ArticlePDF Available

Natural Translating Locomotion Modulates Cortical Activity at Action Observation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study verified if the translational component of locomotion modulated cortical activity recorded at action observation. Previous studies focusing on visual processing of biological motion mainly presented point light walker that were fixed on a spot, thus removing the net translation toward a goal that yet remains a critical feature of locomotor behavior. We hypothesized that if biological motion recognition relies on the transformation of seeing in doing and its expected sensory consequences, a significant effect of translation compared to centered displays on sensorimotor cortical activity is expected. To this aim, we explored whether EEG activity in the theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta 1 (14–20 Hz) and beta 2 (20–32 Hz) frequency bands exhibited selectivity as participants viewed four types of stimuli: a centered walker, a centered scrambled, a translating walker and a translating scrambled. We found higher theta synchronizations for observed stimulus with familiar shape. Higher power decreases in the beta 1 and beta 2 bands, indicating a stronger motor resonance was elicited by translating compared to centered stimuli. Finally, beta bands modulation in Superior Parietal areas showed that the translational component of locomotion induced greater motor resonance than human shape. Using a Multinomial Logistic Regression classifier we found that Dorsal-Parietal and Inferior-Frontal regions of interest (ROIs), constituting the core of action-observation system, were the only areas capable to discriminate all the four conditions, as reflected by beta activities. Our findings suggest that the embodiment elicited by an observed scenario is strongly mediated by horizontal body displacement.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 07 November 2017
doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00083
Natural Translating Locomotion
Modulates Cortical Activity at Action
Observation
Thierry Pozzo 1,2*, Alberto Inuggi 3,Alejo Keuroghlanian 3,Stefano Panzeri 4,
Ghislain Saunier5and Claudio Campus6
1Centro di Neurofisiologia Traslazionale, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Ferrara, Italy, 2INSERM-U1093, CAPS, Campus
Universitaire, Dijon, France, 3Unit of Robotics, Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy,
4Laboratory of Neural Computation, Center for Neuroscience and Cognitive Systems, University of Trento, Istituto Italiano di
Tecnologia, Rovereto, Italy, 5Laboratorio de Cognição Motora, Departamento de Anatomia, Universidade Federal do Pará,
Belém, Brasil, 6U-VIP Unit for Visually Impaired People, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, Genova, Italy
Edited by:
Mikhail Lebedev,
Duke University, United States
Reviewed by:
Yury Ivanenko,
Fondazione Santa Lucia (IRCCS),
Italy
Guy Cheron,
Free University of Brussels, Belgium
Stephan de la Rosa,
Max Planck Society (MPG), Germany
*Correspondence:
Thierry Pozzo
thierry.pozzo@u-bourgogne.fr
Received: 11 July 2017
Accepted: 18 October 2017
Published: 07 November 2017
Citation:
Pozzo T, Inuggi A, Keuroghlanian A,
Panzeri S, Saunier G and Campus C
(2017) Natural Translating
Locomotion Modulates Cortical
Activity at Action Observation.
Front. Syst. Neurosci. 11:83.
doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00083
The present study verified if the translational component of locomotion modulated
cortical activity recorded at action observation. Previous studies focusing on visual
processing of biological motion mainly presented point light walker that were fixed
on a spot, thus removing the net translation toward a goal that yet remains a critical
feature of locomotor behavior. We hypothesized that if biological motion recognition
relies on the transformation of seeing in doing and its expected sensory consequences,
a significant effect of translation compared to centered displays on sensorimotor cortical
activity is expected. To this aim, we explored whether EEG activity in the theta (4–8 Hz),
alpha (8–12 Hz), beta 1 (14–20 Hz) and beta 2 (20–32 Hz) frequency bands exhibited
selectivity as participants viewed four types of stimuli: a centered walker, a centered
scrambled, a translating walker and a translating scrambled. We found higher theta
synchronizations for observed stimulus with familiar shape. Higher power decreases in
the beta 1 and beta 2 bands, indicating a stronger motor resonance was elicited by
translating compared to centered stimuli. Finally, beta bands modulation in Superior
Parietal areas showed that the translational component of locomotion induced greater
motor resonance than human shape. Using a Multinomial Logistic Regression classifier
we found that Dorsal-Parietal and Inferior-Frontal regions of interest (ROIs), constituting
the core of action-observation system, were the only areas capable to discriminate
all the four conditions, as reflected by beta activities. Our findings suggest that the
embodiment elicited by an observed scenario is strongly mediated by horizontal body
displacement.
Keywords: locomotion, action perception, motor resonance, EEG, translation, body shape
INTRODUCTION
Human locomotion is possible thanks to central pattern generators allowing a reciprocal activation
of flexors and extensors muscle (Grillner and Wallen, 1985). However, cyclical locomotor
skill became a decisive step in species evolution when displacement started to be oriented and
goal directed. Beside limbs oscillation, backward or forward displacements to avoid a predator or
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
to reach a prey also include a variety of related cognitive
processes. Instance of this is spatial navigation toward a goal and
the ability to integrate body translation over time that originates
from visual flow and vestibular input. Thus locomotion, as a
teleokinetic behavior (Hess, 1943) is much more than central
pattern generator activation producing limb oscillation as when
walking on the spot. Real locomotion can thus be described as a
goal-oriented action displacing the whole body from one initial
position toward a distant spatial goal. In fact, at the perceptual
level, a walker on the spot corresponds to an erratic walker
without goal, as someone can do for fun in the reverse direction
of an airport treadmill, that is a rather atypical visual stimulus.
Despite artificiality of walking without net body translation,
forward locomotion seems not a crucial variable in visual
processing of biological motion. For example, when a set
of point lights (PL) located on the joints of an invisible
walker on a treadmill is displayed on a screen the observer
reliably distinguishes a human in locomotion in contrast with
PL configurations that do not respect normal body metric
(Johansson, 1973; Blake and Shiffrar, 2007 for a review). This
contrasts with the impossibility to ‘‘read’’ the actions of other
species in one’s vicinity when displacement is absent.
Besides this a priori considerable visual relevance of body
movements for successful interactions with conspecifics and
to interpret action of others living beings, a prominent idea
regarding motion recognition is related to observer’s motor
competencies (Viviani and Stucchi, 1992; Calvo-Merino et al.,
2006; Cannon et al., 2014; Quandt and Marshall, 2014;
Meirovitch et al., 2015). According to this, the visual input
of an observed action would be mapped on to the observers’
own motor repertoire (Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al.,
2001; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). More specifically, when
one observes a living being in motion cortical sensorimotor
activity has been proposed to reflect the transformation of
perceptual representations to executable actions (Hari et al.,
1998; Pavlova and Sokolov, 2003; Pavlova et al., 2003; Oberman
et al., 2005; Pineda, 2005; Hirai et al., 2009; Perry and
Bentin, 2009). Particularly, it was observed that the visual
perception of human action led to an alteration of EEG/MEG
activity characterized by a desynchronization of alpha and beta
rhythms, which reflects the increase of neural activity within
sensorimotor cortices (Pineda, 2005; Pavlidou et al., 2014;
Cevallos et al., 2015). Therefore, if horizontal body displacement
is a key component of locomotion and if visual perception of
kinematic features is tuned by motor representations (Viviani
and Stucchi, 1992; Pozzo et al., 2006; Saunier et al., 2008),
one may assume different motor cortical activities when
displaying real locomotion compared to a walker with no net
translation.
Traditional protocols, although interesting for examining
biological motion recognition processes, presented a strong
limitation to address this question. Indeed, most of studies either
manipulated the exposure duration of the animation (Thornton,
1998; Poom and Olsson, 2002), embedded the PL in an array of
dynamic noise dots (Cutting et al., 1988; Bertenthal and Pinto,
1994; Ikeda et al., 2005; Hiris, 2007) inverted PL (Sumi, 1984)
or compared a translating PL with the translation of an object
at constant velocity (Peuskens et al., 2005). Although systematic
investigation of the role of such ecologically valid component
of action is lacking, several experiments however reported that
extrinsic motion makes PL displays more natural and easily
recognizable (Johansson, 1973; Proffitt et al., 1984; Pavlova and
Sokolov, 2003; Thurman and Lu, 2013).
The current study tests the hypothesis that motor
experience related to natural translating walking modulated
the sensorimotor alpha and beta rhythms. If motion
recognition relies on the transformation of seeing in doing
and its expected sensory consequences, a significant effect of
translation compared to centered displays on sensorimotor
cortical activity is expected. We thus collected EEG from
participants during the observation of different locomotor
patterns, manipulating the gestalt (a scrambled displays that
consists of the same amount of absolute motion but lacks
a body structure vs. a coherent global body configuration)
and the motor/kinematic (walking on a treadmill with no
net translation vs. natural translating locomotion) of the
display. More specifically, we aimed at verifying that translated
scrambled display (that modifies only the body structure but
keeps the biological kinematic) would produce sensorimotor
spectral perturbations in EEG signal and that the translation
cue on its own can link the visual input with the action
system.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Thirteen right-handed volunteers (7 females, 6 males, mean
age: 27, standard deviation: 3.5), with normal or corrected
to normal vision, took part in this study. All participants
provided written informed consent before the experiment began.
The experimental protocol was approved by the local ethical
committee ASL-3 (‘‘Azienda Sanitaria Locale’’, local health unit),
Genoa, and was in agreement with the Helsinki Declaration of
1975, as revised in 1983.
Experimental Protocol
Participants were presented with point-light animations (PLAs)
built from motion capture data. We used a VICON Motion
Capture System with 10 cameras to record the movements, at
100 Hz sampling frequency, of an actor walking naturally (length
of recording: 4.5 s). The actor had 13 passive infrared reflective
markers placed at the main joints and other landmarks following
the VICON Plugin Gait Full Body Template (Eltoukhy et al.,
2012): LBHD (left back of the head, roughly in a horizontal
plane of the front head markers), LSHO (acromio-clavicular
joint), LELB (left outer elbow, lateral epicondyle approximating
elbow joint axis), RELB (right outer elbow), LWRA (left wrist
bar thumb side), RWRB (right wrist bar pinkie side), LPSI
(over the left posterior superior iliac spine), LKNE (lateral
epicondyle of the left knee), RKNE (lateral epicondyle of the
right knee), LANK (left outer ankle, lateral malleolus along
an imaginary line that passes through the transmalleolar axis),
RANK (right outer ankle), LTOE (left toe, over the second
metatarsal head, on the mid-foot side of the equinus break
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
between fore-foot and mid-foot), RTOE (right toe). These data
were processed through Matlab scripts (Mathworks Inc., Natick,
MA, USA) to build the stimuli displayed using functions from
the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997) on an LCD monitor,
with a refresh rate of 60 Hz. Point-lights were white against a
black background.
