ArticlePublisher preview available

Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: from humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

We report a study examining interspecies emotion transfer via body odors (chemosignals). Do human body odors (chemosignals) produced under emotional conditions of happiness and fear provide information that is detectable by pet dogs (Labrador and Golden retrievers)? The odor samples were collected from the axilla of male donors not involved in the main experiment. The experimental setup involved the co-presence of the dog’s owner, a stranger and the odor dispenser in a space where the dogs could move freely. There were three odor conditions [fear, happiness, and control (no sweat)] to which the dogs were assigned randomly. The dependent variables were the relevant behaviors of the dogs (e.g., approaching, interacting and gazing) directed to the three targets (owner, stranger, sweat dispenser) aside from the dogs’ stress and heart rate indicators. The results indicated with high accuracy that the dogs manifested the predicted behaviors in the three conditions. There were fewer and shorter owner directed behaviors and more stranger directed behaviors when they were in the “happy odor condition” compared to the fear odor and control conditions. In the fear odor condition, they displayed more stressful behaviors. The heart rate data in the control and happy conditions were significantly lower than in the fear condition. Our findings suggest that interspecies emotional communication is facilitated by chemosignals.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Anim Cogn (2018) 21:67–78
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1139-x
ORIGINAL PAPER
Interspecies transmission ofemotional information
viachemosignals: fromhumans todogs (Canis lupus familiaris)
BiagioD’Aniello1 · GünRefikSemin2· AlessandraAlterisio1· MassimoAria3·
AnnaScandurra1
Received: 10 July 2017 / Revised: 19 September 2017 / Accepted: 4 October 2017 / Published online: 7 October 2017
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017
Keywords Dogs· Human emotional smell· Interspecies
emotional transfer· Emotional communication· Dog’s
heart rate· Dog–human bond
Introduction
Body odors constitute chemical signals that have evolved
for species-specific communication (e.g., McClintock 2000;
Stevenson 2009; Wyatt 2015). Research has shown that in
humans, chemosignals can carry compound information
ranging from genetic relatedness (Jacob etal. 2002), gender
(Penn etal. 2007), to emotional states (e.g., de Groot etal.
2012; Mujica-Parodi etal. 2009; Prehn etal. 2006; Zhou and
Chen 2009; Mutic etal. 2015) and more (see de Groot etal.
2017). The transmission of olfactory information related to
emotional states occurs without the requirement of com-
municative intent (Semin and de Groot 2013) and is below
the threshold of consciousness (Pause 2012). Nevertheless,
such transmission induces in the receiver a partial affective,
behavioral, perceptual, and neural reproduction of the state
of the sender (Semin 2007). The question we addressed here
was about interspecies transmission of emotional informa-
tion. To this end, we employed an experimental paradigm
used in our previous research (e.g., de Groot etal. 2012),
whereby the signal was human body odor that was pro-
duced while the donors were experiencing experimentally
induced emotional states (i.e., happy, fear). The receivers of
the human chemosignals were pet dogs (Labrador retrievers
and Golden retrievers). Thus, the communication paradigm
we employed exposed pet dogs to chemosignals produced by
humans and analyzed the dogs’ reactions. In the following,
we provide an overview of the relevant research to date with
dogs and then outline the current study.
Abstract We report a study examining interspecies emo-
tion transfer via body odors (chemosignals). Do human
body odors (chemosignals) produced under emotional con-
ditions of happiness and fear provide information that is
detectable by pet dogs (Labrador and Golden retrievers)?
The odor samples were collected from the axilla of male
donors not involved in the main experiment. The experi-
mental setup involved the co-presence of the dog’s owner,
a stranger and the odor dispenser in a space where the dogs
could move freely. There were three odor conditions [fear,
happiness, and control (no sweat)] to which the dogs were
assigned randomly. The dependent variables were the rel-
evant behaviors of the dogs (e.g., approaching, interacting
and gazing) directed to the three targets (owner, stranger,
sweat dispenser) aside from the dogs’ stress and heart rate
indicators. The results indicated with high accuracy that the
dogs manifested the predicted behaviors in the three condi-
tions. There were fewer and shorter owner directed behav-
iors and more stranger directed behaviors when they were
in the “happy odor condition” compared to the fear odor and
control conditions. In the fear odor condition, they displayed
more stressful behaviors. The heart rate data in the control
and happy conditions were significantly lower than in the
fear condition. Our findings suggest that interspecies emo-
tional communication is facilitated by chemosignals.
