ArticlePDF Available

Trajectories of Infants’ Biobehavioral Development: Timing and Rate of A-Not-B Performance Gains and EEG Maturation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study examined how timing (i.e., relative maturity) and rate (i.e., how quickly infants attain proficiency) of A-not-B performance were related to changes in brain activity from age 6 to 12 months. A-not-B performance and resting EEG (electroencephalography) were measured monthly from age 6 to 12 months in 28 infants and were modeled using logistic and linear growth curve models. Infants with faster performance rates reached performance milestones earlier. Infants with faster rates of increase in A-not-B performance had lower occipital power at 6 months and greater linear increases in occipital power. The results underscore the importance of considering nonlinear change processes for studying infants’ cognitive development, as well as how these changes are related to trajectories of EEG power.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The title for this Special Section is The Developing Brain: Evidence for Plasticity
during Childhood and Adolescence, edited by Amanda E. Guyer, Koraly P
erez-
Edgar, and Eveline A. Crone
Trajectories of InfantsBiobehavioral Development: Timing and Rate of
A-Not-B Performance Gains and EEG Maturation
Leigha A. MacNeill , and Nilam Ram
The Pennsylvania State University
Martha Ann Bell
Virginia Tech
Nathan A. Fox
University of Maryland
Koraly P
erez-Edgar
The Pennsylvania State University
This study examined how timing (i.e., relative maturity) and rate (i.e., how quickly infants attain prociency)
of A-not-B performance were related to changes in brain activity from age 6 to 12 months. A-not-B perfor-
mance and resting EEG (electroencephalography) were measured monthly from age 6 to 12 months in 28
infants and were modeled using logistic and linear growth curve models. Infants with faster performance
rates reached performance milestones earlier. Infants with faster rates of increase in A-not-B performance had
lower occipital power at 6 months and greater linear increases in occipital power. The results underscore the
importance of considering nonlinear change processes for studying infantscognitive development as well as
how these changes are related to trajectories of EEG power.
Infants undergo substantial changes in cognitive
functioning in the second half of the rst year (Bell
& Fox, 1992; Cuevas & Bell, 2010; Diamond, 1985).
The A-not-B task, a classic measure of cognitive
development in infancy, was developed by Piaget
(1954) to assess 8- to 12-month-old infantsunder-
standing of object permanence (i.e., knowledge that
an object exists when it is no longer in sight).
Although most infants are capable of completing
the task by age 12 months, the timing and rate of
progression toward successful performance differ
among infants across the second half of the rst
year. Piagets model of qualitative stages in cogni-
tive development implies nonlinearity, in which the
development of childrens thinking is not additive
but undergoes substantial reorganization over time
(van Geert, 1998). Previous work indicates that task
performance is related to variation in brain struc-
ture and function. For example, rhesus monkeys
with bilateral ablations of the dorsolateral pre-
frontal cortex make the A-not-B error at varying
delays, whereas monkeys without these lesions do
not (Diamond, 1990). Prior research with human
infants suggests positive associations between fron-
tal electroencephalogram (EEG) power and object
permanence task performance (Bell & Fox, 1992).
These ndings, among others, illustrate that
advancements in childrens cognitive performance
may parallel concurrent changes in the brain (Fis-
cher & Van Geert, 2014).
Although there are individual differences in
infantsdevelopment of object permanence, particu-
larly when assessing the amount of delay necessary
to evoke the A-not-B error (Bell & Fox, 1992; Dia-
mond, 1985), we have not yet charted age-related
This study was supported by a grant from the National Insti-
tutes of Health (1R01HD026768) to Nathan Fox. The content of
this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not
necessarily represent the ofcial views of the National Institutes
of Health. We thank the many individuals who contributed to
data collection and processing. We especially thank the families
who participated in the study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Leigha A. MacNeill, The Pennsylvania State University, 142
Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802. Electronic mail may
be sent to lam526@psu.edu.
©2018 The Authors
Child Development ©2018 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2018/8903-0003
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13022
Child Development, May/June 2018, Volume 89, Number 3, Pages 711724
changes in A-not-B task performance as a nonlinear
developmental process. Moreover, questions still
remain concerning whether or how changes in EEG
power map onto childrens cognitive development.
The current study used linear growth models with
logistic model parameters as predictors to examine
the relation between timing (i.e., relative maturity)
and rate (i.e., how quickly infants attain pro-
ciency) of A-not-B performance gains from 6 to
12 months of age. In turn, we mapped performance
changes onto age-related changes in baseline EEG
power.
The A-Not-B Error
In 1954, Piaget published work on the develop-
ment of the object concept, or the understanding
that objects exist as unique entities outside of ones
own actions (Piaget, 1954). This skill is thought to
be obtained little by little across the six substages of
sensorimotor development. It was argued that chil-
dren do not conceptualize the object as having its
own pattern of movement that is both distinct from
the child and also predictable and logical until the
second year. Piaget argued that childrens attain-
ment of the object concept, or object permanence, is
essential groundwork for building knowledge of
the physical world. He believed that the intellectual
structures of the child change through stages and
that each of these stages is qualitatively different
from the stages that had come before (Flavell,
Miller, & Miller, 1993).
In the 60+years since these observations took
place, researchers have investigated the underlying
skills necessary to correctly display mastery of
object permanence. Arguably, one of the tasks most
often used to measure object permanence is Piagets
(1954) own A-not-B task. In this task, an attractive
object is hidden at one location (A) multiple times
in front of the infant until it is switched to another,
adjacent location (B). Furthermore, the experimenter
may impose a time delay between hiding the object
at Band allowing the infant to search. The A-not-B
error lies in the infant reaching toward A, despite
having seen the objects placement at B.
The skills necessary to perform the A-not-B task
successfully may extend beyond the simple acquisi-
tion of object permanence. Diamond (1985, 1990)
has demonstrated that as infants get older, the
length of delay between hiding the object and
allowing the child to search for the object must
increase for infants to make the perseverative error.
Thus, the A-not-B task is a marker of the infants
ability to remember previous events, coupled with
the ability to inhibit a prepotent motor response to
form a correct response. The data also imply that
the infants actions are governed by their intention,
as opposed to a developed habit. Diamond (1990)
later also proposed that both the integration of
recall memory and the inhibition of a prepotent
response are jointly necessary for solving the A-not-
B task.
Current conceptualizations contend that the task
can be interpreted as an early measure of childrens
executive function (Diamond, 2006). The delays
between hiding the object and allowing the infant
to search can be taxing on infantsmemories, thus
age-related change in A-not-B task performance
may be indicative of improvements in the capacity
and duration of working memory (Marcovitch &
Zelazo, 1999). Smith, Thelen, Titzer, and McLin
(1999) proposed that in order for the infant to cor-
rectly obtain the object at the second location,
infants need to be able to discriminate locations that
look similar as well as be able to reach for the
object. Building on the large perceptual changes
that co-occur with enhanced locomotor skills in the
second half of the rst year, infants are able to cou-
ple their looking and reaching abilities, such that
each discrimination and reach emerges from memo-
ries of previous discriminations and reaches (Smith
et al., 1999). The A-not-B error thus may not neces-
sarily represent a failure to obtain object perma-
nence insomuch as it demonstrates the difculty of
keeping an objects new location in mind when
exerting control over a motor response. Building on
the various interpretations of the constructs under-
lying the A-not-B task, we argue that the task taps
the intersection of infantsobject permanence, exec-
utive functioning, and working memory. Addition-
ally, we agree with the consensus that the task also
requires exibility in infant cognition and the
deployment of motor behavior (e.g., Smith et al.,
1999).
Given the number of skills required to success-
fully perform the A-not-B task at varying delays, it
comes as no surprise that there are both interindi-
vidual and intraindividual variability in infant per-
formance. Diamond (1985) found large between-
subject differences in the delay at which same-age
children made the A-not-B error through 12 months
of age. Additionally, she demonstrated that infants
persisted in making these errors until 12 months of
age with increases in delay. Other work has found
that performance on the A-not-B task is normally
distributed by 8 months of age, such that there was
a wide range of performance scores with a mode of
3 on a scale from 1 to 6 (Bell & Adams, 1999).
