ArticlePDF Available

Do puzzle pieces and autism puzzle piece logos evoke negative associations?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Puzzle pieces have become ubiquitous symbols for autism. However, puzzle-piece imagery stirs debate between those who support and those who object to its use because they believe puzzle-piece imagery evokes negative associations. Our study empirically investigated whether puzzle pieces evoke negative associations in the general public. Participants' ( N = 400) implicit negative associations were measured with an Implicit Association Task, which is a speeded categorization task, and participants' explicit associations were measured with an Explicit Association Task, which is a standard task for assessing consumers' explicit associations with brands (and images of those brands). Puzzle pieces, both those used as autism logos and those used more generically, evoked negative implicit associations ( t(399) = -5.357, p < 0.001) and negative explicit associations ( z = 4.693, p < 0.001, d = 0.491). Participants explicitly associated puzzle pieces, even generic puzzle pieces, with incompleteness, imperfection, and oddity. Our results bear public policy implications. If an organization's intention for using puzzle-piece imagery is to evoke negative associations, our results suggest the organization's use of puzzle-piece imagery is apt. However, if the organization's intention is to evoke positive associations, our results suggest that puzzle-piece imagery should probably be avoided.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Do puzzle pieces and autism puzzle piece
logos evoke negative associations?
Morton Ann Gernsbacher1, Adam R Raimond1,
Jennifer L Stevenson2, Jilana S Boston1 and Bev Harp3
Abstract
Puzzle pieces have become ubiquitous symbols for autism. However, puzzle-piece imagery stirs debate between those
who support and those who object to its use because they believe puzzle-piece imagery evokes negative associations.
Our study empirically investigated whether puzzle pieces evoke negative associations in the general public. Participants’
(N = 400) implicit negative associations were measured with an Implicit Association Task, which is a speeded categorization
task, and participants’ explicit associations were measured with an Explicit Association Task, which is a standard task
for assessing consumers’ explicit associations with brands (and images of those brands). Puzzle pieces, both those
used as autism logos and those used more generically, evoked negative implicit associations (t(399) = –5.357, p < 0.001)
and negative explicit associations (z = 4.693, p < 0.001, d = 0.491). Participants explicitly associated puzzle pieces, even
generic puzzle pieces, with incompleteness, imperfection, and oddity. Our results bear public policy implications. If
an organization’s intention for using puzzle-piece imagery is to evoke negative associations, our results suggest the
organization’s use of puzzle-piece imagery is apt. However, if the organization’s intention is to evoke positive associations,
our results suggest that puzzle-piece imagery should probably be avoided.
Keywords
free associations, IAT, Implicit Association Test, puzzle
!
Puzzle pieces have become pervasive symbols for autism
(McGuire, 2012; McGuire and Michalko, 2011; Sarrett,
2011; Smukler, 2005). Puzzle pieces that represent autism
appear on numerous products. From t-shirts to designer jewelry,
car magnets to credit cards, and beverage holders to
smartphone cases, “the puzzle piece as a symbol for autism
… is ubiquitous” (Grinker and Mandell, 2015: 643).
Numerous autism organizations use puzzle pieces in their
logos. For example, the US organization Autism Speaks has
officially trademarked a blue puzzle piece as its logo. The
Autism Society of America has common-law trademarked a
ribbon comprising puzzle pieces, and puzzle pieces feature
prominently in the logos of many of the Autism Society of
America’s state chapters (Stevenson et al., 2011).
The depiction of autism as a puzzle piece has permeated
scientific vernacular. Research institutions have titled
their press releases “Finding the Missing Puzzle Piece of
Autism” (O’Connor, 2009) and “Missing Piece Surfaces in
the Puzzle of Autism” (CNRS, 2015). Researchers have
titled their journal articles “The Puzzling Life of Autistic
Toddlers” (Van Rijn and Stappers, 2008), “Solving the
Autism Puzzle a Few Pieces at a Time” (Schaaf and
Zoghbi, 2011), “The Neurobiology of Autism: New Pieces
of the Puzzle” (Acosta and Pearl, 2003), and “Another
Piece of the Autism Puzzle” (State, 2010). The International
Society for Autism Research has used puzzle imagery on
the cover of its annual International Meeting for Autism
Research (IMFAR) program. The conception of autism as
a puzzle piece is pervasive.
The debate over puzzle piece imagery
for autism
Common lore traces the use of puzzle pieces to represent
autism to the United Kingdom. In 1963, the logo of a weeping
child superimposed on a puzzle piece was created by a
(non-autistic) parent-member of the National Autistic
Society. Another (non-autistic) parent-member later related
_____________________________________________________
1University of Wisconsin–Madison, USA
2Ursinus College, USA
3University of Kentucky, USA
Corresponding author:
Morton Ann Gernsbacher, Department of Psychology, University of
Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA.
Email: MAGernsb@wisc.edu
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317727125
Autism
2018, Vol. 22(2) 118-125
© The Author(s) 2017
119!Autism [author copy]
that “the puzzle piece is so effective because it tells us
something about autism: Our children are handicapped by a
puzzling condition; this isolates them from normal human
contact and therefore they do not ‘fit in’” (National Autistic
Society, 2007).
In current day, the US state of Pennsylvania’s Bureau of
Autism Services claims that the “puzzle piece [remains]
the international symbol for autism” because the puzzle
piece conveys “the mystery still surrounding this condition”
(Healy, 2008: 141; emphasis ours). The US state of
Alaska’s Special Education Service Agency claims that
puzzle pieces continue to represent autism because of the
“many years [that] parents and professionals alike” have
been “baffled by this disorder” (Crawford, 2004: 2).
Images of puzzle pieces representing autism flourish on
the Internet and in other visual media. Surfing, scouting,
running, wrestling, making music, and many activities in
which autistic children engage have been represented by
puzzle pieces. Sometimes autistic children themselves are
caricatured as puzzle pieces (e.g. with the puzzle pieces’
top tab representing the children’s heads and the puzzle
pieces’ side tabs representing the children’s arms). Other
images depict autistic children with a puzzle piece missing
from their brain (as on IMFAR program covers). Such
imagery is recapitulated by typically developing children’s
conception of autistic children. When asked why some
children have neurodevelopmental disabilities, such as
autism, typically developing 9- to 12-year old children
report “there is a puzzle piece missing in their brain”
(Nowicki et al., 2014: 77).
