ArticlePDF Available

Naturalistic decision making.

Authors:
  • ShadowBox LLC & MacroCognition LLC

Abstract

Objective: This article describes the origins and contributions of the naturalistic decision making (NDM) research approach. Background: NDM research emerged in the 1980s to study how people make decisions in real-world settings. Method: The findings and methods used by NDM researchers are presented along with their implications. Results: The NDM framework emphasizes the role of experience in enabling people to rapidly categorize situations to make effective decisions. Conclusion: The NDM focus on field settings and its interest in complex conditions provide insights for human factors practitioners about ways to improve performance. Application: The NDM approach has been used to improve performance through revisions of military doctrine, training that is focused on decision requirements, and the development of information technologies to support decision making and related cognitive functions.
ORIGINS OF NDM
Amajor contribution of the naturalistic decision
making (NDM) community has been to describe
how people actually make decisions in real-world
settings. This statement might seem odd because
decision researchers had conducted experiments
and developed models for decades prior to the
emergence of NDM in 1989. However, that re-
search primarily identified optimal ways of making
decisions (defined as choices among alternatives)
in well-structured settings that could be carefully
controlled.
The heuristics and biases paradigm (e.g., Kahne-
man, Slovic, & Tversky, 1982) demonstrated that
people did not adhere to the principles of optimal
performance; respondents relied on heuristic as op-
posed to algorithmic strategies even when these
strategies generated systematic deviations from
optimal judgments as defined by the laws of prob-
ability, the axioms of expected utility theory, and
Bayesian statistics.
So by1989, it was fairly clear how people didn’t
make decisions. They didn’t generate alternative
options and compare them on the same set of eval-
uation dimensions. They did not generate proba-
bility and utility estimates for different courses of
action and elaborate these into decision trees. Even
when they did compare options, they rarely em-
ployed systematic evaluation techniques.
But what did they do instead? Researchers were
not likely to find out how people actually made de-
cisions by conducting experiments to test hypothe-
ses derived from statistical and mathematical
models of ideal choice strategies. Even the deci-
sion researchers who performed studies in field
settings, using experienced participants, primarily
assessed performance according to formal stan-
dards. (For a fuller discussion of this history, see
Lipshitz, Klein, Orasanu, & Salas, 2001.)
Unfortunately, the training methods and deci-
sion support systems developed in accord with the
formal standards did not improve decision qual-
ity and did not get adopted in field settings. People
found these tools and methods cumbersome and
irrelevant to the work they needed to do (Yates,
Veinott, & Patalano, 2003).
The initial NDM researchers tried a different
approach. Instead of beginning with formal mod-
els of decision making, we began by conducting
field research to try to discover the strategies peo-
ple used. Instead of looking for ways that people
were suboptimal, we wanted to find out how peo-
ple were able to make tough decisions under diffi-
cult conditions such as limited time, uncertainty,
high stakes, vague goals, and unstable conditions
Naturalistic Decision Making
Gary Klein, Klein Associates, Division of ARA, Fairborn, Ohio
Objective:This article describes the origins and contributions of the naturalistic decision
making (NDM) research approach. Background: NDM research emerged in the 1980s
to study how people make decisions in real-world settings. Method: The findings and
methods used by NDM researchers are presented along with their implications. Results:
The NDM framework emphasizes the role of experience in enabling people to rapidly
categorize situations to make effective decisions. Conclusion: The NDM focus on field
settings and its interest in complex conditions provide insights for human factors prac-
titioners about ways to improve performance. Application: The NDM approach has
been used to improve performance through revisions of military doctrine, training that
is focused on decision requirements, and the development of information technologies
to support decision making and related cognitive functions.
Address correspondence to Gary Klein, Klein Associates Division, Applied Research Associates, Inc., 1750 Commerce Center
Blvd. N., Fairborn, OH 45324; gary@decisionmaking.com. HUMAN FACTORS,Vol. 50, No. 3, June 2008, pp. 456–460. DOI
10.1518/001872008X288385. Copyright © 2008, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.
GOLDEN ANNIVERSARY SPECIAL ISSUE
NATURALISTIC DECISION MAKING 457
(see Orasanu & Connolly, 1993). Researchers in
fields such as medicine (Elstein, Shulman, &
Sprafka, 1978) and business (Isenberg, 1984) had
already been studying these kinds of issues.