712 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
Building on this foundation, we aimed to examine
individual differences in A-not-B task performance
from 6 to 12 months of age using logistic parame-
ters to capture nonlinearities in this development.
The Role of Brain Development in A-Not-B Performance
Differences in individual task performance may
be associated with brain activity over this develop-
mental window (Bell, 2001, 2012; Bell & Fox, 1992,
1997; Cuevas & Bell, 2011; Cuevas, Bell, Marcovitch,
& Calkins, 2012). Fischer and Van Geert (2014)
argued that changes in the rapidly maturing brain
underlie the quick bursts and discontinuities in
behavior and the presumed knowledge base of the
developing infant. Early in development, neurons
that are relatively unused are pruned in order to
increase neural efciency (Huttenlocher & Dab-
holkar, 1997). With efciency also comes the coordi-
nation of neural systems involved in newly
emerging behaviors, as in mastering object perma-
nence and the related skills needed to overcome the
A-not-B error. Action systems, such as sustaining
multiple actions, although also inhibiting the prepo-
tent response, must be coordinated for successful
performance (Fischer & Van Geert, 2014). The pre-
frontal cortex plays a role in holding this type of
information in the moment while the individual
engages in other actions. Because the A-not-B task
involves the ability to attend to visual stimuli and
track moving objects in space, we may also expect
to see links to the development of the occipital
region. Occipital power has been positively related
to cognitive performance in infants (Bell & Fox,
1997), and visual processes are more sensitive to
the environment by the end of the rst year (Fox,
Kagan, & Weiskopf, 1979).
Much of the research on early brain development
has utilized EEG. EEG has the temporal resolution
to capture quick shifts in brain functioning that
may parallel the rapid cognitive processes that
unfold on the order of milliseconds (Bell, 1998).
EEG power values characterize excitability in a
cluster of neurons and are combined across individ-
ual 1 Hz frequency bins to indicate power measure-
ment in a specic frequency band. The dominant
frequency band during baseline recordings in
infancy is 69 Hz (Bell & Fox, 1992; Marshall, Bar-
Haim, & Fox, 2002), reecting alpha activity in
infancy (Stroganova & Orekhova, 2007). Increases
in EEG power in the alpha frequency band may
suggest extensive organization of neurons (Nunez,
1981) and more neural activity. This neural activity,
in turn, has been associated with increased
cognitive skill in infancy including inhibitory con-
trol (Orekhova, Stroganova, & Posikera, 2001) and
working memory (Cuevas et al., 2012). Linear
increases in baseline power over time are evident
across the scalp at frontal, parietal, and occipital
sites from 7 to 12 months of age in the 69 Hz fre-
quency band, with the greatest increase between 9
and 10 months (Bell & Fox, 1994).
Performance on working memory tasks is associ-
ated with increases in baseline EEG power. Bell and
Fox (1992) found that baseline EEG power
increased monthly in the 69 Hz range from 7 to
12 months of age, and changes in frontal power
were associated with greater delay tolerance on the
A-not-B task. Bell (2002) tested 8-month-old infants
on a spatial working memory task and assessed
EEG power in the 35 Hz, 69 Hz, and 1012 Hz
frequency bands both at baseline and during the
task. She found that the 69 Hz band yielded dif-
ferences in baseline and task power values, demon-
strating increased power during the cognitive task.
In addition, 8-month-old infants who successfully
completed the looking version of the A-not-B task
tolerating a 0-s delay or greater demonstrated
increases in frontal, parietal, and occipital power in
the 69 Hz frequency band from baseline to task
performance. However, there was no increase in
power for the children who did not complete the
task correctly (Bell, 2001). Finally, 8-month-old
infants who performed successfully on the A-not-B
task with a 0-s delay or greater demonstrated
greater medial frontal and occipital baseline EEG
power in the 69 Hz frequency band than infants
who did not successfully complete the task (Bell &
Fox, 1997). These results underscore the evidence
for scalp-wide increases in baseline power for
infants who successfully performed the task com-
pared to infants who did not.
To date, little research has longitudinally
assessed both A-not-B task performance and paral-
lel markers of neural development. Bell and Fox
(1992) examined A-not-B performance from 7 to
12 months (N=13) and collected EEG at rest at
each age. Infants who could tolerate a longer delay
on the A-not-B task at 12 months of age showed a
decrease between 7 and 8 months, and an increase
between 9 and 10 months in frontal EEG power.
The group with a shorter delay period only showed
an increase in frontal power between 10 and
11 months. Additionally, the two groups of infants
rst differed on task performance at 10 months of
age, which was also the point in time when the
long-delay group showed the largest increase in
frontal EEG power. These ndings suggest that
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 713
toward the end of the rst year, the relation
between growth in cortical organization and object
permanence ability becomes more established. The
Bell and Fox (1992) paper was one of the rst to
establish that there is interindividual variability in
A-not-B task performance. Although infants demon-
strate remarkable progress on both brain activity
and A-not-B task performance over the latter half
of the rst year, the literature has yet to chart the
relations across trajectories over time at the level of
the individual.
Timing and Rate of A-Not-B Performance Gains
If observed behavior and the brain are part of a
dynamic network of systems that co-occur and are
responsible for each others change (e.g., Fischer &
Van Geert, 2014), we must model development in
ways that best characterize how particular systems
change over time and how these systems change in
relation to one another. Adolph and Robinson
(2008) argued that the eld has been overly reliant
on examining development through differences
between a beginning time point and an end time
point, often as a single difference score. They con-
tended that developmental systems frameworks
should aim to explain processes of change and the
shape of the change across time. Additionally,
development does not unfold in steady increments.
Rather, it speeds up and slows down at different
points in time (Fischer & Rose, 1997; Thatcher,
1994). Research on infant development is often char-
acterized by spurts in behavior that could be due to
distinct developmental processes, such as verbal or
spatial skills and differences in brain activity.
New behaviors can emerge from changes in clusters
of discontinuities (Fischer & Rose, 1997). Around
8 months, for instance, multiple skills undergo
sharp increases, such as crawling ability (Adolph,
1997), vocal imitation (Petitto & Marentette, 1991),
and object search performance (Bell & Fox, 1994).
These dramatic shifts in behavior imply nonlineari-
ties in infant development. Although many aspects
of development are described as nonlinear and
dynamic, the empirical literature has tended toward
using methods that describe stability and linear
change.
Trajectories of cognitive growth can be portrayed
by timing, or how mature infants are in relation to
their same-age peers, and rate, or how quickly or
slowly infants gain prociency in cognitive perfor-
mance. Infants can be considered early, average,
and late maturers contingent upon their perfor-
mance relative to other infants. They are also
considered as quick, average, or slow maturers
based on the rate at which they progress from no
prociency to prociency on the task. The current
study focused on a time window (612 months)
that would likely encompass the rst emergence
and competence of A-not-B performance.
To our knowledge, A-not-B task performance in
infancy has not been examined using nonlinear
growth models. Although we know that successful
A-not-B performance increases over age (e.g., Bell &
Fox, 1992; Cuevas & Bell, 2010; Diamond, 1985), it
is likely that the shape of the trajectory is nonlinear
and more representative of a sigmoid, or S-
shaped, curve. There are known discontinuities in
the development of cognitive processing, in that
spurts of growth are clustered at specic points of
time in the rst year (Fischer & Rose, 1997). For
example, Munakata (1998) modeled infantsreaches
at both A and B. The percent of correct responses
for reaching at Awas at, between 90% and 100%.
The percent of correct responses for reaching at B,
however, revealed a sigmoid shape with upper and
lower asymptotes. Developmental models must
acknowledge potential patterns of nonlinearity in
infantsearly cognitive trajectories and ages at
which cognitive performance levels off. The current
study used nonlinear growth curve models, a rela-
tively novel method in the eld of infant cognition,
to account for nonlinearities that other studies have
implied but have not directly tested.
