Conference PaperPDF Available

Executive Functions and Personality Features: a Circular Interpretative Paradigm

Authors:

Abstract

The study describes the relationship between personality traits and executive functions within a circularly based paradigm.
Executive Functions and Personality Features:
a Circular Interpretative Paradigm
Raffaele Sperandeo
SiPGI Postgraduate School of Integrated Gestalt
Psychotherapy, D.M. 12.10.2007
Torre Annunziata, Naples, Italy
raffaele.sperandeo@gmail.com
Enrico Moretto
SiPGI Postgraduate School of Integrated Gestalt
Psychotherapy, D.M. 12.10.2007
Torre Annunziata, Naples, Italy
enrico.more@gmai.com
Gesualda Baldo
SiPGI Postgraduate School of Integrated Gestalt
Psychotherapy, D.M. 12.10.2007
Torre Annunziata, Naples, Italy
gesualda.baldo@libero.it
Silvia dell’Orco
Department of Human Science
Università della Basilicata, Italy
silvia.dellorco@unibas.it
Mauro Maldonato
Department of Human Science
Università della Basilicata, Italy
mauro.maldonato@unibas.it
Abstract— The study describes the relationship between
personality traits and executive functions within a circularly based
paradigm.
Keywords— Executive Functions; Personality Features;
I. I
NTRODUCTION
From the first studies on subjects with frontal lobe injuries,
EF were seen as the product of the activity of these brain areas
and, as such, were described as complex functions of
adaptation to the environment by emphasizing the relationship
between them and personality [1].
The EF system is basically responsible in dealing with new
tasks, interrupting inadequate automatic responses, comparing
different behavioral options, choosing the option which is most
suitable for the immediate context. The EF also begin
performing the selected program, focusing on it until it is
completed, monitoring and correcting where necessary during
the performance. [2] The cortical areas involved in executive
control are accessible to conscious awareness and are
characterized by high levels of plasticity. They can evolve and
change throughout the entire lifetime by integrating new
sensory-motor and relational information and generating
continuously innovative plans, thoughts and behaviors. [3]
Considered from an anatomical neurological profile, the close
interaction between prefrontal areas, pre-motor areas, the
parietal cortex and the angular gyrus is of great importance.
These connections suggest that the executive system is a polar
modal network (sensory, motor and relational) that is
responsible for the production of complex, goal-oriented
behaviors. [4, 5] From this point of view, the executive
functions and the underlying neuronal network appear to
overlap the concept of personality which, in its adaptive
expressions, can be described as the creative Skill of an
individual to adapt to the environment and to interact in
relational social contexts. [6]
Modern EF studies, based on the paradigm of cognitive
neuroscience, however, have focused on 6 neuropsychological
functions, considering them only in a cognitive context. Only
some studies focused on personality disorders (PD) by
providing a hierarchical interpretation of some of the
symptoms of PDs that are derived from EF defects [7].
From the beginning of the last century, Luria has proposed
considering an organization of EF in complex neural networks
that include the affective and motor cerebral areas [8].
Numerous recent opinions that have supported this model are
paving the way for an ecological approach in studying higher
cognitive functions [9].
Research regarding the relationship between EF and
personality traits is currently focused on the attempt to explain
some of the pathological character traits as a consequence of
frontal function deficits. [10] Without denying the clinical
value of studies that link trauma, executive deficits, and
personality disorders in a linear causal chain, it is fundamental
to integrate this classical model with circular causative patterns
that best fit clinical data and empirical evidence.
8th IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom 2017) • September 11-14, 2017 • Debrecen, Hungary
978-1-5386-1264-4/17/$31.00 ©2017 IEEE
000063
This study is part of the effective computing (AC) alveo
(range) Research considered from this perspective is proposed
by R. Picard at the MIT in the late 1990s; its goal is to consent
machines to perceive and show (present) human qualities [11].
An objective of AC systems is to simulate the functioning of
the personality as an affective process based on the inter-
human interaction of psychological and emotional states [12].
Our study intends to identify character styles as affective
processes emerging at the intermediate level of experience in a
circular function between environmental experience and its
enacted cognitive processing (aimed at action). Here, this
function is described as the effect that the circularity of the
environment-intermediate stage-action has on the genesis of
the character traits of an individual.
