in the upper pharynx. A combination of rising F1, and falling F2 and F3 seems to be close
to the DRM region R3 in the mid-lower pharynx (see e.g., Carré & Mrayati, 1992, Figure 8,
p. 150 and Figure 12, p. 156). These acoustic results are compatible with the articulatory
accounts presented in Ghazeli (1977, p. 174, as cited in Laufer & Baer, 1988, p. 55)
that ‘emphatic’ consonants have a secondary tongue retraction located midway between
uvulars and pharyngeals.
Following the predictions of the Laryngeal Articulator Model (Esling, 2005; Sylak-
Glassman, 2014a, b) pharyngealization is associated with a retracted production that has
a combined back and down
is expected to observe both a lowering of F2 and a rising of F1 as a combined acoustic
consequence. Thus the distance between these two formants can be seen as an alternative

metrics, e.g., Z1-Z0 and Z3-Z2 as correlates of a more open and a more back articulation
and these correlated well with the traditional F1 and F2 dimensions separately. These

by the acoustic vowels spaces (see Figures 3 and 4).

mid pharyngeal constriction. Compared to /d/, Z2-Z1 in the pharyngealized context was
      
Appendix 3), which is close to the range reported in other studies (see e.g., Al-Tamimi,
   
indicative of a much higher constriction location than ‘true’ pharyngeals that have a
smaller distance of about 3 Bark. Z1-Z0 and F1 results in both dialects are indicative of a


118 Hz) that correlates well with a one-degree change on the openness dimension agrees
with the similarity scales between vowels and pharyngeals or uvulars as reported in Sylak-
Glassman (2014b). When in contact with either category, vowels tend to be produced with

with spectral integration (Fahey et al., 1996; Traunmüller, 1983, 1984). When dealing
with ‘phonetically’ similar vowels, i.e., allophones changing in quality due to (non-)
pharyngealization, a Z2-Z1 distance close to or below 6 Bark signals spectral integration
(Traunmüller, 1983, p. 5–6). This as a whole leads to the conclusions that Z2-Z1 seems to
be the



below the 3.5 Bark threshold for spectral integration (Chistovich & Lublinskaya, 1979;
 
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 28 of 40
            
show a clear separation between the F2 and F3 due to the extremely lowered F2 and the

the pharyngealized context is acting as an enhancement feature to the already spectrally
integrated peak, i.e., Z2-Z1.
The combination of these three psychoacoustic metrics at the onset of the vowels in
the pharyngealized context seems to show a direct psychoacoustic manifestation of
the [+      
1952/1976) as an acoustic correlate to pharyngealization. Z2-Z1 seems to be the main
psychoacoustic correlate as it is close and below the threshold for spectral integration,
Z3-Z2 seems to play a role as an enhancement feature and Z1-Z0 seems to correlate well
with the more open articulation.
A second articulatory consequence of an epilaryngeal constriction is voice quality
changes. Ventricular folds constriction leads to a tense, harsh, laryngealized voice quality,
whereas an aryepiglottic constriction leads to ‘trilling’ as seen in ‘true’ pharyngeals
and an ‘enhanced’ voice quality (Edmondson & Esling, 2006; Esling, 1996; Moisik,
2013a, b; Moisik et al., 2010; Story, 2016). Our results with respect to voice quality
correlates—spectral slope and tilt—provide a secondary description to pharyngealization
in Arabic. Pharyngealization is associated with a tense voice quality that is caused by
constricting the intrinsic muscles of the larynx—the ventricular folds (see e.g., Garellek,
2012; Hanson & Chuang, 1999; Hanson et al., 2001; Keating et al., 2015; Kuang & Keating,
2012). Our estimated F1 and F2 bandwidths correlate well with a tense/pressed voice
quality through a decrease in H1*-A1* and H1*-A2* metrics (Garellek, 2012; Hanson
      

bandwidth was found in pharyngeals but an increased F1 bandwidth in uvulars. Our
          and
uvulars through the combined increase in the bandwidths of F1 and F2. Finally, a lowered
H1*-A3* is also suggestive of a tense voice quality through an abrupt constriction of the

Klatt & Klatt, 1990). In JA, an increase in H1*-A3* is indicative of an increased spectral
tilt and partially reduced energy around F3; consequences of a constricted glottis with a
simultaneous front to back closure of the vocal folds (Hanson & Chuang, 1999; Hanson et
al., 2001). This also seems to correlate well with a tense/pressed voice quality (Garellek,
2012; Hanson et al., 2001; Keating et al., 2015; Kuang & Keating, 2012).
The high frequency components provide an additional correlate to the glottal constriction
through a constricted epilarynx and this constriction provides additional energy to the
upper formants when comparing the two categories (Halle & Stevens, 1969; Stevens, 1977;
Story, 2016). Our results showed that in both JA and MA an increased A1*-A3* and A2*-
A3* is found. This is caused by a decrease in the energy around F3 that is potentially caused
by the type of constriction in the glottis. Constricting the epilarynx on its own would lead

the harmonics closest to F3, F4, and F5. This is seen in the singer’s formant—a primary
technique used in signing to enhance the voice quality (Story, 2016; Titze & Story, 1997).
In our case, constricting the epilarynx is secondary; pharyngeal constriction leads to a
boost in the energy around F1 and F2, and the changes seen with respect to F3 will
be minimal. This causes a decrease in the energy around F3 leading to the increased

A2 and A3 more than what
is seen in A1; the latter is relatively higher in amplitude that the former. Their harmonic
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 29 of 40

here; a relatively increased A1*-A3* and A2*-A
to the normal setting (Liénard & Di Benedetto, 1999, Figure 3, p. 416). In this particular

     
comparable to what is seen in tense voice. Finally, and according to Story (2016, Figure 7,
p. 10), a constricted epilarynx would lead to an increase in the frequency and amplitude
of F3 to make it move closer to F4. This closeness correlates with the singer’s formant and
changes the ‘timbre’ or quality of voice of a particular speaker. F1 and F2 are responsible
for the phonetic quality of a vowel, whereas voice quality is manifested through F3 to F5
frequencies and amplitudes. Hence if we postulate that pharyngealization is associated with
an epilaryngeal constriction, F3 to F5 amplitude and frequencies should be investigated as
well. This future work is currently planned on a forthcoming dataset.