Numerous autistic persons have objected to the use of
puzzle pieces to represent autism and autistic people. For
example, Yergeau writes in the journal College English:
Puzzle pieces … symbolize so much of what is wrong with
popular autism discourse—representing autistic people as
puzzling, mysterious, less-than-human entities who are “short
a few cognitive pieces,” who are utterly self-contained,
disconnected, and [who] need to “fit in” (Heilker and Yergeau,
2011: 494).
Brook (2016) begins a letter to the editor of the journal
Autism, which uses puzzle-piece imagery both on its cover
and in its masthead, by writing, “I will not attempt to detail
why the puzzle piece as a symbol for autism is offensive;
it is easy to search online and find many and varied reasons
from different authors” (p. 251) (see, for example, Endow,
2014; Hillary, 2013; Ised, 2012; Nakhla-Thometz, 2013;
Regan, 2015; Tucker, 2008; Wyatt, 2008).
Thus, while many autism organizations continue to use
puzzle-piece imagery, many autistic people object. Ergo,
puzzle-piece imagery stirs debate (Edmunds, 2013; Goin-
Kochel, 2016; Grinker and Mandell, 2015; Muzikar, 2015).
To our knowledge, however, no research has empirically
examined whether puzzle-piece images evoke negative
associations in the general public. That was the purpose of
our study. We measured participants’ implicit and explicit
associations to puzzle pieces, both those used as autism
logos and those used more generically.
Members of the general public completed an Implicit
Association Test (Greenwald et al., 1998), which is a
speeded categorization task often used to measure implicit
attitudes (including attitudes toward images of brands,
Maison et al., 2004). Participants categorized images of
puzzle pieces and matching images of non-puzzle piece
shapes. During test trials, the images of puzzle pieces or
shapes were paired with either negative or positive concepts.
Shorter latencies to categorize puzzle pieces versus
shapes when they were paired with negative versus positive
concepts indicated negative implicit biases. Participants also
completed an Explicit Association Task, which is a standard task
for assessing consumers’ explicit associations with brands
(using images of those brands; Koll et al., 2010; Krishnan,
1996; Spears et al., 2006).
Method
Participants
We pre-specified our sample size according to Nosek’s
(2005) extensive analysis of Implicit Association Tasks. In
each of Nosek’s analyses, an average of 200 participants
were assigned to each material set. Because our study had
two material sets, our sample size was 400 adults who
were recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk. Data
from 30 other participants were excluded: 19 participants
responded too rapidly (more than 10% of their response
times were faster than 300 ms, which requires removal
from the data set according to Greenwald et al., 2003); 4
participants had complications from either their Internet
connection or their own computers; and 7 participants
failed to provide a Mechanical Turk authorization code.
Of the 400 participants, 53.00% identified as men;
46.00% identified as women; 0.75% identified outside the
gender binary; and 0.25% did not wish to disclose their
gender. The participants’ average age was 33.76 years
(standard deviation (SD) = 10.37), 97.99% reported that
English was their native language, and 98.80% reported
living in the United States for most of the past five years.
Fewer than 1% (0.50%) identified as having an autism
spectrum diagnosis; 3.00% identified as working with
autistic persons; 12.00% as being a friend of an autistic person;
and 15.00% as having an autistic relative.
Participants were kept naive about the research hypothesis.
The experiment was titled “Concepts and Images,”
and, when recruited, participants were told that “In this
study, you will be asked to classify written concepts into
two categories and classify visual images into two categories.”
Participants were compensated US$1 for their participation
in the 10-minute study.
Gernsbacher et al. 120
Stimuli
For the Implicit Association Task, the image stimuli comprised
10 images of generic PUZZLE pieces with 10
matching geometric SHAPEs and 10 images of autism
PUZZLE piece logos with 10 matching geometric SHAPE
logos. The generic puzzle piece images were royalty-free
clip art, and the autism puzzle piece logos were taken from
autism organizations’ logos and products. The geometric
shapes, which were rectangles, ovals, stars, triangles,
rounded rectangles, and the like, matched their respective
puzzle pieces in color, texture, border, shading, and size.
All stimuli are available at the Open Science Framework,
https://osf.io/mv2zt/.
The concept stimuli comprised 10 nouns representing
NEGATIVE concepts (e.g. grief, agony, cancer) and 10
nouns representing POSITIVE concepts (e.g. cheer, paradise,
laughter), which had been used in previous studies
(e.g. Greenwald et al., 1998). The 10 NEGATIVE
and 10 POSITIVE concept words did not differ in length,
as measured in letters (M = 6.500, SD = 1.434 and
M = 6.100, SD = 1.197; t(18) = 0.677, p = 0.507, d = 0.303)
or syllables (M = 2.000, SD = 0.817 and M = 2.100,
SD = 0.738; t(18) = –0.287, p = 0.777, d = –0.129). One
NEGATIVE and one POSITIVE concept, as well as one
generic PUZZLE piece and its matching generic SHAPE
and one autism PUZZLE piece logo and its matching
autism SHAPE logo, were used to illustrate the task and
allow participants to practice during the trials that preceded
the test blocks.
Material sets
Four material sets were constructed, Generic-Material-Set
A (illustrated in Figure 1), Logo-Material-Set A (illustrated
in Figure 2), Generic-Material-Set B, and Logo-Material-
Set B, by counterbalancing two between-subjects variables:
(1) whether PUZZLE pieces were associated with
NEGATIVE concepts during the first test block and
POSITIVE concepts during the last test block (Generic-
Material-Set A and Logo-Material-Set A) or PUZZLE
pieces were associated with NEGATIVE concepts during
the last test block and POSITIVE concepts during the first
test block (Generic-Material-Set B and Logo-Material-Set
B) and (2) whether the images were Generic PUZZLE
pieces and SHAPEs (Generic-Material-Set A and Generic-
Material-Set B) or Autism Logos PUZZLE pieces and
SHAPEs (Logo-Material-Set A and Logo-Material-Set B).
The order of image and concept stimuli was identical
across the four material sets and was pseudo-randomly
arranged so that each image or concept occurred only once
within each of the two test blocks, and no more than three
trials containing concepts or images occurred consecutively.
One hundred participants were randomly assigned to
each material set and did not differ in age (F(3,394) = 1.241,
Figure 1. Example Implicit Association trials for Generic-
Material-Set A.
p = 0.294, η2p = 0.009), gender (χ2(3) = 6.028, p = 0.110,
Cramer’s V = 0.123), having an autism spectrum diagnosis
(χ2(3) = 2.022, p = 0.568, V = 0.073), being friends with an
autistic person (χ2(3) = 1.170, p = 0.760, V = 0.051), working
with autistic persons (χ2(3) = 2.103, p = 0.551, V = 0.074), or
having an autistic relative (χ2(3) = 5.018, p = 0.171,
V = 0.051).
