ArticlePDF Available

Lonelier than ever? Loneliness of older people over two decades

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To live with feelings of loneliness has negative implications for quality of life, health and survival. This study aimed to examine changes in loneliness among older people, both with regard to prevalence rates, and socio-demographic, social and health-related correlates of loneliness. This study had a repeated cross-sectional design and was based on the nationally representative Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD). Analyses of trends in loneliness covered the years 1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014, and included people aged 77 years or older (n = 2 572). Analyses of correlates of loneliness covered 2004 and 2014, and included people aged 70 years or older (n = 1 962). Logistic regression analyses were conducted with findings presented as average marginal effects. Contrary to what is often assumed, there has been no increase in loneliness among older people over time (1992–2014). Regression analyses for 2004 and 2014 showed that social and health-related correlates were more strongly associated with loneliness than socio-demographic correlates. Psychological distress was most strongly associated with loneliness, followed by widowhood. Most associations between the correlates and loneliness were stable over time.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/archger
Lonelier than ever? Loneliness of older people over two decades
Lena Dahlberg
a,b,
, Neda Agahi
a
, Carin Lennartsson
a
a
Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet/Stockholm University, Gävlegatan 16, 113 30 Stockholm, Sweden
b
School of Education, Health and Social Studies, Dalarna University, 791 88 Falun, Sweden
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Loneliness
Trend
Risk factor
Predictor
Sweden
ABSTRACT
To live with feelings of loneliness has negative implications for quality of life, health and survival. This study
aimed to examine changes in loneliness among older people, both with regard to prevalence rates, and socio-
demographic, social and health-related correlates of loneliness.
This study had a repeated cross-sectional design and was based on the nationally representative Swedish
Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD). Analyses of trends in loneliness covered the years
1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014, and included people aged 77 years or older (n= 2 572). Analyses of cor-
relates of loneliness covered 2004 and 2014, and included people aged 70 years or older (n= 1 962). Logistic
regression analyses were conducted with ndings presented as average marginal eects.
Contrary to what is often assumed, there has been no increase in loneliness among older people over time
(19922014). Regression analyses for 2004 and 2014 showed that social and health-related correlates were more
strongly associated with loneliness than socio-demographic correlates. Psychological distress was most strongly
associated with loneliness, followed by widowhood. Most associations between the correlates and loneliness
were stable over time.
1. Introduction
Loneliness has been dened as the discrepancy between an in-
dividuals desired and achieved levels of social relationships (Perlman &
Peplau, 1981). To live with feelings of loneliness is not only a problem
in itself, it also has implications for quality of life, physical and mental
health, and mortality (e.g. Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010; Holt-Lunstad
et al., 2015;Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015;
O'Luanaigh & Lawlor, 2008).
There is a common belief that older people experience loneliness
more often than other age groups. For example, in both 1982 and 2005,
the vast majority of respondents in a Swedish population survey be-
lieved that almost half of the pensioners often feel lonely (Tornstam,
2007) and images that older people suer from loneliness are often
spread in media (cf. Ferreira-Alves, Magalhaes, Viola, & Simoes, 2014).
Another common belief is that recent cohorts of older people experi-
ence loneliness to a larger extent than previous cohorts, as a result of
changes in family patterns, such as smaller family size, increased di-
vorce rates and greater geographical distance between family members
(Dykstra, 2009), and transitions towards more individualistic societies
(World Values Survey, 2016). Changes in family patterns and societal
changes also mean that there may be other groups of people that are
vulnerable to loneliness today than in earlier cohorts, that is, that
factors associated with loneliness may have changed over time. Based
on a Swedish national survey, this study will examine whether lone-
liness among older people has increased in the last two decades and
whether there have been any changes in socio-demographic, social and
health-related factors associated with loneliness.
1.1. Trends in loneliness
The assumption that loneliness among older people has increased
over time has been disputed, and a research review has found a slight
decrease in loneliness (Dykstra, 2009). More recent studies have found
that there is no change over time (Honigh-de Vlaming, Haveman-Nies,
Groeniger, de Groot, & van t Veer, 2014) or decreased levels of re-
ported loneliness (Eloranta, Arve, Isoaho, Lehtonen, & Viitanen, 2015).
In a British study, levels of loneliness among older people in 1999 were
compared to ndings in studies conducted between 1945 and 1960.
Even in such long time perspective, no increase in severe loneliness was
found (Victor et al., 2002).
1.2. Factors associated with loneliness
Factors associated with feelings of loneliness can be grouped into
socio-demographic, social and health-related factors. Starting with
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2017.11.004
Received 29 November 2016; Received in revised form 20 September 2017; Accepted 12 November 2017
Corresponding author at: School of Education, Health and Social Studies, Dalarna University, 791 88 Falun, Sweden.
E-mail address: ldh@du.se (L. Dahlberg).
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
Available online 16 November 2017
0167-4943/ © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
T
socio-demographic factors, loneliness is more common among the
oldest old than in younger age groups (O'Luanaigh & Lawlor, 2008;
Pinquart & Sörensen, 2001; Routasalo & Pitkala, 2003), and loneliness
in older people increases with age (Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee, &
Lennartsson, 2015;Heikkinen & Kauppinen, 2011; Jylhä, 2004).
Loneliness is also more often found in women than men (Aartsen &
Jylhä, 2011; Cohen-Manseld et al., 2009;Dykstra, van Tilburg, & de
Jong Gierveld, 2005). However, research has shown that these asso-
ciations with loneliness have less to do with age and gender per se than
with associated factors such as widowhood and greater levels of health
problems among the oldest old and among women (Dahlberg et al.,
2015).
Low socioeconomic status is another socio-demographic factor as-
sociated with loneliness. Education and income have often been used as
indicators of socioeconomic status. Both these indicators have been
found to be associated with loneliness, partly due to fewer possibilities
for social participation and smaller social networks among people with
low levels of income and education (see Dykstra & de Jong Gierveld,
1999; Jylhä & Saarenheimo, 2010; Pinquart & Sörensen, 2001;
Routasalo & Pitkala, 2003;Savikko, Routasalo, Tilvis, Strandberg, &
Pitkala, 2005).
Social factors inuencing loneliness include, for example, marital
status, social support and social contacts. There is a large body of re-
search showing an association between marital status and loneliness.
More specically, the loss of partner is a key predictor of loneliness in
old age (Aartsen & Jylhä, 2011; Dahlberg & McKee, 2014; Dahlberg
et al., 2015; Dykstra et al., 2005; Jylhä & Saarenheimo, 2010). As
people age and are confronted with health problems, social contacts
may focus more on the need for support, and people with larger social
support networks have been found to be less likely to report loneliness
(Dahlberg, Andersson, & Lennartsson, In press; Dykstra & Fokkema,
2007). Low levels of social contacts also increase the risk of loneliness
(e.g. Ayalon, Shiovitz-Ezra, & Palgi, 2013;Victor, Scambler, & Bond,
2009).
Finally, health problems, such as mobility diculties and depres-
sion have been found to be associated with loneliness (Aartsen & Jylhä,
2011; Cohen-Manseld et al., 2009; Heikkinen & Kauppinen, 2011;
O'Luanaigh & Lawlor, 2008; Tijhuis et al., 1999). People with low
physical functioning are more likely to experience loneliness (Aartsen &
Jylhä, 2011; Honigh-de Vlaming et al., 2014; Jylhä, 2004; Routasalo &
Pitkala, 2003), as mobility diculties may be a barrier to social en-
gagement (Cohen-Manseld & Parpura-Gill, 2007). A recent study has
shown that loneliness has become more common over time among
people with low physical functioning (Honigh-de Vlaming et al., 2014).
Psychological problems, such as depression and anxiety, have also been
found to be associated with higher levels of loneliness (Eloranta et al.,
2015; Heikkinen & Kauppinen, 2011; O'Luanaigh & Lawlor, 2008).
In this study, loneliness among the oldest old in Sweden over the
last two decades is examined. The aim is to identify changes in lone-
liness over time, both with regard to prevalence levels and to socio-
demographic, social and health-related factors associated with lone-
liness.