* Biagio D’Aniello
biagio.daniello@unina.it
1 Department ofBiology, University ofNaples “Federico II”,
Via Cinthia, Naples80126, Italy
2 William James Center forResearch, ISPA - Instituto
Universitário, Lisbon, Portugal
3 Department ofEconomics andStatistics, University
ofNaples “Federico II”, Naples, Italy
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... Such results extend to other domestic species, as human scents of emotions were also assessed in horses (Jardat et al. 2023;Sabiniewicz et al. 2020), cattle (Destrez et al. 2021), cats (d'Ingeo et al. 2023, and laboratory mice (Destrez et al. 2021). Furthermore, emotional contagion seemed to occur in some of these animals: dogs (D'Aniello et al. 2018(D'Aniello et al. , 2021 and mice (Destrez et al. 2021) do indeed exhibit more stress-related behaviors when exposed to a human odor collected under conditions of fear or stress. ...
... Fig. 1 The odor exposure setting for the Habituation-Dishabituation procedure. The odor-dispensing bucket (a); the spatial design of the test pens set up for male (b) and female (c) lambs during the Habituation-Dishabituation test The cotton pads from several donors in the same condition were pooled to attenuate interindividual variability in axillary odors (D'Aniello et al. 2018;de Groot et al. 2015;Gomes et al. 2020;Kamiloğlu et al. 2018;Lanata et al. 2018;Silva et al. 2020). To that aim, each donor's cotton pads were cut into three 25.3 cm 2 pieces to be pooled with those of two other individuals of the same sex. ...
... Both emotion-inducing contexts occurred in morning time for a duration of two hours (10:00-12:00). The axillae sampling adopted procedures from conceptually similar investigations (e.g., Albrecht et al. 2011;D'Aniello et al. 2018;Destrez et al. 2021;Mujica-Parodi et al. 2009;Sabiniewicz et al. 2020). The day before and during axillary sweat sampling, the donors were instructed not to consume foods known to influence body odor (e.g., garlic, onion, leek, cabbage; e.g., Fialova et al., 2016;Schaal and Porter 1991) or to smoke. ...
Article
Full-text available
While sheep can detect and discriminate human emotions through visual and vocal cues, their reaction to human body odors remains unknown. The present study aimed to determine whether sheep (Ovis aries) can detect human odors, olfactorily discriminate stressed from non-stressed individuals, and behave accordingly based on the emotional valence of the odors. Axillary secretions from 34 students were collected following an oral examination (stress odor) or a regular class (non-stress odor). Fourteen female and 15 male lambs were then exposed to these odors through a habituation-dishabituation procedure. The habituation stimulus was presented four times for one minute, followed by the dishabituation stimulus presented once for one minute. Behavioral variables included spatiality relative to target odors, approach/withdrawal, ear positioning, sniffing, ingestion, and vocalization. Both female and male lambs more often positioned their ears backwards/forwards, and asymmetrically when exposed to the dishabituation stimulus, but regardless of their stress or non-stress value. They also changed their approach behavior when exposed to the dishabituation stimuli. Lambs displayed some behavioral signs of discrimination between the habituation and dishabituation odors, but regardless of their relation to stress or non-stress of human donors. In sum, this exploratory study suggests that young sheep respond negatively to the odor of unfamiliar humans, without showing any specific emotional contagion related to the stress odor. This exploratory study suggests young ovines can detect human body odor, a further step toward understanding the human-sheep relationship.
... In this case, it would be less about emotional contagion and fluidity than about responses and adaptation to behaviors and signals from handlers expressing irritation or tension. Results mentioning emotional fluidity and dogs' reaction to handlers' negative emotions confirm previous studies indicating dogs' abilities to recognize human emotions and perceive their valence (Merola & al., 2014;Albuquerque & al., 2016;D'Aniello & al., 2018;Katayama & al., 2019). However, it appears that dogs sensitive to their handler's emotions are not so in olfactory research and bite work. ...