Growth models provide a framework for describ-
ing the timing and rate of this change (Grimm,
Ram, & Hamagami, 2011; McArdle, 2009; Ram &
Grimm, 2007). Following previous work using non-
linear growth models to examine between-person
differences in timing and rate of pubertal develop-
ment (Marceau, Ram, Houts, Grimm, & Susman,
2011), our work examines how between-person dif-
ferences in the timing and rate of infantsperfor-
mance on the A-not-B task are related to changes in
EEG power. Given that infantscapabilities change
rapidly over the latter half of the rst year and that
there is within-person variability in performance on
these tasks, the present study investigated the tim-
ing and rate of infantscognitive and brain growth
within this important developmental time window.
We used linear and nonlinear growth models to
relate timing and rate of A-not-B task performance
to change in EEG power from 6 to 12 months of
age. The age range was chosen based on Diamonds
(1985) work, which assessed A-not-B task develop-
ment every 2 weeks from 6 to 12 months of age.
She found that infants began reaching for the object
at 7.5 months and noted increases in delay
714 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
tolerance at the average rate of 2 s per month.
However, there were large individual differences in
the delay needed for the error at every age. Our
objectives were to (a) describe nonlinear growth tra-
jectories in the timing and rate of performance gain
in the A-not-B task and (b) examine the associations
between nonlinear performance change and linear
age-related changes in infantsEEG power across
the second half of the rst year. We hypothesized
that earlier timing and faster rate of A-not-B task
performance would be associated with higher initial
level of, and linear change in, EEG power.
Method
Given page limitations, an abbreviated discussion
of the methods is presented here with a full
description in Supporting Information.
Participants
Twenty-eight healthy 6-month-old infants (14
boys, 14 girls) and their parents were recruited
through mailing lists to participate in a longitudinal
study of infant development. The participants were
Caucasian from generally middle to upper-middle
class homes in the greater Washington DC
metropolitan area. Infants were born within
3 weeks of their anticipated due date, weighed at
least six pounds at birth, received no special medi-
cal intervention at birth, had no documented neuro-
logical problems, and both parents had at least a
high school education.
Procedures
Families came to the laboratory every month
from 6 to 12 months of age, each time within
5 days of their monthly birthday (total of 7 visits).
At each visit, brain electrical activity (EEG) was
recorded and the infant completed a series of cogni-
tive tasks, including the A-not-B task.
Electrophysiological Recording
Brain electrical activity was recorded during a 2-
min pretask baseline recording session at each visit.
During the recording, the infant watched the exper-
imenter spin balls in a bingo wheel, segmented into
10 s of spinning 1, 3, and 7 balls, each separated by
a 10-s pause. The sequence was then repeated.
Power values were calculated for each electrode site
for each stimulus condition. A composite of EEG
baseline power values were used in the current
analyses.
EEG recordings were taken using an Electro-Cap
(Electro-Cap, Inc., Eaton, OH) stretch cap (10/20
electrode system) from scalp electrodes, left and
right frontal, parietal, and occipital regions (F3, F4,
P3, P4, O1, and O2), as well as channels A1 and A2
located at the ears. All sites were referenced to Cz.
Impedances were kept below 5 KΩ. Electrooculo-
gram (EOG) data were recorded from the external
canthus to the supraorbit of one eye. All leads were
separately amplied by Grass Model 12A5 ampli-
ers as part of the Neuro-data acquisition system
(Grass Model 12-32; Natus Medical Incorporated,
Pleasonton, CA). The EEG data were digitized
online at a sampling rate of 512 Hz per second for
each channel. The EEG data were re-referenced to
an average reference conguration prior to editing.
The EEG data were edited for eye movement and
gross movement artifact by trained coders. On aver-
age, 50%59% of the EEG data were artifact free at
each of the measurement occasions. All analyses
used the artifact-free data only. Individual patterns
of available EEG data were not associated with any
study variables (ps>.08). The EEG data were ana-
lyzed using a discrete Fourier transform (DFT) with
a Hanning window of a 1-s width with 50% overlap.
The mean voltage was subtracted from each data
point to remove any power results due to DC offset
before DFT computation. Power in single Hz bands
was computed for frequencies between 1 and 12 Hz,
and power was expressed in mean square micro-
volts. Plots of spectral power indicated that the dom-
inant frequency in all leads at all ages was in the 6
9 Hz alpha band. Power values were log trans-
formed (ln). Based on missing data patterns across
sessions (see Supporting Information), data were
handled using maximum likelihood estimation.
A-Not-B Task With Delay
After the electrophysiological recording, infants
were assessed on the A-not-B task with delay,
which was modeled after the standard two-location
task. The AB apparatus was a cardboard box with
two wells, A and B (specics in Supporting Infor-
mation). The apparatus was positioned on the oor
in front of the infant, such that the center of the
box was at midline and the wells were within reach
of the infant. While the parent sat behind the infant,
the experimenter sat on the opposite side of the
apparatus facing the infant and parent.
The experimenter signaled the beginning of a
trial by holding up a toy to engage the infants
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 715
attention. Subsequently, the experimenter lifted the
cloth covering one of the wells (A or B), placed the
toy in that well and then covered the well again
with the cloth, completely obscuring any sight of
the toy. If attention was lost during the trial, the
experimenter gained the infants attention and hid
the toy again. A correct response was coded if the
child recovered the toy from side A. Also included
as a correct response was uncovering the correct
well but failing to reach for the toy and uncovering
both wells yet visually xating on the correct well
(see Supporting Information). Uncovering the
empty well, not reaching for the toy, and not xat-
ing on the well housing the toy were all deemed
incorrect responses.
The A-not-B task was scored by delay on a scale
from 0 to 4. A score of 0represented a failure to
complete the task at a 0-s delay. A score of 1rep-
resented competence at completing the task with a
0-s delay, where the infant was allowed to immedi-
ately search for the toy once it was hidden. Scores
of 2,”“3,and 4were given when the infant
completed the task with a 2-, 4-, or 6-s or greater
delay, respectively. During the delay period, the
experimenter broke the infants attention from the
task by clapping their hands together. The parent
held the infants hands to prevent reaching during
the hiding and delay periods. The delay period
began once the experimenter observed the infants
attention breaking from the second well. Two
observers coded A-not-B performance by videotape.
The principal investigator, the experimenter, and
the observer in the session reviewed these codes
independently to resolve any discrepancies.
Data Analysis
We viewed our analyses of this small and very
unique sample as semiexploratoryusing linear
and nonlinear growth models to extract meaningful
parameters from the rich and unique repeated mea-
sures data but not claiming or assessing model ts
as conrmatory evidence for statistical inference.
Interindividual Differences in the Development of A-
Not-B Task Performance
Our rst aim was to quantify interindividual dif-
ferences in the development of infantsA-not-B task
performance from age 6 to 12 months. The data had
some distinctive characteristics that suggested
description using a logistic growth function (Ram &
Grimm, 2007). By design, the A-not-B task imposes
clear lower and upper bounds on performance.
Theoretically, all children progress from a stage
when they are not able to perform the task at a 0-s
delay (performance =0) to a stage when they per-
form the task at a 6-s or greater delay (perfor-
mance =4). As such, we modeled the seven repeated
observations of each infants performance using for-
mula 1, noted in the Supporting Information. The
timing of change was the age at which an infant
would reach a performance score =2 (formally, the
inection point located halfway between the lower
and upper asymptotes). The rate (i.e., rate governing
change from the lower to the upper asymptote) was
also noted. Higher rate values indicate that the infant
is improving in A-not-B task performance more
quickly over time at the inection point, relative to
the other infants. Analytically, the logistic growth
model served as a measurement modelto derive
timing and rate scores (h
2i
and r
i
) that could then be
examined in relation to individual differences in the
development of EEG power. Linearity was accom-
modated in the measurement scale (04). Nonlinear-
ity was accommodated in development of cognitive
performance, given the previous research revealing
nonlinearities in infantscognitive development.
Models were t to each infants repeated measures
data by looping the nls function in R (R Core Team,
2013) with a range of starting values that facilitated
convergence for all N=28 cases.
