ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Two experiments illustrate that the perception of a given time duration slows when white participants observe faces of black men, but only if participants are concerned with appearing biased. In Experiment 1 the concern with the appearance of bias is measured as a chronic state using the external motivation to respond without prejudice scale (Plant & Devine, 1998). In Experiment 2 it is manipulated by varying the race of the experimenter (black versus white). Time perception is assessed via a temporal discrimination task commonly used in the literature. Models of time perception identify arousal as a factor that causes perceived time to slow, and we speculate that arousal arising in intergroup interactions can alter time perception.
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Distortion in time perception as a result of
concern about appearing biased
Gordon B. Moskowitz
1
*, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten
2
, Cynthia M. Gooch
3
1Lehigh University, Psychology Department, Bethlehem, PA, United States of America, 2Lehigh University,
Psychology Department, Bethlehem, PA, United States of America, 3Temple University, Program in
Neuroscience, Philadelphia, PA, United States of America
*gbm@lehigh.edu
Abstract
Two experiments illustrate that the perception of a given time duration slows when white
participants observe faces of black men, but only if participants are concerned with appear-
ing biased. In Experiment 1 the concern with the appearance of bias is measured as a
chronic state using the external motivation to respond without prejudice scale (Plant &
Devine, 1998). In Experiment 2 it is manipulated by varying the race of the experimenter
(black versus white). Time perception is assessed via a temporal discrimination task com-
monly used in the literature. Models of time perception identify arousal as a factor that
causes perceived time to slow, and we speculate that arousal arising in intergroup interac-
tions can alter time perception.
Introduction
Implicit bias is typically defined as stereotyping and prejudice that impacts people outside of
awareness and without conscious intent [1]. It arises when a stereotype or an attitude (or both)
is triggered outside of awareness by a cue associated with a social group. These “primed” stereo-
types and affective responses manifest as bias toward that group by shaping how we categorize,
where we allocate attention, the types of judgments and inferences we form, the expectations
and standards we set, what we consider valid and veridical, how we feel, our approach and
avoidance tendencies, and how we more generally act [2]. Does it manifest in something as
basic, and low-level, in our psychological experience as how we perceive?
A relatively small body of empirical work examines bias in perception due to social stimuli
[311]. These are largely focused on bias to visual perception (by race, but also by culture,
expectations, norms, values and goals). For example, in the domain of race, Eberhardt et al. [6]
had participants view a racially ambiguous face. For some participants the face was labeled
“black,” for others it was labeled “white”. The task was to draw the face while looking at it. The
facial features and skin tone of the drawings made of the face labeled “black” were more stereo-
typic (e.g., darker skin) than those labeled “white”. Such stereotypic drawings are evidence of
the perceiver’s biased perception of the face that served as the model.
Just as what we see is distorted during the perceptual process by race, time perception may
be distorted by race. Prior research outside the domain of race has shown that emotionally-
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 1 / 18
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Moskowitz GB, Olcaysoy Okten I, Gooch
CM (2017) Distortion in time perception as a result
of concern about appearing biased. PLoS ONE 12
(8): e0182241. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0182241
Editor: Warren H. Meck, Duke University, UNITED
STATES
Received: September 7, 2016
Accepted: July 14, 2017
Published: August 8, 2017
Copyright: ©2017 Moskowitz et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: Data are available at
Open Science Framework: osf.io/6y8j4.
Funding: The authors received no specific funding
for this work.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
laden stimuli in the form of angry facial expressions can distort time perception [1214]. The
nature of this distortion is an overestimation of time–time slows. To date, only one published
set of experiments examines the impact of race as a biasing influence in time perception [15].
That research found that white participants perceived faces of black men to be of a longer
duration than faces of white men, but only among participants who found race to be a threat-
ening topic.
The current research examines the question of whether race impacts time perception at the
level of milliseconds to seconds. Specifically, it examines the influence of race on the percep-
tion of the duration of face presentation. We argue that because arousal is known to slow the
perception of time [1618], individuals who associate arousal with race will have time percep-
tion distorted, slowed. In a first experiment we posit that white participants who have strong
chronic concerns about appearing biased to African Americans will exhibit a time perception
bias when perceiving faces of black men. This experiment is an exact replication of an earlier
experiment [15], with an additional condition added to examine the impact of a heightened
egalitarian goal as a mitigating influence on the bias. White individuals in the United States
who associate arousal with race are identified in this experiment using the external motivation
to control prejudice scale (EMCP) that had been used in the earlier experiment [15]. The
nature of this scale, and why it is believed to assess a relationship between race and arousal
[19], is described below.
In a second experiment we manipulated whether white participants were placed in an
experiment with a black experimenter to test the hypothesis that a cross-race interaction will
trigger arousal even in White participants who do not have strong chronic concerns about
appearing biased to African Americans. We posit that when a white participant (who is not
chronically high in EMCP) is placed in a racially defined situation, arousal will be triggered
and this will slow time perception to faces of black men.
Influences on time perception
A dominant model of time perception (Fig 1) argues that time perception occurs through
several processes [16]. First, a pacemaker emits pulses at a constant rate. Second, when
paying attention to the duration of a stimulus, an attentional switch closes, allowing the
pulses to reach an accumulator. Third, the accumulator keeps count of the pulses emitted
by the pacemaker. Fourth, current accumulator values are compared to a reference mem-
ory for the number of pulses that previously marked the learned duration (such as a half
second). Time perception is based on a representation of the subjective count of the
pulses that accrued in the accumulator as it compares to the reference memory for the
number of pulses comprising that duration. (e.g., a representation of the pulses indicat-
ing a half second). Finally, a comparator engages in monitoring of the discrepancy
between accumulated pulses and a threshold level of pulses defined by the stored repre-
sentation. When the comparator reveals the difference between accumulator value and
reference memory value drops below a threshold, one perceives that the duration being
tracked has transpired.
Arousal, and attention to the passage of time, can distort the perception of time by impact-
ing these processing stages. High levels of arousal (induced through noxious noise, drugs,
physical exertion, facial expressions, etc.) speeds up the internal clock by causing more pulses
in the same amount of time. Thus, the accumulator value that matches a reference memory
arrives sooner. A duration of 500 ms might feel like it has arrived after only 250 ms, making
the actual time of 500 ms feel longer. That is, time slows—the world outside seems to be mov-
ing slower [1314][20]. A given time interval is perceived as longer.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 2 / 18
Attention to time’s passage has similar consequences on perception of a stimulus’ duration.
With focus on time (such as when watching the clock when bored), time slows. According to
the model, the attentional switch closes and opens as vigilance waxes and wanes, and when
focused on a stimulus’ duration, vigilance to time is heightened and the switch is closed more
than it typically would be. With the switch closed, more pulses get to the accumulator during a
given duration. Thus, the correct number of pulses to match the standard accrues more
quickly, and the reference value is reached before the target duration has actually transpired.
This causes the experience of the standard time as arriving sooner, making time seem to slow;
the length of a given duration is overestimated [21].
Fig 1. Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) model of time perception. Adapted from Church, 1984. To summarize: a
pacemaker releases isochronous signals, which are collected by an accumulator function when attention is directed to time
passing. The accumulator values can be compared to a reference memory for accumulator values from a previousexposure
to the time interval, to determine whether the currently timed interval has lasted a similar duration (comparator function). If
the difference between the value from reference memory and the current accumulator value is below a given threshold, the
decision is made that the time interval is equal to the memory for a standard. The model was originally developed to explain
animal behavior in a fixed interval/peak interval paradigm, but has been applied to human timing as well.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241.g001
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 3 / 18
The motivation to avoid appearing biased
Cross-race interactions can be arousing. The more one is concerned with bias, the more taxing
and arousing cross-race interactions become [2225]. This manifests in malignant forms of
cardiovascular reactivity [26], nonverbal communication problems such as avoiding eye con-
tact, and setting greater interpersonal distance [2728], showing preference to ingroups via
greater cooperation, friendship seeking, and partner selection [2931], self-regulation failures
[3234], and terminating interactions sooner [3537].