2. Design and methods
2.1. Sample
This study has a repeated cross-sectional design and is based on the
Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD)
(Lennartsson et al., 2014). SWEOLD is a nationally representative
survey of the oldest old (born between 1892 and 1944) living in Sweden
at the time of data collection. SWEOLD provides comparable data from
1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014. The SWEOLD sample includes re-
spondents aged 77 years or older in 1992, 2002 and 2011. In 2004 and
2014, the sample was extended to include individuals aged 70 years or
older. Face-to-face interviews were carried out as the main interview
mode in 1992, 2002 and 2011. In 2004 and 2014, telephone interviews
were used as the main interview mode. In 2004, 2011 and 2014, postal
questionnaires were used if the respondent did not agree to an ordinary
interview or was unable to conduct an ordinary interview due to, for
example, hearing problems. There were no signicant dierences in
reported loneliness across the interview modes (p = 0.192).
The response rates varied between 84.4 and 95.4 percent (see
Table 1). The low non-response rates, the inclusion of institutionalized
persons and the use of proxy informants for people unable to be in-
terviewed directly ensure that the SWEOLD sample is representative of
older people in Sweden in each interview wave. In total, 4 566 inter-
views have been conducted. In some interview waves, only directly
interviewed respondents have received the question about loneliness.
Therefore, the analytical sample for this study excluded respondents
who could not perform the interview on their own (see Table 1).
In this study, analyses of the trends in loneliness from 1992 to 2014
included people aged 77 years or older, with an analytical sample of
2 572 (approximately 500 in each data collection wave; see Table 1). In
analyses to determine whether the association of sociodemographic,
social and health factors with loneliness have changed over time, 2004
and 2014 interview waves were used. These interview waves used the
same main interview mode and included people age 70 years or older,
with an analytical sample of 1 962 (n = 921 in 2004; n = 1 041 in
2014).
Informed verbal consent was obtained prior to each interview.
Ethical approvals for the SWEOLD study have been provided by
Uppsala University Hospital (reg.no. 247/91), Karolinska Institutet
Regional Research Ethics Committee (reg.no. 03-413) and the Regional
Ethical Review Board in Stockholm (reg.no. 04-314/5; 2010/403-31/4;
2014/1003-31/5).
2.2. Material
2.2.1. Dependent variable
Loneliness was measured through the item: Are you ever bothered
by feelings of loneliness?with four response categories. Analyses of
trends in the prevalence of loneliness present data on all four response
categories. Due to small numbers in some of the response categories,
loneliness was transformed into a dichotomous variable for the re-
gression analyses, indicating being frequently lonely (collapsing
Table 1
Number of respondents and response rates in SWEOLD for people aged 77 years or older in 1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014, and for people aged 70 years or older in 2004 and 2014.
1992 2002 2004 2011 2014 2004 2014
Age 77+ Age 77+ Age 77+ Age 77+ Age 77+ Age 70+ Age 70+
n% n% n% n% n% n % n %
Respondents 537 95.4 621 84.4 648 89.3 647 86.4 575 84.6 1110 87.5 1226 84.4
Direct interviews 473 88.1 539 86.8 509 78.5 541 83.6 553 96.2 929 83.7 1106 90.2
Indirect interviews 64 11.9 82 13.2 139 21.5 106 16.4 22 3.8 181 16.3 120 9.8
Non-response 26 4.6 115 15.6 78 10.7 102 13.6 105 15.4 158 12.5 227 15.6
Gross sample 563 100.0 736 100.0 726 100.0 749 100.0 680 100.0 1268 100.0 1453 100.0
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
97
response categories: nearly always and often) and rarely lonely (col-
lapsing response categories: seldom and almost never).
2.2.2. Independent variables
Socio-demographic variables included gender, age in years and
educational level (1 = lower education level, dened as grade school,
0 = higher education level, dened as beyond grade school).
Social factors included marital status (married/cohabiting, di-
vorced/unmarried and widow/widower), social support and social
contacts. Regarding marital status, divorced and unmarried individuals
were grouped due to small numbers of respondents in these groups.
Information about social support was collected via the item One
sometimes needs help and support from someone. Do you have any
relative or close friend who helps out if you need to talk to someone
about personal problems?(response alternatives: Yes; No). Social
contacts were measured via four items concerning visiting and/or being
visited by friends and/or relatives (response alternatives: No (0); Yes,
sometimes (1); Yes, often (2)). This social contact scale was reversed
and ranged from no social contacts (8) to frequent social contacts (0).
Finally, health-related factors covered limitations in activities of
daily living (ADL) and psychological distress. ADL reected the number
of the following areas with which the respondent reported diculties:
eating, getting into and out of bed, going to the toilet, getting dressed,
and washing ones hair (scale range: 05). Psychological distress was
measured via an item asking whether respondents had experienced
anxiety, nervousness or angstin the 12 months preceding the inter-
view (response categories: No; Yes, mild; Yes, severe).
2.3. Analysis
Analysis was directed towards determining trends in loneliness for
the total sample. This was done via descriptive analyses. For tests of
statistical signicance in trends over time, linear and logistic regression
analyses were used. Gender dierences were analysed via Chi-square
tests.
Thereafter, analyses were performed to identify correlates of lone-
liness in 2004 and 2014. As a rst step, descriptive analyses were
performed for the total sample in 2004 and 2014. Thereafter, logistic
regression analyses were undertaken for the same samples with the
margins option in STATA 12. This produces coecients as average
marginal eects (AME). Unlike odds ratios, AMEs can be compared
across models and outcomes. AMEs are interpreted as the average im-
pact on the probability of experiencing loneliness. Bivariate analyses
were performed to identify associations between independent variables
and loneliness (dependent variable) in 2004, and between independent
variables and loneliness (dependent variable) in 2014. In addition,
multivariable logistic regression analyses were performed with AMEs
for 2004 and 2014, respectively. All independent variables were en-
tered simultaneously into each regression analysis. Changes in corre-
lates over time were further examined via analyses of interaction be-
tween correlates and survey year.
Some respondents have been included in SWEOLD at more than one
wave of data collection. Therefore, Huber-White sandwich estimator of
variance was used to account for intra-individual correlations for ana-
lyses of change over time. For all analyses, the criterion for test sig-
nicance was p < 0.05. Data were analysed using STATA 12.
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of the 77+ sample
Analyses of trends in loneliness among older people over the last
two decades are based on samples of people aged 77 years or older in
1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014. The characteristics of these samples
were presented in Table 2.The majority of the respondents were
women and the average age was approximately 83 years. There was a
decrease in people with lower education over the study period. Re-
garding social factors, there was an increase in the proportion of mar-
ried respondents and a decrease in the widowed respondents. Engage-
ment in social contacts and access to social support increased over the
years (although the item on social contacts was not included in 1992).
Seen over the entire study period, there was no signicant change in
ADL problems, but there was a decrease in ADL problems from 1992 to
2004 (p= 0.019), and an increase in the years after that (p= 0.045).
Finally, there was a decrease in psychological distress among the re-
spondents.
3.2. Trends in loneliness from 1992 to 2014 (77+)
Fig. 1 presents trends in loneliness, and shows that the proportion of
older people reporting feelings of loneliness was fairly stable over the
study period. Over the years, between 3.86 (year 1992) and 1.86 (year
2014) percent of the respondents reported being bothered by feelings of
loneliness almost all the time, while between 8.6 (year 1992) and 11.1
(year 2004) percent of the respondents reported being bothered by
feelings loneliness often. Changes in prevalence level of loneliness over
the years were not signicant.
A higher proportion of women than men reported loneliness, sig-
nicant for 2002, 2004 and 2011 (χ
2
= 21.93, p < 0.001;
χ
2
= 18.76, p < 0.001; χ
2
= 18.88, p < 0.001). These years, a lower
proportion of women than men responded almost neverbeing lonely,
while higher proportions of women responded that they were seldom
and oftenlonely.