Associations Between EEG Power and A-Not-B Task
Performance Over Time
Interindividual differences in the development of
EEG power were then modeled using a standard
linear growth model with covariates (Grimm, Ram,
& Estabrook, 2017). As noted in the Supporting
Information with formulas 2 through 4, the seven
repeated observations of EEG power were modeled,
separately for each electrode location while model-
ing the person-specic intercept and linear slope
coefcients. Of particular interest were the relations
of EEG power development with the timing (c
01
and c
11
) and rate (c
02
and c
12
) of A-not-B develop-
ment. Models were t to the repeated measures
data using the nlme package in R (Pinheiro, Bates,
DebRoy, Sarkar, & Core Team R, 2015) using maxi-
mum likelihood estimation, with incomplete data
(5%) treated using missing at random assumptions.
Results
See Table S1 for descriptive statistics for the core
measures. As expected, the mean level of A-not-B
716 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
performance increased with age (from M=0.00
at 6 months to M=3.32 at 12 months), with
peak interindividual differences in performance at
9 months (SD =1.41). In parallel, average EEG
power increased with age at all six electrode loca-
tions, but with no clear pattern of change in the
standard deviations.
Interindividual Differences in the Development of
A-Not-B Task Performance
In the logistic growth trajectories derived from
the logistic growth models (Figure 1), the model
(solid lines) provided a good description of the
repeated observations (dotted lines) of performance
(residual standard errors ranged from 0.73 to 9.29,
M=3.68, SD =2.04). The majority of infants were
unsuccessful on A-not-B performance at age
6 months (score =0), developed rapidly between
ages 7 and 11 months, and were able to complete
the task at a 6-s delay or longer (score =4) by age
12 months. The prototypical (average) infant
obtained a score of 2 (i.e., halfway between lower
and upper asymptotes) 3.68 months after the rst
observation (i.e., 6 +3.68 =9.68 months), at which
point infants were developing at an average instan-
taneous rate of 2.55 (SD =3.04). There were, of
course, substantial individual differences both in
timing and rate of development (SD
h2
=2.04;
SD
r
=3.04). Notably, interindividual differences in
timing and rate were correlated at 0.49, indicating
that infants growing faster reached the point of
inection earlier. A pseudo R-squared comparing
the variance of all residuals to the collection of
residuals from the individual models found
substantial variability around parameter estimates
(pseudo R
2
=.842).
Associations Between EEG Power and A-Not-B Task
Performance Over Time
Interindividual differences in timing and rate of
change in A-not-B task performance were examined
in relation to concurrent development of EEG
power at the six electrode locations using linear
growth models (Supporting Information, equations
24). We used a stricter criterion of p.01 given
the number of tests (Table S2). As expected, all
models indicated that EEG power increased, on
average, across the second half of the rst year. For
example, for O2, the prototypical infant had (ln)
power of c
00
=2.51 at the rst observation, with
power increasing at c
10
=.07 (SE =0.01, p<.001)
units per month across the seven measurements.
Associations between the interindividual differ-
ences in A-not-B performance derived from the
logistic growth model (timing and rate) and the
interindividual differences in the trajectories of
EEG power emerged for only occipital electrodes.
Individuals with faster rates of increase in A-not-B
performance (Figure 2E) had lower O1 EEG (ln)
power of O1 at age 6 months, c
02
=.08 (p=.01),
and marginally greater increases in O1 EEG power
across the study, c
12
=.01 (p=.08). In parallel (Fig-
ure 2F), individuals with faster rates of increase in
A-not-B performance had lower EEG (ln) power of
O2 at age 6 months, c
02
=.08 (p=.01), and mar-
ginally greater increases in O2 EEG (ln) power across
the study, c
12
=.01 (p=.09).
Discussion
This study is the rst to describe the timing and
rate of development of A-not-B performance using
a logistic growth function and to examine how dif-
ferences in the behavioral development of cognitive
performance are related to trajectories of brain
activity. The rst aim of the study was to identify
the timing and rate of A-not-B task performance
change, examining the sigmoid-shaped logistic
curve that traversed performance from a score of 0
to 4. As predicted, the logistic growth model pro-
vided a good depiction of repeated A-not-B perfor-
mance across the latter half of the rst year,
capturing the observation that the majority of
infants were unsuccessful at the task at 6 months,
rapidly increased in performance from 7 to 11 months,
and succeeded at a 6-s or greater delay by
12 months. This description is consistent with Pia-
gets theory of qualitative reorganization of chil-
drens thinking.
Past research has shown nonlinearities in A-not-
B performance, such as a surge in skill appearing
around 8 months of age (Bell & Fox, 1994), and
another between 10 and 12 months (Smith & The-
len, 2003). Other work has implied a sigmoid-
shaped trajectory of reaching at location Bacross
age in infancy (Munakata, 1998). However, previ-
ous research documented nonlinearities by reveal-
ing surges in performance at various ages, while
often modeling change in behavior as linear (Fis-
cher & Rose, 1997). The current study found that
A-not-B performance is indeed nonlinear, with at
lower and upper bounds at 6 and 12 months,
respectively, and a spike in performance between 7
and 11 months of age.
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 717
Figure 1. Predicted trajectories of nonlinear intraindividual change of A-not-B performance for N=28 children across seven assess-
ments. Solid lines show the predicted trajectories for each individual; dotted lines show the raw data for each individual. h=timing of
change; r=rate of change. [Color gure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
718 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
AF3 Predicted Trajectories BF4 Predicted Trajectories
CP3 Predicted Trajectories DP4 Predicted Trajectories
EO1 Predicted Trajectories FO2 Predicted Trajectories
High (+1SD) A-not-B Rate
Low (-1SD) A-not-B Rate
Figure 2. Panels A-F represent predicted trajectories of linear intraindividual change of electroencephalography (EEG) power for N=28
children across seven assessments. Bolded lines show the predicted trajectories for each individual; gray lines show the raw data for
each individual. [Color gure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 719
Regarding timing, infants reached the point of
inection on the A-not-B task on average at age
9.68 months. Nine months of age may be an impor-
tant time for coordinating multiple systems
involved in the A-not-B task, such as motor control,
attending to and tracking visual stimuli, inhibitory
control, and working memory. Previous work
found a sharp increase in performance at 8 months
of age (Bell & Fox, 1994) and that task performance
at 8 months is normally distributed (Bell & Adams,
1999). A second study with eight infants (Fox et al.,
1979) found that all 9-month-old infants were able
to complete the A-not-B task at a 3-s delay, whereas
by 10 months, all infants completed the task at a 7-
s delay. These ndings indicate that between 9 and
10 months, infants experience a rapid increase in
performance on the A-not-B task. It is, however,
important to note that the 7-s delay in Fox et al.
(1979) study is larger than the 2-s delay at the point
of inection in the current study. This difference
could be due to the disparity in sample size.
Although the greatest shift in behavior occurred at
around 9 months of age for both studies, this was
also the age when individual differences were
greatest, and many infants were tolerating delays of
6 s or greater.
The substantial interindividual differences in the
timing of task prociency gains are consistent with
previous work. Diamond (1985) and Bell and Fox
(1992) found large variability at 8 and 7 months of
age, respectively, in delay tolerance. Although pre-
vious work has found that A-not-B performance
increases monthly for at least 6 months (Bell & Fox,
1992; Cuevas & Bell, 2010; Diamond, 1985; Fox
et al., 1979), the present study is the rst to quan-
tify those increases with respect to a logistic func-
tion that accommodates the nonlinear age-related
changes in behavior across this time window.
Indeed, although the relative rate at the point of
inection was 2.55 on average, the range was from
0.25 to 13.04.
This study is the rst to identify signicant indi-
vidual differences in how quickly or slowly infants
attain prociency relative to their peers. Because
there was variation in both timing and rate A-not-B
task performance, we can assume that whereas the
sigmoid-shaped curve describes the infantsperfor-
mance gains across 612 months, the trajectories for
the 28 infants are all somewhat unique (see Fig-
ure 1). Furthermore, the negative correlation of tim-
ing and rate illustrates that earlier timing on the
task is related to faster rates of acquisition, such
that children who reach the halfway point in task
performance at younger ages are mostly the same
individuals who are making successful task gains
more quickly. These ndings suggest that, although
the latter half of the rst year is a time during
which there is substantial normative change, there
is already substantial idiosyncrasy in individual
development.