Even striving to be egalitarian, and to not be biased, does not necessarily prevent one from
the experience of such arousal. Plant and Devine [19] posited two distinct goals that can poten-
tially be associated with egalitarianism. These two goals have disparate effects on the experi-
ence of arousal during cross race interactions. It is even the case that one manner of striving to
be unbiased actually exacerbates arousal and can unintentionally heighten bias.
Individuals with goals to act fair/unbiased that emerge from internal sources of motivation
(concern for equality, passion for social justice, desire for friends from diverse backgrounds
and cultures, etc.) view members of stereotyped outgroups as opportunities that afford one the
chance to pursue a cherished goal [3840]. Such interactions are not anxiety provoking for
people high in the internal motivation to control prejudice, but rather focus their attention on
goal-relevant people and stimuli [4142].
However, for individuals whose goals to act in an egalitarian way stem from external
sources (concern with: others opinions, not doing the socially incorrect thing, not wanting to
seem biased, etc.), cross-race interactions become an arousing chance for bias and social
incompetency to be discovered [19] [24] [43]. It is precisely this type of externalized egalitarian
goal, and its associated racial arousal, that is measured by the external motivation to control
prejudice (EMCP) scale. Prior research has established that although EMCP is not associated
with domain-general measures of social desirability, it does predict high arousal during inter-
group responses [38] [43]. People who are high in EMCP have heightened arousal responses,
heightened self-consciousness, and heightened focus on expressing bias when contemplating
and engaged in cross-race interactions–even when white participants are simply presented
with faces of black men [33] [38] [44]. Anything that triggers race for participants high in
EMCP has been shown to also trigger arousal and bias associated with that arousal (such as
directed attention to the arousing stimulus).
Race and perceptual experience
Models of time perception dictate that when individuals are aroused, the perception of time
(duration) for a target stimulus slows. Research on motivation to control prejudice illustrates
that high EMCP is defined in white American participants by an arousal linked to the concern
with the appearance of being biased toward African Americans. Thus, our hypothesis is that
time perception is impacted by race, but only among people who are concerned with the
appearance of bias. This should occur for people whose basis for egalitarian action is externally
motivated (high EMCP). Time perception being impacted by race will be manifested in esti-
mates of stimulus duration–the duration of black faces will feel longer than the duration of
white faces that are presented for the same length of time. The perceptual distortion, therefore,
should not emerge when people do not experience arousal, such as when they are not chroni-
cally oriented to have external concerns with appearing biased.
However, it might be possible that the arousal that people high in EMCP experience can be
ameliorated by temporary conditions that create egalitarian goals for which the striving to be
non-prejudiced is internally located. That is, if we create a conflict between their chronic
EMCP goals and their temporary internal egalitarian goals, the temporary and internal goals
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 4 / 18
might drive responding. To explore this possibility we manipulated EMCP, but also indepen-
dently introduced a goal priming procedure [45] that temporarily manipulated the strength of
one’s internal egalitarian goals.
Experiment 1
Materials and methods
Procedure. Levels of EMCP [19] were recorded during an online testing session as part of
registration for a voluntary participant pool. Between 2 to 8 weeks after the registration period
participants were brought to the lab one at a time and asked to perform a temporal discrimina-
tion task on the computer. After providing signed consent they were brought to a small room
with a single computer for the task. Task instructions were displayed on screen; the experi-
menter stayed in the room until participants indicated they understood the procedure. A pro-
cedure was used in which a stimulus was presented for a standard time, and participants were
asked to judge whether a comparison stimulus (e.g., a face) appeared for a longer or shorter
time than the standard stimulus. This response choice repeated across more than 200 stimulus
displays (trials). Race of faces and the duration for which they were presented was manipulated
as a within-participants variable across these many trials. Unbeknownst to participants goal
primes were subliminally flashed prior to each trial of the discrimination task, thus manipulat-
ing goals between participants (egalitarian goal versus control condition). When the task fin-
ished, participants were informed by on-screen instructions to exit and meet the experimenter
in an adjacent room (where they had signed their consent). They were then debriefed and
thanked for their participation. This research involved human subjects and the protocol was
approved by Lehigh University’s institutional review board. The work was carried out in accor-
dance with the provisions of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.
Participants. Participants were 63 white students (37 women and 26 men) enrolled in the
introduction to psychology course at Lehigh University, ranging in age from 18–22. Participa-
tion was voluntary and compensated with a credit toward a course requirement. Two partici-
pants’ responses could not be fit to the curve that was used to calculate the point of subjective
equality that is explained below (as they gave the same response, e.g., “longer”, on most trials
of the time perception task despite the varying durations of the stimuli they were meant to be
judging). Because the unit of analysis that summarized their responses (the PSE) could not be
calculated by the MATLAB formula we employed (see below), those two participants’ had no
data to enter into our analyses, and hence their data were dropped in our analyses. The high
EMCP group was 64% women, compared to the group of people not high in EMCP which was
56% women.
The sample size was determined by a power analysis using the software program “More-
Power,” Version 6.0.1 [46]. The power analysis suggested that for an effect size between small
to medium [15] (η
p
= .1 and η
p
= .2, α= .05 and power = .9, repeated measures: 1 (3-level),
independent measures: 2 x 2) we would need between 32 to 64 participants. Thus, we decided
to request 64 white participants from the participant pool and included as many of them in the
experiment as we could recruit. Our stopping rule was to end data collection when we
recruited 64 participants or ran out of participants in the pool eligible for the experiment
(whichever came first).
Assessment of motivation to respond without prejudice. External motivation to
respond without prejudice (EMCP) was assessed with a previously published and validated
scale [19]. The scale consists of five questions assessed on a 9-point scale: Because of today’s
PC (politically correct) standards I try to appear nonprejudiced toward Black people, I try to
hide any negative thoughts about Black people in order to avoid negative reactions from
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 5 / 18
others, If I acted prejudiced toward Black people I would be concerned that others would be
angry with me, I attempt to appear nonprejudiced toward Black people in order to avoid disap-
proval from others, and I try to act nonprejudiced toward Black people because of pressure
from others. The administration of the measure occurred several weeks prior to the experi-
ment during registration for a voluntary participant pool maintained by the psychology
department at Lehigh University. The registration included a battery of measures used among
multiple experiments in the department. Participants with EMCP scores more than one stan-
dard deviation above the mean were labeled as high in EMCP, those remaining were labeled as
not high in EMCP as in past research [15].
Time perception task. Time perception was assessed by a temporal-discrimination task
in which participants responded to objects (i.e., geometric shapes) and faces. The method of
constant stimuli was used; that is, on each trial, participants saw an image of a geometric shape
in dark grey for a standard length of time (the standard duration) and then one image taken
from a series of possible comparison stimuli; the second image, or comparison stimulus,
appeared for different lengths of time. On some trials, the comparison stimuli were again geo-
metric shapes (dark grey in color). On other trials the comparison stimuli were photographs of
male faces selected from a widely-used database of faces that has been normed and pretested
for perceived age and perceived emotional expression [47]. Half the faces were white men and
half were black men, all photographed in black and white, and all were designated in prior
research as having neutral emotional expressions.
On a given trial in this task, two images were presented sequentially on the computer moni-
tor, first the standard stimulus and then the comparison stimulus. The participant was to indi-
cate (by pressing buttons marked “shorter” and “longer” on the keyboard) whether the
comparison stimulus had been presented longer than the standard stimulus had been pre-
sented. On every trial, the standard stimulus was one of several possible geometric shapes pre-
sented for 600 ms. Comparison stimuli were manipulated across trials to be either shapes or
faces. The race of the faces was manipulated to be either black or white. The experiment com-
prised four blocks of trials; each block contained 21 trials with shapes as comparison stimuli,
21 trials with black male faces as comparison stimuli, and 21 trials with white male faces as
comparison stimuli.
