ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

Over the last century, sporadic research has suggested that people whose hand, eye, foot, or ear dominances are not consistently right- or left-sided are at special risk of suffering academic difficulties. This phenomenon is known as crossed laterality. Although the bulk of this research dates from 1960’s and 1970’s, crossed laterality is becoming increasingly popular in the area of school education, driving the creation of several interventions aimed at restoring or consolidating lateral dominance. However, the available evidence is fragmentary. To determine the impact of crossed laterality on academic achievement and intelligence, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of articles published since 1900. The inclusion criteria for the review required that studies used one or more lateral preference tasks for at least two specific parts of the body; they included a valid measure of crossed laterality; they measured the impact of crossed laterality on academic achievement or intelligence; and they included participants between 3 and 17 years old. The final sample included 26 articles that covered a total population of 3578 children aged 5 to 12. Taken collectively, the results of these studies do not support the claim that there is a reliable association between crossed laterality and either academic achievement or intelligence. Along with this, we detected important shortcomings in the literature, such as considerable heterogeneity among the variables used to measure laterality and among the tasks utilized to measure the outcomes. The educational implications of these results are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Is crossed laterality associated with academic
achievement and intelligence? A systematic
review and meta-analysis
Marta Ferrero
1,2
*, Gillian West
3
, Miguel A. Vadillo
4,5
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London,United Kingdom, 2Facultad
de Ingenierı
´a, Universidad de Deusto, Bilbao, Spain, 3Department of Language and Cognition, University
College London, London, United Kingdom, 4Primary Care and Public Health Sciences, King’s College
London, London, United Kingdom, 5Departamento de Psicologı
´a Ba
´sica, Universidad Auto
´noma de Madrid,
Madrid, Spain
*marta.ferrero@deusto.es
Abstract
Over the last century, sporadic research has suggested that people whose hand, eye, foot,
or ear dominances are not consistently right- or left-sided are at special risk of suffering aca-
demic difficulties. This phenomenon is known as crossed laterality. Although the bulk of this
research dates from 1960’s and 1970’s, crossed laterality is becoming increasingly popular
in the area of school education, driving the creation of several interventions aimed at restor-
ing or consolidating lateral dominance. However, the available evidence is fragmentary. To
determine the impact of crossed laterality on academic achievement and intelligence, we
conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of articles published since 1900. The
inclusion criteria for the review required that studies used one or more lateral preference
tasks for at least two specific parts of the body; they included a valid measure of crossed
laterality; they measured the impact of crossed laterality on academic achievement or intelli-
gence; and they included participants between 3 and 17 years old. The final sample included
26 articles that covered a total population of 3578 children aged 5 to 12. Taken collectively,
the results of these studies do not support the claim that there is a reliable association
between crossed laterality and either academic achievement or intelligence. Along with this,
we detected important shortcomings in the literature, such as considerable heterogeneity
among the variables used to measure laterality and among the tasks utilized to measure the
outcomes. The educational implications of these results are discussed.
Introduction
The term “crossed laterality” is employed to refer to people whose hand, eye, foot, or ear domi-
nance are not uniformly right- or left-sided. The idea that crossed laterality is linked to aca-
demic performance is becoming increasingly popular in the area of school education. A simple
search in Google of the terms "Crossed + laterality + right + hand + left + eye + exercises"
returns around 33.000 entries, most of them containing links to articles, fora and specialised
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 1 / 18
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Ferrero M, West G, Vadillo MA (2017) Is
crossed laterality associated with academic
achievement and intelligence? A systematic review
and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 12(8): e0183618.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618
Editor: Jakob Pietschnig, Universitat Wien,
AUSTRIA
Received: May 23, 2017
Accepted: August 8, 2017
Published: August 28, 2017
Copyright: ©2017 Ferrero et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting Information
files.
Funding: MF was supported by Grant AYD-000-
235 from bizkaia:talent, Diputacio
´n Foral de Bizkaia,
and by Programa Posdoctoral de
Perfeccionamiento de Personal Investigador
Doctor, Gobierno Vasco. MAV was supported by
Grant 2016-T1/SOC-1395 from madri+d Science
Foundation. The funders had no role in study
design, data collection and analysis, decision to
publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
centres focused on this issue. The majority of these sites establish a direct association between
crossed laterality and learning difficulties and offer information about different programmes
aimed at remediating these disabilities by restoring unilateral dominance in children. For
instance, The Institute for Neuro-Physiological Psychology (INPP), founded in the UK and now
established in more than 20 countries, offers remedial training for children who show crossed
laterality, with the aim of improving their educational outcome. It also offers licenses to use the
INPP method as an independent practitioner. Similarly, The Brain Balance Achievement Centres,
with around 100 subsidiaries in United States, offer a specific training called The Brain Balance
Program, aimed at restoring natural dominance in children with a mixed-dominance profile.
Finally, The Superior Institute of Psychological Studies offers assessments and interventions for
crossed laterality in more than 15 centres in Spain. These programs may cost up to 350 [1].
Since Orton [2] suggested that an ill-established cerebral dominance was the cause of several
disabilities, a number of interventions, such as the ones mentioned above, have been created to
ameliorate learning disabilities by performing specific physical exercises that are assumed to
restore or consolidate laterality. For instance, a popular method known as patterning [3] is
aimed at imposing cerebral dominance through a series of physical exercises which consist of
manipulating the child’s head and/or extremities in patterns intended to imitate prenatal and
postnatal movements. The method lacks any supporting evidence and has been the object of
severe criticism [4,5]. However, it remains popular in several countries [6]. Another interven-
tion based to some extent on Orton’s theory is Brain Gym
1
, an educational program originally
created by Paul E. Dennison and Gail Dennison in 1980. Brain Gym
1
prescribes a number of
simple movements intended to improve the integration of specific brain functions with body
movements. According to its authors, lateral dominance is essential for reading, writing, listen-
ing, speaking, and the ability to move and think at the same time [7]. In spite of its popularity in
schools all over the world, this intervention has been widely proven as ineffective [8].
Several studies have reported that crossed lateral people are at special risk of suffering learn-
ing disabilities, while others have reported the opposite result [918]. According to these
mixed results, crossed laterality might or might not be a risk factor for learning disabilities and
poor academic performance. In the same vein, studies addressing the relationship between
handedness (regardless of lateral dominance in other parts of the body) and intelligence have
yielded mixed and inconsistent results, although recent meta-analyses point to a slight cogni-
tive advantage of right-handers [19] and a higher prevalence of atypical-handedness among
intellectually disabled individuals [20]. In any case, to the best of our knowledge, the effective-
ness of existing interventions aimed at addressing crossed laterality has never been put to the
test in rigorous studies. The use of interventions with dubious or null evidence in the class-
room or other educational contexts can be harmful in different ways. For instance, educational
professionals and families must often devote much energy and emotion during the implemen-
tation of the interventions [21]. Many of these practices involve all the actors spending a great
deal of time and enormous sums of money in individual counselling sessions for children, for-
mative sessions for teachers and families or acquisition of reference materials by schools. This
waste of time and money involves an opportunity cost, because these resources are taken away
from effective treatments [22]. Even worse, the replacement of evidence-based practices by
non-evidence based practices can harm children [23,24].
The fact that interventions aimed at addressing crossed laterality have not been properly
tested does not necessarily mean that some of their basic tenets regarding the relationship
between crossed laterality, intelligence and academic performance have not been explored. In
fact, as previously mentioned, since the 1960’s a number of studies have tried to measure the
impact of lateral dominance on these variables with mixed results [13,25]. The goal of the pres-
ent study is to conduct a systematic review of all the available evidence to assess the impact of
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 2 / 18
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
crossed laterality on academic achievement and intelligence among students, from kindergar-
ten to high school. If a link between them exists, it would be imperative to design studies that
tackle the effectiveness of the interventions utilized in school settings. By contrast, if no link is
found, it would be reasonable to divert the available resources into testing the effectiveness of
scientifically grounded interventions.
Materials and methods
Search procedures
The present systematic review follows the recommendations of the PRISMA (see S1 Table and
[26]). On August 15th 2016 the first author (MF) conducted an electronic search on the Web
of Science entering the terms “[(preference OR dominance) AND (hand OR eye OR ear OR
foot OR manual OR ocular OR lateral OR cerebral)] OR [(cross) AND (lateralOR domi-
nance OR preference)] OR [(mix) AND (preference OR dominance)] OR [(hemispheric)
AND (indecision OR specialization)] OR asymmetry OR bilateral OR laterality OR lateraliza-
tion OR eyeness OR handedness OR hand skill” into the Title field. The search was limited
to (a) English-language articles (b) published between 1900 and 2015 (c) with categories re-
stricted to “behavioural sciences”, "neuroscience", "psychology", "psychology applied", "psy-
chology biological", "psychology developmental", "psychology experimental", "psychology
multidisciplinary" and "psychology social", and (d) research areas restricted to “behavioural
sciences”, “education/educational research”, “neurosciences/neurology”, and “psychology”.