The basic research program at the Army
Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social
Sciences began funding several of the NDM re-
searchers during the mid-1980s. The U.S. Navy
became interested in naturalistic decisions follow-
ing the 1988 USS Vincennes shoot-down incident,
in which a U.S. Navy Aegis cruiser destroyed an
Iranian commercial airliner, mistaking it for a hos-
tile attacker. Both the Army and the Navy wanted
to help people make high-stakes decisions under
extreme time pressure and under dynamic and un-
certain conditions.
The first NDM conference, in 1989, assembled
researchers studying decision making in field set-
tings. In a chapter that emerged from that meeting,
Raanan Lipshitz (1993) identified no less than nine
NDM models that had been developed in parallel.
One of these was Hammond’s cognitive con-
tinuum theory (Hammond, Hamm, Grassia, &
Pearson, 1987), which asserts that decisions vary
in the degree to which they rely on intuitive and
analytical processes. Conditions such as amount
of information and time available determine where
decisions fall on this continuum and whether peo-
ple rely more on patterns or on functional rela-
tionships. Asecond account of decision making
was Rasmussen’s (1983) model of cognitive con-
trol, which distinguished skill-based, rule-based,
and knowledge-based behavior operating within
the context of a decision ladder that permitted
heuristic cutoff paths. A third, the recognition-
primed decision model (Klein, 1989), is discussed
in more detail later.
Working separately, we all reached similar con-
clusions. People were not generating and compar-
ing option sets. People were using prior experience
to rapidly categorize situations. People were rely-
ing on some kind of synthesis of their experience
call it a schema or a prototype or a category – to
make these judgments. The categories suggested
appropriate courses of action. The static notion of
decisions as gambles, which portrays people as
passively awaiting the outcomes of their bets, did
not fit leaders who were actively trying to shape
events.
The NDM researchers studied people in field
settings, such as Navy commanders, jurors, nuclear
power plant operators, Army small unit leaders,
anesthesiologists, airline pilots, nurses, and high-
way engineers. From this perspective, making a
decision means committing oneself to a course of
action where plausible alternatives exist, even if the
person does not identify or compare these alter-
natives.
The NDM movement shifted our conception
of human decision making from a domain-
independent general approach to a knowledge-
based approach exemplified by decision makers
who had substantial experience. The decision-
making process was expanded to include a prior
stage of perception and recognition of situations,
as well as generation of appropriate responses, not
just choice from among given options. This per-
spective took advantage of advances in cognitive
psychology such as knowledge representation
concepts of scripts, schemas, and mental models,
to contrast expert versus novice behavior.
To provide a fuller account of the NDM view of
decision making, I will describe the recognition-
primed decision model; I am more familiar with
it than with the others, and it has received a fair
amount of attention. However, all of the nine NDM
models Lipshitz (1993) listed show a strong fam-
ily resemblance.
Recognition-Primed Decision Model
The recognition-primed decision (RPD) model
describes how people use their experience in the
form of a repertoire of patterns (Klein, Calder-
wood, & Clinton-Cirocco, 1986). These patterns
describe the primary causal factors operating in the
situation. The patterns highlight the most relevant
cues, provide expectancies, identify plausible
goals, and suggest typical types of reactions in that
type of situation. When people need to make a
decision they can quickly match the situation to
the patterns they have learned. If they find a clear
match, they can carry out the most typical course
of action. In that way, people can successfully
make extremely rapid decisions. The RPD model
explains how people can make good decisions
without comparing options.
However, there is more to the RPD model than
pattern matching. How can a person evaluate an
option without comparing it with others? We found
that the fireground commanders we studied eval-
uated a course of action by using mental simula-
tion to imagine how it would play out within the
context of the current situation. If it would work,
then the commanders could initiate the action. If
458 June 2008 –
Human Factors
it almost worked, they could try to adapt it or else
consider other actions that were somewhat less
typical, continuing until they found an option that
felt comfortable. This process exemplifies Herbert
Simon’s (1957) notion of satisficing – looking for
the first workable option rather than trying to find
the best possible option. Because fires grow expo-
nentially, the faster the commanders could react,
the easier their job.