The duration of the comparison stimulus was manipulated across trials; comparison stimuli
appeared for either 300, 380, 480, 600, 760, 960, or 1,200 milliseconds (ms). These durations
were spaced logarithmically around the standard interval (i.e., 600 ms). Given the scalar prop-
erty of time perception, such that discrimination is finer between two shorter intervals than
between two longer intervals, this spacing promoted equal difficulty in discriminating shorter
and longer intervals. Additionally, when plotting duration on the X axis and percentage of tri-
als in which one responded “longer” on the Y axis, such spacing creates a more proportional
sigmoid curve than would emerge using a linearly spaced set of durations. Paired with each
duration in each block there were three shapes as comparison stimuli, three black male faces as
comparison stimuli, and three white male faces as comparison stimuli. This created twelve
pairings of each type of comparison stimulus type and each duration across the experiment.
This task typically produces high accuracy at the extreme long and short durations (300 and
1,200 ms); among the remaining durations, for which the correct answer is more ambiguous,
accuracy is variable. When duration is plotted on the x-axis and the percentage of “longer”
responses is plotted on the y-axis, the result is an S-shaped curve. When time slows, a compari-
son stimulus that appears for less than 600 ms is more likely to be inaccurately labeled as being
“longer” than the standard time of 600 ms and a comparison stimulus that appears for longer
than 600 ms is more likely to be accurately labeled as “longer”. This results in the sigmoid
curve shifting left as a result of slowed time perception.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 6 / 18
Goal manipulation. The temporary triggering of egalitarian goals was accomplished
through a “goal priming manipulation” commonly used in the literature [45]. This task
exposed participants either to words relating to egalitarian goals or to control words. Before
each trial of the discrimination task a word was subliminally presented (shown for 15 ms).
These two sets of words were drawn from prior research [41]. Half the participants had words
related to egalitarianism subliminally flashed (equity, tolerance, fair, egalitarian, broadminded,
justice), half had control words flashed that were matched for valence, length and frequency in
the English language (warmhearted, responsible, courteous, humility, integrity, kindhearted).
Results
The hypothesis was that levels of external motivation to respond without prejudice would pre-
dict time distortion. The first evidence of time slowing when participants perceived faces of
Black men was provided by the leftward shift of the S-shaped curve that resulted when dura-
tion was plotted on the x-axis and the percentage of “longer” responses was plotted on the y-
axis. As illustrated in Fig 2A and 2B, the curve representing high-EMCP participants’ percep-
tion of duration of presentation of black male faces was shifted leftward compared with their
curves for white male faces. However, participants who were not high in EMCP responded to
the two types of faces in an almost identical fashion.
The primary measure of the slowing of time was the point of subjective equality (PSE). The
PSE is a single period of time that represented the subjective perception of the comparison-stimu-
lus duration being equal to the standard time of 600 ms. If time perception is slowing, the PSE will
be lower (a shorter amount of time is mistakenly perceived as equivalent to the standard dura-
tion). Participants high in EMCP should perceive the PSE to be lower to faces of black men than
to faces of white men. No such difference should emerge in participants not high in EMCP.
The PSE for each type of comparison stimulus is calculated by plotting each person’s dura-
tion judgments (longer/shorter) across the twelve trials of each type of comparison stimulus at
each duration. The duration of stimulus presentation is plotted on the x-axis, and the propor-
tion of trials in which a comparison stimulus was judged to be longer than the standard is plot-
ted on the y-axis. The data points approximate an S-shaped curve and are fitted to the logistic
function 1/10
[a×(xb)]
, in which ais an index of the slope, brepresents the PSE, and xis the
duration. Fits were made using the function lsqcurvefit in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick,
MA). This function optimizes parameters of an equation (in this case, aand b) by minimizing
the squared error between the data points and the curve produced by the equation. It uses the
trust-region-reflective algorithm to make adjustments to the parameters to minimize sum of
squares. This produces the PSE as a time interval signified by a point on the curve at which a
participant is 50% likely to judge the comparison duration to be “longer” and “shorter” than
the standard duration. That is, it is their perception of the standard time of 600 ms.
PSEs for each stimulus type (Black men, White men and Shapes) were calculated for each
participant. This calculation provided three continuous PSE variables (one for each stimulus
type). In addition, difference scores between (a) the PSE for faces of Black men and the PSE for
shapes and (b) the PSE for faces of White men and the PSE for shapes were calculated for each
participant. The difference score provided an index of the probability of misidentifying a time
as longer than 600 ms for faces compared with shapes. That is, responses to shapes served as a
baseline measure for the task against which responses to two different types of faces were eval-
uated. A PSE for a class of stimuli (such as faces of black men) lower than that found for con-
trol stimuli (geometric shapes) would indicate that time had slowed for perceiving that class of
stimuli. Thus, the slowing or accelerating of time was indicated by PSE differences between sti-
muli of interest (white and black male faces) and control stimuli.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 7 / 18
We conducted a Stimulus Type X EMCP X Goal Prime ANOVA with PSE difference score
as the dependent variable and stimulus type, EMCP group and goal prime as the independent
variables. There was no effect of the goal priming–no interactions or main effects with the sub-
liminal goal prime emerged. Therefore, the effect of goal prime will no longer be discussed.
Fig 2. Experiment 1: Percent of “longer” responses at each stimulus duration for each stimulus type.
Participants were divided on the basis of EMCP scores (see Methods). (a) Responses to pictures of Black
Men and White Men in participants that did not score high on this measure. (b) Responses in participants that
did score high on this measure. The dotted arrow in (b) signifies a shift of responses to Black Men in the High
EMCP participants.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241.g002
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 8 / 18
However, the predicted interaction between EMCP and stimulus type emerged in this three-
way ANOVA, F(1, 57) = 4.77, p<.04, η
p
= .08 (and similarly when a Stimulus Type X EMCP
ANOVA was conducted, collapsing across goal prime condition; F(1, 59) = 5.17, p<.03, η
p
=
.08). This is illustrated in Fig 3, which shows a lowering of the PSE for responses to black male
faces relative to white male faces, but only for the high-EMCP group. When we performed the
Stimulus Type X EMCP ANOVA using raw PSE scores (not difference scores) and controlled
for shape PSE by entering it as a covariate, we found the same pattern, F(1, 58) = 4.42, p<.04,
η
p
= .07. The PSE was lower, as predicted, when the comparison stimulus was a black man’s
face than when it was a white man’s face but this was only true for the high-EMCP group.
To further examine this pattern of means, a series of t-tests were conducted. As predicted,
participants high in EMCP were found to have a lower PSE when responding to faces of black
men (M= 568 ms) than when responding to faces of white men (M= 603 ms), t(21) = 2.14, p<
.05, 95% CI [-68.77, -.98], Cohen’s d= .461, as well as when responding to shapes (M= 660 ms),
t(21) = -4.09, p<.01, 95% CI [-138.61, -.45.18], Cohen’s d= -0.916. PSEs for faces of white men
were also lower than shapes, t(21) = 3.40, p<.01, 95% CI [22.18, 91.86], Cohen’s d= 0.767, yet
not as much as faces of black men. For participants not high in EMCP, no difference was found
in PSE when responding to black versus white faces (M= 599 ms and 591 ms respectively),
black faces versus shapes (M= 599 ms and 614 ms respectively), or white faces versus shapes
(M= 591 ms and 614 ms respectively), p’s >.45. Perhaps the most intuitive way to ask the ques-
tion of time slowing is to examine if there is a PSE decrease when examining responses to black
faces versus white faces. For participants high in EMCP there is a reliably greater drop in PSE
when responding to black faces versus white faces (M= -35 ms) than for participants not high
in EMCP (M= 8 ms), t(59) = 2.27, p<.03, 95% CI [5.10, 79.87], Cohen’s d= 0.595 (with partic-
ipants not high in EMCP experiencing no drop in PSE for black vs. white faces, t(38) = .717,
p= .48, 95% CI [-13.88, 29.09], Cohen’s d= 0.115.