Article
Full-text available
The human-dog relationships within the teams working in olfactory search within the French Armed Forces have not been investigated through an exploratory and qualitative approach. In order to expand knowledge on this professional and relational modality, semi-structured interviews were conducted with dog handlers from the French Army (n=16) performing explosive device detection and search and neutralization of individuals. Among the themes and sub-themes identified during thematic analysis, the relationships and interactions between humans and dogs emerge from canine individualities, emotions, and interpersonal bonds. The results suggest that the relationships woven between humans and dogs conducting olfactory searches in the French Army are multifaceted. The human-dog relationship in canine military teams is based on multiple characteristics such as bonds, emotions, affection, trust, communication, care, mutual support, technical learning, cooperation, interdependence, and instrumental factors. This study highlights the importance of valuing moments of closeness between the handler and the dog, outside of training and operational contexts. Furthermore, the opportunity to enhance the emotional skills of handlers is of interest in helping some to regulate their emotions, so as not to disrupt or even to assist their dog.
... Dogs' distinctive physiological and behavioural responses to human emotions is the focus of another line of research that investigates how emotional information may be transferred between humans and dogs. For instance, dogs reacted differently to 'chemosignals' in human sweat representing different emotional conditions (happiness vs. fear) (D'Aniello et al., 2018). Whereas they were more likely to approach a stranger in the happy odour condition, they sought out proximity with their caregivers more in the fearful condition. ...
... In particular, we expected that if dogs are capable of sympathy or empathy, they should exhibit helpful comforting behaviors, particularly in the sessions where the owners were induced with sad emotions. Thus, they should then stay close to the owner, approach and touch him (Meyers-Manor and Botten 2020; Souza et al. 2023;Quervel-Chaumette et al. 2016;D'Aniello et al. 2018). As this has not been tested before, it is unclear whether we could also expect better obedience then as a result of an increased desire to please when the owner is sad (see Bräuer 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Several recent studies have investigated how dogs perceive human emotional expressions. They have measured the reactions of dogs when exposed to stimuli presented in different modalities, such as photographs, audio recordings or odor samples, or to humans simulating various emotional situations. In the current study, dog owners were manipulated to genuinely experience emotions of happiness, sadness, and neutrality. We measured how dogs responded to their owners’ authentic emotions in two different natural situations: induction of the emotion through a video clip and training of a new task. Through a detailed analysis of dog behavior in these naturalistic settings, we investigated whether dogs show behavioral responses to genuine human emotions. We found that dogs behaved differently depending on the owner’s emotional state: they gazed and jumped less at owners when they were sad, and their compliance with the ‘sit’ command was also diminished. When owners were happy, dogs performed better in the trained task. These results are discussed in light of how dogs perceive human emotional expressions and the adaptive value of this skill.
... The field of medical detection by dogs (Edwards et al., 2017;Jendrny et al., 2021;Jezierski et al., 2015;Maughan et al., 2022;Reeve et al., 2021) is experiencing increased interest with the detection of epilepsy (Catala et al., 2019), malaria (Guest et al., 2019), and cancer (McCulloch et al., 2006;Pirrone & Albertini, 2017). For more than fifteen years, scientific research has been conducted on the ability of dogs to detect emotions and stress in humans (Calvi et al., 2020;D'Aniello et al., 2018;Wilson et al., 2022). These studies have led to the testing of dogs' ability to detect human stress-related volatile organic compounds (VOCs), as in the case of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Kiiroja et al., 2024). ...
Article
Studies on the relationships and interactions within human-dog teams in the French Army are scarce. In order to better understand this subject, a qualitative exploratory study was undertaken by conducting semi-structured interviews with dog handlers (n = 16). The interviewees work in the French Army. They are specialized in the detection and neutralization of individuals or explosives. Thematic analysis identified different themes: trust within the team, doubts, interspecific cohesion, and care provided to the dogs. The results highlight the importance of trust within the canine team. It is conceived as a dynamic, constructed process, based on experience, and involving both members of the team. It emerges that trust arises from individual, relational, technical, and experiential factors: the bonds formed, knowledge of one's dog, difficulties related to technical aspects, the reliability of the dog following training, and mission successes. Kindness, support, and mutual protection are also important criteria. A form of interspecific cohesion is envisaged by experienced handlers when affection, knowledge, reliability, mutual assistance, and support are present.
