Content uploaded by Zahra Zamanian
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Zahra Zamanian on Jun 20, 2018
Content may be subject to copyright.
Original Article
37 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
Comprehension of workplace safety signs: A case study in
Shiraz industrial park
Zamanian Z , PhD1, Afshin A, BSc 2, Davoudiantalab AH, BSc 2, Hashemi H, PhD 3*
1- Associate Prof., Dept of Occupational Health, School of Health, Shiraz University of Medical Sciences, Shiraz, Iran.
2- BSc, Dept of Occupational Health, School of Health, Student Research Committee, Shiraz University of Medical
Sciences, Shiraz, Iran. 3- Lecturer, Dept of Environmental Health Engineering, School of Health, Shahrekord University
of Medical Sciences, Shahrekord, Iran.
Abstract Received: May 2013, Accepted: March 2014
Background: Safety signs provide information as well as instructions concerning the hazard or
dangers in the workplace. The correct comprehension of these signs is very important for doing the
necessary feedback in the specific situation which are described.
Materials and Methods: In this descriptive study, the comprehension of 10 selected safety signs
were investigated in 53 randomly selected employees of an industrial company in Shiraz. The
comprehension test was carried out with the aid of a standard questionnaire obtained from ISO
9186-1:2007.
Results: In this study the mean comprehension scores of the tested signs were 65.95 percent with
standard deviation if 28.7. The highest and lowest comprehension scores were for “use hearing
protectors” and “biological hazard” respectively. The comprehension of 40 percent of the tested
safety signs was lower than the minimum acceptable values of ISO 3864 and ANSI Z535.3
standards.
Conclusion: This study showed that there is a significant difference in comprehensions of tested
safety signs. For full comprehension of safety signs it is necessary to train the employees or add
supplementary texts to the symbols in some cases.
Key words: Safety, Signs, Workplace, Color, Culture
Introduction
Safety signs are one of the methods of
informing and warning the staff regarding the
type and severity of the workplace dangers
and are used considering the risk of dangers
where necessary (1-3). According to
International Standard Organization (ISO)
17724: 2003, a safety sign is a sign which
transfers a safety message. In fact, when the
signs are accompanied by colors, geometrical
figures and graphical signs, they transfer a
specific safety message (4).
In general, safety signs may represent a
danger, dangerous conditions, or
consequences of being exposed to dangers
(5,6). Also, some signs include cautions and
safety recommendations for the individuals
who execute unsafe and dangerous
behaviors; and, at the same time, show the
way to prevent such behaviors (3). Based on
some studies, various factors such as the
level of education, working experience,
working time (7-9), type of safety signs
(10,11), background* color of safety signs
and training (12-14) affect the individuals’
comprehension of the safety signs. Yet,
cultural differences are also effective in
comprehension of safety signs (15).
For instance, Chan et al. conducted a study in
U.S. and showed that the Chinese and
* Corresponding author: Hassan Hashemi, Dept of
Environmental Health Engineering, School of Health,
Shahrekord University of Medical Sciences,
Shahrekord, Iran.
Email: hashemi@hlth.mui.ac.ir
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Zamanian et al
38 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
Korean residents of U.S. comprehend the
safety signs less than the Americans.
Besides, comprehension of some signs was
quite difficult for non-Americans (16).
Furthermore, safety signs can be beneficial if
they are diverse and attractive. Also, people
should be able to understand these signs
because in cases they do not understand the
messages, they will not be able to recognize
the dangers and take the necessary
precautions (17). According to National
Safety Council, deficiency in the correct
information transfer by the safety signs is the
third common factor in investigation of the
incidents (18). The studies conducted by
Chan & Chan showed that safety signs have
to be evaluated regarding their correct
comprehension by people before use (19).
Thus, the staff’s familiarity with these signs
is of great importance in order to have a
correct understanding of the signs and their
responsibilities in case of being exposed to
dangers.
Based on what was mentioned above and
considering the fact that most safety signs in
Iran are adopted from other countries, the
present study aims to determine the
comprehensibility of the workplace safety
signs and present strategies for improving
their comprehensibility.
Materials and Methods
The present cross-sectional study was
conducted in one of the industrial companies
of Shiraz, Iran in 2013. According to the
sections 2, 3 and 6 of ISO 9186-1, at least 50
individuals from each country should take
part in each safety signs comprehension test.