3.3. Characteristics of the 70+ sample
Characteristics of the 2004 and 2014 samples of people aged 70
years or older are presented in Table 3. Both years, a majority of the
sample was women, and the average age was approximately 78 years. A
higher proportion of the respondents in 2004 than in 2014 had a lower
education. There was an increase in the proportion of respondents that
was married and a decrease in the proportion of respondents that was
widowed between these years, while the proportion that was unmarried
or divorced was fairly stable.
Regarding social variables, in 2004 the respondents on average had
fewer social contacts than the respondents ten years later. The majority
of the respondents had access to social support both years, and there
was an increase in access to social support over the study period. The
vast majority of the respondents had no ADL limitations and there was
no signicant change over the study period. Similarly, the majority of
the respondents did not report psychological distress and such problems
became less common from 2004 to 2014.
3.4. Loneliness and its bivariate correlates in 2004 and 2014 (70+)
As presented in Table 4, 9.88 percent of the respondents reported
feelings of loneliness in 2004 compared to 7.68 percent in 2014 (not
signicant change). Table 4 also presents descriptive results on lone-
liness in relation to dierent characteristics of the respondents, and
bivariate associations between loneliness and these characteristics in
2004 and 2014, respectively. Starting with socio-demographic factors,
more women than men were bothered by feelings of loneliness, al-
though the association between loneliness and gender was only sig-
nicant in 2014, when 10.0 percent of women reported loneliness
compared to 4.9 percent of men. The likelihood of loneliness was as-
sociated with higher age both years. In 2004, loneliness was more often
reported by people with lower than higher education level. There was
no such association in 2014.
The analyses show that social factors are important for loneliness.
Regarding marital status, a lower proportion of married individuals
reported loneliness. In 2004, a higher proportion of non-married/di-
vorced and widowed respondents reported loneliness, with particularly
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
98
high proportions of loneliness among those widowed. Both years,
people who lacked access to social support and people with fewer social
contacts were more likely to report loneliness. Regarding health factors,
in both years ADL limitation and psychological distress increased the
likelihood of loneliness.
The interaction between each correlate and survey year was tested
(not shown in table). A signicant interaction term was found regarding
ADL limitations (p= 0.043), where there was a signicantly stronger
association between ADL limitations and loneliness in 2004 than in
2014. For all other variables, the interaction with survey year was
Table 2
Characteristics of samples aged 77 years or older in 1992, 2002, 2004, 2011 and 2014 (%), n = 2572.
1992 2002 2004 2011 2014 pfor trend
(n = 466) (n = 518) (n = 505) (n = 536) (n = 547)
Loneliness 12.4 13.3 14.9 13.4 11.3 0.709
Female gender 59.4 57.7 59.9 60.1 57.8 0.854
Lower education level
1
78.7 69.3 65.3 57.1 51.6 0.000
Marital status
Married 39.3 39.2 40.3 46.1 48.8 0.001
Unmarried/divorced 12.0 12.7 11.3 14.9 13.2 0.311
Widowed 48.7 48.1 48.4 39.0 38.0 0.000
Lack of access to social support
2
4.55 9.02 10.3 8.49 8.18 0.038
Psychological distress
3
No problem 76.5 74.3 73.8 71.4 81.6 0.404
Mild problem 18.4 19.7 21.1 21.6 14.9 0.495
Severe problem 5.18 5.98 5.17 7.01 3.49 0.662
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) pfor trend
range range range range range
Age 82.7 (3.95) 82.8 (4.54) 82.6 (4.43) 83.1 (4.91) 82.7 (4.61) 0.574
7798 7798 7797 77101 7795
Fewer social contacts
4
4.15 (1.96) 4.19 (1.97) 3.79 (1.96) 3.91 (1.93) 0.002
08080808
ADL limitations
5
0.40 (0.96) 0.37 (0.90) 0.22 (0.65) 0.27 (0.77) 0.33 (0.97) 0.085
0505040505
1
Item non-response varied between 1 and 12.
2
Item non-response varied between 1 and 65. The item was not included in the postal questionnaire.
3
Item non-response varied between 3 and 8.
4
Item non-response varied between 1 and 2. The item was not included in 1992.
5
Item non-response varied between 1 and 5.
Fig. 1. Trends in loneliness among people aged 77
years or older in the period of 19922014 (%) (1992:
n= 466; 2002: n= 518; 2004: n= 505; 2011:
n= 536; 2014: n= 547).
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
99
Table 3
Characteristics of samples aged 70 years or older in 2004 and 2014 (%), n = 1962.
2004 2014 pfor dierence
(n= 921) (n= 1041)
Loneliness 9.88 7.68 0.078
Female gender 59.1 54.9 0.026
Lower education level 57.2 45.0 0.000
Marital status
Married 52.0 60.8 0.000
Unmarried/divorced 13.2 14.1 0.525
Widowed 34.7 25.1 0.000
Access to social support 90.6 94.1 0.002
Psychological distress
No problem 74.9 83.2 0.000
Mild problem 20.6 13.4 0.000
Severe problem 4.45 3.36 0.199
M (SD) M (SD) pfor dierence
range range
Age 78.2 (5.99) 77.7 (6.13) 0.065
7097 7095
Fewer social contacts 3.81 (2.05) 3.58 (1.80) 0.005
0808
ADL limitations 0.14 (0.53) 0.20 (0.76) 0.063
0405
Table 4
Bivariate associations between loneliness and independent variables among respondents aged 70 years or older in 2004 and 2014, n= 1962.
2004 2014
(n=921) (n = 1041)
Rarely lonely (%) Frequentlylonely (%) AME
2
Rarely lonely (%) Frequently lonely (%) AME
2
Gender
Female 88.6 11.4 0.039 90.0 10.0 0.054
**
Male 92.3 7.7 95.1 4.9
Education level
Lower 88.0 12.0 0.051
*
91.4 8.6 0.012
Higher 92.9 7.1 93 7.0
Marital status
Married 98.1 1.9 1 97.2 2.8 1
Non-married/divorced 86.9 13.1 0.112
***
91.2 8.8 0.060
*
Widowed 79.4 20.6 0.187
***
81.2 18.8 0.159
***
Social support
Access to support 91.6 8.4 93.6 6.4
Lack of support 75.9 24.1 0.108
***
72.1 27.9 0.118
***
Psychological distress
No problem 93.9 6.1 1 94.7 5.3 1
Mild problem 85.3 14.7 0.086 84.3
**
15.7 0.104
**
Severe problem 48.8 51.2 0.451 65.7
***
34.3 0.290
***
M M AME
2
M M AME
2
(SD) (SD) (SD) (SD)
range range range range
Age 77.8 (5.82) 81.8 (6.27) 0.009
***
77.4 (6.04) 81.0 (6.34) 0.006
***
7097 7094 7095 7095
Fewer social contacts 3.64 (2.00) 5.36 (1.85) 0.038
***
3.49 (1.74) 4.70 (2.11) 0.026
***
08080808
ADL limitations 0.10 (0.44) 0.48 (0.96) 0.065
***
0.17 (0.70) 0.58 (1.21) 0.028
***
04040505
Note 1:
*
p< 0.05,
**
p< 0.01,
***
p< 0.001.
Note 2: Bivariate associations presented as average marginal eects (AME).
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
100
statistically non-signicant, which suggests that the bivariate associa-
tions between these correlates and loneliness were stable over the two
time points.
3.5. Multivariable analyses for 2004 and 2014 (70+)
In the multivariable models for 2004 and 2014, presented in
Table 5, none of the socio-demographic variables remained sig-
nicantly associated with loneliness in older people. All social factors
included in the study were still associated with loneliness. Being wi-
dowed was particularly important for loneliness. This group had 15.2
percent higher likelihood of loneliness in 2004 and 12.3 percent higher
likelihood in 2014 than respondents who were married. Also non-
married/divorced respondents were more likely to report loneliness
than married respondents. Furthermore, respondents who lacked access
to social support and had fewer social contacts were more likely to
report loneliness than respondents with access to social support and
more social contacts.
