ArticlePDF Available

Implicit Age Cues in Resumes: Subtle Effects on Hiring Discrimination

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Anonymous resume screening, as assumed, does not dissuade age discriminatory effects. Building on job market signaling theory, this study investigated whether older applicants may benefit from concealing explicitly mentioned age signals on their resumes (date of birth) or whether more implicit/subtle age cues on resumes (older-sounding names/old-fashioned extracurricular activities) may lower older applicants’ hirability ratings. An experimental study among 610 HR professionals using a mixed factorial design showed hiring discrimination of older applicants based on implicit age cues in resumes. This effect was more pronounced for older raters. Concealing one’s date of birth led to overall lower ratings. Study findings add to the limited knowledge on the effects of implicit age cues on hiring discrimination in resume screening and the usefulness of anonymous resume screening in the context of age. Implications for research and practice are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 10 August 2017
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01321
Edited by:
Cort W. Rudolph,
Saint Louis University, United States
Reviewed by:
Justin Marcus,
Özye ˘
gin University, Turkey
Joseph M. Goodman,
Illinois State University, United States
*Correspondence:
Eva Derous
eva.derous@ugent.be
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Organizational Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 29 March 2017
Accepted: 18 July 2017
Published: 10 August 2017
Citation:
Derous E and Decoster J (2017)
Implicit Age Cues in Resumes: Subtle
Effects on Hiring Discrimination.
Front. Psychol. 8:1321.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01321
Implicit Age Cues in Resumes: Subtle
Effects on Hiring Discrimination
Eva Derous1*and Jeroen Decoster2
1Department of Personnel Management, Work, and Organizational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational
Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium, 2Thomas More University College, Antwerp, Belgium
Anonymous resume screening, as assumed, does not dissuade age discriminatory
effects. Building on job market signaling theory, this study investigated whether older
applicants may benefit from concealing explicitly mentioned age signals on their resumes
(date of birth) or whether more implicit/subtle age cues on resumes (older-sounding
names/old-fashioned extracurricular activities) may lower older applicants’ hirability
ratings. An experimental study among 610 HR professionals using a mixed factorial
design showed hiring discrimination of older applicants based on implicit age cues
in resumes. This effect was more pronounced for older raters. Concealing one’s date
of birth led to overall lower ratings. Study findings add to the limited knowledge on
the effects of implicit age cues on hiring discrimination in resume screening and the
usefulness of anonymous resume screening in the context of age. Implications for
research and practice are discussed.
Keywords: age, anonymous resume screening, hiring discrimination, job market signaling theory, recruitment
INTRODUCTION
In Western society people need to work long enough to maintain welfare levels (Administration
on Aging, 2015). Many people also prefer to stay active in the labor market until an older age
(Wöhrmann et al., 2016). However, and despite anti-discrimination legislation, chronologically
older compared to younger job applicants still have lower chances to hold and obtain jobs, even
when their competencies are alike (Neumark et al., 2016;Wanberg et al., 2016).
The present paper focuses on age discrimination in hiring, and more in particular on resume
screening, (Bal et al., 2011;Truxillo et al., 2015). Worldwide, resumes are one of the most
frequently used screening tools that encompass the first selection hurdle. Moreover, due to
the way impressions are formed, this hurdle seems vulnerable for hiring discrimination (Fiske
et al., 2002). Although chronological age has no validity for predicting future job performance
(Schmidt et al., 2016), correspondence audit studies1consistently show that explicitly presenting
one’s chronological age in a resume may decline older applicants’ job chances (Riach and Rich,
2006, 2010;Richardson et al., 2013;Neumark et al., 2016). Moreover, such ageism effects seem
substantial. Ahmed et al. (2012), for instance, found that younger (31 years) compared to equally
Abbreviation: AAP, anonymous application procedures.
1The correspondence audit technique is an experimental research technique that allows to compare labor market outcomes
of applicants who are equally qualified for a job (i.e., identical in all productive characteristics) but who differ in non-
job related or non-productive characteristics like -most typically- demographic characteristics (e.g., gender, ethnicity, and
age). In correspondence audit studies matched pairs of equivalent resumes are sent to the same employer and callback is
registered as the outcome variable in order to investigate whether differential treatment of applicants can be attributed to
hiring discrimination.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 2
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
qualified older (46 years) applicants received over three times
more responses from employers looking to hire restaurant
workers and over four times more responses from employers
looking for sales assistants.
Anti-discrimination regulations have not prevented bias
in resume screening; therefore AAP are offered to combat
illegal discriminatory hiring practices (Åslund and Skans, 2012).
AAP like blind auditions (Goldin and Rouse, 2000), blind
interviewing (Buijsrogge et al., 2016), and anonymous resume
screening (Åslund and Skans, 2012), aim to blot non-job-related,
personal identifiers (like socio-demographic information) to
increase protected job applicants’ chances of advancing to the
next assessment stage, and hence, to increase their hiring
chances. Intriguingly, however, results of AAP are mixed.
Anonymous resume screening also dilutes hiring chances of job
applicants from protected social groups (Krause et al., 2012;
Behaghel et al., 2015), which suggests that resumes in which
demographic information is blotted still reveal information about
job applicants’ group membership, albeit in subtle ways. To
the best of our knowledge, the role of implicit markers of
applicants’ social group membership on their hiring chances
has not been investigated much in recruitment (i.e., resume
screening), especially not regarding applicants’ older age, and will
be considered here.
According to the signaling theory (like the job market
signaling theory; see Spence, 1973, 1974;Rynes, 1991) senders
(like applicants) exchange information with receivers (like
recruiters) through signals/cues (like resume information),
which correlate with unobservable characteristics of the sender
(Connelly et al., 2011;Bangerter et al., 2012). Hence building
on assumptions from both job market signaling theory and
impression formation theory, we first investigate whether older
job applicants benefit from concealing explicitly mentioned
age cues in their resumes (like date of birth) or whether
more implicit/subtle age cues in resumes – i.e., other than
one’s date of birth- may lower older job applicants’ hirability
ratings. Second, surprisingly and with a few exceptions (see
Fasbender and Wang, 2017), few studies investigated recruiter
characteristics on hiring discrimination, and ageism in particular.
Yet, recruiters might differ in their susceptibility to hiring bias
and recruiters’ chronological age has been suggested as one
potential boundary condition. As a second aim, we therefore
explore the potential moderating role of recruiters’ chronological
age on implicit/subtle age-related information in resumes on
the one hand and applicants’ hirability ratings on the other
hand. Some studies showed evidence for in-group favoritism with
chronologically older recruiters favoring older applicants (Krings
et al., 2011), whereas others have shown the opposite (Finkelstein
and Burke, 1998;Axt et al., 2014). Since hiring literature is
inconclusive and mainly considered explicit age cues, we further
explore whether older recruiters may (dis)favor resumes based
on implicit old-age cues (Axt et al., 2014;Marcus and Sabuncu,
2016).
In the next paragraphs, we first discuss why resume screening
is vulnerable to age discrimination, what is known about ageism
effects in this stage of the hiring procedure, and why anonymous
resume screening may fail to avert age discrimination. Next,
we turn our attention to job market signaling theory and
discuss ‘implicit’ age cues in resumes (i.e., applicants’ name and
affiliations). Finally, we explore the potential moderating role
of recruiters’ chronological age on implicit age cues and hiring
discrimination.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
Age Discrimination in Resume Screening
Resumes are one of the first and most important sources of
information when HR managers and recruiters initially screen
applicants for jobs, but they also appear very vulnerable to
bias (Derous et al., 2015). Job applicants are typically judged
on the basis of a one- or two-page resume. This resume
screening provides limited individuating information and is
vulnerable to categorization effects. That is, cognitive models of
impression formation (like the continuum model; see Fiske et al.,
1999) suggest that category-based information processing will
be particularly strong when limited individualized information
is available, such as on resumes (Abrams et al., 2016). Models
of impression formation further suggest that categorization
will occur automatically and once people have categorized
someone as belonging to a particular out-group, associated group
stereotypes may be activated, which can influence how people
judge others (Fiske et al., 1999).
In many Western European societies, it is common to indicate
date of birth on resumes, which explicitly signals applicants’
chronological age. Such an explicit signal might provide
recruiters with information about one’s life/work experiences
but at the same time might come with a cost and lower older
applicants’ hiring chances. When explicit age markers are present
in resumes, recruiters seem to prefer younger applicants over
older ones as evidenced by many recently conducted (field)
experiments (Lahey, 2008;Albert et al., 2011;Krings et al., 2011;
Ahmed et al., 2012;Richardson et al., 2013). For instance, Lahey
(2008) showed in a field experiment (i.e., correspondence audit
study) that chronologically older women received fewer positive
reactions to their applications than comparable younger women.
In another correspondence audit study, Albert et al. (2011)
sent out resumes of equally qualified 24-, 28-, and 38-years old
applicants in response to existing job ads. Resumes from 38-
years old applicants received a significant lower callback than
those from the former two age groups. Similarly, Ahmed et al.
(2012) found 46-years old applicants to get lower callback than
applicants aged 31, whereas Richardson et al. (2013) showed
resumes of applicants aged 54 to less likely be hired than those
of equally qualified applicants aged 42 or 48.
These correspondence audit studies show age discrimination
to be one of the reasons why older workers have a higher
chance of dropping out of the labor force (Neumark et al.,
2016). A survey of the AARP Public Policy Institute also revealed
that 51% of older unemployed workers (aged 47–70) reported
to be discriminated against because of their older age (Koenig
et al., 2015). In support of this, Wanberg et al. (2016) showed
negative relationships between job seekers’ chronological age,
reemployment status, and reemployment speed. Moreover, these
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 3
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
negative relations became stronger over age 50 (see also Perry
et al., 2016). Comparable findings have been reported in several
other Western countries outside the United States, showing less
positive callbacks for older job seekers when compared to their
equally qualified younger counterparts (e.g., Krings et al., 2011;
Åslund and Skans, 2012), indicating age discrimination in resume
screening to be a widespread, substantial, and pressing issue.
Anonymous Resume Screening
To avert age discrimination in the first phase of the screening
procedure, policy makers as well as researchers recommended
AAP (see Edin and Lagerström, 2006;Furtmueller et al., 2010;
Åslund and Skans, 2012). Furtmueller et al. (2010, p. 10),
for instance, concluded that “since employers are prohibited
to select employees based on gender, birth date, nationality
and marital status, resume forms should not ask for this
personal information.” Anonymous resume screening omits
explicit demographic cues from resumes that are non-job-related,
like date of birth, ethnic-sounding name, or gender (Edin and
Lagerström, 2006;Åslund and Skans, 2012;Krause et al., 2012;
Hiemstra et al., 2013).
Intriguingly, however, studies have shown that anonymous
resume screening may not be as effective as typically assumed
as ethnic minority or otherwise stigmatized applicants (e.g.,
like female or older applicants) are still more rejected when
they apply anonymous compared to equally qualified ethnic
majority or their non-stigmatized counterparts (Behaghel et al.,
2015;Maurer, 2016). For instance, in a field experiment in
Germany (Krause et al., 2012), female applicants were less
likely to receive a job interview invitation for a post doc
position in economics compared to equally qualified male
applicants when they applied with an AAP. In France,
organizations were less likely to invite minority applicants
when they received anonymous resumes (Behaghel et al., 2015).
The French government, therefore, abandoned the idea of
making anonymous resume screening mandatory in public
employment service offices. Recently, the Behavioral Economics
Team of the Australian Government (BETA) also showed that
de-identifying applications for senior positions decreased the
number of female and ethnic minority applicants shortlisted
for senior positions in the Australian Public Service (Hiscox
et al., 2017). Corroborating these findings, a recent scenario
study in which both American and European participants had
to screen resumes of chronologically older/younger applicants
(Kaufmann et al., 2016) revealed that hiring intentions of the
chronologically older applicants (i.e., who applied with resumes
that included age cues) did not differ significantly from those
of the ‘anonymous’ candidates (i.e., who applied with resumes
without age cues).
In a Dutch study, Hiemstra et al. (2013) showed that in the
absence of demographic information (as in anonymous resume
screening), real recruiters still gave lower job suitability ratings
to resumes of ethnic minority applicants compared to those of
their majority counterparts. Whereas human capital factors could
explain these findings to some extent, Hiemstra et al. (2013)
could not exclude hiring discrimination, either. Specifically,
other resume information (like applicants’ affiliations) than
explicit signals of one’s demographic background (like applicants’
chronological age or ethnic background) might operate as
‘implicit’ or subtle markers of applicants’ protected group status
and affect hirability ratings (Dovidio and Gaertner, 2000;Cole
et al., 2007;Derous et al., 2012).