Procedure
Implicit Association Task. The Implicit Association Task
comprised 206 trials, divided into seven blocks, as illustrated
in Figures 1 and 2. During Block 1, participants
practiced categorizing images that were either PUZZLE
pieces or SHAPEs. For participants assigned to Generic-
Material-Set A (Figure 1) or Logo-Material-Set A (Figure
2), the category label SHAPE appeared on the left and
PUZZLE appeared on the right side of their computer
screen; therefore, participants pressed the ‘E’ key (on the
left side of their computer keyboards) if the image was a
SHAPE or the ‘I’ key (on the right side of their keyboards)
if the image was a PUZZLE. For participants assigned to
Generic-Material-Set B or Logo-Material-Set B, the category
label PUZZLE appeared on the left and SHAPE
appeared on the right; therefore, participants pressed ‘E’ if
the image was a PUZZLE or ‘I’ if the image was a SHAPE.
121!Autism [author copy]
During Block 2, participants practiced categorizing
lexical concepts that had POSITIVE (e.g. cheer ) or
NEGATIVE connotations (e.g. grief ). For all participants,
the category label POSITIVE appeared on the left and
NEGATIVE appeared on the right; therefore, participants
pressed ‘E’ if the concept was POSITIVE or ‘I’ if the concept
was NEGATIVE. During Block 3, participants practiced
categorizing images (PUZZLE or SHAPE) while
also categorizing concepts (POSITIVE or NEGATIVE).
During Block 4, participants continued to categorize
images (PUZZLE or SHAPE) while also categorizing concepts
(POSITIVE or NEGATIVE), and the 72 trials in
Block 4 served as the first set of test trials.
During Block 5, participants again practiced categorizing
only images because the response keys (and the
location of the image category labels) were reversed. For
participants assigned to Generic-Material-Set A (Figure
1) or Logo-Material-Set A (Figure 2), the category label
PUZZLE appeared on the left and SHAPE appeared on
the right; therefore, participants pressed ‘E’ if the image
was a PUZZLE or ‘I’ if the image was a SHAPE. The
opposite was true for participants assigned to Generic-
Material-Set B or Logo-Material-Set B. During Block 6,
participants practiced categorizing images while also
categorizing concepts, with the response key mapping
Figure 2. Example Implicit Association trials for Logo-
Material-Set A.
established during Block 5. During Block 7, participants
continued to categorize images while also categorizing
concepts, and the 72 trials in Block 7 served as the last
set of test trials.
Participants were encouraged to rest their index fingers
on the ‘E’ and ‘I’ keys for the duration of the task. They
were encouraged to make their categorizations as rapidly
as possible, without making too many errors. If participants
made an error, a red X appeared on the screen, and
they were required to press the correct key to continue.
Explicit Association Task. After completing the Implicit
Association Task, participants completed a short questionnaire,
which also assayed demographic information (e.g. age, gender).
Two items in the short questionnaire provided the Explicit
Association Task: “When I see a shape, the first few thoughts
that come to my mind are ____” and “When I see a puzzle
piece, the first few thoughts that come to my mind are ____.”
The participants were given five lines for typing five free
associations for each of the two statements.
Data analysis
The Implicit Association Task data were analyzed via traditional
analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and planned comparison
t-tests on participants’ mean correct latencies and
error rates for categorizing images in each of the two test
blocks. The Implicit Association Task data were also analyzed
via the Implicit Association Test scoring algorithm
(Greenwald et al., 2003). For both the ANOVAs and the
Implicit Association Test scoring algorithm, categorization
latencies longer than 10,000 ms were removed. The
Explicit Association data were analyzed with chi-square
and z-tests. For all analyses, an alpha level of 0.001 was
adopted to protect against type I error.
Mean correct latencies and error rates. The primary depend-
ent variables comprised participants’ mean correct latencies
and error rates to categorize images in the two test
blocks (i.e. Block 4 and Block 7 in Figures 1 and 2). One
block of test trials provided participants’ mean correct
latencies and error rates for categorizing PUZZLE pieces
paired with NEGATIVE concepts and SHAPEs paired
with POSITIVE concepts (Block 4 for Generic-Material-
Set A and Logo-Material-Set A; Block 7 for Generic-
Material-Set B and Logo-Material-Set B). The other block
of test trials provided participants’ mean correct latencies
and error rates for categorizing PUZZLE pieces paired
with POSITIVE concepts and shapes paired with NEGATIVE
concepts (Block 4 for Generic-Material-Set B and
Logo-Material-Set B; Block 7 Generic-Material-Set A and
Logo-Material-Set A).
Implicit Association Scores. The third dependent variable com-
prised participants’ Implicit Association Scores, computed
Gernsbacher et al. 122
via the algorithm specified by Greenwald et al. (2003). A
mean correct latency was computed for each participant for
each of the two test blocks, averaging across all trials in each
test block. For each participant, a pooled SD (of each participant’s
correct latencies) was computed across the two test
blocks. Next, each participant’s error latencies were replaced
with that participant’s mean correct latency (for that test
block) plus 600 ms. Then, a difference score was computed
for each participant, subtracting one test block from the other
test block so that a negative Implicit Association Score indicated
negative implicit associations to puzzle pieces. Finally,
each participant’s difference score was divided by that participant’s
pooled SD, producing an Implicit Association Score.
Explicit Associations. The fourth dependent variable comprised
the participants’ Explicit Associations, which were
coded independently by two of the authors (JLS and JSB),
naive to which material set the participants had been randomly
assigned (the coders were unable to remain naive to
the prompt to which participants had responded because of
the nature of participants’ responses, for example, responding
“where does it fit?” or “jigsaw” in response to “When
I see a puzzle piece, the first few thoughts that come to my
mind are ____” and responding “oval” and “square” in
response to “When I see a shape, the first few thoughts that
come to my mind are ____”).
Participants’ Explicit Associations were classified into
four mutually exclusive categories: Negative Associations
(e.g. “problem,” “bad,” “ugly,” or “sad”), Positive
Associations (e.g. “fun,” “happy,” “good,” or “pleasant”),
Neutral Associations (e.g. “where does it fit?”, “jigsaw,”
“oval,” or “square”), and No Associations (i.e. the participant
left the line blank). The coders agreed on 96.13% of the cals-
sifications, and disagreements were resolved by consensus.