Finally, we performed a regression analysis on PSE scores to further examine race-based
distortion of time perception as a result of EMCP. PSE scores to shapes were entered first
Fig 3. Experiment 1: PSE difference scores for each stimulus type. Data for each participant, for each
stimulus type, were fit with a sigmoid curve and a resulting point of subjective equality (PSE) was calculated
(see Methods). PSE Difference Score = (Object PSE–White Man PSE) OR (Object PSE—Black Man PSE). X
axis represents different subject groups (EMCP score) and Y axis represents PSE difference score.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241.g003
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 9 / 18
(since we are treating these as a form of baseline responding) with PSE scores to Black faces
and White faces entered into the regression next at the same step. EMCP scores were expressed
as a continuous variable. PSE for shapes did not predict EMCP significantly at the first step, t
(59) <1. However, adding PSEs for faces at the second step brought about a significant change
in R
2
by explaining an additional 10% of the variation in EMCP, F(2, 57) = 3.29, p<. 05. At
this second step, a significant relationship emerged between EMCP and PSE for faces of black
men, β= -.36, t(59) = -2.49, p<.02, 95% CI [-.087, -.009]. No relationship was found between
EMCP and PSE for faces of white men or for shapes, ps >.1 As hypothesized, we found that
increases in EMCP reliably predicted a smaller PSE but only when participants were observing
faces of black men. The greater a participant’s EMCP, the more he or she perceived time to
slow when observing faces of black men.
In summary, the results show that for responses to faces of black men, relative to white men
and shapes, short amounts of time are mistaken for longer ones. A black face is experienced as
lingering longer. We refer to this phenomenon as time perception bias (TPB). But this is con-
tingent on motivation. Participants perceive the time a face is on the screen differently for
black versus white faces as a function of their level of external motivations relating to appear-
ing prejudiced. The higher one’s external motivation to respond without prejudice, the shorter
the duration an image of a black man needs to be on the screen for one to say it is 600 ms. The
more concerned one is with controlling the bias, the more likely this bias in time perception is
to emerge.
Experiment 2
Are only specific types of individuals susceptible to this bias? In the first experiment the focus
was on a set of people known from prior research to have arousal relating to appearing preju-
diced. We hypothesized, and found, that for such individuals a bias to time perception could
be triggered merely by seeing a face of an outgroup member (black men for white participants;
and this was hypothesized as being due to the arousal such individuals are known to have
chronically linked to race). But we further hypothesize that it is not only these types of individ-
uals who would show TPB. Even an individual who was not possessing high level of external
motivation to control prejudice can still experience arousal as it relates to race. For these indi-
viduals (the majority of people, white participants who are not one standard deviation above
the mean in their EMCP score) such arousal would not be triggered merely by seeing a face.
But being placed in a situation that was more fraught with racial anxiety could potentially cre-
ate arousal in such white Americans, and thus bias to time perception would appear for any
white person in such a situation. For this reason we chose to focus the second experiment
exclusively on people who are not high in external motivation to control prejudice. Our goal
was to see if the same effect could be induced in people without chronic levels of anxiety as it
relates to race, and instead have the effect induced by the current situation. Of course, people
high in EMCP, already showing distortion to time perception without the need of a situational
inducement, would likely continue to show the bias (perhaps more strongly) under such con-
ditions. But our concern in this experiment is not with such individual differences, but in the
power of the situation to create time distortion. The situation we selected that we presumed
would introduce arousal was an interaction.
Cross race interactions are stressful even among low-prejudiced people [2226]. Such inter-
actions provide an opportunity for one to appear prejudiced to other people [19][43] and to
affirm personal concerns about whether one is truly egalitarian [32][48]. While people with
high levels of EMCP have been shown to have arousal reactions merely at the thought of race,
by the mere presentation of images of outgroup members [15][33][38][44], even people
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 10 / 18
without such chronic tendencies can become worried about bias when placed in interactions
with members of an outgroup. When contextual conditions create a heightened concern with
appearing biased, and these conditions introduce race-based arousal, then any individual in
that context would be expected to illustrate the same distortion to time perception. Interac-
tions among two white people are less likely to trigger such arousal than an interaction among
a white person and a black person, which for many white Americans introduces concerns
about being perceived by others as prejudiced or saying something inappropriate [32].
It is hypothesized that the source of the perceptual bias is the experience of arousal, and in
Experiment two this can arise from being placed in an interaction that is fraught with such con-
cerns (turning away from the chronic concerns of focus in Experiment one). Therefore, in Experi-
ment two, we manipulated whether the participants interacted with a black man or not. When the
interaction is across race we expected that the arousal arising in such interactions (linked to the
stereotype in the United States of black men as threatening) would be triggered [32]. This should
impact how people react to faces of black men on the time perception task in a manner equivalent
to the bias seen in Experiment one. In the control condition participants do not interact with a
black person, but instead interact with a white woman. In the United States women are stereo-
typed as passive [49], and hence this interaction should not be arousing. Such a manipulation
introduces the possibility that any subsequent effects are caused by the gender rather than race of
the experimenter, but this possibility is easy to rule out with our data. If the effect is caused by gen-
der, participants would respond equally with a time perception distortion to faces of men, both
black and white. If caused by race the distortion would only be seen to faces of black men.
In Experiment 2 we sought to replicate the effect of time slowing illustrated in Experiment
1 by using an identical temporal discrimination procedure, but manipulated the threat/arousal
potential of the experimenter. Following the social stereotypes, we had half of the participants
interact with a black man during the experiment and half interacted with a white woman.
Does a black experimenter cause arousal in white participants that will slow their perception of
time to faces of black men even if the white participants are not high in EMCP? Using a tempo-
ral discrimination task, we examine if a cross-race interaction leads people who are not high in
EMCP to perceive time to faces of black men in a manner similar to people who possess high
levels of EMCP.
Material and methods
Procedure. Several weeks before the experiment, as part of registration for a voluntary
participant pool, ongoing levels of EMCP [19] were recorded during an online testing session.
Between 2 to 8 weeks after the registration period participants who were less than one standard
deviation above the mean in EMCP were brought to the lab one at a time and asked to perform
a temporal discrimination task on the computer. The procedure and task was identical to
Experiment 1 with a single exception. Rather than a single, white, female experimenter, partici-
pants were randomly assigned to one of two possible experimenters–a black man or a white
woman. This research involved human subjects and the protocol was approved by Lehigh Uni-
versity’s institutional review board. The work was carried out in accordance with the provi-
sions of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.
Participants. Participants were 77 white students enrolled in the introduction to psychol-
ogy course at Lehigh University, ranging in age from 18–48. Participation was voluntary and
compensated with a credit toward a course requirement. Three of those participants failed to
perform the task appropriately (gave the same response on most of the trials) so their responses
did not fit to the sigmoid curve and their PSE scores could not be calculated. Analyses were
conducted with 74 participants.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 11 / 18
The sample size was determined by a power analysis using the software program “More-
Power” (Version 6.0.1) [46]. The power analysis suggested that for an effect size between small
to medium (see Experiment 1 for the parameters; repeated measures: 1 (3 level), independent
measures: 1 (2 level) we would need between 28 and 60 participants. We decided to request 80
white participants from the participant pool and included as many of them in the experiment
as we could recruit, Our stopping rule was to end data collection when we recruited the 80 par-
ticipants assigned to our experiment, or ran out of participants in the pool eligible for the
experiment (whichever came first).
Assessment of motivation to respond without prejudice. External motivation to
respond without prejudice (EMCP) was assessed as in Experiment 1. Participants with EMCP
scores more than one standard deviation above the mean were not recruited for participation.
Time perception task. Time perception was assessed by a temporal-discrimination task
identical to that used in Experiment 1.
Experimenter manipulation. The attempt to manipulate the temporary triggering of
intergroup arousal was accomplished through the use of two different experimenters whose
group memberships were associated with cultural stereotypes of threat versus passivity. Partici-
pants were greeted and run through the experiment either by a black man or a white woman.
Results
The first evidence of time slowing when participants perceived faces of Black men was pro-
vided by the leftward shift of the S-shaped curve that resulted when duration was plotted on
the x-axis and the percentage of “longer” responses was plotted on the y-axis. As illustrated in
Fig 4A and 4B, the curve representing participants who interacted with a black experimenter
was shifted leftward when comparing their perception of black male faces to their curves for
white male faces. However, participants who interacted with a white woman as the experi-
menter responded to the two types of faces in an almost identical fashion.