Indeed, over the past decades, workplace discrimination
has become more subtle (Dovidio and Gaertner, 2000;Rosette
et al., 2016). In line with this, one could expect recruiters to
also turn their attention to more subtle cues in resumes to
gain information. Specifically, recruiters may infer applicants’
protected group membership from subtle cues (like one’s
affiliations, Dovidio and Gaertner, 2000;Hiemstra et al., 2013),
which contrasts with the often discussed view that anonymity
prevents recruiters from favoring majority over minority
applicants when credentials are equal, at least in the initial stage
of the hiring process. The above-mentioned findings raise the
question: Is there more into a resume than one’s date of birth
(i.e., or any other explicit age cue, like years of work experience)
that might disclose one’s chronological age and might instigate
age discrimination in hiring, albeit in more subtle ways?
Implicit Cues in Resumes
Job Market Signaling Theory
Recruitment researchers rely on job market signaling theory
(Spence, 1973, 1974) to explain how actors determine what
information is reliable for making job market choices. In its
more general and original sense, signaling theory refers to how
individuals (i.e., job applicants and recruiters/organizations)
with -partly- conflicting interests will communicate and
interpret signals/cues of unknown characteristics (i.e., of the
organization/job seeker) to obtain the biggest gains, like getting
hired or getting the best employees on board. Hence, signaling
systems are characterized by information asymmetry between
senders and receivers but are at the same time shaped by mutual
interests between signalers and receivers (Connelly et al., 2011).
Typically, signaling theory in recruitment research (i.e., job
market signaling theory; see Rynes, 1991) is used to explain the
cues job applicants use to make inferences about the prospective
employer/organization (Carter and Highhouse, 2014). Put
differently, the recruitment literature considers how observable
recruitment characteristics (like recruiter behavior) serve as
signals or cues of the unknown (to job seekers) organizational
quality or organizational characteristics. A warm recruiter, for
instance, may signal to applicants an organization that looks
after its employees (Slaughter et al., 2004). However, recruiters
also look for ‘cues’2about applicants’ overall and unknown
qualities in their resumes (Popken, 1993;Cable and Gilovich,
1998;Cole et al., 2003, 2009;Aguinis et al., 2005;Burns
et al., 2014). According to the normative-predictive model of
2In layman’s terms ‘cues’ and ‘signals’ are used intertwined (as synonyms). In
signaling theory, the notions ‘signal’ and ‘cue’ may differ conceptually from each
other in that ‘signals’ refer to traits or characteristics of the sender that might
change the behavior of the receiver to the benefit of the sender whereas ‘cues’ refer
to traits or characteristics of the sender that might benefit the receiver (Connelly
et al., 2011). Given the overall lower labor market outcomes of chronologically
older compared to younger persons (Wanberg et al., 2016), in the present paper,
implicit resume information referring to an applicant’s older age might be captured
more by the notion ‘cue’ than ‘signal.’
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 4
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
resume screening (Vieira Campos Proença and Valente Dias
de Oliveira, 2009;Guion, 2011), resume screening should be
based on objective and job-related information like applicants’
work experiences and educational background as mentioned in
resumes (Cole et al., 2007). Objective and job-related information
(like qualifications) might function as ‘explicit cues’ about
applicants’ competencies (Bangerter et al., 2012). Yet, Popken
(1993) indicated that recruiters also reply upon inference and
indirect speech acts when they read resumes. Cole et al. (2003,
2009) indeed showed recruiters to infer applicants’ personality
(like agreeableness) from work experiences as mentioned on
resumes (see also Burns et al., 2014). Recently, Kaufmann
et al. (2016) even showed trait-related inferences from resume
pictures to lower applicants’ hiring chances (i.e., with pictures of
applicants with old-appearing faces triggering impressions of low
health and fitness). Hence, recruiters also tend to infer subjective
attributes and even personality characteristics from resume
content in an indirect way (from educational credentials, work
experiences, and so on), albeit often not in very accurate ways
(see Apers and Derous, 2017;Cole et al., 2007). Put differently,
some objective and job-related information in resumes might also
function as ‘implicit cues’ about other applicant characteristics
(like attributes and traits) than this information is originally
intended to be used for.
Interestingly, most studies consider qualification-based
inferences (e.g., cues to applicants’ personality) but do not
consider social group status inferences (i.e., cues to applicants
protected group status, like age), which may also affect recruiters’
impressions of applicants’ overall job qualification (i.e., given
age-based inferences) and even impact recruiters’ hiring
judgments. For instance, applicant skills as mentioned in
resumes might signal applicants’ overall qualification in a very
explicit way but at the same time might communicate something
else being useful to recruiters, namely applicants’ chronological
age (Abrams et al., 2016). Whereas job market signaling theory
mainly focuses on actions taken by senders to communicate
positive, imperceptible qualities of the signaler to the receiver
(like acquired skills), other information could also be conveyed
(i.e., co-vary with positive qualities) that might turn-out to
be less beneficial or even harmful to senders (like their older
age). Implicit cues to one’s social group status, therefore, can
be considered as an unintended consequence of actions taken
by senders to communicate positive qualities through resume
information (Connelly et al., 2011). As shown by Abrams et al.
(2016), applicants’ skills’ set (i.e., observable signal of one’s
qualifications) might co-vary with receivers’ perceptions of
applicants’ chronological age (i.e., indirectly cause chronological
age is not explicitly mentioned in the applicants’ skills or any
other section). Hence, when job applicants provide explicit
information in their resumes about their skills (like ‘being a rapid
decision-maker’; ‘understanding others’ views well’), they might
indirectly signal their age too (when ‘being a rapid decision-
maker’ is associated with chronologically younger age and
‘understanding others’ is associated with chronologically older
age) which might affect recruiters’ stereotypical impressions of
the applicant’s potential productivity. In doing so, recruiters may
subtly factor-in job-irrelevant information, like one’s social group
status, that they infer from implicit age cues in applicant resumes
(Hiemstra et al., 2013;Abrams et al., 2016).
Implicit cues (e.g., regarding one’s chronological age) differ
further from explicit cues in that they may be much more ‘hard-
to-fake’ by applicants and ‘hard-to-resist’ by recruiters (Bangerter
et al., 2012;Abrams et al., 2016). First, cues are considered
‘hard-to-fake’ (or ‘honest’) if signaling happens beyond one’s
conscious control regarding one’s unobserved qualities (Connelly
et al., 2011;Bangerter et al., 2012). Information about one’s social
group status (e.g., age) is implicitly conveyed through ‘honest’
cues, meaning that these cues convey truly useful information to
the receiver about the sender’s social group status in an indirect,
non-manipulable way (i.e., beyond the sender’s awareness; see
Bangerter et al., 2012). Second, implicit cues are ‘hard-to-resist’
by recruiters. Recruiters might look for such implicit, hard-
to-fake cues in resumes because applicants are less likely to
consciously cheat on these implicit age cues as applicants might
not be aware of the age-related associations they indirectly send
to recruiters through this information (e.g., skills as ‘proxy’
of applicants’ chronological age; Abrams et al., 2016). Given
that applications are characterized by information asymmetry
between applicants and recruiters, such information might be
of specific interest to recruiters. Moreover, when implicit cues
refer to one’s social group status, they may increase category
salience and lessen recruiters’ ability to inhibit categorization
(Fiske et al., 1999), which may make them even much ‘harder-to-
resist.’ Hence, implicit age cues may fuel recruiters’ categorization
processes and ageist hiring preferences in rather subtle ways, as
discussed next.
Implicit Age Cues
The present study integrates predictions from theories that
explain hiring discrimination (i.e., cognitive models of
impression formation) with job market signaling theory by
considering recruiters’ use of age-related cues in resumes.
First, according to impression formation theories (Fiske et al.,
2002), resumes may trigger social categorization processes
and instigate hiring discrimination because of the limited
amount of individuating information (i.e., a one or two-page
resume). Second, given the limited amount of individuating
information on resumes and given that resume-screening is
characterized by information asymmetry (Bangerter et al., 2012),
recruiters may particularly look for both explicit and implicit
cues about applicant characteristics to base their hiring decisions
upon. Certain of these cues may signal applicants’ age and
may trigger age-related associations (like applicants’ physical
and psychological ‘fitness’) to recruiters. Whereas effects of
explicit age cues on recruiters’ hiring decisions have already
been demonstrated (e.g., using correspondence audit tests; see
Neumark et al., 2016), effects of implicit age cues remain largely
understudied and are considered here. Specifically, given that
implicit cues are ‘hard-to-fake’ and ‘hard-to-resist,’ one could
expect strong categorization effects from implicit age cues,
thereby affecting job suitability ratings of applicants with old vs.
young-age cues in a different way.
Two implicit age cues of interest to this study are applicants’
first names and extracurricular activities. First, as the popularity
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 5
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
of first names changes over time (Christopher, 1998;Sigurd
et al., 2005), names might indicate in a subtle, indirect way
a person’s chronological age and applicants might not be
aware of such a subtle and honest signal (i.e., age-related
association) in their resumes. Furthermore, because first names
are tied with one’s social and personal identity, they may
activate social categorization processes and ageism (Young et al.,
1993;Bennington and Wein, 2002;King et al., 2006;Derous
et al., 2009). Therefore, first names might be both hard-to-fake
(applicant perspective) and hard-to-resist (recruiter perspective).
For instance, Young et al. (1993) and Christopher (1998) both
showed that recruiters infer age from applicants’ first names.
Moreover, age associations seemed hard to resist as equally
qualified applicants with young-sounding names were perceived
more positively and received higher job suitability ratings than
those with old-fashioned names, showing there is more into a
resume than one’s date of birth (or any other explicit age cue)
that might disclose one’s chronological age and instigate age
discrimination (Perdue et al., 1990;Rudman et al., 1999).
Second, recruiters also evaluate applicants based on their
extracurricular activities/affiliations with socio-cultural groups
(Dovidio and Gaertner, 2000;Cole et al., 2007;Derous et al.,
2009). Cole et al. (2007) showed that if asked directly, recruiters
considered extracurricular activities as the least important
resume characteristic in judging applicants’ employability. Actual
employability ratings, however, showed exactly the opposite,
hence indicating that extracurricular activities were factored
in (hard-to-resist) when recruiters evaluated applicants. This
is buttressed by studies showing ethnic affiliations to lower
ethnic minorities’ job suitability ratings (Dovidio and Gaertner,
2000;Derous et al., 2009): Affiliations with certain socio-cultural
groups seem to affect recruiters’ information processing and
decision-making in subtle ways. Berger (2009) also suggests that
extracurricular activities in resumes can be beneficial to older
applicants when such activities counter stereotypical inferences
about the (older) applicant. Hence, as with names, extracurricular
activities might serve as hard-to-fake cues about applicants’
chronological age, particularly when applicants are unaware of
the age-related associations and such cues seem hard-to-resist
from a recruiter’s perspective (Cole et al., 2007;Abrams et al.,
2016).
Building further on both impression formation (Fiske et al.,
2002), and job market signaling theory (Bangerter et al., 2012)
to explain age discrimination in resume-screening, we expected
effects of implicit age cues in such a way that:
Hypothesis 1. Applicant resumes with old-sounding names
(Hypothesis 1a) and old-fashioned activities (Hypothesis
1b) will receive lower job suitability ratings than those from
equally qualified applicants with young implicit age cues.
The traditional view of bias considers membership in a
particular social group (e.g., with a protected status) as having
the same effect on employment outcomes for all members of
that group; typically studies do not consider multiple cues in
conjunction and within-category differences (i.e., differences
between members from the same social category; see Kaiser
and Pratt-Hyatt, 2009;Marcus and Fritzsche, 2014). That is,
compared to a single cue on a resume (e.g., name only) one would
expect multiple cues (like old-sounding name and old-fashioned
extracurricular activities) to increase category salience and lessen
the ability of even a motivated decision maker to inhibit the
activation of a social category (Kulik et al., 2007;Derous et al.,
2017). Similarly, research has suggested that minority group
members may be rejected in proportion to their ‘outgroupness’
(Crisp and Hewstone, 1999). Several studies offer evidence that
multiple cues to minority membership may lead to greater
hiring discrimination (Uhlmann et al., 2002;Segrest Purkiss
et al., 2006;Derous et al., 2009, 2015). Segrest Purkiss et al.
(2006), for instance, found that two ethnic cues (name; accent)
led to more negative interviewer reactions than one cue only.
