ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Many species of the sexually-deceptive genus Ophrys are characterized by insect-like flowers. Their form has been traditionally considered to play an important role in pollinator attraction and manipulation. Yet the evolution of the floral form remains insufficiently understood. We hypothesize that pollinator-mediated selection is essential for driving floral form evolution in Ophrys, but that form components are being subjected to varying selection pressures depending on their role in mediating interactions with pollinators. By using the Eucera pollinated Ophrys leochroma as a model, our aim has been to assess whether and in what manner pollination effectiveness is altered by experimental manipulation of the flower form. Our results show that the floral form plays an essential and, so far, underestimated role in ensuring effective pollination by mechanically guiding pollinators towards the reproductive structures of the flower. Pollinators are significantly less effective in interacting with flowers having forms altered to resemble those of species pollinated by different hymenopteran genera. Further, those components used by pollinators as gripping points were found to be more effective in ensuring pollinia transfer than those with which pollinators do not directly interact. Thus, mechanically-active and -inactive components appear to be under different selection pressures. As a consequence, mechanically-active components of the flower form could reflect adaptations to the interaction with particular pollinator groups, while inactive components can vary more freely. Disentangling selection patterns between the functionally different components of flower form may provide valuable insights into the mechanisms driving the morphological diversification of sexually-deceptive pollination-systems. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Looks matter: changes in flower form affect pollination
effectiveness in a sexually deceptive orchid
D. RAKOSY*,M.CUERVO,H.F.PAULUS*&M.AYASSE
*Integrative Zoology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Systematic and Evolutionary Botany, Faculty of Life Sciences, University Vienna, Vienna, Austr ia
Institute of Evolutionary Ecology and Conservation Genomics, University Ulm, Ulm, Germany
Keywords:
flower form;
orchids;
pollination effectiveness;
pollinator-mediated selection;
sexual deception.
Abstract
Many species of the sexually deceptive genus Ophrys are characterized by
insect-like flowers. Their form has been traditionally considered to play an
important role in pollinator attraction and manipulation. Yet, the evolution
of the floral form remains insufficiently understood. We hypothesize that
pollinator-mediated selection is essential for driving floral form evolution in
Ophrys, but that form components are being subjected to varying selection
pressures depending on their role in mediating interactions with pollinators.
By using the Eucera-pollinated Ophrys leochroma as a model, our aim has
been to assess whether and in what manner pollination effectiveness is
altered by experimental manipulation of the flower form. Our results show
that floral form plays an essential and, so far, underestimated role in ensur-
ing effective pollination by mechanically guiding pollinators towards the
reproductive structures of the flower. Pollinators are significantly less effec-
tive in interacting with flowers having forms altered to resemble those of
species pollinated by different hymenopteran genera. Further, those compo-
nents used by pollinators as gripping points were found to be more effective
in ensuring pollinia transfer than those with which pollinators do not
directly interact. Thus, mechanically active and inactive components appear
to be under different selection pressures. As a consequence, mechanically
active components of the flower form could reflect adaptations to the inter-
action with particular pollinator groups, whereas mechanically inactive com-
ponents can vary more freely. Disentangling selection patterns between the
functionally different components of flower form may provide valuable
insights into the mechanisms driving the morphological diversification of
sexually deceptive pollination systems.
Introduction
Angiosperm flowers are characterized by a remarkable
diversity of forms, some of the most bizarre being found
in sexually deceptive orchids (van Der Cingel, 2001a, b).
Sexual deception relies primarily on the chemical
mimicry of receptive female insects (e.g. Schiestl, 2005;
Ayasse et al., 2011; Bohman et al., 2016; Johnson &
Schiestl, 2016). However, many species are also charac-
terized by a morphologically complex and often insect-
like third petal, serving as a landing platform for polli-
nators (hereafter referred to as the lip). Although tradi-
tionally considered important, there is little empirical
evidence for the contribution of the insectiform lip to
pollinator attraction and pollination (Gaskett, 2011;
Johnson & Schiestl, 2016).
In principle, the form of the lip might act as a visual
signal to mate-searching males and also ensure the cor-
rect alignment between the body of the pollinator and
the reproductive parts of the plant (Kullenberg, 1961,
1973; Phillips et al., 2014; De Jager & Peakall, 2015;
G
ogler et al., 2015; Martel et al., 2016). Selection on the
lip form could be thus imposed through both the sen-
sory preferences of the pollinator and the mechanical
Correspondence: D. Rakosy, Integrative Zoology, Faculty of Life Sciences,
University Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria.
e-mail: demetra.rakosy@gmail.com
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL.
1
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
fit between the pollinator and the lip (De Jager & Pea-
kall, 2015). So far, empirical evidence provides stronger
support for flower form variation being shaped by
selection for an improved mechanical fit between the
lip and the pollinator’s body (Gaskett, 2012; G
ogler
et al., 2009, 2015; Phillips et al., 2013; De Jager & Pea-
kall, 2015; but see also Ellis & Johnson, 2010). By
selectively facilitating the mechanical fit to a particular
pollinator species, floral form was shown to reinforce
other traits in ensuring reproductive isolation (G
ogler
et al., 2009, 2015; De Jager & Peakall, 2015). However,
most sexually deceptive orchids rely solely on floral
scent for reproductive isolation and the adaptive signifi-
cance of floral form in these species remains little
understood (e.g. Johnson & Schiestl, 2016).
Considering the available evidence, pollinator-
mediated selection would be expected to mainly act by
stabilizing the floral form, leading to an optimized
mechanical fit between pollinators and flowers (Cress-
well, 1998; Gaskett, 2012). However, most orchids are
characterized by an imperfect match between their lip
and the body of the pollinator (or that of its female)
(Benitez-Vieyra et al., 2009; Gaskett, 2012; De Jager &
Peakall, 2015). This might suggest that selection on
flower form is either weak or absent. Alternatively,
varying selection pressures acting on different compo-
nents of the form of the lip could lead to the observed
incomplete correspondence between flowers and their
pollinators (van der Niet et al., 2010). The lip consti-
tutes a very complex structure, comprising several
shape and size components, only a fraction of which
may be involved in effectively guiding pollinators
towards the reproductive structures of the flowers (Pau-
lus, 2007; Gaskett, 2012). The mechanical fit between
pollinators and flowers may thus be mediated through
only a subset of the components making up the overall
form of the lip (e.g. the calli on the lip of the Australian
Chiloglottis or the lateral lobes on the lip of European
Ophrys species).
We hypothesize that components with which pollina-
tors functionally interact will be under strong pollina-
tor-mediated selection, whereas those with which
pollinators do not effectively interact are more likely to
be shaped by relaxed selection and other stochastic fac-
tors. Such a dichotomy is known to occur between ‘be-
haviourally active’ and ‘behaviourally inactive’
components of floral scent and has been essential for
understanding the evolution and diversification of sex-
ually deceptive orchids (e.g. Schiestl et al., 2000; Ayasse
et al., 2011). We thus propose to also differentiate
between mechanically active and inactive components
of floral form. We define mechanically active compo-
nents as those areas or points of the lip which pollina-
tors use as fixed gripping or contact points during
pollination, whereas mechanically inactive components
relate to areas or points on which the pollinators move
around freely or with which they do not come into
contact during pollination. Selection would thus be
expected to only optimize the mechanical fit between
the mechanically active components of the form of the
lip and the pollinator’s body. As far as we know, no
study has investigated whether mechanically active and
inactive components of the floral form can be subjected
to differential selection pressures. Moreover, we do not
know the way in which these potentially variable selec-
tion pressures shape the evolution of the overall flower
form in sexually deceptive orchids.
The Mediterranean orchid genus Ophrys is among the
most well-known examples for pollination through sex-
ual deception (Gaskett, 2011). Its pollinators, males of
various hymenopteran species, are lured to the flowers
primarily through the mimicry of the sexual phero-
mone of their females (Schiestl et al., 1999, 2000;
Ayasse et al., 2003, 2011; Schiestl, 2005). Na
ıve males
are usually unable to discriminate between a conspeci-
fic female and the orchid. This leads them to attempt to
copulate with the flowers, a process during which the
effective transfer of pollinia is ensured. The mimicry of
mate-recognition signals ensures that the relationship
between Ophrys species and their pollinators is generally
species specific (Paulus & Gack, 1990; Xu et al., 2011).
According to the most recent phylogeny of the genus,
Ophrys has diverged by two fundamental pollinator
shifts from wasps to solitary bees, followed by subse-
quent radiations to various species, mainly within the
solitary bee genera Eucera and Andrena (Breitkopf et al.,
2014). In this context, Ophrys flowers can be classified
into three form types, each attracting different pollina-
tor genera: wasp-type flowers (pollinators: Agrogorythes
and Dasyscolia species), tenthredinifera-type flowers (pol-
linators: Eucera and Andrena species) and fusca-type
flowers (pollinators: Andrena species) (Delforge, 2005;
Paulus, 2007) (Fig. 1). It thus appears that the evolu-
tion of novel floral form types in Ophrys is associated
with shifts between pollinator genera, whereas shifts
within genera have resulted in comparatively minor
form changes. Whether pollinators actually select for
particular lip forms in Ophrys has, however, never been
tested.
Because pseudocopulation is essential in order for
pollinia transfer to take place, a close mechanical fit
between Ophrys flowers and their male pollinators
would be expected. However, the genus is also notori-
ous for high intraspecific variation in the form of the
lip (e.g. Delforge, 2005). This pattern could be the con-
sequence of differential selection shaping a stronger fit
towards mechanically active components of the lip
form, leaving mechanically inactive components to vary
more freely. However, whether in Ophrys adaptive dif-
ferences actually occur between mechanically active
and inactive components of flower form remains
unclear. Knowledge of the differential selection pres-
sures acting on components of flower form may provide
valuable insights into the evolution and diversification
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
2D. RAKOSY ET AL.
of sexually deceptive flowers. In fact, distinguishing
between mechanically active and inactive components
of flower form may help to solve such conceptual con-
flicts as those between the observed high floral varia-
tion and the expected stabilizing pollinator-mediated
selection in specialized pollination systems (e.g. Cress-
well, 1998).
Here, we have used an experimental approach to
elucidate whether and in what manner pollinator-
mediated selection drives the evolution and diversifica-
tion of floral forms in Ophrys. By performing overall
manipulations of flower form and directed manipula-
tions of its mechanically active and inactive compo-
nents, our aim has been to assess 1) the way changes
in the overall floral form, corresponding to the three
floral types, affect the behaviour of pollinators as well
as the mechanical fit between the flowers and their
pollinators and 2) whether changes in the mechanically
active components of the flower form have a stronger
impact on the behaviour of the pollinator and the
mechanical fit than inactive components. Our study
confirms that the flower form in Ophrys is likely to be
under pollinator-mediated selection and that selection
pressures vary between mechanically active and inac-
tive components of flower form.
Materials and methods
Study species and sites
The orchid and its pollinator: Ophrys leochroma and
Eucera kullenbergi
Ophrys leochroma Delforge, 2005 is a well-defined spe-
cies, belonging to the O. tenthredinifera complex (Paulus
& Hirth, 2012). Its distribution range spans the islands
of the east Mediterranean basin (including Crete),
where it can be found flowering from the middle of
March to the beginning of April. It is pollinated
throughout its range exclusively by Eucera kullenbergi
males (Paulus & Hirth, 2012; Hirth & Paulus, 2016).
The long-horn bee E. kullenbergi is widespread in the
east Mediterranean basin, where it occurs in a wide
range of open habitats. Males emerge during the middle
of March, some days prior to their females. During the
following 23 weeks, they usually aggregate at and
between nesting sites where they patrol in search of
unmated females (Ayasse et al., 2001).
Morphologically, O. leochroma shows all the features
common for tenthredinifera-type flowers: square to
trapezoid form, distinct lateral lobes and a reflexed api-
cal appendix (Fig. 1). In O. tenthredinifera, a relative of
O. leochroma, histochemical and cytological analyses
have been used to identify the distribution of scent pro-
duction on the lip (Francisco & Ascensao, 2013). It was
thus shown that, as in most Ophrys species, scent is pro-
duced in the epidermal cells on the surface of the lip
and in the tissue within the apical appendix and along
a narrow portion of the margins of the lip (Vogel,
1962; Francisco & Ascensao, 2013). The sexual phero-
mone analogue of O. leochroma was thereby recently
shown to comprise a blend of aldehydes, alcohols, fatty
acids and hydrocarbons (Weber, 2012; Cuervo et al.,
2017).