II. M
ATERIALS AND METHODS
Whenever a situation requires a choice, decision-making is
run by a fronto-parietal motor cognitive network that is our
brain thinking. Items of a personality questionnaire that
require the subject to say whether or not they adhere to a
certain behavior or thought trigger the activation of the
neuronal network involved in the actual execution of the
described behavior. The activation of this network gives rise
to the formulation of a concept characterized by a certain
degree of abstraction or abstractness but incarnate in the
ideomotor processes of the subject that responds to the
questionnaire. Some items of a personality questionnaire
describe psychic and behavioral processes in which the
Functions are involved. In this regard, the study of individual
items of a questionnaire represents a way to correlate
personality to the functions of the frontal lobes more
ecologically and effectively than the traits and the dimensions
of temperament and character that are constructed with
artificial statistical methods.
In a sample of 131 outpatients (55 male and 76 female)
signed in to a mental health clinic in 2015 and 2016, executive
functions were assessed through the Frontal Assessment
Battery (FAB) and the traits of temperament and character
using the Temperament and Character Inventory TCI).
The FAB is a short instrument that extensively evaluates
frontal functions. It consists of 6 sub-tests, each of which
explores frontal lobe correlated functions in line with the
current view of the EF [13]. These are:
• Conceptualization (explored through a similarity test);
• Mental flexibility (explored with a phonemic fluency
test);
• Motor programming (explored through the Luria series);
• Susceptibility to interference (assessed by testing with
Conflicting instructions);
• The Inhibitory Control (assessed with a go-no-go test);
• Autonomy from the environment (assessed by grasping
behavior).
The overall score of FAB describes the global efficiency of
EF. The subjects that do not achieve the cut of the value of 13,
show a defective activity of prefrontal areas [14] .
The TCI is a self-administered questionnaire of 240 items
that evaluates four dimensions of temperament and three
dimensions of character with different subscales for each
dimension. The latest version of the TCI (TCI R) differs from
the previous one because the true / false answers were replaced
by a Likert 5-point scale from absolute false to absolutely true.
The epta-factorial structure foreseen by Cloninger has been
substantially confirmed, as was confirmed the predictive
validity of the instrument with regard to personality disorders.
Although the small sample size examined does not allow
the generalization of the results, it is evident that the two
instruments TCI and TCI R can be overlapped from a
psychometric point of view even with regard to the poor
internal consistency of the subscales. Furthermore, in the case
of the TCI R, a satisfactory set of data is still not available. For
these reasons, we opted to use the first version of the TCI, and
within it only the 240 descriptive items in order to attribute
them ecological value as described in the methodological
paragraph of this paper [15].
III. D
ATA
A
NALYSIS
The scores obtained in the 6 FAB executive tasks (ET)
were related to TCI items. Positive or negative correlated
items with each specific ET were evaluated for their
homogeneity by analyzing both their internal consistency with
the Crombach alpha test and their clinical homogeneity.
Each of Items' sets was found homogeneous with respect
to the personality features the test (Crombach’s) describes.
The sum of the Items scores in each of the homogeneous sets
was used in a logistic regression test as an explanatory
variable while the efficiency of the related executive task was
used as a dependent variable.
IV. S
AMPLE
A sample of 131 subjects that had requested psychotherapy
treatment for non-clinical psychosocial issues.
Patients who showed diagnosis of personality disorders or a
diagnostic history of affective, anxiety and psychotic disorders
were excluded from the study. Patients with cognitive
impairment, neurodegenerative diseases and history of head
trauma were also excluded. All subjects were informed and
accepted that the data collected for the clinical evaluation
would be used in scientific studies according to the
confidentiality of sensitive information and the assurance that
the use of the data would not modify the prescribed treatment.
The subjects of the sample had a mean age of 33.91 years (SD
= 12.25) with a minimum of 18 and a maximum of 66 years. 78
subjects were secondary school graduates, 29 were university
graduates. 83 subjects were unmarried; 50 subjects were
unemployed. From a socio-demographic perspective, the
sample group is representative of the average middle class
found on the Neapolitan territory.
R. Sperandeo et al. • Executive Functions and Personality Features: a Circular Interpretative Paradigm
000064
V. R
ESULTS
Table 1 describes the results of the study. The 6 executive
tasks evaluated by the FAB were correlated, according to their
efficiency, to a set of TCI items; This correlation was both
positive and negative. For example, the first
"conceptualization" task resulted negatively related to 15 TCI
items and positively related to 5 items.