Four types of stimuli were created: a centered walker, a
centered scrambled walker, a translating walker and a translating
scrambled walker (see Figure 1), which will be now on referred to
as cw, cs, tw and ts, respectively. During the centered conditions
the stimulus stayed with its barycenter in the middle of the screen
(visual angle approximately between 0 and 9 degrees); during
the translating conditions the stimulus moved its barycenter
from the middle always to the right of the screen (visual angle
approximately between 0 and 18 degrees). Each PLA was 1 s long
because of the visual processing duration of such stimuli that
occurs within a temporal window of 1000 ms after the display
onset (see Hirai et al., 2003; Jokisch et al., 2005; Krakowski et al.,
2011; Saunier et al., 2013). In order to avoid a possible bias
in the results due to the initial starting positions of the point-
lights, each PLA had 10 different starting positions obtained
by shifting the animation by steps of six frames (see Hirai
et al., 2003). The cw animation was built by translating all
the dots of the tw animation by the opposite of the vector
defining their center of mass with respect to the center of
the screen, at each frame: the cw animation looked like a
person walking on a treadmill. The cs animation was built
by changing randomly the initial positions of the dots in the
cw animation but keeping their velocity vectors unchanged;
the dots’ trajectories were constrained to remain inside the
vertically-oriented rectangle in which the cw animation was
inscribed. This constrain did not affect the velocity vectors of
dots in cs and ts conditions. The ts animation was built by
changing randomly the initial positions of the tw animation
in an analogous way. Therefore, each stimulus was obtained
by combining two factors with two levels each: shape (either
walker or scrambled) and translation (that could be present or
absent).
During the experiment, participants were sitting comfortably
in a darkened room, in front of the screen where PLAs were
displayed, approximately 60 cm apart. The experiment was
organized in 10 blocks. Each block consisted of 48 PLAs
(12 of each of the four types) presented in pseudo-random
order. In order to avoid possible expectation effects due to
extremely regular timing in the presentation of the stimuli,
the inter-stimulus interval (ISI) varied randomly in length
between 2 and 4 s (uniform distribution). In each block, a
random number of animations (between 2–4) changed color
from white to green during 250 ms. This change of color
occurred in a randomly determined period of the animations.
Hereafter, these changing-color stimuli will be referred to as odd
stimuli. Once an odd stimulus was presented, after a random
number of stimuli a question was presented on the screen
asking the participant which was the last animation among
the four types that changed color. Since the question arose
randomly, it was possible that between two odd stimuli no
question was presented. Therefore, the number of questions
was variable across blocks, and was less than or equal to the
number of odd stimuli presented in the respective block (one in
case of two odd stimuli or four in case of four stimuli, in
one block). EEG traces corresponding to odd stimuli were
discarded from analysis. Participants gave their answers through
a keyboard, by pressing a key number between 1, 2, 3 and
4, corresponding to cw, tw, cs and ts, respectively. The four
types of stimuli were clearly identifiable, and each participant
learned the correspondence between key numbers and stimuli
during a training session before starting the recording session.
The training consisted first in displaying the stimuli and the
associated number: participants were allowed to replay it as
many times as they needed to learn the correspondence between
numbers and animations. Then, subjects were tested in a short
experimental block (12 animations, with three odd stimuli,
and two questions) to verify they learnt the correspondence
and understood the task. Participants were allowed to replay
this second step, or to go back to the first part in case they
did not learn the correspondence between PLAs and numbers.
Participants did not receive feedback on their performance
during the actual experiment, while they did during the training
session instead.
Electrophysiological Data Recording
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 62 Ag/AgCl
active electrodes (actiCAP, Brain Products, Munchen, Germany)
placed on the scalp, mounted on a cap according to the
international 10-20 system. The EEG was amplified with two
BrainAmp MR plus amplifiers (Brain Products), digitized at
1000 Hz. The recordings were referenced to electrode FCz.
Impedances of all electrodes were kept below 15 kOhms.
Data Pre-Processing
Raw EEG signals were band-pass filtered between 0.16 and
45 Hz through a Butterworth filter as implemented in
Brain Vision Analyzer software (Brainproducts). Data were
down sampled to 250 Hz and then imported into EEGLAB
software (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) for further analyses. A
visually inspected artifact removal was performed based on
the topographical and spectral distribution and on the time
series of the independent component calculated with the ICA
algorithm implemented by EEGLab. After artifact cleaning
the signal, the percentage of removed events was 7 ±1%
(mean ±SD) for the cs, 6 ±4% for cw, 6 ±3% for cs and
7±2% for tw, therefore, for each condition, the number of
removed events was on average lower than 5% and with a
small variability among all subjects. Data were re-referenced
to the common average reference (CAR) and epochs from
400 (as the best compromise to get at the same time a
long enough time window and a clean baseline) to 1000 ms
with respect to stimulus presentation (time = 0) were then
extracted.
Evaluating ERSP
For each epoch, Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) was applied to
partially overlapping time segments: each segment was 256 ms
long (64 time points) and each shifting step was 8 ms. A 16 points
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 1 | Experimental protocol and stimuli displays. Upper part shows the four categories of stimuli. Body joint trajectories (in white) are depicted on black screen
for illustrative purposes but were not visible during the actual experiment. From left to right (upper horizontal green arrow), stimuli shape change from a scrambled
structure to a real walker body structure. From top to bottom (left lateral yellow arrow), stimuli keep the same shape but change from a centered to a translated
display. Abbreviations: cs and cw for centered scrambled and walker respectively; ts and tw for translating scrambled and walker. The trajectories of the markers
during the whole point light animation (PLA) are shown. The lower part sums up the experimental protocol. During each of the 10 experimental blocks of 48 trials (in
parenthesis), every condition was randomly presented 12 times (upper part, in parenthesis) for 1 s. During each trial, subjects were told to anchor their gaze on the
red cross displayed in the middle of the screen.
zero-padding and a Hanning-window tapering were employed,
respectively to increase smoothness in ERSP estimation and
to limit edge effects. To get a clean estimation of baseline
activity, the period between 200 ms and 10 ms was adopted.
Event related spectral perturbations (ERSPs) were calculated
as event-related power variations (in dB) compared to the
specified baseline (Makeig, 1993). ERSPs were then mediated
across epochs for each condition, considering times from
200 ms before the stimulus to 850 ms after the stimulus (8 ms
resolution) and frequencies from 4 Hz to 32 Hz (approximately
0.5 Hz resolution). Then, based on previous literature, the
EEG spectral perturbations were separately evaluated in the
theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta 1 (14–20 Hz) and
beta 2 (20–32 Hz) frequency bands. For each subject and
condition we used EEGLab to verify that inter trial coherency
was significant, therefore spectral modulations were consistent
among trials.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using the R software
(R Core Team, 2017). We considered the average ERSP for three
regions of interest (ROIs), namely Ventral (PO7, PO8, P7, P8),
Dorsal-Parietal (P1, P2, Pz) and Inferior-Frontal (F7, F8, FT7,
FT8, F5, F6). These regions have been selected because they are
involved in biological motion perception: Ventral ROI mainly
for shape encoding (Grossman and Blake, 2002; Hirai et al.,
2003; Krakowski et al., 2011; Saunier et al., 2013), Parietal for
decoding and integration of shape and kinematic features (Saygin
et al., 2012), while Inferior Frontal reflecting motor resonance
mechanism (Cochin et al., 1999; Saygin, 2007).
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
Spectral modulations of cortical activity are possible neural
correlates related to motion perception. Particularly, the
theta band is selective to shape discrimination whilst the
alpha and beta bands are involved in matching the visual
input to motor repertoire (i.e., here referred to ‘‘motor
resonance’’). For instance, the shape of the observed
stimulus affects theta frequency band (Urgen et al., 2013).
Otherwise, alpha band is affected by both movement perception
(Capotosto et al., 2009; Zumer et al., 2014) and execution
(Cochin et al., 1999). Finally, the motor characteristics
of the observed stimulus specifically affect beta bands
(Meirovitch et al., 2015). Indeed, beta 1 is involved in the
long-range synchronization of distant areas composing a
visuo-motor network, in addition to the matching of perceived
stimuli onto the motor repertoire (Engel and Fries, 2010;
Kopell et al., 2011). Further, Beta 2 is involved in the
process of action selection and monitoring (Botvinick et al.,
1999).
Comparing ERSP between Conditions
We tested if visual stimuli elicited different neural activities
measured through EEG spectral perturbations. For each subject,
ROI and frequency band, we extracted the extreme value,
i.e., the maximum or the minimum ERSP, for each ROI and
band considered: increased mental activity is generally reflected
by a power increase (synchronization) in the theta band and
by a power decrease (desynchronization) in the alpha and
beta bands. Therefore for theta we selected the maximum,
while for other bands the minimum (for further details
about the procedure of extreme selection, see Supplementary
Statistical Analysis). To establish an order in intensity and
timing of different spectral perturbations, we applied repeated
measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA), investigating the
effects of ROI (Ventral, Dorsal-Parietal and Inferior-Frontal)
shape (walker, scrambled), translation (centered, translating),
as well as their interaction on the extreme of ERSP and on
their latencies. Post hoc tests were conducted using paired
two-tailed t-tests and P<0.05 after Bonferroni correction (to
reduce the risk of false positives, we considered all pairwise
comparisons between three ROIs and four conditions and
thus resulting in a correction factor of 18) were retained
as significant. Considering the number of discarded events
among subjects in each condition, standard deviations resulted
quite low (ranging from 1% to 4% depending on condition),
indicating that discarded events were quite uniformly distributed
among all subjects. Therefore, in the performed RM-ANOVA
post hoc we used one averaged value for each subject,
frequency band and condition, without any specific weighting
procedure.