Therefore, the RPD model is a blend of intuition
and analysis. The pattern matching is the intuitive
part, and the mental simulation is the conscious,
deliberate, and analytical part. This blend corre-
sponds to the System 1(fast and unconscious)/Sys-
tem 2 (slow and deliberate) account of cognition put
forward by Kahneman (2003), Epstein (1994), and
others (for an overview, see Evans, 2008). A purely
intuitive strategy relying only on pattern matching
would be too risky because sometimes the pattern
matching generates flawed options. Acompletely
deliberative and analytical strategy would be too
slow; the fires would be out of control by the time
the commanders finished deliberating.
We formulated the RPD model based on in-
depth interviews with fireground commanders
about recent and challenging incidents and found
that the percentage of RPD strategies generally
ranged from 80% to 90% (Klein, 1989) (see Fig-
ure 1). Other researchers have replicated these
findings (see Klein, 1998).
Most critically, we tested the prediction from
the RPD model that for experienced decision mak-
ers, the first option they consider is usually sat-
isfactory. Klein, Wolf, Militello, and Zsambok
(1995) found that chess players were not randomly
generating moves that they would then evaluate.
Rather, the first moves that occurred to them were
much better than would be expected by chance.
These findings support the RPD hypothesis that
the first option considered is usually satisfactory.
These results were later replicated by Johnson and
Raab (2003).
Contributions of NDM
The demands of NDM research have spurred
the development of cognitive field research and
cognitive task analysis methods, as described by
Crandall, Klein, and Hoffman (2006). These meth-
ods have contributed to the field of human fac-
tors and ergonomics by enabling practitioners to
explore the cognitive underpinnings of different
types of work.
NDM has affected Army doctrine. The current
edition of the Army Field Manual on Command
and Control (FM 101-5) includes for the first time
a section on intuitive decision making, largely in-
fluenced by research on the RPD model.
Schmitt and Klein (1999) have adapted the
RPD model to military planning guidance. Their
strategy reduces planning time without sacrific-
ing plan quality (Ross, Klein, Thunholm, Schmitt,
& Baxter, 2004) and has become the basis for tac-
tical decision making in the Swedish armed forces
(Thunholm, 2006).
The field of NDM has also provided guidance
for training decision making and related cogni-
tive skills. Cannon-Bowers and Salas (1998) have
described the range of lessons learned from the
TADMUS (Tactical Decision Making Under
Stress) project initiated by the Navy following the
USS Vincennes shoot-down decision. These in-
clude methods for providing stress inoculation
along with approaches for individual and team
decision training.
The NDM movement has seen a surprisingly
rapid adoption of its findings. Within 10 years of
the initial NDM meeting, experiential models
were accepted as the standard account of decision
making by most practitioners. NDM conferences
have been held every 2 to 3 years, alternating be-
tween the United States and Europe. In addition,
the Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making
Technical Group, formed to provide an outlet for
NDM research, has become one of the largest and
most active in the Human Factors and Ergonom-
ics Society.
Where is NDM heading? Because cognitive
field research methods have proven so effective for
generating insights about decision making, they
are being used to study other “macrocognitive”
functions, such as situation awareness, sensemak-
ing, planning and replanning, and the ways they
are linked (Klein, et al., 2003). Macrocognition, the
study of cognitive adaptations to complexity, may
reflect the next step in the evolution of NDM. Mac-
rocognitive functions are performed at the level of
individuals. These functions are also performed by
teams, as emphasized by Letsky, Warner, Fiore,
Rosen, and Salas (2007), who build on NDM re-
search on shared mental models and team knowl-
edge (e.g., Cooke, Salas, Kiekel, & Bell, 2004). The
growth of interest in macrocognition suggests that
the premises of NDM are stimulating research and
applications that cover a broader and interrelated
NATURALISTIC DECISION MAKING 459
set of cognitive functions at the team, organiza-
tional, and individual levels.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I thank Robert Hoffman, Danny Kahneman,
Beth Veinott, and Raanan Lipshitz for their very
helpful comments and criticisms in reviewing an
earlier draft of this manuscript. I also appreciate
the helpful suggestions of three anonymous re-
viewers.
REFERENCES
Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Salas, E. (1998). Making decisions under
stress: Implications for individual and team training. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Cooke, N. J., Salas, E., Kiekel, P. A., & Bell, B. (2004). Advances in mea-
suring team cognition. In E. Salas & S. M. Fiore (Eds.), Team cogni-
tion: Understanding the factors that drive process and performance
(pp. 83–106). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Crandall, B., Klein, G., & Hoffman, R. R. (2006). Working minds: A
practitioner’s guide to cognitive task analysis. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Elstein, A. S., Shulman, L. S., & Sprafka, S. A. (1978). Medical problem
solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic
unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judg-
ment and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59,
255–278.