Results
Mean correct latencies and error rates
Participants’ mean correct latencies and error rates are illustrated
in Figure 3. As shown in the left panel, the shortest
latencies were observed when participants responded with
the key that paired PUZZLE pieces with NEGATIVE concepts
(M = 846 ms, SD = 194 ms). These PUZZLE-NEGATIVE
pairings were categorized with significantly
shorter latencies than SHAPE-NEGATIVE pairings
(M = 916 ms, SD = 214 ms, t(399) = –7.661, p < 0.001),1
PUZZLE-POSITIVE pairings (M = 889 ms, SD = 203 ms,
t(399) = –4.761, p < 0.001), and SHAPE-POSITIVE pairings
(M = 885 ms, SD = 204 ms, t(399) = –6.749, p < 0.001),
resulting in a significant interaction between image category
(PUZZLE vs SHAPE) and concept connotation (NEGATIVE
vs POSITIVE, F(1,398) = 19.16, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.046).
As shown in the right panel of Figure 3, PUZZLE-NEGATIVE
pairings were also categorized at a significantly
Figure 3. Participants’ mean correct latencies (left side of
figure) and error rates (right side of figure) for categorizing
images as PUZZLE pieces or SHAPEs when paired with
NEGATIVE or POSITIVE concepts. Error bars are 99.9%
confidence intervals (CIs) of the mean.
lower error rate (M = 6.431%, SD = 7.218%) than SHAPE-
NEGATIVE pairings (M = 8.188%, SD = 8.538%,
t(399) = –4.207, p < 0.001), although error rates to PUZZLE-
NEGATIVE pairings did not differ significantly from error
rates to PUZZLE-POSITIVE pairings (M = 6.945%,
SD = 7.705%, t(399) = 1.318, p = 0.188) or SHAPE-POSITIVE
pairings (M = 5.667%, SD = 7.632%, t(399) = 2.550, p = 0.011).
The significantly lower error rates for PUZZLE-NEGATIVE pairings
than SHAPE-NEGATIVE pairings resulted in a significant
interaction between image category (PUZZLE vs SHAPE) and
concept connotation (NEGATIVE vs POSITIVE; F(1,398) = 16.95,
p < 0.001, η2p = 0.041).
Neither participants’ mean correct latencies nor their
error rates led to a significant three-way interaction among
image type (Generic Images vs Autism Logos), image category
(PUZZLE vs SHAPE), and concept connotation
(NEGATIVE vs POSITIVE; F(1,398) = 3.511, p = 0.062,
η2p = 0.009 for mean correct latencies; F(1,398) = 2.132,
p = 0.145, η2p = 0.005 for error rates). The lack of a significant
three-way interaction suggests that participants’ negative
implicit associations to puzzle pieces were unaffected
by whether the puzzle pieces were generic images or
autism logos.
Indeed, Generic Image PUZZLE-NEGATIVE pairings
(M = 796 ms, SD = 142 ms) were categorized significantly
faster than Generic Image SHAPE-NEGATIVE pairings
(M = 885 ms, SD = 206 ms, t(199) = –7.041, p < 0.001),
Generic Image PUZZLE-POSITIVE pairings (M = 851 ms,
SD = 184 ms, t(199) = –4.708, p < 0.001), and Generic
Image SHAPE-POSITIVE pairings (M = 836 ms,
SD = 168 ms, t(199) = –5.374, p < 0.001). Similarly, Autism
Logo PUZZLE-NEGATIVE pairings (M = 896 ms,
SD = 224 ms) were categorized significantly faster than
Autism Logo SHAPE-NEGATIVE pairings (M = 946 ms,
SD = 218 ms, t(199) = –3.872, p < 0.001) and Autism Logo
SHAPE-POSITIVE pairings (M = 934 ms, SD = 224 ms,
123!Autism [author copy]
t(199) = –4.292, p < 0.001), although not significantly
faster than Autism Logo PUZZLE-POSITIVE pairings
(M = 926 ms, SD = 215 ms, t(199) = –2.226, p = 0.027).
To summarize, participants’ mean correct latencies and
error rates demonstrated that members of the general population
have negative implicit biases against puzzle pieces.
Participants’ negative implicit biases against puzzle pieces
encompass generic puzzle pieces and autism logo puzzle
pieces.
Implicit Association Scores
Figure 4 presents the distribution of participants’ Implicit
Association Scores. The dashed line at zero indicates zero
negative or positive implicit bias. Participants’ mean Implicit
Association Score, –0.137 (SD = 0.511), was significantly
more negative than zero (t(399) = –5.357, p < 0.001), indicating
a significant negative bias against puzzle pieces. The
mean Implicit Association Score of participants who categorized
Generic Images (M = –0.199, SD = 0.529) did not differ
significantly from the mean Implicit Association Score of
participants who categorized Autism Logos (M = –0.075,
SD = 0.486; F(1,398) = 5.887, p = 0.016, η2p = 0.015).
To summarize, participants’ Implicit Association Task
Scores, like their mean correct latencies and error rates,
indicate that members of the general population have a
negative implicit bias against puzzle pieces. Participants’
negative implicit bias against puzzle pieces encompasses
autism logo puzzle pieces and generic puzzle pieces.
Explicit Associations
Participants’ negative versus positive Explicit Associations
to “seeing a puzzle piece” differed significantly from their
negative versus positive Explicit Associations to “seeing a
shape” (χ2(1) = 22.00, p < 0.001, d = 0.324). Participants
produced significantly more negative Explicit Associations
in response to “seeing a puzzle piece” (50% ± 6.76%, 99.9%
CI) than in response to “seeing a shape” (33% ± 9.44%;
z = 4.693, p < 0.001, d = 0.491). Participants also produced
significantly fewer positive Explicit Associations in
Figure 4. Distribution of participants’ Implicit Association
Scores. The dashed line at zero indicates zero negative or
positive implicit bias.
response to “seeing a puzzle piece” (50% ± 6.76%) than in
response to “seeing a shape” (67% ± 9.44%; z = –4.693,
p < 0.001, d = 0.441).
Participants produced significantly more negative
Explicit Associations in response to “seeing a puzzle piece”
than “seeing a shape” regardless of whether they had performed
the Implicit Association Task on Generic Images or
Autism Logos (χ2(1) = 0.320, p = 0.572, d = 0.058).