... Birds benefit from monogamy attachment style, this practice may not be beneficial to all pets. Further, there is evidence to suggest that affective state of the owner when an odor sample is collected alters dogs' behavioral responses to their owner's odor, suggesting that affective state of the owner at the time of wearing the article of clothing to be provided for comfort smelling may have an effect on the item's ability to provision comfort (D'Aniello et al. 2017). Finally, this practice may not be beneficial for all animal species. ...
Article
Full-text available
Olfactory information plays an important role in the attachment and bonding processes for both humans and non-human animals. Odor cues obtained via individual body odor facilitate attachment and bonding processes across species with regard to both mate selection and mother-infant bonding. The purpose of the present paper is to summarize the role of odor as it pertains to bond formation and maintenance in the mother-infant bond for human infants and non-human animal infants, and for mate selection among human adults and non-human animals. We then synthesize this summary with literature on attachment and existing evidence for the relationships between olfaction and attachment processes. Finally, we suggest avenues for areas of future research.
Article
This article will analyse human-dog relations in the post-apartheid White South African suburbs to show how they operate in the production of racial and gendered difference. The analysis draws primarily on my experience as a White woman growing up in the suburbs and my work as a dog walker, as well as interviews with the owners of two dog day-cares in Cape Town. Given this locatedness, the article tracks the affective and biopolitical effects of human-dog relationality to consider how they work in the socio-spatial structuring of the White suburbs. To begin, it uses Donna Haraway’s understanding of relation across difference in interspecies ‘becoming’, augmenting this with Harlan Weaver and Sarah Ahmed’s respective theorisations of the work of affect between bodies. It outlines White discourses of fear around crime and security and describes the spatial organisation of the suburb, which informs dogs’ socialisation/enculturation with White people, and their hostility towards Black people. As such, suburban dogs become part of a racialised species kinship, in which they are cast as White people’s companions, while protecting private property and White bodies. Drawing on Ahmed and Fanon’s work on phenomenology, the paper considers how dogs reproduce the historico-racial schema so that Black subjects are made to feel vulnerable in White space. Finally, it looks at gendered racialised narratives of threat and the construction of White women as objects of protection in relation to the imagined threat of Black men. By analysing these modes of relation, this paper hopes to show how interconnectedness yields an ethical responsibility towards others, across differences of race and species.
Article
Full-text available
In many studies that have investigated whether dogs' capacities to understand human pointing gestures are aspects of evolutionary or developmental social competences, family-owned dogs have been compared to shelter dogs. However, for most of these studies, the origins of shelter dogs were unknown. Some shelter dogs may have lived with families before entering shelters, and from these past experiences, they may have learned to understand human gestures. Furthermore, there is substantial variation in the methodology and analytic approaches used in such studies (e.g. different pointing protocols, different treatment of trials with no-choice response and indoor vs. outdoor experimental arenas). Such differences in methodologies and analysis techniques used make it difficult to compare results obtained from different studies and may account for the divergent results obtained. We thus attempted to control for several parameters by carrying out a test on dynamic proximal and distal pointing. We studied eleven kennel dogs of known origin that were born and raised in a kennels with limited human interaction. This group was compared to a group of eleven dogs comparable in terms of breed, sex and age that had lived with human families since they were puppies. Our results demonstrate that pet dogs outperform kennel dogs in their comprehension of proximal and distal pointing, regardless of whether trials where no-choice was made were considered as errors or were excluded from statistical analysis, meaning that dogs living in kennels do not understand pointing gestures. Even if genetic effects of the domestication process on human-dog relationships cannot be considered as negligible, our data suggest that dogs need to learn human pointing gestures and thus underscore the importance of ontogenetic processes.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional contagion, a basic component of empathy defined as emotional state-matching between individuals, has previously been shown in dogs even upon solely hearing negative emotional sounds of humans or conspecifics. The current investigation further sheds light on this phenomenon by directly contrasting emotional sounds of both species (humans and dogs) as well as opposed valences (positive and negative) to gain insights into intra- and interspecies empathy as well as differences between positively and negatively valenced sounds. Different types of sounds were played back to measure the influence of three dimensions on the dogs’ behavioural response. We found that dogs behaved differently after hearing non-emotional sounds of their environment compared to emotional sounds of humans and conspecifics (“Emotionality” dimension), but the subjects responded similarly to human and conspecific sounds (“Species” dimension). However, dogs expressed more freezing behaviour after conspecific sounds, independent of the valence. Comparing positively with negatively valenced sounds of both species (“Valence” dimension), we found that, independent of the species from which the sound originated, dogs expressed more behavioural indicators for arousal and negatively valenced states after hearing negative emotional sounds. This response pattern indicates emotional state-matching or emotional contagion for negative sounds of humans and conspecifics. It furthermore indicates that dogs recognized the different valences of the emotional sounds, which is a promising finding for future studies on empathy for positive emotional states in dogs.