Hammond, K. R., Hamm, R. M., Grassia, J., & Pearson, T. (1987).
Direct comparison of the efficacy of intuitive and analytical cog-
nition in expert judgment. Proceedings of IEEE Transactions on
Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, SMC-17, 753–770.
Isenberg, D. J. (1984, November–December). How senior managers
think. Harvard Business Review, pp. 80–90.
Johnson, J. G., & Raab, M. (2003). Take the first: Option generation and
resulting choices. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 91(2), 215–229.
Experience the Situation in a Changing Context
Is the Situation
Familiar?
Recognition has four aspects
Actions
1…n
Plausible
Goals
Relevant
Cues
Expectancies
Yes
Seek More
Information
Reassess
Situation
No
Mental Simulation
of Action (n)
Will it Work?
Implement
Yes
Yes, but
Modify
Are
Expectancies
Violated?
Yes
No
No
Figure 1. Model of recognition-primed decision making. (Decision making in action: Models and methods. G. A. Klein,
J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood, C. E. Zsambok, Editors. Copyright © 1993 by Ablex Publishing Corporation. Norwood,
NJ. Reproduced with permission of Greenwood Publishing Group, Inc., Westport, CT.)
460 June 2008 –
Human Factors
Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for
behavioral economics. American Economic Review, American
Economic Association, 93(5), 1449–1475.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under
uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge
University Press.
Klein, G. A. (1989). Recognition-primed decisions. In W. B. Rouse (Ed.),
Advances in man-machine systems research (Vol. 5, pp. 47–92).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Klein, G. A. (1998). Sources of power: How people make decisions.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Klein, G. A., Calderwood, R., & Clinton-Cirocco, A. (1986). Rapid deci-
sion making on the fireground. Proceedings of the Human Factors
and Ergonomics Society 30th Annual Meeting, 1, 576–580.
Klein, G. A., Orasanu, J., Calderwood, R., & Zsambok, C. E. (Eds.).
(1993). Decision making in action: Models and methods.Norwood,
NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Klein, G. A., Ross, K. G., Moon, B. M., Klein, D. E., Hoffman, R. R.,
& Hollnagel, E. (2003). Macrocognition. IEEE Intelligent Systems,
18(3), 81–85.
Klein, G. A., Wolf, S., Militello, L., & Zsambok, C. (1995). Character-
istics of skilled option generation in chess. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 62(1), 63–69.
Letsky, M., Warner, N., Fiore, S. M., Rosen, M., & Salas, E. (2007).
Macrocognition in complex team problem solving. In 12th Inter-
national Command and Control Research and Technology Sym-
posium (ICCRTS) (CD-ROM). Washington, DC: Command and
Control Research Program (CCRP). Available from http://www.
dodccrp.org/events/12th_ICCRTS/CD/iccrts_main.html
Lipshitz, R. (1993). Converging themes in the study of decision making
in realistic settings. In G. A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood, &
C. E. Zsambok (Eds.), Decision making in action: Models and
methods (pp. 103–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Lipshitz, R., Klein, G., Orasanu, J., & Salas, E. (2001). Focus article:
Taking stock of naturalistic decision making. Journal of Behavioral
Decision Making, 14, 331–352.
Orasanu, J., & Connolly, T. (1993). The reinvention of decision making.
In G. A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood, & C. E. Zsambok (Eds.),
Decision making in action: Models and methods (pp. 3–20). Nor-
wood, NJ: Ablex.
Rasmussen, J. (1983). Skill, rules and knowledge: Signals, signs, and
symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models.
IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, SMC-13(3),
257–266.
Ross, K. G., Klein, G., Thunholm, P., Schmitt, J. F., & Baxter, H. C.
(2004). The recognition-primed decision model. Military Review,
LXXIV(4), 6–10.
Schmitt, J. F., & Klein, G. (1999). How we plan. Marine Corps Gazette,
83(10), 18–26.
Simon, H. (1957). Models of man: Social and rational. New York: Wiley.
