ArticlePDF Available

Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in project management

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In project management research, it is acknowledged that two perspectives on project performance must be considered: project efficiency (delivering efficient outputs) and project success (delivering beneficial outcomes). The first perspective is embedded in a deterministic paradigm of project management, while the second appears more naturally connected to the emerging non-deterministic paradigm. Complexity and uncertainty are key constructs frequently associated with the non-deterministic paradigm. This conceptual paper suggests that these two concepts could very well explain and define particularities of both paradigms, and seeks to articulate both perspectives in a contingent model. First, the constructs of complexity and uncertainty are clarified. Second, the role of project managers' mental models in managerial decision-making is considered. In the third part of this article, we propose a theoretical model suggesting that project managers should consider contingent variables to differentiate managerial conditions of regulation from managerial conditions of emergence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of
regulation to a paradigm of emergence in project management
Pierre A. Daniel, Carole Daniel
Université Côte d'Azur, SKEMA, Avenue Willy Brandt, 59 777 Euralille, France
Received 15 December 2016; received in revised form 3 July 2017; accepted 3 July 2017
Available online xxxx
Abstract
In project management research, it is acknowledged that two perspectives on project performance must be considered: project efficiency
(delivering efcient outputs) and project success (delivering benecial outcomes). The rst perspective is embedded in a deterministic paradigm of
project management, while the second appears more naturally connected to the emerging non-deterministic paradigm. Complexity and uncertainty
are key constructs frequently associated with the non-deterministic paradigm. This conceptual paper suggests that these two concepts could very
well explain and dene particularities of both paradigms, and seeks to articulate both perspectives in a contingent model.
First, the constructs of complexity and uncertainty are claried. Second, the role of project managers' mental models in managerial decision-
making is considered. In the third part of this article, we propose a theoretical model suggesting that project managers should consider contingent
variables to differentiate managerial conditions of regulation from managerial conditions of emergence.
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd, APM and IPMA. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Complexity; Uncertainty; Mental models; Systems; Project Management; Performance
1. Executive summary
It is generally understood that the world is becoming more
and more complex. Project managers are experiencing this
in their daily activities, being faced with a growing number
of complex situations. The project management literature
particularly in the non-deterministic paradigm has focused
on this issue of complexity. However, two perspectives
project management and the management of projects
co-exist in the project management research community,
as do two paradigms: deterministic and non-deterministic.
This lack of unified theory as well as the difficulty of
agreeing on a definition of complexity does not help project
managers understand how to maximize performance in complex
projects.
The research presented here attempts to propose richer
lenses for looking at project management. We suggest that a
better understanding of the construct of complexity, its
associated construct of uncertainty, and the way human beings
predict these through mental models are possible groundings
for a contingent and comprehensive approach.
In this conceptual work, we first investigate the literature
on complexity, highlighting three levels that can be found in
different research works. We then investigate the literature
on uncertainty, which also converges towards three levels of
uncertainty. Finally, we add the notion of mental models as
a means for project managers to understand the situations in
which they find themselves, and gather all the findings in a
conceptual model of project management.
Our study adds to the literature on complexity and un-
certainty in project management by gathering many existing
research works from different sciences. Tables summarizing
these literatures shed light on the possibility of identifying three
different levels of complexity and of uncertainty, which form
the pillars of a contingent project management model.
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: pierre.daniel@skema.edu (P.A. Daniel),
carole.daniel@skema.edu (C. Daniel).
www.elsevier.com/locate/ijproman
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
0263-7863/00/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd, APM and IPMA. All rights reserved.
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect
International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxx xxx
JPMA-02052; No of Pages 14
Overall, our conceptual paper suggests that it is possible to
bridge the existing gap between the two project management
paradigms. One reason for the difficulty of managing complex
project situations lies in the limitations of classic project
management methods. Complex and uncertain projects require
newer methodologies based on understanding: the modelling
approaches. By understanding the levels of complexity and of
uncertainty in a situation, project managers can adapt their
decision-making approach in order to maximize performance.
2. Introduction
Ten years ago, researchers in project management started to
acknowledge the lack of a unified theory of the management of
projects, whether defined in its narrow (project management:
PM) or broad (management of projects: MoP) sense (Smyth
and Morris, 2007). This concern is still raised by the project
management research community, especially in the area of
project performance, where the streams of research on success
and failure do not converge (Padalkar and Gopinath, 2016b).
The growing complexity of projects led to the emergence of a
non-deterministic paradigm (Padalkar and Gopinath, 2016b),
which raised the question of how to generate performance in
complex projects; one major issue was agreeing on a definition
of complexity itself.
The co-existence of the PM and MoP perspectives is a
source of confusion for project managers, who are faced with
a wide variety of project management conditions, and who
cannot really know which project management approach is
better adapted to the complexity of their project. Is there a way
to reconcile these two perspectives? How can project managers
understand which management principles they should adopt,
depending on the managerial decision-making conditions under
which they are working?
The PM perspective is supported by the execution-based
model of the Project Management Institute (PMI), while the
MoP perspective founded on Peter Morris's research is
more comprehensive and open to a new definition of project
success (Pinto and Winch, 2016). In his definition of the nature
of project management, Turner makes a real distinction be-
tween the operationalproject perspective (which is focused
on the result of the project implementation: the output) and
the strategicproject perspective (focused on the outcome
resulting from the project implementation phase). This dis-
tinction is also found in the project management literature
on success and failure, which differentiates between project
efficiency(project implementation performance), and project
success(project benefits performance) (Cooke-Davies, 2002;
Serrador and Turner, 2015a; Turner and Zolin, 2012).
Beyond the two perspectives of PM and MoP, two
paradigms have emerged from surveys on decades of project
management research. The first is the deterministic paradigm,
which is well established (Pinto and Winch, 2016) and is
strongly dominated by operations research. It contributed
significantly to the increase in project management perfor-
mance with phase-project-planning methodologies in the 1960s
(Morris, 2010). The second is the non-deterministic paradigm,
which emerged in the mid-2000s (Padalkar and Gopinath,
2016b), putting a particular emphasis on complexity in projects
(Crawford et al., 2006; Geraldi et al., 2011a; Whitty and
Maylor, 2009). Non-deterministic research employs not only
complexity but also uncertainty (following Turner's broader
definition of project management) as its main lenses, but both
concepts remain ambiguous, preventing this paradigm from
moving forward (Padalkar and Gopinath, 2016a). For instance
the PMI's view on complexity is far removed from that of
complexity theory (Bakhshi et al., 2016).
Although the first paradigm is well established and the
second is attracting much research interest, there is no clear
way for project managers to understand how to position them-
selves in relation to these two paradigms. Complexity can
sometimes be associated with both the deterministic paradigm
(the PMI's view) and the non-deterministic paradigm (the
complexity-theory view), and sometimes it is linked only to the
non-deterministic paradigm. Complexity in projects is regularly
associated with uncertainty, but these two constructs are not
clearly differentiated in order to understand their specific role in
project management theory.
The first contribution of this conceptual paper is to
synthesize various research literatures (systems theory, decision
theory and planning theory) in two tables, which reveal the
contingency nature of complexity and uncertainty. We reveal
not only that both constructs can be categorized in three levels,
but also that each of these three levels suggests a specific
managerial way of addressing situations: algorithmic, sto-
chastic or non-deterministic. General systems theory revealed
that managers interact with projects through decision models
(mental models) to make their managerial decisions. The
second contribution reveals that the prediction capacity of these
decision models defines the level of uncertainty that project
managers have to address, and impacts the level of complexity
of the project as a whole. The third contribution is a contingent
framework of project management, which positions manage-
ment paradigms of regulation and of emergence according to
the level of complexity and uncertainty that project managers
must face. As a consequence, this comprehensive framework
provides new lenses for project managers in order to select the
appropriate management approach.
In Sections 3 and 4 of this paper, the constructs of
complexity and uncertainty will be explored, and the link
between the two will be developed. In these two sections, three
main approaches are revealed: algorithmic, stochastic and
non-deterministic, which can be linked with the constructs of
both complexity and uncertainty, and which are ingrained in
decision theory.
Section 5 sheds light on the fact that mental models are key
in managerial decision theory. Mental models and, more
specifically, decision models are characterized by their role in
managerial capacity to predict. Predictability is also a key
concept characterizing complexity and uncertainty.
In Section 6, we propose a theoretical framework for
project management that helps to distinguish the decision and
action conditions of risk versus uncertainty. From a contin-
gency perspective, this conceptual framework reveals systemic
2P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
characteristics differentiating and comparing project manage-
ment theories of regulation and of emergence.
3. Towards a clarification of the complexity construct
In the project management community, complexity is not
a clear and unified concept (Padalkar and Gopinath, 2016a;
Vidal and Marle, 2008); rather, complexity takes a variety of
forms (structural complexity, uncertainty, dynamics, pace and
socio-political complexity) and frequently signifies complicat-
edness (Geraldi et al., 2011b). With complicated systems,
outcomes are easily predictable if the starting conditions (the
project's inputs) are known, whereas with complex systems,
outcomes are unpredictable because of continually changing
interactions, even when the starting conditions can be known
(Maylor et al., 2008; Sargut and Mcgrath, 2011). Managers
involved in complex systems must face events that are difficult
to predict or interpret correctly, even retrospectively. These
events influence each other, and produce causality relations
that are not clear for decision-makers (Kauffman, 1993; Rivkin,
2000; Simon, 1969).
Management sciences have borrowed greatly from systems
theory to understand the functioning of projects, and many
authors consider that projects operate as complex systems
(Baccarini, 1996; Williams, 2002). In 1968, Boulding proposed
asystem of systemsclassification to explain how to describe
a system and its behaviour (Boulding, 1968). Each new level
in the hierarchy reveals specific functions and dynamics of a
system, and increases its degree of complexity. Table 1 sum-
marizes this classification.