Finally, health problems were signicantly associated with lone-
liness in the full model. In 2004, but not in 2014, respondents with ADL
limitations had a signicantly higher likelihood of loneliness than
people without ADL limitations. Psychological distress was found to be
the strongest correlate for loneliness. In 2004, respondents reporting
severe psychological distress were 36.6 percent more likely to report
loneliness than respondents without psychological distress. In 2014, the
corresponding gure was 20.7 percent.
The interaction between each correlate and survey year was tested
in the full models (not shown in table). Interactions between correlates
and survey years were found regarding education (p= 0.047). Post hoc
analyses regarding education show that this may be explained by
changes in social factors. For example, increases in social contacts over
the years have been greater among people with lower than higher
education.
4. Discussion
Based on repeated cross-sectional nationally representative surveys
of older people in Sweden, the aim of this study was to examine changes
in loneliness among older people. Firstly, we examined prevalence le-
vels of loneliness in people aged 77 years or older between 1992 and
2014. Overall, it can be concluded that there has been no increase in
loneliness among older people in Sweden in the last two decades.
Recent cohorts of older people do not report loneliness to a greater
extent than previous cohorts of older people, despite societal changes
towards more individualistic societies (World Values Survey, 2016).
Such societal changes have perhaps been counteracted by changes in
other factors associated with loneliness, such as increased proportions
of married older people and people with higher education. This nding
challenges the common myth that loneliness is becoming more pre-
valent among older people (see Dykstra, 2009). Similar trends of stable,
or even slightly decreasing levels, of loneliness have been found in
studies conducted in Finland, the Netherlands and Great Britain
(Eloranta et al., 2015; Honigh-de Vlaming et al., 2014; Victor et al.,
2002). In addition, the study found that more women than men tended
to report loneliness. This nding echoes previous research (Aartsen &
Jylhä, 2011; Cohen-Manseld et al., 2009; Dykstra et al., 2005).
Secondly, we examined changes in correlates of loneliness among
people aged 70 years or older between 2004 and 2014. Generally, the
same correlates were important for loneliness both years, but there
were some exceptions, which will be further discussed below.
This study identied associations between loneliness and female
gender, higher age and lower education. The association between
gender and loneliness was signicant in 2002, 2004 and 2011 for those
aged 77 years or older, and in 2014 for those aged 70 years or older.
Regarding education it was observed that while lower education was
associated with an increased likelihood of loneliness in 2004, there was
no signicant association between education and loneliness in 2014.
There was a signicant interaction between education and survey year
in reporting loneliness, which suggests that the association between
education and loneliness is changing. An explanation for this may be
that the increase in social contacts has been greater among respondents
with lower education than among respondents with higher education.
The importance of other factors than socio-demography was also
conrmed in that none of these socio-demographic correlates were
signicantly associated with loneliness in the multivariable models for
2004 and 2014. In other words, although loneliness is more common
among women, people with higher age and lower level of education, it
is not gender, age and education level as such that are important for
loneliness but other factors that correlate with this (cf. Dahlberg et al.,
2015). Turning to such factors, social factors (widowhood, lack of ac-
cess to social support, fewer social contacts) as well as health-related
factors (ADL limitations, psychological distress) proved inuential on
loneliness. Most social and health-related correlates remained sig-
nicant in the multivariable models. There is strong evidence that so-
cial factors are important for loneliness (for a review, see Routasalo &
Pitkala, 2003). The loss of a partner is a particularly strong trigger for
loneliness (e.g. Aartsen & Jylhä, 2011; Dykstra et al., 2005; Jylhä &
Saarenheimo, 2010), and the importance of widowhood for loneliness
was conrmed in our study.
Our results also conrm earlier research showing that physical and
mental health is related to loneliness (Routasalo & Pitkala, 2003), as
poor health may limit the possibility to lead a socially active life. Dif-
ferent forms of mental health problems, such as depression, are closely
linked to loneliness (Heikkinen & Kauppinen, 2011; O'Luanaigh &
Lawlor, 2008). In this study, mental health was measured via psycho-
logical distress, and this was the most important factor for loneliness in
the analysis. Previous research has shown that lonely individuals are
more likely than non-lonely individuals to perceive the world as
threatening and may develop maladaptive behaviours that enhance the
feeling of loneliness (cf. Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009; Schoenmakers
et al., 2015). In other words, the feeling of loneliness and psychological
distress may reinforce one another (cf. O'Luanaigh & Lawlor, 2008).
The association between ADL limitations and loneliness changed
signicantly over time, with a weaker association in 2014 than in 2004.
The extent to which physical functioning presents a barrier to social
engagement is in part dependent on physical resources in the neigh-
bourhood such as shops, service points, and barriers in the physical
environment including hills and poorly maintained road surfaces
(Honigh-de Vlaming et al., 2014;Phillips, Siu, Yeh, & Cheng, 2005;
Smith, 2009). With more inclusive design and adaptations of existing
Table 5
Multivariable analysis presented as average marginal eects for the likelihood of being
frequently bothered by feelings of loneliness among respondents aged 70 years or older in
2004 and 2014, n= 1962.
2004 (n= 921) 2014 (n= 1041)
Female gender 0.031 0.012
Age 0.002 0.000
Lower education level 0.022 0.022
Marital status
Married 1 1
Non-married/divorced 0.090
***
0.036
Widowed 0.152
***
0.123
***
Lack of social support 0.044
*
0.059
*
Fewer social contacts 0.021
***
0.014
**
ADL limitations 0.023
*
0.012
Psychological distress
No problem 1 1
Mild problem 0.058
**
0.058
*
Severe problem 0.366
***
0.200
**
Note 1: Reference category = rarely lonely.
Note 2:
*
p< 0.05,
**
p< 0.01,
***
p< 0.001.
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
101
environments, people with functional limitations are oered more op-
portunities to stay socially active. The need for improvements of the
physical environment has received attention internationally (e.g. United
Nations, 2006; World Health Organization, 2007), and in Sweden a
legislation has been passed stating that public areas should be acces-
sible for all citizen (Prop. 1999/2000: 79). In so far as the legislation
change has led to improvements in the physical environment, this may
be part of the explanation of a weakening association between ADL
limitation and loneliness in older people. A study conducted in the
Netherlands has found, though, that loneliness had become more
common amongst people with functional limitations over time (Honigh-
de Vlaming et al., 2014). These contradictory ndings suggest that
further research is needed on the development of loneliness in older
people with functional limitations.
4.1. Strengths and limitations of the study
This study is based on SWEOLD, which covers a relatively long
study period and includes a broad range of variables, enabling an ex-
amination of trends in loneliness and its correlates over time. SWEOLD
includes nationally representative samples of older people with high
response rates in each wave of data collection. This means that the
respondents were highly representative of the population, including
frail and institution-based older people (see Lennartsson et al., 2014),
although people who were too frail to be interviewed in person were
not included in the analyses presented in this article. Given the asso-
ciation between loneliness and health problems, this may mean that
this study underestimates the prevalence of loneliness among older
people and may also aect the association between loneliness and
health problems.
The repeated cross-sectional design of the present study means that
no conclusions can be drawn regarding causal associations between
loneliness and its correlates. Previous longitudinal research based on
this data has shown, though, that social factors, such as marital status,
social support and social contacts, and health-related factors are im-
portant predictors of loneliness (Dahlberg et al., 2015;Dahlberg,
Andersson, & Lennartsson, in press).
A potential limitation of the study is that the modes of data col-
lection varied across the data collection points. In 1992, 2002 and 2011
data was predominately collected via face-to-face interviews, and in
2004 and 2014 it was collected via telephone interviews. One could
perhaps assume that the mode of data collection could inuence self-
disclosure and aect reported loneliness. However, there were no sig-
nicant dierences in reported loneliness across the main data collec-
tion modes, and post hoc analysis showed that trends in prevalence le-
vels of loneliness were not signicant even when controlling for data
collection mode. Similarly, Victor, Scambler, Marston, Bond, and
Bowling (2006) argue that reports of loneliness do not vary sub-
stantially with the data collection mode within the context of a struc-
tured questionnaire. However, previous studies comparing the eects
on reported loneliness across dierent data collection modes have
found mixed results (de Leeuw & Hox, 1993; van Tilburg & de Leeuw,
1991). In the present study, analyses of correlates of loneliness were
made for two waves of data collection using the same main data col-
lection mode, which rules out the possibility of any mode eect in these
analyses.