Experimental sites
All experiments were performed at two neighbouring
sites near Neapoli, Crete (Fig. S1). At the first site,
about 4050 E. kullenbergi males patrol an area of three
or four square metres, whereas at the second site hun-
dreds of males patrol an area of about 40 square
metres. The orchid does not occur in these areas, so
males can be presumed to be naı
¨ve to its sexual phero-
mone analogue. The number of males and the size of
the swarming areas are ideal for performing pollination
experiments.
Flower form manipulation
Identification of mechanically active and inactive
components of floral form
In order to be able to generalize our results beyond our
model species, we have focused on the four major struc-
tures of the lip, common to most Ophrys species: the stig-
matic cavity, the lateral lobes, the distal-side lobes and
the appendix (Fig. 1). Mechanically active and inactive
components of the lip form were identified with the aid
of pollination experiments. We thereby photographically
recorded the behaviour of E. kullenbergi males during
pseudocopulation with individually presented flowers
picked from four O. leochroma populations (Fig. S1).
From the photographic series, only those frames were
analysed in which males could be seen removing polli-
nia. We thereby assessed the frequency with which
males used the four flower structures as fixed contact or
gripping points during 36 effective pseudocopulations (1
pseudocopulation/flower) (Video S3). In all cases, males
contacted the stigmatic cavity during pseudocopulations
(100%), whereas in 97.2% of the cases, they also relied
on the lateral lobes as gripping points. In contrast, males
moved around more freely on the distal-side lobes and
the appendix, contacting these structures only during
30.5% and 25% of the pseudocopulations. As a conse-
quence, we classified the lateral lobes and the stigmatic
cavity as mechanically active components and the distal-
side lobes and the appendix as mechanically inactive
(Fig. 1 a-d).
Manipulation of mechanically active and inactive
components of the labellum
We collected O. leochroma inflorescences with an equal
number of unpollinated flowers from two populations
in Crete and kept them in water during the duration of
the experiments (Fig. S1). Manipulations were per-
formed by removing certain areas of the lip. According
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 3
to the changes made, the manipulations were classified
into three groups: 1) nonmanipulated flowers (=con-
trol) (C); 2) flowers with overall changes in floral form
resembling a) the form of wasp-pollinated Ophrys spe-
cies (rectangular flowers as found in O. insectifera,
O. regisferdinandii, etc.) (WT) and b) the flower form
characteristic of the Andrena-pollinated Ophrys fusca
clade (FT); and 3) flowers with manipulations made to
a) areas of the lip that the males use as attachment
points to the flowers (mechanically active components
of flower form), namely the lateral lobes (WLL) and
the stigmatic lobe (WSL); b) areas with which males do
not closely interact (mechanically inactive compo-
nents), namely the appendix (WA) and the distal sides
(WDS) (Figs 1 and 2).
Experimental set-up
Behavioural response of pollinators
Experiments were conducted at the large E. kullenbergi
site during the last 2 weeks of March 2016 and 2017,
between 08:30 and 14:00, provided that the cloud
coverage did not exceed 50%. This corresponds to
the time frame and weather conditions in which
males are active. In 2016, manipulations to the
flower form were performed by removing parts of the
lip, the removed tissue being discarded. All inflores-
cences were replaced after 1 day. In 2017, we
repeated this protocol in order to assess whether the
results obtained are reproducible across years. In addi-
tion, we also performed experiments in which the
removed flower tissue was re-attached below the
flowers (out of reach of approaching males) with an
insect pin in order to supplement the total amount of
scent emitted by manipulated flowers (De Jager &
Peakall, 2015). In this case, each inflorescence was
replaced after 30 min.
In order to ensure that the picked inflorescences
remained attractive towards their pollinators, we per-
formed baiting experiments at the beginning of each
day. Inflorescences were only kept if they elicited pseu-
docopulations. Consequently, inflorescences that
proved to be attractive to the male pollinators were
assigned arbitrarily to the seven manipulation groups
and then tested in a random order. The testing location
was switched between every trial, the distance between
locations measuring 3-4 m. For each inflorescence, the
behaviour of five males was filmed with a Nikon Cool-
pix P330 camera. The behaviour of the males was
assessed from the video recordings and classified as
short landings (<1 s), long landings (>1 s) and pseudo-
copulations (see Videos S1S3).
In 2016, a total of 560 males and 28 inflorescences
were tested (4 inflorescences/manipulation). In 2017,
320 males and 28 inflorescences could be tested for
experiments without re-attached flower tissue (4 inflo-
rescences/manipulation) and 695 males and 49 inflores-
cences for experiments with re-attached flower tissue
(7 inflorescences/manipulation). The number of males
tested was limited in 2017 by poor weather conditions.
Duration of pseudocopulation
Although it is expected that longer pseudocopulation
times increase the number of massulae deposited on
the stigma, this hypothesis has so far not been tested
(De Jager & Peakall, 2015). In 2017, we therefore per-
formed hand and natural pollination experiments in
order to assess the relationship between the number of
massulae deposited and the duration of the pseudocop-
ulation. For hand pollinations, pollinia were removed
using a toothpick and applied by repeated movement to
the stigma of another plant for 5, 10, 20, 30 and 60 s,
respectively (corresponding to the most frequent pseu-
docopulation times observed in the experiments
(a) (b) (c) (d)
Fig. 1 Illustration of the three flower types in Ophrys: (a) Wasp-pollinated flowers O. insectifera (WT) are characterized by a very slender,
almost rectangular lip form; (b) Eucera-pollinated flowers, here Ophrys leochroma, are characterized by a squared or trapezoid lip form; (c)
Andrena-pollinated fusca-type flowers, here O. iricolor (FT), are characterized by a triangle-shaped or narrow trapezoid lip form; (d) Eucera
kullenbergi pseudocopulating on O. leochroma. Areas used by males as gripping points (marked with green arrows/dots) are similar in all
three flower types. Regions of the lip with which males interact less accurately are marked with red arrows/dots.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
4D. RAKOSY ET AL.
performed in 2016). For natural pollination experi-
ments, we chose the smaller E. kullenbergi population,
where a higher frequency of pollinia-carrying males
could be achieved. For this purpose, we allowed males
to remove pollinia from presented inflorescences. After
about 60 min, the flowers were offered again and the
duration of pseudocopulation of pollinia-carrying males
was recorded with a stopwatch. We measured the dura-
tion of pseudocopulations from when the males first
contacted the thecae of the pollinia until they left the
flower. The 10 hand-pollinated and the 13 naturally
pollinated flowers were individually marked and stored
in 70% ethanol. The number of deposited massulae
was then counted under a microscope (x20). Low sam-
ple sizes are due to the very fast habituation of males
in small populations.
In order to assess whether changes in floral form can
affect the duration of pseudocopulation, we measured
the duration of pseudocopulation in the control and the
manipulated flowers in both 2016 and 2017. Sample
sizes correspond to the number of pseudocopulations
reported in the behavioural experiments described
above.
Experiments designed to control for possible scent bias
Currently, there is no evidence which would suggest
that in Ophrys individual components of the floral scent
are produced in different regions of the lip. However,
manipulations performed by removing portions of the
lip are likely to bare the risk of altering the total
amount of scent produced by the flowers. Theoretically,
this might lead to a dose-dependent behavioural
response in males, which could bias the frequency and/
or duration of pseudocopulations on experimentally
altered flower forms. In the following, we describe
three scenarios that reflect the ways in which alter-
ations of the scent production during floral manipula-
tions could have biased our results and the steps taken
to control for each type of bias:
1) The reduction in the scent-producing floral tissue could
lead to less-effective long-distance attraction. This is
highly unlikely to have biased our results as we
performed our experiments directly in the swarming
areas of the males, thus circumventing the necessity
for long-distance attraction. Nevertheless, we have
addressed this issue by:
a comparing the average number of males which
approached control and manipulated flowers dur-
ing a 1-min time frame (i.e. overall attractivity). If
alterations of the scent-producing flower tissue
would have affected long-distance attraction, we
would expect the total number of attracted males
to decrease in relation to the amount of
tissue removed, thus following the pattern:
WT < WDSL < FT < WLL < WA < WSL C.
b comparing the overall attractivity between flow-
ers in which the removed flower tissue was
discarded and flowers in which this tissue was
re-attached. If removing floral tissue has biased
long-distance attraction, we would expect to
find an increase in the overall attractivity in
flowers in which the removed flower tissue was
re-attached.
2) The reduction in the scent-producing floral tissue could lead
to less-effective short-distance attraction and reduced pseu-
docopulation attempts. In this case, we would expect
that manipulations in which the greatest amount of
tissue was removed would show less frequent land-
ings and pseudocopulations than manipulations
where less of the total lip surface was affected. Our
results should thus reflect the following pattern:
WT<WDSL<FT<WLL<WA<WSLC. The same
results would be expected for the duration of pseudo-
copulations. Under these circumstances, we would
also predict that scent supplementation in manipu-
lated flowers would reduce or remove differences
between the manipulation groups.
3) There is no or only a negligible dose-dependent effect on
pollinator behaviour. In this case, we would expect no
significant difference in the number of approaches,
short and long landings between manipulation
groups. Instead, the number and duration of pseu-
docopulations would be lowest in manipulations
Fig. 2 Illustrations of control (C) and manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers: fusca type (FT); wasp type (WT); flower without lateral lobes
(WLL); flower without stigmatic lobe (WSL); flower without appendix (WA); flower without distal-side lobes (WDSL).
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 5
resembling other Ophrys floral types. Manipulations
in which mechanically active components were
removed would in turn be less effective in securing
pseudocopulations than those where mechanically
inactive components were removed: (WT and
FT) (WLL and WSL) < (WDSL and WA) C. Pol-
linators would also be expected to show a consistent
response between manipulated flowers with and
without scent supplementation.
Statistics
Behavioural responses to manipulation of the flower
form
The response of each male was treated as an indepen-
dent case during statistical testing. Although we cannot
exclude that some males visited more than one flower,
the large number of males and their ability to learn to
avoid flowers makes this event unlikely (Ayasse et al.,
2000). Differences between the observed and expected
number of responses between each manipulation group
were tested using a chi-square test of goodness of fit.
The observed frequency of responses in control flowers
was considered as the expected frequency when com-
paring manipulated to control flowers. For comparisons
between manipulation groups, the expected frequencies
were calculated from the observed frequencies of the
group to which the comparison was assessed. Because
of differences in the number of males in each treatment
group, expected frequencies were individually adjusted
to correspond to the number of males in each group.
Changes in the duration of pseudocopulation due to
manipulation of the flower form
We tested whether the duration of pseudocopulation
can predict the number of massulae deposited on the
stigma by means of a linear regression analysis. Further,
pairwise differences in the duration of the pseudocopu-
lation between groups were compared by means of
independent-sample t-tests.
Experiments designed to control for possible scent bias
The overall attractiveness of control and manipulated
flowers was compared using a KruskalWallis test. The
same procedure was used in 2017 to compare the over-
all attractiveness of flowers with and without re-
attached flower tissue.
All statistics were performed using SPSS 20.0.0,
whereas graphical outputs were created in SPSS and
Photoshop CS5 (version 12.0.4 x32).
Results
Our results showed that the experimental alteration of
the floral form had a significant effect on changes in
the frequency and duration of the responses elicited in
the male pollinators. This effect was unlikely to have
been biased by alterations of chemical cues induced by
the manipulation of the lip because:
1) No difference in the effectiveness of long-distance attrac-
tion was found. Instead, the overall attractivity of
control and manipulated flowers was statistically
the same, similar results being found also between
manipulated flowers (2016 without re-attached
flower tissue A : X
2
= 9.344, d.f. = 6, P= 0.155; 2017
without re-attached flower tissue: X
2
= 10.761, d.f. = 6,
P= 0.096; 2017 with re-attached flower tissue: X
2
=
3.422, d.f. = 6, P= 0.754). Scent supplementation did
not improve overall attractivity as no significant differ-
ences were found between experiments where the
removed flower tissue was discarded and those in which
the flower tissue was re-attached (2017: X
2
= 0.106,
d.f. = 6, P=0.744).