Similarly, participants produced significantly fewer positive
Explicit Associations in response to “seeing a puzzle
piece” than “seeing a shape” regardless of whether they had
performed the Implicit Association Task on Generic Images
or Autism Logos (χ2(1) = 0.368, p = 0.544, d = 0.056).
To summarize, participants’ Explicit Associations, like
their Implicit Associations, demonstrate that members of
the general population have negative explicit associations
with puzzle pieces.
Discussion
This study examined the general public’s associations to
puzzle piece images, both those used as autism logos and
those used more generically. Using the Implicit Association
Task, this study demonstrated that the general public has a
negative implicit bias against puzzle pieces. Participants
categorized images of puzzle pieces significantly more
rapidly and accurately when the response key was paired
with identifying negative connotations. Participants’ average
Implicit Association Score was also significantly negative.
Although of moderate magnitude, participants’ mean
Implicit Association Score resembles the magnitude
reported in studies examining the general population’s
implicit bias against images of ethnic minorities (Devos
and Ma, 2008; Foroni and Bel-Bahar, 2010; Nosek, 2005).
Using an Explicit Association Task, this study also
demonstrated that the general public has negative explicit
associations to puzzle pieces. Half the participants’ negative
versus positive explicit associations to puzzle pieces
were negative, whereas only a third of their explicit associations
to non-puzzle piece shapes were negative.
Although for some phenomena, people’s explicit associations
differ from their implicit associations (Nosek, 2005);
the general public’s negative explicit associations to puzzle
pieces corroborate their negative implicit associations.
Stronger corroboration between people’s explicit and
implicit associations indicates a more valid assessment of
each type of association (Greenwald et al., 2009).
This study also demonstrated that the general public’s
negative bias against puzzle pieces applies to generic puzzle
pieces, as well as to puzzle pieces used in autism logos.
Our participants’ explicit associations inform us as to why.
Participants associated puzzle pieces with imperfection,
incompletion, uncertainty, difficulty, the state of being
Gernsbacher et al. 124
unsolved, and, most poignantly, being missing. Some of
the participants’ explicit associations even referenced negative
aspects of puzzle pieces’ physical shape with associations
such as odd shaped, jagged, and uneven.
Apart from the Wikipedia logo, the puzzle-piece imagery
used in non-autism logos usually differs from the puzzle-piece
imagery used in autism logos (see https://osf.io/mv2zt/
for the most frequent results of a Google image search).
Puzzle-piece imagery used in non-autism logos usually
comprises interlocking, completed, and integral puzzle
pieces; in contrast, puzzle-piece imagery used in autism-related
logos usually comprises single puzzle pieces in isolation
or incomplete puzzles with a piece missing, reifying
our participants’ associations of puzzles pieces as reflecting
incompletion, imperfection, and the state of being unsolved.
This study’s results not only support many autistic
persons’ claims about puzzle piece imagery but also bear
public policy implications. If an autism organization’s
intention for using puzzle-piece imagery is to evoke negative
associations, then the use of puzzle-piece imagery
is apt. If, instead, an autism organization’s intention for
using puzzle-piece imagery is, as Grinker and Mandell
(2015) report, “not meant to devalue” autistic persons
“but, quite the opposite, to celebrate them,” (p. 644) our
results suggest that puzzle-piece imagery should probably
be avoided.
Availability of data and materials
The data set and stimuli supporting the conclusions of this article
are available in the Open Science Framework repository,
https://osf.io/mv2zt/.
Ethical approval and consent to participate
Participants provided informed consent, and the experiment was
approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of
Wisconsin–Madison (protocol SE-2013-0965).
Declaration of conflicting interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
article.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Note
1. As Lakens (2013) notes, for matched-pair t-tests, the t-value
and degrees of freedom provide the appropriate estimate of
effect size.
References
Acosta MT and Pearl PL (2003) The neurobiology of autism: new
!pieces of the puzzle. Current Neurology and Neuroscience
!Reports 3: 149–156.
Brook K (2016) A response to ‘notes on a puzzle piece.’ Autism
!20: 251–252.
CNRS (2015) Missing piece surfaces in the puzzle of autism.
!ScienceDaily. Available at: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/
2015/08/150804074037.htm
Crawford C (2004) Solving the autism spectrum puzzle. Alaska
!Special Education Service Area Newsletter 62(2): 2–4.
Devos T and Ma DS (2008) Is Kate Winslet more American than
!Lucy Liu? The impact of construal processes on the implicit
!ascription of a national identity. British Journal of Social
!Psychology 47: 191–215.
Edmunds DL (2013) A puzzle piece? The need for a new symbol
!for autistics. Psychology Today. Available at: https://
www.psychologytoday.com/blog/extreme-states-mind/201305/puzzle-
piece-the-need-new-symbol-autistics
Endow J (2014) Goodnight autism puzzle pieces. Ollibean.
!Available at: http://ollibean.com/goodnight-autism-puzzle-pieces/
Foroni F and Bel-Bahar T (2010) Picture-IAT versus Word-
!IAT: level of stimulus representation influences on the IAT.
!European Journal of Social Psychology 40: 321–337.
Goin-Kochel RP (2016) Musings on the puzzle piece. Autism 20:
!250.
Greenwald AG, McGhee DE and Schwartz JLK (1998)
!Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the
!Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social
!Psychology 74: 1464–1480.
Greenwald AG, Nosek BA and Banaji MR (2003) Understanding
!and using the Implicit Association Test, I: an improved scoring
!algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
!85: 197–216.
Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, et al. (2009)
!Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test, III:
!meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality
!and Social Psychology 97: 17–41.
Grinker RR and Mandell D (2015) Notes on a puzzle piece.
!Autism 19: 643–645.
Healy EM (2008) Improving autism policies beyond the enigma:
!current challenges and future considerations for improving
!policies to support individuals living with autism. Speaker’s
!Journal 8: 141–156.
Heilker P and Yergeau M (2011) Autism and rhetoric. College
!English 73: 485–497.
Hillary A (2013) Red flags. Yes, That Too. Available at: http://
yesthattoo.blogspot.com/2013/01/red-flags.html
Ised E (2012) Puzzling people. The Standard Review. Available
!at: http://diversityrules.typepad.com/my_weblog/2012/04/puzzling-
people.html
Koll O, von Wallpach S and Kreuzer M (2010) Multi-method
!research on consumer–brand associations: comparing free
!associations, storytelling, and collages. Psychology &
!Marketing 27: 584–602.