Derous et al. (2009) also showed that the strength of applicants’
ethnic in-group identification (or social category salience) as
appearing on resumes affected their job suitability ratings with
highly Arab-identified minority applicants receiving lower job
suitability ratings compared to equally qualified but less highly
Arab-identified applicants. Similarly, because category salience
will affect attention to that category (Kulik et al., 2007) and
because protected group members (like older workers) may be
rejected in proportion to their degree of identification with the
protected social group of interest (Kaiser and Pratt-Hyatt, 2009),
we expected for implicit age cues that:
Hypothesis 2. Applicant resumes with more implicit cues
referring to older age will receive lower job suitability
ratings than those with less implicit cues referring to
older age.
Recruiter Age
Recruiters may differ in their susceptibility to bias. Given the
limited number of studies that considered effects of recruiter
characteristics on ageism in hiring (Kulik et al., 2000) and in
resume-screening in particular (Fasbender and Wang, 2017), we
focus on recruiters’ chronological age as a potential boundary
condition of implicit age effects. Two competing perspectives
have been put forward regarding implicit social evaluation.
On the one hand, implicit evaluations may favor members
from the in-group (i.e., in-group favoritism). In a simulated
hiring study, Krings et al. (2011) found recruiters’ own age to
attenuate age bias to some extent: The probability of selecting
the older candidate instead of the younger candidate increased
with increasing age of the evaluator. Specifically, age bias was
no longer observed or even turned into older worker favoritism
when recruiters were equal in age or older than the older
job candidate himself. These findings support the idea of in-
group favoritism. Recently, Axt et al. (2014) showed that for
both race and gender, the hierarchy of implicit evaluations
places in-group members at the top, therefore also evidencing
in-group favoritism. Indeed, to preserve one’s social identity,
one might favor in-group members (Tajfel and Turner, 1979).
Interestingly, however, Axt et al. (2014; see also Nosek et al., 2002)
showed a peculiar feature of implicit age effects: The hierarchy
of implicit evaluations did not reflect in-group favoritism for
age. Instead, older participants also placed older adults at the
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 6
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
bottom of the social hierarchy (Axt et al., 2014). Thus, older
adults were preferred considerably less than younger adults, also
by older-aged participants, which runs counter to the idea of
in-group favoritism as found in the context of implicit race and
gender evaluations. Corroborating with this, Christopher (1998)
also reported older-age effects (based on first names) among both
younger and older participants. Why implicit in-group favoritism
does not occur for age cannot be inferred from these data.
Yet, Marcus and Sabuncu (2016) recently suggest evolutionary
explanations for ageism. Individuals may tend to systematically
avoid and even discriminate against older individuals that are
reminiscent of potential sickness or decline, in order to protect
one’s social group status and one’s individual ego from the
threat of sickness, decline, and eventually death. Since thoughts
about decline and death tend to particularly operate at the
implicit level (Levy and Banaji, 2002), they may exacerbate
the negative effects of implicit old-age cues on both older and
younger recruiters’ hiring decisions. Hence, evolutionary theories
of ageism consider prejudice and discrimination against older
applicants as a ‘defense mechanism’ that equally affects older and
younger evaluators. In line with these assumptions, one could
also expect the opposite, namely a lack of in-group favoritism
from the part of the older-aged recruiter in case he/she evaluates
resumes with implicitly mentioned old-age cues. Yet, given the
limited empirical evidence in the context of resume-screening,
we formulated the following research question on the potential
effect of recruiters’ chronological age:
Research question. Will applicant resumes with more
implicit age cues referring to older age receive higher job
suitability ratings from chronologically older recruiters than
younger recruiters (in-group favoritism) or will applicant
resumes with more implicit old-age cues receive equally low
job suitability ratings from older recruiters as from younger
recruiters (lack of in-group favoritism)?
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Ethics Statement
The study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines
of the ‘General Ethical Protocol for Scientific Research at
the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences’ of
the Ethical Commission of the Faculty of Psychology and
Educational Sciences at Ghent University, which is the relevant
university institutional review board that considers ethical
aspects. In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki,
participants provided informed written consent prior to their
participation. Participants were debriefed after all the data were
collected.
Participants
Participants of the main study were HR professionals in
organizations who regularly recruited applicants and who were
identified through researchers’ professional contacts, databases,
and networks. In total, 1424 HR professionals were emailed the
study link to participate, of which 45.86% (N=653) agreed to
participate. Of this group, 93.42% (N=610) were eligible because
they recruited applicants on a daily base (i.e., spending about 38%
of their daily activities on recruiting). Hence, the final sample
comprised 610 participants who recruited applicants frequently
(also called ‘recruiters’ or ‘raters’) with a mean age of 41.15 years
(SD =11.25 years), 50.70% males, of which 87% had a university
degree (bachelor or higher), and about 1/3rd held a lower/junior
(32.8%), middle/senior (36.4%), or higher (24.9%) position.
Procedure and Design
A field-based randomized experimental study (i.e.,
resume-screening experiment over the internet) was conducted.
Participants received an email with an url and personal code
that asked for participation in a study on the development of a
tool aimed to train/assess recruiters’ competencies (see Derous
et al., 2015, for a similar approach). To mask the study purpose
to a further extent and to reduce potential item priming, we also
included several filler items (Podsakoff et al., 2003).
After having completed the informed consent form,
participants read a job description for a ‘project manager’
(i.e., age-neutral as pilot tested; Perry, 1994). Subsequently,
participants read and evaluated four resumes (i.e., equally
qualified, see ‘Development of study materials and pilot studies’).
Specifically, we conducted a 2 (Date of birth: absent vs. present)
by [2 (Name: young vs. old-sounding) by 2 (Extracurricular
activities: modern vs. old-fashioned)] mixed factorial design.
Date of birth (i.e., explicit cue about applicants’ chronological
age) was the between-subjects factor, with chronologically
younger applicants born in 1987 and older applicants in 1959.
The younger applicants were 26 years’ old (born in 1987) whereas
the chronologically older applicants were 54 years’ old (born
in 1959) at the moment of the data collection. These birth
years were selected because previous studies showed more age
discrimination for people over 30, and especially when being
over 50 (Albert et al., 2011;Ahmed et al., 2012;Richardson
et al., 2013). Name and Extracurricular activities (i.e., implicit
age cues) were the within-subjects factors. We paired the explicit
age cue with the corresponding (young vs. old-sounding) name
on the job applicants’ resumes to avoid unrealistic combinations
(Christopher, 1998). Job applicants’ sex was kept constant (male
applicants only3), and applicants’ resumes were counterbalanced
to avoid order effects. The dependent variable was job suitability
(or hirability rating; see ‘Measures’). After the resume-sifting
task, participants completed a filler task, consisting of several
distractor items (e.g., asking how they screen applicants). As part
of this filler task, we also asked participants to indicate applicants’
age based on names and affiliations (i.e., manipulation checks).
In the end, participants were asked to fill-out an ‘opinion survey’
that included measures of old-age stereotypes4, social desirability,
3Only male applicants were considered because men participate more in the labor
market where this study was conducted than women and because of the need to
remove gender as a potentially confounding factor in the design (Sidanius and
Veniegas, 2000;OECD, 2015).
4We administered the ‘Beliefs About Older Workers Scale’ (Hassell and Perrewe,
1995). Because confirmatory factor analysis with maximum likelihood estimation
(Lisrel v. 9.2) showed a bad fit for a one-factor model (i.e., meaning that the
scale was not unidimensional as suggested by the authors), we did not consider
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 7
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
and participants’ demographics. We ended the study with an
open-ended probe to ask for any suspicion regarding the study
purpose. After data were collected, participants were debriefed.
Development of Study Materials and
Pilot Studies
Prior to the main study, study materials (i.e., advertisement
and resumes) were developed and a series of pilot studies
(Ntotal =183) were conducted to ensure relevance and
equivalence of study materials. (see Supplementary Material for
a more detailed description).
First, 25 jobs were selected to evaluate whether these jobs were
equally accessible for older and younger workers. Given that there
is evidence for the effect of job-related age stereotypes on hiring
outcomes (Perry, 1994;Abrams et al., 2016), we aimed to select
an age-neutral job. Results of Pilot Study 1 (n=47, Mage =27.39,
SDage =5.1, 68% males) showed that the job of ‘project manager’
was perceived as equally accessible for younger and older workers
and therefore selected for this study.
Second, implicit age cues were developed and pilot tested.
Applicant names were selected based on the annual statistical
reports of the local government and onomastics that indicate
the popularity of first names in the geographical area where
this study was conducted. We also selected 24 extracurricular
activities. Based on the results of Pilot Study 2 (n=60,
Mage =27.86, SDage =7.51; 73% males), we selected ‘Fons’
and ‘Frans’ as names of older people and ‘Jens’ and ‘Niels’ as
names of younger people. We further selected old-fashioned
extracurricular activities (i.e., being a member of a bridge club;
being a pigeon/finches fancier; being a walking club member)
and extracurricular activities that are seen as modern/typically
performed by younger people (i.e., being a member of boy scouts;
being a snowboarder; being a life board crew member/rescuer).
Third, we listed all other information needed for the
resume template (i.e., educational degree/level, work experiences,
language, IT proficiency, home address) based on actual resumes
posted on job search websites in the area of interest (note that
identifying information was deleted). We had to make sure this
information to be relevant, age neutral, and equivalent across
resumes (as in the case of work experiences) and had to keep
this resume information constant (as in the case of language, IT
proficiency, home address/neighborhood). Based on Pilot Study 3
(n=76, Mage =25.91, SDage =9.8, 65.4% males), we therefore
selected a ‘Master of Science degree in economics’ (relevant
as pilot tested) and four work experiences (equivalent as pilot
tested). Finally, we held language (English, French, German, and
Dutch) and IT proficiency (SAP, MS office, team foundation
server) constant as well as applicants’ neighborhood (middle
class).
Based on the results of the pilot studies, we then integrated
all information to create the final resumes. In sum, the resume
template included information about the applicant’s name (old-
sounding vs. young-sounding first names as pilot tested), sex
(male), date of birth (born in 1987 vs. 1959), home address
recruiters’ beliefs about older workers in any further analyses (see also Discussion
section).
(middle-class area), relevant educational background and level
(Master of Science degree in economics as pilot tested), relevant
work experiences (without any indication of the number of
years of professional/work experience and considered equivalent
across resumes as pilot tested), language and IT proficiency
(held constant), and extracurricular activities (modern vs. old-
fashioned activities as pilot tested).
Measures
After participants observed the job ad and the four resumes,
they responded to questions using a 5-point Likert-type response
scale (unless otherwise mentioned). Job suitability was measured
with a 4-item measure adapted from Derous et al. (2009). An
example item is “Given all information you read about this
applicant, how suitable do you believe this applicant is for
this function?” (1 =not suitable at all to 5 =very suitable).
Cronbach’s alpha for the four resumes ranged from 0.91 to 0.93
(see Table 1). Second, to check manipulations, participants rated
applicants’ perceived age based on the job applicant’s name and
extracurricular activities using the following, self-constructed
item: “With what age do you associate [extracurricular activities]
or [name of applicant as appearing on the resume]” (1 =very
young to 5 =very old). Third, to control for social desirable
responding, eight items were adapted from the Impression
Management Scale (BIDR, Paulhus, 1991). An example item
is: “When I hear people talking privately, I avoid listening”
(1 =strongly disagree to 5 =strongly agree). Cronbachs alpha was
0.70. Finally, participants indicated demographics including their
chronological age (open answer), their sex (0 =female; 1 =male),
educational level (1 =college; 2 =university), job level (1 =lower,
2=middle, 3 =higher level) as well as recruiting experience.
Recruiting experience was measured with one self-constructed
item, namely “How much time do you spend on recruiting
activities on a daily base?” (1 =1–30%to4=70–100%).
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Before testing the hypotheses, preliminary analyses were
conducted to check model assumptions, randomization,
manipulations, intercorrelations and potential covariate
variables. First, we tested assumptions of normality and
homogeneity of variances, which were met for each of the
applicant resumes. Inspection of the PP-plots, skewness and
kurtosis showed that distributions were approximately normal,
so that there is support for the assumption of normality. (Since
the repeated measure variables only had two levels, assumption
of sphericity was not tested.). Levene’s test further showed that
homogeneity of variances was met for all repeated measures.
Second, female and male applicants were equally distributed
across conditions, χ2(1,610) =0.22, p=0.64, and experimental
conditions did not differ from each other in participant age,
F(1,608) =0.15, p=0.70, participants’ educational level,
χ2(1,610) =0.74, p=0.39, job level, χ2(2,574) =0.32, p=0.85,
and recruiting experience, χ2(3,610) =4.19, p=0.24, supporting
the assumption of randomization.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 8
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
TABLE 1 | Descriptives, reliabilities and correlations among study variables.