The primary measure of the slowing of time was the PSE. Participants interacting with a black
experimenter should perceive the PSE to be earlier to faces of black men than to faces of white
men. No such difference should emerge in participants interacting with a white experimenter.
The PSE was calculated for each participant in a fashion identical to Experiment 1. A PSE differ-
ence score between (a) the PSE for faces of Black men and the PSE for shapes and (b) the PSE for
faces of White men and the PSE for shapes was also calculated as in Experiment 1.
We conducted a Stimulus Type X Experimenter Type ANOVA with PSE difference score as
the dependent variable and Stimulus Type and Experimenter Type as the independent vari-
ables. The predicted interaction between Experimenter Type and Stimulus Type emerged, F(1,
72) = 12.3, p<.01, η
p
= .146. When we performed the analyses using raw PSE scores (not dif-
ference scores) and controlled for shape PSE by entering it as a covariate, we found the same
pattern, F(1, 71) = 11.91, p<.01, η
p
= .144. The PSE was lower, as predicted, when the com-
parison stimulus was a black man’s face than when it was a white man’s face, but only for par-
ticipants who had been placed in a context where they interacted with a black experimenter.
This is illustrated in Fig 5, which shows a lowering of the PSE for responses to black male faces
relative to white male faces, but only for the participants who interacted with the black
experimenter.
To further examine this pattern of means, a series of t-tests were conducted. As predicted,
participants who interacted with the black experimenter were found to have a lower PSE when
responding to faces of black men (M= 607 ms) than when responding to faces of white men
(M= 652 ms), t(36) = -3.46, p<.01, 95% CI [-70.76, -18.41], Cohen’s d= -0.568, as well as
when responding to shapes (M= 685 ms), t(36) = -3.68, p<.01, 95% CI [-120.10, -34.68],
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 12 / 18
Cohen’s d= -0.611. Their PSEs for white men and shapes did not differ significantly, t(36) =
-1.76, p= .09, CI [-70.64, 5.02], Cohen’s d= -0.291. For participants who interacted with the
white experimenter, no difference was found in PSE when responding to black versus white
faces (M= 651 ms and 641 ms respectively, t(36) = 1.16, p= .25, CI [-7.71, 28.51], Cohen’s
d= 0.193) or black faces versus shapes (M= 651 ms and 674 ms respectively, t(36) = -1.81, p=
Fig 4. Experiment 2: Percent of “longer” responses at each stimulus duration for each stimulus type.
Participants experienced either a White experimenter or a Black experimenter (see Methods). (a) Responses
to pictures of Black Men and White Men in participants that participated with a White experimenter. (b) shows
responses in participants that participated with a Black experimenter. The dotted arrow in (b) signifies a shift
of responses to Black Men in the participants that participated with a Black experimenter.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241.g004
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 13 / 18
.08, CI [-49.28, 2.81], Cohen’s d= -0.299). Those participants’ PSEs for white faces were lower
than shapes, t(36) = -2.31, p= .03, CI [-63.13, -4.14], Cohen’s d= -0.380. Perhaps the most
intuitive way to ask the question of time slowing is to examine if there is a PSE decrease when
examining responses to black faces versus white faces. For participants who interacted with the
black experimenter there is a reliably greater drop in PSE when responding to black faces ver-
sus white faces (M= -45 ms) than for participants who interacted with the white experimenter
(M= 10 ms), t(72) = 3.50, p<.01, CI [23.70, 86.27], Cohen’s d= 0.828.
Conclusions
Research in social cognition provides important illustrations of implicit bias and the mecha-
nisms through which such bias is manifested. But it is rarely concerned with bias to perceptual
experience. How should race impact time perception? Like seeing and hearing, perceiving
time is experienced by the individual as an objective reality since it is given no conscious atten-
tion–it proceeds outside awareness and governs our movement and decisions seamlessly. Yet,
like other perceptual activity [311], time perception is open to influence.
We predicted that to white people in the U.S. time would slow when perceiving black men
if they had arousal associated with black men. The experiments revealed this distortion in the
perception of time for our white participants when perceiving images of black men when
manipulating arousal in two distinct ways. In experiment one, the time perception bias (TPB)
emerged among participants with strong external motivations relating to controlling prejudice
and its expression. In experiment 2 the bias emerged when white participants were performing
the time perception task under the supervision of a black experimenter. Across both experi-
ments, concern with controlling bias and with being seen as biased was associated with the
predicted distortion (slowing) in time perception.
Experiment one also explored if a temporary goal–to be egalitarian—might ameliorate the
arousal people high in EMCP typically experience. There was no evidence to support this pos-
sibility. Regardless of their temporary goals, people high in EMCP showed the time slowing
Fig 5. Experiment 2: PSE difference scores for each stimulus type. Data for each participant, for each
stimulus type, were fit with a sigmoid curve and a resulting point of subjective equality (PSE) was calculated
(see Methods). PSE Difference Score = (Object PSE–White Man PSE) OR (Object PSE—Black Man PSE). X
axis represents different subject groups (experimenter group) and Y axis represents PSE difference score.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241.g005
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 14 / 18
effect. Of course, it might be that the subtle manipulation of goals that we employed was not
sufficient to overcome the more powerful arousal. Or it might simply be that providing an
internally motivated goal does not remove arousal, but merely provides another goal that coin-
cides with the external goal and its associated arousal. These issues should be explored in fur-
ther research.
The basic phenomenon of interest–time slowing—was produced in two experiments.
Those who were chronically concerned about appearing prejudiced as well as those who were
supervised by a Black experimenter experienced time slowing in processing Black faces. Future
research should also examine arousal as the mechanism of distortion in time perception dur-
ing intergroup interactions more directly via physiological measures.
The impact of a time overestimation bias on human responding that requires timing as an
essential element for action is another fruitful area for future research. These small scale per-
ceptual differences could impact interaction, contributing to disparities that emerge in health
care, criminal justice, and awkwardness demonstrated in everyday social interaction. Mosko-
witz, Olcaysoy Okten, Gooch, Moore-Berg, and Karpinski [50] have illustrated that how par-
ticipants perform in a “shooter” task, where they must fire a simulated weapon at black versus
white men, is impacted by their level of TPB as assessed by the discrimination task described
above. Kenrick, Sinclair, Richeson, Verosky, and Lun [51] found that the speed with which a
black man versus a white man appeared to be moving toward a white participant depended
on arousal, speculating that time perception mediated the judgments of motion. It is already
known that doctors terminate intake interviews with patients differentially as a function of the
patient’s race, and there is widespread concern in the United States that police officers initiate
force differentially as a function of race. Does bias in the perception of the duration of these
interactions or the speed of movement contribute to these disparities? This is another fruitful
topic of exploration.
Finally, TPB should not be limited to white participants. An interaction among a white per-
son and a black person is known to be arousing for the black individuals in the encounter as
well [32]. For many black Americans such interactions introduce concerns about being the tar-
get of bias, or being treated in a stereotypical way. It also introduces stereotype threat, a form
of arousal that emerges from fearing one may confirm the group stereotype through one’s
actions in the encounter [52]. Thus, black participants, indeed any individuals who may expe-
rience arousal in an interaction, should experience bias to time perception.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Gordon B. Moskowitz, Cynthia M. Gooch.
Data curation: Cynthia M. Gooch.
Formal analysis: Gordon B. Moskowitz, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten.
Investigation: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Methodology: Gordon B. Moskowitz, Cynthia M. Gooch.
Project administration: Gordon B. Moskowitz, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten.
Resources: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Software: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Supervision: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Validation: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 15 / 18
Writing original draft: Gordon B. Moskowitz.
Writing review & editing: Gordon B. Moskowitz, Irmak Olcaysoy Okten.
References
1. Banaji MR, & Greenwald AG. Blindspot: Hidden Biases of Good People. 1st ed. New York: Delacorte
Press; 2013.