In this sense, subjects who responded "yes" to one of the
other negatively related items showed scores in the first task
which are rather lower than the norm. On the other hand,
subjects, who answered "yes" to the positively related items
showed normal scores on the first task. (Table 1)
The items negatively or positively correlated to each of the
FAB's tasks make up homogeneous sets that describe the
character modes that are psychologically related to the
executive functions evaluated by the FAB. For example, items
negatively related to the first task describe temperamental
modes of functioning which are characterized by discomfort
when confronted with novelty, with risk situations and the need
for gratification, and as regards character, they are
distinguished by a low level of cooperativeness and low self-
determination.
Therefore, if a subject responds "yes" to a larger number of
items in a particular set, that subject expresses most of the
character modes described by that set.
For each of the sets, the score of the items that compose it
has been added, and the obtained values have been used as
explanatory variables in a logistic regression test where the
dependent variables were the six executive tasks of the FAB.
The logistic regression test showed a significant link between
the sets of items and the executive tasks related to them; the
link was particularly relevant for the tasks: "conceptualization",
"mental flexibility" and "environmental autonomy". The
presence of a "yes" answer to a item will vary the likelihood
that the subject achieves, in the related task, a score below
77%, 79% and 87% respectively, with a dimension of the effect
between 50% and 86%. (Table 2)
VI.
DISCUSSION
First of all our study shows that the specificity of the
relationship between homogeneous sets of descriptive items of
personality and precise executive tasks indicates that EFs are
independent processes, even though they are interconnected as
proposed by Goldstein [16].
The Items that make up the homogeneous sets correlated
with FE describe traits of non-pathological personality. The
presence of multiple "yes" responses within a specific set of
items is closely related to a causal relationship to the deficiency
of the related executive tasks, or to their efficiency, but this
link does not appear in any pathology.
For example, subjects who fall into the FE defined as
"conceptualization" respond "yes" to many items negatively
related to it; however, these subjects have no clinically relevant
pathologies or features either for personality or for
neuropsychological functioning.
Subjects show a neuropsychological performance that is
based on their style of adaptation to the environment; In turn,
the neuropsychological performance reinforces the adaptation
style by creating an individual pattern of operation (closely
related to interaction with the environment) without interfering
in the pathology of the character. For example, an individual
who does little to co-operate with others, who avoids novelties
and takes little risk, probably has less need to conceptualize
semantic links between certain types of mental objects (task
conceptualization); This function is less used (less practiced)
and therefore less efficient, and circularly reinforces the
avoiding style of this subject. The statistically significant
linkage found in our study, between FE and normal character
aspects, seems to describe a circular mechanism of causal
action to explain the mechanisms of adaptation of the
individual to the environment. The model which comes forth
from the study is not compatible with a simple, linear
relationship which exists between neuropsychological
deficiency and personality disorders, which is traditionally
used to explain the pathology.
VII. C
ONCLUSIONS
The presence of positive or negative relationships between
normal personality functions and the efficiency of executive
functions outlines the existence of a circular (non-hierarchical)
relationship between these variables. In this sense, EF deficits
are not at the origin of pathological personality traits, but rather
the FE have a two-way relationship with the dimensions of
normal personalities whose expression is enhanced or
weakened, and, in turn, sustains or hinders them. This
information (point) is extremely important for the structuring
of computational systems that intend to emulate the
characteristic functioning of human beings. Affective
functioning seems to emerge from the interactive process
between the individual and the environment and is proposed as
a structure on an intermediate level with both repetitive and
flexyble characteristics. In computational networks, the middle
layer of nodes is the heart of the self-learning process and can
store the interaction mechanism between the subject and the
environment rather than the static response of the subject to the
environmental stimuli. It is thus possible to describe the shape
and function of an individual's character considering it as the
emerging quality of the encounter between a body in action and
its environment [17].
VIII. F
UTURE
D
IRECTIONS
The present analysis is a pilot study that requires an
extension of the number of participants in order to confirm the
relationships observed and their statistical structure.
It is fundamental to increase the sample size and apply
experimental customized designs so as to confirm the value of
these evidences.