Krishnan HS (1996) Characteristics of memory associations: a
!consumer-based brand equity perspective. International
!Journal of Research in Marketing 13: 389–405.
Lakens D (2013) Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate
!cumulative science: a practical primer for. Frontiers in
!Psychology 4: 863.
McGuire AE (2012) On the “puzzle of autism” and the incompleteness
!of autism awareness. Journal on Developmental
!Disabilities 18: 96–100.
125!Autism [author copy]
McGuire AE and Michalko R (2011) Minds between us: autism,
!mindblindness and the uncertainty of communication.
!Educational Philosophy and Theory 43: 162–177.
Maison D, Greenwald AG and Bruin RH (2004) Predictive validity
!of the Implicit Association Test in studies of brands,
!consumer attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Consumer
!Psychology 14: 405–415.
Muzikar D (2015) The autism puzzle piece: a symbol that’s
!going to stay or go? The Art of Autism. Available at: http://the-art-
of-autism.com/the-autism-puzzle-piece-a-symbol-of-what/
Nakhla-Thometz A (2013) Today on “what really grinds my
!gears”: a puzzle piece as a symbol for autism. Angry Autie.
!Available at: https://angryautie.wordpress.com/2013/06/14/today-
on-what-really-grinds-my-gears-a-puzzle-piece-as-a-symbol-for-
autism/
National Autistic Society (2007) Perspectives on a puzzle piece.
!Available at: http://web.archive.org/web/20070714093137/http:/
www.nas.org.uk/nas/jsp/polopoly.jsp?d=364&a=2183
Nosek BA (2005) Moderators of the relationship between
!implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental
!Psychology: General 134: 565–584.
Nowicki EA, Brown J and Stepien M (2014) Children’s structured
!conceptualizations of their beliefs on the causes of
!learning difficulties. Journal of Mixed Methods Research
!8: 69–82.
O’Connor J (2009) Finding the missing puzzle piece of autism.
!New Science 17: 14.
Regan R (2015) Why the puzzle piece is an offensive symbol.
!Social Skills for Autonomous People. Available at: http://
realsocialskills.org/post/115578535827/why-the-puzzlepiece-is-
an-offensive-symbol
Sarrett JC (2011) Trapped children: popular images of children
!with autism in the 1960s and 2000s. Journal of Medical
!Humanities 32: 141–153.
Schaaf CP and Zoghbi HY (2011) Solving the autism puzzle a
!few pieces at a time. Neuron 70: 806–808.
Smukler D (2005) Unauthorized minds: how “theory of mind”
!theory misrepresents autism. Mental Retardation 43:
!11–24.
Spears N, Brown TJ and Dacin PA (2006) Assessing the corporate
!brand: the unique corporate association valence (UCAV)
!approach. Journal of Brand Management 14: 5–19.
State MW (2010) Another piece of the autism puzzle. Nature
!Genetics 42: 478–479.
Stevenson JL, Harp B and Gernsbacher MA (2011) Infantilizing
!autism. Disability Studies Quarterly 31(3): 1675.
Tucker T (2008) Post-puzzle piece autism. I Am An Autism
!Parent. Available at: http://iamanautismparent.com/post-puzzle-
piece-autism/
Van Rijn H and Stappers PJ (2008) The puzzling life of autistic
!toddlers: design guidelines from the LINKX project.
!Advances in Human-Computer Interaction 2008: 639435.
Wyatt CS (2008) Logos, symbols, ribbons … The Autistic Me.
!Available at: http://theautisticme.blogspot.com.au/2008/04/logos-
symbols-ribbons.html
... It is regrettable that all of the assessed models perpetuated common stereotypes of autism. The most prevalent themes were those of the white (Brickhill et al., 2023;Cruz et al., 2024), the young (Aylward et al., 2021), the boy (Williams, 2022), the puzzle symbol (Gernsbacher et al., 2018), and the blue colour. The puzzle symbol implies that individuals on the autism spectrum are analogous to incomplete puzzles, lacking the necessary components for completion. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Avoiding systemic discrimination requires investigating AI models' potential to propagate stereotypes resulting from the inherent biases of training datasets. Our study investigated how text-to-image models unintentionally perpetuate non-rational beliefs regarding autism. The research protocol involved generating images based on 53 prompts aimed at visualizing concrete objects and abstract concepts related to autism across four models: DALL-E, Stable Diffusion, SDXL, and Midjourney (N=249). Expert assessment of results was performed via a framework of 10 deductive codes representing common stereotypes contested by the community regarding their presence and spatial intensity, quantified on ordinal scales and subject to statistical analysis of inter-rater reliability and size effects. The models frequently utilised controversial themes and symbols which were unevenly distributed, however, with striking homogeneity in terms of skin colour, gender, and age, with autistic individuals portrayed as engaged in solitary activities, interacting with objects rather than people, and displaying stereotypical emotional expressions such as pale, anger, or sad. Secondly we observed representational insensitivity regarding autism images despite directional prompting aimed at falsifying the above results. Additionally, DALL-E explicitly denied perpetuating stereotypes. We interpret this as ANNs mirroring the human cognitive architecture regarding the discrepancy between background and reflective knowledge, as justified by our previous research on autism-related stereotypes in humans.
... Chosen by non-autistic people, it is a semiotic representation of the 'mystery' of autism. The negative implications of this are borne out by studies showing puzzle-piece imagery that is implicitly associated with 'oddity, incompleteness, imperfection' (Gernsbacher et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, there has been growing interest in exploring the experiences of autistic adults through a lens that adopts emancipatory theorisations of autism. However, despite this changing terrain, autistic people remain a highly subjugated population. Research has begun to theorise a distinctive form of epistemic injustice they encounter in which they are denied access to epistemic resources by a society that valorises cognitively normative ways of being, knowing and existing. An under-explored aspect of this emerging literature relates to the experiences of autistic mothers who are, themselves, much more likely to have autistic children. Evidence suggests that they may be at a substantially increased risk of involuntary social work interventions. This study explores the nature of these experiences, drawing on interviews with autistic mothers as well as my own, lived experiences as an autistic mother. It finds that, through a neuro-normative lens of social work scrutiny, indicators of neuro-divergency in both mothers and children are considered perplexing and assigned malign meanings by those with hermeneutic privilege. This was particularly evident in social work responses to children’s difficulties in attending school, with these difficulties located in mothers rather than in exclusionary, hostile school environments.