Unpublished dissertations, reviews and meta-analysis were excluded at this stage. After remov-
ing 503 duplicates this initial search yielded a sample of 10,540 studies.
Titles and abstracts of these studies were screened by MF using the inclusion criteria C1-C4
explained below. This resulted in 36 full-text articles assessed for eligibility. Authors MF and
MAV independently read the full texts of these 36 articles to check whether they fulfilled the
inclusion criteria. Among the initial set of 36 articles assessed for eligibility, eight articles com-
plied with the inclusion criteria. Thereupon, descendancy searches of articles citing or cited by
these eight papers were conducted to identify additional studies. This resulted in 21 full-text
articles which were also read independently by MF and MAV. Among them, a total of 14 stud-
ies were selected by the inclusion criteria. A second round of descendancy searches was con-
ducted for articles citing or cited by each of these 14 papers, which led to 21 extra articles
assessed for eligibility. Among this set, 4 additional studies were selected. Therefore, the final
sample of articles reviewed for inclusion comprised 26 articles (8 + 14 + 4). These articles
[11,12,15,18,2748] are shown in Table 1. A PRISMA flowchart summarizing the literature
search process is depicted in Fig 1. Two of the studies selected were based on data that had
already been collected and analysed in other articles, also included in our selection
[28,29,40,41]. Across all the full-text articles read for inclusion, initial inter-rater agreement
was 93.58%. Disagreements were resolved by discussion and consensus between the two
researchers until there was 100% agreement.
Selection criteria
Studies were selected for inclusion in this review if they met the following criteria: (C1) They
used one or more lateral preference tasks for at least two of the following parts of the body:
hand, eye, foot, or ear; (C2) they included a measure of crossed laterality according to the
absolute or relative operative definitions described below; (C3) they measured the impact of
crossed laterality on academic achievement or intelligence; and (C4) they included participants
between 3 and 17 years old.
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 3 / 18
Table 1. Articles that met inclusion and quality criteria.
Author;
year
N Gender Age
Selection
criteria
Setting
Laterality
Academic achievement
Intelligence
Body
part
Task Reading Spelling Arithmetic
Language
Annet &
Turner, 1974
[27] [A]
224 120F
104M
60–132
months
school
population
city nursery school and
infant and junior
schools
Hand,
eye
1
,
foot
non-standardised
(
8
)
n.s. __ __ The Peabody
Picture
Vocabulary Test
The maze test of
the Wechsler
Scale
Balow, 1963
[28] [A]
302 151F,
151M
84 months school
population
n.s. hand,
eye
1
Test Harris (
8
) The Gates Reading
Readiness Tests
Gates Primary
Reading Tests
PPR-Paragraph
Reading
__ __ __ Lorge-Thorndike
Intelligence Test
Balow &
Barlow 1964
[29] [A]
250 n.s. 96 months school
population
n.s. hand,
eye
1
Test Harris (
8
) The Gates Reading
Readiness Tests
Gates Primary
Reading Tests
PPR-Paragraph
Reading
__ __ __ Lorge-Thorndike
Intelligence Test
Bishop et al.
1979 [30] [A]
147 n.s. 102 months school
population
rural general practice hand,
eye
2
non-standardised
(
8
)
Neale Reading
Ability Test
__ __ __ Wechsler Scale
for Children-R
Brod &
Hamilton, 1971
[31] [AB]
54,51,52 (A,
N,D)
n.s. 132 months school
population
parochial school hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
and the hole-in-
the- card test(
8
)
Reading of
passages
__ __ __ __
Bryden, 1970
[11] [B]
234 n.s. 84 months
(GR2)
108 months
(GR4)
132 months
(GR6)
school
population
n.s. hand,
ear
5
non-standardised
(
8
)
The Gates-
MacGinitie Reading
Test
__ __ __ The Otis Quick-
Scoring Mental
Ability Test
Clymer & Silva,
1985 [32] [A]
890 n.s. 84 months all born in the
same
hospital
n.s. hand,
eye
1
,
foot
4
Test Harris (
8
) __ __ __ The Dunedin
Articulation
Check
The Illinois Test
of Psycho-
linguistic Abilities
Wechsler Scale
for Children-R
Coleman &
Deutsch, 1964
[33] [A]
121 7F, 28M
(N)
56M (D)
26M, 4F
(D)
108.5–144.3
months (D)
120.3–144
months (N)
reading
disabilities
n.s. hand,
eye
1
,
foot
Test Harris (
8
) __ __ __ __ __
Conolly, 1983
[34] [AB]
91 29F, 62M 84–141
months
learning
disabilities
private elementary
school for learning
disabled children
hand,
eye
1
,
foot
Test Harris (
8
) n.s. __ n.s. __ __
Dunlop et al.,
1973 [12] [A]
30
(15N, 15D)
n.s. 103.3 months
(N)
118.7 months
(D)
reading
disabilities
local district primary
schools
hand,
eye
2
non-standardised
(
8
)
Neale Reading
Ability Test
__ __ __
__
Fagard et al.,
2008 [15] [A]
42
(18GR1,
24GR5)
10F, 8M
(GR1)
11F, 14M
(GR5)
72 months
(GR1)
120.4 months
(GR5)
school
population
regular public school hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
9
)
Alouette
standardized
reading test
__ __ __ __
(Continued)
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 4 / 18
Table 1. (Continued)
Author;
year
N Gender Age
Selection
criteria
Setting
Laterality
Academic achievement
Intelligence
Body
part
Task Reading Spelling Arithmetic
Language
Gates & Bond,
1936 [35] [A]
129
(64N, 65D)
n.s. 103.32 months reading
disabilities
n.s. hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
Gates Reading
Diagnostic Test
__ __ Tests of word
pronunciation
__
Harris, 1957
[36] [A]
561
(245D, 316N)
42F,274M
(D),
n.s (N)
120 months reading
disabilities
n.s. hand,
eye
1
Test Harris (
8
) n.s. __ __ __ __
Hillerich, 1964
[37] [A]
400 n.s. 60 months
90 months at
follow-up
assessment
school
population
kindergarten schools hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
California
Achievement Test
__ __ __ California Short-
Form Test of
Mental Maturity
Kirk, 1934 [38]
[B]
61 30F, 31M high school intellectual
disability
state-funded institution
for developmentally-
disabled children
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
and Miles V-
scope (
8
)
Gray Oral Reading
Tests
__ __ __ __
Mahone et al.,
2006 [39] [AB]
99 53F, 46M 36–70 months school
population
local preschools and
day care centers
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
9
)
__ __ __ Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test
__
Muehl, 1963
[40] [B]
62
(23Y, 39O)
n.s. 48.7 months
(Y)
58.2 months
(O)
school
population
cooperative
preschools
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
and Miles V-
scope (
8
)
non-standardised __ __ __ __
Muehl & Fry,
1966 [41] [B]
40 21F, 19M 60–72 months school
population
cooperative
preschools
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
and Miles V-
scope (
8
)
Metropolitan,
Achievement Test
__ Metropolitan,
Achievement
Test
__ __
Roszkowski
et al., 1987 [42]
[B]
58 29F, 29M 111 months school
population
catholic elementary
school
hand,
eye
1
,
foot,
ear
6
DKSLD (
8
) Educational
Development
Series
__ Educational
Development
Series
__ Otis-Lennon
Test
Shaywitz et al.
1984 [18] [AB]
104
(32N, 37G,
35D)
104M 132 months intelligence
learning
disabilities
public school hand,
eye
7
,
foot
7
non-standardised
(
8
)
Woodcock-
Johnson
achievement
battery
Woodcock-
Johnson
achievement
battery
Woodcock-
Johnson
achievement
battery
__ Wechsler Scale
for Children-R
Smith, 1950
[43] [AB]
100 100M 120 months reading
disabilities
parochial and public
schools
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
n.s. __ __ __ __
Stephens
et al., 1967 [44]
[B]
89 45F,44M first grades school
population
elementary
schools
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
Metropolitan
Reading Readiness
Test
__ __ __ California Test of
Mental Maturity
Thomson,
1975 [45] [A]
120 (60N,
60D)
n.s. 84.93 months
(N)
84.82 months
(D)
reading
disabilities
primary schools hand,
eye
1
,
foot,
ear
6
non-standardised
(
8
)
Schonell Reading
Test
__ __ __ __
Trussell, 1969
[46] [A]
75 36F,39M first and
second grades
school
population
public elementary
schools
hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
Metropolitan
Achievement Test
__ __ __ __
Ullman, 1977
[47]
[B]
648 325F,
323M
66–149
months
school
population
public schools hand,
eye
1
,
foot
3
non-standardised
(
8
)
Wide Range
Achievement Test-
Revised
Wide Range
Achievement
Test-Revised
Wide Range
Achievement
Test-Revised
__ Lorge-Thorndike
Intelligence Test
(Continued)
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 5 / 18
Table 1. (Continued)
Author;
year
N Gender Age
Selection
criteria
Setting
Laterality
Academic achievement
Intelligence
Body
part
Task Reading Spelling Arithmetic
Language
Witty & Kopel.,
1936 [48] [A]
[B]
200
(100N,100D)
34F, 66M
(N)
34F, 66M
(D)
120.4 months
(N)
108.2 (D)
reading
disabilities
public schools hand,
eye
1
non-standardised
(
8
)
Metropolitan
Achievement Test
__ __ __ __
Note: [A] Absolute crossed laterality. [B] Relative crossed laterality. [AB] Absolute or relative crossed laterality. (A) Advanced group. (N) Normal group. (D) Disabled group. (GR1)
First grade. (GR5) Fifth grade. (Y) Young group. (O) Old group. (G) Gifted group. (n.s.) Not specified. (F) Female. (M) Male.