5. Conclusions and practice implications
Contrary to what is often assumed, there has been no increase in
loneliness among older people over the last two decades. Many older
people who reported loneliness also experienced psychological distress.
Loneliness and psychological distress may reinforce one another, and
are also associated with other health problems. Work to prevent and
reduce loneliness should therefore be of high priority. The importance
of social factors for loneliness, shown in this study and previous
research, calls for eorts to support social integration and a sense of
belonging. This could be done in the form of interventions and activities
provided by formal care providers or voluntary organisations.
Unfortunately, the evidence base for eective loneliness interventions is
weak (Cohen-Manseld & Perach, 2015) and there is no specic fra-
mework guiding practitioners in how to prevent loneliness (De Jong
Gierveld & Fokkema, 2015). Still, it has been recommended that in-
terventions should be targeted at specic groups (Cattan, White, Bond,
& Learmouth, 2005). Our study and previous research, has found that
widows and widowers are particularly vulnerable to loneliness.
Work to prevent and reduce loneliness among older people who are
in receipt of care could also be undertaken as an integrated part of
already existing services, such as home help or residential care. The
Swedish Social Service Act states that social services should support
older people in leading an active life together with other people.
However, with enhanced economic pressure, local authorities in
Sweden have introduced stricter guidelines and raised the thresholds
for services, thus concentrated their resources on older people with
larger rather than smaller care needs (Szebehely & Trydegård, 2012).
An ambition to prevent or reduce loneliness within existing services
would require that social and psychological needs are fully acknowl-
edged and that the currently dominating priority of physical and
medical needs over social needs in care services (National Board of
Health & Welfare, 2010) is reconsidered.
Funding
This work was supported by Swedish Research Council for Health,
Working Life and Welfare (FORTE), grant numbers 2015-00440 and
2012-1704.
Conict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conict of interest.
References
Aartsen, M., & Jylhä, M. (2011). Onset of loneliness in older adults: Results of a 28 year
prospective study. European Journal of Ageing, 8(1), 3138. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1007/s10433-011-0175-7.
Ayalon, L., Shiovitz-Ezra, S., & Palgi, Y. (2013). Associations of loneliness in older mar-
ried men and women. Aging & Mental Health, 17(1), 3339. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1080/13607863.2012.702725.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005.
Cattan, M., White, M., Bond, J., & Learmouth, A. (2005). Preventing social isolation and
loneliness among older people: A systematic review of health promotion interven-
tions. Ageing and Society, 25(1), 4167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
Soi44686X04002594.
Cohen-Manseld, J., & Parpura-Gill, A. (2007). Loneliness in older persons: a theoretical
model and empirical ndings. International Psychogeriatrics, 19(2), 279294. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1041610206004200.
Cohen-Manseld, J., & Perach, R. (2015). Interventions for alleviating loneliness among
older persons: A critical review. American Journal of Health Promotion, 29(3),
e109e125. http://dx.doi.org/10.4278/ajhp.130418-LIT-182.
Cohen-Manseld, J., Shmotkin, D., & Goldberg, S. (2009). Loneliness in old age:
Longitudinal changes and their determinants in an Israeli sample. International
Psychogeriatrics, 21(6), 11601170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
s1041610209990974.
Dahlberg, L., & McKee, K. J. (2014). Correlates of social and emotional loneliness in older
people: Evidence from an English community study. Aging & Mental Health, 18(4),
504514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2013.856863.
Dahlberg, L., Andersson, L., McKee, K. J., & Lennartsson, C. (2015). Predictors of lone-
liness among older women and men in Sweden: A national longitudinal study. Ageing
and Mental Health, 19(7), 409417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2014.
944091.
Dahlberg, L., Andersson, L., & Lennartsson, C. (in press). Long-term predictors of lone-
liness in old age: Results of a 20-year national study. Aging & Mental Health.https://
dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1247425. [Published online ahead of print 1
November 2016].
De Jong Gierveld, J., & Fokkema, T. (2015). Strategies to prevent loneliness. In A.
Sha'ked, & A. Rokach (Eds.). Addressing loneliness. Coping, prevention and clinical in-
terventions (pp. 218230). New York: Routledge.
de Leeuw, E. D., & Hox, J. J. (1993). Mode eects in structural modeling; A LISREL multi-
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
102
group comparison of mail, telephone and face to face survey data. In J. Reinecke, & G.
Krekeler (Eds.). Metodische Grundlagen und Anwendungen von
Strukturgleichungsmodellen. Band I. Mannheim: FRG e. V.
Dykstra, P. A., & Fokkema, T. (2007). Social and emotional loneliness among divorced
and married men and women: Comparing the decit and cognitive perspectives. Basic
and Applied Social Psychology, 29(1), 112.
Dykstra, P. A., & de Jong Gierveld, J. (1999). Loneliness dierentials among older adults:
The importance of type of partner, partner history, health, socioeconomic position,
and social relationships. Tijdschrift voor Gerontologie en Geriatrie, 30(5), 212225.
Dykstra, P. A., van Tilburg, T. G., & de Jong Gierveld, J. (2005). Changes in older adult
loneliness: Results from a seven-year longitudinal study. Research on Aging, 27(6),
725747. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0164027505279712.
Dykstra, P. A. (2009). Older adult loneliness: Myths and realities. European Journal of
Ageing, 6(2), 91100.
Eloranta, S., Arve, S., Isoaho, H., Lehtonen, A., & Viitanen, M. (2015). Loneliness of older
people aged 70: A comparison of two Finnish cohorts born 20 years apart. Archives of
Gerontology and Geriatrics, 61(2), 254260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.
2015.06.004.
Ferreira-Alves, J., Magalhaes, P., Viola, L., & Simoes, R. (2014). Loneliness in middle and
old age: Demographics, perceived health, and social satisfaction as predictors.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 59(3), 613623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
archger.2014.06.010.
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical
review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2),
218227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8.
Heikkinen, R. L., & Kauppinen, M. (2011). Mental well-being: A 16-year follow-up among
older residents in Jyvaskyla. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 52(1), 3339.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2010.01.017.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness
and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives
on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1745691614568352.
Honigh-de Vlaming, R., Haveman-Nies, A., Groeniger, I. B.-O., de Groot, L., & van t Veer,
P. (2014). Determinants of trends in loneliness among dutch older people over the
period 20052010. Journal of Aging and Health, 26(3), 422440. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0898264313518066.
Jylhä, M., & Saarenheimo, M. (2010). Loneliness and aging: Comparative perspectives. In
C. R. Phillipson, & D. Dannefer (Eds.). The SAGE handbook of social gerontology (pp.
317328). .
Jylhä, M. (2004). Old age and loneliness: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses in the
Tampere longitudinal study on aging. Canadian Journal on Aging-Revue Canadienne Du
Vieillissement, 23(2), 157168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/cja.2004.0023.
Lennartsson, C., Agahi, N., Hols-Salén, L., Kelfve, S., Kåreholt, I., Lundberg, O., ...
Thorslund, M. (2014). Data resource prole: The swedish panel study of living con-
ditions of the oldest old (SWEOLD). International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(3),
731738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyu057.
National Board of Health and Welfare (2010). Social tillsyn. Länsstyrelsernas iakttagelser
2008 och 2009 [Social inspection. Observations by county administrative boards in 2008
and 2009]. Stockholm: National Board for Health and Welfare.
O'Luanaigh, C., & Lawlor, B. A. (2008). Loneliness and the health of older people.
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(12), 12131221. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1002/gps.2054.
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology of loneliness. In S. Duck,
& R. Gilmour (Eds.). Personal relationships in disorder (pp. 3156). London: Academic
Press.