ANOVAs were used to establish if a difference existed among
the magnitudes or the latencies of corresponding to the extreme
values. Then, post hoc comparisons were used to establish which
latencies (amplitudes) were lower/larger, i.e., to establish an
order (or hierarchy) between different values. For example, if the
extreme value was larger in one scalp area than in another, we
inferred that intensity of the activation (estimated as the extreme
value) was larger in the first area; if the latency of the extreme
value in one area was lower than in another area, we inferred that
the timing of the first area was earlier.
Predicting Visual Stimuli Using ERSP in Frequency
Bands
To draw more reliable functional conclusions, we applied
a multinomial logistic regression model (Press and Wilson,
1978) which allows to overcome EEG a-specificity by filtering
significant results provided by classical ANOVA followed
by post hoc comparisons through a greater constraint. We
thus searched not only for significant differences between
conditions, but for any feature that allows predicting, based on
EEG spectral features, the structure (walker or scramble) and
kinematic (with or without extrinsic movement) characteristics
of the visual input. Moreover, this allows establishing the
different weight of body shape (walker or scrambled) and
kinematic (centered or in translation) in eliciting motor
resonance.
Traditional statistical analyses rely only on raw comparisons
between distributions across all trials and subjects of different
groups or conditions, merely based on means and standard
deviations: describing properties of a sample using these two
parameters can however lead to miss information related to
subject specificity. To overcome this limitation, we used a
Multinomial Logistic Regression model that better takes into
account inter subject variability (going beyond group level
comparisons and allowing to predict for each single subject
the probability of observing a certain kind of stimulus given
recorded EEG activity in different frequency bands) and does
not assume a normally distributed sample. Moreover, we aimed
to use the model as a predictor of the visual stimuli from
the single subject measured ERSPs. In this way, we went
beyond statistical comparisons between ERSPs and built a
classification model that can decode information extracted
from EEG giving the probability of an observed stimulus
as a continuous function of the measured ERSP. We used
multinomial logistic regressions (MLRs), mainly for two reasons:
first, they are more generalizable, robust and provide better or
comparable performance than linear classification models (Press
and Wilson, 1978). Second, they allow not only distinguishing
different conditions as classical clustering based approaches, but
also allow testing the contribution of each band in decoding
visual stimuli. For consistency with ANOVAs and post hoc
comparisons, the model was fitted considering stimuli as
dependent variable and ERSP values of all subjects as predictor,
considering one value for each subject, ROI and frequency
band.
The first step (Figure 2A) consisted in extracting the model
Mo(using a multinomial logistic regression function) from the
original values V0(ERSP data). Then we calculated its predictive
ability and discriminative performance using Somers’ D index
(Do) that ranges from 0 to 1 (Somers, 1962). Predictive ability of
the model refers here to the inverse process that starts from the
ERSP of each subject and comes back to the corresponding visual
stimulus. Small D values indicate random predictions unable to
discriminate visual conditions; high D values indicate perfect
predictions and condition identification.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 2 | Schema of the applied procedure for fitting and validating the models. See text for further details. (A) Calculate the coefficients and initial estimation of
Somers’ D for original model fitted to the original ERSP data. (B) For 10,000 bootstraps of original data: calculate coefficients of model fitted to bootstrapped data,
apply it to original data and original to boostrapped data, computing the difference between respective Somers’ D, i.e., Harrell’s optimism. (C) Average Harrell’s
optimism over all bootstraps. (D) Obtain a corrected estimation of Somers’ D by subtracting Harrell’s optimism from the initial estimation.
As shown in Figure 2B, the reproducibility of discriminative
performance was verified by a validation based on bootstrap
technique (Harrell, 2015), which is based on re-estimation of
model parameters on data which were repeatedly randomly
sampled with replacement by the original data set and which
was found to provide better results than classical cross-validation
techniques (Steyerberg et al., 2001). Specifically, we assessed
the robustness of the predictive ability of the model with a
different set of data Vbobtained by bootstrapping original data
(Vofrom which we extracted the model Mb(Figure 2B, left
part). Then we calculated the D index obtained by crossing the
models and the data from which they were extracted: on one
side we applied Moto Vbobtaining Dbo(Figure 2B, middle
part); on the other side we applied Mbto Voobtaining Dob. At
last, as a measure of the reproducibility of model’s performance
(Figure 2B, right part), we calculated the difference between
the two D indices Dboand D0b, i.e., the Harrel’s Optimism
OH(Harrell, 2015). We repeated this step 10,000 times, thus
getting a distribution of OH, i.e., of the differences between Dbo
and Dob.
We then (Figure 2C) calculated the average of this
distribution thus obtaining the Optimism OHwhich is an index
describing the bias present in the initial estimation of the model’s
performance: the closer is the average Optimism to 0, the more
reproducible is the performance of the model.
In the last step (Figure 2D) we subtracted OHfrom the
initial estimation of model’s performance Do, thus obtaining a
corrected and unbiased performance estimation Dc(for further
details, see Supplementary Statistical Analysis and Harrell, 2015).
We checked the potential value of ERSP in decoding body
shape (cw and tw vs. cs and ts) or translation (tw and ts vs.
cw and cs). Moreover, we investigated the respective weight
of shape and translation in classifying all visual stimuli at the
same time.
To sum up, using multinomial logistic regression model
allowed us overcoming the limitations of classical group statistics
(Press and Wilson, 1978). On one hand the models selected the
most robust effects among those showed by classical ANOVA
followed by post hoc comparisons, specifically highlighting the
peculiar role of cortical areas in the integration of stimuli’
form and motion. On the other hand, the models made it
possible to infer from ERSP in different EEG frequency bands
the probability of observing a certain kind of stimulus not
only at a group, but also at the single subject level. Therefore
having stronger (de)synchronizations in specific bands, for
each single subject, increased the probability of observing a
specific stimulus. At last, the selected class of models allowed
us to establish an order between the considered experimental
conditions, providing the strength of the ‘‘jump’’ from each
condition to another one.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
RESULTS
As shown in Figure 3 (see also Supplementary Figure S1),
all the visual stimuli produced in all the considered ROIs an
early event related synchronization (ERS) and a later Event
Related Desynchronization (ERD). ERS spanned approximately
from around 100 ms and 400 ms after the stimulus onset
and mostly involved the theta band (4–8 Hz). ERD spanned
approximately from 200 ms to the end of the considered epoch
and mostly involved alpha (8–12 Hz), beta 1 (14–20 Hz) and beta
2 (20–32 Hz) frequency bands.
General Trend of ERSP for Shape and
Translation Effects
Based on ANOVA and post hoc comparisons (see Table 1 and
Supplementary Figures S2–S4), theta and beta 1 were modulated
by shape depending on cortical regions (see green rows in
Table 1, Supplementary Figure S2), while alpha and beta bands by
translation (see red rows in Table 1, Supplementary Figure S3).
For what concerns the shape effect, as shown on top-left of
Figure 4, in Ventral ROI, theta showed higher synchronizations
for walker (cw and tw) than for scrambled (cs and ts) shape
(t(12)= 12.33, P= 0.0000005): this was found also when
separately comparing centered walker with centered scrambled
(left, t(12)= 8.73, P= 0.00002) and translating walker with
translating scrambled (right, and t(12)= 11.91, P= 0.0000006).
A similar pattern was found in Inferior-Frontal (top-right
Figure 4,t(12)= 19.42, P= 0.000000002), also when separately
comparing cw with cs (left, t(12)= 11.28, P= 0.000001) and tw
with ts (right, t(12)= 19.56, P= 0.000000002). Conversely, in
Dorsal-Parietal (bottom-left Figure 4), walker produced deeper
beta 1 desynchronizations than scrambled shape (t(12)= 12.33,
P= 0.0000005), also when separately comparing cw with
cs (left, t(12)=20.02, P= 0.000000001) and tw with ts
(right, t(12)=25.16, P= 0.0000000001). A similar result was
found in Inferior-Frontal (bottom-right Figure 4, for walker
vs. scrambled t(12)= 32.22 P= 0.000000000006; left for cw
vs. cs(12)=21.63, P= 0.0000000007; right for tw vs. ts
t(12)=19.36, P= 0.000000003). For what concerns translation
effect, as shown on bottom-left Figure 4, in Dorsal-Parietal
beta 1 showed deeper desynchronizations for translating than
for centered stimuli (t(12)=34.07, P= 0.0000000000008,
for ts vs. cs t(12)=34.71, P= 0.000000000003 and for tw
vs. cw t(12)=22.49, P= 0.0000000004). A similar result
was found in Inferior-Frontal (for translating vs. centered
t(12)=38.07, P= 0.0000000000008, for ts vs. cs t(12)=38.33,
P= 0.0000000000008 and for tw vs. cw t(12)=16.50,
P= 0.00000002). Translating stimuli produced also deeper
alpha desynchronization in Dorsal-Parietal t(12)=5.65,
P= 0.001 (See Supplementary Figure S3). Interestingly, both
Dorsal-Parietal (see bottom-left Figure 4,t(12)=7.32,
P= 0.0001) and Inferior-Frontal beta 1 (see bottom-right
Figure 4,t(12)=6.89, P= 0.0002) showed a deeper
desynchronization for translating scrambled than for centered
walker.
Comparing the Latencies of Extreme ERSP
Values
For all bands, the latencies of extreme ERSP values followed, as
expected, a caudal-to-rostral order: extreme values occurred in
Ventral, then in Dorsal-Parietal and finally in Inferior-Frontal.
The average peak latencies for different ROIs spanned from
110 ms to 544 ms for theta, from 119 ms to 551 ms for alpha,
from 142 ms to 574 ms for beta 1 and from 154 ms to 586 ms for
beta 2 (see also Supplementary Figures S5, S6).
ERSPs in Specific Frequency Bands Can
Predict the Observed Visual Stimuli
Figures 57show separately probability to perceive a particular
visual stimulus (changing in shape and/or in kinematic) as
a function of ERSP pattern, for theta, alpha, beta 1 and
beta 2 bands. The probability of observing a stimulus with
a scrambled shape increases for low theta synchronization in
FIGURE 3 | Spectrograms obtained from superior parietal ROI (SP) for the four visual stimuli. From left to right, cs and cw for centered scrambled and walker; ts and
tw for translating scrambled and walker). Event related spectral perturbations (ERSP) are expressed at different times (x-axis) and frequencies (y-axis) as in dB,
i.e., Log(Power(t)/Power during baseline). Blue indicates event related desynchronizations (ERD), i.e., power decreases; green indicates null variations, while red
indicates event related synchronizations (ERS), i.e., power increases. SP area was selected for representative purposes: in all the ROIs and conditions we found a
similar pattern with an initial ERSs, mainly involving low frequencies, followed by an extended ERD mainly involving high frequencies (see also Supplementary Results).