Therefore, 53 non-colorblind staff of the
above mentioned company were randomly
selected for taking part in the safety signs
comprehension test using the table of random
numbers. The study data were gathered using
the standard questionnaire of International
Standard Organization (20) on quantification
of safety signs comprehension. The validity
of the questionnaire was confirmed by 3
safety and ergonomics professors. This
questionnaire contained open-ended
questions and consisted of 4 sections: 1- how
to complete the questionnaire, 2-
demographic characteristics including age,
sex, level of education, health status and type
of probable disability, 3- an example of how
to complete the safety signs comprehension
test and 4- the safety signs comprehension
test.
After all, ten 8×8 colored, back labeled signs,
including 4 prohibition signs, 2 mandatory
signs, 2 warning signs and 2 signs related to
safe conditions, were randomly selected and
glued on ten A4 papers. The used safety
signs and their meanings are presented in
table 2.
The data were analyzed using the SPSS
statistical software (v. 11.5) and the staff’s
responses to the safety signs comprehension
test were compared with the acceptable
ranges of American National Standards
Institute (ANSI) Z535.3 (21)and ISO 3864
(22)standards. Since the wrong answers
which reversely transfer the safety signs’
meanings are highly important, these answers
were separately investigated.
Results
In this study, 89.1% of the participants were
male and the rest were female. In addition,
35.5%, 44.6%, 19.2% and 7% of the subjects
were 20-30, 31-40, 41-50 and >51 years old,
respectively. Twenty three point eight
percent of the study's participants had under
high school diploma degrees, 52.1% had high
school diplomas and 25.1% had academic
degrees (table1). It should be noted that all
the study subjects were healthy.
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Comprehension of workplace safety signs
39 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
Table 1: Some demographic characteristics in one
industrial company (n= 53)
level of education
Job tenure
Age
23.8%
52.1% 25.1%
Under
Diploma
Diploma academic
degrees
4
43
3.
.5
5%
%
42.2% 7.5% 3.4% 3.4%
<
<5
5
5
5-
-1
10
0
1
10
0-
-1
15
5
1
15
5-
-2
20
0 >
>2
20
0
35.5%
44.6% 19.2%
0.7%
20-30
31-40
41-50 51>
The tested safety signs and their
comprehension rate by the study subjects are
presented in table 2.
As the table depicts, the mean score of
correct comprehension (correct responses)
was 70.94+27.38.
Table 2: The tested safety signs and their comprehension by the study subjects
Sign
Meaning
Responses
Correct
Incorrect
Reverse
I don’t know
No response
Number
%
Number
%
Number
%
Number
%
Number
%
First aid
49
92.45
4
7.54
0
0
0
0
0
0
Use ear
protection
53
100
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
Arrange
correctly
24
45.28
25
47.16
1
1.88
2
3.77
1
1.88
Toxic
material
49
92.45
3
5.66
0
0
1
1.88
0
0
Biological
danger
16
30.18
16
30.18
0
0
18
33.96
3
5.66
No
smoking
52
98.11
1
1.88
0
0
0
0
0
0
Do not
operate the
device
19
35.84
29
54.71
0
0
2
3.77
3
5.66
Not
drinking
water
41
77.35
8
15.09
0
0
3
5.66
1
1.88
Don’t
repair
27
50.94
20
37.73
0
0
5
9.43
1
1.88
Emergency
exit
46
86.79
2
3.77
2
3.77
1
1.88
2
3.77
Total
Mean
37.6
70.94
10.8
20.37
0.3
0.56
3.2
6.03
1.1
2.07
Standard
deviation
14.51
27.38
10.79
20.37
0.67
1.27
5.43
10.24
1.19
2.25
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Zamanian et al
40 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
Comparison of the subjects’ comprehension
of the safety signs is shown in Figure 1. As it
can be seen, the highest and lowest rates of
correct comprehension were related to “Use
ear protection” (100%) and “Biological
danger” (30.2%) signs, respectively.
In addition, most of the incorrect responses
were related to “Do not operate the device”
(54.7%) and “Arrange correctly” (42.2%)
signs. Besides, 2 subjects had provided
reverse responses for the “Emergency exit”
sign.
Figure 1: The subjects’ comprehension of the studied safety signs
Discussion
In the present study, correct responses were
considered as the criteria for correct
comprehension of the safety signs. The
relatively high standard deviation of the
correct responses (27.38%) shows that the
comprehension of these safety signs was
significantly different from each other and
each sign has had its own specific
comprehension pattern. According to the
results, the mean of correct responses to the
studied safety signs was 70.94% which is in
agreement with the study performed by Ng et
al (3) on the Chinese students in Hong Kong.
In that study, 67.54% of the participants
answered the study safety signs correctly.