Our second aim was to examine the associations
between linear change in baseline EEG power and
nonlinear change in A-not-B performance. First, the
results demonstrated that for all electrode sites,
there was a linear increase in baseline EEG power
values across 612 months of age. These ndings
are consistent with previous work that has
described an overall linear increase in power at
frontal, parietal, and occipital sites across the rst
year (Bell & Fox, 1992; Cuevas & Bell, 2011). The
fact that the 69 Hz alpha band captured increases
in power validates the existing literature that this is
an appropriate frequency band for measuring brain
electrical activity during infancy (Marshall et al.,
2002). Previous work has suggested that increases
in baseline EEG power in the infancy are indicative
of emerging neural organization (Nunez, 1981). The
increase in baseline power at all six electrode sites
over time is suggestive of neural maturation and
myelination that support the coordination of higher
order systems (Fischer & Van Geert, 2014). In order
to capture the functional consequences of brain
activity, it is necessary to investigate how changes
in baseline EEG power are related to changes in
cognitive processing.
Interestingly, the current study found that infants
with a faster rate of increase in A-not-B perfor-
mance had lower baseline EEG power of O1 and
O2 at 6 months of age, and they had larger
increases in baseline power at these electrode loca-
tions across the 7-month period. Infants with higher
power at the rst time point (6 months of age)
tended to advance on the A-not-B task more slowly
over time. It may be that these infants were already
performing the A-not-B task more successfully with
longer delays and were thus ahead of infants with
lower levels of initial power. Infants with lower ini-
tial levels of baseline EEG power had relatively
more catching up to do, which required steeper (or
faster) rates of performance over time.
Specic to occipital power, previous work has
found that infants who could tolerate longer delays
in A-not-B task performance also demonstrated
greater baseline left hemisphere occipital power
over age (Bell & Fox, 1992). Additionally, infants
who did not complete the A-not-B task at 8 months
had signicantly lower baseline occipital power
values than children who could complete the task
720 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
at 0- or 2-s delays (Bell & Fox, 1997). The A-not-B
task is a spatial working memory task, which
requires attending to objects in space and tracking
moving objects. Infants show an increase in general
attentiveness to their environment by the end of the
rst year (Fox et al., 1979), which may provide fur-
ther support for the relations noted here. In other
words, infantsability to advance quickly on cogni-
tive tasks may be in part linked to their increasing
capability to attend to nonsocial visual events. Tim-
ing of A-not-B performance, on the other hand, was
not related to initial levels or change in baseline
occipital power. This suggests that the average
point of change in the curvature of the A-not-B tra-
jectory is not dependent on levels or changes in
occipital power.
Substantial changes in childrens cognitive ability
can be attributed to the coordination of multiple
brain and behavior systems (Fischer & Van Geert,
2014). It is somewhat surprising, then, that the fron-
tal sites were not signicantly associated with rates
of increase in A-not-B performance for our sample,
given previous literature suggesting that the pre-
frontal cortex plays an important role in infants
development of object permanence (Bell, 2001, 2012;
Bell & Fox, 1992, 1997; Cuevas et al., 2012). Occipi-
tal development may be more robust than frontal
development in the rst year. Past research sug-
gests that the occipital lobe typically develops ear-
lier in infancy, whereas frontal development is
particularly variable and develops at differing rates
in the latter half of the rst year (Johnson, 2001).
The stability of occipital development may con-
tribute to the signicant associations between
A-not-B task performance and occipital power
changes. However, we are hesitant to suggest that
changes in baseline occipital power are the only
neural correlates of performance on the A-not-B
task. Future research should replicate these analyses
with a larger sample size to generalize these nd-
ings and isolate the mechanism of A-not-B perfor-
mance associated with baseline occipital power.
In sum, the current ndings demonstrate that
faster improvement in A-not-B performance could
reect changes in the organization and excitability
of neurons in the occipital region. The use of associ-
ations between nonlinear cognitive performance
change and linear baseline EEG power change is an
innovative way of examining interrelated develop-
mental processes. However, increases in occipital
power need not cause increased cognitive
performance or vice versa. Additionally, it is only
assumed that scalp electrodes are indicative
of brain activity from specic cortical regions
underlying the electrodes, and we have not directly
tested whether changes in baseline EEG power are
reective of brain maturation. Although this work
does not measure brain maturation directly or EEG
power in-task, knowledge of these patterns of brain
activity in infants, in conjunction with cognitive
performance, opens up the possibility to predict,
and eventually understand, individual differences
in executive function over time.
This study is the rst to use growth curve mod-
eling to delineate trajectories of infantscognitive
growth in relation to their brain development. Bell
and Foxs (1992) study was pivotal in longitudinally
assessing change in baseline EEG power over time
for two groups of infants varying in A-not-B task
performance, but the longitudinal sample was smal-
ler (N=13), and sensitive analytic techniques could
not be applied. Conversely, Cuevas et al. (2012)
used a much larger sample (N=290) but fewer
time points (5 and 10 months) and traditional
regression analyses. We used nonlinear growth
curves of task performance to more accurately
describe the monthly change occurring for infants
(N=28) from 6 to 12 months of age. In this way,
we illustrate how contemporary analytic tools can
be used to assess, and reassess, classic developmen-
tal phenomena. Using nonlinear mixed-effects mod-
els allowed us to most effectively describe trends in
A-not-B development that support the discontinu-
ities central to Piagets theory of cognitive develop-
ment while also revealing how brain development
maps onto the rates at which children advance in
task attainment. Additionally, individual differences
in timing and rate of A-not-B performance were
shown in the sigmoid shape of the trajectories, per-
mitting a closer and more comprehensive under-
standing of how variable cognitive development is
in the rst year. It is important to mention that the
sigmoid shape of the A-not-B performance trajec-
tory is dependent on the sampling method
(Adolph, Robinson, Young, & Gill-Alvarez, 2008),
and more frequent assessments within the age win-
dow could have demonstrated linear development.
However, we found little change between 6 and
7 months, and noted that between 11 and
12 months performance levels off. Without the
addition of these beginning and end time points,
one might assume linear increases between these
missing time points. Moreover, individuals vastly
differed in their A-not-B performance (Figure 1),
and the inclusion of initial emergence through to
competence therefore allowed us to capture the full
developmental progression for most of the infants
in the study.
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 721
The current results should be viewed in the con-
text of study limitations. Primarily, the sample was
small for detecting interindividual differences in
intraindividual change. More participants will be
needed to generalize these ndings. Second, the
A-not-B task involved a well that was uncovered
then recovered again once the toy was placed
rather than having both wells covered simultane-
ously. The procedure we used may have primed
the infant to maintain attention on the correct well
(Diamond, Cruttenden, & Neiderman, 1994). Third,
infant behavior was not coded during the EEG
baseline session. Had there been information
regarding the time the infant spent attending to the
spinning wheel or amount of infant motor move-
ment, we may have been able to more closely tie
individual differences in occipital power to individ-
ual differences in visual attention. Fourth, EEG was
not recorded during the A-not-B task so we could
not capture any task-related variations in power.
Fifth, the mathematical equation used in the analysis
relied on the inection point to measure interindivid-
ual differences at a score of 2. As this score is in the
middle of the continuous scale from 0 to 4, its mean-
ing on the scale is somewhat unclear. Moving the
inection point to different locations on the growth
curve would likely reveal how rate changes across
the logistic function. The selection of the score of 2
was simply to have a common point, or a parame-
terized feature of the trajectory, on which to com-
pare individuals parsimoniously using the entire
measurement scale. Moreover, we assumed that the
measurement scale was linear and that cognitive
development was nonlinear. An alternative approach
would be to accommodate a nonlinear measurement
scale and linear development. Finally, the current
study did not assess outcome behaviors beyond the
age of 12 months, so the implications of these vary-
ing trajectories on later developmental processes and
outcomes remain unknown. Future research should
assess larger samples and extend measurement of
these constructs beyond the age of 12 months so that
we may better understand the role of early brain
maturation in the long-term development of cogni-
tive skills.