... Figure 4(b) is a generic image of a man who is also objectivated as puzzle set with a piece of his brain being removed. To understand the symbolic social construal found here, parallels are drawn with the ubiquitous puzzle piece imagery in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Gernsbacher et al., 2018). In both dementia and ASD, the incomplete puzzle with one isolated piece may reflect a circumstance unresolved, constructing the people as imperfect and incomplete. ...
Article
Introduction This study examines the underlying visual communication strategies found in existing images of dementia in the public domain. By delineating how experiences of dementia are visualized and their consequent social interpretations, we aim to inform and advocate for a dementia-inclusive visual communication, that is, visual depictions that cultivate and represent a dementia-inclusive society. Methods The visuals were analyzed by employing the Visual Discourses of Disability (ViDD) framework that juxtaposes the “ perspectivizing-personizing” cline with the “ enabling-disabling” continuum . The data studied comprise a total of 432 images sourced from three domains, namely (1) The Star, a Malaysian mainstream English newspaper (2012–2021); (2) Alzheimer’s Disease Foundation Malaysia (ADFM) website (2019–2021); and (3) the Ministry of Health Malaysia (2019–2021) website. Findings from the visual analyses were corroborated by four representatives of ADFM in a group interview. Results There is a predominance of stigmatized images, constructing dementia as a loss and deficit, thus depicting individuals in distanced suffering. Generic representations of people through stock images, a unique focus on the hands, representations of brain degeneration through abstractions and missing puzzle pieces are also prevalent. Despite these, the interview data confirmed that the perspectivizing aspect may be necessary to educate the public on what dementia entails. While personizing images that depict personhood and actual persons living with dementia are ideal, the use of stock images may be necessary if there is a need to maintain confidentiality and observe sensitivities. Similarly, images with positive emotions are encouraged but disabling ones are equally important to reflect reality and inculcate empathy. Conclusion When capturing, selecting and publishing images of dementia, organizations should deliberate on different visual elements which evoke empowerment, advocacy, handicapping and othering implications as outlined in the ViDD framework. Any decision should only be made after considering the purposes of publications and implications such images have on the intended audiences.
Article
It is documented that more Autistic woman are becoming university students, but their experiences are not often explored. Eleven Autistic women students made artefacts (such as writing, paintings or poems) and took part in interviews to share their experiences at university. Five participants were undergraduate students and six participants were postgraduate students. The artefacts and words participants shared were looked at closely and some common themes were decided. How lecturers talk to Autistic women students was looked at. Participants said they were lucky if they met helpful lecturers. Also, autism acceptance was explored. Overall, how lecturers treat Autistic women at university is very important. Also, Autistic women students' opinions about making university a more welcoming place need to be listened to.
Article
Full-text available
INTRODUCTION: Despite the increasing global prevalence of autism spectrum disorders (ASD), there is limited information about ASD in Africa. Existing research on ASD in Africa shows that autistic children are diagnosed relatively late or not at all. The purpose of this study was to understand the barriers to an autism diagnosis and to engage key stakeholders to action plan steps to reduce the barriers. METHODS: We conducted a participatory, mixed methods study using semi-structured interviews, a survey, photo elicitation, and focus groups with 11 participants (four parents and seven health professionals) in the Greater Accra region of Ghana. FINDINGS: Neuro-paediatricians in our study reported that it takes, on average, two to six visits over two weeks to two years or more to diagnose a child. Our thematic qualitative analysis yielded three overarching themes, with barriers and facilitators for each: 1) Systemic, 2) Community, and 3) Parent/Family factors that influence the diagnostic process. The action plan of our stakeholder focus group prioritizes community education to dispel myths and encourage autism acceptance within the Ghanaian community. CONCLUSION: Knowledge about the diagnostic odyssey can help facilitate early diagnosis and intervention. IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE This research study confirmed known challenges to the autism diagnostic process. It contributed nuanced insights into the role of culture, the importance of education, and the need for community involvement in improving the diagnostic process, early occupational therapy intervention, and autism acceptance within the community.
Article
Purpose The purpose of this tutorial is to educate speech-language pathologists (SLPs) on the need for and positive outcomes associated with neurodiversity-affirming care. This primer will outline what neurodiversity is, why it is central to the provision of evidence-based practice and culturally responsive care, and introduce the reader to neurodiversity-affirming clinical practices. Method This tutorial is based on a review of literature, including academic research and written works from autistic and disability activists, existence in autistic spaces, conversations within the autistic community, lived experience as autistic individuals, and clinical experience as SLPs. Discussion SLPs specializing in developmental disabilities will provide services for children with a variety of diagnoses and co-occurring conditions. Consequently, SLPs are likely to work with autistic and otherwise neurodivergent individuals. In supporting autistic clients, SLPs may promote communication development, provide and support augmentative and alternative communication, teach executive functioning strategies, educate family members and/or other members of the support team, and more. As such, it is imperative that SLPs have an understanding of the neurodiversity paradigm, how it relates to providing culturally responsive care, and the positive outcomes associated with incorporating it into our practice. This tutorial provides descriptions of neurodiversity-affirming care principles and definitions of related terms through visuals, discussion, and fictitious case scenarios and examples. Conclusion Clinicians may use this primer to integrate neurodiversity-affirming care principles into their practice.
Article
Purpose Much of the current criminological research regarding police and the autistic community focuses on police training for interacting with autistic individuals or the experiences, fears and perceptions of parents or caregivers. Largely absent from the criminological research are the opinions and perceptions of autistic adults. The purpose of the paper is to examine perceptions of the police and police-led initiatives among these individuals. Design/methodology/approach The authors analyze data from 121 autistic adults regarding their perceptions of the police and police-led autism awareness efforts using ordinary least squares (OLS) regression. Variables of interest include perceptions of procedural justice, police treatment of autistic individuals, fear of interacting with police and perceptions of police autism awareness campaigns. Findings Findings indicate that autistic respondents vary in perceptions of the police. Prior negative experiences with police have a stronger influence on perceptions than do positive experiences. Support for awareness campaigns is also varied. Originality/value Despite high-profile police incidents involving autistic individuals, there have been no empirical examinations of autistic adults' global perceptions of the police or police-led autism awareness campaigns. The current study addresses that oversight by directly examining autistic adults' perceptions. The approach is particularly salient given the ongoing police public scrutiny surrounding officer interactions with individuals from special populations, which is largely uninformed by research centering the voices of impacted individuals.