M SD 12345678910
(1) Job suitability ‘Jens’a3.79 0.67 (0.92)
(2) Job suitability ‘Frans’b3.54 0.71 0.361∗∗ (0.91)
(3) Job suitability ‘Niels’c3.21 0.76 0.461∗∗ 0.307∗∗ (0.93)
(4) Job suitability ‘Fons’d3.32 0.70 0.310∗∗ 0.328∗∗ 0.395∗∗ (0.91)
(5) Social desirability 3.73 0.55 0.069 0.006 0.011 0.010 (0.70)
(6) Chronological agee41.15 11.3 0.066 0.0990.0890.111∗∗ 0.114∗∗
(7) Recruiting experiencef1.37 0.83 0.031 0.078 0.0940.0880.015 0.359∗∗
(8) Genderg0.51 0.50 0.033 0.0840.147∗∗ 0.0860.0850.333∗∗ 0.275∗∗
(9) Educational levelh1.87 0.33 0.058 0.043 0.021 0.050 0.031 0.063 0.063 0.097
(10) Job leveli1.92 0.77 0.008 0.070 0.025 0.042 0.0950.270∗∗ 0.174∗∗ 0.288∗∗ 0.015 -
Note. Cronbach’s alpha are on the diagonal. a‘Jens’: Resume with young-sounding name and modern extracurricular activities; b‘Frans’: resume with old-sounding name
and modern extracurricular activities; c‘Niels’: resume with young-sounding name and old-fashioned extracurricular activities; d‘Fons’: resume with old-sounding name
and old-fashioned extracurricular activities. eChronological age of the participants. fOnly participants who actually recruited applicants were included in the final sample
(N =610); recruiting experiences were coded as: 1 =less than or about 30% recruiting activities per day, 2 =30–50% recruiting activities per day; 3 =50–70% recruiting
activities per day; 4 =70% or more recruiting activities per day. gGender: 0 =female; 1 =male. hEducational level: 1 =college, 2 =university. iJob level: 1 =lower,
2=middle, 3 =higher. p<0.05, ∗∗ p<0.01.
Third, manipulation checks were successful: Young-sounding
names were perceived as significantly younger (M=2.07;
SD =0.47) than old-sounding names (M=4.00; SD =0.47),
t(609) =59.82, p=0.00. Old-fashioned activities were perceived
as significantly older (M=4.09; SD =0.52) than modern
activities (M=1.96; SD =0.46), t(609) = −64.64, p=0.00.
Fourth, inspection of the correlation table (see Table 1)
indicated that correlations were not overly strong and in line with
what could be expected (e.g., no relation between job suitability
ratings of any of the applicant profiles on the one hand and
social desirable responding, educational level, and job level on
the other hand). Finally, as literature suggests that recruiters’
gender and recruiting experience might affect resume evaluations
(e.g., Cole et al., 2003, 2007;Waung et al., 2015), we also
checked whether participants’ gender and recruiting experience
needed to be controlled for in the main analyses (Bernerth and
Aguinis, 2016). Gender and resume screening experience related
significantly to the job suitability ratings of some of the applicant
profiles/resumes. However, because further analyses showed that
homogeneity of regressions was not supported, participants’
gender and recruiting experience did not appear to be good
covariates and therefore were not included in the final analyses5
(Weinfurt, 1995;Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007).
Testing of Hypotheses and Research
Question
Table 1 presents descriptives, reliabilities, and correlations
among study variables. Hypotheses were tested with a series of
mixed analyses of covariances and simple effects analyses, given
the experimental set-up and nature of this study (see Derous et al.,
2015, for a similar approach). Hypothesis 1 investigated whether
applicant resumes with old-sounding names (Hypothesis 1a) and
old-fashioned activities (Hypothesis 1b) would receive lower job
5Additional analyses in which participants’ gender and recruiting experience were
included did not result in different effects than when gender and recruiting
experience were not included (see Results section).
TABLE 2 | Results of mixed analyses of variance for job suitability.
Source df F p η2
Between subjects
Date of birth (A) 1 5.32 0.02 0.02
Error (A) 608 (0.26)
Within subjects
Name (B) 1 9.32 0.00 0.02
Date of birth (A) ×Name (B) 1 0.56 0.45 0.00
Error (B) 608 (0.36)
Activities (C) 1 278.91 0.00 0.31
Date of birth (A) ×Activities (C) 1 0.69 0.41 0.00
Error (C) 608 (0.34)
Name (B) ×Activities (C) 1 73.72 0.00 0.11
Date of birth (A) ×Name (B) ×Activities (C) 1 0.53 0.47 0.00
Error (B ×C) 608 (0.27)
Note. The between-subjects factor refers to the explicit age cue (date of birth:
absent vs. present) whereas the within-subjects factors refer to the implicit age
cues (name/extracurricular activities: old vs. young) on resumes. The pattern of
results remained the same when participants’ recruiting experience and gender
were controlled for.
suitability ratings than those from equally qualified applicants
with young implicit age cues (i.e., young-sounding names;
modern activities) in their resumes. Job suitability was lower for
resumes with old-sounding names than young-sounding names,
F(1,608) =9.32, p<0.01, η2=0.02, and old-fashioned activities
than modern activities, F(1,608) =278.91, p<0.001, η2=0.31,
thereby supporting effects of implicit age cues (Hypotheses 1a
and 1b were supported). Interestingly, results further showed that
job suitability was lowest when the explicit age cue (i.e., date
of birth) was omitted from resumes, F(1,608) =5.32, p=0.02,
η2=0.02 (see Table 2).
Hypothesis 2 further investigated whether resumes with more
implicit cues referring to older age would receive lower job
suitability ratings than those with less implicit cues referring
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 9
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
FIGURE 1 | Interaction of implicit age cues (names; extracurricular activities)
in resumes on job suitability ratings.
to older age. A significant two-way interaction was found
for implicit age cues (Name ×Extracurricular activities),
F(1,608) =73.72, p<0.001, η2=0.11, and this effect did
not depend on explicit age cues, F(1,608) =0.53, p=0.47,
η2=0.00. Simple effects analyses (i.e., to break down the
interaction term) further showed significant effects of each
of the repeated measures variable (i.e., name or activities) at
levels of the other repeated measures variable (i.e., activities
or name, respectively) with p=0.00 (in all cases). Inspection
of simple effects and Figure 1 showed that the resume with
both a young-sounding name and modern activities received the
highest ratings, followed by the resume with an old-sounding
name/modern activities, and the resume with an old-sounding
name/old-fashioned activities. The resume with an old-sounding
name and old-fashioned activities was rated significantly higher
than the one with a young-sounding name and old-fashioned
activities (Table 2 and Figure 1). Hypothesis 2, therefore, was
partially supported.
Finally, we formulated a research question to explore potential
effects of recruiters’ chronological age. Specifically, we questioned
whether resumes with more implicit cues referring to the
applicant’s older age would receive higher job suitability ratings
when recruiters were chronologically older, or whether applicant
resumes with more implicit cues referring to the applicant’s
older age would receive equally low job suitability ratings
from both older and younger recruiters? Results showed that
participants’ chronological age moderated effects of implicit age
cues (i.e., name and activities) on applicants’ job suitability
ratings, F(1,607) =6.07, p=0.014, η2=0.01. The test of
within-subjects effects further showed that chronologically older
participants gave lower scores to resumes of applicants with old-
sounding names, whereas no differences were found for resumes
of applicants with young-sounding names, F(1,607) =6.73,
p=0.01, η2=0.01. In a similar vein, chronologically older
participants gave lower scores to resumes of applicants with
old-fashioned activities, whereas no differences were found for
resumes of applicants with modern activities, F(1,607) =6.31,
p=0.01, η2=0.01 (see Figure 2).
FIGURE 2 | Moderating effects of recruiters’ chronological age and implicit
age cues (names; extracurricular activities) on job suitability ratings.
DISCUSSION
Intrigued by the ongoing debate about the usefulness of
anonymous resume screening in many Western societies (e.g.,
Behaghel et al., 2015;Maurer, 2016), coupled with limited studies
that considered AAP effectiveness and subtle mechanisms in
hiring discrimination, this study investigated whether omitting
explicit age cues (like date of birth) might be beneficial to
older applicants or whether more implicit/subtle age cues in
resumes may still affect older job applicants’ hirability ratings.
Further, because research in personnel recruitment and selection
has often failed to consider differences in raters’ tendencies to
differentiate among applicants (e.g., Fasbender and Wang, 2017),
we investigated raters’ chronological age as a potential moderator.
Overall Findings
Antidiscrimination legislation and diversity policies typically
focus on the impact of explicit age markers on resumes.
Unfortunately, the implicit age cues on hiring decisions may
pass somewhat unnoticed. Building on the principles of job
market signaling theory, we found support for the assumption
that recruiters make inferences about applicants’ age based on
implicit (i.e., ‘hard-to-fake’ and ‘hard-to-resist’) cues in resumes
(i.e., applicant first names; extracurricular activities), even in the
absence of explicit age cues. Specifically, resumes with both a
young-sounding name and modern activities received the highest
job suitability ratings, followed by resumes with an old-sounding
name/modern activities, and resumes with an old-sounding
name/old-fashioned activities. This provides support for so-
called ‘within-category’ effects (i.e., Kaiser and Pratt-Hyatt, 2009):
Resumes with more implicit cues referring to older age received
lower job suitability ratings than less strongly ‘old-age’ identified
resumes or those from presumably younger applicants.
Remarkably and somewhat unexpectedly, resumes with a
young-sounding name and old-fashioned activities received
the lowest job suitability ratings. Because this resume
might not have matched the ‘prototypical’ image of young
applicants, recruiters might have rated this applicant
lowest in overall suitability due to attributional discounting
(Kelley, 1973). Indeed, as young applicants are expected
to engage rather in modern activities than old-fashioned
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 10
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
activities, young applicants (as perceived on the basis of
their young-sounding name) who do not do so might
have disconfirmed and discounted the dominant young age
stereotypes.
Furthermore, omitting explicit age cues led to overall lower job
suitability ratings. In many Western European societies (as where
this study was conducted) it is good practice to indicate date of
birth on one’s resume. Not doing so might deviate from the social
norm and result in overall lower ratings. Alternatively, implicit
age cues might have subtle though stronger effects in the absence
of explicit age cues, given that they are hard-to-fake by applicants
and hard-to-resist by raters. Moreover, some resume information
(like unexplained interruptions in one’s work history) might
not be understood properly when recruiters do not know the
applicant’s socio-demographic background (like age). Although
this was not the case in the present study design (and no
significant interaction of the implicit age cues with the explicit
age cue was found, either), it has been suggested that anonymous
resume screening might not be ideal when there are structural
differences between majority-minority applicants (like disparate
length of unemployment or educational attainment) as explicit
age cues (like age) might also explain, contextualize, and alleviate
any subgroup differences (Behaghel et al., 2015). The latter could
be investigated to a further extent.
Finally, compared to applicant characteristics, recruiter
characteristics have been investigated to a considerable lesser
extent in hiring discrimination literature (Finkelstein and Burke,
1998); we explored HR professionals’ chronological age to
address this gap. First, individuals of different ages may differ
in their attitudes toward older adults (Gordon and Arvey,
2004). Indeed, and although effects were small, older participants
compared to younger participants gave somewhat lower job
suitability scores to older applicants than to younger applicants.
Interestingly, this finding does not support in-group favoritism
which predicts participants to prefer rather than to disfavor
in-group members. According to Levy and Banaji (2002), the
psychologically permeable nature of boundaries between age
groups might allow one to dissociate from his/her own age
group. This finding further seems to fit evolutionary theories
on (implicit) ageism, that explain bias against older persons
as a function of mortality salience, and – at the individual
level- as one’s fear of aging and a way to avoid psychologically
and physically weak and parasitized individuals to protect one’s
own ego and sense of self-esteem (Martens et al., 2005;Marcus
and Sabuncu, 2016). Alternatively, findings also remind of the
‘black sheep effect’ (Marques et al., 1988;Brewer, 2007): Older
recruiters may reject in-group members as a self-enhancement
strategy (i.e., positive distinctiveness) or as a manner of securing
one’s position in an organization (i.e., optimal distinctiveness).
Finkelstein and Burke (1998) found similar results on in-group
bias. However, they used the availability heuristic to explain why
older raters disfavored older applicants when applicants’ age was
highly salient and when the raters identified with their age groups.