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
TABLE 1 | Results of ANOVAs on event related spectral perturbations (ERSP) extreme values.
Effect Band F p Ges
theta 12.30 0.0002 0.13
roi (2,24) alpha 88.70 0.000000000008 0.56
beta1 719.10 0.0000000000000000000004 0.94
beta2 310.32 0.000000000000000007 0.85
theta 133.00 0.00000007 0.23
shape (1,12) alpha 0.16 0.7 0.00025
beta1 498.95 0.00000000004 0.42
beta2 2.53 0.1 0.0022
theta 0.59 0.5 0.0023
translation (1,12) alpha 7.60 0.02 0.023
beta1 431.61 0.00000000009 0.57
beta2 0.27 0.6 0.00068
theta 124.00 0.0000000000002 0.15
roishape (2,24) alpha 0.01 1 0.0000069
beta1 232.24 0.0000000000000002 0.30
beta2 1.46 0.3 0.0015
theta 1.06 0.4 0.0014
roitranslation (2,24) alpha 5.38 0.01 0.0063
beta1 314.99 0.000000000000000006 0.39
beta2 4.70 0.02 0.0087
theta 0.02 0.9 0.000037
shapetranslation (1,12) alpha 0.51 0.5 0.00081
beta1 2.85 0.1 0.0063
beta2 0.80 0.4 0.0020
theta 0.47 0.6 0.00016
roishapetranslation (2,24) alpha 11.60 0.0003 0.0043
beta1 4.45 0.02 0.0054
beta2 3.44 0.04 0.0021
Columns represent respectively: experimental effect, frequency band, F value p values and generalized eta squared index. Degrees of freedom are in parentheses.
Significant effects (P <0.05) are in bold. In green bands are shown which more responding to shape, in red bands are shown which more responding to translation effect,
while in blue bands are shown which responding to their interaction.
Ventral and Inferior-Frontal (Figure 5, first row, red area)
and progressively decreases for greater theta synchronization.
Conversely, the probability of observing a coherent body
structure (walker, in blue) increases with theta synchronization.
Interestingly, theta better discriminates shape in Inferior-Frontal
(χ2
(1)= 45.82, P= 0.00000000001) than in Ventral (χ2
(1)= 22.86,
P= 0.00005) showing a smaller overlap between scrambled
(red area) and walker (blue area). Considering now both
Dorsal-Parietal (χ2
(1)= 16.88, P= 0.00004) and Inferior-Frontal
(χ2
(1)= 22.66, P= 0.000002) ROIs (see third row of Figure 5),
the probability of observing a scrambled shape is highest for low
beta 1 desynchronization and progressively decreases for greater
beta 1 desynchronization. Conversely, the probability to see a
coherent body walker increases with beta 1 desynchronization.
Instead, alpha (second row of Figure 5) and beta 2 (fourth
row of Figure 5) show similar probability for scrambled and
walker shapes for all ERSP values (mostly overlapped red and
blue areas), therefore unable to discriminate scrambled from
walker.
Concerning translation effect (see Figure 6) only beta 1
discriminates translating from centered stimuli (see third row
of Figure 6): deeper beta 1 desynchronizations in Dorsal-
Parietal (χ2
(1)= 42.12, P= 0.00000000009) and Inferior-Frontal
(χ2
(1)= 37.14, P= 0.000000001) decrease the probability of a
centered stimulus and increase the probability of a translating
stimulus. Noticeably, translation was even better discriminated
than shape, as indicated by less overlapped areas in Figure 6
compared to Figure 5.
Figure 7 shows the probability of each observed stimulus
with respect to ERSP pattern and sums up all the previous
observations: beta 1 is the only band discriminating both shape
and translation. Specifically, as shown by third row of Figure 7,
for Dorsal-Parietal (χ2
(1)= 92.79, P<0.0000000000000002)
and Inferior-Frontal (χ2
(1)= 92.19, P<0.0000000000000002)
ROIs, the probability of observing a centered scrambled
is highest for lowest beta 1 desynchronization. Increasing
beta 1 desynchronization increases the probability to
see a centered walker, then a translating scrambled and
finally a translating walker. Interestingly, a greater beta
1 desynchronization is necessary to predict an observed
translating scrambled (blue area), while a centered walker (green
area) can be predicted with a lower beta 1 desynchronization.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 4 | Effect of the four visual stimuli on the four regions of interest (ROIs) considered for both theta and beta bands. Each subplot represents a ROI. On x-axis
are visual conditions (cs, cw, ts, tw, same abbreviation than in Figure 1); on y-axis are ERSP in dB. Bars correspond to means and standard errors. Horizontal lines
correspond to significant differences (p<0.05). Top: in Ventral (top left), theta is affected by the main effect of shape, synchronizing more for walker than for
scrambled shape in Ventral and in Inferior-Frontal. Bottom: both in Dorsal-Parietal (left) and in Inferior-Frontal (right) beta 1 is affected by shape and translation at the
same time, progressively desynchronizing in centered scrambled, centered walker, translating scrambled and translating walker.
For detailed results of the models, see Supplementary
Tables S1–S3.
Possible Effects of Ocular Movements
In this study, we controlled for possible bias due to potential
eye movements induced by the translation animation. Subjects
were told to anchor their gaze on the red cross; however,
since half of the stimuli translated along the screen, eye
movements could occur during the observation of translating
stimuli, creating spurious effects on measured cortical activity.
Therefore, we linearly detrended ERSP data using the mean
magnitude of eye movements evaluated as the time course of
the absolute value of voltages at electrode AF7 referenced to
AF8 (hEye = abs(AF7 AF8)). These electrodes have been
previously considered among the most significant forehead
electrodes to detect eye movements (Belkacem et al., 2014)
and used to detect eye artifacts during a covert horizontal
tracking task (Makin et al., 2012). Their position, near the
left and right eye respectively, make their amplitudes deflect
coherently with horizontal eye movements, due to the pointing
direction of the corneo-retinal dipoles of the eyes, similarly
to the signal provided by an horizontal electro-oculogram
(Croft and Barry, 2000). Importantly, results were statistically
unaffected by this correction. Moreover, especially for beta
bands, we found differences between conditions with the same
level of translation (i.e., cw vs. cs and tw vs. ts). Finally,
when considering the mean amplitude and the 95% confident
interval (CI) of ocular moments in all considered conditions (see
Supplementary Figure S7 in Supplementary Material), the CI
overlapped indicating the lack of significant difference between
conditions.
DISCUSSION
The aim of the present study was to examine ERSPs during
the perception of different kind of locomotor stimuli. We
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 5 | Model prediction on stimulus shape (scamble vs. walker) based on ERSP. Rows and columns of subplots correspond respectively to different frequency
bands and ROIs. In each subplot, on x-axis are ERSP in dB; on y-axis are probabilities of observing a scrambled (s) or a walker (w). Stars correspond to significant
shape discrimination (p<0.05). Theta band in Ventral and Inferior-Frontal, as well as beta 1 in Dorso-Parietal and in Inferior-Frontal predict the shape of the visual
stimuli: increasing theta synchronizations and beta1 desynchronization decrease the probability of observing a scrambled stimulus (blue area) and increase the
probability of observing walker stimulus (red area). To obtain the probability of observing a kind of visual stimulus we applied the Predict function of the rms package
of R. Specifically, given the coefficients estimated for the Multinomial Logistic Regression model and a vector representing putative ERSPs, the function calculated for
each value of ERSP (xcoordinate) the corresponding probability (ycoordinate).
wanted to evaluate how the visual perception of different
locomotor patterns, changing in gestalt and kinematic,
modulates cortical activity, from EEG signal recorded in
several areas.
More precisely, we made the specific hypothesis that
forward translation affects EEG activity in the theta
(4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta 1 (14–20 Hz) and beta 2
(20–32 Hz) frequency bands. In support to the recorded data
a multinomial logistic regressions models (MLRs) showed
that the two variables tested (shape and translation) were
encoded in different ways. Theta ERS increased with the
more familiar shape (i.e., a walker instead of a scrambled)
within Ventral and Inferior-Frontal ROIs, while translation
induced significant attenuation in the power of beta (1 and
2) oscillations in Dorsal-Parietal and Inferior-Frontal
ROIs. Mainly, Dorsal-Parietal and Inferior-Frontal ROIs,
constituting the core of action-observation system, were the
only areas capable to discriminate all the four conditions, as
reflected by beta activities. The different effects of shape and
kinematics variables are successively discussed in the following
sections.
Shape Effect
The result that the Inferior-Frontal theta band was particularly
sensitive to body structure adds to the classical result showing
a main activity located in the temporal area when manipulating
shape factor. Consistent activation recurrently recorded in
the superior temporal gyrus when viewing a coherent gestalt
(CW) compared to scrambled animations (CS) or inverted
walker (Hirai et al., 2003; Jokisch et al., 2005; Peuskens
et al., 2005) supports a privileged role of the STS in body
form encoding. Here, we demonstrate for the first time
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 10 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 6 | Model prediction on stimulus kinematic (centered vs. translated) based on ERSP. Rows and columns of subplots correspond respectively to different
frequency bands and ROIs. In each subplot, on x-axis are ERSP in dB; on y-axis are probabilities of observing a centered (c) or a translated (t) stimulus. Stars
correspond to significant models (p<0.05). Beta 1 in Dorso-Parietal and in Inferior-Frontal predicts the kinematic of the stimuli: increasing beta 1 desynchronization
decrease the probability of observing a centered stimulus (green area) and increase the probability of observing a translating stimulus (violet area).
that theta synchronization in ventral and frontal cortices
is able to discriminate a human figure from a random
structure. Further, the model predicted a better discrimination
of the walker from the scrambled stimuli in frontal area,
an unexpected result regarding previous investigations that
mainly attributed to STS a role in shape recognition (Hirai
et al., 2003; Jokisch et al., 2005; Peuskens et al., 2005).
This outcome supports the idea of a significant role of
motor regions in the specific theta band for body figure
encoding.