Thunholm, P. (2006). A new model for tactical mission planning for the
Swedish Armed Forces. In Proceedings of the 2006 Command and
Control Research and Technology Symposium: The state of the art
and the state of the practice (CD-ROM). Washington, DC: Com-
mand and Control Research Program (CCRP). Available from http://
www.dodccrp.org/html4/events_past.html#2006
Yates, J. F., Veinott, E. S., & Patalano, A. L. (2003). Hard decisions, bad
decisions: On decision quality and decision aiding. In S. L. Schneider
& J. Shanteau (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on judgment and deci-
sion research(pp. 13–63). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gary Klein is the chief scientist of Klein Associates, a
group he formed in 1978. The Klein Associates Division
is now part of Applied Research Associates. Dr. Klein re-
ceived his Ph.D. in experimental psychology from the
University of Pittsburgh in 1969. He was an assistant pro-
fessor of psychology at Oakland University (1970–1974)
and worked as a research psychologist for the U.S. Air
Force (1974–1978).
Date received: October 1, 2007
Date accepted: February 6, 2008
... Looking for adequate solutions and courses of action is also key to managing complex situations such as outbreaks. In this context, the naturalistic decision-making approach uses the recognition-primed decision (RPD) model to describe how experienced professionals make decisions in highly to moderately familiar situations (Klein, 1997(Klein, , 2008. The RPD model describes how decision-makers are able to draw on their experience to assess situations and build adapted courses of action. ...
... These steps were featured by few ideas elaboration in relatively short periods of time. The others steps are related to different disease hypothesis, supporting the building of serial options to be evaluated until the good one is identified (Klein, 1997(Klein, , 2008. Their durations increased, as the experts conducted complex activities, corresponding to successive situation diagnosis and decision-making. ...
Article
Decision-making during critical outbreak management may require standard strategies, but also more creative ones. Our goal was to characterize the expert decision processes that take place during critical situations, where rule-based strategies and usual procedures cannot be satisfactorily applied. More specifically, we focused on the strategies experts use to deal with epidemiological problems, depending on the complexity of the situation. To this end, we carried out a simulated outbreak alert, to place two experts in a situation of epidemiological problem management, based on usual practice but also conducive to implementing creative solutions. To analyze the data, we considered not only the relevance of the solutions proposed by the experts, but also the four creativity criteria defined by Torrance (fluency, flexibility, elaboration and originality). Results allowed us to identify similarities but also differences between the solutions proposed by the experts, depending on their level of experience in this area.
... They can without a doubt lead to suboptimal decision making in many areas. However, cognitive biases are an essential part of naturalistic decision-making (Klein 2008). Eventually, when considering complex real-world environments, human biases and heuristics can be turned from a phenomenon that has to be avoided into a tool that might improve decisions, which is transferable in a certain way to artificial learning systems (Gadzinski and Castello 2020). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This paper stresses the importance of biases in the field of artificial intelligence (AI) in two regards. First, in order to foster efficient algorithmic decision-making in complex, unstable, and uncertain real-world environments, we argue for the structurewise implementation of human cognitive biases in learning algorithms. Secondly, we argue that in order to achieve ethical machine behavior, filter mechanisms have to be applied for selecting biased training stimuli that represent social or behavioral traits that are ethically desirable. We use insights from cognitive science as well as ethics and apply them to the AI field, combining theoretical considerations with seven case studies depicting tangible bias implementation scenarios. Ultimately, this paper is the first tentative step to explicitly pursue the idea of a re-evaluation of the ethical significance of machine biases, as well as putting the idea forth to implement cognitive biases into machines.
... Human factors approaches [5][6][7] have been used to characterize decision making of professional experts in real-world domains of aviation, nuclear power, military, and health care. Findings have informed models of knowledge acquisition and decision making. ...
Article
Background Limited research exists on patient knowledge/cognition or “getting inside patients' heads.” Because patients possess unique and privileged knowledge, clinicians need this information to make patient-centered and coordinated treatment planning decisions. To achieve patient-centered care, we characterize patient knowledge and contributions to the clinical information space. Methods and Objectives In a theoretical overview, we explore the relevance of patient knowledge to care provision, apply historical perspectives of knowledge acquisition to patient knowledge, propose a representation of patient knowledge types across the continuum of care, and include illustrative vignettes about Mr. Jones. We highlight how the field of human factors (a core competency of health informatics) provides a perspective and methods for eliciting and characterizing patient knowledge. Conclusion Patients play a vital role in the clinical information space by possessing and sharing unique knowledge relevant to the clinical picture. Without a patient's contributions, the clinical picture of the patient is incomplete. A human factors perspective informs patient-centered care and health information technology solutions to support clinical information sharing.