Boulding's classification of systems uses metaphors at each
level of the hierarchy. As an example, the operational (PM)
perspective would naturally be positioned at the level of the
thermostat, using the metaphor of a cybernetic system, as
evidenced by Shewhart's quasi-cybernetic loop-of-control
model and Deming's plandocheckadjusttheory of control.
This naturally raises the question: which metaphor is most
appropriate for systems from the MoP perspective?
Systems theorists define systems along two dimensions:
their structural functions and their dynamic behaviour. This
suggests that these two dimensions of projects as complex
systems must be taken into account if we are to improve our
capacity to model and manage them. Fig. 1 depicts these two
dimensions.
Most writings on project complexity highlight the two
perspectives of structural complexity and dynamic complexity
(Maylor et al., 2008; Remington and Pollack, 2007; Ribbers
and Schoo, 2002; Xia and Lee, 2005). Both perspectives
establish a natural relationship between project complexity and
managerial capacity of prediction: (1) structural complexity
focuses on interactions producing unexpected effects that
cannot be explained or deduced; and (2) dynamic complexity
focuses on processes that generate unpredictable change in
systems (Floricel et al., 2016). Scientists and practitioners
have highlighted the need to better understand the relationship
between complexity and management, and particularly how
individuals and organizations should act in situations of com-
plexity (Augustine et al., 2005; Austin et al., 2002; Thomas and
Mengel, 2008). Interesting literature reviews based on com-
plexity theory were produced in order to define new directions
for research in organization science in general (Anderson,
1999), and more specifically in project management science
(Cooke-Davies et al., 2007). In the management and organiza-
tion science literature, complexity science (the paradigm of
complexity) is usually contrasted with Newtonian science (the
paradigm of complication), emphasizing the dichotomy and
contradictions between the old mind setand new thinking
(Sanders, 1998). The philosophy of sciences addresses the
question of complexity science in a more nuanced way, reveal-
ing its multiple implications for human thinking and rationality,
and providing philosophical and anthropological foundations
for its opposition to Newtonian science based on reductionism,
determinism and objective knowledge (Heylighen et al., 2007).
The epistemological perspective taken by Alhadeff-Jones is
a good example of such a perspective on complexity. In his
article, complexity theory is presented through the perspective
of three generations of complexity theories (Alhadeff-Jones,
2008). Considering complexities rather than complexity is a
fertile opportunity to try to reconcile the various schools
of thought about complexity and project management. This
approach to complexities is rooted in the seminal work of
Weaver on complexity and science (Weaver, 1948). In his
historical perspective on science, Weaver reveals that scientists
have addressed complexity in three specific ways, leading to
three categories of problems: the problems of simplicity, the
Table 1
Boulding's classification of systems.
Level of the system Name of the system Structural function of the system Dynamic behaviour of the system
FrameworkStatic Static relationships Static equilibrium
ClockSimple dynamic Dynamic relationships Stable equilibrium
ThermostatCybernetic Transmission and interpretation of information Maintaining a given equilibrium
within limits
CellOpen Exchanges with the environment Self-maintaining equilibrium
PlantGenetic societal Differentiated and mutually dependent parts Equifinal growth
AnimalBehavioural Capture of information and transformation of this into a knowledge structure
(an image) of the environment
Teleological behaviour
Individual humanHuman being Knowledge structure more complex than animal, based on language and symbolism Teleological behaviour
Social organizationSocial Complex knowledge structure, influenced by communication between systems Teleological behaviour
3P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
problems of disorganized complexity, and the problems of
organized complexity.Table 2 shows that complexity theories
adapted to management science are not unified but rather reveal
that human beings developed three ways of facing complex
situations based on three different scientific assumptions and
technical perspectives. The first two approaches are fundamen-
tally based on a deterministic paradigm, considering that
human beings can simplify the complex reality to control it
through regulation. In contrast, the third approach is rooted in a
non-deterministic paradigm, considering that: (1) human beings
are agents intrinsically subjective and uncertain about their
environment and future; and (2) a global organization emerges
out of local agents' interactions (Heylighen et al., 2007).
Fig. 2 develops Fig. 1 by emphasizing the three levels
of complexity detailed in Table 1 (i.e., three different project
dynamics).
4. Towards a clarification of the uncertainty construct
Complexity and uncertainty are frequently associated in the
project management literature (Sommer et al., 2009; Turner
and Cochrane, 1993; Williams, 1999, 2005)e.g., in project
typologies (Little, 2005; Shenhar and Dvir, 1996), in the
definition of project performance (Anderson, 1999; Levinthal,
1997; Levinthal and March, 1993; Rivkin, 2000), and in the
selection of managerial tactics for facing uncertainty (Sommer
and Loch, 2004). The imbrication of both concepts is so usual
in this literature that we could wonder whether complexity
and uncertainty are distinct concepts; a theoretical clarification
of the relationship between the two constructs is required
(Padalkar and Gopinath, 2016a). Analysis of the literature
on uncertainty in project management reveals similarities with
the analysis of the literature on complexity: both literatures
are considered non-unified, and each concept is prone to be
confused with the other.
In the previous section, we pointed out that the main issue
with the construct of complexity is the confusion between
complexity and complicatedness. In this section, we acknowl-
edge the confusion between complexity and uncertainty, but
focus our discussion on the confusion between uncertainty and
risk, explaining our rationale for this choice.
In the project management literature, there is a tendency to
confuse the terms uncertaintyand risk; this means that
uncertainty is treated in the same way as risk, or is ignored
(Perminova et al., 2008). It is dangerous to confuse risk and
uncertainty since doing so tends to focus attention on planning
and operational control, at the expense of strategic issues
(Atkinson et al., 2006). Table 3, building on the work of Daniel
(2010) represents an attempt to synthesize previous research
work on the different levels of risk/uncertainty on which
PRESENT (tn)FUTURE (tn+1)
MANAGEMENT
SUB-SYSTEM
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn
Output /
outcome
In tn
Output /
outcome
In tn+1
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn+1
Implementing
Fig. 1. Structure and dynamic of project management systems.
4P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
managers are placed. Researchers in statistics (Littauer, 1967;
Littauer and Ehrenfeld, 1964), decision theory (Rubinstein,
1975), strategy (Courtney et al., 1997) and project management
(De Meyer et al., 2002; Sanderson, 2012) have identified three
or four levels of risk/uncertainty that they link to managerial
conditions: certainty conditions,risk conditionsand uncer-
tainty conditions.Table 3 distinguishes between all four levels,
but we have chosen to regroup levels 2 and 3 under one label.
We suggest labelling the three main levels algorithmic,
stochasticand non-deterministic. Each of these situations
implies different management modes.
The seminal work of Milton Spencer (Spencer, 1962;
Spencer and Siegelman, 1959) is very helpful when it comes
to pointing out the key question of predictability in this
discussion, focusing on distinguishing risk from uncertainty; it
also builds bridges between the theory of uncertainty, the
theory of decision, and the theory of planning:
Risk may be defined as the quantitative measurement of an
outcome, such as a gain or a loss, in a manner such that the
probability of the outcome can be predicted .
Uncertainty, like risk, is also prediction-oriented, but unlike
the latter its measurement is not objective and does not assume
perfect knowledge. (Spencer, 1962, p. 197).
Knight restricted the notion of risk to two types of situations:
probabilities and statistics. In both cases, decision-makers were
regarded as able to define objective probabilities within a
known range of future events or results (Knight, 1921). Some
authors have thought that subjective probabilities could also
be a response to ambiguous situations in which leaders found
themselves. But subjective probabilities have their limitations,
because even when the information is available, it can be of
little interest for future results if the conditions of reality change
(Rotheim, 1995); this is typically the case in conditions of
dynamic complexity.
Uncertainty appears when decision-makers cannot consoli-
date past observations to form a subjective probability or
relative frequencies for the future (Davidson, 1991). This
difficult and specific state in which managers cannot know
all the important parameters and the possible results is termed
unawarenessor unforeseen contingencies(Kreps, 1992;
Modica and Rustichini, 1994), unstable non-determination
(Littauer, 1967), wicked problemsas opposed to tame
problems(Rittel and Webber, 1973), and unknown unknowns
by extension of the Knightian concept of known unknowns
(Wideman, 1992). Business developers often do not properly
forecast market opportunities or the best way to treat them. They
Table 2
Three approaches to complexity in management sciences.
Algorithmic approach Stochastic approach Non-deterministic approach
Weaver (1948) Problems of simplicity: where complex
problems can be reduced to simple issues
thanks to the paradigm of rational mechanics
Problems of disorganized complexity:where
disorder is an integral part of the natural
phenomena (and can be addressed by statistics
and the theory of probability)
Problems of organized complexity: where a
sizeable number of factors are interrelated
into an organic whole (being too complicated
for rational mechanics, and not sufficiently
disordered for statistics)
Cramer (1993),
Mckelvey (2004)
Subcritical complexity: the amount of
information to describe the system is less
complex than the system itself
Newtonian complexity
Fundamental complexity: the minimum
amount of information to describe the system
is equal to the complexity of the system itself
Stochastic complexity
Critical complexity: emergent simple
deterministic structures, with underlying
phenomena made of fundamental complexity
Emergent complexity
Heylighen et al. (2007) Phenomena characterized by order: like
those studied in Newtonian mechanics and
systems science
Phenomena characterized by disorder: like
those investigated by statistical mechanics
and postmodern social science
Neither order nor disorder: situated
somewhere in-between, in the zone that is
commonly called the edge of chaos
Scientific assumptions Mathematical models allow optimization
of the decision and the management of
complex activities composed of very large
number of parameters
Heuristic models improve understanding and
accompany learning in condition of
uncertainty; human decision-making processes
require the mediation of modelling instruments
to learn from apparent disorder
The emergentnature of unpredictable
activities requires a constant adaptation of
groups of actors that are sources of order
and disorder; emerging processes and
experimentations are management dynamics
generating opportunities and bifurcations
Classic schools of
thought in the
sciences of
complexity
Operational research is interested in
phenomena involving hundreds or thousands
of variables, in order to transform them and
reduce them to linear mathematical formulas
that can be managed by computers
(Beer, 1959; Churchman et al. , 1957)
Cybernetics contributes to the definition of
the concept of feedbackto describe the way
in which a system can follow a predefined
purpose, adapting to its environment
(Wiener, 1948/1961)
Engineering sciences strengthen an
understanding of complexity based on a
quantitative assessment, leading to the
concept of computational complexity
(Ashby, 1957; Knuth, 1968; Marcus, 1977)
Science of systems with work on the
dynamics of systems(Forrester, 1961),
and on the system approach(Churchman,
1968)favours the emergence of techniques
reducing the complexity of a system to the
study of its components and their relationships;
any organized set should be described and
explained through the use of the same
categories (Von Bertalanffy, 1951)
Self-organization theories attempt to define
complexity as emergences produced not only
by the order that constitutes them but also from
the disorder that characterizes the relations
to their internal components (Ashby, 1957;
Atlan, 1972/2006; Von Foerster, 1960, 1974)
Second-order cybernetics favours a definition
of complex systems recognizing the
constructivist nature inherent in their design
(Bateson, 1973; Von Foerster, 1974); the
autopoesis proposes the development of new
representations around concepts such as
adaptation, evolution, self-esteem, autonomy
and emergence (Maturana and Varela, 1992)
5P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
are therefore forced to adapt and modify their approach over
time (Drucker, 1985; Mcgrath and Macmillan, 1995); the
information does not exist until the results have produced their
effects (Minsky, 1996). When launching a new company, leaders
often know very little, and are unable to recognize and artic-
ulate the variables and their functional relationships (Schrader
et al., 1993); the unpredictable uncertainties are rampant
(Bank, 1995).