2. Sherman JW, Gawronski B, & Trope Y. Dual-process theories of the social mind. New York: The Guil-
ford Press; 2014.
3. Bruner JS. On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review. 1957; 64(2): 123–152. PMID: 13420288
4. Campbell DT. Distinguishing differences of perception from failures of communication in cross-cultural
studies. In Northrop FSC, Livingston HH, editors. Cross-cultural understanding: Epistemology in anthro-
pology. New York: Harper & Row; 1964. pp. 308–336.
5. Campbell DT. Herskovits, cultural relativism, and metascience. In Herskovits MJ, editor. Cultural relativ-
ism. New York: Random House; 1972. pp. v–xxiii.
6. Eberhardt JL, Dasgupta N, & Banaszynski TL. Believing is seeing: The effects of racial labels and
implicit beliefs on face perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003; 29(3): 360–370.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202250215 PMID: 15273013
7. Freeman JB, Pauker K, Sanchez DT. A Perceptual Pathway to Bias: Interracial Exposure Reduces
Abrupt Shifts in Real-Time Race Perception That Predict Mixed-Race Bias. Psychological Science.
2016; 27(4): 502–517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615627418 PMID: 26976082
8. Kitayama S, Duffy S, Kawamura T, & Larsen JT. Perceiving an object and its context in different cul-
tures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science. 2003; 14: 201–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/
1467-9280.02432 PMID: 12741741
9. Levin DT, & Banaji MR. Distortions in the perceived lightness of faces: The role of race categories. Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2006; 135: 501–512.
10. Ratner KG, Dotsch R, Wigboldus DHJ, van Knippenberg A, & Amodio DM. Visualizing minimal ingroup
and outgroup faces: Implications for impressions, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology. 2014; 106: 897–911. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036498 PMID: 24841095
11. Riccio M, Cole S, & Balcetis E. Seeing the expected, the desired, and the feared: influences on percep-
tual interpretation and directed attention.Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2013; 7(6):
401–414.
12. Gil S, & Droit-Volet S. “Time flies in the presence of angry faces”. . . depending on the temporal task
used!.Acta Psychologica, 2011; 136: 354–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.010 PMID:
21276583
13. Lee KH, Seelam K, & O’Brien T. The relativity of time perception produced by facial emotion stimuli.
Cognition & Emotion. 2011; 25: 1471–1480.
14. Tipples J. Time flies when we read taboo words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2010; 17(4): 563–
568.
15. Moskowitz GB, Olcaysoy Okten I, & Gooch CM. On race and time. Psychological Science. 2015; 26
(11): 1783–1794. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615599547 PMID: 26423460
16. Church RM. Properties of the internal clock. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1984; 423
(1): 566–582.
17. Maricq AV, Roberts S, & Church RM. Methamphetamine and time estimation. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1981; 7(1): 18–30. PMID: 7229573
18. Meck WH. Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1983; 9(2): 171–201. PMID: 6842136
19. Plant EA, & Devine PG. Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology. 1998; 75(3); 811–832.
20. Bar-Haim Y, Kerem A, Lamy D, & Zakay D. When time slows down: the influence of threat on time per-
ception in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion. 2010; 24(2): 255–263.
21. Fortin C, Rousseau R, Bourque P, & Kirouac E. Time estimation and concurrent nontemporal process-
ing: Specific interference from short-term-memory demands. Perception & Psychophysics. 1993; 53
(5): 536–548.
22. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Hunter SB., Lickel B, & Kowai-Bell N. Perceiver threat in social interactions
with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001; 80(2): 253–267. PMID:
11220444
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 16 / 18
23. Kawakami K, Dunn E, Karmali F, & Dovidio JF. Mispredicting affective and behavioral responses to rac-
ism. Science. 2009; 323(5911): 276–278. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1164951 PMID: 19131633
24. Stephan WG, & Stephan CW. Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues. 1985; 41(3): 157–175.
25. Vorauer JD, & Kumhyr SM. Is this about you or me? Self-versus other-directed judgments and feelings
in response to intergroup interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2001; 27(6): 706–
719.
26. Mendes WB, Blascovich J, Hunter SB, Lickel B, & Jost JT. Threatened by the unexpected: physiological
responses during social interactions with expectancy-violating partners. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology. 2007; 92(4): 698–716. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.698 PMID:
17469953
27. Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB, & Jetten J. Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the
rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994; 67(5): 808–817.
28. Word CO, Zanna MP, & Cooper J. The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial
interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1974; 10(2): 109–120.
29. Neuberg SL. The goal of forming accurate impressions during social interactions: attenuating the impact
of negative expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989; 56(3): 374–386. PMID:
2926635
30. Peck BM, & Denney M. Disparities in the conduct of the medical encounter: The effects of physician
and patient race and gender. SAGE Open. 2012; 2(3): 1–14.
31. Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, & Cunningham WA. Modulation of the fusiform face area following minimal
exposure to motivationally relevant faces: evidence of in-group enhancement (not out-group disregard).
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011; 23(11): 3343–3354. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00016
PMID: 21452952
32. Richeson JA, & Shelton JN. Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities.
Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007; 16: 316–320.
33. Richeson JA, & Trawalter S. The threat of appearing prejudiced and race-based attentional biases. Psy-
chological Science. 2008; 19(2): 98–102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02052.x PMID:
18271854
34. Shelton JN, Richeson JA, & Salvatore J. Expecting to be the target of prejudice: Implications for inter-
ethnic interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005; 31(9): 1189–1202. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0146167205274894 PMID: 16055639
35. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Carson KA, Beach MC, Sabin JA, Greenwald AG, et al. The associations of clini-
cians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal
care. American Journal of Public Health. 2012; 102(5): 979–987. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011.
300558 PMID: 22420787
36. Hebl MR, Foster JB, Mannix LM, & Dovidio JF. Formal and interpersonal discrimination: A field study of
bias toward homosexual applicants. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2002; 28: 815–825.
37. Pearson AR, Dovidio JF, & Gaertner SL. The nature of contemporary prejudice: Insights from aversive
racism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2009; 3(3): 314–338.
38. Amodio DM, Harmon-Jones E, & Devine PG. Individual differences in the activation and control of affec-
tive race bias as assessed by startle eyeblink responses and self-report. Journal of Personalityand
Social Psychology. 2003; 84(4): 738–753. PMID: 12703646
39. Moskowitz GB, Gollwitzer PM, Wasel W, & Schaal B. Preconscious control of stereotype activation
through chronic egalitarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999; 77(1): 167–184.
40. Moskowitz GB, & Li P. Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype con-
trol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2011; 47(1): 103–116.
41. Moskowitz GB. Preconscious effects of temporary goals on attention. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology. 2002; 38: 397–404.
42. Moskowitz GB, Li P, Ignarri C, & Stone J. Compensatory cognition associated with egalitarian goals.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2011; 47(2): 365–370.
43. Plant EA, & Devine PG. The antecedents and implications of interracial anxiety. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin. 2003; 29(6): 790–801. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029006011 PMID:
15189634
44. Bean MG, Slaten DG, Horton WS, Murphy MC, Todd AR, & Richeson JA. Prejudice concerns and race-
based attentional bias: New evidence from eyetracking. Social Psychological and Personality Science.
2012; 3(6): 722–729.
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 17 / 18
45. Chartrand TL, & Bargh JA. Automatic activation of impression formation and memorization goals: Non-
conscious goal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. 1996; 71(3): 464–478.
46. Campbell JI, & Thompson VA. MorePower 6.0 for ANOVA with relational confidence intervals and
Bayesian analysis. Behavior Research Methods. 2012; 44(4): 1255–1265. https://doi.org/10.3758/
s13428-012-0186-0 PMID: 22437511
47. Minear M, & Park DC. A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instru-
ments, & Computers. 2004; 36(4): 630–633.