... When the holes in many layers momentarily line up, an incident can occur. The mindful action is weakened when organisations stop investing in the competence of their people, maintaining efficacy and encouraging growth (Sutcliffe and Vogus, 2003), as well as the structures and practices people become inattentive (Simons and Chabris, 1999), mindless (Langer, 1989) and lose situational awareness (Klein, 2008). In hierarchical organisations, those with expertise who are closest to the problem are not empowered to act (Weick and Sutcliffe, 2007) and people diffuse responsibility for taking action (Latané and Darley, 1970). ...
Article
Full-text available
The paper discusses the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the Italian chemical and process industries, where Directive 2012/18/EU Seveso III, for the control of Major Accident Hazard (MAH) is enforced. The Safety Management System (SMS) for the control of MAH, which has been mandatory for 20 years in Italian Seveso Establishments, has been highly stressed by the external pressure, related in some way to the COVID-19 pandemic. Fairly, most companies, in particular in oil and gas sectors, have demonstrated an adequate capability to reconcile operation continuity and health requirements. This experience is providing the establishment operators and the regulators with valuable suggestions for the improvements of the SMS-MAH. Within this framework, an innovative organisational resilience model is proposed, aiming at the development of a higher capability to face future new crisis. The current SMS-MAH already includes some basic pillars to enhance resilience, which were valuable during pandemic crisis, but a full and rationale development is still needed. Starting from the first pandemic phase experience, this paper presents a novel tool to assess the degree of “resilience” of a SMS-MAH. It is based on a questionnaire, featuring 25 questions grouped into eight items, according to the typical SMS-MAH structure. A two level AHP model has been developed, in order to define the weights to be assigned to each point. The AHP panel included industrial practitioners, regulators, authorities and researchers. The results are based on the COVID-19 experience and consequently the developed model is tailored to face health emergencies, but the approach may be easily transferred to other external crises.
... 10 Naturalistic Decision Making research explores how people make decisions in naturalistic (rather than experimental or controlled) contexts. 11 The resulting descriptive Naturalistic Decision Making models have a number of strengths, outlined in Table 1. ...
Article
Objective This paper presents the first Naturalistic Decision Making model of prehospital resuscitation decision‐making, which has clear implications for education, practice and support. Methods A mixed‐methods exploratory sequential research design consisting of interviews with ambulance personnel (study 1), focus groups with ambulance educators, managers and peer supporters (study 2), and an online survey of graduating paramedic students (study 3). This paper reports the model developed from integrated findings, across all three studies. All research was undertaken in New Zealand and underpinned by a critical realist worldview. Results The Prehospital Resuscitation Decision Making model identifies key processes, challenges and facilitators before, during and after ambulance personnel attend a cardiac arrest event. It is the only descriptive model of resuscitation decision making which acknowledges the decision‐maker, non‐prognostic factors and the importance of adequate preparation and support. Conclusions This research project is the first to comprehensively explore and model ambulance personnel perspectives on decisions to start, continue or stop resuscitation. The decision‐making process is complex and difficult to simply formularise. Education and supports must assist ambulance personnel in navigating this complexity. Where resuscitation is withheld or terminated, ambulance personnel need to feel confident that they can effectively provide after‐death care.
Article
Strategic Decision-Making is always challenging because it is inherently uncertain, ambiguous, risky, and complex. By contrast to tactical and operational decisions, strategic decisions are decisive, pivotal, and often irreversible, which may result in long-term and significant consequences. A strategic decision-making process usually involves many aspects of inquiry, including sensory perception, deliberative thinking, inquiry-based analysis, meta-learning, and constant interaction with the external world. Many unknowns, unpredictabilities, and environmental constraints will shape every aspect of a strategic decision. Traditionally, this task often relies on intuition, reflective thinking, visionary insights, approximate estimates, and practical wisdom. With recent advances in artificial intelligence/machine learning (AI/ML) technologies, we can leverage AI/ML to support strategic decision-making. However, there is still a substantial gap from an AI perspective due to inadequate models, despite the tremendous progress made. We argue that creating a comprehensive taxonomy of decision frames as a representation space is essential for AI because it could offer surprising insights beyond anyone's imaginary boundary today. Strategic decision-making is the art of possibility. This study develops a systematic taxonomy of decision-making frames that consists of 6 bases, 18 categorical, and 54 elementary frames. We formulate the model using the inquiry method based on Bloom's taxonomy approach. We aim to lay out the computational foundation that is possible to capture a comprehensive landscape view of a strategic problem. Compared with many traditional models, this novel taxonomy covers irrational, non-rational and rational frames capable of dealing with certainty, uncertainty, complexity, ambiguity, chaos, and ignorance.