results showed a clear separation between formant-based and voice quality-based measures.

pharyngealization in Arabic as involving a constricted glottis through constriction of the

the vowel quality into a more open (high F1 and Z1-Z0), a more compact (low Z2-Z1),
a more retracted (low F2, and high Z3-Z2) with spectral integration (low Z2-Z1) and
 

and spectral divergence, while in MA it is a combined F1 and F2-based through a more
compact spectrum with retraction. When considering absolute formant frequencies alone,
both dialects display the same pattern i.e., F2-based followed by F1-based; results that


is implemented (with some dialects having a more ‘guttural’ quality) can be evaluated

JA (see e.g., Bellem, 2007; Embarki et al., 2011), though it is not clear whether these
   
More detailed articulatory and acoustic description is required to shed light into these

4.1 Implications for formal representations


        
           
[     
(Lindau, 1975, 1978); [      
[        
(Czaykowska-Higgins, 1987); [     
2015); [
(Shahin, 1996, 1997, 2011; Watson, 2007). This list is not exhaustive, however, it highlights


the activity of the retraction of the root of the tongue. It is not clear however whether
this retraction causes only lowering of F2 frequencies (i.e., Z2 and Z3-Z2) or whether it
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 30 of 40
accounts for the more open production with rising of F1 (i.e., Z1, Z1-Z0). In addition,
       
by [+ 
for by [+
[+
that the tongue retraction as seen here is a by-product of constricting the epilarynx (e.g.,
Esling, 2005; Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a, b, and Section 1.2), we follow Moisik
et al. (2012), Moisik and Esling (2011), and Sylak-Glassman (2014a) and postulate that
pharyngealization in Arabic is produced by an epilaryngeal constriction that causes the
tongue root and body to be retracted by a back and down gesture. This induces a laryngeal
constriction leading to a raised larynx posture and a tense/pressed voice quality. Based on
these descriptions and on our results, pharyngealization in Arabic will then be associated
with a [+
[+
though it is not clear whether [+
added as an additional feature to describe pharyngealization (see Sylak-Glassman, 2014a,
p. 136–140). The inclusion of voice quality metrics allows for pharyngealization in Arabic
to be described with [+         
would be that [+    
2011) is the only correlate of pharyngealization. Of course this analysis is only based on
the outcomes of this study and does not look at the patterns in the language. However,
highlighting this laryngeal activity as associated with pharyngealization in Arabic can
potentially shed light into what a ‘guttural’ quality is from a phonetic point of view and
whether it is a ‘voice quality’ in the sense of ‘timbre’ of the vowel (following Story, 2016),
a ‘darker’ or ‘heavier’ auditory quality from formant merging, or both.
4.2 Limitations
This exploratory study highlights some novel acoustic evidence in describing
pharyngealization in Arabic. However, it should be noted that only coronal voiced stops
were investigated. Our formant-based measures are congruent with those reported in

amount of distance between these consonants (on F1, F2, and Z2-Z1 values). It is not clear
whether some of the results obtained are restricted to this category of sounds or whether
they can also be extended to other coronal consonants. We expect the acoustic correlates
          
         
category of sounds, as it is associated with a tense/pressed voice quality through an
epilaryngeal constriction in general, thus we expect to obtain the same degree of high
frequency components regardless of the consonant category. An additional limitation
to our study is the use of stimuli varying in location of the pharyngealized consonants
according to syllable structure and to the surrounding sounds, i.e., other gutturals. This
 

             

to evaluate the results on such a corpus to allow for testing this particular hypothesis.
Our exploratory study did not investigate the acoustic correlates of ‘true’ pharyngeals
or uvulars as our aim was to evaluate which acoustic correlates are mostly associated
with pharyngealization to allow for an extension to other categories. Finally, our study
is acoustic in nature, and any conclusions on exact articulatory consequences should be
  
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 31 of 40

a combined articulatory (e.g., ultrasound, and electoglottagraphy) and advanced acoustic
data of all front and back consonants in Arabic to shed light into the laryngeal constriction
and the exact constriction location of each consonant; this will allow for a meaningful
articulatory-to-acoustic mapping.
5 Conclusion

multiple acoustic correlates in describing pharyngealization. By following the predictions
of the Laryngeal Articulator Model (Esling, 2005) and its subsequent developments
(Moisik, 2013a; Sylak-Glassman, 2014a), we showed how pharyngealization in Arabic is
associated with a ‘retracted’ production, with a combined back and down gesture through

correlates obtained at each of onset and midpoint of the vowel, including formant-based
and voice quality-based measures, it was possible to evaluate the ‘retraction,’ compaction,
and epilaryngeal constriction in our data. Formant distance measures (through

         
between the two dialects. In summary, the results suggest that MA is associated with a
more mid-low pharyngeal constriction and JA with an upper-mid pharyngeal constriction.
Voice quality measures from spectral slope and high frequency components correlated
well with a tense/pressed voice quality and an epilaryngeal constriction respectively.
These novel results were assessed through GLMM and exploratory random forest analyses.
‘Retraction’ (i.e., combined back and down gesture) is the primary acoustic correlate of
pharyngealization in Arabic with an epilaryngeal constriction leading to a [+

measures were used alone, a simple ‘retraction’ would be the main and only correlate to
pharyngealization. Voice quality correlates allowed for the epilaryngeal constriction to be
included. A combined articulatory and acoustic investigation of the state of the epilarynx
in Arabic pharyngeals and pharyngealization is worth pursuing in order to evaluate the
role of the epilarynx as an active ‘articulator.’ A subsequent perceptual study will shed
light into which acoustic correlates are the most prominent in identifying pharyngeals

and discrimination as presented in the current study.
Additional Files

Appendix 1. 
labphon.19.s1
Appendix 2. 
org/10.5334/labphon.19.s2
Appendix 3. 
org/10.5334/labphon.19.s3
Acknowledgements
The author would like to thank Lisa Davidson, Associate Editor of Laboratory
Phonology, Kip Wilson, Editorial Assistant of Laboratory Phonology, three anonymous
reviewers, and Ghada Khattab, Danielle Turton, Dan McCarthy, Scott Moisik, John
Esling, Bodo Winter, François Pellegrino, Thami BenKirane, the audiences of BAAP
2014, the 18th ICPhS, the 2nd Arabic Linguistics Forum, the 2nd PaPE, and the Phonetics