2) Short-distance attraction and pseudocopulation rates did
not change in relation to the amount of tissue removed:
We thus found that in manipulated flowers short
landings increased, whereas the number of long
landings remained relatively unchanged as pseudo-
copulations became less frequent. However, differ-
ences found in the frequency of landings and
pseudocopulations did not decrease proportionally
to the increase in the amount of tissue removed
within each manipulation group. Neither did the
duration of pseudocopulations (Table 1a, b). In addi-
tion, no difference in the repertoire of pollinator
behaviour was detected between flowers for which
we explicitly controlled for any potential effect on
floral scent, and those we did not (Tables 2a, b and
3a, b).
3) Our results are consistent with the third hypothesis,
suggesting that floral manipulations had no or only
a negligible dose-dependent effect on floral scent
production. Changes in the behaviour of the polli-
nators can be thus attributed to the alteration of the
floral form.
Frequency of landings and pseudocopulations
The response pattern of E. kullenbergi males to alter-
ations of the flower form in O. leochroma was found to
remain consistent between years and experimental set-
ups (Tables 13, Fig. 3ac). Manipulated flowers thus
usually elicited significantly more landings and less
pseudocopulations than the control (Tables 13,
Fig. 3ac). Although the frequency of landings was
relatively similar between manipulated flowers, we
found that the frequency of pseudocopulations varies
according to the type of manipulation (Tables 13,
Fig. 3ac). Manipulations to the overall flower form
thereby elicited the lowest number of pseudocopula-
tions, followed by manipulations of mechanically
active components of the flower form. Manipulation
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
6D. RAKOSY ET AL.
Table 1 Experiments without re-attached flower tissue (2016): a) Comparisons between the frequency of behavioural responses in male Eucera kullenbergi towards the control and
manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers: statistical values for chi-square test of goodness of fit (lower left half); b) pairwise comparisons of the amount of time males spent
pseudocopulating with the control and manipulated orchid flowers: statistical values for independent-sample t-test (upper right half).
b) Duration of pseudocopulation
C FT WT WLL WSL WA WDSL
Ct=6.04 t=6.77 t=4.7 t=3.07 t=2.11 t=-2.00
d.f. =52.3, P=0.0000001** d.f. =48.43, P=0.0000001** d.f. =55.36, P=0.000018** d.f. =53.03, P=0.002* d.f. =78.24, P=0.019* d.f. =65.22, P=0.025*
FT X
2
=66.49, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001** t=-1.39 t=2.52 t=2.71 t=4.57 t=3.7
SL
r
=16.2 LL
r
=17 K
r
=33.2 d.f. =12, P=0.05 d.f. =34, P=0.009* d.f. =16.79, P=0.008* d.f. =38.24, P=0.000049** d.f. =26.41, P=0.0005**
WT X
2
=159.44, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=24.23, d.f. =2,
P=0.000005** t=2.40 t=3.54 t=5.55 t=4.45
SL
r
=40 LL
r
=0K
r
=-40 SL
r
=15 LL
r
=-8 K
r
=7 d.f. =28, P=0.012* d.f. =14.72, P=0.002* d.f. =33.69, P=0.000003** d.f. =23.89, P=0.00017**
WLL X
2
=19.68, d.f. =2,
P=0.000053**
X
2
=10.19, d.f. =2,
P=0.006*
X
2
=51.73, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001** t=1.13 t=2.84 t=2.28
SL
r
=12 LL
r
=5K
r
=17 SL
r
=-8 LL
r
=4K
r
=12 SL
r
=28 LL
r
=5K
r
=23 d.f. =18.17, P=0.135 d.f. =42.25, P=0.004* d.f. =28.14, P=0.02*
WSL X
2
=51.93, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=1.21, d.f. =2,
P=0.55
X
2
=46.36, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=10.02, d.f. =2,
P=0.007* t=1.07 t=0.93
SL
r
=18 LL
r
=14 K
r
=32 SL
r
=5LL
r
=3K
r
=2SL
r
=24 LL
r
=13 K
r
=11 SL
r
=6LL
r
=8K
r
=14 d.f. =46, P=0.145 d.f. =37, P=0.09
WA X
2
=28.57, d.f. =2,
P=0.00001**
X
2
=11.51, d.f. =2,
P=0.03*
X
2
=42.93, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=0.96, d.f. =2,
P=0.62
X
2
=12.61, d.f. =2,
P=0.02* t=0.07
SL
r
=18 LL
r
=1K
r
=19 SL
r
=5.7 LL
r
=7.7 K
r
=13.3 SL
r
=25 LL
r
=1K
r
=24 SL
r
=3LL
r
=4K
r
=1SL
r
=3LL
r
=12 K
r
=15 d.f. =55, P=0.25
WDSL X
2
=27.33, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=7.16, d.f. =2,
P=0.028*
X
2
=112.72, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=0.69, d.f. =2,
P=0.71
X
2
=6.02, d.f. =2,
P=0.0049
X
2
=1.14, d.f. =2,
P=0.57
SL
r
=15 LL
r
=5K
r
=20 SL
r
=6LL
r
=4K
r
=10 SL
r
=25 LL
r
=5K
r
=20 SL
r
=3LL
r
=0K
r
=3SL
r
=2LL
r
=8K
r
=10 SL
r
=0LL
r
=4K
r
=4
a) Behavioural responses
*P<0.05 and **P<0.001. C, control flowers; FT, fusca-type flowers; WT, wasp-type flowers; WLL, flowers without lateral lobes; WSL, flowers without stigmatic lobe; WA, flowers
without appendix; WDSL, flowers without distal-side lobes; SL, short landings; LL, long landings; K, pseudocopulations.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 7
Table 2 Experiments without re-attached floral tissue (2017): a) Comparisons between the frequency of behavioural responses in male Eucera kullenbergi towards the control and
manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers: statistical values for chi-square test of goodness of fit (lower left half); b) pairwise comparisons of the amount of time males spent
pseudocopulating with the control and manipulated orchid flowers: statistical values for MannWhitney U-test (upper right half).
b) Duration of pseudocopulation
C FT WT WLL WSL WA WDSL
Ct=2.27 t=10.58 t=3.1 t=2.14 t=0.46 t=1.02
d.f. =40, P=0.015* d.f. =19.2 P=0.000001** d.f. =43, P=0.015* d.f. =39, P=0.019* d.f. =57, P=0.32 d.f. =53, P=0.155*
FT X
2
=19.64, d.f. =2,
P=0.000054** t=3.13 t=0.31 t=0.08 t=1.84 t=1.96
SL
r
=5LL
r
=10 K
r
=15 d.f. =9, P=0.006* d.f. =10.45, P=0.38 d.f. =7, P=0.47 d.f. =25, P=0.039* d.f. =21, P=0.032*
WT X
2
=20.46, d.f. =2,
P=0.000036
X
2
=0.52, d.f. =2,
P=0.77 t=2.37 t=2.91 t=8.28 t=6.38
SL
r
=6LL
r
=9K
r
=15 SL
r
=1LL
r
=2K
r
=1 d.f. =8.61, P=0.022* d.f. =8, P=0.01* d.f. =23.46,
P=0.000001**
d.f. =22,
P=0.000002**
WLL X
2
=15.74, d.f. =2,
P=0.00038**
X
2
=2.41, d.f. =2,
P=0.3
X
2
=1.35, d.f. =2,
P=0.51 t=0.21 t=2.48 t=2.06
SL
r
=4LL
r
=10 K
r
=14 SL
r
=2LL
r
=1K
r
=3SL
r
=2LL
r
=0K
r
=2 d.f. =7.75, P=0.42 d.f. =28, P=0.01* d.f. =9.42,
P=0.034*
WSL X
2
=31.73, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=3.02, d.f. =2,
P=0.22
X
2
=5.96, d.f. =2,
P=0.048*
X
2
=4.57, d.f. =2,
P=0.10 t=1.76 t=1.91
SL
r
=3LL
r
=18 K
r
=21 SL
r
=3LL
r
=4K
r
=2SL
r
=4LL
r
=6K
r
=3SL
r
=1LL
r
=6K
r
=4 d.f. =24, P=0.046* d.f. =20, P=0.036*
WA X
2
=5.11, d.f. =2,
P=0.078
X
2
=10.61, d.f. =2,
P=0.049*
X
2
=10.41, d.f. =2,
P=0.006*
X
2
=8.51, d.f. =2,
P=0.014*
X
2
=67.65, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001** t=0.49
SL
r
=4LL
r
=3K
r
=7 SLr =-2 LL
r
=-8 K
r
=10 SL
r
=3LL
r
=7K
r
=10 SL
r
=0LL
r
=8K
r
=9SL
r
=1LL
r
=-19 K
r
=17 d.f. =38, P=0.315
WDSL X
2
=2.848, d.f. =2,
P=0.24
X
2
=10.13, d.f. =2,
P=0.006*
X
2
=10.15, d.f. =2,
P=0.006*
X
2
=6.83, d.f. =2,
P=0.033*
X
2
=19.08, d.f. =2,
P=0.00007**
X
2
=0.95, d.f. =2,
P=0.62
SL
r
=2LL
r
=4K
r
=6SL
r
=3LL
r
=7K
r
=-10 SL
r
=4LL
r
=5K
r
=10 SL
r
=2LL
r
=6K
r
=9SL
r
=1LL
r
=14 K
r
=15 SL
r
=2LL
r
=2K
r
=0
a) Behavioural responses
*P<0.05 and **P<0.001. C, control flowers; FT, fusca-type flowers; WT, wasp-type flowers; WLL, flowers without lateral lobes; WSL, flowers without stigmatic lobe; WA, flowers
without appendix; WDSL, flowers without distal-side lobes; SL, short landings; LL, long landings; K, pseudocopulations.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
8D. RAKOSY ET AL.
Table 3 Experiments with re-attached flower tissue (2017): a) Comparisons between the frequency of behavioural responses in male Eucera kullenbergi towards the control and
manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers: statistical values for chi-square test of goodness of fit (lower left half); b) pairwise comparisons of the amount of time males spent
pseudocopulating with the control and manipulated orchid flowers: statistical values for MannWhitney U-test (upper right half).
b) Duration of pseudocopulation
C FT WT WLL WSL WA WDSL
Ct=5.26 t=12.1 t=1.10 t=4.20 t=0.181 t=1.79
d.f. =45, P=0.000004** d.f. =42.8, P=0.000001** d.f. =48, P=0.14 d.f. =48, P=0.000114** d.f. =52, P=0.43 d.f. =62, P=0.039*
FT X
2
=53.43, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001** t=1.07 t=2.79 t=1.29 t=4.87 t=3.01
SL
r
=10 LL
r
=20 K
r
=-30 d.f. =10, P=0.155 d.f. =21, P=0.006* d.f. =21, P=0.11 d.f. =35, P=0.00002** d.f. =35, P=0.003*
WT X
2
=105.08, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=10.11, d.f. =2,
P=0.006 t=4.37 t=3.01 t=9.96 t=6.42
SL
r
=19 LL
r
=14 K
r
=33 SL
r
=10 LL
r
=5K
r
=5 d.f. =12.69, P=0.005* d.f. =13.54, P=0.005* d.f. =30.41, P=0.000001 d.f. =29.41, P=0.000001**
WLL X
2
=34.81, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=2.37, d.f. =2,
P=0.31
X
2
=24.25, d.f. =2,
P=0.000005** t=1.98 t=1.09 t=0.25
SL
r
=7LL
r
=18 K
r
=25 SL
r
=3LL
r
=1K
r
=4SL
r
=13 LL
r
=4K
r
=9 d.f. =24, P=0.03* d.f. =38, P=0.14 d.f. =38, P=0.4
WSL X
2
=47.01, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=0.69, d.f. =2,
P=0.71
X
2
=19.41, d.f. =2,
P=0.00006**
X
2
=0.36, d.f. =2,
P=0.84 t=3.92 t=2.07
SL
r
=9LL
r
=20 K
r
=29 SL
r
=2LL
r
=0K
r
=2SL
r
=12 LL
r
=4K
r
=8SL
r
=1LL
r
=1K
r
=2 d.f. =38, P=0.00018** d.f. =38, P=0.023*
WA X
2
=10.23, d.f. =2,
P=0.006*
X
2
=41.71, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=106.44, d.f. =2,
P=0.00001**
X
2
=13.82, d.f. =2,
P=0.001**
X
2
=16.51, d.f. =2,
P=0.0003** t=1.72
SL
r
=6LL
r
=4K
r
=10 SLr =3LL
r
=15 K
r
=18 SL
r
=12 LL
r
=10 K
r
=22 SL
r
=1LL
r
=14 K
r
=15 SL
r
=2LL
r
=15 K
r
=17 d.f. =52, P=0.046*
WDSL X
2
=10.77, d.f. =2,
P=0.005*
X
2
=31.13, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001
X
2
=89.28, d.f. =2,
P=0.000001**
X
2
=10.51, d.f. =2,
P=0.005*
X
2
=17.91, d.f. =2,
P=0.00013**
X
2
=5.31, d.f. =2,
P=0.07
SL
r
=2LL
r
=13 K
r
=15 SL
r
=9LL
r
=7K
r
=16 SL
r
=19 LL
r
=2K
r
=21 SL
r
=5LL
r
=6K
r
=11 SL
r
=7LL
r
=-7 K
r
=14 SL
r
=52 LL
r
=9K
r
=3
a) Behavioural responses
*P<0.05 and **P<0.001. C, nonmanipulated flowers; FT, fusca-type flowers; WT, wasp-type flowers; WLL, flowers without lateral lobes; WSL, flowers without stigmatic lobe; WA,
flowers without appendix; WDSL, flowers without distal-side lobes; SL, short landings; LL, long landings; K, pseudocopulations.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 9
of mechanically inactive components elicited the high-
est number of pseudocopulations among the manipu-
lation types (Tables 13, Fig. 3ac). Differences within
manipulation groups were less pronounced. Wasp-type
flowers (WT) generally elicited significantly less pseu-
docopulations than fusca-type flowers (FT). Flowers
with removed lateral lobes (WLL) usually elicited
pseudocopulation as frequently as flowers without the
stigmatic lobe (WSL), whereas flowers without distal-
side lobes (WDSL) were as successful as flowers with-
out the appendix (WA). The only major difference
was found in the number of long landings, the fre-
quency of males landing without pseudocopulating on
the lips increasing in 2017. However, this increase was
observed in experiments with and without re-attached
floral tissue.