Specifically, older HR professionals might be more aware of
potential risks when hiring peers (e.g., negative attributes, more
expensive, early retirement intentions; Kite et al., 2005;Rau and
Adams, 2014). Younger generations of workers, on the other
hand, may grow-up in a climate where discrimination issues
receive much more attention than previously was the case, which
may make younger HR professionals somewhat more cautious
about age-related bias.
One might also consider other recruiter characteristics
than chronological age, like recruiters’ old-age stereotypes.
Old-age stereotypes typically include views that older people
are less productive, economically beneficial, competent, creative,
flexible, and harder to train (Finkelstein and Burke, 1998;
Kulik et al., 2000;Fiske et al., 2002). However, old-age
stereotypes are not unequivocal negative in nature (Fiske
et al., 2002). Finkelstein et al. (2013), for instance, revealed
a balanced view of older workers’ stereotypes, with many
positive older-worker stereotypes (e.g., well-mannered, strong
work ethic, and reliable). Hence, old-age stereotypes may
affect hiring decisions in a complex way (Krings et al., 2011).
Yet, few studies considered recruiters’ old-age stereotypes as
potential moderators of hiring decisions (Krings et al., 2011;
Lu et al., 2011;Fasbender and Wang, 2017). Research might
therefore include validated measures of old-age stereotypes (like
the Work-related Age-based Stereotypes scale; Marcus et al.,
2016). Also more implicit measures of old-age stereotypes
might be particularly interesting when one aims to investigate
effects of implicit older-age cues on resumes (like the Implicit
Association Test; see Levy and Banaji, 2002;Axt et al.,
2014).
In sum, this paper explored one recruiter characteristic that
might contribute to implicit old-age bias in the context of resume
screening, namely recruiters’ own chronological age. No evidence
was found for in-group favoritism, which is in line with previous
findings on implicit age attitudes (Axt et al., 2014). Older-age
bias seems a pressing and complex issue. Yet, as illustrated, and
with the exception of a few studies (e.g., Marcus and Sabuncu,
2016;Fasbender and Wang, 2017), its psychological roots are still
less well understood, particularly as regards implicit age bias, and
therefore in need for further investigation.
Limitations and Further Research
Opportunities
As with any study, limitations need to be mentioned. First,
we used a hypothetical job with job applicants in the form
of ‘paper’ people (Landy, 2008). Yet, in an early phase of
the application procedure, real applicants are ‘paper’ people
as the only information we have is the information applicants
provide on their resume. Moreover, in preliminary screening
situations, individuating information about applicants is still
limited, therefore, the use of paper people/resumes is ecologically
valid in this setting (Copus, 2005).
Second, in our study we employed a mixed-factorial design
with implicit age cues as within-subjects factors, which is more
realistic than using them in a between-subjects manner (i.e.,
as this is what recruiters do, namely comparing applicants’
resumes to each other), but which has also been criticized
for its potential to inflate hiring discrimination scores (e.g.,
Bal et al., 2011). A meta-analysis on age bias in evaluations
(i.e., hiring, promotion, and performance appraisal) by Gordon
and Arvey (2004), however, showed higher mean ds for
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 11
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
between-subjects designs than for within-subjects designs.
Nevertheless, given that age may become more salient if
operationalized in a within-subjects manner, follow-up studies
may use between-subjects designs to further investigate implicit
age effects.
Third, scenario-based studies can also be criticized on the
ground of their lower external validity, but serve other purposes
than field studies (like correspondence audit tests) as they
may be conducted in a more controlled way and may test
contingencies surrounding discriminatory decisions in hiring.
Future studies, however, could investigate more and other
moderators related to the applicant (like gender; Sidanius
and Veniegas, 2000), the job (like type of job, differences
in responsibility, and decision-making power; Abrams et al.,
2016), and the recruiter (like prejudices and other individual
difference variables of relevance; see Self et al., 2015;Fasbender
and Wang, 2017). For instance, Brtek and Motowidlo (2002)
and Self et al. (2015) showed that recruiters’ accountability
might affect discriminatory decision-making, with some types
of accountability (like identity-blind accountability) leading to
less bias and more objective decision-making than other types
of accountability (like identity-conscious accountability). In our
scenario-based study, accountability was not primed in any way
and participants were randomized over conditions. Therefore, we
believe accountability may not have played a large role in affecting
findings. Further research on implicit age effects, however, might
either control for types of rater accountability or might look
at potential effects of recruiters’ accountability on implicit age
effects (e.g., by priming accountability). Further, despite our
initial attempt to consider both recruiters’ chronological age and
older worker stereotypes as potential moderators of implicit age
effects, we refrained from further investigating and reporting
effects of raters’ older-age stereotypes because of the suboptimal
psychometric properties of the ‘Beliefs About Older Workers
scale (Hassell and Perrewe, 1995) in our sample. Researchers
interested in investigating moderating effects of recruiters’ older
worker stereotypes may use more recently developed and
validated scales such as, for instance, the ‘Beliefs about Older
Workers’ Ability and Desire for Learning and Development’ scale
(Maurer et al., 2008) or the ‘Work-related Age-based Stereotypes’
scale (Marcus et al., 2016).
Fourth, we distinguish explicit from implicit age cues.
Whether cues are rather ‘implicit’ or ‘explicit’ may depend on
the nature of the (protected) group status under consideration.
For instance, applicants’ names may signal in a rather explicit,
direct way one’s ethnic group membership (e.g., Mohammed
vs. Mark), but in a rather implicit, indirect manner one’s age.
Furthermore, according to signaling theory, implicit age cues are
more ‘hard-to-fake’ (or ‘honest’). However, one could argue that
both names and extracurricular activities are ‘fakeable’ to some
extent. This would be the case when applicants are aware of
the particular cues and associations they communicate through
affiliations/names (Bangerter et al., 2012).
Another potential study limitation to our field-based,
randomized experimental study may be non-response bias.
About half of the HR professionals we emailed the study link
also participated in this study, which is considerable. Yet, as
the likelihood of non-response bias is inversely related to the
response rate, this also means that about 50% of respondents
did not participate in the study for some or another reason.
Non-response bias may be a threat to the external validity of
an experimental study if non-respondents’ profiles and answers
differ substantially from the profiles and answers of those who
did respond to the study (Stone-Romero, 2002). We were not
able to log personal information of non-respondents, nor were we
able to register reasons of non-response. Given that participants
were randomly assigned to the experimental conditions, one
might expect non-response also to be random. Yet, whether
non-response is random should be investigated empirically.
Therefore, further research should take this potential limitation
into account, for instance, by finding ways to examine reasons
for non-response and by controlling for non-respondents’
demographic attributes.
Finally, this study was conducted in a Western European
country. Whereas there seems little evidence of a connection
between cultural practices and recruitment and selection
practices (see Ryan et al., 2017 for a recent review), cross-cultural
differences could exist in the interpretation of age-related cues
and attitudes toward older workers (e.g., North and Fiske,
2015), which was not considered here and may warrant further
investigation.
Theoretical and Practical Implications
Our study aimed to extend work on age bias in resume screening
in several ways. First, although the Nobel Prize winning paper
of Spence (1973) included hiring as an example (Bangerter
et al., 2012), relatively little research has applied Spence’s
job market signaling theory to (age-based) discrimination in
recruitment and more particularly to the act of resume screening.
Studies that applied job market signaling theory in recruitment
typically considered how applicants interpret unobservable
characteristics from signals sent by employers/recruiters through
recruitment devices (like job advertisements; see Carter and
Highhouse, 2014). Recruitment, however, is a two-way process
in which applicants also send signals or cues upon which
recruiters/employers may make hiring decisions, which we
considered here. Second, research suggests that bias has become
more subtle (Rosette et al., 2016), but few studies have measured
this in the context of early screening and resume screening
in particular, which we did. Hence, with our study, we extend
applications of job market signaling theory in recruitment by
considering how employers/recruiters may interpret age-related
information from subtle cues in job applicants’ resumes. Third,
studies that did consider recruiters’ inferences and indirect
speech acts mainly focused on applicants’ qualifications including
personality inferences (Cole et al., 2007;Aguinis et al., 2005;
Burns et al., 2014;Apers and Derous, 2017) but not on social
group status, which we investigated and -to the best of our
knowledge- has not been considered much. Finally, given the
widespread use of resume screening there is a growing interest
in studying age effects in this screening stage. Although job
applicants’ age shows no validity for predicting future job
performance (Schmidt et al., 2016), age discrimination seems
substantial as evidenced by a large number of recently conducted
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 12
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
correspondence audit studies. Correspondence audits offer a
great amount of control and are a very powerful tool to detect
labor market discrimination but fail to examine unobserved
factors, such as recruiter characteristics. Our study adds to the
literature on age discrimination in hiring that just began to
investigate contingencies in recruiters (see for another example:
Fasbender and Wang, 2017).
Findings might also be relevant to practitioners. When more
individuating information about candidates becomes available
in later stages of the screening procedure (such as in the job
interview or assessment centers), category-based biases might
have less of a chance to color decisions; however, this suggests
the critical importance of ensuring a lack of discrimination
at the earliest stage of resume screening. Anonymous resume
sifting may be one tool to level the playing field, but is much
debated by HR professionals and society at large (Maurer,
2016;Hiscox et al., 2017). Indeed, anonymous resume screening
might be much more complex than it appears at first glance
and our results bear that out. First, omitting explicit cues
to one’s chronological age (like one’s date of birth) led to
overall lower job suitability ratings, and this might have
to do with codes of conduct (i.e., what recruiters deem
appropriate to be mentioned in resumes). However, it has also
been suggested that explicit age cues in resumes might help
recruiters understand to a better extent some other, potentially
stigmatizing resume information (Behaghel et al., 2015) and -
hence- make recruiters even more attentive/sensitive to ageism,
which could paradoxically counter age-related bias. Finally,
blind screening is at odds with targeted recruitment initiatives
if one aims to hire for more diversity (Newman and Lyon,
2009).
Whereas names are considered as identifying information
and –hence- blotted in anonymous resume screening,
extracurricular activities are typically not blotted. Yet, HR
professionals still made age inferences based on implicit age cues
like extracurricular activities. This raises the question whether
one needs to entirely eliminate resumes or whether structured
sifting processes with competency and experience checklists
should be considered instead of blotting personal information?
For instance, some consider resumes to be ‘dead’ and have
moved to requiring anonymous work samples from applicants
(Feintzeig, 2016), which seems promising given the fact that work
samples mirror relevant future work behavior (Joseph, 2016).
Despite this promising approach, applicants’ socio-demographic
group characteristics (such as age) will always become apparent
at later stages of the hiring procedure. Therefore, any effect of
(partially) blind recruiting might be nullified if such procedures
do not safeguard against hiring bias in later assessment stages,
too.
Given these findings, as well as older raters’ slight tendency
to in-group bias, organizations may deploy a mix of strategies
to minimize ageism in hiring. Aside from screening and training
recruiters for diversity (e.g., in how to deal with/interpret explicit
and implicit stigmatizing cues in resume information), team-
based hiring consisting of a mixed age group of raters (e.g., both
older and younger recruiters) might counter age bias in screening
too. Furthermore, both initiatives may signal to job seekers that
the organization is committed to diversifying the workforce, at
least as regards age (Barber, 1998), thereby also affecting the
overall organizational image positively.
Conclusion
For a better understanding and averting hiring discrimination,
one needs to move beyond prevalence studies and investigate
determinants of hiring discrimination. Results of an experimental
study among HR professionals showed hiring discrimination
of older applicants based on implicit age cues in resumes
and may help understand mixed effects of anonymous resume
screening initiatives. Such findings may help organizational
decision makers to understand the complexity of fair hiring
and the effectiveness of interventions in order to limit age
discrimination upon organizational entry.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
ED designed, coordinated, helped collecting the data, and wrote
the study/paper. JD supervised research assistants and collected
data.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.
2017.01321/full#supplementary-material
REFERENCES
Abrams, D., Swift, H. J., and Drury, L. (2016). Old and unemployable? How age-
based stereotypes affect willingness to hire job candidates. J. Soc. Issues 72,
105–121. doi: 10.1111/josi.12158
Administration on Aging (2015). A Profile of Older Americans: 2015. Washington,
DC: Administration on Aging.
Aguinis, H., Michaelis, S. E., and Jones, N. M. (2005). Demand for certified human
resources professionals in internet-based job announcements. Int. J. Sel. Assess.
13, 160–171. doi: 10.1111/j.0965-075X.2005.00310.x
Ahmed, A. M., Andersson, L., and Hammarstedt, M. (2012). Does age matter for
employability? A field experiment on ageism in the Swedish labour market.