The uncertainty and complexity constructs appear to be
connected by the concept of predictability, which is rooted in
decision theory. Two interesting parallels appear to exist between
uncertainty theory and the complexity theory: (1) decision-
making conditions of risk (as defined by Spencer) are concep-
tually close to problems of disorganized complexity(as defined
by Weaver); and (2) decision-making conditions of uncertainty
(as defined by Spencer) are conceptually close to problems
of organized complexity(as defined by Weaver). Beyond the
parallels revealed in these two sections, a clear distinction can
be made between the complexity and uncertainty constructs:
complexity defines the structure and dynamics of the project as a
system (system of production, and system of management), and
uncertainty defines the decision-making conditions of the system
of management (the manager as a decision-maker). Fig. 3 adds
these decision-making conditions to Fig. 2.
5. Decision theory, mental models and predictability
Classical project management methods/methodologies face
limitations when applied to complex projects. In an empirical
study of project management practices, respondents identified
inadequacy for complex projects(27%) and make it difficult
to model the real world(15%) as the first two limitations/
drawbacks of such methods (White and Fortune, 2002). While
conventional techniques may be well suited to tackling com-
plicated projects (with large numbers of elements), they are
unsuited to projects subject to high uncertainty (Cicmil et al.,
2006). Complex and uncertain projects require newer method-
ologies that help the project emergerather than being fully
pre-planned, and that are based on understanding (model-based
theories) (Williams, 2005). Recent research works illustrated
the relevance of modelling approaches for complex and un-
certain projects (Qazi et al., 2016).
PRESENT (tn)FUTURE (tn+1)
MANAGEMENT
SUB-SYSTEM
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn
Output /
outcome
In tn
Output /
outcome
In tn+1
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn+1
PROJECT DYNAMIC (tnto tn+1)
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC
– stable (simple)
– stable under fixed limits (complicated)
– unstable (complex)
Fig. 2. Three levels of complexity in project management systems.
6P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
5.1. Mental models and the question of complexity management
The theory of mental models could be the missing link
between complexity and uncertainty in project systems, as
predictability is a referential concept in complexity theory and
in uncertainty theory. General systems theory applied to man-
agement revealed that managers interact with reality through
representation models to make their management decisions
(Forrester, 1961; Sterman, 2001). The fundamental property of
thought is its ability to predict events (Craik, 1943; Jones et al.,
2011). Mental models are generalizations (or even images) that
influence how we understand and act in the world (Senge,
1990). They are constantly adjusted, refined and recreated in
dynamic environments subject to constant change, and they
play an important role in the construction and interpretation of
reality (Chermack, 2003; Ruona and Lynham, 2004). A mental
model is a cognitive structure that allows us to describe, explain
and predict the purpose, form, function and state of a system
(Rouse and Morris, 1986); it establishes causal knowledge
about how the system works (Moray, 1998). Mental models
guide, draw and provide the basis through which individuals
interpret and construct the meaning of life in organizations
(Weick, 1990).
Systems modelling is one tool for dealing with decision in
conditions of uncertainty, because decisions can be tested out
with hypothetical consequences (Morecroft, 1983). If someone
has a small-scale mental modelof an external reality and
of their own possible actions, they are able to define several
alternatives, figure out which one is best, respond to future
situations before they occur, use knowledge from past events to
deal with the present and the future, and react more prudently
and skilfully to what emerges (Craik, 1943). Systems-dynamics
researchers use constructs of mental models in a pragmatic
way, as tools to better understand complex and dynamic
systems, and ultimately to improve their design and use (Doyle
and Ford, 1998; Moray, 2004).
With complex projects, the learning dynamic arises from the
relationship between project leaders and the project systems
they manage. Since models are vehicles for learning about the
world, studying a model makes it possible to discover the
system characteristics that it describes. This cognitive function
of models is well known and has given rise to model-based
reasoning(Magnani and Nersessian, 2002). Mental models act
as inferential frameworks (Gentner and Gentner, 1983) and
influence decision-making, which takes place through feedback
loops (Forrester, 1961). To learn, we must use the limited
and imperfect feedback that is at our disposal to understand
the effects of our decisions, to adjust them accordingly, and to
align the state of the system with our goals (the simple learning
loop). Thus, we can revise our mental models and redesign
Table 3
Different levels in the conditions of uncertainty of leaders.
ALGORITHMIC approach STOCHASTIC approach NON-DETERMINISTIC approach
Littauer (1967),
Littauer and
Ehrenfeld (1964)
Deterministic certainty
In these situations, an action
leads to a unique consequence
Probabilistic certainty
An action leads to a set of
consequences with known
probabilities of occurrence
Stable uncertainty
In some situations, there is an
even lower degree of knowledge
in relation to the action and its
consequences
Unstable uncertainty
The possible consequences of an
action are unsure, but in addition we
cannot assign probabilities to various
consequences
Rubinstein (1975) The decision under certainty
Actions lead to a defined result
that will definitely occur
Decision under risk
Each state of nature has a known objective probability
Decision under uncertainty
An action can have at least two
consequences, but the probabilities
for the states of nature are unknown
Courtney et al.
(1997)
A clear-enough future
Managers can make an accurate
forecast for the development of
a strategy
Alternative future
The analysis cannot identify what
the outcome will be, but may
establish probabilities there are
different scenarios
A range of future
A range of future potentials can
be identified by a limited number
of key variables, but the end
result can settle anywhere
The real ambiguity
Multiple dimensions of uncertainty
interact to create an environment that
is virtually impossible to predict
De Meyer et al.
(2002)
Variation
Cost, duration and performance
levels vary randomly, but in a
predictable field
Predictable uncertainty
A small number of known factors
will influence the project goal in a
predictable way; the foreseeable
uncertainties are identifiable; the
predictable uncertainty may require
several alternative plans
Unpredictable uncertainty
One or more influencing factors
cannot be predicted; the
unpredictable uncertainty
concerns projects that take place
in a partially known market
Chaos
Unpredictable events invalidate
completely the objectives, the
planning and the project approach;
even the structure of the project plan
is uncertain
Sanderson (2012) Risk of category 1
(probability a priori)
The decision-maker can define
objective probabilities based on
mathematical probabilities
Risk of category 2
(statistical probability)
The decision-maker can define
objective probabilities on the basis
of an empirical sample/statistics
from past data
Uncertainty in category 1
(subjective probability)
The decision-maker lacks data
needed to define a probability
objective; they then define a
subjective probability based on
forecasts built on past experiences
Uncertainty in category 2
(socialized)
The decision-maker faces unknown
future situations; the future is
fundamentally unpredictable it is
socially constructed and cannot be
linked to the past or the present
7P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
the system itself (the double learning loop) (Argyris, 1985;
Sterman, 2000).
5.2. The challenges, uncertainty and complexity of mental
models
Having stressed the key role of mental models in complex
and uncertain projects, it is important to consider two main
difficulties that can disturb the prediction mechanism: cognitive
limitations and socio-organizational issues.
Results from experimental research in the psychology of
decision-making identify significant limitations in the cognitive
abilities of human beings (Gilovich et al., 2002; Kahneman et al.,
1982). Indeed, mental models integrate individuals' biases, such
as beliefs, experiences and values (Ford and Sterman, 1998). The
limitations inherent to human cognition have an impact on how
decision-makers face risk and uncertainty; for instance, decisions
and judgements under conditions of uncertainty are subject
to numerous biases that preclude prediction (Kahneman et al.,
1982) and generate an illusion of control(Langer, 1975).
Self-regulation theory explains that since individuals are driven
by their internal goals concerning control over their environment,
they try to reassert their control under conditions of chaos,
uncertainty and stress. One way they can deal with their lack
of control is to incorrectly assume control over the situation
(Fenton-O'creevy et al., 2003). Confirmation-bias theory sug-
gests that individuals look for information that corresponds
to their understanding of the world at a given moment. New
information may strengthen existing mental models or be rejected
categorically (Klayman and Ha, 1989).
In complex systems, actions and decisions can amplify
counter performance, notably by the effect of mental represen-
tation, which are counter-intuitive. Effective management is
difficult in a world of high dynamic complexity. Decisions
can create unanticipated adverse effects and lagged conse-
quences over time. Attempts to stabilize the system can in fact
destabilize it (Sterman, 2000); this phenomenon is known
as counter-intuitive behaviour of social systems(Forrester,
1971). Learning in situations of dynamic complexity is often
very poor (Paich and Sterman, 1993).