Duration of pseudocopulations
The simple linear regression analysis we performed
revealed that the number of massulae deposited on the
stigma can be predicted based on the duration of the
pseudocopulation. We thereby found a significant
regression equation for both the hand-pollinated flow-
ers (R
2
=0.376, F
(1,18)
=10.854, P=0.004) and flowers
pollinated by E. kullenbergi males (R
2
=0.604,
F
(1,13)
=19.864, P=0.001) (Fig. 4a, b). For hand polli-
nation, the number of massulae deposited was
Fig. 3 Male Eucera kullenbergi behavioural response to control and manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers. I control flowers (C); II
overall manipulation of flower form: fusca-type (FT) and wasp-type (WT) flowers; III manipulation of mechanically active components of
floral form: flowers without lateral lobes (WLL) and without the stigmatic lobe (WSL); IV manipulation of mechanically inactive
components of flower form: flowers without appendix (WA) and without the distal-side lobes (WDSL). The responses of the males were
classified as short landings, long landings and pseudocopulations. Significance values are for overall comparisons between groups. Each
letter designates a manipulation group, whereas different cases indicate significant differences between the number of responses in each
behavioural group. (a) Experiments conducted in 2016 without scent supplementation (i.e. without re-attaching the removed flower parts
to the manipulated flowers). (b) Experiments conducted in 2017 without scent supplementation (i.e. without re-attaching the removed
flower parts to the manipulated flowers). (c) Experiments conducted in 2017 with scent supplementation (i.e. by re-attaching the removed
flower parts to the manipulated flowers).
Fig. 4 Simple linear regression analysis showing that the number of massulae deposited during one pseudocopulation event can be
predicted from the duration of the pseudocopulation: the number of massulae increases when pseudocopulations last longer. (a) Hand
pollination experiments; (b) natural pollination experiments.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
10 D. RAKOSY ET AL.
predicted to increase by 0.364 with each second of
pseudocopulation, whereas for flowers pollinated by
E. kullenbergi males, the number of massulae was pre-
dicted to increase by 0.26 massulae with each second of
the pseudocopulation.
The time that males spent pseudocopulating with the
flowers varied significantly depending on the type of
manipulation (KruskalWallis, 2016: X
2
=37.32,
d.f. =6, P=0.000002; 2017 with re-attached flower
tissue: X
2
=41.57, d.f. =6, P=0.000001; 2017 with
re-attached flower tissue: X
2
=25.1, d.f. =6,
P=0.000328). These results were found to be mostly
consistent between years and experiments with and
without re-attached flower tissue (Fig. 5ac). Manipu-
lated flowers thereby elicited significantly shorter pseu-
docopulations than the control (P<0.05; Tables 13,
Fig. 5ac). The strongest decrease in pseudocopulation
time in comparison with control flowers was observed
in fusca- and wasp-type flowers (Fig. 5ac). The dura-
tion of pseudocopulation in groups in which mechani-
cally active components were manipulated usually
showed significantly shorter pseudocopulation times
than in groups in which mechanically inactive compo-
nents were manipulated. Pairwise differences within
these two groups were, however, not significant
(Tables 13, Fig. 5ac). Thus, pollinators spent similar
amounts of time pseudocopulating with flowers with
removed lateral lobes and flowers in which the stig-
matic lobe was removed. The same was true for flowers
in which the appendix had been removed and those
without distal-side lobes. The only difference was found
between the duration of pseudocopulation in flowers
without lateral lobes (WLL), which showed an increase
in the duration of pseudocopulations in experiments in
which the removed flower tissue was re-attached.
Discussion
The striking visual and morphological resemblance
between many sexually deceptive orchids and their
insect models has traditionally been assumed to have
been shaped by pollinator-mediated selection (Agren
et al., 1984; Paulus, 2007; Gaskett, 2012). Several
recent experimental studies conducted across cases of
pollinator sharing among sexually deceptive orchids
have supported this assumption by demonstrating the
plausibility of pollinator-mediated selection (De Jager &
Peakall, 2015; G
ogler et al., 2015). Our study extends
this work in a different dimension by experimentally
manipulating floral morphology within a species. We
could thus show that in O. leochroma, the form of the
lip is essential for ensuring the contact between the
pollinators and the reproductive structures of the flow-
ers. However, the effectiveness of pollen transfer varied
significantly depending on which components of floral
form were manipulated. We could thus provide the first
empirical evidence showing that mechanically active
components of flower form are under stronger pollina-
tor selection than mechanically inactive components.
This finding sheds new light into the way pollinator-
mediated selection may have driven floral form evolu-
tion in Ophrys.
The role of flower form in mediating the interactions
between orchids of the genus Ophrys and their
pollinators
The sexual pheromone analogue produced by sexually
deceptive orchids is essential for ensuring species-speci-
fic pollinator attraction and for eliciting copulatory
behaviour in male pollinators (Ayasse et al., 2011;
Fig. 5 Mean duration of pseudocopulations on control and manipulated Ophrys leochroma flowers. I control flowers (C); II overall
manipulation of flower form: fusca-type (FT) and wasp-type (WT) flowers; III manipulation of mechanically active components of floral
form: flowers without lateral lobes (WLL) and without the stigmatic lobe (WSL); IV manipulation of mechanically inactive components
of flower form: flowers without appendix (WA) and without the distal-side lobes (WDSL). Each letter designates a manipulation group,
whereas different cases indicate significant differences between the duration of the pseudocopulations in each behavioural group. (a)
Experiments conducted in 2016 without scent supplementation (i.e. without re-attaching the removed flower parts to the manipulated
flowers). (b) Experiments conducted in 2017 without scent supplementation (i.e. without re-attaching the removed flower parts to the
manipulated flowers). (c) Experiments conducted in 2017 with scent supplementation (i.e. by re-attaching the removed flower parts to the
manipulated flowers).
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 11
Peakall & Whitehead, 2014). However, the effectiveness
of pollen transfer depends on whether males are able to
contact the reproductive parts of flowers during pseu-
docopulation (e.g. Nilsson, 1992). Flower form could
affect the frequency of pollen transfer by mediating pol-
linator attraction and/or by ensuring the mechanical fit
between the body of the pollinator and the flower
(Cresswell, 1998; Benitez-Vieyra et al., 2009; Gaskett,
2012; De Jager & Peakall, 2015; G
ogler et al., 2015). In
turn, the amount of pollen deposited is likely to depend
on the accuracy of the mechanical fit and the time
males spend attempting to copulate with the flowers
(Cresswell, 1998; De Jager & Peakall, 2015).
Despite the primary role of the sexual pheromone
analogue for mediating the interaction between sexu-
ally deceptive orchids of the genus Ophrys and their
pollinators, our study suggests that floral form plays a
so far underestimated role in ensuring pollinia removal
and deposition. We found that in O. leochroma the
manipulation of the form of the lip did not reduce the
overall attractivity of the flowers towards their pollina-
tors. Instead, most approaching males landed on the
O. leochroma flowers but only started pseudocopulating
when they found proper gripping points on the lip. In
manipulated flowers, this happened less frequently and
pseudocopulations were significantly shorter than in
control flowers. Consequently, the flower form seems
to mediate efficient pollen transfer in Ophrys predomi-
nantly by ensuring the mechanical fit between the
flowers and their pollinators rather than by providing a
visual signal to approaching males. This result is consis-
tent with other recent experimental studies that investi-
gated the function of floral form in a diverse spectrum
of sexually deceptive systems (G
ogler et al., 2015; Phil-
lips et al., 2013; Phillips et al., 2014; De Jager & Peakall,
2015). Further studies are, however, needed to clarify
the contribution of floral form to visual attraction in
sexually deceptive systems, a contribution that may
strongly vary between species (Benitez-Vieyra et al.,
2009; De Jager & Peakall, 2015).
All studies investigating the function of floral form in
sexually deceptive orchids face one major challenge:
altering floral form without affecting the emission of the
sexual pheromone analogue (De Jager & Peakall, 2015).
For instance in Ophrys where the lip is the main scent-
producing organ, alterations of the form of the lip could
also result in changes in the amount of scent produced
(Francisco & Ascensao, 2013). However, in O. leochroma,
we did not find any conclusive evidence for scent bias
altering the response of males towards the manipulated
flowers. A possible explanation might lie in the fact that
Ophrys flowers produce very high amounts of the sexual
pheromone analogue, often higher than the amount of
sex pheromone in females (Ayasse et al., 2003; Schiestl,
2004). Therefore, even manipulated flowers with
strongly reduced lips can apparently still emit enough
scent to be highly attractive to their male pollinators.
We thus conclude that the differences in the beha-
vioural responses of males towards control and manipu-
lated O. leochroma flowers were mostly due to
alterations of the flower form. This suggests that the
manipulated flowers are less adaptive than the control,
the altered floral form leading to less efficient pollen
transfer and reduced reproductive success.
Flower form and reproductive success: evidence for
pollinator-mediated selection
In sexually deceptive orchids, it is expected that pseu-
docopulations are associated with the removal of one
or both pollinia, whereas successful massulae deposition
requires males to spend a certain period of time pseu-
docopulating (Paulus, 2007). The frequency of pseudo-
copulations could thus be considered a good proxy of
male reproductive success, whereas the duration of
pseudocopulations could act as a proxy of female repro-
ductive success (De Jager & Peakall, 2015). Field obser-
vations have shown that if males align with the
reproductive structures of Ophrys flowers, about 90% of
pseudocopulations are associated with pollinia removals
(H. Paulus, D. Rakosy pers. obs.). By performing both
hand and natural pollination experiments, we could
now show that the number of pollen grains deposited
on the stigma during a single pollination event
increases significantly with the time males spent pseu-
docopulating. The number of pollen grains deposited on
control flowers of O. leochroma was thereby found to be
sufficient to fertilize all available ovules (Paulus, 1988;
Nazarov & Gerlach, 1997). Although more data are
needed, we consider that alterations in the lip form can
not only reduce the frequency of successful pollinia
removals, but will also affect the number of ovules
being fertilized.
Extreme variations in the lip form may therefore
greatly reduce reproductive success (Ayasse et al., 2000;
Schiestl & Ayasse, 2001; Paulus, 2007). One would thus
expect that pollinator-mediated stabilizing selection has
shaped the evolution of species-specific flower form
optima, leading to an increased frequency of accurate
pollinia removal and higher pollen loads on the stigma
(e.g. Cresswell, 1998; Gaskett, 2012; Joly et al., 2016).