Appl. Econ. Lett. 19, 403–406. doi: 10.1080/13504851.2011.581199
Albert, R., Escot, L., and Fernández-Cornejo, J. A. (2011). A field experiment
to study sex and age discrimination in the Madrid labour market.
Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 22, 351–375. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2011.
540160
Apers, C., and Derous, E. (2017). Are they accurate? Recruiters’ personality
judgments in paper versus video resumes. Comput. Hum. Behav. 73, 9–19.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.063
Åslund, O., and Skans, O. N. (2012). Do anonymous job application procedures
level the playing field? ILR Rev. 65, 82–107. doi: 10.1177/00197939120650
0105
Axt, J. R., Ebersole, C. R., and Nosek, B. A. (2014). The rules of implicit
evaluation by race, religion, and age. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1804–1815. doi: 10.1177/
0956797614543801
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 12 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 13
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
Bal, A. C., Reiss, A. E. B., Rudolph, C. W., and Baltes, B. B. (2011). Examining
positive and negative perceptions of older workers: a meta-analysis. J. Gerontol.
B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66B, 687–698. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr056
Bangerter, A., Roulin, N., and König, C. J. (2012). Personnel selection as a signaling
game. J. Appl. Psychol. 97, 719–738. doi: 10.1037/a0026078
Barber, A. E. (1998). Recruiting Employees: Individual and Organizational
Perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Behaghel, L., Crépon, B., and Le Barbanchon, T. (2015). Unintended effects of
anonymous résumés. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 7, 1–27. doi: 10.1257/app.
20140185
Bennington, L., and Wein, R. (2002). Aiding and abetting employer discrimination:
the job applicant’s role. Empl. Responsib. Rights J. 14, 3–16. doi: 10.1023/A:
1015710311395
Berger, E. D. (2009). Managing age discrimination: an examination of the
techniques used when seeking employment. Gerontologist 49, 317–332.
doi: 10.1093/geront/gnp031
Bernerth, J. B., and Aguinis, H. (2016). A critical review and best-practice
recommendations for control variable usage. Pers. Psychol. 69, 229–283.
doi: 10.1111/peps.12103
Brewer, M. B. (2007). The importance of being we: human nature and intergroup
relations. Am. Psychol. 62, 728–738. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.8.728
Brtek, M. D., and Motowidlo, S. J. (2002). Effects of procedure and outcome
accountability on interview validity. J. Appl. Psychol. 87, 185–191. doi: 10.1037/
0021-9010.87.1.185
Buijsrogge, A., Derous, E., and Duyck, W. (2016). Often biased but rarely in doubt:
how initial reactions to stigmatized applicants affect interviewer confidence.
Hum. Perform. 29, 275–290. doi: 10.1080/08959285.2016.1165225
Burns, G. N., Christiansen, N. D., Morris, M. B., Periard, D. A., and Coaster, J. A.
(2014). Effects of applicant personality on resume evaluations. J. Bus. Psychol.
29, 573–591. doi: 10.1007/s10869-014- 9349-6
Cable, D. M., and Gilovich, T. (1998). Looked over or overlooked? Prescreening
decisions and postinterview evaluations. J. Appl. Psychol. 83, 501–508.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.501
Carter, N. T., and Highhouse, S. (2014). “You will be known by the company you
keep: understanding the social identity concerns of job seekers,” in The Oxford
Handbook of Recruitment, eds K. Y. T. Yu and D. M. Cable (New York, NY:
Oxford University Press), 454–462.
Christopher, A. N. (1998). The psychology of names: an empirical reexamination.
J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 1173–1195. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01673.x
Cole, M., Feild, H., Giles, W., and Harris, S. (2009). Recruiters’ inferences of
applicant personality based on resume screening: do paper people have a
personality? J. Bus. Psychol. 24, 5–18. doi: 10.1007/s10869-008-9086- 9
Cole, M., Rubin, R., Feild, H., and Giles, W. (2007). Recruiters’ perceptions and use
of applicant resume information: screening the recent graduate. Appl. Psychol.
Int. Rev. 56, 319–343. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00288.x
Cole, M. S., Feild, H. S., and Giles, W. F. (2003). Using recruiter assessments
of applicants’ resume content to predict applicant mental ability and big five
personalitydimensions. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 11, 78–88. doi: 10.1111/1468-2389.
00228
Connelly, B. L., Certo, S. T., Ireland, R. D., and Reutzel, C. R. (2011).
Signaling theory: a review and assessment. J. Manag. 37, 39–67. doi: 10.1177/
0149206310388419
Copus, D. (2005). “A lawyer’s view: avoiding junk science,” in Employment
Discrimination Litigation, ed. F. J. Landy (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass),
450–462.
Crisp, R. J., and Hewstone, M. (1999). Differential evaluation of crossed category
groups: patterns, processes, and reducing intergroup bias. Group Process.
Intergroup Relat. 2, 307–333. doi: 10.1177/1368430299024001
Derous, E., Nguyen, H.-H., and Ryan, A. M. (2009). Hiring discrimination against
Arab minorities: interactions between prejudice and job characteristics. Hum.
Perform. 22, 297–320. doi: 10.1080/08959280903120261
Derous, E., Pepermans, R., and Ryan, A. M. (2017). Ethnic discrimination during
resume screening: interactive effects of applicants’ ethnic salience with job
context. Hum. Relat. 70, 860–882. doi: 10.1177/0018726716676537
Derous, E., Ryan, A. M., and Nguyen, H. H. D. (2012). Multiple categorization
in resume screening: examining effects on hiring discrimination against Arab
applicants in field and lab settings. J. Organ. Behav. 33, 544–570. doi: 10.1002/
job.769
Derous, E., Ryan, A. M., and Serlie, A. W. (2015). Double jeopardy upon resumé
screening: when Achmed is less employable than Aïsha. Pers. Psychol. 68,
659–696. doi: 10.1111/peps.12078
Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions:
1989 and 1999. Psychol. Sci. 11, 315–319. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00262
Edin, P., and Lagerström, J. (2006). Blind Dates: Quasi-experimental Evidence on
Discrimination. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/78668
Fasbender, U., and Wang, M. (2017). Negative attitudes toward older workers
and Hiring decisions: testing the moderating role of decision makers’
core self-evaluations. Front. Psychol. 7:2057. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.
02057
Feintzeig, R. (2016). The Boss Doesn’t Want Your Resume. The Wall Street
Journal. Available at: https://www.wsj.com/articles/the-boss-doesnt-want-
your-resume-1452025908
Finkelstein, L. M., and Burke, M. J. (1998). Age stereotyping at work: the role of
rater and contextual factors on evaluations of job applicants. J. Gen. Psychol.
125, 317–345. doi: 10.1080/00221309809595341
Finkelstein, L. M., Ryan, K. M., and King, E. B. (2013). What do the young
(old) people think of me? Content and accuracy of age-based metastereotypes.
Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 22, 633–657. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.
673279
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed)
stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived
status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.82.6.878
Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., and Neuberg, S. (1999). “The continuum model, ten years
later,” in Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology, eds S. Chaiken and Y. Trope
(New York, NY: Guilford Press), 231–254.
Furtmueller, E., Wilderom, C., and Muller, R. (2010). Online Resumes: Optimizing
Design to Service Recruiters. Enschede: University of Twente.
Goldin, C., and Rouse, C. (2000). Orchestrating impartiality: the impact of “blind”
auditions on female musicians. Am. Econ. Rev. 90, 715–741. doi: 10.1257/aer.
90.4.715
Gordon, R. A., and Arvey, R. D. (2004). Age bias in laboratory and field settings:
a meta-analytic investigation1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34, 468–492. doi: 10.1111/j.
1559-1816.2004.tb02557.x
Guion, R. M. (2011). Assessment, Measurement, and Prediction for Personnel
Decisions. New York, NY: Routledge.
Hassell, B. L., and Perrewe, P. L. (1995). An examination of beliefs about older
workers: do stereotypes still exist? J. Organ. Behav. 16, 457–468. doi: 10.1002/
job.4030160506
Hiemstra, A. M. F., Derous, E., Serlie, A. W., and Born, M. P. (2013). Ethnicity
effects in graduates’ résumé content. Appl. Psychol. 62, 427–453. doi: 10.1111/j.
1464-0597.2012.00487.x
Hiscox, M. J., Oliver, T., Ridgway, M., Arcos-Holzinger, L., Warren, A., and
Willis, A. (2017). Going Blind to see more Clearly: Unconscious Bias in Australian
Public Service Shortlisting Processes. Available at: https://pmc.gov.au/sites/
default/files/publications/BETA-report-going- blind-to-see-moreclearly.pdf
Joseph, J. (2016). What Companies Use Blind/Anonymous Resumes and What
Benefits have they Reported? Available at: http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.
edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1108&context=student
Kaiser, C. R., and Pratt-Hyatt, J. S. (2009). Distributing prejudice unequally: do
whites direct their prejudice toward strongly identified minorities? J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 96, 432–445. doi: 10.1037/a0012877
Kaufmann, M. C., Krings, F., and Sczesny, S. (2016). Looking too old? How an
older age appearance reduces chances of being hired. Br. J. Manag. 27, 727–739.
doi: 10.1111/1467-8551.12125
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. Am. Psychol. 28, 107–128.
doi: 10.1037/h0034225
King, E. B., Mendoza, S. A., Madera, J. M., Hebl, M. R., and Knight, J. L. (2006).
What’s in a name? A multiracial investigation of the role of occupational
stereotypes in selection decisions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 1145–1159.
doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00035.x
Kite, M. E., Stockdale, G. D., Whitley, B. E., and Johnson, B. T. (2005). Attitudes
toward younger and older adults: an updated meta-analytic review. J. Soc. Issues
61, 241–266. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00404.x
Koenig, G., Trawinski, L., and Rix, S. (2015). The Long Road Back: Struggling to find
Work after Unemployment. Washington, DC: AARP.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 13 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 14
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
Krause, A., Rinne, U., and Zimmermann, K. F. (2012). Anonymous job applications
of fresh Ph.D. economists. Econ. Lett. 117, 441–444. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2012.
06.029
Krings, F., Sczesny, S., and Kluge, A. (2011). Stereotypical inferences as mediators
of age discrimination: the role of competence and warmth. Br. J. Manag. 22,
187–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2010.00721.x
Kulik, C. T., Perry, E. L., and Bourhis, A. (2000). Ironic evaluation processes: effects
of thought suppression on evaluations of older job applicants. Organ. Behav.
Hum. Decis. Process. 21, 689–711. doi: 10.1002/1099-1379(200009)21:6<689::
AID-JOB52>3.0.CO;2-W
Kulik, C. T., Roberson, L., and Perry, E. L. (2007). The multiple-category problem:
category activation and inhibition in the hiring process. Acad. Manag. Rev. 32,
529–548. doi: 10.5465/AMR.2007.24351855
Lahey, J. N. (2008). Age, women, and hiring. J. Hum. Resour. 43, 30–56.
doi: 10.3368/jhr.43.1.30
Landy, F. J. (2008). Stereotypes, bias, and personnel decisions: strange and stranger.
Ind. Organ. Psychol. 1, 379–392. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00071.x
Levy, B. B., and Banaji, M. R. (2002). “Implicit ageism,” in Ageism: Stereotyping and
Prejudice, ed. T. D. Nelson (Cambridge, MA: The MIT press), 49–76.
Lu, L., Kao, S.-F., and Hsieh, Y.-H. (2011). Attitudes towards older people and
managers’ intention to hire older workers: a Taiwanese study. Educ. Gerontol.
37, 835–853. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2010.485007
Marcus, J., and Fritzsche, B. A. (2014). One too many categories: an experimental
test on the effectiveness of a dual-identity recategorization intervention on
age-based bias. Curr. Psychol. 33, 578–599. doi: 10.1007/s12144-014-9230-9
Marcus, J., Fritzsche, B. A., Le, H., and Reeves, M. D. (2016). Validation of the work-
related age-based stereotypes (WAS) scale. J. Manag. Psychol. 31, 989–1004.
doi: 10.1108/JMP-11-2014- 0320
Marcus, J., and Sabuncu, N. (2016). “Old oxen cannot plow”: stereotype themes
of older adults in Turkish folklore. Gerontologist 56, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1093/
geront/gnv108
Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., and Leyens, J.-P. (1988). The “black sheep
effect”: extremity of judgments towards ingroup members as a function of
group identification. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 18, 1–16. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.24201
80102
Martens, A., Goldenberg, J. L., and Greenberg, J. (2005). A terror management
perspective on ageism. J. Soc. Issues 61, 223–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.