(1)
Sighting task.
(2)
Sighting task and binocular task combined.
(3)
Bilateral task.
(4)
Bilateral and unilateral task combined.
(5)
Dichotic listening task.
(6)
Unilateral listening task.
(7)
Not specified.
(
8
) Behavioural tasks.
(
9
) Behavioural tasks and questionnaire.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618.t001
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 6 / 18
Definition of crossed laterality
Throughout this article, we adopted the term “crossed laterality” because it was the most fre-
quent in the literature reviewed. However, it should be noted that some researchers employ
different terms to refer to the same phenomenon, such as “confused laterality” [49], “mixed
preference” [47], “mixed hand-eye” [40] or “mixed dominance” [18]. In this regard, it is worth
mentioning that nowadays the term “mixed dominance” usually refers to the lack of preference
with respect to the same type of limb or organ, not to the combination of preferences of two
different types of limbs or organs (i.e., “mixed handedness” refers to an unclear preference of
the right or left hand).
There is little consensus on the operative definition of crossed laterality through the
reviewed literature. To address this problem, we established two operative definitions. Absolute
crossed laterality refers to always using the same opposite sides of the body while performing
different tasks with any combination of hand, eye, foot, or ear. In contrast, relative crossed
laterality refers to the marked (but not exclusive) preference for using the same opposite sides
of the body while performing different tasks. For example, a child whose dominant eye is the
Fig 1. PRISMA flowchart.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618.g001
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 7 / 18
right in 80% of the observations and whose dominant hand is the left in 80% of them would
not be considered crossed-lateral according to the first operative definition, but would be con-
sidered crossed-lateral according to the second one.
Absolute and relative crossed laterality differ from mixed laterality in that the latter refers to
an undistinguishable or equivocally determined preference for using either side of the body
while performing different tasks [37]. To continue the example above, a child whose dominant
eye is the right in 50% of the observations and whose dominant hand is the left in 50% of the
observations would be considered mixed lateral according to our definitions and consequently
would not comply with the inclusion criteria of this review. Studies that only measured mixed
laterality or that conflated mixed and crossed laterality in their analyses were not included in
this review. For instance, studies that considered laterality as a continuous variable, from
totally right to totally left, were not included [10,25,5055]. Similarly, studies where mixed and
crossed laterality were merged were also excluded [9,49,5660]. In either case, intermediate
scores could refer either to genuine crossed laterality or to mixed laterality. In addition, studies
that failed to report any measure of crossed laterality [13,6174] or that failed to explain how
they categorized crossed lateral participants were excluded [7578].
Data extraction and coding
The 26 studies included were summarized in terms of characteristics of participants and set-
ting (e.g., public school, catholic school), measures of crossed laterality, academic achievement,
and intelligence (see Table 1).
Results
Participant characteristics
The number of participants included in each study ranged from 30 to 890 (mean = 201.3;
SD = 204.8). The age of participants ranged from 53 to 132 months (mean = 104.4; SD = 24.4).
Ten studies did not report the gender of participants and one study only provided the gender
of one of the groups compared (see Table 1). Among the remaining 16 studies, 984 participants
were female (38.2%) and 1590 were male (61.7%). The majority of participants (54%) were
selected solely on the basis of being enrolled in a specific school, 23.5% were selected based on
the presence or not of reading difficulties, 5.4% were selected based on the presence or not of
learning difficulties or low intelligence, and 17% were selected based on being born in a spe-
cific hospital. In addition, 37% of participants came from public schools, 6.9% from private
schools and 5% from religious schools. In the case of the remaining 49% of participants, the
type of school was not specified.
Methods used to measure laterality
The final sample of studies included in this review used a wide variety of questionnaires and
behavioural tasks to measure laterality, independently of the definition of crossed laterality
adopted (i.e., absolute or relative). It is worth mentioning that not all the tests employed to
assess laterality were equally valid and reliable. They differed substantially in terms of the
quantity of tasks included as well as in terms of the availability of normative data. The studies
included in this review also focused on different combinations of hand, eye, foot or ear to
assess crossed laterality (see Table 1). It is worth underlining that the measure of each part of
the body comprises specific peculiarities and that the type of test employed may sharply con-
strain the results. The study performed by [79] offers a comprehensive analysis of the measure-
ment of lateral preference and an extensive analysis of the prevalence and interrelationship of
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 8 / 18
lateral preferences among the general population. Detailed information about each type of test
that appeared in the present study is offered in the Supporting Information.
Qualitative analysis
Crossed laterality and reading skill. As can be seen in Table 1, 23 studies measured
the association between crossed laterality and reading performance. Only four of them
[11,12,31,41] detected a significant positive association between crossed laterality and reading
performance (i.e., children with crossed hand-eye laterality performing significantly worse
than children without crossed laterality). Furthermore, two studies obtained significant results
in the opposite direction: Shaywitz et al. [18] found that crossed lateral children showed fewer
serious problems in reading than uncrossed lateral children, while Stephens et al. [44] found
that children with crossed hand-eye laterality performed better in reading than children with-
out crossed laterality.
Crossed laterality and spelling. Two studies measured the association between crossed
laterality and spelling performance (see Table 1). None of them found a statistically significant
association.
Crossed laterality and arithmetic skills. Four studies analyzed the effects of crossed later-
ality in arithmetic performance (see Table 1). Only Muehl and Fry [41] reported a significant
contribution of crossed dominance to arithmetic achievement. Specifically, children with con-
sistent right preference in hand and eye obtained better results than children with right hand
and left eye preference.
Crossed laterality and language. Four studies measured the association between crossed
laterality and language in terms of vocabulary or articulation (see Table 1). None of them
found a significant relationship between crossed laterality and language performance.
Crossed laterality and intelligence. Eleven studies analyzed the relationship between
laterality and intelligence (see Table 1). Only one of these studies found a relationship between
crossed laterality and intelligence. Specifically, Bishop et al. [30] found a significant association
between crossed laterality and intelligence in a small subgroup of children classified as pre-
dominantly uncrossed or predominantly crossed according to reference eye.
Other results. Together with the outcomes described above, one of the studies included
in this review [39] explored the association between crossed laterality and visual attention. The
results showed no relationship between these variables. In addition, Conolly [34] explored the
association between crossed laterality and learning disabilities. The results showed that crossed
laterality was more prevalent in learning disabled children than in normal children.
Quantitative meta-analysis
The 26 studies selected for the present systematic review rely on a wide range of methods to
assess crossed laterality, academic achievement, and intelligence. Consequently, any quantita-
tive synthesis of these studies must be taken with caution. However, to avoid relying exclu-
sively on a vote-counting approach we conducted a meta-analysis of the effect sizes reported in
these studies. Unfortunately, about half of them failed to report sufficient information to com-
pute a valid effect size estimate. The remaining studies provided enough information to com-
pute an effect size in the Cohen’s dscale or reported contingency tables that could be used to
compute an odds ratio, which we converted to a Cohen’s deffect size using the equations pro-
vided by [80]. All the effect sizes we computed focused on the comparison of crossed lateral
and uncrossed lateral participants, which means that some of the comparisons covered in the
previous qualitative review, which focused in specific sub-groups of crossed-lateral or prefer-
entially crossed lateral children, were not included in the meta-analysis.
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 9 / 18
Although we could only extract a single effect size from most studies, some of them con-
tained sufficient information to compute several effect sizes. For instance, Bishop et al. [30],
measured crossed laterality using two different measures of eyeness (sighting eye and reference
eye) and they also measured two outcome measures, reading and intelligence. Furthermore,
they reported results in a way that allowed crossed laterality to be conceptualized according to
both our absolute and relative definitions. Accordingly, we were able to extract six equally
valid effect sizes from this study. Six other studies also contributed with several effect sizes to
our meta-analysis. More information about the procedure followed to compute each effect size
can be found at the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/akg3h/.
Given that some studies contributed several effect sizes, while others were represented by a
single effect size, these studies cannot be integrated using a simple univariate meta-analysis,
because this would give extra weight to studies reporting several effect sizes. To overcome this
problem, we fitted a three-level random effects models, clustering effect sizes at the study level.