Beyond cognitive challenges, mental models must face
social/organizational challenges. The decisions and actions
taken by managers result from many interactions between
various stakeholders. Both at the individual and at the col-
lective levels, facing complexity requires the ability to filter
strategically a vast amount of available information, and to
integrate this into an implicit or explicit prediction model
(Beratan, 2007). The effective functioning of teams requires
the existence of a mental model shared by team members
(Langan-Fox et al., 2000). A shared mental model is the mental
model built within a team, and shared by its members. It
represents the cognition shared among groups of individuals
(Langan-Fox et al., 2001). A team model is the collective
knowledge that team members bring to a specific situation
i.e., the collective understanding that team members share
about a specific situation, also termed the team situation
PRESENT (tn)FUTURE (tn+1)
MANAGEMENT
SUB-SYSTEM
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn
Output /
outcome
In tn
Output /
outcome
In tn+1
PRODUCTION SUB-
SYSTEM
Structure in tn+1
PLANNING ‘conditions’
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC
CONTROLLING ‘conditions’
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC
full predictability
(certainty)
limited predictability
(risk)
unpredictability
(uncertainty)
no variation (certainty)
detect an error in the
‘reality’ (risk)
detect an error in
the ‘model’ (uncertainty)
Fig. 3. Three levels of uncertainty in project management systems.
8P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
model(Cooke et al., 2000). More recently, research work on
shared mental models (SMMs) proved that higher SMMs in
project teams improved team learning and performance (Yang
et al., 2008), and more specifically improved performance in
project requirement analysis (Xiang et al., 2016).
6. Reconciling deterministic and non-deterministic
paradigms in a new contingent theoretical framework
In this section, we propose a systemic model in an attempt
to integrate the complexity and uncertainty constructs discussed
in Sections 3 and 4, together with the modelling function
discussed in Section 5. We bring these together in a theoretical
framework presenting the three resulting contingent approaches
of project managers' decisions and actions: algorithmic, stochas-
tic and non-deterministic.
6.1. Theoretical model for an integrative perspective of
complexity and uncertainty in project management
The theoretical model of project management presented
in Fig. 4 is an attempt to unify the various approaches of
complexity and uncertainty that were presented in Sections 3
and 4.
In this model, the construct of complexity is depicted in
the project system's dynamic (the black box at the bottom of
Fig. 1), which can be simple (simplified by algorithmic models),
complicated (patterned by stochastic models) or complex
(experimented with through non-deterministic approaches).
This is consistent with the usual categories of systems presented
in the business literature (Sargut and Mcgrath, 2011).
The construct of uncertainty is depicted in the two key
characteristics of the management model: prediction and
detection. For instance, when the project system is simple, the
management system can design a model able to predict the
project dynamic with certainty (model prediction) and reveal
no gap between the prediction of the model and the output
delivered by the production system (model detection).
Through a single systemic model, we differentiate two para-
digms of project management: regulation (deterministic), based
on a planningimplementingcontrolling cycle (number 1 in the
triangle of arrows in Fig. 4); and emergence (non-deterministic),
based on a modellingexperimentinglearning cycle (number 2
in the triangle of arrows).
PRESENT (tn)
MODEL ‘DETECTION’
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC
PROJECT SYSTEM’s ‘DYNAMIC (tnto tn+1)’
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC
FUTURE (tn+1)
MODEL ‘PREDICTION’
1. ALGORITHMIC
2. STOCHASTIC
3. NON-DETERMINISTIC –
1 & 2. PLANNING*
3. MODELING** 1 & 2 CONTROLLING*
3. LEARNING**
MANAGEMENT SUB-SYSTEM
Modelling capability
MODEL
3. EXPERIMENTING**
1 & 2. IMPLEMENTING*
* PM PARADIGM
OF REGULATION
** PM PARADIGM
OF EMERGENCE
PRODUCTION SUB-SYSTEM
Structure in tn
Output /
outcome
In tn
REALITY
Output /
outcome
In tn+1
REALITY
PRODUCTION SUB-SYSTEM
Structure in tn+1
no variation
(certainty)
detect an error
in the ‘reality’ (risk)
detect
an error in the ‘model’
(uncertainty)
stable (simple)
stable under fixed limits (complicated)
unstable (complex)
full
predictability (certainty)
limited
predictability (risk)
unpredictability (uncertainty)
Fig. 4. The theoretical model of project management.
9P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
In Fig. 4, white boxes represent the project system, com-
posed of two sub-systems interacting together, both contribut-
ing to generating project complexity:
(1) A production sub-system represents the project-
implementation function, delivering project outputs and
outcomes. It is defined by its structure, outcomes and
dynamics (revealing the potential changes in the project
structure and outcomes over time, the dynamic from tn
to tn + 1).
(2) A management sub-system represents the project-
management function focused on achieving project
performance. It interacts with the production sub-system
over time through a modelling function based on a
capability to predict the production sub-system behav-
iours, as well as a capability to detect a gap between the
reality produced by the production sub-system and the
prediction of the model.
The interactions between the production sub-system and the
management sub-system constitute a dynamic process compris-
ing feedback loops, the management system that impacts the
dynamic of the production system, and the production system
that impacts the modelling capability of the management
system (Sterman, 2001).
In our model, the uncertainty construct is a characteristic of
the management sub-system; it defines the decision-making
conditions of project managers interacting with the production
sub-system. It is a fundamental characteristic of the modelling
capability of the management sub-system.
In contrast, the complexity construct is a characteristic of
the project system, including the management sub-system, the
production sub-system and their interactions. Complexity is a
characteristic of the production sub-system (multiple interac-
tions with the environment, and changing conditions from
the environment), amplified by the interactions between the
production sub-system and the management sub-system through
positive feedback loops, and by the cognitive limitations of
mental models.
6.2. The deterministic paradigm of regulation
The project management literature has clearly presented the
difference between a classical project management paradigm
and a model-based theory paradigm (Williams, 2005). The
latter challenges three characteristics of the former: (1) a heavy
emphasis on planning; (2) the influence of the cybernetic-
control model; and (3) a low sensitivity to environmental
influences. The classic planning-and-control paradigm is fully
influenced by cybernetics, a systemic theory of regulation
based on: (1) the capacity of the agents in the management
system to predict the behaviour of the production system;
and (2) the deterministic or statistical stability of the production
system (Littauer, 1967). Cybernetics deals with all forms of
behaviour insofar as they are regular, determinate or reproduc-
ible (Ashby, 1957). The real strength of cybernetics is in its
scientific application by Shewhart (through the quasi-cybernetic
loop-of-control model) in management science, and more
specifically in operations research (Shewhart, 1931). The famous
plandocheckadjusttheory of operational control, popular-
ized as the Deming wheel, is a perfect example of the planning-
and-control paradigm proposed by cybernetics (Deming, 1986).
A phenomenon will be said to be in control when, through
the use of past experience, we can predict, at least within
limits, how the phenomenon may be expected to vary in the
future(Shewhart, 1931). Consequently, in the planning-and-
control paradigm, the behaviour of the production sub-system
(identified as a phenomenon) is characterized by stability
(subjectonlytovariation),andthemanagement sub-system
can predict the behaviour of the production sub-system
through its repetition (past experiences). Thus the manage-
ment sub-system plays the role of a regulator that must model
what it regulates, modelling being a necessary part of regu-
lation (Conant and Ashby, 1970). Finally, following the words
of Ashby, Cyberneticsoffersthehope of providing effective
methods for the study, and control, of systems that are intrin-
sically extremely complex(Ashby, 1957).
In our model, the deterministic paradigm of regulation
corresponds to this classic project-management paradigm of
planning and control i.e., to the deterministic paradigm of
risk.
6.3. The non-deterministic paradigm of emergence
When it comes to unique events (changes that have never
happened before) or so-called discontinuities (e.g., technolog-
ical innovations, price increases, evolution of consumers'
attitudes, and legislative decisions), forecasts become virtually
impossible (Makridakis and Hibon, 1979). In these contexts,
the retrospective approach often fails (Pant and Starbuck,
1990). The understanding of unique events is delicate, as
their modelling is often impossible to build (Makridakis and
Wheelwright, 1981). System-dynamics theory reveals that
sometimes the project behaviour is non-intuitive, leading to
non-linear behaviour, being difficult for the human brain to
predict and understand intuitively (Sterman, 1989). Project-
management researchers have used the systems theories char-
acterized by unpredictability and instability (such as chaos
theory, dissipative structures, and complex adaptive systems)
to identify the theoretical aspects that should be analysed in
project-management science: non-linearity, emergence, insta-
bility and radical unpredictability (Cooke-Davies et al., 2007).
While the epistemology of cybernetics is very influential in
the paradigm of regulation, the epistemology of second-order
cybernetics is key to understanding the paradigm of emergence.
The main thesis of second-order cybernetics is that human
beings, as observers, are also cybernetic systems (Von Foerster,
1974). Their knowledge is a subjective construction, not an
objective reflection of reality, which means that the emphasis
should shift from the apparently objective systems around us to
the cognitive and social processes by which we construct our
subjective models of those systems (Heylighen et al., 2007).
The concept of emergence has never been analysed clearly
in the project management literature, yet it is central to the
10 P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
concept of the non-deterministic paradigm, just as the concept
of regulation is central to the deterministic paradigm. In the
deterministic paradigm, regulation is based on the modelling
capacity of the management sub-system to produce a good
model of the production sub-system (named the isomorphic
or homomorphicmodel) (Conant and Ashby, 1970). This
high-quality model enables us to regulate the production
sub-system, which means maintaining its stability. In the
non-deterministic paradigm, emergence is the result of the
incapacity of the management sub-system to produce a good
model of the production sub-system, as the production
sub-system itself is unstable over time. Therefore, the project
management modelis not a planable to create stability;
it is a model able to generate both stability and instability in
the production sub-system. The result of such an unstable
interaction is the emergence of unexpected outputs and out-
comes in the project. In the non-deterministic paradigm of
emergence, as in model-based theories, the imperfect model is a
management artefact enabling decision-makers to learn from
reality through feedback loops; by so doing, they improve the
quality of their model, and consequently the quality of their
management decisions.