Based on the standard number ISO 3864 (22)
of the International Standard Organization
and standard number ANSI Z535.3 of ANSI
(21), the mean of correct responses to safety
signs must be at least 67% and 85% of the
study subjects, respectively.
First aid
Use ear
protection
Arrange
correctly
Toxic
material
Biological
danger
No
smoking
Don’t
operate the
device
Not
drinking
water
Don’t repair
Emergency
exit
No response
I don’t
know
Reverse
Incorrect
Correct
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Comprehension of workplace safety signs
41 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
The results show no significant relation
between some demographic characteristics
such as age, job and level of education with
the mean of the correct responses to safety
signs (Pval> 0.05). These results could be
existed due to the small size of the sample.
According to Table 2, the mean of correct
responses to 5 signs investigated in this
study; i.e., “First aid”, “Use ear protection”,
“Toxic material”, “No smoking” and
“Emergency exit”, was above 85%.
Therefore, they had the minimum rate of
American National Standards Institute (21).
Considering the International Standard
Organization's standards, in addition to the 5
above mentioned signs, the mean of correct
comprehension of “Not drinking water” sign
was also above 67%. Thus, the mean of
correct responses was below the acceptable
ranges in ISO 3864 and ANSI Z535.3 (21)
standards in 40% of the signs, including
“Arrange correctly”, “Biological danger”,
“Do not operate the device” and “Don’t
repair”. In the study by Ng et al., (3) the
mean of correct comprehension of 60% of
the studied safety signs was below the
acceptable ranges of ISO 3864 (22) and
ANSI Z535.3 (21) standards. Also, in the
study of Papastavrou and Lehto (14) only
5.88% and 35.29% of the study signs
satisfied the acceptable ranges of ANSI (21)
and ISO (20-22). On the other hand, Manop
conducted a study and showed that 79% of
the studied safety signs in Thailand chemical
industries achieved the acceptable criterion
of 85% (10). Yet, one other study which was
performed in the Intensive Care Units (ICUs)
of China showed that among the tested signs,
3 and 4 satisfied the acceptable ranges of
ANSI and ISO, respectively (19).
Based on what was mentioned above, it
seems that the differences, in the correct
comprehension rates of safety signs in
various studies are related to the cultural
differences, the features of the study
population, previous training and the
commonness of the sign in the study
industry.
In the present study, although the staff knew
the overall concepts of the safety signs, the
results showed that the percentage of
incorrect answers to the signs which were
less used in that industry was significantly
high. For instance, 57.4% of the study
subjects had incorrectly answered to “Do not
operate the device” sign. In other words, this
sign was not able to completely introduce
itself and had low self-explanation. This was
also the case for “Biological danger” sign
which received the lowest rate of correct
responses (30.18%) because it was quite
uncommon in the study industry. In contrast
to the other signs, the form of this sign was
also quite unfamiliar to the study subjects.
On the other hand, 98.11% of the participants
provided correct answers to “No smoking”
sign because this sign is quite common in
both the society and the workplace. Overall,
the signs which were more commonly used
in the industry had a higher comprehension
level. This is in line with the results of other
studies conducted on the issue (14, 19).
In the case that the concept of a safety sign is
reversely transferred, its utilization in safety
conditions is of utmost importance.
According to ANSI Z535.3 (21) standard, the
acceptable rate of reverse responses is 5%
and a sign with above 5% reverse responses
is considered as a confusing one. As Table 2
depicts, in this study, 2 reverse responses
(3.8%) were related to “Emergency exit”
sign and 1 (1.88%) was related to “Arrange
correctly” sign, which is within the
acceptable range of ANSI standard. In other
words, based on ISO 3864 (22) standard, the
designed safety signs were appropriate and
not confusing. In spite of the fact that the
“Emergency exit” sign is one of the main
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Zamanian et al
42 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
safety signs and all the industrial workers are
expected to be familiar with this sign, 11.4%
of the responses to this sign were incorrect,
reverse and no response and 1.9% were “I
don’t know”. This might be due to the lack of
the staff’s training regarding the meaning of
this sign. In such cases, the text is
recommended to be written under the sign.
Conclusions
The findings of the current study showed
differences in the rate of correct
comprehension of different study safety
signs. In this study, the rate of correct
comprehension of 40% of the safety signs
was below the minimum acceptable range of
ISO 3864 and ANSI Z535.3 standards. The
staff’s familiarity with the signs, training
them regarding the meanings of safety signs
and sometimes writing texts under the signs
can significantly affect their comprehension
of the signs.