All the statistical analyses were conducted with the metafor R package [81].
All the effect sizes included in the general meta-analysis, with their corresponding 95% con-
fidence interval, are depicted in Fig 2. Negative effect sizes denote a relative disadvantage of
crossed lateral children over children without crossed laterality. As can be seen, only a handful
of studies yield effect sizes that depart noticeably from zero, in either direction. The meta-ana-
lytic effect size for all studies, shown at the bottom, is d= -0.03, 95% CI [-0.10, 0.04], which is
not significantly different from zero, z= -0.73, p= .46. Furthermore, the amount of heteroge-
neity across studies failed to reach statistical significance, Q(26) = 31.50, p= .21, suggesting
that in principle the variation observed across studies can be attributed to chance alone. Fig 2
also presents the meta-analytic average for two subsets of studies that focused on the impact of
crossed laterality on reading and intelligence. The meta-analytic estimates were d= -0.03
[-0.13, 0.06] and -0.04 [-0.17, 0.08], respectively, and in both cases failed to reach statistical sig-
nificance, z= -0.67 and z= -0.69.
A funnel plot, with all the effect sizes plotted against their standard errors, is offered in Fig
3. All the effect sizes falling within the grey contour would be statistically non-significant in a
two-tailed test. As can be seen, only studies with low precision (high standard errors) yield
effect sizes that depart noticeably from zero, while studies comprising more participants tend
to yield effect sizes consistently close to zero. Furthermore, the funnel plot shows that the dis-
tribution of effect sizes is clearly symmetrical, suggesting that the results of the meta-analysis
are unlikely to be affected by publication bias.
Discussion
The majority of the studies included in this review failed to find any relationship between the
preference for different sides of the body when performing tasks that involved any combina-
tion of hand, eye, foot, or ear and performance in reading, spelling, arithmetic, language or
intelligence tests. As explained in the qualitative analysis, some studies showed that crossed
laterality was associated with lower scores in reading [11,12,31,41], arithmetic [41,42,47], intel-
ligence [30] or other variables [34]. However, these results were not consistently replicated in
other studies and sometimes they even failed to be replicated within the same study with differ-
ent selection criteria or different dependent variables. Furthermore, the quantitative meta-
analysis shows that among those studies reporting sufficient information to compute an effect
size the average effect size was not significantly different from zero.
For instance, in the study of Dunlop et al. [12] the lower performance of children with
crossed laterality was only observed when measuring eye dominance with the criterion of the
controlling eye but not with the criterion of the sighting eye, which is the most widely used in
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 10 / 18
the literature (see the S1 File and [82]). In addition, they measured the preferred hand, using a
unique writing task, which has questionable validity for being frequently culturally biased [83].
It is also worth noting that the sample size of the study conducted by Dunlop et al. [12] is only
30 participants (the smallest sample in Table 1) and that the two groups of children (dyslexics
Fig 2. Forest plot of the effect sizes included in the meta-analysis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618.g002
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 11 / 18
and controls) came from very different educational and social backgrounds, without any
attempt to match them in terms of age or other variables. In a study specifically designed as a
replication of Dunlop et al., [12] Bishop et al. [30] failed to replicate these results with a much
larger sample (N= 147), although as discussed below she did detect lower intelligence in a
small subset of crossed lateral children.
Similar problems apply to the other three studies reporting a positive relationship between
crossed laterality and reading problems. In the study by Bryden [11] the relationship between
crossed laterality and lower reading performance was confined to boys with an intermediate
or low reading achievement in relation to their IQ. In the case of girls, no relationship between
crossed laterality and reading disabilities was observed under any assumption. In a study with
only 40 participants (the second smallest study in Table 1, after [12]), Muehl and Fry [41]
Fig 3. Funnel plot of the effect sizes included in the meta-analysis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618.g003
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 12 / 18
found that uncrossed lateral children (right-hand and right-eye) performed better in a reading
task than crossed lateral children (right-hand and left-eye) and left-eyed children. However,
due to the reduced number of children with left-hand and right-eye laterality and left-hand
and left-eye laterality, the authors could not determine whether crossed laterality or eyeness
was the outcome associated to lower reading achievement. The same problem applies to the
study conducted by Brod and Hamilton [31], who actually concluded that the poor perfor-
mance of crossed lateral children was not due to crossed laterality itself but to the fact that
most children in this sample were left-eye dominant.
Furthermore, two studies reported better reading skills in crossed-lateral children than in
control participants. Specifically, Shaywitz et al. [18] found that children with crossed laterality
showed fewer problems of a severe nature in reading. Similarly, Stephens et al. [44] found that
children with crossed laterality had a tendency to perform better in reading than those with an
uncrossed laterality pattern, although this trend failed to reach full statistical significance. As
in other studies finding a significant relationship between crossed laterality and reading per-
formance, these two studies relied on relatively small samples of only 104 and 89 participants,
respectively.
Overall, the collective weight of this evidence does not support the conclusion that there is a
reliable association between crossed laterality and reading performance. Significant (either
positive or negative) associations between both variables were observed predominantly among
the smallest studies and only in some of the dependent variables or in small subgroups of par-
ticipants. The majority of studies, including those with the largest sample sizes, found no rela-
tionship between crossed laterality and reading skills. This conclusion is further supported by
the results of the meta-analytic synthesis, as can be seen in Fig 3.
A small number of studies reported positive correlations between crossed laterality and
other variables related to academic achievement or intelligence. Muehl and Fry [41] detected
that uncrossed lateral children (right hand and right eye) obtained better results in arithmetic
tasks than crossed lateral children (right hand and left eye). However, as explained above, the
authors could not establish whether crossed laterality or eyeness was the outcome related to
poorer performance. Additionally, the small sample size of this study raises suspicions about
the reliability of this finding, bearing in mind that the other three studies exploring this associ-
ation failed to detect a significant result.
One study detected a significant association between crossed laterality and intelligence [30].
Interestingly, in this study there were no significant differences in intelligence between
uncrossed- and crossed-lateral children. Rather, the significant differences found by Bishop
et al. [30] were entirely due to the poor performance of a small group of children (N= 31) with
‘predominantly uncrossed reference’ or ‘predominantly crossed reference’, that is to say, chil-
dren meeting our relative definition of crossed laterality. Far from concluding that there is a
reliable association between crossed laterality and intelligence, the author herself concluded
that ‘this study finds no evidence that “crossed reference” [i.e., laterality] has any particular sig-
nificance.’ (p. 665). Furthermore, this trend fails to reach statistical significance when all par-
ticipants are considered, as confirmed by the confidence intervals reported in Fig 2.
Likewise, one study found that the proportion of children with crossed laterality was higher
among learning disabled than among normal subjects [34]. However, the author did not spec-
ify the tools employed to classify children as learning disabled. In addition, she did not use a
control group, but simply compared her results with the pattern observed in two earlier studies
with completely different and unmatched samples [84].
Finally, no study detected an association between crossed laterality and spelling or lan-
guage. Overall, the fact that the majority of studies failed to observe significant differences and
that the few studies with significant findings yielded inconsistent results suggests that the
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 13 / 18
significant effects obtained in children with crossed laterality are likely to be unreliable. This
conclusion is consistent with the results of similar studies that were excluded from this review
because they failed to provide an operative definition of crossed laterality or because they
treated crossed and mixed laterality indistinctly (e.g., [16,56,57,59,60,85], but see [13]). Simi-
larly, these results are also consistent with previous non-systematic reviews done in the field of
laterality [86,87].
This review also highlights the substantial degree of heterogeneity that characterises studies
of crossed laterality. In particular, there is little or no consensus on the operative definition of
crossed laterality. While some studies establish strict criteria to define this phenomenon, other
studies are quite flexible and vague or do not provide any operative definition of the concept at
all (e.g., [60]). Furthermore, the tools employed to measure laterality are quite miscellaneous
(see the Supporting Information) and, in many cases, the authors do not report indices of reli-
ability and validity for their measures [12,15,39,45]. Finally, there is great diversity among the
characteristics of the samples and the outcomes measured. Taken together, these limitations
hinder any progress in this area of research.
The results of this study have important implications for education. The lack of strong evi-
dence in favour of a relationship between crossed laterality and academic achievement calls
into question the fact that educational psychologists spend time measuring crossed laterality
and, even more, that crossed laterality should be the object of direct intervention to ameliorate
learning disabilities. These results, in turn, echo the voices that have been raised previously
against evaluations and interventions of this kind [49,86,88]. At present, there is no solid evi-
dence that justifies the adoption of interventions addressed at treating laterality by education
practitioners, let alone the use of them as a replacement for evidence-based interventions
directly aimed at the target difficulties.
Supporting information
S1 Table. PRISMA checklist.
(DOC)
S1 File.