7. Discussion and conclusion
The systemic model presented in this paper shows the
contingency nature of project management systems where
degrees of uncertainty and of complexity are embedded,
uncertainty being a characteristic of the management sub-
system, and complexity being a characteristic of the project-
management system, combining the management and produc-
tion sub-systems. In this theoretical model of project manage-
ment, the paradigm of regulation is clearly related to the
deterministic paradigm of project management (Padalkar and
Gopinath, 2016b) that one can associate with the operational-
project perspective (Turner et al., 2010). Consequently, project
managers applying the PMI's execution-based model should
verify the conditions of stability of the project's production
systems (inputs, outputs and outcomes). Without such require-
ments, the planningimplementingcontrolling paradigm of
regulation is inappropriate, because of management models
that are unable to predict or unable to detect an error in
the production systems. In the theoretical model, the paradigm
of emergence is related to the non-deterministic paradigm
of project management. It emphasizes a project management
theory based on modellingexperimentinglearning processes,
built on imperfect management models. We believe that
the paradigm of emergence would be fruitful to improve the
strategic project perspective (Turner et al., 2010).
Recently, Serrador and Turner asked an interesting question
about the quantityof planning that is required in a project
(Serrador and Turner, 2015b). The legitimacy of planning
projects is questioned in dynamic environments if activities
cannot be foreseen, or if planning leads to false expectations
(Andersen, 1996; Collyer et al., 2010; Collyer and Warren,
2009). The theoretical model presented in this paper sug-
gests that this question could be interestingly supplemented
by another: what is the nature of the planning that is
required? The rationale of the paradigm of regulation is
questioned in the sense that in the uncertainty paradigm,
there is no further reference to a goodmodel or plan.
While the project plan (or schedule) is a good model of
project reality in the risk paradigm, it is an imperfect model
in the uncertainty paradigm.
Organizational improvisation theory is an example of
management practices that are faced with imperfect plans,
even in structured contexts such as projects (Moorman and
Miner, 1998, 2001). Improvisation in organizations is a man-
agerial capacity to explore unexpected opportunities and to
neutralize unpredicted threats (Cunha et al., 1999, 2003). The
perspective of complex responsive processes of relating is
another attempt to transfer elements of complex adaptive
systems theory to organizations and complexity management,
through an emphasis on the interactions among people, based
on acts of communication, power relations, and interplay
between people's choices (Stacey, 2007). Systems-modelling
approaches, such as systems dynamics, are based on building
better models of the complexity of the project (the production
sub-system) to improve the learning process of decision-
making (the management sub-system) (Diehl and Sterman,
1995; Sterman, 1992, 2001). However, no attention has been
paid to how project decision models (project plans, work
breakdown structures, product breakdown structures etc.) are
built, adapted and applied in the project-management loop. One
of the basic assumptions of the paradigm of emergence is that
imperfect decision models impact the production sub-system
and create emergent outcomes. But we know very little about
how this emergent dynamic of production systems interacts
with management systems. Management under unforeseeable-
uncertaintytheories promotes management processes based on
selectionismand trial-and-error learning(Loch et al., 2001,
2008). A preliminary step consists of breaking down the project
into sub-projects under conditions of foreseeable uncertainty
and unforeseeable uncertainty, in order to contingently adapt
the management process (routine execution, or novel strategic
project) (Lenfle, 2011; Lenfle and Loch, 2010). But the
literature is poor when it comes to present management
models able to reveal/analyse the conditions of predictability
and control of the management sub-system towards the
production sub-system. Project-management tools and tech-
niques mainly focus on descriptions of the production sub-
system, whereas project performance is a consequence of
the unstable and emergent interactions between the production
sub-system and the management sub-system. More than
describing the static conditions of predictability and control,
project management scholars and professionals need to know
more about the evolution of this interaction over time.
Studying the dynamic of the emergent interaction between
the production sub-system and the management sub-system
is central to understanding how process performance and
success are created, as it cannot be controlled in conditions
of unforeseeable uncertainty. The next challenge of project
management science should be to generate a theory of
emergence, just as a theory of regulation.
11P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
Conict of interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
Alhadeff-Jones, M., 2008. Three generations of complexity theories: nuances
and ambiguities. In: Mason, M. (Ed.), Complexity Theory and the
Philosophy of Education. Wiley-Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp. 6278.
Andersen, E.S., 1996. Warning: activity planning is hazardous to your project's
health! Int. J. Proj. Manag. 14, 8994.
Anderson, P., 1999. Complexity theory and organization science. Organ. Sci.
10, 216232.
Argyris, C., 1985. Strategy, Change, and Defensive Routines. Pitman, Boston.
Ashby, R., 1957. An Introduction to Cybernetics. Methuen, London.
Atkinson, R., Crawford, L., Ward, S., 2006. Fundamental uncertainties in projects
and the scope of project management. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 24, 687698.
Atlan, H., 1972/2006. L'Organisation Biologique et la Théorie de l'information.
Hermann, Paris.
Augustine, S., Payne, B., Sencindiver, F., Woodcock, S., 2005. Agile project
management: streering from the edges. Commun. ACM 48, 8589.
Austin, S., Newton, A., Steele, J., Waskett, P., 2002. Modelling and managing
project complexity. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 20, 191198.
Baccarini, D., 1996. The concept of project complexity - a review. Int. J. Proj.
Manag. 14, 201204.
Bakhshi, J., Ireland, V., Gorod, A., 2016. Clarifying the project complexity
construct: past, present and future. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34, 11991213.
Bank, D., 1995. The java saga. Wired 166-169, 238246 December.
Bateson, G., 1973. Steps to an Ecology of Mind. Paladin, London.
Beer, S., 1959. What has cybernetics to do with operational research? Oper.
Res. Q. 10, 121.
Beratan, K.K., 2007. A cognition-based view of decision processes in complex
social-ecological systems. Ecol. Soc. 12, Art. 27.
Boulding, K.E., 1968. Beyond Economics: Essays on Society, Religion, and
Ethics. University of Michigan Press.
Chermack, T.J., 2003. Mental models in decision making and implications for
human resource development. Adv. Dev. Hum. Resour. 5, 408422.
Churchman, C.W., 1968. The Systems Approach. Delacorte Press, New York.
Churchman, C.W., Ackoff, R.L., Arnoff, E.L., 1957. Introduction to Operations
Research. Wiley, New York.
Cicmil, S., Williams, T., Thomas, J., Hodgson, D., 2006. Rethinking project
management: researching the actuality of projects. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 24,
675686.
Collyer, S., Warren, C.M.J., 2009. Project management approaches for dynamic
environments. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 27, 355364.
Collyer, S., Warren, C., Hemsley, B., Stevens, C., 2010. Aim, fire, aim - project
planning styles in dynamic environments. Proj. Manag. J. 41, 108121.
Conant, R.C., Ashby, W.R., 1970. Every good regulator of a system must be a
model of that system. Int. J. Syst. Sci. 1, 8997.
Cooke, N., Salas, E., Cannon-Bowers, J.A., Stout, R., 2000. Measuring team
knowledge. Hum. Factors 42, 151173.
Cooke-Davies, T., 2002. The realsuccess factors on projects. Int. J. Proj.
Manag. 20, 185190.
Cooke-Davies, T., Cicmil, S., Crawford, L., Richardson, K., 2007. We're not
in Kansas anymore, Toto: mapping the strange landscape of complexity
theory, and its relationship to project management. Proj. Manag. J. 38,
5061.
Courtney, H., Kirkland, J., Viguerie, P., 1997. Strategy under uncertainty. Harv.
Bus. Rev. 6779.
Craik, K., 1943. The Nature of Explaination. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Cramer, F., 1993. Chaos and Order: The Complex Structure of Living Things.
VCH, New York.
Crawford, L., Pollack, J., England, D., 2006. Uncovering the trends in project
management: journal emphases over the last 10 years. Int. J. Proj. Manag.
24, 175184.
Cunha, M.P., Cunha, J.V., Kamoche, K., 1999. Organizational improvisation:
what, when, how and why? Int. J. Manag. Rev. 1, 299341.
Cunha, M.P., Kamoche, K., Cunha, R.C., 2003. Organizational improvisation
and leadership. A field study in two computer-mediated settings. Int. Stud.
Manag. Organ. 33, 3457.
Daniel, P., 2010. Pilotage stratégique de projets et management des systèmes
dynamiques. Innovations 31, 5180.
Davidson, P., 1991. Is probability theory relevant for uncertainty? A post
Keynesian perspective. J. Econ. Perspect. 5, 129143.
De Meyer, A., Loch, C.H., Pich, M.T., 2002. From variation to chaos. Sloan
Manag. Rev. 6067.
Deming, W.E., 1986. Out of the Crisis. M.I.T. Press.
Diehl, E., Sterman, J.D., 1995. Effects of feedback complexity on dynamic
decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 62, 198215.
Doyle, J.K., Ford, D.N., 1998. Mental models concepts for system dynamics
research. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 14, 329.
Drucker, P., 1985. Innovation & Entrepreneurship: Practice and Principles.
Harper & Row, New York.
Fenton-O'creevy, M., Nicholson, N., Soane, E., Willman, P., 2003. Trading on
illusions: unrealistic perceptions of control and trading performance.
J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 76, 5368.
Floricel, S., Michela, J.L., Piperca, S., 2016. Complexity, uncertainty-reduction
strategies, and project performance. Int. J. Proj. Manag.
Ford, D.N., Sterman, J.D., 1998. Expert knowledge elicitation to improve
formal and mental models. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 14, 309340.
Forrester, J., 1961. Industrial Dynamics. MIT Press.
Forrester, J., 1971. Counterintuitive behavior of social systems. Technol. Rev.
73, 5268.