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 32 of 40
of this study. This work was partially supported by the French Research Ministry
o. 41, P.I. R. Carré), the French
2, P.I. F. Pellegrino), and
by three funds to attend BAAP 2014, ICPhS 2015, and PaPE 2017 conferences from

Competing Interests
The author has no competing interests to declare.
References
Al-Ani, S. 1970. Arabic Phonology: An Acoustical and Physiological Investigation


           Proceedings
of the 2003 Texas Linguistics Society conference    

   

Z. (Eds.), Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics,    

Al-Tamimi, J. 2002. Variabilité phonétique en production et en perception de la parole: Le cas
de l’arabe jordano-palestinien (Unpublished master’s thesis). Université Lyon 2, Lyon,
France.

Actes des 25èmes Journées d’Études
sur la Parole (JEP), Dinard, France, 9–12.
Al-Tamimi, J. 2007a. Indices dynamiques et perception des voyelles : Étude translinguistique
en arabe dialectal et en français  
Lyon 2.

Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of
Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS), 541–544. Saarbrücken, Germany.

Workshop “Pharyngeals and
Pharyngealisation”.

 Proceedings of the
British Association of Academic Phoneticians (BAAP) Colloquium.
Al-Tamimi, J. 2015. Spectral tilt as an acoustic correlate to pharyngealisation in Jordanian
    The Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015 (Ed.), Proceedings
of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences    
   
www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2015/Papers/
ICPHS0436.pdf.
Al-Tamimi, J., & Barkat-Defradas, M. 2003. Inter-dialectal and inter-individual variability

    5th AIDA Proceedings (Association
Internationale de Dialectologie Arabe), 171–186. Cádiz, Spain.
Al-Tamimi, J., & Ferragne, E. 2005. Does vowel space size depend on language vowel
Interspeech, 2465–2468.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 33 of 40
Al-Tamimi, J., & Khattab, G. 2015. Acoustic cue weighting in singleton vs geminate contrast
The Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America, 138
Alwan, A. 1986. Acoustic and perceptual correlates of pharyngeal and uvular consonants
       
.
Alwan, A. 1989. Perceptual cues for place of articulation for the voiced pharyngeal and
uvular consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86(2), 549–556.


      The 17th International Congress of Phonetic
Sciences, 240–243. Hong Kong.
         
 (R
package version 2.2.1).
Baayen, R. H. 2008. Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R.


Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.

Baguley, T. 2012. Serious stats: A guide to advanced statistics for the behavioral sciences.
Palgrave Macmillan.
Barkat, M. 2000. Détermination d’indices acoustiques robustes pour l’identication
automatique des parlers arabes     
Lyon 2.
Barkat-Defradas, M., Al-Tamimi, J., & Benkirane, T. 2003. Phonetic variation in
           
Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS), 857–860.
Barcelona, Spain.
               
   Journal of Memory and Language,
68

Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67

Bellem, A. 2007. Towards a Comparative Typology of Emphatics: Across Semitic and Into
Arabic Dialect Phonology.      
Oriental and African Studies, University of London.
Bin-Muqbil, M. S. 2006. Phonetic and phonological aspects of Arabic emphatics and gutturals

Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. 2009. Praat. Doing Phonetics by Computer
.
Breiman, L. 2001. Random forests. Machine learning, 45   

            
Journal
of Pragmatics, 66
Cantineau, J. 1960. Etudes de linguistique arabe
Carré, R., & Mrayati, M. 1992. Distinctive regions in acoustic tubes. Speech production
modeling. Journal d’Acoustique, 5, 141–159.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 34 of 40
Catford, J. C. 1977. Fundamental problems in phonetics
Press.
The Vowel: Its Nature and Structure. Tokyo-Kaiseikan Pub.
Co., Ltd., Tokyo.
              
          
perception of vowel-like stimuli. Hearing Research, 1  
org/10.1016/0378-5955(79)90012-1
Chung, Y., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Dorie, V., Gelman, A., & Liu, J. 2013. A nondegenerate
penalized likelihood estimator for variance parameters in multilevel models.
Psychometrika, 78
Cohen, D. 1962. Koinè, langues communes et dialectes arabes. Arabica, 9

Czaykowska-Higgins, E. 1987. Characterizing tongue root behavior. Manuscript, MIT,
Cambridge, Massachusetts.
DeLong, E. R., DeLong, D. M., & Clarke-Pearson, D. L. 1988. Comparing the areas under
         
approach. Biometrics
Edmondson, J. A., & Esling, J. H. 2006. The valves of the throat and their functioning in
Phonology, 23(2), 157–191.

Embarki, M., Ouni, S., Yeou, M., Guilleminot, C., & Al Maqtari, S. 2011. Acoustic and
 

(Eds.), Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics, 
doi.org/10.1075/cilt.319
Esling, J. 1996. Pharyngeal consonants and the aryepiglottic sphincter. Journal of the
International Phonetic Association, 26   
S0025100300006125
Esling, J. 1999. The IPA Categories “Pharyngeal” and “Epiglottal” Laryngoscopic
Observations of Pharyngeal Articulations and Larynx Height. Language and Speech,
42
The Canadian
Journal of Linguistics/La revue canadienne de linguistique, 50


varying F1F0 Bark distance. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99(4),

Fant, G. 1960/1971. Acoustic theory of speech production: with calculations based on X-ray
studies of Russian articulations, second edition. Walter de Gruyter.
Fant, G. 2004. Speech Acoustics and Phonetics: Selected Writings, 24. Springer Science &



Fulop, S., Kari, E., & Ladefoged, P. 1998. An Acoustic Study of the Tongue Root Contrast in
Degema Vowels. Phonetica, 55
Garellek, M. 2012. The timing and sequencing of coarticulated non-modal phonation
in English and White Hmong. Journal of Phonetics, 40

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 35 of 40
Gelman, A., Jakulin, A., Pittau, M. G., & Su, Y.-S. 2008. A weakly informative default prior
distribution for logistic and other regression models. The Annals of Applied Statistics,

Ghazali, S., Hamdi, R., & Barkat, M. 2002. Speech Rhythm Variation in Arabic Dialects.
 Proceedings of the 1st International conference on Speech Prosody, 331–334.
Aix-en-Provence, France.
Ghazali, S., Hamdi, R., & Knis, K. 2007. Intonation and rhythmic patterns across the
Papers from the annual symposium
on Arabic Linguistics, 97–122.    