Nevertheless, this work provides evidence for
the relations between brain activity and cognitive
performance trajectories across the second half of
the rst year. By capturing the initial emergence of
a skill and tracing attainment from absence through
to advanced performance, this approach has the
potential to not only inform our understanding of
cognitive processing in infancy but also provide
theoretical and methodological foundations for
future investigations of emerging developmental
pathways. The contemporary analytic techniques
illustrated here provide an exciting opportunity to
empirically reassess our theoretical understanding
of core developmental functions and processes.
References
Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of
infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research
in Child Development,62(Serial No. 251). https://doi.
org/10.2307/1166199
Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2008). In defense of
change processes. Child Development,79, 16481653.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01215.x
Adolph, K. E., Robinson, S. R., Young, J. W., & Gill-
Alvarez, F. (2008). What is the shape of developmental
change? Psychological Review,115, 527543. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.3.527
Bell, M. A. (1998). The ontogeny of the EEG during
infancy and childhood: Implications for cognitive
development. In B. Garreau (Ed.), Neuroimaging in child
neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 97111). Berlin, Germany:
Springer-Verlag.
Bell, M. A. (2001). Brain electrical activity associated with
cognitive processing during a looking version of the A-
not-B task. Infancy,2, 311330. https://doi.org/10.
1207/S15327078IN0203_2
Bell, M. A. (2002). Power changes in infant EEG fre-
quency bands during a spatial working memory task.
Psychophysiology,39, 450458. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0048577201393174
Bell, M. A. (2012). A psychobiological perspective on
working memory performance at 8 months of age.
Child Development,83, 251265. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01684.x
Bell, M. A., & Adams, S. E. (1999). Comparable perfor-
mance on looking and reaching versions of the A-not-B
task at 8 months of age. Infant Behavior and Develop-
ment,22, 221235. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383
(99)00010-7
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations between
frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive develop-
ment during infancy. Child Development,63, 11421163.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01685.x
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1994). Brain development over
the rst year of life. In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer
(Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 314
345). New York, NY: Guilford.
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1997). Individual differences in
object permanence performance at 8 months: Locomo-
tor experience and brain electrical activity. Developmen-
tal Psychobiology,31, 287297.
Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2010). Developmental progres-
sion of looking and reaching performance on the A-
not-B task. Developmental Psychology,46, 13631371.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020185
722 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2011). EEG and ECG from 5 to
10 months of age: Developmental changes in baseline
activation and cognitive processing during a working
memory task. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
80, 119128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.
009
Cuevas, K., Bell, M. A., Marcovitch, S., & Calkins, S. D.
(2012). Electroencephalogram and heart rate measures
of working memory at 5 and 10 months. Developmental
Psychology,48, 907917. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026
448
Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the Ability to Use
Recall to Guide Action, as Indicated by InfantsPerfor-
mance on AB. Child Development,56, 868883. https://
doi.org/10.2307/1130099
Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases
of memory functions as indexed by the AB and
delayed response tasks in human infants and infant
monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
608, 267317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.
tb48900.x
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive
functions. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifes-
pan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 7095). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994).
AB with multiple wells: I. Why are multiple wells
sometimes easier than two wells? II. Memory or mem-
ory + inhibition? Developmental Psychology,30, 192205.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.192
Fischer, K. W., & Rose, S. P. (1997). Dynamic growth
cycles of brain and cognitive development. In R. W.
Thatcher, G. R. Lyon, J. Rumsey, & N. Krasnegor
(Eds.), Developmental neuroimaging: Mapping the develop-
ment of brain and behavior (pp. 263279). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Fischer, K. W., & Van Geert, P. (2014). Dynamic develop-
ment of brain and behavior. In P. C. M. Molenaar, R.
M. Lerner, & K. M. Newell (Eds.), Handbook of develop-
mental systems theory and methodology (pp. 287315).
New York, NY: Guilford.
Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (1993). Cogni-
tive development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Fox, N., Kagan, J., & Weiskopf, S. (1979). The growth of
memory during infancy. Genetic Psychology Monographs,
99,91130.
Grimm, K. J., Ram, N., & Estabrook, R. (2017). Growth
modeling: Structural equation and multilevel modeling
approaches. New York, NY: Guilford.
Grimm, K. J., Ram, N., & Hamagami, F. (2011). Nonlinear
growth curves in developmental research. Child Devel-
opment,82, 13571371. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2011.01630.x
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional
differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex.
Journal of Comparative Neurology,387, 167178.
Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in
humans. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience,2, 475483.
Marceau, K., Ram, N., Houts, R. M., Grimm, K. J., & Sus-
man, E. J. (2011). Individual differences in boysand
girlstiming and rate of puberty: Modeling develop-
ment with nonlinear growth models. Developmental
Psychology,47, 13891409. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0023838
Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error:
Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development,
70, 12971313. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.000
95
Marshall, P. J., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Devel-
opment of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age.
Clinical Neurophysiology,113, 11991208. https://doi.
org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00163-3
McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differ-
ences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual
Review of Psychology,60, 577605. https://doi.org/10.
1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612
Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implica-
tions for object permanence theories: A PDP model of
the AB task. Developmental Science,1, 161184.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00021
Nunez, P. (1981). Electrical elds of the brain. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., & Posikera, I. N.
(2001). Alpha activity as an index of cortical inhibi-
tion during sustained internally controlled attention
in infants. Clinical Neurophysiology,112, 740749.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00502-8
Petitto, L. A., & Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the
manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language.
Science,251, 1493.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., & Sarkar, D.; Core
Team R. (2015). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects
models. R Package version 3, 1120. Retrieved from
http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for
statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for
Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-
project.org/
Ram, N., & Grimm, K. (2007). Using simple and complex
growth models to articulate developmental change:
Matching theory to method. International Journal of
Behavioral Development,31, 303316. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0165025407077751
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a
dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,7, 343
348. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00156-6
Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999).
Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of
the A-not-B error. Psychological Review,106, 235260.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.235
Stroganova, T. A., & Orekhova, E. V. (2007). EEG and
infant states. In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-
related potentials (pp. 251287). New York, NY: Psychol-
ogy Press.
Trajectories of A-Not-B and EEG 723
Thatcher, R. W. (1994). Cyclic cortical reorganization: Ori-
gins of human cognitive development. In G. Dawson &
K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing
brain (pp. 232266). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
van Geert, P. (1998). A dynamic systems model of basic
developmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky, and
beyond. Psychological Review,105, 634677. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.634-677
Supporting Information
Additional supporting information may be found in
the online version of this article at the publishers
website:
Table S1. Descriptive Statistics for the Seven
Monthly Repeated Measurements of Behavior (A-
Not-B Performance) and Brain Activity (Electroen-
cephalography [EEG] Power)
Table S2. Results From the Linear Growth Mod-
els With Logistic Model Parameters as Predictors
Data S1. Method
724 MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, and P
erez-Edgar
... Yet, the majority of developmental research focuses on maturational changes in EEG activity in children above 3 years of age (e. g., Perone et al., 2018aPerone et al., , 2018bCellier et al., 2021), a time when higher-order self-regulatory skills typically develop (Garon et al., 2008;Hendry et al., 2016). One of the few studies that examined the developmental trajectories of baseline frontal EEG activity during the first year of life in relation to child self-regulation showed that maturational changes in frontal alpha activity were not related to variations in self-regulation in infants observed from the A-not-B task (MacNeill et al., 2018). However, different results were found in a study focusing on the preschool period. ...
... An important goal in developmental neuroscience is to disentangle the complex link between the emergence of cognitive functions and the underlying maturational changes in brain structure and function (Johnson, 2001). In recent years, great progress has been made in examining the developmental trajectories of frontal power underlying the emergence of self-regulation in infancy and early childhood (e.g., MacNeill et al., 2018;Whedon et al., 2020). However, as the majority of previous studies solely focuses on the alpha rhythm, to our knowledge, it is currently unknown how maturation of frontal theta power from infancy to early childhood is related to the emergence of self-regulation in the preschool period. ...