Article
Artificial intelligence (AI) is fundamentally changing organizational decision-making processes. With the ability to self-learn and improve decision quality, AI is now taking over many decision responsibilities that were formally assigned to humans alone. However, the effectiveness of AI for ill-structured and uncertain decision environments is still in question. In such decision contexts that have no precedent to base a solution, humans have historically relied on their intuition to make decisions. Yet, intuition too has been found to have weaknesses that impact decision quality. Therefore, this article introduces a decision-making model that effectively integrates the strengths of both intuition and AI while minimizing the vulnerabilities of each method. The model specifies when and how both modes should be combined for effective organizational decision-making. In addition, the article presents important future research considerations relating to AI that impact both practitioners and academics.
Thesis
Full-text available
Abstract Preventing suicides is a major issue for patient safety in mental health wards. Safety is assumed to be achieved for suicidal inpatients in clinical practice when procedures are well implemented, without any gaps between practice guidelines and work as done in clinical practice. The approach to implementing safety practices assumes linear causality in which the implementation of a safety measure will yield predictable outcomes in clinical practice. While this approach can provide successful outcomes in systems that are well understood, well tested and well-behaved, it has some limitations when applied to complex and dynamic practices in which the risk is not completely understood, i.e., involving patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis. Suicidal patients are characterised by aetiological heterogeneity, and each patient needs to be understood and approached differently. Deviations from standards may be necessary to maintain safe clinical practice for patients due to their complexity. However, knowledge of the complexity of safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis is lacking. Patients and healthcare professionals are valuable sources of information about everyday clinical practice in this setting. Still, no studies have explored how suicidal patients experience safe clinical practice, and the knowledge of healthcare professionals’ experiences with safe clinical practice is limited. There is a need to understand the idiosyncrasy of safety within this context and acknowledge its complexity. The overall aim of this thesis was therefore to gain a deeper understanding of the complexity of safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised in mental health wards during a suicidal crisis, as experienced by patients and healthcare professionals. Objectives • To synthesise and describe the qualitative literature regarding suicidal patients’ experiences of safety during hospitalisation in mental healthcare. • To explore suicidal patients’ experiences of safe clinical practice during hospitalisation in mental healthcare. • To explore HCPs’ experiences with safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis. • To synthesise the characteristics of the complexity of safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis. Methods A qualitative case study design utilised multiple methods and data sources, including a systematic review of qualitative literature, individual interviews with patients, and a multi-method approach comprising individual interviews and focus group interviews with healthcare professionals. The complexity of safe clinical practice for suicidal patients was defined as the case, and mental health wards were defined as its context. Results Safe clinical practice as experienced by suicidal patients appears to be related to more than the absence of suicide risk and the need for physical protection. Safe clinical practice for the suicidal patient is highly dependent on patients’ perceptions of their connections with healthcare professionals, the fulfilment of their needs during care and their psychological safety (article I). Furthermore, suicidal patients are multifaceted, showing fluctuating suicidal behaviour, which highlights the importance of embracing personalised activities for safe clinical practice. Patients experience safe clinical practice during hospitalisation in mental health wards during a suicidal crisis, when they are being detected by mindful healthcare professionals, being protected by an adaptive practice and receiving tailor-made treatment (article II). Healthcare professionals experience safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis as dependent on using expertise to make sense of suicidal behaviour, individualising the therapeutic milieu and managing uncertainty (article III). These are examples of capacities that enable healthcare professionals to adapt to challenges and changes in clinical care, and they are vital to the complex dynamic work practices involved in safe clinical practice in this setting. Through synthesising across suicidal patients’ and healthcare professionals’ experiences, the safe clinical practice involves a set of complex characteristics: collaborative detection, adaptive protection and individualised control which all depend on systems of trust. These characteristics demonstrate how nonlinearity and uncertainty characterise the complexity in this context. Additionally, the complexity in safe clinical practice is characterised by establishing psychological and relational safety, which is only created through