Thesis
Full-text available
In France, the public discourse on autism is almost solely carried by parent-led organizations. However, they are now confronted with the arising discourses of autistic-led organizations. This thesis aims to answer the following question: what are the characteristics of the discourses held by French associations focusing on autism on their websites? This work analyses the enunciation of eight french organizations from a perspective of Discourse Analysis. Beyond the common features bringing together those organizations together within one discursive formation, this work highlights some significant differences between the discourses held by those two groups. Thus, if the discourses of autistic-led organizations are militant and polyphonic, we observe the beginning of an institutionalization of the speeches held by parent-led organizations. This process led the latter to conceal the dissensions existing between them, autistic-led organizations and psychoanalysis. This work thus highlights an important transformation in the french public discourse held on autism in recent years. --- En France, la parole publique sur l’autisme est presque exclusivement portée par des associations de familles. Cependant, elles se trouvent aujourd’hui confrontées aux discours de nouveaux acteurs émergents, les associations dirigées par des personnes elles-mêmes autistes. Ce mémoire s’attache à identifier les caractéristiques des différents discours que tiennent les associations relatives à l’autisme sur leurs sites web officiels. Il s’intéresse à huit associations françaises en utilisant des outils issus de l’Analyse de discours. Au-delà des points communs qui les réunissent au sein d’une même formation discursive, il met en évidence d’importantes différences entre les discours de ces groupes pourtant proches. Ainsi, si les associations de personnes autistes produisent des discours militants et polyphoniques, on constate le début d’une institutionnalisation des discours des associations de familles, qui se traduit par l’effacement des dissensions existant entre elles, les associations de personnes autistes et la psychanalyse. Ce travail de mémoire vient donc souligner une transformation importante du discours public tenu sur l’autisme au cours de ces dernières années.
Article
In this autoethnographic piece, Riki Entz writes from their lived experience of Disability in the Canadian theatre industry and clearly outlines the ableism they have experienced as a multiply Disabled person. By providing honest examples of ableism, while also searching for ways that they can fit into this industry, Entz offers grounds for reflecting on ableism and the arts while remaining hopeful for the future.
Article
Full-text available
In reporting Implicit Association Test (IAT) results, researchers have most often used scoring conventions described in the first publication of the IAT (A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998). Demonstration IATs available on the Internet have produced large data sets that were used in the current article to evaluate alternative scoring procedures. Candidate new algorithms were examined in terms of their (a) correlations with parallel self-report measures, (b) resistance to an artifact associated with speed of responding, (c) internal consistency, (d) sensitivity to known influences on IAT measures, and (e) resistance to known procedural influences. The best-performing measure incorporates data from the IAT's practice trials, uses a metric that is calibrated by each respondent's latency variability, and includes a latency penalty for errors. This new algorithm strongly outperforms the earlier (conventional) procedure.
Article
Full-text available
When members of the public envision the disability of autism, they most likely envision a child, rather than an adult. In this empirically based essay, three authors, one of whom is an autistic self-advocate, analyzed the role played by parents, charitable organizations, the popular media, and the news industry in infantilizing autism. Parents portrayed the face of autism to be that of a child 95% of the time on the homepages of regional and local support organizations. Nine of the top 12 autism charitable organizations restricted descriptions of autism to child-referential discourse. Characters depicted as autistic were children in 90% of fictional books and 68% of narrative films and television programs. The news industry featured autistic children four times as often as they featured autistic adults in contemporary news articles. The cyclical interaction between parent-driven autism societies, autism fundraising charities, popular media, and contemporary news silences adult self-advocates by denying their very existence. Society’s overwhelming proclivity for depicting autism as a disability of childhood poses a formidable barrier to the dignity and well-being of autistic people of all ages.
Article
Full-text available
Effect sizes are the most important outcome of empirical studies. Most articles on effect sizes highlight their importance to communicate the practical significance of results. For scientists themselves, effect sizes are most useful because they facilitate cumulative science. Effect sizes can be used to determine the sample size for follow-up studies, or examining effects across studies. This article aims to provide a practical primer on how to calculate and report effect sizes for t-tests and ANOVA's such that effect sizes can be used in a-priori power analyses and meta-analyses. Whereas many articles about effect sizes focus on between-subjects designs and address within-subjects designs only briefly, I provide a detailed overview of the similarities and differences between within- and between-subjects designs. I suggest that some research questions in experimental psychology examine inherently intra-individual effects, which makes effect sizes that incorporate the correlation between measures the best summary of the results. Finally, a supplementary spreadsheet is provided to make it as easy as possible for researchers to incorporate effect size calculations into their workflow.
Article
Full-text available
Three studies investigated implicit brand attitudes and their relation to explicit attitudes, product usage, and product differentiation. Implicit attitudes were measured using the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). Study 1 showed expected differences in implicit attitudes between users of two leading yogurt brands, also revealing significant correlations between IAT-measured implicit attitudes and explicit attitudes. In Study 2, users of two fast food restaurants (McDonald's and Milk Bar) showed implicit attitudi-nal preference for their favorite restaurant. In Study 3, implicit attitudes of users of two soft drinks (Coca-Cola and Pepsi) predicted brand preference, product usage, and brand recognition in a blind taste test. A meta-analytic combination of the three studies showed that the use of IAT measures increased the prediction of behavior relative to explicit attitude measures alone.
Article
Following is a brief musing on Roy Grinker's discussion of what the puzzle piece symbolizes for autism during his presentation at the 2015 International Meeting for Autism Research. In his words, "The puzzle piece is ubiquitous." It likely holds a different meaning for each of us, and this is how one autism researcher sees it.
Article
Elementary school children between 9 and 12 years of age were interviewed on what they believed to be the causes of learning difficulties and were invited to take part in the analysis of the data. We achieved this with Trochim's concept mapping approach that combines qualitative and quantitative data analyses. Study results indicated that children were more knowledgeable than expected. Although each participant gave relatively few ideas, they collectively generated a list of 42 unique statements, which they categorized into a meaningful structured conceptualization. Results showed that children were competent and reliable participants in the concept mapping process. Educational, research, and methodological implications are discussed.
Article
The article introduces a new approach for assessing corporate associations, the Unique Corporate Association Valence (UCAV) measure. The UCAV integrates the quantitative and qualitative approaches with the specific intent of capturing the advantages while avoiding some of the disadvantages of either approach. The initial qualitative and quantitative assessments of the Unique Corporate Association Valence (UCAV) measure support its usefulness as a measure of what people know about companies.Journal of Brand Management (2006) 14, 5–19. doi:10.1057/palgrave.bm.2550051