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
MF was supported by Grant AYD-000-235 from bizkaia:talent, Diputacio
´n Foral de Bizkaia, and
by Programa Posdoctoral de Perfeccionamiento de Personal Investigador Doctor, Gobierno
Vasco. MAV was supported by Grant 2016-T1/SOC-1395 from madri+d Science Foundation.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Marta Ferrero, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Formal analysis: Marta Ferrero, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Funding acquisition: Miguel A. Vadillo.
Investigation: Marta Ferrero, Gillian West, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Methodology: Marta Ferrero, Gillian West, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Writing – original draft: Marta Ferrero, Gillian West, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Writing – review & editing: Marta Ferrero, Gillian West, Miguel A. Vadillo.
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 14 / 18
References
1. The Learning Success Blog [Internet]. California: Learning Success 2016. Available from: https://www.
learningsuccessblog.com/
2. Orton ST. Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: Norton; 1937
3. Doman RJ, Spitz EB, Zucman E, Delacato CH, Doman G. Children with severe brain injuries: Neurolog-
ical organizations in terms of mobility. J Amer Med Assoc. 1960; 174: 257–262.
4. Cruickshank WM. To the editor. Except Children. 1968; 35: 93–94.
5. American Academy of Pediatrics. The treatment of neurologically impaired children using patterning.
Pediatrics. 1999; 104: 1149–1151. PMID: 10545565
6. The Institute for the Achievement of Human Potential. 2016. Available from: https://iahp.org/
7. Official Brain Gym®Web Site. 2016. Available from: http://www.braingym.org/
8. Hyatt KJ. Brain Gym. Building stronger brains or wishful thinking? Rem Spec Educ. 2007; 28: 117–124.
9. Sulzbacher S, Thomson J, Farwell JR, Temkin NR, Holubkov AL. Crossed dominance and its relation-
ship to intelligence and academic achievement. Dev Neuropsychol. 1994; 10: 473–479.
10. Batchelor ES, Williams RN, Hill SK, Dean RS. The neuropsychological significance of lateral preference
for complex visual activities in children with learning disabilities. Int J Neurosci. 1997; 89: 241–251.
PMID: 9134459
11. Bryden MP. Laterality effects in dichotic listening: Relations with handedness and reading ability in chil-
dren. Neuropsychologia. 1970; 8: 443–450. PMID: 5522571
12. Dunlop DB, Dunlop P, Fenelon B. Vision-laterality analysis in children with reading disability: The results
of new techniques of examination. Cortex. 1973; 9: 227–236. PMID: 4744368
13. Leconte P, Fagard J. Lateral preference in children with intellectual deficiency of idiophatic origin. Dev
Psychobiol. 2006; 48: 492–500. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20167 PMID: 16886190
14. Rosa F, Ferrazoli R, dos Santos APM, Nunes K, Florêncio R, Schilling L. Cross-dominance and reading
and writing outcomes in school-aged children. Rev CEFAC. 2013; 15: 864–871.
15. Fagard J, Monzalvo-Lo
´pez K, Mamassian P. Relationship between eye preference and binocular
rivalry, and between eye-hand preference and reading ability in children. Dev Psychobiol. 2008; 50:
789–798. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20328 PMID: 18688806
16. Longoni AM, Scalisi TG, Grilli M. Lateral preference and verbal skills: A survey in schools settings. Int J
Neurosci. 1989; 44: 41–52. PMID: 2485828
17. Sappington J, Topolski R. Maths performance as a function of sex, laterality, and age of pubertal onset.
Laterality. 2005; 10: 369–379. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500442000157 PMID: 16020373
18. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, McGraw K, Groll S. Current status of the neuromaturational examination,
as an index of learning disability. J Pediatr. 1984; 104: 819–825. PMID: 6726512
19. Somers M, Aukes MF, Ophoff RA, Boks MP, Fleer W, de Visser K(C) L, et al. On the relationship
between degree of hand-preference and degree of language lateralization. Brain Lang. 2015; 144: 10–
15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.03.006 PMID: 25880901
20. Papadatou-Pastou M, Tomprou D-M. Intelligence and handedness: Meta-analyses of studies on intel-
lectually disabled, typically developing, and gifted individuals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015; 56: 151–
165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.017 PMID: 26143509
21. Novella S. Psychomotor Patterning. Connect Skeptic. 1996; 1: 6.
22. Busso DS, Pollack C. No brain left behind: Consequences of neuroscience discourse for education.
Learn Media Technol. 2014 Apr 22. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439884.2014.908908
23. Lilienfeld SO. Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007; 2: 53–70. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00029.x PMID: 26151919
24. Pasquinelli E. Neuromyths: Why do they exist and persist? Mind Brain Educ. 2012; 6: 89–96.
25. Di Nuovo S, Buono S. Cognitive correlates of laterality in mental retardation. Percept Motor Skill. 2003;
96: 400–402.
26. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement
for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions:
Explanation and elaboration. Brit Med J. 2009; 339: b2700. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2700 PMID:
19622552
27. Annett M, Turner A. Laterality and the growth of intellectual abilities. Brit J Edu Psychol.1974; 44: 37–
46.
28. Balow IH. Lateral dominance characteristics and reading achievement in the first grade. J Psy-
chol.1963; 55: 323–328. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1963.9916624 PMID: 13966225
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 15 / 18
29. Balow IH, Balow B. Lateral dominance characteristics and reading achievement in the second grade.
Am Educ Res J. 1964; 1: 139–143.
30. Bishop DVM, Jancey C, Steel AMcP. Orthoptic status and reading disability. Cortex.1979; 15: 659–666.
PMID: 548227
31. Brod N, Hamilton D. Monocular-binocular coordination vs. eye-hand dominance as a factor in reading
performance. Amer J Opt Arch Am A.1971; 48: 123–9.
32. Clymer PE, Silva PA. Laterality, cognitive ability and motor performance in a sample of seven years old.
J Hum Movement Stud. 1985; 11: 59–68.
33. Coleman RI, Deutsch CP. Lateral dominance and right-left discrimination: A comparison of normal and
retarded readers. Percept Motor Skill. 1964; 19: 43–50.
34. Conolly BH. Lateral dominance in children with learning disabilities. Phys Ther. 1983; 63: 183–187.
PMID: 6823468
35. Gates AI, Bond GL. Relation of handedness, eye-sighting and acuity dominance to reading. J Educ Psy-
chol.1936; 27: 450–456.
36. Harris AJ. Lateral dominance, directional confusion, and reading disability. J Psychol. 1957; 44: 283–
294.
37. Hillerich RL. Eye-hand dominance and reading achievement. Am Educ Res J. 1964; 1: 121–126.
38. Kirk SA. A study of the relation of ocular and manual preference to mirror reading. Pedagog Semin J
Gen. 1934; 44: 192–205.
39. Mahone EM, Wodka EL, Hiemenz JR. Hand and eye preference and their association with task
approach by preschoolers. Percept Motor Skill. 2006; 102: 691–702.
40. Muehl S. Relation between word-recognition errors and hand-eye preference in preschool children. J
Educ Psychol. 1963; 54: 316–321.
41. Muehl S, Fry M. The relation between hand-eye preference and first-grade reading: A follow-up study.
CEC Convention, Toronto. 1966. 8–12.
42. Roszkowsky MJ, Snelbecker GE. Peripheral laterality, field independence, and academic achievement:
A reexamination of their interrelationship. Dev Neuropsychol. 1987; 3: 53–65.
43. Smith LC. A study of laterality characteristics of retarded readers and reading achievers. J Exp Educ.
1950; 18: 321–329.
44. Stephens WE, Cunningham ES, Stigler BJ. Reading readiness and eye hand preference patterns in 1st
grade children. Except Children. 1967; 33: 481–488.
45. Thomson M. Laterality and reading attainment. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1975; 45: 317–321. PMID:
1239296
46. Trussell EM. Relation of performance of selected physical skills to perceptual aspects of reading readi-
ness in elementary school children. Res Quart. 1969; 40: 383–390.
47. Ullman DG. Children’s lateral preference patterns: Frequency and relationships with achievement and
intelligence. J School Psychol. 1977; 15: 36–43.
48. Witty PA, Kopel D. Sinistral and mixed manual-ocular behaviour in reading disability. J Educ Psy-
chol.1936; 27: 119–134.
49. Sabatino DA, Becker JT. Relationship between lateral preference and selected behavioural variables
for children failing academically. Child Dev. 1971; 42: 2055–2060. PMID: 5146029
50. Boake C, Salmon PG, Carbone G. Torque, lateral preference, and cognitive ability in primary-grade chil-
dren. J Abnorm Child Psych. 1983; 11: 77–84.
51. Dean RS, Schwartz NH, Smith LS. Lateral preference patterns as a discriminator of learning difficulties.
J Consult Clin Psych. 1981; 49: 227–235.
52. Kershner JR. Lateralization in normal 6-years-old as related to later reading disability. Dev Psychobiol.
1978; 11: 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420110404 PMID: 669056
53. Schwartz NH, Dean RS. Laterality preference patterns of learning disabled children. Percept Motor
Skill. 1978; 47: 869–870.