personalised and trusted relationships. Conclusion This thesis offers a deeper understanding of the complexity of safe clinical practices for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis by considering the experiences of patients and HCPs. The inherent complexity of safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis implies that there are unpredictable consequences of top-down safety interventions and that outcomes change over time and for each patient. Thus, safe clinical practice cannot be ensured just by following standards; it also depends on adaptations. To improve safe clinical practices, efforts should be made to embrace rather than efface variability in clinical care. This includes supporting adaptive capacities that enable HCPs to cope with challenges and changes in clinical care. Strategies should be directed toward strengthening expertise development, feedback systems, and systems ensuring support and predictability. Has parts Paper 1: Berg, S.H., Rørtveit, K. & Aase, K. (2017) Suicidal patients’ experiences regarding their safety during psychiatric in-patient care: a systematic review of qualitative studies. BMC Health Services Research, 17 Paper 2: Berg, S.H., Rørtveit, K., Walby, F.A. & Aase, K. (2020) Safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised in mental health wards during a suicidal crisis: a qualitative study of patient experiences. Submitted to BMJ open. Paper 3: Berg, S.H., Rørtveit, K., Walby, F.A. & Aase, K. (2020) Adaptive capacities for safe clinical practice for patients hospitalised during a suicidal crisis: a qualitative study. BMC Psychiatry. 20 (1): 316
Article
Full-text available
We review the progress of naturalistic decision making (NDM) in the decade since the first conference on the subject in 1989. After setting out a brief history of NDM we identify its essential characteristics and consider five of its main contributions: recognition-primed decisions, coping with uncertainty, team decision making, decision errors, and methodology. NDM helped identify important areas of inquiry previously neglected (e.g. the use of expertise in sizing up situations and generating options), it introduced new models, conceptualizations, and methods, and recruited applied investigators into the field. Above all, NDM contributed a new perspective on how decisions (broadly defined as committing oneself to a certain course of action) are made. NDM still faces significant challenges, including improvement of the quantity and rigor of its empirical research, and confirming the validity of its prescriptive models. Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Full-text available
Direct comparisons were made of expert highway engineers' use of analytical, quasirational, and intuitive cognition on three different tasks, each displayed in three different ways. Use of a systems approach made it possible to develop indices for measuring the location of each of the nine information display conditions on a continuum ranging from intuition-inducing to analysis-inducing and for measuring the location of each expert engineer's cognition on a continuum ranging from intuition to analysis. Individual analyses of each expert's performance over the nine conditions showed that the location of the task on the task index induced the expert's cognition to be located at the corresponding region on the cognitive continuum index. Intuitive and quasirational cognition frequently outperformed analytical cognition in terms of the empirical accuracy of judgments. Judgmental accuracy was related to the degree of correspondence between the type of task and the type of cognitive activity on the cognitive continuum.
Chapter
"The reinvention of decision making" Naturalistic Decision Making (NDM) provides a new approach for understanding and supporting decision makers as they work in complex, time-pressured, and high-risk environments. An NDM framework provides a perspective for understanding command decision making in complex organizations such as the Combined Joint Task Force and for supporting this decision making in a variety of operations including peace operations. This paper provides a detailed description of the field of NDM. The authors begin with a description of three different decision scenarios. These scenarios facilitate a discussion of the characteristics of naturalistic decision settings and the importance of expertise for naturalistic decision making. The authors conclude the chapter with discussions of research methods and the findings from NDM research that distinguish it from more traditional decision research.
Article
The introduction of information technology based on digital computers for the design of man-machine interface systems has led to a requirement for consistent models of human performance in routine task environments and during unfamiliar task conditions. A discussion is presented of the requirement for different types of models for representing performance at the skill-, rule-, and knowledge-based levels, together with a review of the different levels in terms of signals, signs, and symbols. Particular attention is paid to the different possible ways of representing system properties which underlie knowledge-based performance and which can be characterised at several levels of abstraction-from the representation of physical form, through functional representation, to representation in terms of intention or purpose. Furthermore, the role of qualitative and quantitative models in the design and evaluation of interface systems is mentioned, and the need to consider such distinctions carefully is discussed.