Article
Full-text available
By 2.5 years of age humans are more skilful than other apes on a set of social, but not nonsocial, cognitive tasks. Individual differences in human infants, but not chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, are also explained by correlated variance in these cooperative communicative skills. Relative to nonhuman apes, domestic dogs, Canis familiaris, perform more like human infants in cooperative communicative tasks, but it is unknown whether dog and human cognition share a similar underlying structure. We tested 552 dogs in a large-scale test battery modelled after similar work with humans and nonhuman apes. Unlike chimpanzees, but similarly to humans, individual differences in dogs were explained by correlated variance in skills for solving cooperative communicative problems. Direct comparisons of data from all three species revealed similar patterns of individual differences in cooperative communication between human infants (N = 105) and domestic dogs (N = 430), which were not observed in chimpanzees (N = 106). Future research will be needed to examine whether the observed similarities are a result of similar psychological mechanisms and evolutionary processes in the dog and human lineages.
Chapter
Shows how an appreciation of the full complexity of the context of reproduction actually simplifies and clarifies our understanding of reproductive behavior. Historically, reproductive science has focused on reproductive behaviors divorced from the contexts in which they occur. Taking a more integrated view, this book explores the neuroendocrine bases of reproduction in relation to their environmental and social contexts. The contributors provide compelling accounts of reproductive behaviors in animals ranging from turtles and lizards to humans and nonhuman primates. They examine these behaviors from the perspectives of ethology, endocrinology, behavioral genetics, and evolutionary ecology. Together, they illuminate the dynamic interplay between the ecological and social contexts of a species and the biological mechanisms regulating reproductive behavior. The book shows how an appreciation of the full complexity of the context of reproduction actually simplifies and clarifies our understanding of reproductive behavior. Contributors Gregory F. Ball, George E. Bentley, Franklin H. Bronson, David Crews, Jeffrey A. French, Michael R. Gorman, Kay E. Holecamp, Jerry D. Jacobs, Sabra L. Klein, Theresa M. Lee, Donna L. Maney, Martha K. McClintock, Simone Meddle, Randy J. Nelson, Nicole Perfito, Emilie F. Rissman, Colleen M. Schafner, Patricia A. Schiml, Jill E. Schneider, Rae Silver, Ann-Judith Silverman, Laura Smale, Kira Soma, Jennifer M. Swann, Anthony D. Tramontin, George N. Wade, Kim Wallen, Scott R. Wersinger, John C. Wingfield, Ruth I. Wood Bradford Books imprint
Chapter
This book provides an up-to-date description of the behavioural biology of dogs. It is written for students of animal behaviour or veterinary medicine at advanced levels and dog owners. This book is divided into 4 parts and 14 chapters. The first part (chapters 1-3) focuses on the evolution and development of the dog. The second part (chapters 4-8) deals with the basic aspects of animal behaviour with particular emphasis on dogs. The third part (chapters 9-12) places the modern dog in its present ecological framework in the niche of human coexistence. A broad overview of the behavioural aspects of living close to humans is given. The fourth part (chapters 13 and 14) focuses on behavioural problems, their prevention and cure.
Article
Despite an increasing focus on the neural basis of human decision making in neuroscience, relatively little attention has been paid to decision making in social settings. Moreover, although human social decision making has been explored in a social psychology context, few neural explanations for the observed findings have been considered. To bridge this gap and improve models of human social decision making, we investigated whether acquiring a good reputation, which is an important incentive in human social behaviors, activates the same reward circuitry as monetary rewards. In total, 19 subjects participated in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments involving monetary and social rewards. The acquisition of one's good reputation robustly activated reward-related brain areas, notably the striatum, and these overlapped with the areas activated by monetary rewards. Our findings support the idea of a "common neural currency" for rewards and represent an important first step toward a neural explanation for complex human social behaviors.