Gentner, D., Gentner, R.R., 1983. Flowing waters or teeming crowds: mental
models of electricity. In: Gentner, D., Stevens, A. (Eds.), Mental models.
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, USA, pp. 99130.
Geraldi, J., Maylor, H., Williams, T., 2011a. Now, let's make it really complex
(complicated). Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 31, 966990.
Geraldi, J.G., Maylor, H., Williams, T., 2011b. Now, let's make it really
complex (complicated): a systematic review of the complexities of projects.
Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 31, 966990.
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. , Kahneman, D., 2002. Heuristics and Biases: The
Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Heylighen, F., Cilliers, P., Gershenson, C., 2007. Complexity and philosophy.
In: Bogg, J.A.R.G. (Ed.), Complexity, Science and Society. Radcliffe
Publishing, Oxford.
Jones, N.A., Ross, H., Lynam, T., Perez, P., Leitch, A., 2011. Mental models:
an interdisciplinary synthesis of theory and methods. Ecol. Soc. 16, 46.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., Tversky, A., 1982. Judgment Under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Kauffman, S.A., 1993. The Origins of Order: Self-Organization and Selection
in Evolution. Oxford University Press, New York.
Klayman, J., Ha, Y.-W., 1989. Hypothesis testing in rule discovery: strategy,
structure and content. J. Exp. Psychol. 5, 596604.
Knight, F.H., 1921. Risk, Uncertainty and Profit. Beard Books, Washington
DC.
Knuth, D.E., 1968. The art of computer programming. Fundamental Algorithms
vol. 1. Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA.
Kreps, D., 1992. Static choice and unforeseen contingencies. In: DASGUPTA,
P., GALE, D., HART, O., MASKIN, E. (Eds.), Economic Analysis
of Markets and Games: Essays in Honor of Frank Halm. MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA, pp. 259281.
Langan-Fox, J., Code, S., Langfield-Smith, K., 2000. Team mental models:
techniques, methods and analytic approaches. Hum. Factors 42, 242271.
Langan-Fox, J., Wirth, A., Code, S., Langfield-Smith, K., 2001. Analysing
shared and team mental models. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 28, 99112.
Langer, E.J., 1975. The illusion of control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 32, 311328.
Lenfle, S., 2011. The strategy of parallel approaches in projects with un-
foreseeable uncertainty: the Manhattan case in retrospect. Int. J. Proj.
Manag. 29, 359373.
Lenfle, S., Loch, C., 2010. Lost roots: how project management came to
emphasize control over flexibility and novelty. Calif. Manag. Rev. 53.
12 P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
Levinthal, D.A., 1997. Adaptation on rugged landscapes. Manag. Sci. 43,
934950.
Levinthal, D.A., March, J.G., 1993. The myopia of learning. Strateg. Manag. J.
14, 95112.
Littauer, S.B., Littauer, S.B., 1967. Aspects scientifiques essentiels du marketing
et progres des modeles en marketing. In: Langhoff, P., Churchman, C.W.,
Kuhn, H.W., M.K.S. (Eds.), Modèles, Mesures et Marketing. Entreprise
Moderne d'Editions.
Littauer,S.B., Ehrenfeld, 1964. Introduction to Statistical Methods. Mc Graw-Hill.
Little, T., 2005. Context-adaptative agility: managing complexity and uncertainty.
IEEE Softw. 22, 2835.
Loch, C.H., Teerwiesch, C., Thomke, S., 2001. Parallel and sequential testing
of design alternatives. Manag. Sci. 47, 663678.
Loch, C.H., Solt, M.E., Bailey, E.M., 2008. Diagnosing unforeseeable
uncertainty in a new venture. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 25, 2846.
Magnani, L., Nersessian, N., 2002. Model-Based Reasonin: Science, Technology,
Values. Kluwer, Dordrecht.
Makridakis, S.C., Hibon, M., 1979. Accuracy of forecasting: an empirical
investigation. J. R. Stat. Soc. 142, 97125.
Makridakis, S.C., Wheelwright, S.C., 1981. Forecasting an Organization's
Futures, Handbook of Organizational Design. Oxforf University Press,
Oxford, pp. 122138.
Marcus, M., 1977. The theory of connecting networks and their complexity: a
review. Proc. IEEE 65, 12631271.
Maturana, H.R., Varela, F.J., 1992. The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological
Roots of Understanding. Shambhala, Boston.
Maylor, H., Vidgen, R., Carver, S., 2008. Managerial complexity in project-
based operations: a ground model and its implications for practice. Proj.
Manag. J. 39, 1526.
Mcgrath, R.G., Macmillan, I., 1995. Discovery driven planning. Harv. Bus.
Rev. 73, 4454.
Mckelvey, B., 2004. Complexity science as order-creation science: new theory,
new method. E:CO 6, 227.
Minsky, H.P., 1996. Uncertainty and the institutional structure of capitalist
economies. J. Econ. Issues 30, 357368.
Modica, S., Rustichini, A., 1994. Awareness and partial information structure.
Theor. Decis. 37.
Moorman, C., Miner, A.S., 1998. The convergence of planning and execution:
improvisation in new product development. J. Mark. 62, 120.
Moorman, C., Miner, A.S., 2001. Organizational improvisation and learning: a
field study. Adm. Sci. Q. 46, 304337.
Moray, N., 1998. Identifying mental models of complex human-machine
systems. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 22, 293297.
Moray, N., 2004. Models of models of mental models. In: Moray, N. (Ed.),
Ergonomics: Major Writings. Taylor and Francis, London, UK, pp. 506526.
Morecroft, J.D.W., 1983. System dynamics: portraying bounded rationality.
Omega 11, 131142.
Morris, P.W.G., 2010. A brief history of project management, Chapter 1. In:
Morris, P.W.G., Pinto, J.K., Söderlund, J. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of
Project Management. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 3559.
Padalkar, M., Gopinath, S., 2016a. Are complexity and uncertainty distinct
concepts in project management? A taxonomical examination from literature.
Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34, 688700.
Padalkar, M., Gopinath, S., 2016b. Six decades of project management
research: thematic trends and future opportunities. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34,
13051321.
Paich, M., Sterman, J.D., 1993. Boom, bust, and failures to learn in experimental
markets. Manag. Sci. 39, 14391458.
Pant, P.N., Starbuck, W.H., 1990. Review of forecasting and research methods.
J. Manag. 16, 443460.
Perminova, O., Gustafsson, M., Wikström, K., 2008. Defining uncertainty in
projects: a new perspective. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 28, 7379.
Pinto, J.K., Winch, G., 2016. The unsettling of settled science: the past and
future of the management of projects. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34, 237245.
Qazi, A., Quigley, J., Dickson, A., Kirytopoulos, K., 2016. Project complexity
and risk management (ProCRiM): towards modelling project complexity
driven risk paths in construction projects. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34,
11831198.
Remington, K., Pollack, J., 2007. Tools for Complex Projects. Gower, London.
Ribbers, P.M.A., Schoo, K.-C., 2002. Program management and complexity of
ERP implementations. Eng. Manag. J. 14, 4552.
Rittel, H.W.J., Webber, M.M., 1973. Dilemmas in a general theory of planning.
Policy. Sci. 4, 155169.
Rivkin, J.W., 2000. Imitation of complex strategies. Manag. Sci. 46, 824844.
Rotheim, R., 1995. Keynes on uncertainty and individual behavior within a
theory of effective demand. In: Dow, S., Hillard, J. (Eds.), Keynes,
Knowledge and Uncertainty. Edward Elgar, Aldershot.
Rouse, W.B., Morris, N.M., 1986. On looking into the black box: prospects and
limits in the search for mental models. Psychol. Bull. 100, 349363.
Rubinstein, M.F., 1975. Patterns of Problem Solving. Prentice-Hall, New
Jersey.
Ruona, W.E.A., Lynham, S.A., 2004. A philosophical framework for thought
and practice in human resource development. Hum. Resour. Dev. Int. 7,
151164.
Sanders, T.I., 1998. Strategic Thinking and the new Science: Planning in the
Midst of Chaos Complexity and Change. Free Press, New York.
Sanderson, J., 2012. Risk, uncertainty and governance in megaprojects: a
critical discussion of alternative explanations. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 30,
432443.
Sargut, G., Mcgrath, R.G., 2011. Learning to live with complexity. Harv. Bus.
Rev. 89.
Schrader, S., Riggs, W.M., Smith, R.P., 1993. Choice over uncertainty and
ambiguity in technical problem solving. J. Eng. Technol. Manag. 10,
7399.
Senge, P.M., 1990. The Fifth Discipline, the art & Practice of the Learning
Organization. Random House Business Books, London.
Serrador, P., Turner, R., 2015a. The relationship between project success and
project efficiency. Proj. Manag. J. 46, 3039.
Serrador, P., Turner, R., 2015b. What is enough planning? Results from a
global quantitative study. IEEE Trans. Eng. Manag. 62, 462474.
Shenhar, A.J., Dvir, D., 1996. Toward a typological theory of project
management. Res. Policy 25, 607632.
Shewhart, W.A., 1931. Economic Control of Quality of Manufactured Product.
Van Nostrand, New York.
Simon, H.A., 1969. The Science of the Artificial. MIT Press, Boston.
Smyth, H.J., Morris, P.W.G., 2007. An epistemological evaluation of research
into projects and their management: methodological issues. Int. J. Proj.
Manag. 25, 423436.
Sommer, S.C., Loch, C.H., 2004. Selectionism and learning in projects with
complexity and unforeseeable uncertainty. Manag. Sci. 50, 13341347.
Sommer, S.C., Loch, C.H., Dong, J., 2009. Managing complexity and
unforeseeable uncertainty in startup companies: an empirical study. Organ.
Sci. 20, 118133.
Spencer, M.H., 1962. Uncertainty, expectations and foundations of the theory
of planning. J. Acad. Manag. 5, 197205.
Spencer, M.H., Siegelman, L., 1959. Managerial Economics. Decision Making
and Forward Planning. R.D. Irvin.