00403.x
Maurer, R. (2016). Blind Hiring may be Missing the Point. Available at:
https://www.shrm.org/resourcesandtools/hr-topics/talent-acquisition/pages/
blind-hiring-practices.aspx
Maurer, T. J., Barbeite, F. G., Weiss, E. M., and Lippstreu, M. (2008). New measures
of stereotypical beliefs about older workers’ ability and desire for development.
J. Manag. Psychol. 23, 395–418. doi: 10.1108/02683940810869024
Neumark, D., Burn, I., and Button, P. (2016). Experimental age discrimination
evidence and the Heckman critique. Am. Econ. Rev. 106, 303–308. doi: 10.1257/
aer.p20161008
Newman, D. A., and Lyon, J. S. (2009). Recruitment efforts to reduce adverse
impact: targeted recruiting for personality, cognitive ability, and diversity.
J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 298–317. doi: 10.1037/a0013472
North, M. S., and Fiske, S. T. (2015). Modern attitudes toward older adults in
the aging world: a cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 141, 993–1021.
doi: 10.1037/a0039469
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., and Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group
attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn. 6, 101–115.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101
OECD (2015). OECD Economic Surveys: Belgium 2015. Belgium: OECD Economic
Surveys. doi: 10.1787/eco_surveys- bel-2015-en
Paulhus, D. L. (1991). “Measurement and control of response bias,” in Measures of
Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes, eds J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver,
and L. S. Wrightsman (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 17–59. doi: 10.1016/
B978-0-12-590241- 0.50006-X
Perdue, C. W., Dovidio, J. F., Gurtman, M. B., and Tyler, R. B. (1990). Us and them:
social categorization and the process of intergroup bias. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59,
475–486. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.475
Perry, E. (1994). A prototype matching approach to understanding the role of
applicant gender and age in the evaluation of job applicants. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.
24, 1433–1473. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01558.x
Perry, E. L., Golom, F. D., Catenacci, L., Ingraham, M. E., Covais, E. M., and
Molina, J. J. (2016). Talkin’ ‘bout your generation: the impact of applicant age
and generation on hiring-related perceptions and outcomes. Work Aging Retire.
3, 186–199. doi: 10.1093/workar/waw029
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common
method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and
recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.
88.5.879
Popken, R. L. (1993). An empirical study of indirect speech acts in resumes. Engl.
Specif. Purposes 12, 239–249. doi: 10.1016/0889-4906(93)90004- 8
Rau, B. L., and Adams, G. A. (2014). “Recruiting older workers: realities and needs
of the future workforce,” in The Oxford Handbook of Recruitment, eds K. Y. Yu
and D. M. Cable (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 88–109.
Riach, P. A., and Rich, J. (2006). An Experimental Investigation of Age
Discrimination in the French Labor Market (IZA Discussion Papers, No. 2522).
Available at: http://nbnresolving.de/urn:nbn:de:101:1-2008050287
Riach, P. A., and Rich, J. (2010). An experimental investigation of age
discrimination in the English labor market. Ann. Econ. Stat. 9, 169–185.
doi: 10.2307/41219164
Richardson, B., Webb, J., Webber, L., and Smith, K. (2013). Age discrimination in
the evaluation of job applicants. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 35–44. doi: 10.1111/j.
1559-1816.2012.00979.x
Rosette, A. S., Akinola, M., and Ma, A. (2016). “Subtle discrimination in the
workplace,” in The Oxford Handbook of Workplace Discrimination, Vol. 1, eds
A. J. Colella and E. B. King (Oxford: Oxford University Press), doi: 10.1093/
oxfordhb/9780199363643.013.2
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999).
Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: flexibility and generality of
the implicit association test. Soc. Cogn. 17, 437–465. doi: 10.1521/soco.1999.17.
4.437
Ryan, A. M., Reeder, M. C., Golubovich, J., Grand, J., Inceoglu, I., Bartram, D.,
et al. (2017). Culture and testing practices: is the world flat? Appl. Psychol. 66,
434–467. doi: 10.1111/apps.12095
Rynes, S. L. (1991). “Recruitment, job choice, and post-hire consequences: a call
for new research directions,” in Handbook of Industrial and Organizational
Psychology, 2nd Edn, eds M. D. Dunnette and L. M. Hough (Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press), 399–444.
Schmidt, F. L., Oh, I.-S., and Shaffer, J. A. (2016). The Validity and
Utility of Selection Methods in Personnel Selection: Practical and
Theoretical Implications of 100 Years of Research Findings. Available
at: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Frank_Schmidt10/publication/
309203898_The_Validity_and_Utility_of_Selection_Methods_in_Personnel_
Psychology_Practical_and_Theoretical_Implications_of_100_Years_of_
Research_Findings/links/58051e2608aef87fbf3bbb52.pdf
Segrest Purkiss, S. L., Perrewé, P. L., Gillespie, T. L., Mayes, B. T., and Ferris,
G. R. (2006). Implicit sources of bias in employment interview judgments and
decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 101, 152–167. doi: 10.1016/j.
obhdp.2006.06.005
Self, W. T., Mitchell, G., Mellers, B. A., Tetlock, P. E., and Hildreth, J. A. D. (2015).
Balancing fairness and efficiency: the impact of identity-blind and identity-
conscious accountability on applicant screening. PLoS ONE 10:e0145208.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145208
Sidanius, J., and Veniegas, R. C. (2000). “Gender and race discrimination: the
interactive nature of disadvantage,” in Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination,
ed. S. Oskamp (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 47–69.
Sigurd, B., Eeg-Olofsson, M., and Ouren, J. (2005). Modeling the Changing
Popularity of Names. Available at: http://journals.lub.lu.se/index.
php/LWPL/article/view/2345
Slaughter, J. E., Zickar, M. J., Highhouse, S., and Mohr, D. C. (2004). Personality
trait inferences about organizations: development of a measure and assessment
of construct validity. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 85–103. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.
89.1.85
Spence, A. M. (1973). Job market signaling. Q. J. Econ. 87, 355–374. doi: 10.2307/
1882010
Spence, A. M. (1974). Market Signaling: Informational Transfer in Hiring and
Related Screening Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Stone-Romero, E. (2002). “The relative validity and usefulness of various
empirical research designs,” in Handbook of Research Methods in Industrial and
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 14 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321
fpsyg-08-01321 August 8, 2017 Time: 15:41 # 15
Derous and Decoster Age Cues in Resumes
Organizational Psychology, ed. S. G. Rogelberg (London: Blackwell Publishers),
77–98.
Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics. Boston, MA:
Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. (1979). “An integrative theory of intergroup conflict,” in
The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds W. Austin and S. Worchel
(Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole), 33–47.
Truxillo, D. M., Finkelstein, L. M., Pytlovany, A. C., and Jenkins, J. S. (2015).
“Age discrimination at work: a review of the research and recommendations
for the future,” in The Oxford Handbook of Workplace Discrimination, eds
A. J. Colella and E. B. King (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).
doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199363643.013.10
Uhlmann, E., Dasgupta, N., Elgueta, A., Greenwald, A. G., and Swanson, J.
(2002). Subgroup prejudice based on skin color among Hispanics in the United
States and Latin America. Soc. Cogn. 20, 198–226. doi: 10.1521/soco.20.3.198.
21104
Vieira Campos Proença, M. T., and Valente Dias de Oliveira, E. T. (2009). From
normative to tacit knowledge: CVs analysis in personnel selection. Empl. Relat.
31, 427–447. doi: 10.1108/01425450910965469
Wanberg, C. R., Kanfer, R., Hamann, D. J., and Zhang, Z. (2016). Age and
reemployment success after job loss: an integrative model and meta-analysis.
Psychol. Bull. 142, 400–426. doi: 10.1037/bul0000019
Waung, M., Hymes, R., Beatty, J. E., and McAuslan, P. (2015). Self-promotion
statements in video resumes: frequency, intensity, and gender effects on
job applicant evaluation. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 23, 345–360. doi: 10.1111/ijsa.
12119
Weinfurt, K. P. (1995). “Multivariate analysis of variance,” in Reading and
Understanding Multivariate Statistics, eds L. G. Grimm and P. R. Yarnold
(Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 245–267.
Wöhrmann, A. M., Fasbender, U., and Deller, J. (2016). Using work values
to predict post-retirement work intentions. Career Dev. Q. 64, 98–113.
doi: 10.1002/cdq.12044
Young, R. K., Kennedy, A. H., Newhouse, A., Browne, P., and Thiessen, D.
(1993). The effects of names on perception of intelligence, popularity, and
competence. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 23, 1770–1788. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.
tb01065.x
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2017 Derous and Decoster. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in
this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 15 August 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1321

Supplementary resource (1)

... In addition to the increment in organisational performance, several other benefits can be seen in blind hiring. It creates more choices for both employer and employee, and the blind hiring method greatly emphasizes an employee's skills, and there are no discriminations (Derous and Decoster, 2017). Hence, this allows suitable candidates to be selected and join organisations without any informal bias. ...
... Social media includes Facebook, Twitter, LinkedIn, YouTube, and flicker. Organizations should utilize well-managed social recruiting tactics (Derous and Decoster, 2017;Rathee and Bhuntel, 2017;Lievens and Chapman, 2019;Moseson, Kumar and Juusola, 2020). ...
... It's also important to hear the person's opinions from others. LinkedIn also lets users utilize recommendations to validate their abilities and certifications (Lohse et al., 2012;Derous and Decoster, 2017;Rathee and Bhuntel, 2017;Lievens and Chapman, 2019;Moseson et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The recruiting process is more effective by a realistic mindset matching the organization's goals and the applicants' demands. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effective practices of blind selection from the organizational perspective and to find the impact of blind selection in achieving organizational diversity. Study used a systematic review and explored the document using thematic analysis to aggregate the most common causes and consequences of proposing blind selection. According to the findings of the research, most individuals have favorable sentiments toward blind selection in organizational performance. However, a company's nature significantly impacts how this concept is adopted within the organization. Most of the literature emphasized that blind recruiting is characterized by varied approaches and perspectives on the problem, resulting in increased organizational effectiveness due to diversity.
... Moreover, when integrating multiple cues to form their judgments, individuals tend to linearly integrate available cues, especially in the presence of few cues [Karelaia & Hogarth, 2008] and when they consistently point into the same direction of a judgment [Slovic, 1966;Anderson, 1962]. Research in the context of hiring decisions supports the usefulness of a lens-model approach to investigate the integration of implicit as well as explicit identity cues into judgments [Derous & Decoster, 2017;Kleissner & Jahn, 2021]. For example, implicit (e.g., candidate name) and explicit (e.g., candidate age) cues resulted in similar discrimination of older job candidates in a hiring scenario presented to recruitment professionals [Kleissner & Jahn, 2021]. ...
... Moreover, in the presence of both implicit and explicit political identity cues, both cues jointly and independently shaped individuals' perception of the political orientation of the messenger. This supports evidence that individuals integrate small numbers of cues relating to the same unobserved criterion in an additive manner [Anderson, 1962;Slovic, 1966;Hurst & Stern, 2020;Voelkel & Willer, 2019;Kleissner & Jahn, 2021;Derous & Decoster, 2017]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bridging the political divide between liberals and conservatives is one of the biggest challenges in reaching broad public support of climate policies. Research has suggested that framing climate policies with respect to the past may reduce opposition by political conservatives, but recent attempts to replicate this effect have failed. A new perspective on these inconsistent findings may be offered by taking the influence of temporal framing on individuals' perception of the messenger into account. The present work investigated how implicit cues contained in temporal message framing as well as explicit political identity cues shape the perceived political orientation of a messenger and subsequent climate policy support by partisans. Across three experiments (Ntotal = 2012), we found that past (vs. future) framing and conservative (vs. liberal) party affiliation independently contributed to the messenger being perceived as more conservative. Past framing and conservative party affiliation increased endorsement of the messenger and the message by conservatives, but decreased endorsement by liberals. However, past framing and conservative party affiliation independently increased conservatives’ climate policy support, with mixed effects on liberals. Moreover, a temporal framing effect only emerged when messenger party affiliation was made explicit, suggesting that activating individuals' political identity facilitates the integration of implicit identity cues contained in temporal framings. We discuss the theoretical implications of our integrated account for the observation of partisan effects and reassess the potential of temporal framings to reduce the political divide on climate change.