The evolution of floral form types in Ophrys
The three major floral types associated with particular
pollinator genera may represent such form optima, each
floral type being adapted to maximize the mechanical fit
between the flowers and their specific pollinators. Our
results show that changes in floral form types produced
the strongest reduction in terms of both the frequency
and the duration of pseudocopulations. These overall
changes in floral form were aimed at resembling the
forms of two relatives of the study group, namely those
pollinated by wasp species and Andrena bees, respectively
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
12 D. RAKOSY ET AL.
(Breitkopf et al., 2014). These species rely on highly
different sexual pheromone analogues for pollinator
attraction, but they also present strikingly different mor-
phologies (Ayasse et al., 2003; St
okl et al., 2005). Our
experiments show that E. kullenbergi males copulated less
frequently (and if they did, they held on to the flowers
for only a short time) with flowers having forms corre-
sponding to species pollinated by either wasps or Andrena
bees, even though the flowers emitted the specific scent
of their females. The reduced frequency of pseudocopu-
lations and their short duration indicate that, on flowers
manipulated to resemble other floral types, form compo-
nents were missing that the pollinators would normally
use as gripping and/or contact points, thus reducing the
mechanical fit.
If particular pollinator groups differ in the efficiency
with which they interact with floral types, then selection
would be expected to lead to an increased accuracy in
the mechanical fit and a more effective pollinia transfer
by the specialized pollinator (e.g. Cresswell, 1998). If
morphological and behavioural differences between the
pollinators are large enough, such adaptations could in
time lead to morphological differentiation even between
closely related species (Sedeek et al., 2014). A recent
study has found that in Ophrys species sharing a common
pollinator, morphological differences can maintain
reproductive isolation (G
ogler et al., 2015). It appears,
however, unlikely that if the floral scent barrier between
Ophrys species, which rely on the sexual pheromone ana-
logue for reproductive isolation, would break down,
morphological differences alone would be able to filter
out unspecific visitors (Cortis et al., 2009; Johnson &
Schiestl, 2016). Instead, our results suggest that flower
form acts to maximize reproductive success in a system
which is severely pollinator limited (Tremblay et al.,
2005). In most species, floral form is thus likely to com-
plement the function of floral scent, by increasing the
probability that the specific pollinator can ensure effi-
cient pollen transfer. The functional interaction between
floral scent and other floral traits, such as form and col-
our, in ensuring successful pollen transfer will, however,
require some additional attention.
Pollinator-mediated selection and the evolution of
mechanically active and inactive components of
floral form
Strong pollinator-mediated selection is not only
expected to lead to the evolution of floral forms
adapted to specific pollinator groups, but to also reduce
the amount of intraspecific variation (e.g. Cresswell
et al., 1998). However, the genus Ophrys is notorious
for high intraspecific variation in its floral form (Del-
forge, 2005); this has been traditionally interpreted as
either the outcome of hybridization or relaxed selec-
tion (Soliva & Widmer, 2003; Tremblay et al., 2005;
Devey et al., 2008). Alternatively, pollinators may be
able to select for particular lip forms, but the strength
of selection may vary between the various components
of the lip. This would leave mechanically inactive
components to be more variable than mechanically
active ones. Although the concept of classifying flower
form components into mechanically active and inactive
has been previously discussed, we here provide the
first evidence for the potential of different selection
pressures on these two components of the lip form
(Gaskett, 2012).
In O. leochroma, we have classified the stigmatic and
the lateral lobes as mechanically active and the appen-
dix and the distal-side lobes as mechanically inactive.
According to our expectations, differences in the
response frequency and duration of pseudocopulations
were relatively clear cut between the two trait compo-
nents. We thus found that changes in the mechanically
active components of floral form reduced the frequency
and duration of pseudocopulations more strongly than
changes in mechanically inactive components. The
presence of mechanically active components seems thus
essential for ensuring effective pollen transfer. It can
therefore be concluded that mechanically active traits
are likely to be under stronger selection pressure by
pollinators than mechanically inactive traits.
Weaker selection on particular lip form components
probably results in higher variation. Another possibility
is that mechanically inactive components are shaped by
other selective mechanisms such as selection for visual
display. High variation in particular traits might also be
an adaptation to reduce avoidance learning in males, a
mechanism that was first shown in relation to variation
in the sexual pheromone analogue (Ayasse et al., 2000,
2011; St
okl et al., 2005). Variation may also be a conse-
quence of varying selection pressures across years due
to changing preferences in the males. However, our
results were highly consistent over a 2-year period and
no evidence was found for a change in the preference
of males for particular flower manipulations. Thus, the
evolution of the complex and highly variable floral
form in Ophrys is likely to be driven by differential
selection pressures acting on the two form components.
The mechanically active and inactive components
identified in O. leochroma can be expected to be also
found in most other Ophrys species. We thus consider
that the evolution of variously shaped lateral lobes (e.g.
short in O. tenthredinifera vs. extremely long in
O. scolopax) and stigmatic lobes (e.g. O. tenthredinifera vs.
O. sphegodes) has been driven by pollinator-mediated
selection for improved mechanical fit. The mechanically
inactive components are in turn much more conserved.
While occurring in most Ophrys species, both the lateral
lobes and the appendix show little differentiation
among species, but high variation within species. Thus,
the position of the lateral lobes can range from spread
to completely folded beneath the lip, whereas the
appendix can be triangular to trapezoid, contacting the
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 13
lip or bent to various degrees. This suggests that in most
Ophrys species, variation in these two structures is likely
to be strongly influenced by stochastic factors. How-
ever, further experiments, preferably conducted with
several pollinator species, are necessary to confirm the
adaptive function of the various components of floral
form throughout Ophrys.
Outlook
Mechanically active and inactive components of floral
form can be found not only in the genus Ophrys, but also
in other sexually deceptive orchids and other flowers
that mechanically guide their pollinators towards their
reproductive parts (e.g. flowers with spurs, trap flowers)
(Whittall & Hodges, 2007; Phillips et al., 2014). Failure to
distinguish between these traits in selection studies will
limit our further understanding of floral evolution. With
the rise of three-dimensional (3D) geometric morpho-
metrics and 3D printing, new pathways are opening up
allowing the finer manipulation of both components of
floral form and the integration of this variation with that
in other traits, such as colour and scent. This would
enable the better quantification of the amount of selec-
tion pressure on various traits and trait components and
provide a better understanding of the relative role of pol-
linator-mediated selection and stochastic factors on the
evolution of flower phenotypes.
Acknowledgments
We thank Y. Staedler for his help in planning the
experiments, E. Rakosy-Tican for proofreading the
manuscript, T. Jones for improving our English and two
anonymous reviewers for their substantial contribution
to improving previous versions of the manuscript. The
research has been supported by funds provided by the
Vienna Orchid Society and the Institute of Evolutionary
Ecology and Conservation Genomics, University Ulm.
Collection permits were granted by the Ministry of
Reconstruction of Production, Environment and Energy,
Directorate General for the Protection and Development
of Forests and the Rural Environment in Athens
(Greece) (permission number 121894/490).
Conflict of interest
The authors do not have any conflict of interest to
declare.
References
Agren, L., Kullenberg, B. & Sensenbauch, T. 1984. Congru-
ences in pilosity between three species of Ophrys (Orchi-
daceae) in their hymenopteran pollinators. Nova Acta Reg.
Soc. Sci. Upsaliensis 5:1525.
Ayasse, M., Schiestl, F.P., Paulus, H.F., L
ofstedt, C., Hansson,
B., Ibarra, F. et al. 2000. Evolution of reproductive strategies
in sexually deceptive orchid Ophrys sphegodes: how does
flower-specific variation of odor signals influence reproduc-
tive success? Evolution 54: 19952006.
Ayasse, M., Paxton, R.J. & Teng
o, J. 2001. Mating behaviour
and chemical communication in the order Hymenoptera.
Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46:3178.
Ayasse, M., Schiestl, F.P., Paulus, H.F., Ibarra, F. & Francke,
W. 2003. Pollinator attraction in a sexually deceptive orchid
by means of unconventional chemicals. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
Biol. Sci. 270: 517522.
Ayasse, M., St
okl, J. & Francke, W. 2011. Chemical ecology
and pollinator-driven speciation in sexually deceptive orch-
ids. Phytochemistry 72: 16671677.
Benitez-Vieyra, S., Medina, A.M. & Cocucci, A.A. 2009. Vari-
able selection patterns on the labellum shape of Geoblasta
pennicillata, a sexually deceptive orchid. J. Evol. Biol. 22:
23542362.
Bohman, B., Flematti, G.R., Barrow, R.A., Pichersky, E. & Pea-
kall, R. 2016. Pollination by sexual deceptionIt takes
chemistry to work. Curr. Opin. Plant Biol. 32:3746.
Breitkopf, H., Onstein, R.E., Cafasso, D., Schl
uter, P. M. &
Cozzolino, S. 2014. Multiple shifts to different pollinators
fuelled rapid diversification in sexually deceptive Ophrys
orchids. New Phytol. 270: 377389.
Cortis, P., Vereecken, N.J., Schiestl, F.P., Barone Lumaga,
M.R., Scrugli, A. & Cozzolino, S. 2009. Pollinator conver-
gence and the nature of species’ boundaries in sympatric
Sardinian Ophrys (Orchidaceae). Ann. Bot. 104: 497506.
Cresswell, J.E. 1998. Stabilizing selection and the structural
variability of flowers within species. Ann. Bot. 81: 463473.
Cuervo, M., Rakosy, D., Martel, C., Schultz, S. & Ayasse, M.
2017. Sexual Deception in the Eucera-pollinated Ophrys leo-
chroma: a chemical intermediate between wasp- and
Andrena-pollinated species. J. Chem. Ecol. 43: 469479.
De Jager, M.L. & Peakall, R. 2015. Does morphology matter?
An explicit assessment of floral morphology in sexual decep-
tion. Funct. Ecol. 30: 537546.
Delforge, P. 2005. Orchids of Europe, North Africa and the Middle
East. A&C Black, London. 640 p. ISBN 071367525X.
van Der Cingel, N. 2001a. An atlas of Orchid Pollination: Euro-
pean Orchids. A. A. Balkema, Rotterdam.
van Der Cingel, N. 2001b. Atlas of Orchid Pollination: America,
Africa, Asia and Australia. A.A. Balkema, Rotterdam.
Devey, D.S., Bateman, R.M., Fay, M.F. & Hawkins, J.A. 2008.
Friends or relatives? Phylogenetics and species delimitation
in the controversial European orchid genus Ophrys.Ann. Bot.
101: 385402.
Ellis, A.G. & Johnson, S.D. 2010. Floral mimicry enhances pol-
len export: the evolution of pollination by sexual deceit out-
side of the Orchidaceae. Am. Nat. 176: 143151.
Francisco, A. & Ascensao, L. 2013. Structure of the osmophore
and labellum micromorphology in the sexually deceptive
orchids Ophrys bombyliflora and Ophrys tenthredinifera (Orchi-
daceae). Int. J. Plant Sci. 174: 619636.
Gaskett, A.C. 2011. Orchid pollination by sexual deception:
pollinator perspectives. Biol. Rev. 86:3375.
Gaskett, A.C. 2012. Floral shape mimicry and variation in sex-
ually deceptive orchids with a shared pollinator. Biol. J. Linn.
Soc. 106: 469481.
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
14 D. RAKOSY ET AL.
G
ogler, J., St
okl, J., Sramkova, A., Twele, R., Francke, W.,
Cozzolino, S. et al. 2009. M
enage
A TroisTwo endemic
species of deceptive orchids and one pollinator species. Evo-
lution 63: 22222234.
G
ogler, J., St
okl, J., Cortis, P., Beyrle, H., Lumaga, M.R.B., Coz-
zolino, S. et al. 2015. Increased divergence in floral morphol-
ogy strongly reduces gene flow in sympatric sexually deceptive
orchids with the same pollinator. Evol. Ecol. 29: 703717.
Hirth, M. & Paulus, H.F. 2016. Neue Best
auer-Beobachtungen
in der Ophrys tenthredinifera-Artengruppe der
Ag
ais mit der
Beschreibung von Ophrys lychnitis aus Paros. J. Eur. Orch. 48:
346388.
Johnson, S.D. & Schiestl, F.P. 2016. Floral Mimicry. Oxford
University Press, Oxford. ISBN 9780198732709.