8th IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom 2017) • September 11-14, 2017 • Debrecen, Hungary
000065
TABLE I. C
ORRELATIONS BETWEEN
TCI
ITEMS AND
FAB
EXECUTIVE
TASK
EF Task
TCI Items
Items (Spearman Rho)
Conceptualization 20(-0,21), 21(-021), 47(-0,21), 49(-0,18),
114(-0,23), 120(-0,20), 144(-0,23),149(-0,25),
178(-0,25),184(-0,18), 185(-0,18), 198(-0,18),
217(-0.18), 229(-0,21), 238(-0,17)
Negative Correlation
68(0,30), 145(0,18), 172(0,19), 199(0,25),
240(0,18)
Positive Correlation
Mental flexibility 14(-0,25), 17(-0,22), 72(-0,21), 90(-0,21),
112(-0,29), 120(-0,18), 142(-0,24),218(-0,19),
223(-0,17)
Negative Correlation
43(0,20), 98(0,18), 116(0,17), 176(0,20),
213(0,17)
Positive Correlation
Planning 29(-0,20), 75(-0,20), 88(-0,20), 194(-0,21),
200(-0,19)
Negative Correlation
Sensitivity to
interference
14(-0,18), 25(-0,18), 42(-0,20), 44(-0,20), 71(-
0,19), 76(-0,20), 86(-0,18), 92(-0,17), 96(-
0,27), 132(-0,18), 141(-0,27), 148(-0,32),
157(-0,17), 167(-0,23), 237(-0,18)
Negative Correlation
Inhibitory control 5(0.24), 18(-0.17), 42(-0.18), 64(-0.18),
96(-0,20), 112(-0,34), 120(-0,19), 160(-0.27),
162(-0.17),198(-0.20), 235(-0.24)
Negative Correlation
3(0,22), 16(0,17), 158(0,26)
Positive Correlation
Environmental
autonomy
90(-0,21), 232(-0,21)
Negative Correlation
1(0,24), 15(0,19), 35(0,19), 58(0,19), 74(0,18),
85(0,25), 101(0,30), 174(0,20), 179(0,23),
215(0,18), 221(0,22), 228(0,19)
Positive Correlation
TABLE II. R
ELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EXECUTIVE TASKS AND
HOMOGENEOUS SETS OF PERSONALITY ITEMS
S
PEARMAN CORRELATIONS
AND LOGISTIC REGRESSION
Executive
task
Set of
items
Speram
an’s
Rho
B Esp
(B)
R
2
di
Nagelke
rk
Conceptuali
zation
Negative
correlati
on
-0.51 -0.35 0,71 0.5
Positive
correlati
on
0.44 0.77 2.17
Mental
flexibility
Negative
correlati
on
-0.47 -0.24 0.79 0.72
Positive
correlati
on
0.33 0.79 2.2
Planning Negative
correlati
on
-0.33 -0.14 1.15 0.28
Sensitivity
to
interference
Positive
correlati
on
-0.52 -0.08 1.1 0.08
Inhibitory
control
Negative
correlati
on
-0.53 -0.02 0.98 0.01
Environme Positive 0.41 0.87 2.39 0.86
Executive
task
Set of
items
Speram
an’s
Rho
B Esp
(B)
R
2
di
Nagelke
rk
ntal
autonomy
correlati
on
A
CKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors would like to thank Bianca Tino for the
translation.
R
EFERENCES
[1] Baron, I. S. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the child . New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
1
[2] Burgess, P. W. (2003). Assessment of executive function. Handbook of
clinical neuropsychology, 302-321.
[3] LeDoux, J. (2002). Il sé sinaptico. Raffaello Cortina, Milano.
[4] Liston, C., Matalon, S., Hare, T. A., Davidson, M. C., & Casey, B. J.
(2006). Anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortices are sensitive to
dissociable forms of conflict in a task-switching
paradigm. Neuron, 50(4), 643-653.
[5] Coulthard, E. J., Nachev, P., & Husain, M. (2008). Control over conflict
during movement preparation: role of posterior parietal
cortex. Neuron, 58(1), 144-157.
[6] Stern, D. N. (2005). Il momento presente. Raffaello Cortina, Milano.
[7] Marvel, C. L., & Desmond, J. E. (2010). Functional topography of the
cerebellum in verbal working memory. Neuropsychology Review, 20
(3), 271–279.
[8] Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic.
[9] Levine, B., & Craik, F. I. (2012). Unifying clinical, experimental, and
neuroimaging studies of the human frontal lobes. In B. Levine & F. I.
Craik (Eds.), Mind and the frontal lobes: Cognition, behavior, and brain
imaging (pp. 3–15). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
[10] R. Meares, “A Dissociation Model of Borderline Personality Disorder
Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology. New York: WW Norton
& Company 2012
[11] R. Picard. Affective Computing. The MIT Press, 1997.