Phillips, D. R., Siu, O.-L., Yeh, A. G.-O., & Cheng, K. H. C. (2005). Ageing and the urban
environment. In G. J. Andrews, & D. R. Phillips (Eds.). Ageing and place: Perspectives,
policy and practice (pp. 147163). Oxon & New York: Routledge.
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2001). Inuences on loneliness in older adults: A meta-
analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23(4), 245266. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1207/153248301753225702.
Prop. 1999/2000: 79. Från patient till medborgare [From patient to citizen]. Stockholm :
Social Ministry.
Routasalo, P., & Pitkala, K. H. (2003). Loneliness among older people. Reviews in Clinical
Gerontology, 13, 303311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095925980400111X.
Savikko, N., Routasalo, P., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., & Pitkala, K. H. (2005).
Predictors and subjective causes of loneliness in an aged population. Archives of
Gerontology and Geriatrics, 41(3), 223233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.
2005.03.002.
Schoenmakers, E. C., van Tilburg, T. G., & Fokkema, T. (2015). Problem-focused and
emotion-focused coping options and loneliness: How are they related? European
Journal of Ageing, 12(2), 153161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-015-0336-1.
Smith, A. E. (2009). Ageing in urban neighbourhoods. Place attachment and social exclusion.
Bristol: The Policy Press.
Szebehely, M., & Trydegård, G. B. (2012). Home care for older people in Sweden: A
universal model in transition. Health & Social Care in the Community, 20(3), 300309.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j. 13652524.2011.01046.x.
Tijhuis, M. A. R., de Jong Gierveld, J., Feskens, E. J. M., & Kromhout, D. (1999). Changes
in and factors related to loneliness in older men. The Zutphen Elderly Study. Age and
Ageing, 28(5), 491495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/28.5.491.
Tornstam, L. (2007). Stereotypes of old people persist: A swedish facts on aging quiz in a
23-year comparative perspective. International Journal of Ageing and Later Life, 2(1),
3359.
United Nations (2006). Convention on the rights of persons with disabilities. Retrieved from
United Nations website.
van Tilburg, T. G., & de Leeuw, E. D. (1991). Stability of scale quality under various data
collection procedures: A mode comparison on the de Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale'.
International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 3(1), 6985. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1093/ijpor/3.1.69.
Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Shah, S., Cook, D. G., Harris, T., Rink, E., & de Wilde, S.
(2002). Has loneliness amongst older people increased? An investigation into var-
iations between cohorts. Ageing & Society, 22, 585597.
Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Marston, L., Bond, J., & Bowling, A. (2006). Older peoples
experiences of loneliness in the UK: Does gender matter? Social Policy and Society,
5(1), 2738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474746405002733.
Victor, C. R., Scambler, S., & Bond, J. (2009). The social world of older people.
Understanding loneliness and social isolation in later life. Maidenhead: Open University
Press.
World Health Organization (2007). Global age-friendly cities: A guide. Geneva: World
Health Organization.
World Values Survey (2016). Findings and insights. Retrieved 28 November 2016 from
http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSContents.jsp.
L. Dahlberg et al. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 75 (2018) 96–103
103
... They found no difference in loneliness mean scores over time. This result was confirmed by Dahlberg et al. (2018) studying trends in loneliness between 1992 and 2014 in Swedish older adults. ...
... Further, loneliness was positively associated with being male, age, not being married, income problems and chronic diseases. Dahlberg et al. (2018) confirmed the association between loneliness and not being married, psychological distress, lack of social support and fewer social contacts. ...
... Aunsmo et al., 2023). Overall, it was hypothesized that loneliness will be positively associated with symptoms of psychological distress and poor lifestyle behaviours, and negatively with measures of well-being (Dahlberg, 2022;Dahlberg et al., 2018;Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015;Park et al., 2020;Tesch-Römer et al., 2013), despite the analysis of some factors (e.g. victimization) was exploratory in nature. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study analysed changes in loneliness between 2006 and 2015 and associated factors using publicly available data (N = 128,718) from the European Social Survey from 17 countries. The study protocol was pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/eq63j/). Loneliness-weighted prevalence (and mean) decreased from 30% to 27% over time. The decreasing trend was significant for both sexes. Young and old age groups reported a decline in loneliness over time while other age groups did not. Loneliness did not demonstrate a significant decline – but rather a stable trend – in persons with disability and first- and second-generation immigrants. Sociodemographic characteristics, social factors, well-being and psychological distress were associated with loneliness. These findings update those from previous studies indicating that loneliness trends and differences between European regions might be better explained by differences in psychological distress.
... A systematic review identified four repeated cross-sectional studies reporting multiple comparable prevalence estimates of loneliness (8), of which one study concerned older adults, showing no significant change in the trend between 1992 and 2014 in Sweden (18). Similarly, no significant change in loneliness among older adults was found over a five-year period in the Netherlands (19), a 10-year period in Sweden (20) and a 10-year period in the United states (21), while decreases in loneliness were found in 20-year follow-up studies in Finland (22), Germany (23) and the United States (24). ...
... Findings suggest that the majority of people aged 77 years or older were not lonely, with an average of 12.5 percent who were categorized as often or almost always lonely. In line with our previous analyses (18), the present study shows that the prevalence of loneliness has been fairly stable; with the addition of 2021 to the analysis, a slight decrease in the mean score of loneliness was identified. A decrease in loneliness is in line with previous studies with follow-up periods of 20 years or more and starting in the 1990s (22)(23)(24), while previous research covering trends during 5-10 years in the 2000s have not identified any significant change in loneliness (19)(20)(21). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Loneliness and social isolation are public health concerns. This study aimed to examine levels and trends in loneliness and social isolation among older adults (77+ years) in Sweden, assess subgroup variations, and determine associations between loneliness and social isolation. Methods The 1992, 2002, 2004, 2011, 2014 and 2021 waves of the Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (SWEOLD) were analysed through ordered logistic and linear regressions. Results On average, 12.5 percent of the participants experienced loneliness often/nearly always, while 6 percent were categorised as severely isolated. Loneliness and social isolation were more common in women, those aged 85+, and persons with basic education, in psychological distress or with mobility limitations. Loneliness was also associated with living alone. No increases in loneliness or isolation were identified; instead, loneliness tended to decrease in groups already experiencing lower levels of loneliness. Loneliness and social isolation were moderately associated each survey year. Discussion This study challenges perceptions of high and increasing levels of loneliness and social isolation. Given the impact on health and wellbeing and the ageing of populations, policy and practice should still address these issues and target vulnerable groups. Subgroup analyses in trends are scarce and should be explored further in future research.
... It is an emotional and mental isolation rather than physical distance. On the other hand, loneliness is also considered as a sad feeling that accompanies the perception that one's social needs are not met in the desired amount and especially in the desired quality (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010) and is defined as the inconsistency between the levels of social relationships desired and achieved by the individual (Dahlberg, Agahi, & Lennartsson, 2018). The perception of loneliness is defined by Yılmaz and Altınok (2009) as the situation experienced because of isolating oneself from one's environment, limiting interpersonal relationships and weakening social interaction. ...
... 22 Among older adults, a decline was reported in various studies, [23][24][25][26] but not found in Dahlberg, Agahi. 27 Third, resilience to the social impact of the pandemic may differ by age. Attention was called to the vulnerability of older adults soon after the pandemic outbreak. ...
... Lastly, loneliness was assessed predominantly by asking how often it is experienced (frequency; N = 21, 55%), but also by investigating its general occurrence in someone's life (prevalence; N = 3, 8%;Barnow et al., 1997;de Koning et al., 2017;Leitch et al., 2018), or by asking if someone is bothered by it (intensity; N = 1, 3%;Perrig-Chiello et al., 2016). A mix of the former was also common (N = 13, 34%;Dahlberg et al., 2018;Dahlberg et al., 2015;Dugan & Kivett, 1994;Kumpusalo et al., 1991;Savikko et al., 2005 + all studies using DJG-LS mentioned above). ...