Stacey, R., 2007. The challenge of human interdependence: consequences for
thinking about the day to day practice of management in organizations. Eur.
Bus. Rev. 19, 292302.
Sterman, J.D., 1989. Modeling managerial behavior: misperceptions of
feedback in a dynamic decision making experiment. Manag. Sci. 35,
321339.
Sterman, J.D., 1992. System Dynamics Modeling for Project Management.
Sterman, J.D., 2000. Learning in and about complex systems. Reflections 1.
Sterman, J.D., 2001. System dynamics modeling: tools for learning in a
complex world. Calif. Manag. Rev. 43, 825.
Thomas, J., Mengel, T., 2008. Preparing project managers to deal with
complexity - advanced project management education. Int. J. Proj. Manag.
26, 304315.
Turner, J.R., Cochrane, R., 1993. Goals-and-methods matrix: coping with
projects ill-defined goals and/or methods of achieving them. Int. J. Proj.
Manag. 11, 93102.
Turner, R., Zolin, R., 2012. Forecasting success on large projects: developing
reliable scales to predict multiple perspectives by multiple stakeholders over
multiple time frames. Proj. Manag. J. 43, 8799.
13P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
Turner, R., Huemann, M., Anbari, F., Bredillet, C., 2010. Preface. Perspect.
Proj. xxixxii.
Vidal, L.A., Marle, F., 2008. Understanding project complexity: implications
on project management. Kybernetes 37, 10941110.
Von Bertalanffy, L., 1951. General System Theory: A new Approach to Unity
of Science. John Hopkins Press, Baltimore.
Von Foerster, H., 1960. On self-organizing systems and their environments. In:
Yovits, C., Cameron, S. (Eds.), Self-Organizing Systems. Pergamon Press,
London, pp. 3150.
Von Foerster, H., 1974. Cybernetics of Cybernetics. Illinois, Urbana.
Weaver, W., 1948. Science and complexity. Am. Sci. 36, 536544.
Weick, K.E., 1990. Introduction: cartographic myths in organizations. In: Huff,
A.S. (Ed.), Mapping Strategic Thought. John Wiley, New York, pp. 110.
White, D., Fortune, J., 2002. Current practice in Project Management - an
empirical study. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 20, 111.
Whitty, S.J., Maylor, H., 2009. And then came complex project management
(revised). Int. J. Proj. Manag. 27, 304310.
Wideman, R.M., 1992. Project & Program Risk Management. Project
Management Institute, Newton Square, PA.
Wiener, N., 1948/1961. Cybernetics, or Control and Communication in the
Animal and the Machine. Wiley & Sons, New York.
Williams, T.M., 1999. The need for new paradigms for complex projects. Int.
J. Proj. Manag. 17, 269273.
Williams, T.M., 2002. Modelling Complex Projects. John Wiley & Sons.
Williams, T., 2005. Assessing and moving on from the dominant project
management discourse in the light of project overruns. IEEE Trans. Eng.
Manag. 52, 497508.
Xia, W., Lee, G., 2005. Complexity of information systems development
projects: conceptualization and measurement development. J. Manag. Inf.
Syst. 22, 4583.
Xiang, C., Yang, Z., Zhang, L., 2016. Improving IS development teams'
performance during requirement analysis in projectthe perspectives from
shared mental model and emotional intelligence. Int. J. Proj. Manag. 34,
12661279.
Yang, H.-D., Kang, H.-R., Mason, R.M., 2008. An exploratory study on meta
skills in software development teams: antecedent cooperation skills and
personality for shared mental models. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 17, 4761.
14 P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel / International Journal of Project Management xx (2017) xxxxxx
Please cite this article as: P.A. Daniel, C. Daniel, 2017. Complexity, uncertainty and mental models: From a paradigm of regulation to a paradigm of emergence in
project management, Int. J. Proj. Manag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijproman.2017.07.004
... With the world becoming more and more complex (Daniel & Daniel, 2018), the involvement of various stakeholders with different perspectives is needed to address complexity and enhance our understanding of the situations faced (Patton, 2011). To explore the growing complexity, research projects are including individuals and organisations from varied worldviews, cultures and disciplines (Daniel & Daniel, 2018). ...
... With the world becoming more and more complex (Daniel & Daniel, 2018), the involvement of various stakeholders with different perspectives is needed to address complexity and enhance our understanding of the situations faced (Patton, 2011). To explore the growing complexity, research projects are including individuals and organisations from varied worldviews, cultures and disciplines (Daniel & Daniel, 2018). For instance, agricultural research for development projects usually comprise teams composed of scientists and researchers, farmers and development professionals from different institutions and organisations. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
How a project is perceived by its stakeholders affects how it is implemented, and how the outcomes of the project are interpreted by the stakeholders influences the impact project can have on those stakeholders. Often this diversity of perspective is considered an impediment to the effectiveness of the project in meeting its goals. Standard project evaluation techniques dependent on linear and conventional methods to assess and present outputs and outcomes from projects fail to consider the complexity in projects. Complexity in a project arises from the involvement of multiple stakeholders from diverse disciplines, backgrounds, and geographies, and having varied perceptions, expectations, and understanding of the project and its aspects. The overall aim of this PhD is to improve the understanding of evaluation of complex projects by studying the projects from the perspectives of the multiple stakeholders involved in them. The first objective is to explore and understand the approaches to evaluation drawing on perspectives from literature, and observations from the field. The second objective is to understand the perspectives of stakeholders operating at various levels of a complex project on different aspects of the project such as its nature, approach, outputs, and outcomes. The third objective is to relate outcomes at various levels in the project to processes used, as well as associate outputs with outcomes. The fourth objective is to develop an integrated approach to evaluate complex multi- stakeholder projects, which enhances a project’s outcomes and enables learning for the stakeholders involved. With the aim of improvement in the existing knowledge on evaluating complex projects, the methodological approach is developed from a combination of theories and practices on evaluation. Central themes of the methodology are methodological pluralism, multiple perspectives, systems thinking, and appreciation and learning. To facilitate flexibility in navigating through a variety of theories and perspectives to enable both change and enhancement, the PhD is undertaken and presented as an action research. Three complex projects with stakeholders from diverse backgrounds and disciplines are examined in two stages of this thesis. These projects situated on the Chotanagpur Plateau in India with different intervention areas are, i) an agricultural research for development (AR4D) project, ii) a project to develop the skills of community youth to impart education, and iii) a Corporate Social Responsibility initiative. Data are collected from 82 project participants chosen by purposive sampling in the form of narratives, through semi-structured questionnaires. Findings from examining multiple perspectives were similar across the three studied projects. Stakeholders interpreted the nature and outcomes of the project uniquely. This study confirmed the existence of diverse stakeholder perspectives that were not captured or acknowledged in the evaluation of the three projects. These perspectives, however, were important for the stakeholders in how they identified with the project, how they functioned in it, and eventually, how it impacted their lives. Moreover, largely, there was no cognisance of this diversity in the stakeholders of the project. In instances where the stakeholders were aware of the multiple views, there was no mechanism for interaction of, or sharing those perspectives. Neither did the project stakeholders learn to acknowledge and work with varied perspectives, nor did they learn from multiple views in the project which were different from theirs. Besides the standard outputs and outcomes from the project, the project stakeholders outlined long-term personal changes. In particular, the learning which they underwent was considered profound and significant. The subtle shifts in learning and development of capabilities in project stakeholders were capabilities that enhance their sense of agency and change their worldviews, which they may further utilise to impact the project, themselves, and others. In considering these findings and addressing the challenge of incorporating complexity in project evaluation, the thesis develops a framework to evaluate complex projects. The framework is complexity-appreciative which acknowledges, appreciates, and integrates multiple perspectives in the design and evaluation of projects. Evaluation frameworks are always dependent on the contexts in which they are applied, and on those who design and use them, and the kind of boundary judgements they make. Hence, the framework provided in this PhD is not a tool to be used at the end of a project to measure its outcomes; rather, it is a process that must be part of a project from inception as a feedback tool to enhance outcomes. The framework can become a means to create spaces and processes in a project to enable stakeholders to share perspectives, listen to others, understand the diversity in the project, and acknowledge, appreciate and learn from each other’s perspectives as well as each other’s process of learning. Such a space will also allow stakeholders to find their voice and purposes in the project, to help each other do the same, and to further develop those purposes
... Multi-task personnel to handle operational and project (Daniel & Daniel, 2018), ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The Lumut Balai geothermal field in Indonesia, operated by PT Pertamina Geothermal Energy Tbk, faced challenges during the EPC (Engineering, Procurement, Construction) Power Plant project of Unit 1 from 2014 to 2019, which was significantly different from the Power Plant and FCRS development of Unit 2 from 2022 to 2024. Despite facing different challenges, effective project management is crucial for the successful execution of the project. This study aims to introduce a business model for project management in the consortium scheme of the EPC Lumut Balai Geothermal Power Plant Unit 2. The consortium comprises three contractors with expertise in EPC project construction. The business model focuses on the simplified business process, command chain, and the relationships among authority, stakeholders, shareholders, media, communities, and reporting procedures between the owner and the consortium. The results are based on lesson learned and an understanding of the actual conditions from the project's initiation to the current phase of execution and control. The business model framework has been developed to address the uniqueness, complexity, challenges, and strategies of handling the EPC project profile of Lumut Balai Power Plant development.
... Drawing on the work of Keynes (1937) and Knight (1921) and the theoretical distinction between risk and uncertainty several researchers have attempted to develop typologies of risk and uncertainty along a continuum that elaborates the dichotomy further in an attempt to develop models linking cognition and action (Daniel and Daniel, 2018). We summarize the most relevant for construction project organizing in table 3.1 taking the resolvable-radical dichotomy from Kay and King (2020) to provide a conceptualization of the uncertainty continuum. ...