[12] Baranyi, Péter, and Adam Csapo. "Definition and synergies of cognitive
Infocommunications." Acta Polytechnica Hungarica 9.1 (2012): 67-83.
[13] Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., &
Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention
understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662-667.
[14] Appollonio, I., Leone, M., Isella, V., Piamarta, F., Consoli, T., Villa, M.
L., ... & Nichelli, P. (2005). The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB):
normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurological Sciences,
26(2), 108-116.
[15] C.R. Cloninger, D.M. Svrakic, T.R. Przybeck, “A psychobiological
model of temperament and character”. Archives of general psychiatry.
50, 12, pp. 975-990, 1993.
[16] Goldstein, Sam, and Jack A. Naglieri, eds. Handbook of executive
functioning. Springer Science & Business Media, 2013.
[17] Villani, M., Benedettini, S., Roli, A., Lane, D., Poli, I., & Serra, R.
(2014). Identifying emergent dynamical structures in network models.
In Recent Advances of Neural Network Models and Applications (pp. 3-
13). Springer, Cham.
R. Sperandeo et al. • Executive Functions and Personality Features: a Circular Interpretative Paradigm
000066
... Most authors say that dyspraxia is accompanied by a difficulty in the mental representation of actions, with problems in creating mental images representative of first-person movements maybe connected to neural networks dysfunctions of the parietal cortex [11]. ...
... CogInfoCom and narrative mind. The therapeutic action on the conscious contents of thought can be facilitated by the identification of latent clusters of thought in the patient's speech and by the possibility of verifying the modification of the dysfunctional clusters [22,23]. ...
... A network is a simple architecture formed by nodes connected by bridges. In the patient therapist system the construction of the network identifies the interacting parts of the patient and the therapist as nodes; these parts correspond to their psychic functions [12][13][14] such as attention, emotion management [15], concentration, self-image, adult functions [16,17], empathy [18], etc., measured using a standardized test [19]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Psychotherapy, unanimously described as a particular organized and systematic relationship between a patient and a therapist, is a real complex system. The interaction between the numerous variables belonging to the patient, the therapist and the context in which the therapeutic couple is inserted, presents auto-poietic characteristics and generates emergent qualities that, at the current state of psychotherapy research, have not been effectively addressed. The methods of machine learning are suitable for analyzing complex systems and in our opinion, at the moment, they are the most appropriate for studying the therapeutic relationship, understood as a quality emerging from patient-therapist interaction. In fact, through the use of artificial intelligence methods it is possible to construct a model of interaction between therapist and patient by integrating in it the non-linearity of information exchanges between the components of the system. The humanistic therapies vision of the patient-therapist relationship as a complex and organized interaction between the parts of a system is comparable to the networks of cellular chemical reactions described by Varela and Maturana. In these networks, which are a complex systems, what is important for maintaining the cell’s integrity and its functioning it is not the nature of every single chemical reaction but the form and dynamics of their interaction. This research is a pilot study that intends to evaluate the possibility of describing the complexity of therapeutic relationships using the methods of machine learning and complex networks, ordinarily used to study systems composed of numerous interacting variables. From this pilot study emerges that the use of graphs is certainly a valid tool for the analysis of both the psychotherapeutic sessions and the evolution of the care relationship over time. Numerous suggestions on the dynamics within the patient-therapist system emerge from the construction of a complex network useful for describing the trend of psychotherapy, which in this way can be described without losing the value of the wealth of each individual experience.
... The movements make the cognitive, motor and psychic characteristics manifest, at the same time recalling the interest of an observer. Modern research on the development of the psyche in children sets the movement and the possibility of manipulating its environment at the base of the birth of the sense of self, of the structuring of the personality [1], [2], [3] [4] and of the subsequent, and more complex, forms of self-awareness [5], [6], [7], [8]. In this sense, the interaction between the child and a significant adult can be described as the first development environment, in which a state of continuous interchange and mutual influence between the two subjects is created [9]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
In this paper we will illustrate the progress of a research that intends to use computational methods to analyze the complex interaction between a child and his caregiver, focusing on motor aspects. According to the theory of Ruella Frank, Gestalt psychotherapist, the child acquires motor patterns through the interaction with the caregiver that will be repeated over the course of life. Six fundamental movements have been identified and defined, which allow the child to develop self-awareness. The objective of the study is the creation of an observation tool of dyadic interaction through the automatic recognition of these movements whose purpose is the identification and differentiation of the peculiar vocabulary of movements that is established between parent and child. Thanks to the use of Kinect v2 and through the Kinect Studio program, we have trained a network to identify three of the six basic movements: “Reaching”, “Pushing” and “Pulling”. The results of this training show that the machine discriminates the gestures that have been defined and recognized through different features, although they visually share a part of the progression. The future perspectives of this study foresee to apply the methodology described in this paper to the other three gestures, widen the database and refine the tagging process, to obtain more precise results. The ultimate aim is to include the database in an application that we have proposed to develop, useful for the recognition of the peculiar dyadic baby-caregiver movements.