Joly, S., Lambert, F., Alexandre, H., L
eveill
e-Bourret,
E. &
Clark, J.L. 2016. Pollinator specialization imposes stronger
evolutionary constraints on flower shape. bioRxiv, 041533.
Kullenberg, B. 1961. Studies in Ophrys pollination. Zoologiska
bidrag fran Uppsala 34:1340.
Kullenberg, B. 1973. New observations on the pollination of
Ophrys L. (Orchidaceae). Zoon, suppl 1:915.
Martel, C., Cairampoma, L., Stauffer, F.W. & Ayasse, M. 2016.
Telipogon peruvianus (Orchidaceae) flowers elicit pre-mating
behaviour in Eudejeania (Tachinideae) males for pollination.
PloS One 11: e0165896.
Nazarov, V.V. & Gerlach, G. 1997. The potential seed produc-
tivity of orchid flowers and peculiarities of their pollination
systems. Lindleyana 12: 188204.
van der Niet, T., Zollikofer, C.P.E., Le
on, M.S.P.D., Johnson,
S.D. & Linder, H.P. 2010. Three-dimensional geometric mor-
phometrics for studying floral shape variation. Trends Plant
Sci. 15: 423426.
Nilsson, L.A. 1992. Orchid pollination biology. Trends Ecol. Evol.
7: 255259.
Paulus, H.F. 1988. Beobachtungen und Experimente zur Pseu-
dokopulation auf Ophrys Arten (Orchidaceae) Kretas 2: mit
einer Beschreibung von Ophrys sitiaca HF Paulus & CA Alib-
ertis nov. spec. aus dem Ophrys fusca-omegaifera-Formenkreis.
Mitteilungsblatt des Arbeitskreises Heimische Orchideen Baden-
Wurttemberg 20: 817882.
Paulus, H.F. 2007. Wie Insekten-M
annchen von Orchi-
deenbl
uten get
auscht werden - Best
aubungstricks und Evo-
lution in der mediterranen Ragwurzgattung Ophrys. In:
Evolution - Ph
anomen Leben, vol. 20 (F. Gussenleitner, G.
Aubrecht, E. Aescht & M. Pfosser, eds), pp. 255294. Biolo-
giezentrum der Ober
osterreichischen Landesmuseen,
Denisia.
Paulus, H.F. & Gack, C. 1990. Pollinators as prepollinating iso-
lation factors: evolution and speciation in Ophrys (Orchi-
daceae). Isr. J. Bot. 39:4379.
Paulus, H.F. & Hirth, M. 2012. Best
aubungsbiologie und Sys-
tematik der Ophrys tenthredinifera-Artengruppe in der
Ost
ag
ais (Orchidaceae, Insecta). J. Eur. Orch. 44: 625686.
Peakall, R. & Whitehead, M.R. 2014. Floral odour chemistry
defines species boundaries and underpins strong reproduc-
tive isolation in sexually deceptive orchids. Ann. Bot. 113:
341355.
Phillips, R.D., Tingbao, Xu, Hutchinson, M.F., Dixon, K.W. &
Peakall, R. 2013. Convergent specializationThe sharing of
pollinators by sympatric genera of sexually-deceptive orch-
ids. J. Ecol. 101: 826835.
Phillips, R.D., Scaccabarozzi, D., Retter, B.A., Hayes, C.,
Brown, G.R., Dixon, K.W. et al. 2014. Caught in the act: pol-
lination of sexually deceptive trap-flowers by fungus gnats
in Ptersostylis (Orchidaceae). Ann. Bot. 113: 629641.
Schiestl, F.P. 2004. Floral evolution and pollinator mate choice
in a sexually deceptive orchid. J. Evol. Biol. 17:6775.
Schiestl, F.P. 2005. On the success of a swindle: pollination by
deception in orchids. Naturwissenschaften 92: 255264.
Schiestl, F.P. & Ayasse, M. 2001. Post-pollination emission of a
repellent compound in a sexually deceptive orchid: a new
mechanism for maximising reproductive success? Oecologia
126: 531534.
Schiestl, F.P., Ayasse, M., Paulus, H.F., Lofstedt, C., Hansson,
B.S., Ibarra, F. et al. 1999. Orchid pollination by sexual
swindle. Nature 399: 421.
Schiestl, F.P., Ayasse, M., Paulus, H.F., Lofstedt, C., Hansson,
B.S., Ibarra, F. et al. 2000. Sex pheromone mimicry in the
early spider orchid (Ophrys sphegodes): patterns of hydrocar-
bons as the key mechanism for pollination by sexual decep-
tion. J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 186: 567
574.
Sedeek, K.E.M., Scopece, G., Staedler, Y.M., Sch
onenberger,
J., Cozzolino, S., Schiestl, F.P. et al. 2014. Genic rather
than genome-wide differences between sexually deceptive
Ophrys orchids with different pollinators. Mol. Ecol. 23:
61926205.
Soliva, M. & Widmer, A. 2003. Gene flow across species
boundaries in sympatric, sexually deceptive Ophrys (Orchi-
daceae) species. Evolution 57: 22522261.
St
okl, J., Paulus, H., Dafni, A., Schulz, C., Francke, W. &
Ayasse, M. 2005. Pollinator attracting odour signals in sexu-
ally deceptive orchids of the Ophrys fusca group. Plant Syst.
Evol. 254: 105120.
Tremblay, R.L., Ackerman, J.D., Zimmerman, J.K. & Calvo,
R.N. 2005. Variation in sexual reproduction in orchids and
its evolutionary consequences: a spasmodic journey to diver-
sification. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 84:154.
Vogel, S. 1962. Duftdr
usen im Dienste der Best
aubung.
Abhandlungen der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen
Klasse, Akademie der Wissenschaften und der Literatur in
Mainz 10: 599763.
Weber, E. 2012. Pollinator attraction in long-horned bee polli-
nated Ophrys species on Crete. Master thesis. Ulm University.
Whittall, J.B. & Hodges, S.A. 2007. Pollinator shifts drive
increasingly long nectar spurs in columbine flowers. Nature
447: 706709.
Xu, S., Schl
uter, P.M., Scopece, G., Breitkopf, H., Gross, K.,
Cozzolino, S. et al. 2011. Floral isolation is the main repro-
ductive barrier among closely related sexually deceptive
orchids. Evolution 65: 26062620.
Supporting information
Additional Supporting Information may be found
online in the supporting information tab for this article:
Figure S1 Map of all the sampled O. leochroma popula-
tions (pink circles from left to right: Prina, Kritsa, Schi-
nokapsala and Orino) and the site at which
experiments were conducted with E. kullenbergi males
(black: Neapoli)
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Flower form affects pollination effectiveness 15
Video S1 Video recording of males approaching a con-
trol flower without landing or pseudocopulating
Video S2 Video recording of a male landing on a con-
trol flower without pseudocopulating
Video S3 Video recording of a male pseudocopulating
and removing pollinia on a control flower while
another male attempts to push him away from the per-
spective female
Data deposited at Dryad: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.q7p5j
Received 15 November 2016; revised 23 July 2017; accepted 29 July
2017
ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY. J. EVOL. BIOL. doi: 10.1111/jeb.13153
JOURNAL OF EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY ª2017 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
16 D. RAKOSY ET AL.
... Given the weakness or non-existence of postmating barriers, these orchids rely on the robust prepollination reproductive isolation provided by their floral scents and labella to prevent hybridisation [23][24][25][26]28]. Besides floral scent-which has a primary significance for both pollinator attraction and reproductive isolation in sexually deceptive orchids-morphological features of the flowers such as shape, colour, size, and texture provide visual and tactile cues to male pollinators, playing a decisive role in effectively turning attraction into pollination [11,16,18,[30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37]. Therefore, characterising the key floral traits involved in the attraction of specific pollinators is crucial to an understanding of the process of pollinator-driven speciation [28,29,[36][37][38][39]. ...
... Besides floral scent-which has a primary significance for both pollinator attraction and reproductive isolation in sexually deceptive orchids-morphological features of the flowers such as shape, colour, size, and texture provide visual and tactile cues to male pollinators, playing a decisive role in effectively turning attraction into pollination [11,16,18,[30][31][32][33][34][35][36][37]. Therefore, characterising the key floral traits involved in the attraction of specific pollinators is crucial to an understanding of the process of pollinator-driven speciation [28,29,[36][37][38][39]. ...
... Both the submarginal hairiness and the unique configuration of the stigmatic cavity and basal labellum region of O. speculum should provide tactile stimuli to male wasps, similarly to those offered by the hairiness of the female, hence guiding males to the correct position upon the labellum so that effective pollination could take place [11,37,41,43,60]. The importance of tactile cues for the success of pseudocopulation in Ophrys was also inferred from certain micromorphological details on the labellum of other species [42,52,54,60] and has long been tested in the field through choice experiments with their respective male insect pollinators [11,37,46,83]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Pollination by sexual deception specifically attracts male insects, through the floral scent and particular morphological features of the flower that serve as visual and tactile stimuli. The unique bond between the Ophrys speculum orchid and the male Dasyscolia ciliata wasp primarily stems from a few distinctive semiochemicals that mimic the female wasp’s sex pheromone, although the floral scent comprises a variety of compounds. An osmophore producing highly volatile compounds has been documented in four close relatives of O. speculum and is now being also investigated in this species. Given the existing debates regarding the structure of the labellum and stigmatic cavity in O. speculum, this study details their micromorphology. Additionally, comparisons of O. speculum flowers and female D. ciliata wasps under stereomicroscopy and scanning electron microscopy are conducted to seek new evidence of visual and tactile mimicry. The findings confirm that (i) an osmophore is present at the apical margin of the labellum in O. speculum flowers; (ii) the labellum features a distinct basal field homologous to those found in other Ophrys species; and (iii) the basal labellum region closely mimics the female wasp’s thorax and wings. The implications of these novel floral features are discussed within an evolutionary context.
... However, our results suggest that using morphometric characters for taxonomic purposes is relevant in the section Pseudophrys and emphasize the potential role of orchid enthusiasts in providing valuable data for taxonomic research (Véla et al., 2015). As in the case of molecular markers, more informative morphometric or chemical markers could be developed using more sophisticated techniques, such as geometric morphometrics (Rakosy et al., 2017;Gibert et al., 2022). More importantly, it would be interesting to distinguish between selected (i.e., functionally significant) and neutral phenotypic traits through electrophysiological and/or behavioral studies (Schiestl et al., 1999;Rakosy et al., 2017). ...
... As in the case of molecular markers, more informative morphometric or chemical markers could be developed using more sophisticated techniques, such as geometric morphometrics (Rakosy et al., 2017;Gibert et al., 2022). More importantly, it would be interesting to distinguish between selected (i.e., functionally significant) and neutral phenotypic traits through electrophysiological and/or behavioral studies (Schiestl et al., 1999;Rakosy et al., 2017). The distinction between biologically active and non-active floral scents, in particular, would likely provide further insights into the taxonomy of the section Pseudophrys (Stökl et al., 2005(Stökl et al., , 2009, as shown in the section Euophrys (e.g., Mant et al., 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Accurate species delimitation is critical for biodiversity conservation. Integrative taxonomy has been advocated for a long time, yet tools allowing true integration of genetic and phenotypic data have been developed quite recently and applied to few models, especially in plants. In this study, we investigated species boundaries within a group of twelve Pseudophrys taxa from France by analyzing genetic, morphometric and chemical (i.e., floral scents) data in a Bayesian framework using the program integrated Bayesian Phylogenetics and Phylogeography (iBPP). We found that these twelve taxa were merged into four species when only genetic data were used, while most formally described species were recognized as such when only phenotypic (either morphometric or chemical) data were used. The result of the iBPP analysis performed on both genetic and phenotypic data supports the proposal to merge Ophrys bilunulata and O. marmorata on the one hand, and O. funerea and O. zonata on the other hand. Our results show that phenotypic data are particularly informative in the section Pseudophrys and that their integration in a model-based method significantly improves the accuracy of species delimitation. We are convinced that the integrative taxonomic approach proposed in this study holds great promise to conduct taxonomic revisions in other orchid groups.