Article
The vital role social relationships play in mental health and well-being has been well-documented. Disruption of an intimate bond through bereavement can be enduringly stressful, with loneliness featuring prominently, possibly compromising mental and physical health. We systematically reviewed studies examining loneliness across marital status groups, focusing on the widowed. The aim was to establish what is known about the prevalence of loneliness in widowhood, to compare loneliness between widowed and people with other marital statuses (divorced, married, never-married persons), and to investigate correlates of loneliness. Furthermore, our objective was to compare the empirical understanding of loneliness within the context of widowhood to how loneliness is addressed in contemporary grief literature. Searches were conducted via multiple, relevant databases. Studies investigating loneliness across marital statuses were considered if they included an adult, bereaved sample, were written in English, and used quantitative research methods. Thirty-eight studies met inclusion criteria. Widowhood was associated with a greater likelihood of loneliness. On average, the widowed were found to be lonelier than divorced, married, and never-married people. Additionally, correlates of loneliness were identified, including mental health adversities, lack of social support, recent loss, and gender. Comparing empirical information from the two domains of marital status and grief research contributes to a more comprehensive knowledge about loneliness and bereavement. However, evidence remains limited due to several methodological shortcomings, such as inconsistent use of comparison groups, insufficient control for bereaved individuals in non-widowed marital statuses, lack of prospective, longitudinal studies, unreported means, and the use of different measures of loneliness.
... Significant negative correlations were also found between loneliness and cognitive deterioration, suggesting that loneliness can worsen it, as shown in other studies [10,23,[49][50][51]. The scientific literature supports these findings, indicating that loneliness is related to higher psychological and physical morbidity, including depression, cognitive deterioration, dementia, and other chronic diseases [42,[52][53][54][55][56], and the onset of psychotic symptoms without prior neurological conditions [26]. Lastly, sleep quality showed a significant correlation with loneliness, which was not maintained in the multivariate analysis, and it is therefore believed that this variable could be linked with others, such as depression or cognitive deterioration. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background/Objectives: Population aging poses many challenges to public health, highlighting loneliness and social isolation as severe problems that affect the physical and mental health of older adults. During the COVID-19 pandemic, these became aggravated. The objective of the present study was to assess the prevalence of loneliness and its relationship with social isolation, depression, cognitive deterioration, sleep quality, and the level of physical mobility and functioning of older adults in Health Area 3 of the Region of Murcia. Methods: A descriptive, observational, and cross-sectional study was performed. The inclusion criteria were age ≥ 65, living in Health Area 3 of the Region of Murcia, and not being institutionalized. The following variables were evaluated: sociodemographic variables, loneliness (UCLA scale), social isolation (DUFSS), depression (GDS), cognitive deterioration (Pfeiffer), sleep quality (PSQI), and mobility (Barthel index). A univariate and multivariate regression model was created to examine how the dependent variable was related to the independent variables. Results: A total of 102 older adults participated in the study. Of these, 31.4% perceived unwanted loneliness and 14.7% low social support. The multivariate regression analysis showed that social isolation, geriatric depression, and cognitive deterioration were significant predictors of loneliness. Conclusions: The findings highlight the importance of developing multifaceted interventions that address not only social isolation but also other interrelated factors such as depression, cognitive deterioration, and sleep quality. The strategies should be centered on community programs and support networks. It is fundamental to perform longitudinal studies to better understand the causal relationships between these variables.
... Furthermore, the analysis revealed a lack of substantial correlation (p = 0.148) between age and feelings of loneliness (r = 0.169). According to Dahlberg et al. (2018), there is a correlation between advancing age and an elevated likelihood of experiencing loneliness among those who have not previously had such feelings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Loneliness and social isolation, which are significant determinants of both mortality and illness, demand immediate attention. They are critical public health and policy subjects, particularly for older people in long-term care institutions. This review delves into the profound impact of these feelings, such as loneliness, depressive symptoms, and social isolation, on their overall well-being. The objective of this study is to present a comprehensive examination of the characteristics associated with these feelings among older people residing in long-term facilities, drawing attention to this frequently disregarded subject matter. Following PRISMA guidelines, a comprehensive scoping review of articles published between 2010 and 2023 was conducted. Six databases (CINAHL, PubMed, Web of Science, Scopus, Google Scholar, and Oxford Academic) were searched for relevant articles about loneliness in nursing homes and long-term facilities. The final review, a comprehensive examination of 71 qualitative and quantitative studies, has resulted in seven main themes: sociodemographic factors, physical and mental health well-being, social network, the COVID-19 pandemic, meaningful activities, and loss of autonomy. These findings provide a robust foundation for understanding and addressing loneliness and social isolation among older people in long-term facilities. This scoping review has highlighted six themes, providing evidence and incentive for you, as healthcare professionals, researchers, and policymakers, to take a leading role in promoting person-centred care and educating your peers about the consequences of loneliness and social isolation. It is crucial to involve family, friends, and other multidisciplinary team members in care planning and decision-making, emphasizing the importance of collaborative efforts. Most importantly, it would be best to empower older adults to maintain their autonomy, engage in enjoyable activities, and socialise with other service users.
... Almost similar findings revealed by the study done in Indonesia [60], in which loneliness was 64% among older people. However, studies reported lower prevalence of loneliness in Sweden [61], Norway [62] and China [63]. The difference in findings might be due to breakdown of conventional family structures, insufficient social welfare, and a lack of mental health care which may contribute to loneliness among seniors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Older people are vulnerable to various psycho-social problems such as depression, anxiety, insomnia, dementia, and loneliness that have profound impact on overall health and their quality of life and higher use of health services. Prevention and treatment of psychological problems in this risk group is critical for improving their quality of life. Objective This study aimed to find out the psycho-social problems among older people residing in community. Methods The study was a cross-sectional study design and 388 older people aged 65 years and above residing in different wards of Bharatpur Metropolitan city were selected using probability simple random sampling technique. Data were collected using interview schedule containing geriatric depression scale-15; Geriatric anxiety scale-10, University of California Loss Angels Loneliness Scale (UCLA-20), Anthens Insomnia Scale and Mini-Cog Test. Obtained data were analyzed in SPSS version 20 for windows. Chi-square test was applied to measure the association between psychosocial problems and selected variables. Result Findings of the study revealed that the mean age (± SD) of respondents was 72.92 (±7.12) years. Almost all (93.6%) older people had full functioning of activity of daily living and two third (66.5%) had other co-morbid conditions. More than two third (67.0%) of older adults had depression, 60.3% had anxiety, 53.6% had moderate to high level loneliness, 47.2% had insomnia, and 33.3% had dementia. Age, functional dependency, sex, co-morbidity, financial dependence, education and occupation were significantly associated with the psychosocial problems among older people. Conclusion and recommendation Psychosocial problems are common among older people residing in community of Chitwan. Hence, there is need to develop and implement health care strategy by local health care planner to prevent, treat and manage the psychosocial problems among this risk groups. Further, health care providers working in geriatric problems or psychosocial health need to conduct regular screening programs for the early diagnosis and treatment of these problems.
Book
La obra aborda la psicocardiología y la comunicación de malas noticias en el ámbito de la salud. Se ofrece una visión profunda y humana de cómo estas dos áreas se entrelazan, proporcionando tanto perspectivas teóricas como experiencias prácticas. Este trabajo pretende no solo informar, sino también sensibilizar a los profesionales de la salud sobre la importancia de la comunicación empática y efectiva con los pacientes. Tal como sugiere el título, se explora el Apoyo Social pero no desde la perspectiva tradicional, sino desde la óptica de que el exceso de muestras de afecto puede resultar en un efecto negativo para la salud mental. El manuscrito está dirigido a un público compuesto por profesionales de la salud, estudiantes y cualquier persona interesada en la psicocardiología. Los autores consideramos que este libro puede enriquecer y aportar un contenido novedoso y relevante en el campo de las ciencias de la salud.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: The understanding of social phenomena is enhanced if individuals can be studied over longer periods. Regarding loneliness in old age, there is a general lack of longitudinal research. The aim of this study was to examine whether there is an association between loneliness in old age and social engagement 20 years earlier, as stated by life course theory and the convoy model. Method: Data from the nationally representative Swedish Panel Study of Living Conditions of the Oldest Old (2002 and 2011 data collection waves) and the Swedish Level of Living Survey (1981 and 1991 data collection waves) were used. The sample included 823 individuals with an average age of 62.2 years at baseline and 82.4 years at follow-up. Results: Each form of social engagement in old age was significantly associated with the same form of social engagement 20 years earlier. Close forms of social engagement were associated with loneliness in old age; as were more distant forms of social engagement, but only when they were considered solely in old age. Conclusion: Patterns of social engagement in old age were established at least 20 years earlier and close forms of social engagement are long-term predictors of loneliness, although current social engagement tended to be more influential on loneliness. The study underlines the importance of interventions targeted at close relationships that can provide social support in old age.