48. Monin B, & Miller DT. Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology. 2001; 81(1): 33–43. PMID: 11474723
49. Dovidio JF, & Fiske ST. Under the radar: How unexamined biases in decision-making processes in clini-
cal interactions can contribute to health care disparities. American Journal of Public Health. 2012; 102
(5): 945–952. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011.300601 PMID: 22420809
50. Moskowitz GB, Olcaysoy Okten I, Gooch CM, & Moore-Berg S, & Karpinski A (2017). Time perception
bias and decisions to shoot. Manuscript under review.
51. Kenrick A, Sinclair S, Richeson JA, Versoksy S, & Lun J. Moving while Black: Intergroup attitudes influ-
ence judgments of speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology (General). 2016; 145: 147–154.
52. Steele CM, & Aronson J. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995; 69: 797–811. PMID: 7473032
Race and time perception
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182241 August 8, 2017 18 / 18
... Indeed, this finding applies specifically to White participants concerned with bias. In brief, the study revealed a racially implicit bias exerted at the perceptual level (see also Moskowitz et al., 2017). ...
... This study shows that judgments about time can be a subtle tool for interrogating internal biases. It has proven to be powerful for detecting implicit prejudices toward others (Moskowitz, 2015(Moskowitz, , 2017, belonging to a (religious) group (Tewari et al., 2020), or even for studying attitudes toward food (Gagnon et al., 2018;Gil et al., 2009). The tool now extends its usefulness to the study of political allegiances. ...
Article
Full-text available
The main objective of this study was to determine if the estimation of time could be used to reveal an implicit political bias. The study also aimed at determining if a political bias is related to a specific temporal perspective or to other personality factors. The main demonstration is based on a bisection temporal task where the participants were asked to say if the duration of the presentation of a politician’s photo is short or long. There were three independent variables of interest: the location of politicians on the left (liberal) or right (conservative) on the political axis, the emotions expressed on a politician’s photo (joy, anger or neutral), and the political allegiances of the participants. Overall, compared to conditions with neutral faces or faces expressing joy, participants overestimated the duration of faces expressing anger. This effect, however, depends on the political allegiance of the participants. Compared to the neutral face condition, liberal participants overestimated the length of presentation of politicians’ faces in the joy and anger conditions. The results also showed that, compared to the condition in which photos of conservative politicians are presented, conservative participants underestimated the presentation duration of liberal politicians’ photos; such an influence of the orientation of presented politicians was not observed with liberal participants. The results also reveal that conservative participants differed from liberal participants on the future-oriented scale and on the past-positive-oriented scale of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI). The study shows that time perception can be used to reveal a kind of implicit political bias.
... Previous investigations have found that time expectation may distort individuals' duration perception. Individuals overestimate time when they want it to speed up, such as when they are presented with images they do not like, are required to say words they do not want to say or are required to hide something (Jia et al., 2021;Matsuda et al., 2020;Moskowitz et al., 2017). In contrast, individuals underestimate time when they want it to stand still, for instance, when presented with their preferred stimuli (Conti, 2001;Danckert & Allman, 2005;Gable & Poole, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
People often express feeling that time passes quickly or slowly in their daily lives, which is termed passage of time judgment (PoTJ). Past studies have shown that PoTJ is affected by emotional valence and arousal; however, few studies have verified the effects of alertness, attention to time, and time expectation on PoTJ and whether the effects are stable over different time periods. Using the experience sampling method (ESM) and diary method, the present study collected data from 105 participants and examined for the first time whether alertness, attention to time, and time expectation affect PoTJ based on daily life data, as well as whether above factors, emotional valence, and arousal are stable over different time periods. All participants answered a questionnaire five times a day on their in-the-day PoTJ and related factors regarding the last 30 min, and answered the same questionnaire once a day at 23:00 regarding the of-the-day PoTJ. The results showed that alertness and time expectation, as well as emotional valence and arousal, predicted an individual’s in-the-day PoTJ over a shorter period (i.e., the last 30 min); in contrast, only time expectation and emotional arousal predicted of-the-day PoTJ over a longer period (i.e., the past day). These results suggest that, alertness and time expectation are important factors influencing PoTJ, in addition to emotional state. Of-the-day PoTJ correlates most strongly with the mean and latest in-the-day PoTJ, implying that overall perception of time passage is influenced by both cumulative temporal experience and recent temporal experience.
... Thus, PM differences between individuals from the Caribbean and those non-Caribbean individuals born in the U.S. seem to center around time-based tasks. This is intriguing because it may relate to theories of differences in time perception based on culture (e.g., Sinha & Gardenfors 2014) or social setting (Moskowitz 2017). ...
Chapter
"... In the current chapter, we focus our attention on the relationship between culture and 1) working memory (an aspect of short-term memory), 2) prospective memory (utilizing both working memory and long-term memory) and 3) autobiographical memory ( an aspect of long term, episodic memory). In each section below, we first describe what the component of memory is and how it is frequently assessed in the literature, and then describe research on how culture might mitigate each type of memory. We also [...] focus our attention to the work on culture and memory being done in the Caribbean, to move away from the predominantly East-West comparison, and particularly focus on research coming from Trinidad and Tobago."
Article
Racial disparities in clinical recommendations can result in racial disparities in health. While healthcare providers’ implicit racial attitudes (affective component of bias) are theorized to be one major factor contributing to racial disparities in clinical recommendations, empirical evidence to support the link is lacking. This study aimed to bridge this gap by moving beyond the standard approach of operationalizing the quality of clinical recommendations as a guideline-consistent vs. -inconsistent dichotomy. The present secondary study examined the role of provider implicit racial attitudes in the quality of clinical recommendations, operationalized as behaviors reflecting providers’ psychological investment in patient care (i.e., number of words used to describe clinical recommendations, and number of treatment options recommended). Two-hundred-and-ten White medical trainees reviewed a clinical vignette of either a White or Black male patient and provided clinical recommendations. Their implicit racial attitudes were evaluated using the Implicit Association Test. Participants with more biased implicit racial attitudes (i.e., stronger implicit preference for White vs. Black individuals) used fewer words to describe their clinical recommendations and provided fewer clinical recommendations for the Black (vs. White) patient, while there were no significant differences between Black and White patients among participants with less biased implicit racial attitudes. These results illustrate the insidious impact of implicit racial attitudes in healthcare provision and underscore the need for researchers to consider the complex, nuanced ways in which provider implicit racial attitudes might manifest in clinical decision-making.
Article
Full-text available
Time is fundamental to organizing all aspects of human life. When invested in relationships, it has a psychological meaning as it indicates how much individuals value others and their interest in maintaining social relationships. Previous research has identified an intergroup time bias (ITB) in racialized social relations, defined as a discriminatory behavior in which White individuals invest more time in evaluating White than Black individuals. This research proposes an aversive racism explanation for the ITB effect and examines its consequences in the medical context. In four experimental studies (N = 434), we found that White medical trainees invested more time in forming impressions of White (vs. Black) male patients. Study 5 (N = 193) further revealed more time investment in diagnosing, assessing pain, and prescribing opioids for White than Black male patients. This biased time effect mediated the impact of patients’ skin color on health care outcomes, leading to greater diagnostic accuracy and pain perception, and lower opioid prescriptions. A meta-analytical integration of the results (Study 6) confirmed the ITB effect reliability across experiments and that it is stronger in participants with an aversive racist profile (vs. consistently prejudiced or nonprejudiced). These findings provide the first evidence that bias in time investment favoring White (vs. Black) patients is associated with aversive racism and impacts medical health care outcomes. Furthermore, these results offer insights into the sociopsychological meaning of time investment in health care and provide a theoretical explanation for an understudied insidious form of discrimination that is critical to comprehending the persistency of racial health care disparities.
Article
Full-text available
This research shows stereotype activation is controlled by chronic egalitarian goals. In the first 2 studies it was found that the stereotype of women is equally available to individuals with and without chronic goals, and the discriminant validity of the concept of egalitarian goals was established. In the next 2 experiments, differences in stereotype activation as a function of this individual difference were found. In Study 3, participants read attributes following stereotypical primes. Facilitated response times to stereotypical attributes were found for nonchronics but not for chronics. This lack of facilitation occurred at stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) where effortful correction processes could not operate, demonstrating preconscious control of stereotype activation due to chronic goals. In Study 4, inhibition of the stereotype was found at an SOA where effortful processes of stereotype suppression could not operate. The data reveal that goals are activated and used preconsciously to prevent stereotype activation, demonstrating both the controllability of stereotype activation and the implicit role of goals in cognitive control.