... Surprisingly, the current study found that frontal alpha and theta power in baseline EEG generally remained stable from infancy to preschool years. One explanation for these discrepant findings may be that studies on maturational changes in baseline frontal EEG power solely focused on infancy (MacNeill et al., 2018) or predominantly used non-age-adjusted frequency ranges (Whedon et al., 2020). The approach of using non-age-adjusted frequency ranges potentially overestimates alpha activity and underestimates theta activity in older children, given the increase in theta rhythm peak frequency towards about 6 Hz in preschoolers (Orekhova et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
There is increasing interest in examining the development of frontal EEG power in relation to self-regulation in early childhood. However, the majority of previous studies solely focuses on the brain’s alpha rhythm and little is known about the differences between young boys and girls. The aim of the current study was therefore to gain more insight into the neural mechanisms involved in the emergence of self-regulation. The sample consisted of 442 children and data were collected at approximately 5 months, 10 months, and around 3 years of age. Latent growth curve models indicated that,while the neurobiological foundations of self-regulation are established during infancy,it is the maturation of the frontal alpha rhythm that contributes to variations in both observed and parent-reported self-regulation. In addition, it appears that boys might have a greater reliance on external regulation than girls during early childhood, as evident by higher scores of girls on both measures of self-regulation. More insight into the role of external regulators in brain maturation can help to implement interventions aimed at establishing bottom-up self-regulatory skills early in life, in order to provide the necessary foundations for the emergence of top-down self-regulatory skills in the preschool period.
... As such, the assessment of EF in childhood, particularly in early infancy, should include consideration of attention as a construct and an ability that may underpin later EF development. Both attention and EF development are underpinned by maturation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and closely related regions [24][25][26]. ...
... While the PFC is an important neural underpinning of EF, it is a much broader system of connectivity both within the PFC and between the PFC and other cortical, subcortical, and limbic regions that subserve EF [26]. Although the PFC develops extensively in infancy [24,25], the broader EF network that centres around the PFC remains largely premature and characterised by minimal development during the first year of life [40,41]. While such brain maturation processes may be important to the emergence of NDCs, pre-emptive intervention approaches in the first year of life may not be able to use attention or EF markers to warrant targeted intervention. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this review was to evaluate attention and executive function performance in children with neurodevelopmental conditions across the first 5 years of life, compared to neurotypical peers. MEDLINE, EMBASE, and PsycINFO databases were searched until June 30, 2023, and studies comparing attention or executive function between children with (or at risk for) neurodevelopmental conditions and neurotypical (or low risk) peers, 0 to 5 years old, were included. Of the 4338 studies identified, 111 studies with 12292 participants were included in the meta-analysis. The qualitative analysis of brain development included 5 studies. Primary outcomes were the standardised mean difference (Hedges’ g) in attention and executive function between groups. Meta-regressions examined moderating effects of age, biological sex, diagnosis, and measure type. Children with neurodevelopmental conditions showed small delays in attention (n = 49 studies, k = 251 outcomes, g = 0.36, 95% CI 0.23-0.48, p < 0.001) and moderate delays in executive function (n = 64 studies, k = 368 outcomes, g = 0.64,95% CI 0.53–0.76, p < 0.001). Attention and executive function delays could not be identified in the first year (equivalence tests, p < 0.001), small to moderate delays were found in toddlerhood and moderate delays by preschool. Delays identified were largely transdiagnostic, although there was some evidence of diagnosis-specific delays for attention and moderation by measure type (informant rating vs performance-based vs physiological). Qualitative analysis described how delays were underpinned by a divergence of brain development in medial prefrontal regions. These findings highlight the potential of using attention and executive measures to detect delay and to intervene in neurodevelopmental conditions early in life.
... Also, a peak of activity in the infant alpha range emerges around the fourth month of life. Alpha peak appears as a sudden energy "bump" between 5 and 7 Hz, moves toward higher frequencies, and augments its relative power with age during the first years of life (Clarke et al., 2001;Gasser et al., 1988;MacNeill et al., 2018;Orekhova et al., 2006;Stroganova et al., 1999;Whedon et al., 2020). Furthermore, the alpha band relative power appears to show withinindividual stability along infancy (Marshall et al., 2002). ...
... Finally, although research on higher frequency bands in infants is still scarce, a study by Tierney et al. (2012) suggests a reduction in frontal beta and gamma between the fifth month and the second year of life. Importantly, the relative power in different frequency bands appears to be related to individual differences in cognitive development (Anderson & Perone, 2018;Bell & Wolfe, 2007;Benasich et al., 2008;MacNeill et al., 2018;Perone et al., 2018;Whedon et al., 2020) as well as infants' risk of neurodevelopmental disorders (Arns et al., 2013;Begum-Ali et al., 2022;Gabard-Durnam et al., 2019;Tierney et al., 2012), which speaks for the relevance of understanding the early development of EEG activity at rest. ...
Article
Full-text available
Brain function rapidly changes in the first 2 years of life. In the last decades, resting-state EEG has been widely used to explore those changes. Previous studies have focused on the relative power of the signal in established frequency bands (i.e., theta, alpha, and beta). However, EEG power is a mixture of a 1/f-like background power (aperiodic) in combination with narrow peaks that appear over that curve (periodic activity, e.g., alpha peak). Therefore, it is possible that relative power captures both, aperiodic and periodic brain activity, contributing to changes in electrophysiological activity observed in infancy. For this reason, we explored the early developmental trajectory of the relative power in theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands from infancy to toddlerhood and compared it with changes in periodic activity in a longitudinal study with three waves at age 6, 9, and 16 to 18 months. Finally, we tested the contribution of periodic activity and aperiodic components of the EEG to age changes in relative power. We found that relative power and periodic activity trajectories differed in this period in all the frequency bands but alpha. Furthermore, aperiodic EEG activity flattened between 6 and 18 months. More importantly, only alpha relative power was exclusively related to periodic activity, whereas aperiodic components of the signal significantly contributed to the relative power of activity in theta and beta bands. Thus, relative power in these frequencies is influenced by developmental changes of the aperiodic activity, which should be considered for future studies.
... Apart from its use in clinical medicine and psychology, this technology has also found applications in various other fields 30 . This study utilized the high temporal resolution characteristics of EEG to capture short-term cognitive events during the experiment and analyze brain waves (alpha and beta waves) 31 . ...
Article
Full-text available
With the advancement of society, there has been an increasing focus on the health status of college students, with plants playing a significant role in the campus environment. Investigating the impact of the aromatic scent of plants on the physical and mental well-being of college students is essential. This study centered on the common indoor potted flower Gardenia jasminoides Ellis to delve into its aroma and the influence of its volatiles on the physical, mental, and emotional recovery of college students. The findings revealed that the subjects in both the olfactory group (G1) and the control group (G2) exhibited a decrease in blood pressure, pulse, β wave power, and skin electrical signal, alongside an increase in α wave power and heart rate variability (HRV) index. Notably, G1 saw a rise in α wave power by 0.17 µV²/Hz and G2 experienced an increase in HRV index by 0.184, while the power of β wave decreased by 2.589 and 0.01 µV²/Hz, respectively. Moreover, psychological indicators showed a significant increase in ‘energy’ and ‘self’ scores for both G1 and G2. Additionally, the perception of gardenia odor by college students was found to be associated with various physiological and psychological indicators in the experiment. The study highlighted that the volatiles of Gardenia jasminoides Ellis are rich in terpenes and alcohols, with terpenes playing a role in blood pressure regulation and relaxation, while alcohols like linalool contribute to air freshness, nervous system regulation, and sedative effects. The findings of our research offer backing for utilizing aromatic plants with terpenes and alcohols, such as gardenia, to enhance their health-promoting properties.
... These new experiences and tools may lead to the developmental change that we have traditionally captured at the mean level. For example, infants tackled a Piagetian A-not-B task at monthly intervals (MacNeill et al., 2018). We saw a great deal of variation between infants, ...