54. Kraft RH, Hsia TC, Roberts T, Hallum A. Reading comprehension performance and laterality: Evidence
for concurrent validity of dichotic, dichhaptic and EGG laterality measures. Neuropsychologia. 1978; 25:
817–827.
55. Lewandowski LJ, Kohlbrenner R. Lateralization in gifted children. Dev Neuropsychol. 1985; 1: 277–282.
56. Belmont L., & Birch H.G. (1965). Lateral dominance, lateral awareness, and reading disability. Child
Development. 1965; 36: 57–71. PMID: 14300076
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 16 / 18
57. Guyer BL, Friedman MP. Hemispheric processing and cognitive styles in learning-disabled and normal
children. Child Dev. 1975; 46: 658–668. PMID: 1157603
58. Kraft RH. Laterality and school achievement: Interactions between familial handedness and assessed
laterality. Percept Motor Skill. 1985; 61: 1147–1156.
59. Richardson JTE, Firlej MDE. Laterality and reading attainment. Cortex. 1979; 15: 581–595. PMID:
548222
60. Wheeler TJ, Watkins EJ, McLaughlin SP. Reading retardation and cross-laterality in relation to short-
term information processing tasks. Brit J Educ Psychol. 1977; 47: 126–131. PMID: 880274
61. Annett M, Kilshaw D. Mathematical ability and lateral asymmetry. Cortex. 1982; 18: 547–568. PMID:
7166042
62. Annett M, Kilshaw D. Lateral preference and skill in dyslexics: Implications of the right shift theory. J
Child Psychol Psy. 1984; 25: 357–377.
63. Annett M, Manning M. Reading and a balance polymorphism for laterality and ability. J Child Psychol
Psy. 1990; 31: 511–529.
64. Annett M, Manning M. Arithmetic and laterality. Neuropsychologia. 1990; 28: 61–69. PMID: 2314564
65. Fenell EB, Satz P, Morris R. The development of handedness and dichotic ear listening asymmetries in
relation to school achievement: A longitudinal study. J Exp Child Psychol. 1983; 35: 248–262. PMID:
6842130
66. Gordon HW. Cognitive Asymmetry in dyslexic families. Neuropsychologia. 1980; 18: 645–656. PMID:
7465024
67. Hunter EJ, Johnson LC. Developmental and psychological differences between readers and nonread-
ers. J Learn Disabil. 1971; 10: 572–577.
68. Kraft RH. The effect of sex, laterality and familial handedness on intellectual abilities. Neuropsychologia.
1983; 21: 79–89. PMID: 6843819
69. Lewandowski LJ, Kobus DA. Perceptual assymetries of learning disabled children. International J Clin
Neuropsychol. 1986; 8: 173–177.
70. Sawyer CE, Brown BJ. Laterality and intelligence in relation to reading ability. Educ Rev. 1977; 29: 81–
86.
71. Shakouri N, Maftoon P. On the relationship between brain laterality and language proficiency in L2: A
replication study. BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience. 2016; 7, ISSN
2067-3957.
72. Shearer E. Physical skill and reading backwardness. Educ Res. 1968; 10: 197–206.
73. Stein GM, Gibbons RD, Meldman MJ. Lateral eye movement and handedness as measures of func-
tional brain asymmetry in learning disability. Cortex. 1980; 16: 223–229. PMID: 7471763
74. Zurif EB, Carson G. Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance and temporal analysis. Neuropsycholo-
gia. 1970; 8: 351–361. PMID: 4941969
75. Koos EM. Manifestations of cerebral dominance and reading retardation in primary grade children. J
Genet Psychol. 1964; 104: 155–165. PMID: 14128958
76. Silver AA, Hagin RA. Specific reading disability: Delineation of the syndrome and relationship to cere-
bral dominance. Compr Psychiat. 1960; 1: 126–134. PMID: 14446733
77. Silver AA, Hagin RA. Maturation of perceptual functions in children with specific reading disability. Read
Teach. 1966; 19: 253–259.
78. Wussler SM, Barclay A. Cerebral dominance, psycholinguistic skills and reading disability. Percept
Motor Skill. 1970; 31: 419–425.
79. Tran US, Stieger S, Voracek M. Evidence for general right-, mixed-, and left-sidedness in self-reported
handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness, and a primacy of footedness in a large-sample
latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2014; 62: 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2014.07.027 PMID: 25093966
80. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. 2009. Chichester,
UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
81. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 2010; 36: 1–
48.
82. Mapp AP, Ono H, Barbeito R. What does the dominant eye dominate? A brief and somewhat conten-
tious review. Percept Psychophys. 2003; 65: 310–317. PMID: 12713246
83. Hardyck C, Petrinovich LF. Left-handedness. Psychol Bull. 1977; 84: 385–404. PMID: 859955
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 17 / 18
84. Belmont L, Birch HG. Lateral dominance and right-left awareness in normal children. Child Dev. 1963;
34: 257–270. PMID: 13967185
85. Wolfe LS. Differential factors in specific reading disability: I. Laterality of function. Pedagog Semin J
Gen. 1941; 58: 45–5.
86. Obrzut JE, Boliek CA. Lateralization characteristics in learning disabled children. Journal of Learning
Disabilities. 1986; 19: 308–314. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221948601900511 PMID: 3519812
87. Touwen BCL. Laterality and Dominance. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1972; 14: 747–755. PMID: 4566108
88. Annett M. Dyslexia and handedness: Developmental phonological and surface dyslexias are associated
with different biases for handedness. Percept Motor Skill. 2011; 112: 417–425.
Crossed laterality, academic achievement and intelligence
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183618 August 28, 2017 18 / 18

Supplementary resources (2)

... However, a meta-analysis by Bourassa, MacManus & Bryden (1996) with 54,087 participants from 47 studies on hand-eye laterality did not find enough evidence to associate hand-eye laterality with learning and indicated the necessity of conducting more research in the field. In a more recent systematic review, Ferrero, West & Vadillo (2017) also found a lack of scientific evidence on the relationship between C-HELPs, academic achievement, and intelligence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Laterality effects on sports performance have been a field of interest for the sports sciences, especially in asymmetrical sports, which require the preferential use of one side of the body. Some sports in particular involve the visual system and ocular laterality, due to the need to clearly focus on a dynamic object (ball, opponent, projectile, etc. ). The relationship between manual and ocular laterality results in two perceptual-motor profiles, one where the dominant hand and eye are ipsilateral (uncrossed hand-eye laterality profile, UC-HELP), and the other where they are contralateral (crossed hand-eye laterality profile, C-HELP). Methodology A systematic review of the literature was carried out to determine the prevalence of hand-eye laterality profiles in the different sports modalities and their relationship with psychological factors and sports performance. Searches of PsycInfo, Medline, Scopus and grey literature identified 14 studies (2,759 participants) regarding hand-eye laterality in sports that met the eligibility criteria. Results Previous studies have estimated that between 10–30% of the general population exhibit a C-HELP, and 70–90% have an UC-HELP. The results of the reviewed studies indicate that in some sports the percentage of C-HELP is higher in regular and high-level athletes than in the normal population: golf (52.55%), soccer (53%), tennis (42%) and team sports (50.7%). In target sports (archery and shooting) athletes with an UC-HELP seem to have an advantage given the significant concentration of this profile in the highest performing populations (82.3%). In basketball, cricket and golf, the literature reviewed also reported biomechanical differences in the execution of some techniques between the two profiles. We did not find any study in our review that related hand-eye laterality with cognitive, tactical, or psychological aspects of athletes. Conclusions These results should be taken with great caution due to the potential bias linked to the methodologies used in the investigations, the heterogeneity in the assessment of hand-eye laterality, the few studies available on the subject and the indirect nature of many of the observed relationships between performance and laterality. For further investigation, we propose a standardized terminology and protocol of hand-eye laterality assessment in sports. The advancement in knowledge about hand-eye laterality profiles, along with the study of the relationship with psychological or tactical-sports patterns, can contribute to more effective development plans for athletes and can be a complement to talent detection.
... Their students found it hard to differentiate between some concepts and their hand, eye, foot, or ear dominance were not consistently right-or left-sided (crossed laterality). This crossed laterality was discovered around forty years ago and affects the organization of the upper functions in our system, this disorder affects language and mathematics learning, analytic, logical, understanding and concentration skills, time-space perception and balance, among others [23]. Thus, the goal of one activity is for the student to associate concepts and the other one is to improve the laterality issue. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Interaction is a fundamental part of using any computer system but it is still an issue for people with special needs. In order to improve this situation, this paper describes a new device-interaction model based on adaptation rules for user models. The aim is the adaptation at the interaction level, taking into account the interaction device features in order to improve the usability through the user experience in the education sector. In the evaluation process, several students from a special education center have participated. These students have either a physical or sensory disability or autism. The results are promising enough to consider that this model will be able to help students with disabilities to interact with a computer system which will inevitably provide tremendous benefits to their academic and personal development.