... There are many different methodological approaches to measuring hiring discrimination in the lab and field (Bertrand & Duflo, 2017, Corell et al., 2013. One approach to assessing discrimination in the resume screening stage relies on experiments that simulate different hiring scenarios and let participants evaluate and rate hypothetical resumes to determine if unequal treatment occurs (for example, see: Agerström, 2014;Derous & Decoster, 2017;Krings et al., 2011). A critique of these experiments has questioned the ecological validity of the findings. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Hiring discrimination research has predominantly focused on labor market outcomes based on one or two group memberships, most commonly ethnicity and gender. The main aim of the doctoral dissertation is to explore warmth and competence perceptions associated with multiple demographic group membership and how they may affect evaluations and behavior in a workplace and hiring context. Study I relies on the stereotype content model (SCM, Fiske et al., 2002) to explore how employees perceive intersections of demographic groups on universal dimensions of social perception (warmth and competence). Namely, the intersection of ethnicity (Arab/Swedish), gender (male/female), age (30-year-old/55-year-old), and sexual orientation (gay/heterosexual). Results show that when stigmatized group categories are added to one another, this does not necessarily produce additive negative effects. Study II addressed perceptions of 22 common occupations in Sweden (e.g., Mechanic, Surgeon). Results show that warmth and competence can account for perceptions of the pre-selected occupations with a five-cluster solution found most descriptive of the data. Study III uses a modernized version of the “lost letter” technique (Milgram et al., 1965) and experimentally tests how ethnic discrimination is affected by signals of gender and sexual orientation. We sent out 6 654 emails as a job opportunity follow-up which ostensibly reached the wrong recipient, and measured the number of returned emails notifying the sender of the mistake. We find evidence of ethnic discrimination, with Arabs receiving fewer replies than Swedes. No discrimination based on sexual orientation or intersectional effects were found. Study IV had the main aim of testing whether matching stereotype content of job position and intersected group membership (gender and sexual orientation) in terms of warmth and competence affects the job suitability evaluations of job seekers. The results show no evidence of matching effects operating, as the gay job seekers, regardless of job position or gender, receive higher job suitability ratings than their straight counterparts. Further research should address whether there is an ongoing positive attitude change towards gay groups and whether the positive perceptions affect behavioral outcomes. I conclude that intersecting categories generates important knowledge on the perception and treatment of groups at work.
... One may be led to believe that age discrimination and biases can be hidden by concealing age cues, but research indicates the opposite effect. Derous and Decoster (2017) discovered that anonymous resume screening does not dissuade age-discriminatory effects. Using a mixed factorial design, 610 HR professionals showed hiring discrimination against older applicants based on implicit age cues in resumes. ...
Article
Full-text available
As populations age, an increasing number of workers beyond the traditional retirement age are opting to continue working. Nevertheless, discrimination against older job seekers seeking new employment opportunities remains widespread. To address this issue, we enlisted a pool of crowdworkers to assess the resumes of IT job candidates and guess each candidate's age, race, and gender. Using this crowdsourced data, we trained an AI model and applied bias correction techniques from IBM's AI 360 and Microsoft's Fairlearn toolkits to correct for biases based on race, gender, and age. We analyzed the effectiveness of these tools in mitigating different types of bias in job hiring algorithms, explored why age may be more challenging to eliminate than other forms of bias, and discussed additional approaches to enhance fairness. Our results indicate that implicit age bias, or ageism, is prevalent in hiring decisions and more pervasive than other well-documented forms of bias, such as race and gender biases.
... diverse world, while blind recruitment is viewed as a more scientific method because it uses the same assessment for every candidate. Moreover, blind recruitment eliminates unconscious bias (Derous & Decoster, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recruitment is a major activity of human resources management that decides the performance of the organization. One of the major drawbacks is that the selection of the candidate can be dependent on the demographics, appearance, and cultural differences of candidates. According to the literature review analysis, blind recruitment is linked to only a few studies, and a gap has been identified in knowledge areas. Thus, through this study, the researcher has reviewed the concept of blind recruitment. Then, the objectives of the study were to identify new themes in blind recruitment and to explore the blind recruitment concepts and to provide a critical review of blind recruitment. The author adopts the desk research approach by conducting an extensive literature review. The author, using systematic review, conducts the conduct of two or more research sources, each conducted rigorously and complete in itself, into one project. In the current study, desk research has been used, and the researcher has collected data from past research articles relevant to the topic. According to the results of the study, blind recruitment is a proper solution to minimize bias in the recruitment and selection process. Blind recruitment enables companies to guarantee they are selecting the best candidates for available vacant positions because they remove characteristics that may lead to bias. Furthermore, hidden demographic data, skill-based selection, avoidance of social media pre-screening, and anonymization of initial interviews are some of the key initial themes of blind recruitment identified, as well as criticism in the practical context of blind recruitment.
Article
Full-text available
Most developed societies are facing the challenge of an ageing population, which poses several issues, including low labour force participation and financial pressures on the sustainability of the pension system. Countries typically address this situation by raising the retirement age, which often results in dissatisfaction among workers as such decisions are perceived as imposed. Therefore, our work’s main purpose was to research the analysis of implementing measures for managing older workers in companies, which would indirectly influence workers’ decisions to voluntarily choose longer work participation.
Preprint
Full-text available
Background This study explores age discrimination's profound impact on job seekers aged 35+ in China. Despite their experience, they encounter unfair treatment and missed opportunities due to their age, resulting in stress and diminished mental well-being. This bias further erodes the motivation of experienced workers, ultimately hampering the efficiency of companies. The Chinese government's 35-year age limit for certain roles fuels ageism, reinforcing biased stereotypes. Methods This study investigates the influence of age discrimination on HR choices by employing mediators: perceived stereotypes (PS) and perceived age bias (PAB). The connections between these variables are analyzed using structural equation modeling (SEM), revealing the intricate impact of ageism on the dynamics of decision-making. Results The findings of this study reveal pivotal insights. Age discrimination exerts a significant influence on mental well-being (MWB) and shapes prejudiced hiring choices. Perceived stereotypes (PS) strongly impact recruitment decisions (DTH), alongside perceived age bias (PAB). Notably, these relationships exhibit substantial statistical significance (p < 0.01), some demonstrating even higher significance (p < 0.001). What is more, gender moderates these effects, shedding light on nuanced dynamics within age bias effects. Conclusion This inquiry uncovers harmful effects of age bias on job seekers aged 35+ in China. The unfair treatment arising from age-related factors generates stress, affecting mental well-being. Ageism permeates into the realm of hiring decisions, where stereotypes and bias wield influence over the choices made. The findings underscore the urgency of addressing age discrimination's repercussions on both individual well-being and organizational efficiency. The research enhances understanding of ageism's effects on individuals and workplaces in China.
Article
Age stereotyping at the workplace can lead to discrimination. We investigated stereotype influence on recall. In the who‐said‐what paradigm participants studied older and younger speakers making stereotypical and counterstereotypical statements about their work, followed by memory tests for the statements and speakers. Statements were more likely attributed to a speaker from the same age category than the other age category, replicating earlier findings. Stereotypicality had no effect on statement recognition but participants were more likely to guess a stereotypical speaker even though speakers made equal numbers of stereotypical and counterstereotypical statements (Experiments 1 and 3). When speakers made more counterstereotypical statements (Experiment 2) this bias was reduced but less so than the actual proportion of stereotypical statements warranted. Speaker judgments were also influenced by prior stereotypes. We conclude that participants used prior stereotypes to supplement recall from memory. Measures to reduce stereotypes should include procedures to improve memory accuracy.
Article
Age as an individual characteristic is ubiquitous in social science research because it has important associations with numerous outcomes of interest. Yet age is rarely treated as a phenomenon that requires explanation or theoretical attention. To advance research in sociology and beyond, we bring together previously siloed literatures on the conceptualization and measurement of age. Our framework presents age as a system of inequality that can be understood through concepts and processes that operate at multiple levels of analysis. At the individual level, we argue age is best conceptualized and operationalized as multidimensional. We review a range of measures, from birth cohort to physical appearance, that can be fruitfully combined in empirical research to account for this complexity. The multidimensionality of age also highlights how it is “done” in interactions, connecting the social construction of age to the intersectional production of inequality. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Sociology, Volume 49 is July 2023. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Chapter
Full-text available
Despite the laws that protect employee rights, discrimination still persists in the workplace. This chapter examines individual- level factors that may influence subtle discrimination in the workplace. More specifically, it examines how social categories tend to perpetuate the use of stereotypes and reviews contemporary theories of subtle prejudice and discrimination. In addition, the chapter divides discrimination in the workplace along two dimensions, gateways and pathways, and examines the extent to which stereotypes, prejudice, and social categorization processes influence subtle discrimination at these critical junctures in an individual's career. Finally, it considers the extent to which individual differences may influence a person's propensity toward prejudice and discrimination.
Article
Full-text available
Much has been speculated regarding the influence of cultural norms on the acceptance and use of personnel selection testing. This study examined the cross-level direct effects of four societal cultural variables (performance orientation, future orientation, uncertainty avoidance and tightness-looseness) on selection practices of organizations in 23 countries. 1,153 HR professionals responded to a survey regarding testing practices in hiring contexts. Overall, little evidence of a connection between cultural practices and selection practices emerged. Implications of these findings for personnel selection and cross-cultural research as well as directions for future work in this area are described.
Article
Full-text available
Organizational hiring practices have been charged for unfair treatment on the grounds of age. Drawing on theories of planned behavior and core self-evaluations, this research investigated the impact of negative attitudes toward older workers on hiring decisions and examined the moderating role of decision-makers’ core self-evaluations. We tested our hypotheses based on a structured online questionnaire and a vignette study using a sample of 102 participants working in human resource management across different industries. As predicted, negative attitudes toward older workers were positively related to avoidance of hiring older people, which in turn was negatively related to the likelihood to select the oldest candidate. Because hiring decisions are not only about the hiring subject but also about the decision-maker, we tested the moderating role of decision-makers’ core self-evaluations. Results showed that core self-evaluations buffered the relationship between negative attitudes toward older workers and avoidance of hiring older people. Theoretical implications of the findings with regard to hiring decisions about older people and practical recommendations to improve diversity management strategies and age-balanced hiring practices in organizations are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Systematic research considering job context as affecting ethnic discrimination in hiring is limited. Building on contemporary literature on social categorization and cognitive matching, the interactive effect of context characteristics (client contact; industry status) and person characteristics (i.e. ethnic cues: Maghreb/Arab vs Flemish-sounding name; dark vs light skin tone) were investigated using an experimental field study among 424 white majority HR professionals. Findings showed that equally qualified applicants with a dark skin tone received lower job suitability ratings than applicants with a light skin tone, particularly when they were screened for high client contact/low industry status positions and low client contact/high industry status positions. It is concluded that some ethnic cues (such as skin tone) may be more salient compared with other cues and that job context may influence the salience of ethnic cues and steer hiring discrimination in subtle ways. Implications of these findings for hiring discrimination research and organizations are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Two scenario-based studies were conducted to explore the relative impact of a job candidate's age compared to gener-ational membership on hiring-related perceptions and outcomes. Study 1 used paper stimulus materials and manipulated whether a job candidate was described as 60 years old, a Baby Boomer, 29 years old, or a Gen-Y/Millennial. Graduate student participants evaluated the job candidates across a number of traits and assessed their suitability for the job of Sales Director. Study 2 employed equivalent stimulus materials but was conducted 3 years later using an older and more age-diverse sample and an online data collection methodology. Consistent with hypotheses, there was strong evidence across the 2 studies that job candidates described as 60-year-olds were perceived as the least motivated and adaptable relative to the other job candidates. By contrast, compared to younger job candidates (Gen-Y/Millennial and 29-year-old), job candidates described as Baby Boomers were perceived to have similar levels of motivation across studies and similar levels of adaptability in Study 2. Baby Boomers were also perceived as significantly more motivated and adaptable than older applicants (60-year-old) in Study 2. Finally, both studies provided evidence of an indirect effect of applicant age and generation on hiring outcomes through perceptions of motivation, adaptability, and in Study 1, competence.
Article
Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (a) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (b) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (c) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (d) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth.
Article
Whether recruiters accurately infer personality from resumes is currently a topic of great interest in the light of technological advancements and new types of resume formats (like video resumes) that are increasingly being used. Based on predictions from the realistic accuracy model and the media richness theory, we investigated whether real recruiters (N = 296) are able to infer applicants’ Big Five personality characteristics more accurately from video and audio resumes than from less information-rich resume formats (like paper resumes) and whether applicants’ perceived attractiveness (physical/vocal) affects accuracy judgments. As expected and with the exception of extraversion, personality was not accurately judged from paper resumes. Interestingly, information-rich resume formats and applicants’ perceived attractiveness did not affect accuracy judgments. Despite recruiters’ stubborn reliance on their capacity to infer personality from resume information, study findings clearly showed that different types of resumes –also the new, more information-rich ones– are no valid tools to infer candidates’ personality from.