... Generating interpretable and explainable rules is essential for gaining insights into the underlying patterns and relationships present in the data. Traditional rule generation approaches often face challenges in handling uncertainty and producing rules that are both accurate and comprehensible (Daniel & Daniel, 2018) (Cheng et al., 2018) (Govindan et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
This research proposes a hybrid approach for adaptive fuzzy grid partitioning and rule generation using rough set theory to address the problem of customer segmentation based on purchasing behavior. The objective is to minimize the fuzziness of the partitioning while maximizing the accuracy and interpretability of the generated rules. The research utilizes a dataset consisting of customer transactions, including demographics, purchase details, and satisfaction ratings. The fuzzy grid partitioning process divides the customer space into grid cells, representing different segments. Fuzzy membership values are assigned to data points based on their association with each grid cell. Rough set theory is employed for attribute reduction, identifying the most relevant attributes for customer segmentation. Rule induction algorithms generate rules that capture the patterns and dependencies among customer attributes and their association with specific grid cells. The hybrid approach combines the advantages of adaptive fuzzy grid partitioning and rough set-based rule generation. The optimization process adjusts fuzzy membership values and refines the generated rules to improve accuracy and interpretability. A numerical example and a case study in the retail industry are presented to demonstrate the effectiveness of the proposed approach. Results show successful customer segmentation and generation of actionable rules for marketing strategies. The research contributes to the field of customer segmentation by providing a comprehensive methodology that integrates adaptive fuzzy grid partitioning and rule generation using rough set theory. The hybrid approach offers valuable insights into customer behavior, enabling targeted marketing campaigns, personalized recommendations, and enhanced customer satisfaction.
... Neste estudo, a complexidade é tratada como complexidade tecnológica. Nos estudos de gerenciamento de projetos, a complexidade não é um conceito claro e unificado (Daniel & Daniel, 2018), e sua definição não encontra consenso entre os estudiosos (Bakhshi et al., 2016). Para trazer esse conceito alinhado ao VUCA, mas em um contexto de projeto, este estudo considera complexidade como complexidade tecnológica, definida por Baccarini (1996) como muitas variáveis e partes inter-relacionadas operacionalizadas em diferenciação e interdependência. ...
... In this study, complexity is treated as technological complexity. In project management studies, complexity is not a clear and unified concept (Daniel & Daniel, 2018), and its definition does not find consensus among scholars (Bakhshi et al., 2016). To bring this concept into alignment with VUCA, but in a project context, this study considers complexity as technological complexity, defined by Baccarini (1996) as many variables and interrelated parts operationalized in differentiation and interdependence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to the ever-changing work environment in the age of digital transformation, project managers need to adapt to an environment which is volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous (VUCA). Many organizations adopt management methods based on strict project management planning, assuming that they are the best way to succeed in any situation. However, projects may depend on flexibility to achieve success. This study aims to analyze the impact of adverse project environments on project success and the effect of the management method choice. A PLS-SEM model is tested on a survey of 332 project professionals. Findings showed that choosing a method that best fits the project's environment can help catch up on project success only when it undergoes frequent changes throughout its life cycle.
... Regarding the constitution of PPB, there is a consensus among scholars that PPB is highly complex, which is not only determined by financial metrics but also depends on multiple nonfinancial aspects [13], [14], [15], such as politics, situation, company reputation, culture, decision-making style, leaderships, and prospects [16]. Furthermore, as Tansakul and Yenradee [17] pointed out, PP is also a combination of its components, among them, there exist multiple interrelationships, which could promote the generation of synergies, such as sharing of resources, knowledge, and alike [18], leading to the benefits result of "1+1>2," i.e., synergy benefits [14], [15]. This indicates that measuring PPB by directly summing across the benefits generated by constituent projects is inadequate [19], [20]. ...
Article
To facilitate the project portfolio benefits (PPB) management and realize the maximization of the benefits, a PPB measurement model based on the genetic algorithm (GA)-BP neural network group (GA-BPNNG) is proposed in current research. Unlike traditional benefits management approaches, the proposed model takes full cooperation and information sharing advantages of group learning to measure PPB more accurately, which considers both the financial and nonfinancial benefits and synergy benefits generated from the interrelationships among project portfolio components. To ascertain the priority of the PPB measurement model, the GA-BPNNG model is compared with BPNN and GA-BPNN, two common models in the literature. The mean square error of the GA-BPNNG model reduced by 94% and 83%, respectively, when compared to the BPNN and GA-BPNN models. The results suggest that the PPB measurement model performs more effectively. Therefore, it could be concluded that the proposed model has a better nonlinear fitting effect for PPB measurement. This article can support managers, decision-makers, and management in realizing strategy by providing a practical and scalable tool for PPB measurement. Index Terms-Benefits management (BM), genetic algorithm (GA)-BP neural network group (GA-BPNNG), project portfolio (PP), project portfolio benefits (PPBs) measurement, synergy benefits.
Article
The identification of plausible desirable and undesirable future events is fundamentally a cognitive process of prospecting possible futures. Yet, organizational practices designed for the identification and management of these uncertain futures, specifically project risk management, heed little attention to the role of cognition in these processes. Building on the theory of ‘pragmatic prospection’, we address this gap and examine the cognitive processes involved in prospection of desirable vs. undesirable project futures while identifying opportunities vs. risks. Empirically, we analyse the information search behaviour and post-hoc search verbalization in an experimental project risk and opportunity identification task. We find that risk identification relies often on simpler approaches with lower cognitive load, while opportunities triggered more explicit information search strategies and were more prone to evoke agency within the imagined futures. These findings challenge the assumption – widely held in risk management practice – that risks and opportunities can be approached by the same processes. We conclude with an outlook on how a better understanding of the involved cognitive processes can support foresight activities in project planning and beyond.
Article
Full-text available
The field of marketing strategy often makes the important assumption that marketing strategy should occur by first composing a plan on the basis of a careful review of environmental and firm information and then executing that plan. However, there are cases when the composition and execution of an action converge in time so that, in the limit, they occur simultaneously. The authors define such a convergence as improvisation and develop hypotheses to investigate the conditions in which improvisation is likely to occur and be effective. The authors test these hypotheses in a longitudinal study of new product development activities. Results show that organizational improvisation occurs moderately in organizations and that organizational memory level decreases and environmental turbulence level increases the incidence of improvisation. Results support traditional concerns that improvisation can reduce new product effectiveness but also indicate that environmental and organizational factors can reduce negative effects and sometimes create a positive effect for improvisation. These results suggest that, in some contexts, improvisation may be not only what organizations actually practice but also what they should practice to flourish.
Article
Both practitioners and researchers in the field of project management have referred to problems caused by complexity or problems of particular significance to complex projects. In different scientific disciplines investigations into the behavior of complex dynamical systems are revealing insights that, taken together, amount to a challenge to the prevalent Cartesian/Newtonian/Enlightenment paradigm from which the practice of project management has emerged. Concepts such as nonlinearity, emergence, self-organization, and radical unpredictability have major implications for the uncodified paradigm that underpins project management practice and research. Taken together, they amount to a complementary way of thinking and talking about projects and their management that might shed new light on intractable problems that appear to plague certain areas of project management practice. One strand within complexity studies that holds particular promise is complex responsive processes of relating, a means of talking about how human beings interact and learn and how their interactions evolve over time and across space. A new program of research, of which this paper forms part, will apply this conceptual framework to the lived experience of project teams, including executive sponsors, project managers and project team members.
Book
It is widely acknowledged that traditional Project Management techniques are no longer sufficient, as projects become more complex and client's demand reduced timescales. Problems that arise include inadequate planning and risk analysis, ineffective project monitoring and control, and uninformed post-mortem analysis. Effective modelling techniques, which capture the complexities of such projects, are therefore necessary for adequate project management. This book looks at those issues, describes some modelling techniques, then discusses their merits and possible synthesis.
Article
Project management as a discipline possesses a rich body of literature characterized by early determinism and later expansion to broader contexts aided by paradigmatic, thematic, and methodological diversity. The dynamic nature of research entails many parallel streams of enquiry under differing perspectives without convergence to parsimonious theories. We argue that an integrated view of project management research in terms of its thematic evolution and trends is necessary for an understanding of future directions. Our study fills this gap by tracing the evolution of themes in project management research, trends, and future opportunities through a systematic review of literature. We find the research to be dominated by empirical and deterministic perspectives while non-deterministic research enquiry remains weak and sporadic. We contend that stronger focus on non-deterministic perspective and a methodological convergence is necessary for the research to meaningfully advance towards theory building, and discuss potential avenues for further research.
Article
Numerous studies have investigated factors affecting the project requirement analysis of information system development (ISD) teams from the view of technology, but our research focused on how developers' behaviors affected project team members' requirements analysis work from the emotional intelligence (EI) and shared mental model (SMM) perspectives. Specifically, we separated SMM into task-related SMM and member-related SMM to examine their impacts on ISD teams' performances during requirement analysis phase. Then we chose four scales of EI to research the relationships between them and SMMs. Using the approach of structural equation model, the results indicated that two aspects of SMM both have significant and positive impact on team performance, and EI could be the antecedents of SMM. The results indicate that SMM could enhance the influences of EI on project team performance, so the choice of individual team members and the team building are both significant to ISD teams for better performance in project requirement analysis. © 2016 Elsevier Ltd and Association for Project Management and the International Project Management Association
Article
Project complexity has been extensively explored in the literature because of its contribution towards the failure of major projects in terms of cost and time overruns. Focusing on the interface of Project Complexity and Interdependency Modelling of Project Risks, we propose a new process that aids capturing interdependency between project complexity, complexity induced risks and project objectives. The proposed modelling approach is grounded in the theoretical framework of Expected Utility Theory and Bayesian Belief Networks. We consider the decision problem of identifying critical risks and selecting optimal risk mitigation strategies at the commencement stage of a project, taking into account the utility function of the decision maker with regard to the importance of project objectives and holistic interaction between project complexity and risk. The proposed process is supported by empirical research that was conducted in the construction industry and its application is illustrated through a simulation study.
Article
This paper addresses several issues of broad concern in the United States: population trends; the quality of urban life; national policy for urban growth; and the unexpected, ineffective, or detrimental results often generated by government programs in these areas.