... To compare the prevalence of crossed laterality, study participants were grouped based on using the opposite sides of the body while performing different tasks with any combination of hand, eye, foot, or ear [14]. We considered three used definitions of crossed laterality. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to assess the hypothesis that functional laterality features are associated with scoliosis incidence. The study included 59 patients with radiologically confirmed idiopathic scoliosis (mean age 13 years, 41 girls and 18 boys) and 55 controls (mean age 10.5 years, 38 girls and 17 boys). Side dominance was determined by the Lateral Preference Inventory. Direction, strength, and consistency of lateral dominance was obtained. Continuous data were compared by Student’s t-test or U Mann-Whitney test where appropriate. Categorical data were compared by chi-squared test and Fisher’s exact test. Groups were significantly different in terms of age (p < 0.001) and dependent variables: height (p < 0.001) and weight (p < 0.001). Lateralization analysis showed some trends, but the results obtained were not statistically significant. Statistical significance of lateralization direction are respectively: for hand (p = 0.364); leg (p = 0.277); eye (p = 0.804); ear (p = 0.938); number of right/left sided participants p = 0.492; p = 0.274; p = 0.387; p = 0.839, and right/mixed/left sided participants p = 0.930; p = 0.233; p = 0.691; p = 0.804. For laterality consistency depending on definition used, p = 0.105; p = 0.108; p = 0.380. The relationship between scoliosis and laterality is not a simple causal relationship and needs further investigation.
... 12,13 Some clinical studies have reported compromised brain lateralization in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 14 and there has been a growing interest on the link between crossed laterality (which refers to people whose hand, eye, foot, or ear dominance are not uniformly right-or left-sided) and academic achievement. 15,16 Although the available evidence is fragmentary, lateralityspecific training and rehabilitation are commonly used in the field of education in children with developmental dyslexia and learning disabilities. 17,18 On the other hand, there is evidence in the literature of an association between laterality in permanent tooth eruption and functional lateralities (eyedness, handedness and footedness), 19,20 suggesting that tooth eruption patterns may be indicators of handedness. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives To assess whether the order of permanent tooth eruption may be a useful indicator of motor function laterality. Methods We conducted a cross-sectional study in schoolchildren aged 6–8 years old evaluated in the annual school-based routine dental health examinations conducted by the staff of the primary care centre of an urban district in Barcelona, Spain. We also evaluated hand, foot, eye, and auditory lateralities using a battery of simple exercises. Bivariate and multivariate analysis of data was performed. Results The study sample included 388 children, 51.3% female, with a mean age of 6.5 years. Right laterality was the predominant side in every variable under study, especially in tooth eruption (310 children; 80%), handedness (349; 89.9%), and footedness (337; 86.8%). In the bivariate analysis, we found a statistically significant association of tooth eruption laterality with handedness and footedness, and of tooth eruption laterality with ocular and auditory lateralities (p < .001). In the multivariate analysis, tooth eruption laterality and foot laterality were independent variables significantly associated with hand laterality. The diagnostic accuracy of tooth eruption laterality and foot laterality in relation to hand laterality as reference, showed a similar sensitivity and positive and negative predictive values, but the specificity of dentition laterality was higher (79% versus 66%). Conclusions Laterality in the order of dental eruption is a useful indicator of right or left motor function laterality in developing individuals that may be particularly helpful to determine the main dominance in cases of crossed laterality.
... Aunque el papel de la lateralidad de los hemisferios cerebrales es objeto de debate, se ha sugerido que una asimetría cerebral inadecuada podría afectar a las capacidades de aprendizaje normales, independientemente de su asociación con los trastornos del comportamiento 12,13 . Algunos estudios clínicos han descrito alteraciones de la lateralidad cerebral en niños con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad 14 y hay un interés creciente en la relación entre la lateralidad cruzada (término que hace referencia a aquellos individuos en los que no se observa una dominancia uniforme izquierda o derecha en la lateralidad de mano, ojo, pie y oído) y el desempeño académico 15,16 . Aunque la evidencia disponible es fragmentaria, en el campo de la educación de utilizan actividades específicas para entrenar y rehabilitar la lateralidad en niños con dislexia del desarrollo y trastornos del aprendizaje 17 Por otro lado, se ha descrito la asociación entre la lateralidad en la erupción dental y las dominancias funcionales (ojo, mano y pie) en la literatura 19,20 , lo que sugiere que los patrones de erupción dental podrían ser indicadores de la lateralidad manual 21 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Objetivos Evaluar si el orden de la erupción dental es un buen indicador de la lateralidad motora. Métodos Estudio transversal en escolares de ambos sexos de 6 a 8 años tratados mediante las revisiones orales rutinarias anuales realizadas en los colegios incluidos en un área de atención primaria urbana en Barcelona (España). También se evaluaron las lateralidades de manos, pies, ojos y oídos mediante una serie de ejercicios simples. Se realizaron análisis bivariantes y multivariantes de los datos. Resultados La muestra comprendió 388 escolares, 51,3% niñas, con una edad media de 6,5 años. La lateralidad derecha predominó en todas las variables de estudio, especialmente en la dentición (310 escolares; 80%), la mano (349; 89,9%) y el pie (337; 86.8%). En el estudio bivariante se observó una asociación estadísticamente significativa (p < 0,001) entre la lateralidad de la dentición y la de la mano y el pie, así como entre la lateralidad de la dentición y las lateralidades de oído y de ojo. En el estudio multivariante, las lateralidades de la dentición y del pie se asociaron significativamente a la lateralidad de la mano. En lo concerniente a la precisión de la lateralidad de la dentición y del pie como prueba diagnóstica de la lateralidad de la mano, ambas mostraron una sensibilidad y valores predictivos positivos y negativos similares, pero la especificidad de la lateralidad de la dentición fue mayor (79% vs. 66%). Conclusiones La lateralidad en el orden de la erupción dental es un buen indicador para determinar la lateralidad motora durante el desarrollo, que podría ser particularmente útil para ayudar a precisar la lateralidad más predominante en casos de lateralidad cruzada.
... Their students found it hard to differentiate between some concepts and their hand, eye, foot, or ear dominance were not consistently right-or left-sided (crossed laterality). This crossed laterality was discovered around forty years ago and affects the organization of the upper functions in our system, this disorder affects language and mathematics learning, analytic, logical, understanding and concentration skills, time-space perception and balance, among others [23]. Thus, the goal of one activity is for the student to associate concepts and the other one is to improve the laterality issue. ...
Article
Full-text available
Interaction is a fundamental part of using any computer system but it is still an issue for people with special needs. In order to improve this situation, this paper describes a new device-interaction model based on adaptation rules for user models. The aim is the adaptation at the interaction level, taking into account the interaction device features in order to improve the usability through the user experience in the education sector. In the evaluation process, several students from a special education center have participated. These students have either a physical or sensory disability or autism. The results are promising enough to consider that this model will be able to help students with disabilities to interact with a computer system which will inevitably provide tremendous benefits to their academic and personal development.
... Crossed laterality is the phenomenon of individuals who do not exhibit hand, eye, foot or ear dominance consistently as right-or left-sided. Despite some initial findings, it has been affirmed that this phenomenon does not entail impairments in academic achievement or intelligence [61]. However, the consequences for motor functions have not been extensively studied. ...
Article
Full-text available
The behavioral preference for the use of one side of the body starts from pre-natal life and prompt humans to develop motor asymmetries. The type of motor task completed influences those functional asymmetries. However, there is no real consensus on the occurrence of handedness during developmental ages. Therefore, we aimed to determine which motor asymmetries emerged differently during childhood. A total sample of 381 children in grades 1 to 5 (6-11 years old) of primary school were recruited and tested for two fine coordination tasks (Floppy, led by dexterity, and Thumb, led by speed-dominated skills) and handgrip strength (HS). Data about their handedness, footedness and sports participation were also collected. Children performed better with their dominant side, especially for the Floppy and HS tests. The asymmetries were more marked in right-handed children and did not differ by age, gender or type of sport. Our findings support the thesis of a functional lateralization in complex coordinative tasks and in maximal strength during developmental ages. Furthermore, our findings extend the evidence of a stronger lateralization in right-handed individuals, demonstrating it at a functional level in primary school children performing motor tasks. Fine motor skills allow a "fine" understanding of developmental trajectories of lateralized behavior.