Ghazeli, S. 1977. Back Consonants and Backing Coarticulation in Arabic (Doctoral

Guion, S. G., Post, M. W., & Payne, D. L. 2004. Phonetic correlates of tongue root vowel
contrasts in Maa. Journal of Phonetics, 32
wocn.2004.04.002
             Quarterly
Progress Report No. 94, Research Laboratory of Electronics, M.I.T, 209–215.

correlates and comparison with female data. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 106
         
models of phonation. Journal of Phonetics, 29


 (R package version 3.17-4).

       
Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics,     
org/10.1075/cilt.319
Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R., & Friedman, J. 2009. The elements of statistical learning.
       
387-84858-7
Hawks, J. W., & Miller, J. D. 1995. A formant bandwidth estimation procedure for vowel
synthesis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97  

             
feature. Transactions of the Philological Society, 87   

        The Oxford handbook of Arabic
linguistics,      
oxfordhb/9780199764136.013.0003
Heselwood, B., & Al-Tamimi, F. 2011. A study of the laryngeal and pharyngeal consonants
        
Heselwood, B., & Hassan, Z. (Eds.), Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics, 101–128.

Hess, S. A. 1998. Pharyngeal Articulations (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Los Angeles,

Journal
of Speech Sciences, 1
.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 36 of 40
Hoberman, R. D. 1987. Emphasis (pharyngealization) as an autosegmental harmony
feature. Parasession on Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology, 23, 167–181.
F1-F0 distance.
The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96   
org/10.1121/1.410305
Hothorn, T., Bühlmann, P., Dudoit, S., Molinaro, A., & Van Der Laan, M. J. 2006. Survival
ensembles. Biostatistics, 7
Howard, D. M., & Angus, J. 2009. Acoustics and psychoacoustics

Iseli, M., Shue, Y.-L., & Alwan, A. 2007. Age, sex, and vowel dependencies of acoustic
measures related to the voice source. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
121

R. (Ed.), Selected Writings I: Phonological Studies (originally published in 1957 in E. Pulgram
(Ed.): Studies presented to Joshua Whatmough), 
Jakobson, R., Fant, G., & Halle, M. 1952/1976. Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The
distinctive features and their correlates
James, G., Witten, D., & Hastie, T. 2013. An Introduction to Statistical Learning: With
Applications in R
Johnson, K. 2012. Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. Wiley-Blackwell, A John Wiley & Sons,
Ltd., Publication.
Jongman, A., Herd, W., Al-Masri, M., Sereno, J., & Combest, S. 2011. Acoustics and
perception of emphasis in Urban Jordanian Arabic. Journal of Phonetics, 39(1), 85–95.

Kang, H., & Ko, S. 2012. In search of the acoustic correlates of tongue root retraction in three
Altai Hakpo, 22, 179–203.
  
paper.pdf.
 
         , Proceedings of the
18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences     
        
www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2015/Papers/
ICPHS0821.pdf.

Perspectives
on Arabic Linguistics XVI: Papers from the sixteenth annual symposium on Arabic linguistics,
266
Klatt, D., & Klatt, L. 1990. Analysis, synthesis and perception of voice quality variations
among male and female talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87,

Kuang, J., & Keating, P. 2012. Glottal articulations of phonation contrasts and their
acoustic and perceptual consequences. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 111, 123–161.
.
Kuang, J., & Keating, P. 2014. Vocal fold vibratory patterns in tense versus lax phonation
contrasts. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 136

Kuhn, M., & Johnson, K. 2013. Applied predictive modeling, 26
org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6849-3
Ladefoged, P., & Maddieson, I. 1996. The Sounds of the World’s Languages
Blackwell.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 37 of 40
Laufer, A., & Baer, T. 1988. The emphatic and pharyngeal sounds in Hebrew and in Arabic.
Language and Speech, 31
Laver, J. 1980. The Phonetic Description of Voice Quality. Cambridge University Press.
Laver, J. 1994. Principles of Phonetics     
org/10.1017/CBO9781139166621
Lehn, W. 1963. Emphasis in Cairo Arabic. Language, 39   
org/10.2307/410760
R News, 2(3),
18–22.

vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106
doi.org/10.1121/1.428140
UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 30, 1–163.
Lindau, M. 1978. Vowel Features. Language, 54
lan.1978.0066
            
larynx movement. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 50(4B), 1166–1179.

Marçais, P. 1977. Esquisse grammaticale de l’arabe maghrébin
           
Keating, P. (Ed.), Phonological Structure and Phonetic Form: Papers in Laboratory
Phonology III,      
CBO9780511659461.012


Melike. 2016. Heatmap Table Using ggplot2.   
heatmapTable.
Moisik, S. 2013a. The epilarynx in speech  
University of Victoria.
Moisik, S. 2013b. Harsh voice quality and its association with blackness in popular
American media. Phonetica, 69

conceptual tool for understanding lingual-laryngeal contrasts. McGill Working Papers
in Linguistics, 22   
moisik2012.pdf.
Moisik, S., & Esling, J. 2010. Examining the Acoustic Contributions of the Epilaryngeal
Tube to the Voice Source and Vocal Tract Resonance. Canadian Acoustics, 38(3),
.
             
Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS), 1406–1409.
Hong Kong.
Moisik, S., Esling, J., & Crevier-Buchman, L. 2010. A high-speed laryngoscopic
investigation of aryepiglottic trilling. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
127

of speech production. Speech Communication, 7   
org/10.1016/0167-6393(88)90073-8
The phonetic bases of speaker recognition. Cambridge studies in speech science
and communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
A phonetic study of emphasis and vowels in Egyptian Arabic (Unpublished

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 38 of 40
Obrecht, D. H. 1968. Eects of the Second Formant on the Perception of Velarisation Consonants
in Arabic
Oshiro, T. M., Perez, P. S., & Baranauskas, J. A. 2012. How many trees in a random forest?
International Workshop on Machine Learning and Data Mining in Pattern Recognition,


Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 413–


.
       
             
curves. BMC Bioinformatics, 12
Samlan, R. A., & Kreiman, J. 2014. Perceptual consequences of changes in epilaryngeal
area and shape. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 136(5), 2798–2806.

Sarkar, D. 2008. Lattice: Multivariate Data Visualization with R   
     

           
      
 (R package version 0.6-28).

Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1


D. (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics IX: Papers from the Ninth Annual Symposium on
Arabic Linguistics, 
doi.org/10.1075/cilt.141.10sha
          
Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics: Papers from the Annual Symposium on Arabic Linguistics,
10

(Eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, 604–627. Blackwell Publishing.
  Phonetica, 34(4),

          Journal of Phonetics, 17, 3–45.
.
Acoustic Phonetics. MIT Press.
             
Consonants. Language, 65
               
J. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Singing.  
oxfordhb/9780199660773.013.012
Strobl, C., Boulesteix, A.-L., Kneib, T., Augustin, T., & Zeileis, A. 2008. Conditional
variable importance for random forests. BMC Bioinformatics, 9
doi.org/10.1186/1471-2105-9-307
Strobl, C., Boulesteix, A.-L., Zeileis, A., & Hothorn, T. 2007. Bias in random forest variable
BMC Bioinformatics, 8(1),

Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic Art. 28, page 39 of 40

         
random forests. Psychological Methods, 14
a0016973

Quarterly Progress and Status Report, Speech Transmission Laboratory, Royal Institute of
Technology, 22(2–3), 23–36.

   Quarterly Progress and Status Report, Speech Transmission
Laboratory, Royal Institute of Technology, 17, 35–39.
         Annual Meeting of
the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 37
org/proceedings/index.php/BLS/article/download/812/595.
Sylak-Glassman, J. 2014a. Deriving Natural Classes: The Phonology and Typology of Post-
Velar Consonants       
California, Berkeley.
          
Proceedings of the Annual Meetings on Phonology
amp.v1i1.44
Syrdal, A. K., & Gopal, H. S. 1986. A perceptual model of vowel recognition based on
the auditory representation of American English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 79
        
Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and
Change, 24
Thomas, E., & Kendall, T. 2007. NORM: The vowel normalization and plotting suite. [Online

          The
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123   
org/10.1121/1.2832337
Titze, I. R., & Story, B. H. 1997. Acoustic interactions of the voice source with the lower
vocal tract. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101

Traunmüller, H. 1981. Perceptual dimension of openness in vowels. The Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 69
Traunmüller, H. 1983. On Vowels: Perception of Spectral Features, Related Aspects of
Production, and Sociophonetic Dimensions. (Doctoral dissertation, Summary of six papers,
      

Traunmüller, H. 1984. Articulatory and perceptual factors controlling the age- and
sexconditioned variability in formant frequencies of vowels. Speech Communication,
3
Traunmüller, H. 1990. Analytical expressions for the tonotopic sensory scale. The Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America, 88
Versteegh, K. 2001. The Arabic Language. Edinburgh University Press.
Watson, J. C. E. 2007. The Phonology and Morphology of Arabic. Oxford University Press.
Wickham, H. 2007. Reshaping Data with the reshape Package. Journal of Statistical
Software, 21.
Winter, B. 2016. Analysis of rapid prosody transcription experiment. 
.
Al-Tamimi: Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in
Jordanian and Moroccan Arabic
Art. 28, page 40 of 40

palatal vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 80

      
perception. Revue de La Faculté des Lettres El Jadida, 6, 51–70.
Zawaydeh, B. 1999. The phonetics and phonology of gutturals in Arabic (Unpublished


Heselwood, B., & Hassan, Z. (Eds.), Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics, 257–276.


R., & H. van der Hulst (Eds.), Features in Phonology and Phonetics: posthumous Writings
by George N. Clements and Coauthors
Zeroual, C., Esling, J. H., & Hoole, P. 2011. EMA, endoscopic, ultrasound and acoustic