Because centered scrambled stimulus does not contain
gestalt-like patterns, one may have predicted the lack of
sensorimotor activity for scrambled display. Instead, the
desynchronization of beta band, even if reduced, was still
present for scrambled stimuli. Nevertheless, a CS stimulus
still displays PLD motion compatible with human kinematic,
especially the dot located on the lower limbs. Precisely, the
centered scrambled preserved local motions compatible with
motor representation, namely the 2/3 power law (Lacquaniti
et al., 1983), also present during treadmill locomotion
(Ivanenko et al., 2002). For instance a cloud of dots moving
along elliptical trajectories strictly according to this motor
rule is enough to activate dorsal premotor and supplementary
motor areas (Dayan et al., 2007; Meirovitch et al., 2015).
Similarly, the present local oscillations produce by central
pattern generator can generate ERD modulation during motion
observation. A recent study performed with patient with lesion
to the form visual pathways also agrees with the idea that form
cues are not critical for biological motion perception (Gilaie-
Dotan et al., 2015) and that observer can still discriminate
locomotion direction or identify living being from spatially
scrambled displays that contain solely local biological motion
cues (Sumi, 1984; Pavlova, 1989; Chang and Troje, 2008).
Nonetheless, beta 1 band suppression in fronto-parietal areas
was stronger when the stimulus displayed a coherent body
structure compared to the scrambled version suggesting that
human body geometry improves the sensorimotor integration of
the visual input.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 11 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
FIGURE 7 | Model prediction on both stimulus shape and kinematic based on ERSP. Same legends than as Figures 5,6. Beta1 in Dorso-Parietal and in
Inferior-Frontal predicts both shape and kinematic of the visual stimulus: increasing beta 1 desynchronization predicts the observed stimulus following this order: cs
(corresponding to lowest desynchronization), cw, ts and then tw (corresponding to highest desynchronization). Abbreviations: cs and cw for centered scrambled and
walker respectively; ts and tw for translating scrambled and walker.
Translation Effect
Surprisingly we did not find any translation effect on alpha
oscillations, a frequency band classically considered as a
correlate of the Action-Perception network activity. Indeed, Mu
suppression refers to an attenuation in the alpha frequency
range (8–13 Hz) recorded over sensorimotor cortex both during
action execution and action observation (Cochin et al., 1999;
Babiloni et al., 2002; Pineda, 2005; Hari, 2006; Orgs et al., 2008;
Perry and Bentin, 2009). A recent investigation by Kraskov
et al. (2014) recorded mirror neurons in area F5 of macaque
monkeys while they observed a reach-to grasp action may
however explain such inconsistency. These authors found that
the local field potential activity in F5 neurons recorded in the
beta-frequency range (15–23 Hz) was attenuated during action
observation. More precisely the power in the 15–23 Hz beta
range recorded in area F5 was significantly attenuated in the first
300 ms after movement onset. Moreover, Urgen et al. (2013)
found alpha band suppression for both human and robot action
observation. All together, these experimental evidences indicate
a lack of sensitivity of alpha band for human action perception,
at least for early sensory stages of action visual processing.
In support to this possibility, a recent investigation performed
during observation of human gait suggested that early alpha ERS
contributes to a general clearance of noise or distracting event
in order to selectively update relevant incoming information
and increase involvement of cognitive resources (Zarka et al.,
2014).
Comparison between centered and translated displays showed
greater beta 1 suppression in the dorso-parietal and inferior-
frontal ROIs for translating compared to centered stimuli. This
confirms that sensorimotor areas are precociously involved in
differentiating between movement kinematics (Press et al., 2011;
Di Dio et al., 2013; Meirovitch et al., 2015). This also suggests
that the translating scrambled (a sort of ‘‘blob in motion’’
as reported a posteriori by the participants) brings significant
sensorimotor information in the motor resonance process,
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 12 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
probably because local intrinsic movements are interpreted as
the cause of extrinsic translating motion (see Thurman and Lu,
2013).
We found that beta band suppression was more pronounced
toward medial and posterior locations (centro-parietal
locations) than in central or fronto-central electrodes.
Previous investigation showed sensorimotor suppression
during movement preparation and execution (Pfurtscheller
et al., 1997), motor imagery (Pfurtscheller et al., 2006) or action
observation (Muthukumaraswamy and Johnson, 2004; Ulloa
and Pineda, 2007) in a somatotopic way. For instance hand
movement is accompanied by a central-lateral alpha rhythm
suppression, whereas feet movements by centro-medial alpha
rhythm suppression (Pfurtscheller et al., 1997). The present
stronger beta rhythms suppression along medial locations,
i.e., those most likely overlaying somatosensory areas of the feet
and legs (Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950) corroborates the link
between perception and action systems.
It has been recurrently proposed that sensorimotor ERD
reflects the transformation of perceptual representations to
executable actions (Pineda, 2005). The finding that beta ERD
responses in expert dancers viewing dance movements are
stronger than in non dancers observing similar movements
(Orgs et al., 2008) also agrees with the idea of a role of the
motor system in visual perception of action. According to this,
the present significant effect of translation on the recorded
sensorimotor ERD indicates that body translation, in contrast to
treadmill walking, facilitates the matching between visual input
and motor representation. Accordingly, when walking on the
spot optical flow is missing. Moreover, locomotor reactions in
response to the passive displacement of the base of support is
not compatible with integration of the relative motion of the
head, the torso and the eyes, that is crucial to build sensorimotor
(Berthoz et al., 1992; Grasso et al., 1998; Pham et al., 2011)
and navigational components of voluntary locomotion (Plank
et al., 2010; Gramann et al., 2011; Chiu et al., 2012). The
artificial sensory context created by the treadmill locomotion
could degrade the matching between visual input incongruent
with stored representation.
Further, even if the observation of a walker without definite
goal (as walking on the spot) might activate the motor cortex
(Saunier et al., 2013), the coupling between perception and action
system is enhanced when a goal is present (Umiltà et al., 2001;
Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). However, inference on walker’s
goal is easier if the observer can make sensorimotor predictions
about the current state of the actor-environment system on
the basis of previous sensory input generated during natural
body translation. A simple illustration of this is the difficulty
one has to perform covert artificial tasks, as locomotion on a
treadmill compared to real forward locomotion, or when sensory
feedback information is lacking (see Courtine and Pozzo, 2004).
Even if locomotion on the spot may elicit a vivid impression
of translational motion (Pavlova et al., 2002; Saygin et al.,
2010; Viviani et al., 2011), the translating visual stimuli would
assign more easily a spatial goal to the perceived motion and
thus would facilitate the recall of specific kinematic details.
Accordingly, Thurman and Lu (2013) found that introducing
extrinsic translational motion congruent with the direction
implied by the intrinsic movements increased the perceived
animacy of spatially scrambled walkers. These authors proposed
that extrinsic motion would convey a clearer impression of
directionality and intentional behavior in the moving agents.
At last, because cyclical locomotor behavior of most
vertebrates relies on similar neural networks (Grillner and
Zangger, 1979; Lacquaniti et al., 1999; Dominici et al., 2011)
and because successful social interactions and survival of species
rely on efficient visual processing of biological motion, one
may predict that forward displacement will represent a prior
knowledge and ‘‘a strong attractor’’ for both human and
animal visual system. A recent behavioral study showing human
newborns preference for the translated locomotion supports the
existence of a privileged neural imprint constraining the visual
perception for horizontal displacements (Bidet-Ildei et al., 2014).
One possible hypothesis to explain the present impact of the
translational component on sensorimotor ERD would be that
artificial ‘‘on the spot treadmill locomotion’’ would mainly rely
on visual decoding mechanism. The occipital cortex and the STS
would ensure the visual recognition of cyclical displacements of
each body part controlled by central pattern generators similar
in several species (Orlovskil et al., 1999) whereas the horizontal
displacement would be necessary to motor resonance, that a
congruent body structure associated to biological kinematic
would improve.
CONCLUSION
Our results suggest that body translation prevails compare to
pictorial information in the process mapping the visual input
onto stored representations of movements. Nevertheless, the
exact manner by which perception of locomotion matches
the motor system remains elusive and requires further
neurophysiological investigations. Indeed, one central question
is how motor resonance initially identified for hand movement
is activated for cyclical lower limb movement mainly encoded in
the spinal cord.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
TP and AK conceived and designed the experiments. AK
performed the experiments. CC, AI and SP analyzed the data.
TP, CC, AI, GS and SP wrote the article. TP supervised the whole
project.
FUNDING
CAPES-COFECUB (project no. 819-14) supported this work.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnsys.20
17.00083/full#supplementary-material
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 13 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
REFERENCES
Babiloni, C., Babiloni, F., Carducci, F., Cincotti, F., Cocozza, G., Del Percio, C.,
et al. (2002). Human cortical electroencephalography (EEG) rhythms during
the observation of simple aimless movements: a high-resolution EEG study.
Neuroimage 17, 559–572. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91192-4
Belkacem, A. N., Hirose, H., Yoshimura, N., Shin, D., and Koike, Y. (2014).
Classification of four eye directions from EEG signals for eye-movement-
based communication systems. J. Med. Biol. Eng. 34, 581–588. doi: 10.5405/
jmbe.1596
Bertenthal, B. I., and Pinto, J. (1994). Global processing of biological motions.
Psychol. Sci. 5, 221–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
Berthoz, A., Graf, W., and Vidal, P. P. (1992). The Head-Neck Sensory Motor
System. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Bidet-Ildei, C., Kitromilides, E., Orliaguet, J. P., Pavlova, M., and Gentaz, E. (2014).
Preference for point-light human biological motion in newborns: contribution
of translational displacement. Dev. Psychol. 50, 113–120. doi: 10.1037/a0032956
Blake, R., and Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 58, 47–73. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (1999).
Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex.
Nature 402, 179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436.
doi: 10.1163/156856897x00357
Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P.
(2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action
observation. Curr. Biol. 16, 1905–1910. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065
Cannon, E. N., Yoo, K. H., Vanderwert, R. E., Ferrari, P. F., Woodward, A. L.,
and Fox, N. A. (2014). Action experience, more than observation, influences
mu rhythm desynchronization. PLoS One 9:e92002. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
0092002
Capotosto, P., Babiloni, C., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2009). Frontoparietal
cortex controls spatial attention through modulation of anticipatory αrhythms.
J. Neurosci. 29, 5863–5872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0539-09.2009
Cevallos, C., Zarka, D., Hoellinger, T., Leroy, A., Dan, B., and Cheron, G. (2015).