Article
Temperament traits are early appearing and relatively stable phenotypic profiles of behavior that are present across space and time. This definition invariably reflects the timescale imposed when gathering repeated measures of our variables of interest and our reliance on aggregate, mean-level values. However, if the time scale of observations is shortened, and the frequency of observations increased, underlying or latent fluctuations and variability may emerge. Embedding short-term fluctuations into slower developmental trajectories may improve our understanding of behavior in the moment, while also strengthening prediction. Researchers should embrace a more granular time scale in research, incorporating new technology and analytical approaches, enhancing our ability to capture developmental change. This review illustrates with the temperament literature how shifting time scales can provide new insight into social, behavioral, and cognitive processes across development.
... The sequential touching task (Ellis & Oakes, 2006) has been adapted as an infant CF measure to assess flexible attention set-shifting from 12 months of age, particularly when the shift is scaffolded by a social partner (Fig. 2a). Piaget's A-not-B task, a classic test of infant cognitive development, requires basic shifting, memory and inhibitory control skills, and children still make errors on this task until ∼12 months of age (MacNeill, Ram, Bell, Fox, & Pérez-Edgar, 2018). Together with other core EF skills, CF development accelerates during the preschool years (Hughes, 1998) and is only thought to reach maturity during late childhood or early adolescence (Kupis & Uddin, 2023). ...
... Allowing for 10% attrition, and based on estimated effect sizes from prior studies (which revealed moderate to large effects when examining early markers of social and executive functioning, and risk for autism [125][126][127];), a sample size of N = 90 will yield power of 0.97 (f 2 = 0.30, α = 0.01). While this sample size is larger than that suggested by an a priori power analysis (N = 71, based f 2 = 0.30, α = 0.01, 1-ß = 0.90), this will enable additional exploratory analyses to determine broader social-emotional, general developmental and epidermal predictors of social and executive function as well as the development of autism. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Delays in early social and executive function are predictive of later developmental delays and eventual neurodevelopmental diagnoses. There is limited research examining such markers in the first year of life. High-risk infant groups commonly present with a range of neurodevelopmental challenges, including social and executive function delays, and show higher rates of autism diagnoses later in life. For example, it has been estimated that up to 30% of infants diagnosed with cerebral palsy (CP) will go on to be diagnosed with autism later in life. Methods This article presents a protocol of a prospective longitudinal study. The primary aim of this study is to identify early life markers of delay in social and executive function in high-risk infants at the earliest point in time, and to explore how these markers may relate to the increased risk for social and executive delay, and risk of autism, later in life. High-risk infants will include Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) graduates, who are most commonly admitted for premature birth and/or cardiovascular problems. In addition, we will include infants with, or at risk for, CP. This prospective study will recruit 100 high-risk infants at the age of 3–12 months old and will track social and executive function across the first 2 years of their life, when infants are 3–7, 8–12, 18 and 24 months old. A multi-modal approach will be adopted by tracking the early development of social and executive function using behavioural, neurobiological, and caregiver-reported everyday functioning markers. Data will be analysed to assess the relationship between the early markers, measured from as early as 3–7 months of age, and the social and executive function as well as the autism outcomes measured at 24 months. Discussion This study has the potential to promote the earliest detection and intervention opportunities for social and executive function difficulties as well as risk for autism in NICU graduates and/or infants with, or at risk for, CP. The findings of this study will also expand our understanding of the early emergence of autism across a wider range of at-risk groups.
... While the internal components of the EF construct remain under debate, EF is typically defined as the set of high-order cognitive abilities that supports goal-oriented behavior (Diamond, 2013), including working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control (e.g., Blair, 2002;Buss & Spencer, 2014;Diamond, 2013;Miyake et al., 2000). The theoretical question of what processes underlie EF is also a difficulty for developmental studies of EF during the first years of life, despite the evidence that shows the existence of rudimentary inhibitory control and working memory skills in infants as young as 5 and 6 months of age (Cuevas et al., 2012;Holmboe et al., 2018;MacNeill et al., 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Executive functions (EFs) relate to a range of important outcomes across the lifespan, yet relatively little is known about the earliest emergence of these skills in infancy and toddlerhood. This chapter provides an overview of the measurement, development, and neural correlates of Early EFs. First, we review current empirical and theoretical frameworks for understanding Early EFs. Then we review key studies to examine how Early EFs have been measured, and what these findings are beginning to reveal about the developmental trajectories of EFs. We then review the neural correlates of Early EFs, before offering some recommendations for how to continue to expand knowledge in this field, with an emphasis on task development.
Article
Full-text available
The current study examined the relation between infant sustained attention and infant EEG oscillations. Fifty-nine infants were tested at 6 (N = 15), 8 (N = 17), 10 (N = 14), and 12 (N = 13) months. Three attention phases, stimulus orienting, sustained attention, and attention termination, were defined based on infants' heart rate changes. Frequency analysis using simultaneously recorded EEG focused on infant theta (2-6 Hz), alpha (6-9 Hz), and beta (9-14 Hz) rhythms. Cortical source analysis of EEG oscillations was conducted with realistic infant MRI models. Theta synchronization was found over fontal pole, temporal, and parietal electrodes during infant sustained attention for 10 and 12 months. Alpha desynchronization was found over frontal, central and parietal electrodes during sustained attention. This alpha effect started to emerge at 10 months and became well established by 12 months. No difference was found for the beta rhythm between different attention phases. The theta synchronization effect was localized to the orbital frontal, temporal pole, and ventral temporal areas. The alpha desynchronization effect was localized to the brain regions composing the default mode network including the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, medial prefrontal cortex, and inferior parietal gyrus. The alpha desynchronization effect was also localized to the pre- and post-central gyri. The present study demonstrates a connection between infant sustained attention and EEG oscillatory activities.
Data
Chapter Data, Program Inputs and Outputs for all LGM Examples in the textbook "An Introduction to Latent Variable Growth Curve Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications, Second Edition". Model specifications are included providing program syntax for Amos, EQS, LISREL, and Mplus software programs. The files are arranged by chapter and include syntax, data, and output files for all examples a particular software program is capable of estimating. The first three chapters (specification of the LGM, LGM and repeated measures ANOVA, and multivariate representations of growth and development) cover the development of the LGM. These are followed by three chapters involving multiple group issues and extensions (analyzing growth in multiple populations, accelerated designs, and multilevel longitudinal approaches), and followed by the chapter on growth mixture modeling, which addresses multiple-group issues from a latent class perspective. The remainder of the book covers 'special topics' (chapters on interrupted time series approaches to LGM analyses, growth modeling with ordered categorical outcomes, Missing data models, a latent variable framework for LGM power analyses and Monte Carlo estimation, and latent growth interaction models). The zipfile is quite large (1MB) since it contains all files for the various software programs.
Book
This book creates a bridge across cognitive development and cognitive aging. Pairs of researchers study the rise and fall of specific cognitive functions, such as attention, executive functioning, memory, working memory, representations, language, problem solving, intelligence, and individual differences to find ways in which the study of development and decline converge on common processes and mechanisms. The chapters are framed by an introduction that sets out the problems to be discussed and a conclusion that extracts the common themes and speculates on the implications for theory building. The book offers a lifespan approach to cognition by experts in the individual facts of cognitive functioning from either the developmental or the aging perspective. © 2006 by Ellen Bialystok, Fergus I.M. Craik. All rights reserved.
Chapter
Berger’s initial publication (1929) on his recordings of electrical activity from the human scalp became the impetus for research describing the rhythmic nature of the EEG and its association with behavioural state and mental activity. It was not until Berger’s (1932) report of EEG differences between children and adults, however, that interest in the normal developmental course of he electroencephalogram was generated. This work immediately spawned longitudinal studies of EEG development during infancy and childhood (Henry, 1944; Lindsley, 1939; Smith 1938a; b). The value of these initial developmental studies was their pictoral account of the appearance and changing nature of typical scalp electrical activity recorded during the first few years of life. As technology made EEG recording techniques more feasible, there was intensive research on visual analysis of the infant EEG during periods of sleep and alertness (Ellingson, 1958; Samson-Dollfus et al., 1964) and on the EEG in premature infants and neonates (Dreyfus-Brisac, 1964). These standards for visual analysis are still used by clinicians today in the assessment of diagnostic EEG recordings (Tharp, 1990).