... resulted in a reduction in the duration of pseudocopulation from male wasp pollinators (De Jager, Peakall & Campbell, 2016;De Jager & Peakall, 2019). In Ophrys leochroma P.Delforge (= O. tenthredinifera Willd.), experimental manipulation of labellum morphology has shown that certain regions of the labellum act as 'gripping points', and experimental removal of these regions resulted in a reduced frequency and duration of copulation (Rakosy et al., 2017). Meanwhile, the South African daisy Gorteria diffusa Thunb. is unusual among sexually deceptive plants in that both male and female bee-flies are attracted to flowers, where dark spots on ray florets with UV-reflectivity and raised papillae elicit attempted copulation from the males (Ellis & Johnson, 2010;De Jager & Ellis, 2012). ...
... Both P. cycnocephala and L. glicensteinii represent extreme cases of sexual exploitation in that they require full pseudocopulation (termed 'genitalic pseudocopulation' by Blanco & Barboza, 2005) for pollen to be transferred. In contrast, in most sexually deceptive orchids pre-copulatory behaviour or attempted copulation from male insects is often sufficient to ensure pollination, although the chances of affecting pollination may increase with additional time spent attempting to copulate (Peakall, 1990;Rakosy et al., 2017;De Jager & Peakall, 2019). ...
Article
Sexually deceptive orchids exploit the innate sexual preferences of their male insect pollinators to achieve pollen transfer. Although floral volatiles are critical for pollinator attraction in sexually deceptive systems, floral morphology is also expected to exploit the sexual preferences of the pollinator. Here, we investigate the pollination of the Australian orchid Pterostylis cycnocephala. We confirm that male fungus gnats of a single undescribed Bradysia sp. (Diptera, Sciaridae) act as pollinators and display sexual behaviour on flowers, including wing fanning, abdomen curling and genitalic clasping of the prominent labellum appendage. Gnats only triggered the labellum and became trapped in the flower after attempting pseudocopulation with the appendage, a process necessary for pollen removal and deposition. Male gnats located flowers hidden from view, suggesting long-range attraction is primarily due to floral odour. However, male gnats displayed reduced copulatory behaviour when the labellum was absent and when the labellum appendage was inaccessible, suggesting that appropriate morphology may be required to elicit the copulatory behaviour needed for pollination. Our study is the first detailed investigation of sexual deception of male Sciaridae in Australian orchids and represents an example of convergent evolution with some Neotropical Lepanthes orchids, which also sexually deceive male Bradysia.
... While it is difficult to imagine a mechanism selecting for a given petal width, selection for a given labellum size or shape has already been hypothesized in the literature. The labellum shape and size were expected to morphologically mimic the females of the pollinator species (Peakall, 1990), or to be mechanically conform to the morphology of the male pollinator to improve copulation and/or contact with pollinia (de Jager & Peakall, 2016) with flowers form components used as gripping points to be more effective in ensuring pollinia transfer (Rakosy et al., 2017). Yet, an alternative hypothesis to trait conservatism is canalization, where the genetic backgrounds of populations share the same genetic constraint (low or no genetic variance) rather than the same optima (Hansen & Houle, 2004;Lamy et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Adaptive radiation in Ophrys orchids leads to complex floral phenotypes that vary in scent, color and shape. Using a novel pipeline to quantify these phenotypes, we investigated trait divergence at early stages of speciation in six populations of Ophrys aveyronensis experiencing recent allopatry. By integrating different genetic/genomic techniques, we investigated: variation and integration of floral components (scent, color and shape); phenotypes and genomic regions under divergent selection; and the genomic bases of trait variation. We identified a large genomic island of divergence, likely associated with phenotypic variation in particular in floral odor. We detected potential divergent selection on macular color, while stabilizing selection was suspected on floral morphology and for several volatile olfactive compounds. We also identified candidate genes involved in anthocyanin and in steroid biosynthesis pathways associated with standing genetic variation in color and odor. This study sheds light on early differentiation in Ophrys, revealing patterns that often become invisible over time, that is the geographic mosaic of traits under selection and the early appearance of strong genomic divergence. It also supports a crucial genomic region for future investigation and highlights the value of a multifaceted approach in unraveling speciation within taxa with large genomes.
... Although attractive traits influence pollination performance in deceptive species (e.g. Benitez-Vieyra et al., 2009;de Jager & Peakall, 2018;Jersáková et al., 2012;Newman et al., 2012;Phillips & Peakall, 2018;Rakosy et al., 2012Rakosy et al., , 2017, other overlooked nonattractive traits can promote pollen transfer between flowers (de Jager & Peakall, 2018). This may be the case of microstructures on the adaxial epidermis of flowers, which represent the first point of contact with pollinators (Whitney et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Trapping trichomes, a common trait found in protogynous trap flowers, are thought to enable interaction with pollinators, promoting both pollen removal and fruit set. However, this hypothesis has yet to be empirically confirmed. In this study, we experimentally tested whether trapping trichomes favour male and female functions in trap flowers. Using protogynous South American Aristolochia esperanzae trap flowers, we observed the temporal variation in both sexual functions and associated them with the wilting of the flower and trapping trichomes. Next, we described the trapping trichomes features associated with the capture, entrapment and release of pollinators. Finally, we used manipulative experiments involving trapping trichome removal to quantify their influence on pollinator entrapment, pollen removal and fruit set. While flowers wilted after the end of the female phase, the trapping trichomes only wilted some hours after pollen presentation, promoting a delay in the exit of pollinators, so they carried pollen. Trapping trichomes features, such as a density gradient towards the interior of the tube, unidirectional orientation and epicuticular waxes, may be important in the capture and entrapment of pollinators. Trapping trichome removal decreased drastically the probability of pollinator entrapment and pollen removal, leading to no fruit set. These results indicate that the evolution and maintenance of trapping trichomes in trap flowers probably occurred through selective pressures from both sexual functions and highlight their role in the functioning of this highly phenotypically specialized pollination system. Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog.
... Such novel approaches will hopefully resolve the contradictions that remain following decades of research on the pollination biology of cacao. Previous methods relied on a combination of two-dimensional imaging with landmarks and traditional morphometrics measurements using calipers (e.g., Jürgens, 2006;Rakosy et al., 2017;Wilson et al., 2017;Bilbao et al., 2021). The increasing use of 3D imaging, such as micro-CT, allows for the high-resolution study of floral organs and their internal anatomy (e.g., van der Niet et al., 2010;Dellinger et al., 2014;Wang et al., 2015;Staedler et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Premise Imaging technologies that capture three‐dimensional (3D) variation in floral morphology at micro‐ and nano‐resolutions are increasingly accessible. In herkogamous flowers, such as those of Theobroma cacao , structural barriers between anthers and stigmas represent bottlenecks that restrict pollinator size and access to reproductive organs. To study the unresolved pollination biology of cacao, we present a novel application of micro‐computed tomography (micro‐CT) using floral dimensions to quantify pollinator functional size limits. Methods We generated micro‐CT data sets from field‐collected flowers and museum specimens of potential pollinators. To compare floral variation, we used 3D Slicer to place landmarks on the surface models and performed a geometric morphometric (GMM) analysis using geomorph R. We identified the petal side door (an opening between the petal hoods and filament) as the main bottleneck for pollinator access. We compared its mean dimensions with proposed pollinators to identify viable candidates. Results We identified three levels of likelihood for putative pollinators based on the number of morphological (body) dimensions that fit through the petal side door. We also found floral reward microstructures whose presence and location were previously unclear. Discussion Using micro‐CT and GMM to study the 3D pollination biology of cacao provides new evidence for predicting unknown pollinators. Incorporating geometry and floral rewards will strengthen plant–pollinator trait matching models for cacao and other species.
... Rakosy et al. [100] observed the behaviour of bees visiting flowers of O. leochroma (macrospecies Tenthredinifera) from which contrasting segments of the labellum had been removed, noting that the removal of those portions used more frequently by the bees as gripping or contact points caused greater reductions in the frequency and effectiveness of pollination, especially in the deposition of pollinaria. These observations led the authors to predict that those regions most important in ensuring mechanical fit between flower and pollinator-in this case, the stigma and "shoulders/horns" of the labellum-would operate under strong stabilising selection within this microspecies, whereas regions such as lateral lobes, appendix and associated apical notch would be less critical and would therefore show greater variation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Our frequently deployed approach to optimally circumscribing species requires large-scale field sampling within and between populations for large numbers of morphometric characters, followed by multivariate ordinations to objectively seek discontinuities (or, failing that, zones of limited overlap) among sets of populations considered to represent species. Corresponding boundaries are sought in DNA-based outputs, either phylogenies or preferably ordinations based on population genetic data. Herein, we analyse within a molecular phylogenetic framework detailed morphometric data for the charismatic bee orchids (Ophrys), seeking a ‘mesospecies’ species concept that might provide a compromise between the nine ‘macrospecies’ recognised primarily through DNA barcoding and the several hundred ‘microspecies’ recognised primarily through perceived pollinator specificity. Our analyses failed to find robust groupings that could be regarded as credible mesospecies, instead implying that each macrospecies constitutes a morphological continuum. This problematic result encouraged us to reappraise both our morphometric approach and the relative merits of the contrasting macrospecies and microspecies concepts, and to reiterate the key role played by genetics in species circumscription. Abstract Despite (or perhaps because of) intensive multidisciplinary research, opinions on the optimal number of species recognised within the Eurasian orchid genus Ophrys range from nine to at least 400. The lower figure of nine macrospecies is based primarily on seeking small but reliable discontinuities in DNA ‘barcode’ regions, an approach subsequently reinforced and finessed via high-throughput sequencing studies. The upper figure of ca. 400 microspecies reflects the morphological authoritarianism of traditional taxonomy combined with belief in extreme pollinator specificity caused by reliance on pollination through pseudo-copulation, enacted by bees and wasps. Groupings of microspecies that are less inclusive than macrospecies are termed mesospecies. Herein, we present multivariate morphometric analyses based on 51 characters scored for 457 individual plants that together span the full morphological and molecular diversity within the genus Ophrys, encompassing 113 named microspecies that collectively represent all 29 mesospecies and all nine macrospecies. We critique our preferred morphometric approach of accumulating heterogeneous data and analysing them primarily using principal coordinates, noting that our conclusions would have been strengthened by even greater sampling and the inclusion of data describing pseudo-pheromone cocktails. Morphological variation within Ophrys proved to be exceptionally multidimensional, lacking strong directional trends. Multivariate clustering of plants according to prior taxonomy was typically weak, irrespective of whether it was assessed at the level of macrospecies, mesospecies or microspecies; considerable morphological overlap was evident even between subsets of the molecularly differentiable macrospecies. Characters supporting genuine taxonomic distinctions were often sufficiently subtle that they were masked by greater and more positively correlated variation that reflected strong contrasts in flower size, tepal colour or, less often, plant size. Individual macrospecies appear to represent morphological continua, within which taxonomic divisions are likely to prove arbitrary if based exclusively on morphological criteria and adequately sampled across their geographic range. It remains unclear how much of the mosaic of subtle character variation among the microspecies reflects genetic versus epigenetic or non-genetic influences and what proportion of any contrasts observed in gene frequencies can be attributed to the adaptive microevolution that is widely considered to dictate speciation in the genus. Moreover, supplementing weak morphological criteria with extrinsic criteria, typically by imposing constraints on geographic location and/or supposed pollinator preference, assumes rather than demonstrates the presence of even the weakest of species boundaries. Overall, it is clear that entities in Ophrys below the level of macrospecies have insufficiently structured variation, either phenotypic or genotypic, to be resolved into discrete, self-circumscribing (“natural”) entities that can legitimately be equated with species as delimited within other less specialised plant genera. Our search for a non-arbitrary (meso)species concept competent to circumscribe an intermediate number of species has so far proven unsuccessful.
... For many Ophrys species, the similarity between the plant and the shape, the dimension, the hairiness, and the colouration of the insect is obvious [30,31]. Several studies have demonstrated it experimentally [32], whereas others have shown that the colour values and colouration patterns of the flower can be perceived and memorised by the pollinator, thus having ecological and evolutionary significance in this particular system ( [33][34][35][36][37], but see [38]). The evolution of these sexually deceptive orchids [28] favours intraspecific variation in floral morphology and colours, which often makes it difficult to delimit each species and to assign the most divergent individuals to a species. ...