Oscillations in the human brain during walking execution, imagination and
observation. Neuropsychologia 79, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
2015.06.039
Chang, D. H., and Troje, N. F. (2008). Perception of animacy and direction from
local biological motion signals. J. Vis. 8:3. doi: 10.1167/8.5.3
Chiu, T. C., Gramann, K., Ko, L. W., Duann, J. R., Jung, T. P., and Lin, C. T.
(2012). αmodulation in parietal and retrosplenial cortex correlates with
navigation performance. Psychophysiology 49, 43–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.
2011.01270.x
Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Roux, S., and Martineau, J. (1999). Observation
and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified
electroencephalography. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1839–1842. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-
9568.1999.00598.x
Courtine, G., and Pozzo, T. (2004). Recovery of the locomotor function
after prolonged microgravity exposure. I. Head-trunk movement and
locomotor equilibrium during various tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 158, 86–99.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1877-2
Croft, R. J., and Barry, R. J. (2000). Removal of ocular artifact from the EEG: a
review. Neurophysiol. Clin. 30, 5–19. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(00)00055-1
Cutting, J. E., Moore, C., and Morrison, R. (1988). Masking the motions of human
gait. Percept. Psychophys. 44, 339–347. doi: 10.3758/bf03210415
Dayan, E., Casile, A., Levit-Binnun, N., Giese, M. A., Hendler, T., and Flash, T.
(2007). Neural representations of kinematic laws of motion: evidence for
action-perception coupling. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 20582–20587.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710033104
Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for
analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component
analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.
10.009
Di Dio, C., Di Cesare, G., Higuchi, S., Roberts, N., Vogt, S., and Rizzolatti, G.
(2013). The neural correlates of velocity processing during the observation
of a biological effector in the parietal and premotor cortex. Neuroimage 64,
425–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.026
Dominici, N., Ivanenko, Y. P., Cappellini, G., d’Avella, A., Mondì, V., Cicchese, M.,
et al. (2011). Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and their development.
Science 334, 997–999. doi: 10.1126/science.1210617
Eltoukhy, M., Asfour, S., Thompson, C., and Latta, L. (2012). Evaluation of
the performance of digital video analysis of human motion: dartfish tracking
system. Int. J. Sci. Eng. Res. 3, 1–6.
Engel, A. K., and Fries, P. (2010). β-band oscillations—signalling the status quo?
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in
the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593
Gilaie-Dotan, S., Saygin, A. P., Lorenzi, L. J., Rees, G., and Behrmann, M.
(2015). Ventral aspect of the visual form pathway is not critical for the
perception of biological motion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, E361–E370.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414974112
Gramann, K., Gwin, J. T., Ferris, D. P., Oie, K., Jung, T. P., Lin, C. T., et al. (2011).
Cognition in action: imaging brain/body dynamics in mobile humans. Rev.
Neurosci. 22, 593–608. doi: 10.1515/RNS.2011.047
Grasso, R., Bianchi, L., and Lacquaniti, F. (1998). Motor patterns for human gait:
backward versus forward locomotion. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1868–1885.
Grillner, S., and Wallen, P. (1985). Central pattern generators for locomotion,
with special reference to vertebrates. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 233–261.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.8.1.233
Grillner, S., and Zangger, P. (1979). On the central generation of locomotion in the
low spinal cat. Exp. Brain Res. 34, 241–261. doi: 10.1007/bf00235671
Grossman, E. D., and Blake, R. (2002). Brain areas active during visual
perception of biological motion. Neuron 35, 1167–1175. doi: 10.1016/s0896-
6273(02)00897-8
Hari, R. (2006). Action-perception connection and the cortical mu rhythm. Prog.
Brain Res. 159, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(06)59017-x
Hari, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., and Rizzolatti, G.
(1998). Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation:
a neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 15061–15065.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061
Harrell, F. (2015). Regression Modeling Strategies: With Applications to Linear
Models, Logistic and Ordinal Regression, and Survival analysis. Berlin: Springer.
Hess, W. (1943). Teleokinetisches und ereismatisches Kräftesystem in der
Biomotorik. Helv. Physiol. Pharmacol. Acta 1, C62–C63.
Hirai, M., Fukushima, H., and Hiraki, K. (2003). An event-related potentials
study of biological motion perception in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 344, 41–44.
doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00413-0
Hirai, M., Watanabe, S., Honda, Y., and Kakigi, R. (2009). Developmental changes
in point-light walker processing during childhood and adolescence: an event-
related potential study. Neuroscience 161, 311–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.
2009.03.026
Hiris, E. (2007). Detection of biological and nonbiological motion. J. Vis. 7:4.
doi: 10.1167/7.12.4
Ikeda, H., Blake, R., and Watanabe, K. (2005). Eccentric perception of biological
motion is unscalably poor. Vision Res. 45, 1935–1943. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.
2005.02.001
Ivanenko, Y. P., Grasso, R., Macellari, V., and Lacquaniti, F. (2002). Two-thirds
power law in human locomotion: role of ground contact forces. Neuroreport
13, 1171–1174. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200207020-00020
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its
analysis. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201–211. doi: 10.3758/bf03212378
Jokisch, D., Daum, I., Suchan, B., and Troje, N. F. (2005). Structural encoding and
recognition of biological motion: evidence from event-related potentials and
source analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 157, 195–204. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.06.025
Kopell, N., Whittington, M. A., and Kramer, M. A. (2011). Neuronal
assembly dynamics in the β1 frequency range permits short-term memory.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 3779–3784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019
676108
Krakowski, A. I., Ross, L. A., Snyder, A. C., Sehatpour, P., Kelly, S. P., and Foxe, J. J.
(2011). The neurophysiology of human biological motion processing: a
high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage 56, 373–383. doi: 10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2011.01.058
Kraskov, A., Philipp, R., Waldert, S., Vigneswaran, G., Quallo, M. M., and
Lemon, R. N. (2014). Corticospinal mirror neurons. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond.
B Biol. Sci. 369:20130174. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0174
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 14 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
Lacquaniti, F., Grasso, R., and Zago, M. (1999). Motor patterns in walking. News
Physiol. Sci. 14, 168–174.
Lacquaniti, F., Terzuolo, C., and Viviani, P. (1983). The law relating the kinematic
and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 54, 115–130.
doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(83)90027-6
Makeig, S. (1993). Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum
and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 86,
283–293. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90110-h
Makin, A. D., Poliakoff, E., Ackerley, R., and El-Deredy, W. (2012). Covert
tracking: a combined ERP and fixational eye movement study. PLoS One
7:e38479. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038479
Meirovitch, Y., Harris, H., Dayan, E., Arieli, A., and Flash, T. (2015). αand βband
event-related desynchronization reflects kinematic regularities. J. Neurosci. 35,
1627–1637. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5371-13.2015
Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., and Johnson, B. W. (2004). Primary motor cortex
activation during action observation revealed by wavelet analysis of the EEG.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1760–1766. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.004
Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L.,
Ramachandran, V. S., and Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron
dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 190–198.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014
Orgs, G., Dombrowski, J. H., Heil, M., and Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Expertise
in dance modulates α/βevent-related desynchronization during action
observation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 3380–3384. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.
06271.x
Orlovski˘
l, G. N., Deliagina, T., and Grillner, S. (1999). Neuronal Control of
Locomotion: From Mollusc to Man. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Pavlidou, A., Schnitzler, A., and Lange, J. (2014). βoscillations and their
functional role in movement perception. Transl. Neurosci. 5, 286–292.
doi: 10.2478/s13380-014-0236-4
Pavlova, M. A. (1989). The role of inversion in perception of biological motion
pattern. Perception 8:510.
Pavlova, M., Krägeloh-Mann, I., Birbaumer, N., and Sokolov, A. (2002). Biological
motion shown backwards: the apparent-facing effect. Perception 31, 435–443.
doi: 10.1068/p3262
Pavlova, M., and Sokolov, A. (2003). Prior knowledge about display inversion in
biological motion perception. Perception 32, 937–946. doi: 10.1068/p3428
Pavlova, M., Staudt, M., Sokolov, A., Birbaumer, N., and Krägeloh-Mann, I.
(2003). Perception and production of biological movement in patients with
early periventricular brain lesions. Brain 126, 692–701. doi: 10.1093/brain
/awg062
Penfield, W., and Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral cortex of man: a clinical
study of localization of function. JAMA 144:1412. doi: 10.1001/jama.1950.
02920160086033
Perry, A., and Bentin, S. (2009). Mirror activity in the human brain while observing
hand movements: a comparison between EEG desynchronization in the µ-
range and previous fMRI results. Brain Res. 1282, 126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.
brainres.2009.05.059
Peuskens, H., Vanrie, J., Verfaillie, K., and Orban, G. A. (2005). Specificity
of regions processing biological motion. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2864–2875.
doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04106.x
Pfurtscheller, G., Brunner, C., Schlögl, A., and Lopes da Silva, F. H. (2006). Mu
rhythm (de)synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different
motor imagery tasks. Neuroimage 31, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2005.12.003
Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., Flotzinger, D., and Pregenzer, M. (1997). EEG-
based discrimination between imagination of right and left hand movement.
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 103, 642–651. doi: 10.1016/s0013-
4694(97)00080-1
Pham, Q.-C., Berthoz, A., and Hicheur, H. (2011). Invariance of locomotor
trajectories across visual and gait direction conditions. Exp. Brain Res. 210,
207–215. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2619-x
Pineda, J. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: translating
‘‘seeing’’ and ‘‘hearing’’ into ‘‘doing’’. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.
brainresrev.2005.04.005
Plank, M., Müller, H. J., Onton, J., Makeig, S., and Gramann, K. (2010). ‘‘Human
EEG correlates of spatial navigation within egocentric and allocentric reference
frames,’’ in International Conference on Spatial Cognition VII, eds C. Hölscher,
T. F. Shipley, M. Olivetti Belardinelli, J. Bateman and N. S. Newcombe (Berlin:
Springer), 191–206.
Poom, L., and Olsson, H. (2002). Are mechanisms for perception of
biological motion different from mechanisms for perception of nonbiological
motion? Percept. Mot. Skills 95, 1301–1310. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.95.
3f.1301
Pozzo, T., Papaxanthis, C., Petit, J. L., Schweighofer, N., and Stucchi, N. (2006).
Kinematic features of movement tunes perception and action coupling. Behav.