Article
Full-text available
Az elmúlt évtizedekben pedagógusok, gyógypedagógusok és pszichológusok körében egyaránt elterjedt az a nézet, mely szerint a keresztezett lateralitás, vagyis a páros végtagok és érzékszervek "nem egységes" lateralizációja kóros jelenség, amelynek vizsgálata segítheti kognitív és tanulási zavarok szűrését. Mennyiben támasztják alá ezt a feltevést az említett szervek működésével kapcsolatos korszerű ismereteink? Mik a neurobiológiai alapjai az emberi testben kialakuló aszimmetriáknak? Milyen összefüggés van a keresztezett lateralitás és a tanulási zavarok között? Áttekintő tanulmányomban ezeket a kérdéseket szeretném körüljárni nemzetközi szakirodalmi adatokra támaszkodva.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Learning is a complex and multi-dimensional process that depends on cognitive and emotional processes and the development of functional systems of the human body. One type of learning is motor learning. Its course and effects depend on many factors. One of the factors may be lateralization. Differences between intellectual and motor learning tend to be blurred when learning a complex motor activity takes place. Purpose The cognitive aim of the study is to assessment of the effectiveness (speed, efficiency, durability) of learning complex motor activity in relation to the selected aspect of physical development (the model of manual dexterity) of a selected group of girls and boys. Materials and methods: The study was conducted among 73 children (44 boys and 29 girls) aged 9 - 10 years. Authors used the pedagogical experiment method and the research tool was the program for learning how to juggle three tennis balls. To assess the selected aspects of physical development we chose the direct categorized observation method. As research tools Authors used the Vienna Test System (VTS) Results: Most of the examined girls and boys were characterized by an established model of manualness.Subjects with a fixed model of manualness learned faster and more effectively than people with an undetermined model of manualness. The examined girls with a fixed model of manuality achieved more lasting learning outcomes than girls with an unspecified model. Such a relationship was not observed in the group of boys. Conclusions: Boys and girls who are characterized by a fixed model of handedness achieve a higher effectiveness of learning a complex motor activity.
Thesis
Ce travail de recherche vise à rapprocher la psychomotricité de l'accès au langage écrit, tant dans une perspective développementale que pathologique, afin de formuler des hypothèses quant à de nouvelles pratiques pédagogiques et préventives (pour permettre de faciliter l'apprentissage de la lecture chez l'apprenti-lecteur), mais également rééducatives (afin de mieux remédier à ses troubles). L'approche théorique permettra de mettre en lien un domaine psychomoteur précis, les compétences temporelles (incluant les compétences rythmiques), avec la conscience phonologique et la lecture, mais également avec les fonctions cognitives et exécutives (mémoire et inhibition) communes à l'accès au langage écrit et aux compétences temporelles. En conséquence, les effets d'une intervention psychomotrice, basée sur le développement des compétences temporelles, sur l'accès au langage écrit, à la fois chez des enfants apprenti-lecteurs (protocole 1) et chez des enfants dyslexiques (protocole 2) sont questionnés. Notre premier protocole expérimental, mené auprès de 61 enfants suivis de la moyenne section de maternelle jusqu'à la fin du cours préparatoire. Ces enfants ont été divisés en deux groupes, un groupe contrôle et un groupe expérimental, ayant reçu un entrainement psychomoteur aux compétences temporelles en moyenne et grande section de maternelle. Par l'intermédiaire des résultats des enfants du groupe contrôle, nous avons puis mettre en évidence les relations entre le rythme, les compétences en lien avec l'accès au langage écrit, la mémoire et l'inhibition, en moyenne section et en grande section de maternelle. En outre, notre analyse a permis de mettre en évidence les valeurs prédictives du rythme sur les compétences en préfecture en maternelle et en lecture au CP. Finalement, nous avons pu comprendre les bénéfices de l'entrainement psychomoteur sur la conscience phonologique et l'inhibition, en comparant l'évolution des compétences des enfants ayant reçu l'entrainement rythmique, aux enfants du groupe contrôle. Le second protocole a été réalisé auprès de 10 enfants, âgés de 9 à 12 ans présentant une dyslexie développementale à prédominance phonologique. Il leur a également été proposé un entraînement psychomoteur, mais cette fois-ci en individuel. Le protocole mis en place été composé de quatre évaluations espacées chacune de 10 semaines. La période entre la première et la seconde évaluation a servi de période témoin, tout comme la période entre la 3ème et la 4ème évaluation. Entre la 2ème et la 3ème évaluation, les enfants ont reçu en individuel un entrainement psychomoteur (période expérimentale). L'évolution des enfants dyslexiques sur la période expérimentale est comparée à leur propre évolution sur les périodes contrôles. L'analyse des résultats permet alors de suggérer l'intérêt des pratiques liées à l'éducation psychomotrice pour faciliter l'apprentissage de la lecture, notamment pour le développement de la conscience phonologique et de l'inhibition, tout comme montrer l'intérêt de la rééducation psychomotrice de la dyslexie.
Article
Full-text available
The present paper attempted to investigate whether there is any significant relationship between participants' brain laterality and L2 proficiency level. To carry out the experiment, 30 participants administered in the present study. Fifteen of them did not have any English language learning experience and were at the start of language learning, while the rest had attended L2 learning classes for about 2 years in a popular English language center, located in Bandar-e Anzali, Iran. Finally, the researchers concluded that the activity of the right hemisphere went up by the increase in language proficiency among bilinguals. Thereupon, the result of the paper was at variance with Albert and Obler's (1978) early work on hemispheric differentiation, which indicated that bilinguals were less hemispheric dominant than monolinguals.
Article
Full-text available
Systematic reviews and meta-analyses are essential to summarize evidence relating to efficacy and safety of health care interventions accurately and reliably. The clarity and transparency of these reports, however, is not optimal. Poor reporting of systematic reviews diminishes their value to clinicians, policy makers, and other users.Since the development of the QUOROM (QUality Of Reporting Of Meta-analysis) Statement--a reporting guideline published in 1999--there have been several conceptual, methodological, and practical advances regarding the conduct and reporting of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Also, reviews of published systematic reviews have found that key information about these studies is often poorly reported. Realizing these issues, an international group that included experienced authors and methodologists developed PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses) as an evolution of the original QUOROM guideline for systematic reviews and meta-analyses of evaluations of health care interventions.The PRISMA Statement consists of a 27-item checklist and a four-phase flow diagram. The checklist includes items deemed essential for transparent reporting of a systematic review. In this Explanation and Elaboration document, we explain the meaning and rationale for each checklist item. For each item, we include an example of good reporting and, where possible, references to relevant empirical studies and methodological literature. The PRISMA Statement, this document, and the associated Web site (http://www.prisma-statement.org/) should be helpful resources to improve reporting of systematic reviews and meta-analyses.
Article
This study examined hand, eye, and foot laterality in a sample of 890 7-yr-old children to determine whether the degree of left and right congruence between hand, eye, and foot preference related to a variety of measures of cognitive ability or motor performance or whether there was an association between hand, eye, and foot preferences and differences in cognitive ability and motor performance. With regard to left and right congruence, the only significant difference found related to a test of target throwing. There was a decrease in target throwing ability with decreased total left or right congruence. When the sample was grouped according to strength of left or right preference and all the measures were compared, there were no significant differences. Overall, this study found no significant association between any aspect of laterality studied and cognitive ability and only one significant association between laterality and a measure of motor performance. The implications of these results are discussed.
Article
them will tend to approach a word from the wrong direction. Monroe (1932) suggested that in moving the eyes to the right, as is required in reading, the left field of vision is obstructed by the bridge of the nose, and consequently the development of left-to-right eye movements may be more difficult for the left-eyed child. She also suggested that a child who has opposite eye-hand dominance may prefer different directional movements for eye and hand, and therefore find the complex hand-eye coordinations necessary in space perception difficult. Harris (1957) found mixed hand preference much more common in a clinical group of disabled readers than in the general school population. He suggested that this lack of consistent hand preference may be the result of a special maturational difficulty or slowness. Balow (1963), working with 302 first-grade children, studied the effect on three measures of reading achievement of these various types and degrees of hand and eye dominance, singly and in interaction: strong, moderate, and mixed hand dominance; normal, crossed, and mixed dominance; and directional confusion. He reasoned that if these characteristics are associated with reading disability, they are also associated with reading achievement in the first grade. Yet, he found no combination of hand dominance and eye dominance, hand-eye dominance and knowledge of right and left, or strength of dominance and knowledge of right and left significantly associated with reading achievement. The problem considered in this study is whether the dominance anomalies specified by Dearborn, Harris, and Monroe are significantly associated with reading achievement in the second grade.
Article
Understanding the relationship between cerebral laterality and intelligence is important in elucidating the neurological underpinnings of individual differences in cognitive abilities. A widely used, behavioral indicator for cerebral laterality, mainly of language, is handedness. A number of studies have compared cognitive abilities between groups of left-and right-handers, while others have investigated the handedness prevalence between groups of different cognitive abilities. The present study comprises five meta-analyses of studies that have assessed the handedness prevalence in (a) individuals with intellectual disability (ID) of unknown/idiopathic nature compared to typically developing (TD) individuals, and (b) individuals with intellectual giftedness (IG) compared to TD individuals. Nineteen data sets totaling 16,076 participants (5,795 ID, 8,312 TD, and 1,969 IG) were included in the analyses. Elevated levels of atypical handedness were found to be robust only for the ID to TD comparison. Findings constrain the range of acceptable theories on the handedness distribution for different intelligence levels. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.