Article
Full-text available
Characterising phenotypic differentiation is crucial to understand which traits are involved in population divergence and establish the evolutionary scenario underlying the speciation process. Species harbouring a disjunct spatial distribution or cryptic taxa suggest that scientists often fail to detect subtle phenotypic differentiation at first sight. We used image-based analyses coupled with a simple machine learning algorithm to test whether we could distinguish two vicariant population groups of an orchid species complex known to be difficult to tease apart based on morphological criteria. To assess whether these groups can be distinguished on the basis of their phenotypes, and to highlight the traits likely to be the most informative in supporting a putative differentiation, we (i) photographed and measured a set of 109 individuals in the field, (ii) extracted morphometric, colour, and colour pattern information from pictures, and (iii) used random forest algorithms for classification. When combined, field- and image-based information provided identification accuracy of 95%. Interestingly, the variables used by random forests to discriminate the groups were different from those suggested in the literature. Our results demonstrate the interest of field-captured pictures coupled with machine learning classification approaches to improve taxon identification and highlight candidate traits for further eco-evolutionary studies.
Preprint
Mimicry implies that an organism gains fitness by resembling a model species, and one example is rewardless plants that attract pollinators by resembling co-flowering species that provide rewards. While trait matching between mimic and model has been characterised in many cases of putative floral mimicry, few have demonstrated that resemblance is adaptive and dependent on model presence. Sun orchids ( Thelymitra ) are believed to mimic flowers of buzz-pollinated rewarding plants by displaying false anthers. To test the adaptive value of the false anthers we examined whether fruit production of T. crinita and T. macrophylla was reduced when anthers were experimentally removed or obscured, and whether the reduction was stronger when putative model plants were abundant. We also assessed visual flower similarity of both orchids and their putative model plants according to bee colour perception and identified shared pollinators and whether their behaviour on T. crinita was similar to that on co-flowering rewarding plants. Fruit production of both sun orchids was strongly reduced (60-71%) by removal or painting of false anthers but was not affected by the abundance of model plants. Sun orchid flowers closely matched flower colour of co-flowering pollen-rewarding species, and T. crinita shared pollinators with the rewarding species. Visiting bees attempted to buzz and manipulate the false anther, with a behaviour similar to that observed on model plants. The experimental results demonstrate that the false anther is an important adaptation to pollination in sun orchids. Striking visual flower similarity and shared pollinators between orchids and models suggest that sun orchids are pollinated by bees that mistake orchids for buzz-pollinated rewarding plants. The adaptive value of the false anther did not depend on model plant abundance in the local population, indicating that the relevant spatial scale is larger, or that the effects of the model species are weak in comparison to effects of other rewarding species, i.e., that magnet effects of nectar-rewarding species are dominating. False anthers are widespread in the genus Thelymitra , and this “mimicry trait” seems to represent an evolutionary novelty that offers unique opportunities to explore adaptations to pollination in deceptive plants.
Preprint
Full-text available
DELFORGE, P.-Ophrys brachyotus REICHENBACH pat. 1830 in the French Pre-Alps. The Ophrys fuciflora species complex has long been considered in France as consisting of two species, O. fuciflora and O. scolopax. Some supporters of a reductive classification, based on morphological and biological concepts of the species, still accept this view today. These ways of seeing things are briefly debated. However, in Western Europe in recent years, many species of the O. fuciflora and O. tetraloniae groups have been recognized, sometimes described or rehabilitated. Among these, O. brachyotus, described from the Genua region (Liguria, Italy) in 1830 by REICHENBACH pat., remains neglected. The presence of about 15 species of the O. tetraloniae group has been reported in recent years in the French Prealps, but some are not formally delimited and are known only by working names: "Ophrys du Tricastin" and "Ophrys du Bugey". The exceptional climatic conditions of 2022 favored the flowering of O. brachyotus in Southern France, while the other fucifloroid species were less frequent or even absent. This situation allowed us to understand that the Ophrys du Tricastin and the Ophrys du Bugey, which flowered with relative abundance in 2022, were not undescribed taxa, but actually represented O. brachyotus in the French Prealps. A list of 48 O. brachyotus sites and a distribution map, based on personal observations in France over the last 40 years, are presented.
Article
Full-text available
Ophrys flowers mimic sex pheromones of attractive females of their pollinators and attract males, which attempt to copulate with the flower and thereby pollinate it. Virgin females and orchid flowers are known to use the same chemical compounds in order to attract males. The composition of the sex pheromone and its floral analogue, however, vary between pollinator genera. Wasp-pollinated Ophrys species attract their pollinators by using polar hydroxy acids, whereas Andrena-pollinated species use a mixture of non-polar hydrocarbons. The phylogeny of Ophrys shows that its evolution was marked by episodes of rapid diversification coinciding with shifts to different pollinator groups: from wasps to Eucera and consequently to Andrena and other bees. To gain further insights, we studied pollinator attraction in O. leochroma in the context of intra- and inter-generic pollinator shifts, radiation, and diversification in the genus Ophrys. Our model species, O. leochroma, is pollinated by Eucera kullenbergi males and lies in the phylogeny between the wasp and Andrena-pollinated species; therefore, it is a remarkable point to understand pollinator shifts. We collected surface extracts of attractive E. kullenbergi females and labellum extracts of O. leochroma and analyzed them by using gas chromatography with electroantennographic detection (GC-EAD) and gas chromatography coupled with mass spectrometry (GC-MS). We also performed field bioassays. Our results show that O. leochroma mimics the sex pheromone of its pollinator's female by using aldehydes, alcohols, fatty acids, and non-polar compounds (hydrocarbons). Therefore, in terms of the chemistry of pollinator attraction, Eucera-pollinated Ophrys species might represent an intermediate stage between wasp- and Andrena-pollinated orchid species.
Article
Full-text available
To clarify some systematic questions within the Ophrys tenthredinifera species group we investigated the pollination biology of some species on the Aegean islands of Crete, Kythera, Paros and Skyros. During several trips, we paid attention to Ophrys tenthredinifera (s. str.) subsp. villosa, O. ulyssea, O. amphidami, O. lycomedis, and O. dictynnae. From the island of Paros we describe the new species O. lychnitis. Some further results are: 1. Ophrys ulyssea from Kefalonia, Ithaki and Zakynthos is pollinated by the small long horned bee Eucera bidentata. The Ophrys species also occurs along the western and southern Peloponnes. Eucera bidentata also pollinate Ophrys amphidami described from Kythera. Flower types of ulyssea and amphidami always occur together and are only forms of one single species. O. amphidami is a synonym of O. ulyssea. 2. Ophrys tenthredinifera subsp. villosa could also be found on Kythera. This species is variable in flower size similar as the other islands (e.g. Rhodos). The pollinator Eucera nigrilabris ssp. rufitarsis could be observed by pseudocopulations. 3. Ophrys lycomedis together with O. tenthredinifera s. str. is on the island of Journal Europäischer Orchideen 48 (2-4): 2016. 347 Skyros widely distributed, but only in rare and small populations. It is a well-defined species and is pollinated by Eucera caspica. 4. During three trips on Paros we could find only one species of the tenthredinifera group. It is small to middle sized, is variable but well characterized. It is pollinated by the common long horned bee Eucera albofasciata. It is described as a new species: Ophrys lychnitis, possibly restricted to the islands of the central Cyclades. 5. This bee was also observed as pollinator of Ophrys dictynnae from Crete. In spite of the same pollinator of O. lychnitis and O. dictynnae we think that this is a case of an independent selection of the same bee species. This selection resulted in similar flower forms within the tenthredinifera group but with clear differences. It is also discussed an alternative possibility of a secondary colonization from Crete during the ice age period. 6. Further observations of pollinators on Kythera and Paros. Kythera: Ophrys bilunulata subsp. punctulata with Andrena flavipes, O. sicula with Andrena hesperia, O. heldreichii (incl. “calypsus”) with Eucera berlandi, O. herae with Andrena thoracica. It is described a single observation of a female seeking male of Xylocopa iris inspecting the flower entrance of the large flowered Serapias orientalis subsp. moreana. This male creeped into the flower during the day and took out the pollinariae. Paros: Ophrys israelitica with Andrena flavipes, O. parosica with Andrena similis, O. omegaifera (incl. „polycratis”) with Anthophora atroalba, O. cf. phryganae with Andrena cf. humilis, O. mammosa with Andrena morio, O. bombyliflora with Eucera nigrescens, O. heldreichii with Eucera berlandi, O. ferrum-equinum with Chalicodoma parietina, O. ariadnae with Melecta albifrons, O. cretica subsp. beloniae with Melecta tuberculata.
Article
Full-text available
Several neotropical orchid genera have been proposed as being sexually deceptive; however , this has been carefully tested in only a few cases. The genus Telipogon has long been assumed to be pollinated by male tachinid flies during pseudocopulatory events but no detailed confirmatory reports are available. Here, we have used an array of methods to elucidate the pollination mechanism in Telipogon peruvianus. The species presents flowers that have a mean floral longevity of 33 days and that are self-compatible, although spontaneous self-pollination does not occur. The flowers attract males of four tachinid species but only the males of an undescribed Eudejeania (Eudejeania aff. browni; Tachinidae) species are specific pollinators. Males visit the flowers during the first few hours of the day and the pollination success is very high (42% in one patch) compared with other sexually deceptive species. Female-seeking males are attracted to the flowers but do not attempt copulation with the flowers, as is usually described in sexually deceptive species. Nevertheless, morphological analysis and behavioural tests have shown an imperfect mimicry between flowers and females suggesting that the attractant stimulus is not based only on visual cues, as long thought. Challenging previous conclusions, our chemical analysis has confirmed that flowers of Telipogon release volatile compounds; however, the role of these volatiles in pol-linator behaviour remains to be established. Pollinator behaviour and histological analyses indicate that Telipogon flowers possess scent-producing structures throughout the corolla. Our study provides the first confirmed case of (i) a sexually deceptive species in the Onciidi-nae, (ii) pollination by pre-copulatory behaviour and (iii) pollination by sexual deception involving tachinid flies.
Book
Mimicry is a classic example of adaptation through natural selection. The traditional focus of mimicry research has been on defense in animals (protective mimicry), but there is now also a highly developed and rapidly growing body of research on floral mimicry in plants. Being literally rooted to one spot, plants generally have to use food bribes to cajole animals into acting as couriers for their pollen. Plants that lack these food rewards often deploy elaborate color and scent signals in order to mimic food sources, oviposition sites, or mating partners of particular animals, and thereby exploit these animals for the purposes of pollination. This book addresses the question of whether the evolutionary and ecological principles that were developed for protective mimicry in animals also apply to floral mimicry in plants. Visual, olfactory, and tactile signals can all be important in floral mimicry systems. The traditional focus has been on visual cues, but there is increasing evidence that some forms of mimicry, notably sexual and oviposition-site mimicry, are largely based on chemical cues. The molecular basis for these signals, their role in cognitive misclassification of flowers by pollinators, and the implications of these signals for plant speciation are among the topics covered in the book. The chapters of this book are designed to highlight particular systems of floral mimicry and to integrate them into the broader theory of mimicry.
Article
Semiochemicals are of paramount importance in sexually deceptive plants. These plants sexually lure specific male insects as pollinators by chemical and physical mimicry of the female of the pollinator. The strategy has evolved repeatedly in orchids, with a wide diversity of insect groups exploited. Chemical communication systems confirmed by field bioassays include: alkenes and alkanes in bee pollinated Ophrys species, keto-acid and hydroxy-acids in scoliid wasp pollinated O. speculum, and cyclohexanediones and pyrazines in thynnine wasp pollinated Chiloglottis and Drakaea orchids, respectively. In Ophrys, stearoyl-acyl carrier protein desaturase (SAD) enzymes have been confirmed to control species level variation in alkene double bond position. The production of cyclohexanediones in Chiloglottis unexpectedly depends on UVB light, a phenomenon unknown for other plant specialised metabolites. Potential biosynthetic pathways for other systems are explored, and alternative approaches to further accelerate chemical discovery in sexually deceptive plants are proposed.
Article
Congruences in pilosity between two verified Hymenoptera/orchid associations: Campsoscolia ciliatalOphrys speculum and Argogorytes sp./O. insectifera, and the suggested association Eucera nigrilabrislO. scolopax were investigated using the scanning electron microscope. Morphological evidence. suggests that Ophrys flowers mimic the hair patterns of hymenopteran females, offering males copulatory stimuli and positioning them correctly at the labellum for pollination. Males are initially attracted olfactorily and visually at short distances. It is not believed to be the visual appearance of the hairs that is mimicked, but their ability to stimulate males tactilely at appropriate mechano/proprioceptive sites and by being distributed in optimal patterns on the flowers.