... A plural model of consciousness could explain the birth of the Self, with a space of centrencephalic functional integration [15,42,43] that supports both integration and global communication at its base. Consciousness would thus appear as a multiple unity rather than an undifferentiated unity. ...
Article
Full-text available
The so-called Posthuman question - the birth of organisms generated by the encounter of biological and artificial entities (humanoid robots, cyborgs and so on) - is now on the agenda of science and, more generally, of contemporary society. This is an issue of enormous importance, which not only poses ethical questions but also, and above all, methodological questions about how it will be achieved on a scientific plane. How such entities will be born and what their functions will be? For example, what kind of consciousness will they be equipped with, in view of the function of consciousness for distinguishing the Self from others, which is the foundation of the interactive life of relationships? Many scholars believe that rapid technological progress will lead to the emergence of organisms that will simulate the functions of the mind, learn from their experiences, decode real-world information, and plan their actions and choices based on their own values elaborated from vast amounts of data and metadata. In the not-too-distant future, it is believed that these entities will acquire awareness and, consequently, decisional freedom, and perhaps even their own unique morals. In this paper, we try to show that the path towards this goal cannot avoid clarification of the problems that neuroscience has ahead of it. These problems concern: a) the way in which consciousness comes about on the basis of well-defined brain processes; b) how it represents its own organization and not a simple brain function; c) how simultaneously contains multiple distinct contents, each with its own intentionality; d) how it expresses dynamic evolutionary relations and not a set of phenomena that may be isolated; e) finally, how its order is not rigidly hierarchical, but is supported by a multiplicity of horizontal levels, each of which is in structural and functional continuum with different phenomenal events. The empirical and theoretical research effort on this topic provides an intensive contribution to the development of IC Technologies. © 2020 Scientific Association for Infocommunications. All rights reserved.
... Considering the importance of visual attention in predicting learning [29], [30] or attention problems we propose a technology-based paradigm in which to accurately measure psychological outcomes. Also we believe that different obstacles in the assessment of executive functions [31], [32] can be overcome with the use of new technologies, such us eye-tracker system. ...
... Within this paradigm, it comes to be configured as a distributed feature, composed of different processing modes and inherent to different natural domains. Among these are particular modulation systems that cooperate within larger areas of brain processing, such as the limbic system and the neocortex, considered as depository brain areas of emotional processes and cognitive processes [15] [16], assigned to different levels of processing. These levels, however, although distinguishable, seem to be deeply correlated and bound to each other; and it is precisely in the areas of possible connection between the limbic system and the neocortex that neuroscientific research has located nerve nuclei involved in learning and memory processes [17]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Memory is one of the most fascinating and complex faculties of human beings. Thanks to numerous studies in the neuroscientific field, today we are aware that when we try to remember the past, we do not entirely repeat an event like a movie, infact, the tendency of memory is to change, deform or fade its contents, with the time and with the accumulation of new experiences, to the point of completely forgetting. The aim to this study is to represent the elements of the process of remembering paying attention also to oblivion. It has been been shown that the substrate of memory is at least partially localized in the brain, in certain areas of the brain, but none of the mechanisms highlighted can work in isolation. So, the aim of this papers is to understand how to study the faculty of memory in the light of new neuroscientific discoveries and beyond.