Acknowledgements
The present study was financially supported
by Student Research Committee of the
research vice-chancellor of Shiraz University
of Medical Sciences, Shiraz, Iran (No; 90-
5867).
Conflict of interest: Non declared
References
1. Cox S, Jones B, Rycraft H. Behavioral
approaches to safety management within UK
reactor plants. Saf Sci 2004; 42(9): 825-39.
2. Luria G, Rafaeli A. Testing safety
commitment in organizations through
interpretations of safety artifacts. J Safety
Res 2008; 39(5):519-28.
3. Ng, AWY, Lo HWC, Chan AHS. Measuring
the usability of safety signs: A use of System
Usability Scale (SUS). Proceeding of the
International Multi Conference of Engineer
and Computer Scientists; 2011 March 16-18;
Hong Kong. IMECS 2011; 2. P1-6.
4. ISO 17724:2003 graphical symbols-
vocabulary. International Organization for
Standardization, Geneva, Switzerland.
Available from:
http://www.iso.org/iso/catalogue_detail.htm?
csnumber=33186
5. Wogalter MS, Conzola VC, Smith-Jackson
TL. Research-based guidelines for warning
design and evaluation. Appl Ergon 2002;
33(3):219-30.
6. Jorgensen F, Wentzel-Larsen T. Optimal use
of warning signs in traffic. Accid Anal Prev
1999; 31(6):729-38.
7. Piamonte DPT, Abeysekera JDA, Ohlsson K.
Understanding small graphical symbols: a
cross-cultural study. Int J Ind Ergon 2001;
27(6):399-404.
8. Edworthy J. The design and implementation
of non-verbal auditory warnings. Appl Ergon
1994; 25(4):202-10.
9. Belz SM, Robinson GS, Casali JG. A new
class of auditory warning signals for complex
systems: auditory icons. Hum Factors 1999;
41(4):608-18.
10. Chunin M. Factors determining compliance
with safety signs in industrial settings. [PhD
Thesis]. Sydney: University of New South
Wales; 2001.
11. Lehto M, Salvendy G. Warnings: a
supplement not a substitute for other
approaches to safety. Ergonomics 1995;
38(11):2155-63.
12. Chan AH, Ng AW. Effects of sign
characteristics and training methods on safety
sign training effectiveness. Ergonomics 2010;
53(11):1325-46.
Yu RF, Chan AHS, Salvendy G. Chinese
perceptions of implied hazard for signal
words and surround shapes. Human Factors
and Ergonomics in Manufacturing & Service
Industries 2004; 14(1):69-80.
13. Papastavrou JD, Lehto MR. Improving the
effectiveness of warnings by increasing the
appropriateness of their information content:
some hypotheses about human compliance.
Saf Sci 1996; 21(3):175-89.
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]
Comprehension of workplace safety signs
43 JOHE, Winter-spring 2013; 2 (1-2)
14. Gherardi S, Nicolini D. Learning the trade: A
culture of safety in practice. Organization
2002; 9(2):191-223.
15. Chan AHS, Han SH, Ng AWY, Park W.
Hong Kong Chinese and Korean
comprehension of American security safety
symbols. Int J Ind Ergon 2009; 39(5):835-50.
16. Arphorn S, Augsornpeug N, Srisorrachatr S,
Pruktharathikul V. Comprehension of safety
signs for construction workers: Comparison
of existing and newly designed signs. J Hum
Ergol 2003; 32(7):87-94.
17. NSC 2008. National Safety Council annual
report. National Safety Council.
Available from: http://www.nsc.org/news
18. Chan KL. Chan AHS. Understanding
industrial safety signs: implications for
occupational safety management. Industrial
Management & Data Systems 2011;
111(9):1481-510.
19. ISO 9186-1:2007. Graphic1al symbols-test
methods-part 1: Methods for testing
comprehensibility. International Organization
for Standardization, Geneva, Switzerland.
Available from:
http://www.iso.org/iso/catalogue_detail.htm?
csnumber=41090
20. America. American National Standards
Institute. American national standard for
Criteria for safety symbols. National
Electrical Manufacturers Association.
NewYork; 2011.
21. ISO 3864-2:2004. Graphical symbols-safety
colours and safety signs-part 2: Design
principles for product safety labels.
International Organization for
Standardization. Geneva, Switzerland.
Available from:
http://www.iso.org/iso/catalogue_detail.htm?
csnumber=31020.
Downloaded from johe.rums.ac.ir at 11:37 IRDT on Wednesday June 20th 2018 [ DOI: 10.18869/acadpub.johe.2.1.2.37 ]