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments supported the hypothesis that people are more willing to express attitudes that could be viewed as prejudiced when their past behavior has established their credentials as nonprejudiced persons. In Study 1, participants given the opportunity to disagree with blatantly sexist statements were later more willing to favor a man for a stereotypically male job. In Study 2, participants who first had the opportunity to select a member of a stereotyped group (a woman or an African American) for a category-neutral job were more likely to reject a member of that group for a job stereotypically suited for majority members. In Study 3, participants who had established credentials as nonprejudiced persons revealed a greater willingness to express a politically incorrect opinion even when the audience was unaware of their credentials. The general conditions under which people feel licensed to act on illicit motives are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In two national samples, we examined the influence of interracial exposure in one’s local environment on the dynamic process underlying race perception and its evaluative consequences. Using a mouse-tracking paradigm, we found in Study 1 that White individuals with low interracial exposure exhibited a unique effect of abrupt, unstable White-Black category shifting during real-time perception of mixed-race faces, consistent with predictions from a neural-dynamic model of social categorization and computational simulations. In Study 2, this shifting effect was replicated and shown to predict a trust bias against mixed-race individuals and to mediate the effect of low interracial exposure on that trust bias. Taken together, the findings demonstrate that interracial exposure shapes the dynamics through which racial categories activate and resolve during real-time perceptions, and these initial perceptual dynamics, in turn, may help drive evaluative biases against mixed-race individuals. Thus, lower-level perceptual aspects of encounters with racial ambiguity may serve as a foundation for mixed-race prejudice.
Article
Full-text available
Four experiments examined whether intergroup attitudes shape the speed with which Blacks are thought to be moving. When participants rated the speed of Black and White faces that appeared to be moving toward them, greater intergroup anxiety was associated with judging Black targets as moving more slowly relative to White targets (Experiments 1a and 1b). Experiment 2 demonstrated that this effect occurs only for approaching targets. Experiment 3 showed that this slowing bias occurs, at least in part, because of the perceived duration of time each image was moving. Such a slowing bias is consistent with the time expansion and perceptual slowing reported by people who experienced threatening events.
Article
Full-text available
Arousal is known to shape time perception, and heightened arousal causes one to perceive that time has slowed (i.e., a given length of time feels longer than it actually is). The current experiments illustrate that among White people who experience arousal when contemplating race (specifically those for whom appearing biased is an ongoing concern), time perception slows when they observe faces of Black men. We asked participants to judge the duration of presentation for faces of White and Black men (shown for periods ranging from 300 to 1,200 ms) relative to a standard duration of 600 ms. Evidence of bias emerged when White participants concerned with bias saw faces of Black men (e.g., durations of less than 600 ms were perceived as being greater than 600 ms). The current findings have implications for intergroup interactions in which timing is essential-for example, length of job interviews, police officers' perception of the length of an encounter and when force should be initiated, and doctors' perception of the length of medical encounters. Racially biased time perception is a new form of implicit bias, one exerted at the perceptual level.
Article
Full-text available
More than 40 years of research have shown that people favor members of their ingroup in their impressions, attitudes, and behaviors. Here, we propose that people also form different mental images of minimal ingroup and outgroup members, and we test the hypothesis that differences in these mental images contribute to the well-established biases that arise from minimal group categorization. In Study 1, participants were assigned to 1 of 2 groups using a classic minimal group paradigm. Next, a reverse correlation image classification procedure was used to create visual renderings of ingroup and outgroup face representations. Subsequently, a 2nd sample naive to the face generation stage rated these faces on a series of trait dimensions. The results indicated that the ingroup face was significantly more likely than the outgroup face to elicit favorable impressions (e.g., trusting, caring, intelligent, attractive). Extending this finding, Study 2 revealed that ingroup face representations elicited more favorable implicitly measured attitudes than did outgroup representations, and Study 3 showed that ingroup faces were trusted more than outgroup faces during an economic game. Finally, Study 4 demonstrated that facial physiognomy associated with trustworthiness more closely resembled the facial structure of the average ingroup than outgroup face representation. Together, these studies suggest that minimal group distinctions can elicit different mental representations, and that this visual bias is sufficient to elicit ingroup favoritism in impressions, attitudes and behaviors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Four experiments studied the scaling of time by rats. The purpose was to determine if internal clock and memory processes could be selectively adjusted by pharmacological manipulations. All of the experiments used a temporal discrimination procedure in which one response ("short") was reinforced following a 2-sec noise signal and a different response ("long") was reinforced following an 8-sec noise signal; unreinforced signals of intermediate duration were also presented. The proportion of "long" responses increased as a function of signal duration. All drugs were administered intraperitoneally (ip) and their effect on clock or memory processes was inferred from the observed pattern of change in the point of subjective equality of the psychophysical functions under training and testing conditions. Experiment 1 demonstrated that methamphetamine (1.5 mg/kg) can selectively increase clock speed and that haloperidol (.12 mg/kg) can selectively decrease clock speed. Experiment 2 demonstrated that footshock stress (.2 mA) can selectively increase clock speed during continuous administration but leads to a decrease in clock speed below control values when the footshock is abruptly terminated. Experiment 3 demonstrated that vasopressin (.07 pressor units/kg) and oxytocin (.02 pressor units/kg) can selectively decrease the remembered durations of reinforced times, which suggests that memory storage speed increased. Experiment 4 demonstrated that physostigmine (.01 mg/kg) can selectively decrease the remembered durations of reinforced times and that atropine (.05 mg/kg) can selectively increase these remembered durations, which suggests that memory storage speed was differentially affected. The conclusion is that internal clock and memory processes can be dissociated by selectively adjusting their speed of operation and that these changes can be quantitatively modeled by a scalar timing theory.
Article
journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier's archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit: Two experiments examine nonconscious processes that facilitate pursuing egalitarian goals. It was hypothesized that when working on a task not known to be relevant to egalitarian goals there is heightened ability to detect opportunities to goal pursuit (goal-relevant people) embedded in the task, even when they are best ignored for optimal performance. Further, this selective attention should cease when the goal is sated, despite increased semantic accessibility of these opportunities that results from satiation. Experiment one introduced egalitarian goals via writing an essay about failing to be egalitarian to Black men. Next, an ostensibly unrelated task presented Black and White men in an array of faces as distracters to a focal task. Task performance was disrupted only by arrays containing Black men, and only among participants primed with egalitarian goals. This was not due to increased semantic accessibility of the concept "Black men." Experiment two had all participants write failure essays and then write second essays. Half wrote affirming essays about egalitarianism and Black men. Despite this increased semantic accessibility of the group "Black men," distracted attention was not evidenced. Instead, the goal had been satisfied and goal pursuit shut down. In contrast, the remaining participants wrote affirming essays in an irrelevant domain. Despite the decreased semantic accessibility, goal accessibility remained and was evidenced by selective attention to Black men. These findings reveal Black men are associated not with stereotypes, but egalitarian goals. They also point to the role goal completion versus self affirmation play in goal pursuit.
Article
Though people often believe their visual experiences reflect the objective state of the surrounding world, a wealth of recent evidence suggests that perceptions are systematically biased. We draw from contemporary research and supplement with real world anecdotes to suggest that two aspects of perception are subject to influence by states internal to perceivers themselves. We focus on the biases that arise from two difficult tasks faced by the visual system. First, visual information is often unclear and ambiguous; yet visual input must be categorized quickly and efficiently. Second, people can focus on only a subset of perceptual information at once, and as a result visual attention is necessarily directed at a subset of the surrounding world. We document ways in which expectations, desires, and fears help to resolve perceptual ambiguity and direct attention. Finally, we discuss the functional nature of visual biases and note potential consequences in important domains.