Article
We assessed how highly trained dogs respond to gestural versus verbal signals when their handlers or an unfamiliar person asked them to perform an obedience task. Dogs were requested to perform four different actions (“Sit”, “Down”, “Stay” and “Come”) upon receiving congruent (only gestural or only verbal) or incongruent signals (gestural and verbal signals contradict each other). The dogs’ performance measures were the frequency of correct responses and their response latency. Generalized Estimation Equation models were used to determine whether the type of signal, the coherence of the signals and familiarity with the signaler influenced dogs’ responses. Our results show that the probability of dogs expressing the requested behaviour was lower when the stranger gave verbal signals, than in any of the other conditions. In the incongruent condition, the probability that dogs expressed the behaviour indicated by the verbal signal was lower for signals provided by the stranger than for signals provided by the owner. The reverse was observed for gestural signals. In general, longer latencies to perform the “Come”, “Down” and “Sit” behaviours were observed in response to the stranger’s verbal signals than when the stranger gave gestural or incongruent signals. Additionally, the response latency to the stranger’s verbal stimuli took longer than verbal stimuli were provided by the owner in the case of “Come” (P = 0.002) and “Sit” (P < 0.001) actions. Our data support the argument that for highly trained dogs, gestural signals are less dependent upon signal-giver familiarity, whereas verbal signals are less effective when they are given by an unfamiliar person.
Article
Reports of variability in the social behavior of the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris) are common across populations, breeds, and individuals. This has often been considered a challenge for characterizing the nature and origins of the domestic dog’s social cognition. Here, we propose that this variability might be explained by social plasticity, a trait that could contribute to the success of the domestic dog and facilitate the dog-human bond. Additional research specifically aimed at investigating population and individual variation in canine social behavior, such as attachment-style research, may provide important insight into domestic dogs’ biological success, as well as knowledge that could benefit both dogs and humans in a wide range of applied settings.
Article
Humans use multiple senses to navigate the social world. Among these, our sense of smell is arguably the most underestimated one. One intriguing function of the sense of smell is its social communicative function. Research has shown that human odors can convey information about a range of states (e.g., emotions, sickness) and traits (e.g., individuality, gender). Yet, what underlies the communicability of these states and traits via smell? We aim to fill this explanatory gap by furnishing a framework that highlights the dynamic and flexible aspects of human olfactory communication. In particular, we explain how multiple-message body odors, associative learning (i.e., the systematic co-occurrence of certain odorants with state- or trait-related information), and top-down contextual influences could interact to shape human odor perception. Not only does our model help to integrate past research on human olfactory communication, but it also opens new avenues for future research on this fascinating, yet to date poorly understood field.
Article
We assessed how water rescue dogs, which were equally accustomed to respond to gestural and verbal requests, weighted gestural versus verbal information when asked by their owner to perform an action. Dogs were asked to perform four different actions (“sit”, “lie down”, “stay”, “come”) providing them with a single source of information (in Phase 1, gestural, and in Phase 2, verbal) or with incongruent information (in Phase 3, gestural and verbal commands referred to two different actions). In Phases 1 and 2, we recorded the frequency of correct responses as 0 or 1, whereas in Phase 3, we computed a ‘preference index’ (percentage of gestural commands followed over the total commands responded). Results showed that dogs followed gestures significantly better than words when these two types of information were used separately. Females were more likely to respond to gestural than verbal commands and males responded to verbal commands significantly better than females. In the incongruent condition, when gestures and words simultaneously indicated two different actions, the dogs overall preferred to execute the action required by the gesture rather than that required verbally, except when the verbal command “come” was paired with the gestural command “stay” with the owner moving away from the dog. Our data suggest that in dogs accustomed to respond to both gestural and verbal requests, gestures are more salient than words. However, dogs’ responses appeared to be dependent also on the contextual situation: dogs’ motivation to maintain proximity with an owner who was moving away could have led them to make the more ‘convenient’ choices between the two incongruent instructions.