Brain Res. 169, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.12.005
Press, C., Cook, J., Blakemore, S.-J., and Kilner, J. (2011). Dynamic modulation
of human motor activity when observing actions. J. Neurosci. 31, 2792–2800.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1595-10.2011
Press, S. J., and Wilson, S. (1978). Choosing between logistic regression and
discriminant analysis. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 73, 699–705. doi: 10.1080/01621459.
1978.10480080
Proffitt, D. R., Bertenthal, B. I., and Roberts, R. J. Jr. (1984). The role of occlusion
in reducing multistability in moving point-light displays. Percept. Psychophys.
36, 315–323. doi: 10.3758/bf03202783
Quandt, L. C., and Marshall, P. J. (2014). The effect of action experience on
sensorimotor EEG rhythms during action observation. Neuropsychologia 56,
401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.015
R Core Team. (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing.
Vienna, Austria. Available online at: http://www.R-project.org/
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev.
Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological
mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation
of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670. doi: 10.1038/350
90060
Saunier, G., Martins, E. F., Dias, E. C., de Oliveira, J. M., Pozzo, T., and
Vargas, C. D. (2013). Electrophysiological correlates of biological motion
permanence in humans. Behav. Brain Res. 236, 166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.
2012.08.038
Saunier, G., Papaxanthis, C., Vargas, C. D., and Pozzo, T. (2008). Inference
of complex human motion requires internal models of action: behavioral
evidence. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 399–409. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1162-2
Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas
necessary for biological motion perception. Brain 130, 2452–2461.
doi: 10.1093/brain/awm162
Saygin, A. P., Chaminade, T., Ishiguro, H., Driver, J., and Frith, C. (2012). The
thing that should not be: predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving
human and humanoid robot actions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 413–422.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr025
Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., and Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual
thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in
autism spectrum conditions. PLoS One 5:e13491. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
0013491
Somers, R. H. (1962). A new asymmetric measure of association for ordinal
variables. Am. Soc. Rev. 26, 799–811. doi: 10.2307/2090408
Steyerberg, E. W., Harrell, F. E. Jr., Borsboom, G. J., Eijkemans, M., Vergouwe, Y.,
and Habbema, J. D. F. (2001). Internal validation of predictive models:
efficiency of some procedures for logistic regression analysis. J. Clin. Epidemiol.
54, 774–781. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00341-9
Sumi, S. (1984). Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot
pattern. Perception 13, 283–286. doi: 10.1068/p130283
Thornton, I. M. (1998). The visual perception of human locomotion. Cogn.
Neuropsychol. 15, 535–552. doi: 10.1080/026432998381014
Thurman, S. M., and Lu, H. (2013). Physical and biological constraints govern
perceived animacy of scrambled human forms. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1133–1141.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612467212
Ulloa, E. R., and Pineda, J. A. (2007). Recognition of point-light biological motion:
mu rhythms and mirror neuron activity. Behav. Brain Res. 183, 188–194.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007
Umiltà, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., et al.
(2001). I know what you are doing. a neurophysiological study. Neuron 31,
155–165. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3
Urgen, B. A., Plank, M., Ishiguro, H., Poizner, H., and Saygin, A. P.
(2013). EEG theta and Mu oscillations during perception of human
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 15 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83
Pozzo et al. Cortical Correlates of a Translating Point-Light Walker
and robot actions. Front. Neurorobot. 7:19. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2013.
00019
Viviani, P., Figliozzi, F., Campione, G. C., and Lacquaniti, F. (2011). Detecting
temporal reversals in human locomotion. Exp. Brain Res. 214, 93–103.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2809-6
Viviani, P., and Stucchi, N. (1992). Biological movements look uniform: evidence
of motor-perceptual interactions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18,
603–623. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.3.603
Zarka, D., Cevallos, C., Petieau, M., Hoellinger, T., Dan, B., and Cheron, G. (2014).
Neural rhythmic symphony of human walking observation: upside-down and
Uncoordinated condition on cortical theta, α,βand γoscillations. Front. Syst.
Neurosci. 8:169. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00169
Zumer, J. M., Scheeringa, R., Schoffelen, J. M., Norris, D. G., and Jensen, O.
(2014). Occipital αactivity during stimulus processing gates the information
flow to object-selective cortex. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001965. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pbio.1001965
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2017 Pozzo, Inuggi, Keuroghlanian, Panzeri, Saunier and Campus.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums
is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the
original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply
with these terms.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 16 November 2017 | Volume 11 | Article 83

Supplementary resource (1)

... Of note, the mu rhythm is formed by the variable contribution of both alpha-and beta-band activity, and also the latter is suppressed by both action execution, MI and AO. Thus, both the alpha and the beta-band mu components appear to be modulated by MNS activity (Babiloni et al., 2002;Hobson and Bishop, 2016;Pozzo et al., 2017). Importantly, the two EEG frequency bands implicated in these processes align with those modulated by cerebellar NIBS . ...
Article
Full-text available
Mirror neurons show activity during both the execution (AE) and observation of actions (AO). The Mirror Neuron System (MNS) could be involved during motor imagery (MI) as well. Extensive research suggests that the cerebellum is interconnected with the MNS and may be critically involved in its activities. We gathered evidence on the cerebellum's role in MNS functions, both theoretically and experimentally. Evidence shows that the cerebellum plays a major role during AO and MI and that its lesions impair MNS functions likely because, by modulating the activity of cortical inhibitory interneurons with mirror properties, the cerebellum may contribute to visuomotor matching, which is fundamental for shaping mirror properties. Indeed, the cerebellum may strengthen sensory-motor patterns that minimise the discrepancy between predicted and actual outcome, both during AE and AO. Furthermore, through its connections with the hippocampus, the cerebellum might be involved in internal simulations of motor programs during MI. Finally, as cerebellar neuromodulation might improve its impact on MNS activity, we explored its potential neurophysiological and neurorehabilitation implications.
... In addition, we also observed the cortical tracking of BM signals on the central electrodes. Previous research has found critical contribution of motor regions to BM processing ( Fraiman et al., 2014 ;Martins et al., 2017 ;Pozzo et al., 2017 ;Saygin, 2007 ;Saygin et al., 2004 ;Ulloa and Pineda, 2007 ;van Kemenade et al., 2012 ), with a causal relationship between premotor areas and BM perception ( Saygin, 2007 ;van Kemenade et al., 2012 ). Thus, although we did not find a significant behavioral-neural correlation in the central region, this region remains a possible candidate for the cortical tracking response. ...
Article
Full-text available
Biological motion (BM) perception is of great survival value to human beings. The critical characteristics of BM information lie in kinematic cues containing rhythmic structures. However, how rhythmic kinematic structures of BM are dynamically represented in the brain and contribute to visual BM processing remains largely unknown. Here, we probed this issue in three experiments using electroencephalogram (EEG). We found that neural oscillations of observers entrained to the hierarchical kinematic structures of the BM sequences (i.e., step-cycle and gait-cycle for point-light walkers). Notably, only the cortical tracking of the higher-level rhythmic structure (i.e., gait-cycle) exhibited a BM processing specificity, manifested by enhanced neural responses to upright over inverted BM stimuli. This effect could be extended to different motion types and tasks, with its strength positively correlated with the perceptual sensitivity to BM stimuli at the right temporal brain region dedicated to visual BM processing. Modeling results further suggest that the neural encoding of spatiotemporally integrative kinematic cues, in particular the opponent motions of bilateral limbs, drives the selective cortical tracking of BM information. These findings underscore the existence of a cortical mechanism that encodes periodic kinematic features of body movements, which underlies the dynamic construction of visual BM perception.
... Nevertheless, the possibility that kinematic cues are sufficient to override other information -even when it is seemingly more readily available -might be restricted to conditions in which the information density contained in the visual stimuli is limited, as other results also suggest (Flach et al. [96]; cf. Pozzo et al. [257]). It might be that the more we go down levels in the kinematic hierarchy, the more we enter a blurred zone whereby task-independent kinematic invariants of biological motion progressively lose importance to the eyes of human observers, as long as other reliable sources of information can be easily gathered. ...
Article
Full-text available
The nervous system is sensitive to statistical regularities of the external world and forms internal models of these regularities to predict environmental dynamics. Given the inherently social nature of human behavior, being capable of building reliable predictive models of others' actions may be essential for successful interaction. While social prediction might seem to be a daunting task, the study of human motor control has accumulated ample evidence that our movements follow a series of kinematic invariants, which can be used by observers to reduce their uncertainty during social exchanges. Here, we provide an overview of the most salient regularities that shape biological motion, examine the role of these invariants in recognizing others' actions, and speculate that anchoring socially-relevant perceptual decisions to such kinematic invariants provides a key computational advantage for inferring conspecifics' goals and intentions.
... As expected, direct comparisons showed significant differences between approach and control within beta and alpha, as well as avoidance and control within beta (Katsumi et al., 2020). This pattern is in line with previous research showing changes in EEG power during processing of actions such as walking (Pozzo et al., 2017;Cevallos et al., 2015;Pavlidou et al., 2014;Zarka et al., 2014) and suggests enhanced sensorimotor processing (Cheyne, 2013). Interestingly, this pattern was not shown for avoidance compared with control within alpha for the overall time window (100-900 msec). ...
Article
Full-text available
Cooperation behaviors during social decision-making have been shown to be sensitive to manipulations of context. However, it remains unclear how aspects of context in dynamic social interactions, such as observed nonverbal behaviors, may modulate cooperation decisions and the associated neural mechanisms. In this study, participants responded to offers from proposers to split $10 in an Ultimatum Game following observation of proposer approach (friendly) or avoidance (nonfriendly) behaviors, displayed by dynamic whole-body animated avatars, or following a nonsocial interaction control condition. As expected, behavioral results showed that participants tended to have greater acceptance rates for unfair offers following observed nonverbal social interactions with proposers compared with control, suggesting an enhancing effect of social interactions on cooperative decisions. ERP results showed greater N1 and N2 responses at the beginning of social interaction conditions compared with control, and greater sustained and late positivity responses for observed approach and avoidance proposer behaviors compared with control. Event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) results showed differential sensitivity within theta, alpha, and beta bands during observation of social interactions and offers that was associated with subsequent decision behaviors. Together, these results point to the impact of proposers' nonverbal behaviors on subsequent cooperation decisions at both behavioral and neural levels. The ERP and ERSP findings suggest modulated attention, monitoring, and processing of biological motion during the observed nonverbal social interactions, influencing the participants' responses to offers. These findings shed light on electrophysiological correlates of response to observed social interactions that predict subsequent social decisions.