Article
Full-text available
Background Work-related stress is a relevant phenomenon in terms of health and safety at work, as occupational distress has a negative impact on individual and organisational well-being. It is a complex and multifactorial phenomenon, whose evaluation must be carried out through a specific and adequate methodology. Objective This work aims to identify versatile tools that can quickly provide reliable measures of work distress. It analyzes the proposal elaborated by the “Comitato Unico di Garanzia per le Pari Opportunità, la Valorizzazione del Benessere di chi lavora e contro le Discriminazioni” of Rome, i.e . the tool “Valutazione è prevenzione, Sicurezza è partecipazione”. Methods A study was carried out on a sample of 474 employees of the Neapolitan Judicial Offices, who were given a standardized questionnaire to investigate the stress, associated with the proposal of the Comitato Unico di Garanzia. Results From the elaboration of the results, it emerges that the conditions of working wellbeing are linked to two main factors related to the perception of workers both of physical-environmental and organisational-relational aspects. In particular, it emerged that the new assessment tool, consisting of a small number of items, contributes to the detection of work stress, so it is necessary to deepen through future research the contribution that this tool can offer to the survey on work-related stress. Conclusion Having highlighted two factors that significantly saturate the presence of a working discomfort, through an agile tool, allows us to plan a new research path, which can approach the complexity of the phenomenon through the methods of deep learning.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The way individuals make choices and decisions influences their behavior and life style. The choice is not simply based on rational and formal logic, but it's influenced by many emotional and social factors. Starting from this assumption, some studies investigated the way in which emotions influence decision-making processes, while others explored the possibility that decision-making processes are compromised by the onset of psychiatric pathologies and by the presence of prefrontal cortexlesions, which can induce a radical change in basic preferences of the subject. In our opinion, the decision-making style is strictly connected to the normal personality structure while the relationship with psychopathology can be framed within the non-causal and non-linear effects typical of risk or protection factors. The present pilot study, first, intends to explore the relation between decision-making styles, non-pathological personality traits and psychic disorders; second, it intends to propose a psychometric tool administrable via mobile app, aimed at studying the influence of decision-making styles, related to personality, on the quality and effectiveness of decisions. The present pilot study, was developed in a sample of outpatients in a private psychotherapy service made up of subjects diagnosed with personality disorder. The TCI test was been administered to identify the decision-making styles related to the character traits and temperament. Through statistical analyses, five factors were extracted (5 decision-making styles) and only two personality disorders appear to be related to decision-making styles. This results describe a significant but marginal impact of decisional styles on the pathology of personality, with respect to which they play a role of risk or protection factor.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we provide the finalized definition of Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom). Following the definition, we briefly describe the scope and goals of CogInfoCom, and discuss the common interests between CogInfoCom and the various research disciplines which contribute to this new field in a synergistic way.
Article
Full-text available
Inferior parietal lobule (IPL) neurons were studied when monkeys performed motor acts embedded in different actions and when they observed similar acts done by an experimenter. Most motor IPL neurons coding a specific act (e.g., grasping) showed markedly different activations when this act was part of different actions (e.g., for eating or for placing). Many motor IPL neurons also discharged during the observation of acts done by others. Most responded differentially when the same observed act was embedded in a specific action. These neurons fired during the observation of an act, before the beginning of the subsequent acts specifying the action. Thus, these neurons not only code the observed motor act but also allow the observer to understand the agent's intentions.
Article
Full-text available
The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) is a short cognitive and behavioural six-subtest battery for the bedside screening of a global executive dysfunction; although recently devised, it is already extensively used thanks to its ease of administration and claimed sensitivity. The aim of the present study was to derive Italian normative values from a sample of 364 control subjects (215 women and 149 men) of different ages (mean: 57.4+/-17.9 years; range: 20-94 years) and educational level (mean: 10.4+/-4.3 years; range: 1-17 years); the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was concurrently administered. Multiple linear regression analysis revealed significant effects for age and education whereas gender was not significant; thus, from the derived linear equation, a correction grid for FAB raw scores was built. Based on nonparametric techniques, inferential cut-off scores were subsequently determined and equivalent scores (ES) computed. Test-restest and interrater reliabilities were both satisfactory. Interestingly, MMSE was significantly correlated with FAB raw scores, whereas adjusted scores were not. The present data may improve the accuracy in the use of the FAB both for clinical and research purposes.
Book
Planning. Attention. Memory. Self-regulation. These and other core cognitive and behavioral operations of daily life comprise what we know as executive functioning (EF). But despite all we know, the concept has engendered multiple, often conflicting definitions and its components are sometimes loosely defined and poorly understood. The Handbook of Executive Functioning cuts through the confusion, analyzing both the whole and its parts in comprehensive, practical detail for scholar and clinician alike. Background chapters examine influential models of EF, tour the brain geography of the executive system and pose salient developmental questions. A section on practical implications relates early deficits in executive functioning to ADD and other disorders in children and considers autism and later-life dementias from an EF standpoint. Further chapters weigh the merits of widely used instruments for assessing executive functioning and review interventions for its enhancement, with special emphasis on children and adolescents. Featured in the Handbook: The development of hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence. A review of the use of executive function tasks in externalizing and internalizing disorders. Executive functioning as a mediator of age-related cognitive decline in adults. Treatment integrity in interventions that target executive function. Supporting and strengthening working memory in the classroom to enhance executive functioning. The Handbook of Executive Functioning is an essential resource for researchers, scientist-practitioners and graduate students in clinical child, school and educational psychology; child and adolescent psychiatry; neurobiology; developmental psychology; rehabilitation medicine/therapy and social work. © Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014. All rights reserved.