Z. (Eds.), Instrumental studies in Arabic phonetics,    

How to cite this article: Al-Tamimi, J. 2017 Revisiting acoustic correlates of pharyngealization in Jordanian and
Moroccan Arabic: Implications for formal representations.
Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for
Laboratory Phonology
8(1): 28, pp. 1–40, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.19
Submitted: 25 April 2016 Accepted: 20 June 2017 Published: 20 November 2017
Copyright: © 2017 The Author(s). This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
OPEN ACCESS
Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology
is a
peer-reviewed open access journal published by Ubiquity Press.
... In vowels, acoustic correlates of emphasis include lowering of the frequency of the second formant (F2) (Al-Tamimi, 2017;Ghazeli, 1977;Hassan & Esling, 2011;Zawaydeh, 1999, among others) and occasional raising of the frequency of the first formant (F1) (Al-Ani, 1970;Al-Tamimi, 2017). In consonants, the emphatic property typically correlates with lower frequency of noise, the effect being relatively weak (Jongman et al., 2011). ...
... In vowels, acoustic correlates of emphasis include lowering of the frequency of the second formant (F2) (Al-Tamimi, 2017;Ghazeli, 1977;Hassan & Esling, 2011;Zawaydeh, 1999, among others) and occasional raising of the frequency of the first formant (F1) (Al-Ani, 1970;Al-Tamimi, 2017). In consonants, the emphatic property typically correlates with lower frequency of noise, the effect being relatively weak (Jongman et al., 2011). ...
... frequencies (Al-Ani, 1970;Al-Tamimi & Heselwood, 2011;Al-Tamimi, 2017;Bukshaisha, 1985;Ghazeli, 1977;Khattab et al., 2006). The co-articulatory effect of emphatic articulation is very strong, compared to other types of CV coarticulation. ...
Preprint
This paper investigates the role of F2 and VOT in realization of the contrast in emphasis among speakers of Arabic varieties of the Levant (Lebanese, Syrian) and the Gulf (Saudi, Qatari). The results show that the two dialect groups systematically differ in acoustic realization of plain and emphatic voiceless stops. While Lebanese and Syrian varieties reveal the traditional pattern, in which the contrast is predominantly realized as a difference in F2 (Plain: 1808 Hz, Emphatic: 1097 Hz), Qatari and Saudi ones demonstrate a pattern with VOT as the main acoustic correlate. Plain [t] is produced with aspiration (M = 72 ms), and emphatic [tˁ] is unaspirated (M = 17 ms). The difference in F2 in the Gulf speech is, in contrast, smaller: low vowel [aː] is back in both contexts, with more retraction in the emphatic context (Plain: 1230 Hz; Emphatic: 1108 Hz).
... In this study, we systematically compare the performance of the two approaches at quantifying within and between-subject and gender variability using UT data, by examining a particular contrast in Levantine Arabic: impact of the phonological secondary pharyngealisation vs plain coronal (plain henceforth) contrasts using a whole tongue approach. Previous research has shown that pharyngealisation has a general backing and retraction effect observed on the consonant itself and surrounding vowels [10]. The backing effect is related to the front-back dimension of the vocal tract, while, retraction is described as a general backing and lowering effect of the tongue dorsum and root. ...
... PC2 showed a non-statistically significant raised tongue front, lowered tongue body and marginal retracted tongue dorsum (p=0.5, Figure 1d dashed lines); PC3 showed a lowered tongue mid and root (p<0.01, Figure 1e dotted lines) and PC4 showed a tendency for a raised tongue back and lowered root (p=0.09, Figure 1f dashed lines). The results obtained from the four PCs are correlated and confirm the general tongue retraction, tilting, backing and raising, with marginal tongue tip changes reported in [8,10]. Figure 2 shows the predictions from our GAMMs. ...
... This study confirmed prior results on the impact of pharyngealisation on the tongue shape reported in [8,10]. Both PCA and GAMMs showed similar patterns. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Accounting for between and within-subject variability is a relatively easy task when one uses mixed effects regression [1, 2]. Modelling speaker (and items) as random effects allows for coefficients of the main effects to be adjusted to account for this type of variability [1, 2]. Using a maximal specification approach [1] allows for an accurate estimation of the within and between-subject variation, although this heavily depends on the structure of the data and the need to assess the model’s fit to allow for a meaningful interpretation of the patterns observed. When dealing with Ultrasound Tongue Imaging (UTI) data, however, anatomical differences between speakers, for instance in tongue size, lead to important within and between subject variation that can hinder any cross-speaker generalisations, unless properly dealt with. The aim of our paper is to demonstrate how this can be achieved using two types of analyses, namely static and dynamic Principal Component Analysis (PCA), and static and dynamic Generalised Additive Mixed-effects Models (GAMMs). PCA emerges as a relatively easy, simple and robust approach to account for between-subject variability using UTI data [3] and has since been employed in various studies using UTI data on laterals [4], rhotic consonants [5] and acquisition of laterals/rhotics [6]. GAMMs on the other hand, while it has already been used on UTI data trying to account for within and between-subject variability [7, 8, 9], they are still not widely used by the community to account for between-subject and gender differences observed using UTI data.
... Despite differences in details, the overall picture is consistent: the emphatics and q have a constriction in the upper pharynx similar to that of the uvular gutturals χ and ʁ. Although there are suggestions (Keating, 1988) at "Arabic emphatics are uvularized" (Al-Tairi et al., 2016, p. 1) does not exclude the possibility that these consonants may additionally involve the same epiglotto-pharyngeal constriction as pharyngeals in certain varieties (Wallin, 1855, p. 612;Laufer and Baer, 1988;Al-Tamimi and Heselwood, 2011;Hassan and Esling, 2011;Al-Tamimi, 2017). However, recall from Section 2.1 that "the tongue body is not back but front with the Arabic pharyngeals, as we can see by the adjacent front allophone of the low vowel: compare pharyngeal [ħaeːl] 'condition' with uvular [χɑːl] 'maternal uncle"' (McCarthy, 1994, p. 197). ...