... Specifically, observing dynamic nonverbal behaviors was associated with greater suppression of power in the beta range from ∼700 to 990 msec poststimulus over central electrode sites. This result is overall consistent with previous studies identifying similar changes in beta power during the observation of simple everyday actions such as hand movements and walking (Pozzo et al., 2017;Zarka et al., 2014;Darvas, Rao, & Murias, 2013). Available evidence suggests that the attenuation of EEG activity in the alpha and beta ranges during action observation may reflect increased activity within the sensorimotor cortices (reviewed in Cheyne, 2013;Pineda, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite evidence identifying the role of group membership in social cognition, the neural mechanisms associated with the perception and evaluation of nonverbal behaviors displayed by in-group versus out-group members remain unclear. Here, 42 white participants underwent electroencephalographic recording while observing social encounters involving dynamic displays of nonverbal behaviors by racial in-group and out-group avatar characters. Dynamic behaviors included approach and avoidance poses and expressions, followed by the participants' ratings of the avatars displaying them. Behaviorally, participants showed longer RTs when evaluating in-group approach behavior compared with other behaviors, possibly suggesting increased interest and attention devoted to processing positive social encounters with their in-group members. Analyses of ERPs revealed differential sensitivity of the N450 and late positivity components to social cues, with the former showing initial sensitivity to the presence of a humanoid avatar character at the beginning of social encounters and the latter showing sensitivity to dynamic nonverbal behaviors displayed by the avatars. Moreover, time–frequency analysis of electroencephalography data also identified suppression of beta-range power linked to the observation of dynamic nonverbal behaviors. Notably, the magnitude of these responses was modulated by the degree of behavioral racial in-group bias. This suggests that differential neural sensitivity to nonverbal cues while observing social encounters is associated with subsequent in-group bias manifested in the evaluation of such encounters. Collectively, these findings shed light on the mechanisms of racial in-group bias in social cognition and have implications for understanding factors related to successful interactions with individuals from diverse racial backgrounds.
Article
Full-text available
The finding of mirror neurons (MNs) has provided a biological substrate to a new concept of cognition, relating data on actions and perceptions not only to integrate perception in action planning and execution but also as a neural mechanism supporting a wide range of cognitive functions. Here we first summarize data on MN localization and role in primates, then we report findings in normal human subjects: functional magnetic resonance imaging and neurophysiological studies sustain that MNs have a role in motor learning and recognizing actions and intentions of others, and they also support an embodied view of language, empathy, and memory. Then, we detail the results of literature searching on MNs and embodied cognition in Parkinson's disease (PD), frontotemporal dementia (FTD)/amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), and in mild cognitive impairment (MCI)/Alzheimer's disease (AD). In PD the network of MN could be altered, but its hyperactivation might support motor and cognitive performances at least in early stages. In the ALS/FTD continuum, preliminary evidence points out to an involvement of the MN network, which could explain language and inter‐subjectivity deficits shown in patients affected by these clinical entities. In the MCI/AD spectrum, a few recent studies suggest a possible progressive involvement from posterior to anterior areas of the MN network, with the brain putting in place compensatory mechanisms in early stages. Reinterpreting neurodegenerative diseases at the light of the new views about brain organization stemming from the discovery of MN could help to better comprehend clinical manifestations and open new pathways to rehabilitation.
Article
The head carries most of the sensory systems that enable us to function effectively in our three-dimensional habitat. Without adequate head movement control, efficient spatial orientation and motor responses to visual and auditory stimuli could not be carried out. This book is an account of the control of vertebrate head movements and its biomechanical and neural basis. It covers the entire spectrum of research on head-neck movements, ranging from the global description and analysis of a particular behavior to its underlying mechanisms at the level of neurotransmitter release and membrane biophysics. Physiological and anatomical aspects are stressed. The role of head movements in upright stance and other functional contexts within the vertebrate hierarchy is juxtaposed with the mechanisms of orienting behavior in a number of invertebrates. This reveals a plethora of solutions among different animal species for the problem of orientation in three-dimensional space. Although head movement control in humans figures prominently in this volume, the anatomical-physiological comparisons show that the human system is not unique. The conference from which this volume originated surveyed research and theory on motor control mechanisms in the head-neck sensory-motor system. It was held in Fontainbleau, France, from July 17-24, 1989. The book provides a broad panorama of methodological and theoretical approaches to the field of head movement control.
Article
How does biological motion perception change with display orientation? As previously shown, display inversion (180°) completely prevents veridical perception of biological motion. However, with upright orientation (0°), observers are able to recover the invariant structure through biological motion despite reverse transformation (showing the film backwards) or changing the presentation rate (Pavlova, 1995 Perception 24 Supplement, 112). In the present experiments, observers saw the biological motion pattern at various display deviations, from inverted to upright orientation (180°, 150°, 120°, 90°, 60°, 30°, 0°), in the right or left hemifield, on a circular screen monitor. The display consisted of an array of 11 dots on the main joints of an invisible walker moving as if on a treadmill. While viewing (60 s), observers pressed a key each time their perception changed from one stable percept to another (eg when the direction of apparent rotation of the pattern reversed). The perceived multistability (the number of key-presses) increased as orientation was varied from inverted to 90°, and then decreased between 90° and upright. The recognition of walking figure improved abruptly with changing orientation: at deviations of 60° and 30° most observer reported seeing the walking figure spontaneously, yet the pattern was seen as multistable. The findings imply the relative power of constraints (such as orientation) in perception of biological motion that is discussed in relation to the KSD principle in event perception [Runeson, 1994, in Perceiving Events and Objects Eds Jansson, Epstein, Bergström (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum) pp 383 – 405].
Article
We recorded electrical activity from 532 neurons in the rostral part of inferior area 6 (area F5) of two macaque monkeys. Previous data had shown that neurons of this area discharge during goal-directed hand and mouth movements. We describe here the properties of a newly discovered set of F5 neurons ("mirror neurons', n = 92) all of which became active both when the monkey performed a given action and when it observed a similar action performed by the experimenter. Mirror neurons, in order to be visually triggered, required an interaction between the agent of the action and the object of it. The sight of the agent alone or of the object alone (three-dimensional objects, food) were ineffective. Hand and the mouth were by far the most effective agents. The actions most represented among those activating mirror neurons were grasping, manipulating and placing. In most mirror neurons (92%) there was a clear relation between the visual action they responded to and the motor response they coded. In approximately 30% of mirror neurons the congruence was very strict and the effective observed and executed actions corresponded both in terms of general action (e.g. grasping) and in terms of the way in which that action was executed (e.g. precision grip). We conclude by proposing that mirror neurons form a system for matching observation and execution of motor actions. We discuss the possible role of this system in action recognition and, given the proposed homology between F5 and human Brocca's region, we posit that a matching system, similar to that of mirror neurons exists in humans and could be involved in recognition of actions as well as phonetic gestures.
Article
In the present high-resolution electroencephalographic (EEG) study, we computed event-related desynchronization and synchronization (ERD/ERS) of α (about 10 Hz) and β (about 20 Hz) rhythms in association with the execution (with visual feedback) and observation of brisk unilateral right and left aimless finger movements. A first scope was to test the topographical “functional equivalence” of cortical rhythmicity related to movement execution and observation, which would represent an ideal cortical observation/execution matching system. A second scope was to evaluate the hypothesis of a left or right hemisphere prevalence of the cortical rhythmicity related to the movement observation compared to the movement execution. EEG (128 electrodes) was recorded in 10 healthy right-handed volunteers. Surface Laplacian estimation spatially enhanced EEG data over a MRI-constraint head model. Under both conditions, ERD peaked during the movement execution or observation and was replaced by a ERS “rebound” or “recovery,” which peaked during the postevent period. Topographical results are in favor of a “functional equivalence” (i.e., similar ERD/ERS values in magnitude and timing) of α and β rhythmicity in central scalp regions overlying premotor/primary sensorimotor cortex. On the contrary, the functional equivalence of α rhythmicity was negligible (i.e., different ERD/ERS values in magnitude and timing) in parietal–occipital scalp regions overlying posterior parietal and parieto-occipital cortex, which could be the neural substrate to distinguish among the own motor intensions and others' aimless movements (i.e., visuomotor transformation integrated with sensorimotor, postural, and kinematics representations). Finally, the pattern of hemispherical cortical rhythmicity did not support a “simple concentration” of movement observation functions in the left or right hemisphere.
Article
Summary Recent neuroimaging and psychophysical findings suggest that perception and production of human body motion share a common representational network. In the present study, we address the issue of whether early disorders in production of biological movement correspond to impairment in biological motion perception. By using the simultaneous masking paradigm, we examined visual sensitivity to biological motion in adolescents (aged 13‐16 years) who were born very preterm (at 27‐ 33 gestational weeks). In a confidence rating procedure, the presence of a point-light walking figure embedded in a moving mask was judged. The participants differed in their locomotion ability, ranging from normal to a complete walking disability exhibiting signs of leg-dominated bilateral spastic cerebral palsy (BS-CP) caused by periventricular leukomalacia (PVL). Irrespective of an ability to produce movement, patients with a similar extent of PVL in the parieto-occipital complex exhibit nearly the same sensitivity to biological motion. Sensitivity correlates negatively with the extent of PVL over the parieto-occipital complex, whereas neither the severity of motor disorder nor the severity of pyramidal tract affection relate significantly to the sensitivity index. The data suggest that perception of biological motion is not substantially affected by an observer’s early restrictions in body movement. Instead, the findings favour the assumption that the common network for perception and production of biological motion might be inherent for the brain. Motor experience per se does not appear to be necessary for the visual analysis of human movement.
Article
A pair of asymmetric coefficients is introduced which are appropriate for measuring the association in ordered contingency tables. Earlier statistical methods are reviewed, and the new coefficients are shown to be closely related to both Kendall's tau-b and to Goodman and Kruskal's gamma, as well as the commonly used "percentage-difference." They are also shown to have a operational interpretation (in the Goodman-Kruskal sense). Their utility in both square and non-square contingency tables is discussed, as well as the way they may be interpreted as ordinal analogues of the traditional regression coefficients.