Chapter
Full-text available
Loneliness and ageing: Comparative perspectives Social relationships, and their structures and meanings, comprise some of the major issues in social gerontology. This chapter discusses one dimension in this field, that of loneliness. The concept of loneliness is closely linked with themes such as family life, social integration, quality of life, and life satisfaction. This chapter examines the experience of loneliness, with particular attention given to issues of theory, measurement, and comparisons across different societies. The chapter reviews a number of important questions: To what extent does loneliness belong to old age? To what extent is loneliness similar for young and older people? To what degree do the conditions and forms of loneliness vary according to different cultural environments? The chapter assesses a number of approaches to measuring loneliness, the main risk factors involved, and models of intervention to reduce the experience of loneliness in community settings. Classic social research on ...
Article
Full-text available
Actual and perceived social isolation are both associated with increased risk for early mortality. In this meta-analytic review, our objective is to establish the overall and relative magnitude of social isolation and loneliness and to examine possible moderators. We conducted a literature search of studies (January 1980 to February 2014) using MEDLINE, CINAHL, PsycINFO, Social Work Abstracts, and Google Scholar. The included studies provided quantitative data on mortality as affected by loneliness, social isolation, or living alone. Across studies in which several possible confounds were statistically controlled for, the weighted average effect sizes were as follows: social isolation odds ratio (OR) = 1.29, loneliness OR = 1.26, and living alone OR = 1.32, corresponding to an average of 29%, 26%, and 32% increased likelihood of mortality, respectively. We found no differences between measures of objective and subjective social isolation. Results remain consistent across gender, length of follow-up, and world region, but initial health status has an influence on the findings. Results also differ across participant age, with social deficits being more predictive of death in samples with an average age younger than 65 years. Overall, the influence of both objective and subjective social isolation on risk for mortality is comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality. © The Author(s) 2015.
Article
Full-text available
We examine the extent to which coping options endorsed by older adults help alleviate loneliness, and experiences with loneliness influence the coping options. Two ways of coping are distinguished: problem-focused, i.e. improving one’s relationships, and emotion-focused, i.e. lowering one’s expectations about relationships. Loneliness is assessed using three observations over six years among 1033 61-to-99-year-old respondents in the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Combining the first two observations yielded four loneliness types: not lonely at T0 and T1, recently lonely, persistently lonely and recovered from loneliness. Between the second and third observations, respondents were asked to evaluate which coping options lonely peers described in various vignettes had. From this, individual coping scores were calculated. The option to improve relationships did not affect the likelihood of one’s own loneliness, and the option to lower expectations even increased it. Compared to non-lonely respondents, recently lonely ones endorsed both ways of coping equally frequently, persistently lonely ones endorsed improving relationships less frequently and lowering expectations more frequently, and recovered respondents endorsed improving relationships equally frequently and lowering expectations more frequently. We conclude that considering various ways of coping does not help alleviate loneliness and that persistently lonely and recovered respondents are at risk of a circular process with loneliness experiences resulting in considering lowering expectations more frequently, which results in a greater likelihood of loneliness, thus contributing to sustaining or re-establishing loneliness.
Book
Many western nations have experienced a rise in the number of marginalised and deprived inner-city neighbourhoods. Despite a plethora of research focused on these areas, there remain few studies that have sought to capture the ‘optimality’ of ageing in place in such places. In particular, little is known about why some older people desire to age in place despite multiple risks in their neighbourhood and why others reject ageing in place. Given the growth in both the ageing of the population and policy interest in the cohesion and sustainability of neighbourhoods there is an urgent need to better understand the experience of ageing in marginalised locations. This book aims to address the shortfall in knowledge regarding older people’s attachment to deprived neighbourhoods and in so doing progress what critics have referred to as the languishing state of environmental gerontology. The author examines new cross-national research with older people in deprived urban neighbourhoods and suggests a rethinking and refocusing of the older person’s relationship with place. Impact on policy and future research are also discussed. This book will be relevant to academics, students, architects, city planners and policy makers with an interest in environmental gerontology, social exclusion, urban sustainability and design of the built environment.
Article
This study aimed to examine loneliness among two birth cohorts, born 20 years apart, when they were 70 years of age, and to identify factors explaining loneliness. The cohorts consisted of older home-dwelling residents of Turku, Finland, from the birth cohort 1920 in 1991 (N=1530) and the birth cohort 1940 in 2011 (N=1307). Suffering from loneliness was assessed with the question: 'Do you suffer from loneliness?' Cross-tabulations with chi-square test, general linear model (GLM) and multiple regression analysis were used in statistical testing and modeling. In the 1940 cohort, around one-fifth (18%) of the respondents suffered from loneliness at least sometimes, while the corresponding figure in the 1920 cohort was around one-fourth (26%). Our analyses indicated that the effect of cohort was not a statistically significant explanatory factor of loneliness. Living status, self-rated health and memory compared to age peers were statistically significant explanatory factors for suffering from loneliness. When we controlled the effect of depressiveness on the experience of loneliness, it was shown that the effects of living status and self-rated health remained statistically significant, whereas memory compared to age peers did not. Depressiveness itself was highly important. The combined effect of living status and self-rated health emerged as the most significant explanatory factor for loneliness. Older people with poor self-rated health who lived alone were most likely to suffer from loneliness. The findings give healthcare professionals an opportunity to plan for interventions aimed at combating loneliness among home-dwelling older people. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
Aim: to investigate (i) whether loneliness increases in old age, and if so, whether it relates to ageing itself, to time trends or to cohort effects and (ii) the relationship between changes in institutionalization, partner status and health and loneliness. Methods: 939 men born between 1900 and 1920 completed the De Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale, and answered questions about their partner status, health and institutionalization in 1985, 1990 and 1995. Results: for the oldest group (born between 1900 and 1910) loneliness scores increased, but not for the younger groups. The increase in loneliness was attributable to ageing. No birth cohort or time effects were found. Loneliness was related to changes in institutionalization, partner status and subjective health but not to limitations in activities of daily living or cognitive function. Conclusions: the increased loneliness experienced by very old men is influenced by loss of a partner, moving into a care home or not feeling healthy.
Article
Objectives: Longitudinal research on loneliness in old age has rarely considered loneliness separately for men and women, despite gender differences in life experiences. The objective of this study was to examine the extent to which older women and men (70+) report feelings of loneliness with a focus on: (a) changes in reported loneliness as people age, and (b) which factors predict loneliness. Method: Data from the 2004 and 2011 waves of SWEOLD, a longitudinal national survey, was used (n = 587). The prediction of loneliness in 2011 by variables measured in 2004 and 2004-2011 variable change scores was examined in three logistic regression models: total sample, women and men. Variables in the models included: gender, age, education, mobility problems, depression, widowhood and social contacts. Results: Older people moved into and out of frequent loneliness over time, although there was a general increase in loneliness with age. Loneliness at baseline, depression increment and recent widowhood were significant predictors of loneliness in all three multivariable models. Widowhood, depression, mobility problems and mobility reduction predicted loneliness uniquely in the model for women; while low level of social contacts and social contact reduction predicted loneliness uniquely in the model for men. Conclusion: This study challenges the notion that feelings of loneliness in old age are stable. It also identifies important gender differences in prevalence and predictors of loneliness. Knowledge about such differences is crucial for the development of effective policy and interventions to combat loneliness in later life.