... 124). Alternative replacements include [lower pharynx] (Czaykowska-Higgins, 1987, p. 13), [laryngopharynx] (Hess, 1998, p. 268-271), [constricted epilaryngeal tube] (Moisik and Esling, 2011;Moisik et al., 2012;Al-Tamimi, 2017;Al-Tairi, 2018;Esling et al., 2019), and [constricted epilarynx] (Sylak-Glassman, 2014). ese various features were introduced to model the phonetic realities of pharyngealization more accurately than [low]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article applies the notion of redeployment in second language acquisition to contact-induced diachronic changes. Of special interest are cases where a marked phonological contrast has spread across neighboring languages. Such cases suggest that listeners can re-weight and re-map phonetic cues onto novel phonological structures. On the redeployment view, cues can indeed be re-weighted, but phonological structures which underlie a new contrast are not expected to be fully novel; rather, they must be assembled from preexisting phonological structures. Emphatics are an instructive case. These are (mostly) coronal consonants articulated with tongue-root retraction. Phonological emphasis is rare among the world's languages but it is famously endogenous in Arabic and in Interior Salish and it has spread from these to not a few neighboring languages. The present study describes and analyzes the genesis of phonological emphasis and its exogenous spread to a dozen mostly unrelated languages—from Arabic to Iranian and Caucasian languages, among others, and from Interior Salish to Athabaskan and Wakashan languages. This research shows that most languages acquire emphatics by redeploying the phonological feature [RTR] (retracted tongue root) from preexisting uvulars. On the other hand, some languages acquire imitations of emphatics by redeploying the consonantal use of [low] from preexisting pharyngeals. Phonological emphasis is apparently not borrowed by neighboring languages where consonants lack a phonological feature fit for redeployment. The overall impression is that a language in contact with emphatics may newly adopt these sounds as [RTR] or [low] only if the relevant feature is already in use in its consonant system. This pattern of adoption in language contact supports the redeployment construct in second language acquisition theory.
... Pour ce qui est des mesures acoustiques, Kuang & Keating (2014) et Al-Tamimi (2017) ont montré qu'une voix tendue, craquée et/ou laryngalisée conduit à un abaissement global de la pente spectrale, en suivant le modèle psychoacoustique de la qualité de voix (Garellek et al., 2016;Kreiman et al., 2021). Ces changements induisent un abaissement des mesures suivantes 2 : ⇓H1-H2, ⇓H1-A1, ⇓H1-A2, ⇓H2-H4, ⇓H4-H2kHz, ⇓H2kHz-H5kHz, ⇓HNR et ⇓SHR (Fulop et al., 1998;Guion et al., 2004;Aralova et al., 2011;Kuang & Keating, 2014;Al-Tamimi, 2015;Garellek et al., 2016;Al-Tamimi, 2017). De plus, un renforcement de la saillance spectrale entre F1 et F2, quantifié via ⇓A1-A2, est observé. ...
... Le QF reste le prédicteur le plus important pour C, tandis que les mesures acoustiques A1*-A3*, A2*-A3*, H1*-A3*, H4*-H2kHz*, H2kHz*-H5kHz* étaient parmi les prédicteurs les plus importants dans toutes les séquences pour discriminer les six classes. Toutes ces mesures sont indicatives de variations liées à la pente spectrale, avec des pentes plus raides indicatives d'une augmentation d'énergie dans les hautes fréquences causée soit par une fermeture abrupte de la glotte (Hanson et al., 2001;Al-Tamimi, 2017) consonnes pharyngales montrant un pattern ⇑HL tout au long de la séquence VCV, les consonnes pharyngalisées avec un pattern ⇓ dans V1, ⇑ dans C et ⇓ dans V2 ; la consonne uvulaire suit un pattern similaire mais avec ⇑HL dans la phase de relâchement (intervalle 9). Pour le QF (Figure 2b) maximale du larynx tout au long de la séquence VCV causée par une constriction épilaryngale, suivant les prédictions du LAM. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Cette étude examine le rôle de l’activité laryngale dans la production des consonnes d’arrière en arabe levantin. 26 mesures incluant la hauteur du larynx (HL), le contact de glotte (quotient fermé; QF) et la pente spectrale (PS) ont été obtenues de données d’éléctroglottographies et d’acoustiques synchronisées. À partir des classifications via des forêts aléatoires (Random Forests), sept mesures ont été identifiées comme les plus importantes pour discriminer entre les six classes. Ensuite, une modélisation via des Régressions Additives à Effets-Mixtes montre que les consonnes pharyngales sont associées à ⇑HL, ⇑QF et ⇑PS, résultant d’une différence de la saillance spectrale causée par une constriction épilaryngale. Les consonnes pharyngalisées induisent des traits ⇓HL, ⇓QF et ⇓PS causés par une fermeture abrupte de la glotte; les consonnes uvulaires induisent des traits ⇑HL, ⇓QF et ⇓PS. Ces changements sont corrélés avec le trait [+Constricted Glottis] et suivent les prédictions du Laryngeal Articulator Model. This study examines the role of the laryngeal activity during the production of back consonants in Levantine Arabic. 26 measures including Larynx Height (LH), glottal contact (Closed Quotient; CQ) and Spectral Tilt (ST) were obtained from synchronised electroglottographic and acoustic data. Using classifications via Random Forests, seven measures were identified as the most important to discriminate between the six classes. Next, modelling the data via Generalised Additive MixedModels revealed that pharyngeal consonants to be associated with an ⇑LH, a ⇑CQ and an ⇑ST, resulting from a difference in spectral saliency due to an epilaryngeal constriction. Pharyngealised consonants induced the features ⇓LH, ⇓CQ and ⇓ST caused by an abrupt closure of the glottis. Uvular consonants show the features ⇑LH, ⇓CQ and ⇓ST. These changes are correlated with the feature [+Constricted Glottis] and follow predictions of the Laryngeal Articulator Model.
... The articulatory and acoustic data suggest that the pharyngeal vowels involve an epilaryngeal constriction for most speakers, a maneuver readily distinguished from lower vocal tract articulations often called "pharyngealization", such as emphasis or uvularization (Evans et al. 2016;al-Tamimi 2017). Lower pharyngeal or epilaryngeal constriction is clearly suggested by both the F2-raising effect seen in the pharyngeal vowels (as opposed to F2-lowering for uvularization) and the characteristic double-bunching also observed in languages with lower pharyngeal constrictions (Catford 1983;Arkhipov et al. 2019). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
L'utilisation de corrélats acoustiques dans la production de l’ironie a été bien documentée. Cependant, dans quelle mesure les résultats sont comparables dans différentes langues reste une question inexplorée. Cette étude vise à réaliser une comparaison des caractéristiques de l’intonation ironique entre le français et le mandarin, en utilisant un protocole expérimental unifié. Une expérience de production a été menée pour susciter l’énoncé ironique. Les résultats ont d'abord été analysés par forêts aléatoires pour explorer le poids relatif de huit corrélats acoustiques comme marqueur de l'ironie. Ensuite, des modèles linéaires à effets mixtes (LMM) ont été utilisés pour explorer davantage les principaux corrélats acoustiques. Nos résultats ont confirmé que la caractéristique de l’intonation ironique est spécifique à chaque langue, révélant des schémas différents de corrélats acoustiques utilisés pour produire l’ironie en français et en mandarin. De plus, un effet de genre sur l’énoncé ironique en français a été identifié. The use of acoustic correlates in the production of ironic speech has been well-documented. However, the extent to which we can compare the results in different languages remains questionable. This study aims to conduct a comparison of ironic tone of voice in French and Mandarin, using a unified experimental protocol. A production experiment was conducted to elicit ironic speech. The results were first subjected to a Random Forest analysis in which we explored the relative weight of eight acoustic correlates for ironic speech. Secondly, a series of linear mixed-effects models (LMM) were built to further explore the major acoustic correlates. Our results revealed that the ironic tone of voice is language-specific, indicating distinct patterns of acoustic correlates used in French and Mandarin for ironic speech. Additionally, a gender effect on French ironic speech was identified.