Chapter
The identification of emergent structures in dynamical systems is a major challenge in complex systems science. In particular, the formation of intermediate-level dynamical structures is of particular interest for what concerns biological as well as artificial network models. In this work, we present a new technique aimed at identifying clusters of nodes in a network that behave in a coherent and coordinated way and that loosely interact with the remainder of the system. This method is based on an extension of a measure introduced for detecting clusters in biological neural networks. Even if our results are still preliminary, we have evidence for showing that our approach is able to identify these “emerging things” in some artificial network models and that it is way more powerful than usual measures based on statistical correlation. This method will make it possible to identify mesolevel dynamical structures in network models in general, from biological to social networks.
Book
In the past 25 years, the frontal lobes have dominated human neuroscience research. Functional neuroimaging studies have revealed their importance to brain networks involved in nearly every aspect of mental and cognitive functioning. Studies of patients with focal brain lesions have expanded on early case study evidence of behavioral, emotional, and cognitive changes associated with frontal lobe brain damage. The role of frontal lobe function and dysfunction in human development (in both children and older adults), psychiatric disorders, the dementias, and other brain diseases has also received rapidly increasing attention. In this Festschrift for Donald T. Stuss, one of the world's leading frontal lobe researchers, 14 researchers review and synthesize the current state of knowledge on frontal lobe function, including structural and functional brain imaging, brain network analysis, aging and dementia, traumatic brain injury, rehabilitation, attention, memory, and consciousness. The book therefore provides a state-of-the-art account of research in this exciting area, and also highlights a number of new findings by some of the world's top researchers.
Chapter
The frontal lobes are central to human cognition and consciousness. This chapter reviews the history of research on frontal lobe function in three eras, from the mid-19th to the mid-20th century, characterized by clinical case studies and qualitative observations of animals with experimental frontal lesions, from the mid-20th century to the late 20th century, containing more organized experimental group studies of animals and humans with frontal lobe lesions, and the current era, characterized by a proliferation of detailed anatomical research using multimodal imaging and sophisticated cognitive science paradigms. Stuss and Benson's The Frontal Lobes, published in 1986, provided a springboard into the current era by cogently synthesizing anatomical and experimental human and animal research to date. The remainder of this chapter summarizes the contributions to this volume, a Festschrift for Don Stuss.
Article
This desk reference provides a collection of normative data in child neuropsychology. It compiles published and previously unpublished normative data for the neuropsychological tests that are most commonly used with children. Besides presenting a collection of data, it also integrates concepts and models central to the neuropsychological assessment of children into the discussions of data. All these discussions have a practical, clinical focus. As background, the author considers the current status of child neuropsychology practice, test models, behavioral assessment techniques, observational data, procedures to optimize child evaluation, communication of results through the interpretive session and report writing, and preliminary assessment methods. Then she reviews the tests and data under the broad domains of intelligence, executive function, attention, language, motor and sensory-perceptual function, visuoperceptual, visuospatial, and visuoconstructional function, and learning and memory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Speech-both overt and covert-facilitates working memory by creating and refreshing motor memory traces, allowing new information to be received and processed. Neuroimaging studies suggest a functional topography within the sub-regions of the cerebellum that subserve verbal working memory. Medial regions of the anterior cerebellum support overt speech, consistent with other forms of motor execution such as finger tapping, whereas lateral portions of the superior cerebellum support speech planning and preparation (e.g., covert speech). The inferior cerebellum is active when information is maintained across a delay, but activation appears to be independent of speech, lateralized by modality of stimulus presentation, and possibly related to phonological storage processes. Motor (dorsal) and cognitive (ventral) channels of cerebellar output nuclei can be distinguished in working memory. Clinical investigations suggest that hyper-activity of cerebellum and disrupted control of inner speech may contribute to certain psychiatric symptoms.