Chapter
The volume espouses an ecosystemic standpoint on multilingual acquisition and learning, viewing language development and use as both ontogenesis and phylogenesis. Multilingualism is inclusively used to refer to sociolinguistic diversity and pluralism. Whether speech, writing, gesture, or body movement, language is a conduit that carries meaning within a complex, fluid, and context-dependent framework that engages different aspects of the individual, the communicative interaction, communicative acts, and social parameters. Continually modified over the years to better represent its multidisciplinary scope, the sociobiological notion of language has found steady and productive ground within major theoretical frameworks, which, individually or holistically, contribute to a rounded understanding of language acquisition, learning, and use by exploring both system-internal and system-external factors and their interaction. Summoning the work of leading academics, the volume outlines the changing dynamics of multilingualism in children and adults internationally with the latest advances and under-represented coverage that highlight the ecosystemic nature of multilingual acquisition, learning, and use.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Introduction. This study aims at evaluating the role of laryngeal changes associated with guttural consonants in Levan-tine Arabic. Laryngeal changes are reported in a handful of studies on Arabic guttural consonants; e.g., variable creaky voice using H1-H2 measure for the variable types of /Q/ across Arabic dialects (Heselwood 2007); raised larynx in pha-ryngealised coronals via videofluoroscopy (F. Al-Tamimi and Heselwood 2011); raised larynx and creaky voice quantified via videofluoroscopy and H1-H2 in pharyngeals (Heselwood and F. Al-Tamimi 2011); or more recently lower spectral tilt measures of Voice Quality quantifying the degree of tense voice in pharyngealised coronals (J. Al-Tamimi 2017). Following these findings, we wanted to evaluate laryngeal changes in gutturals using non-invasive techniques. Gutturals are traditionally composed of uvulars /q X K/ and pharyngeals /è Q/ (McCarthy 1994) although pharyngealised consonants /t Q d Q ð Q s Q / and glottals /P h/ are also included due to the constriction location being somewhere in the lower vocal tract. Recently, J. Al-Tamimi and Palo (2023) provided an empirical evidence that gutturals share articulatory similarities in how they impact on the tongue contours quantified via Ultrasound Tongue Imaging (UTI). The results also suggested that gutturals (uvular, pharyngealised and pharyngeal) show a potential for a raised larynx posture due to a variable degree of tongue root retraction. However, and due to the difficulty in quantifying laryngeal changes from UTI alone, this study uses a combined acoustic and Electroglottography (EGG) measures to quantify the (dis-)similarities between gutturals with respect to systematic laryngeal changes. We ask the question whether gutturals show a systematic larynx raising, when compared to plain coronals (or velars and glottals) and whether there is a systematic spectral tilt lowering, which could be indicative of a more creaky or tense voice quality. Following the predictions of the Laryngeal Articulator Model; LAM (Esling et al. 2019) and the findings from J. Al-Tamimi and Palo (2023), we expect gutturals to show systematic differences to non-gutturals but to show a more gradient larynx height and constriction (quantified via Closed Quotient; CQ and Peak Increase in Contact; PIC). We also expect them to show a gradient spectral tilt decrease as quantified via the various acoustic measures of voice quality.
Article
Despite the significant body of research in linguistics, there has been limited exploration of the attrition or retention of the first language in a homeland after a person ceases to use or learn it. However, this phenomenon has become apparent in the Saudi community, particularly in the younger generation who are losing their mother tongue (L1) despite living in their homeland. To address this issue, the present study focuses on L1 attrition and examines lexical disfluency in the oral production of Arabic among 36 Saudi children living in Saudi Arabia. They are L1 Arabic speakers who learned English as their second language L2 while studying in international schools from grade one to six. The study compares the level of their Arabic proficiency to that of other Saudi children studying in national schools. The latter group speaks Arabic as their L1 and English as a foreign language. The aim of the study is to identify the extent to which exposure to English as L2 affects the oral production of Arabic among L1 Arabic speakers. This study is significant because the loss of L1 can result in the erosion of cultural identity and the weakening of social ties within a community (Genesee, 2008). The study provides insights into the factors that contribute to L1 attrition. This can inform efforts to preserve Arabic language and culture in Saudi Arabia, particularly among younger generations who are at risk of losing their L1. The results showed high level of attrition in the attritor group.
Book
Underlying the apparent diversity shown by thousands of mutually incomprehensible languages of the world, there is a remarkable, elegant and principled unity in the way that these languages exploit the phonetic resources of speech. It is these principles that Professor Laver sets out to describe in this major new textbook. Assuming no previous knowledge of the subject, it is designed for readers who wish to pursue the study of phonetics from an initial to an advanced stage, equipping them with the necessary foundations for independent research. The book moves from a discussion of general concepts to a total of eleven chapters on phonetic classification, and it includes discussion of other issues such as the relationship between phonetics and phonology. There are illustrations from over 500 of the world's languages. Principles of Phonetics will be required reading for all serious students of speech and language.
Chapter
Arabic is one of the world’s largest languages, spoken natively by about 300 million speakers. It is by a large margin the largest language in Africa (nearly 200 million speakers), and one of the biggest in Asia (120 million). It has been estimated to be the fifth largest language in the world in terms of native speakers. Strength of numbers alone guarantees it communicative centrality in the world language system. This Handbook reflects the full breadth of research on Arabic Linguistics in the West, covering topics such as pidgins and creoles, Arabic second language acquisition, loanwords, Arabic dialects, codeswitching, psycholinguistics, sociolinguistics, and Arabic lexicography. The Handbook brings together different approaches and scholarly traditions, an invitation to the reader to explore the many faceted world of Arabic Linguistics. The articles in this volume expertly explore the nature of the house of Arabic from many angles. Many argue for specific points of view, others give descriptions of synoptic breadth, while others provide exhaustive overviews of the state of the art. The parts may or may not come together to describe a common structure; they do provide blueprints for a better understanding of it.