ArticlePDF Available

Making Logarithms Accessible — Operational and Structural Basic Models for Logarithms

Authors:
  • University of Teacher Education Lucerne

Abstract and Figures

Logarithms have a reputation for being difficult and inaccessible. As an analysis of their historical, mathematical and educational background suggests, this problem might be due to the way in which logarithms are interpreted and explained in textbooks: as the inverse of exponents. If this conclusion is right, additional interpretations of logarithms are required. By combining the theoretical construct of 'Grundvorstellungen' (translated as 'basic models') and the distinction between operational and structural conceptions, I identify and elaborate four interpretations of logarithms: (i) the basic model of 'multiplicative measuring', (ii) the basic model of 'counting the number of digits', (iii) the basic model of 'decreasing the hierarchy level', and (iv) the basic model of 'inverse exponent'. Three models (i–iii) reflect operational conceptions and interpret logarithms in contexts familiar to students. In combination with (iv), a structural basic model, this paper argues on a theoretical level that they could help to make logarithms accessible and understandable to students. Following the tradition of 'Stoffdidaktik' ('subject-matter didactics'), the study thus aims to unpack some of the content knowledge required for the teaching of logarithms.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This is an Original Manuscript of an article published by Springer in Journal für
Mathematik-Didaktik, 2016, Vol. 37, Issue 1 Supplement, 69–98, available
online: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s13138-016-0104-6.
Making Logarithms Accessible — Operational
and Structural Basic Models for Logarithms
Christof Weber
Pädagogische Hochschule Nordwestschweiz (School of Education, University of
Applied Sciences Northwestern Switzerland), Benzburweg 30, 4410 Liestal,
Switzerland
Tel. +41 61 925 77 63
Fax +41 61 925 77 66
E-mail: christof.weber@fhnw.ch
URL: http://www.fhnw.ch/people/christof-weber/
Abstract Logarithms have a reputation for being difficult and inaccessible. As an analysis of their
historical, mathematical and educational background suggests, this problem might be due to the
way in which logarithms are interpreted and explained in textbooks: as the inverse of exponents. If
this conclusion is right, additional interpretations of logarithms are required.
By combining the theoretical construct of Grundvorstellungen(translated as basic models) and
the distinction between operational and structural conceptions, I identify and elaborate four
interpretations of logarithms: (i) the basic model of ‘multiplicative measuring’, (ii) the basic model
of counting the number of digits’, (iii) the basic model of ‘decreasing the hierarchy level’, and
(iv) the basic model of ‘inverse exponent’. Three models (iiii) reflect operational conceptions and
interpret logarithms in contexts familiar to students. In combination with (iv), a structural basic
model, this paper argues on a theoretical level that they could help to make logarithms accessible
and understandable to students. Following the tradition of Stoffdidaktik’ (‘subject-matter
didactics), the study thus aims to unpack some of the content knowledge required for the teaching
of logarithms.
Keywords logarithm · concept formation · subject-matter didactics
· Grundvorstellungen · basic models · operational-structural · content knowledge
for teaching · upper secondary school
Operationale und strukturelle Grundvorstellungen zum Logarithmus
Zusammenfassung Der Logarithmus gilt als schwierig und unverständlich. Wie eine historische,
mathematische und didaktische Sachanalyse zeigt, könnte dieses Problem darauf zurückgehen,
dass der Logarithmus in Schulbüchern primär als inverser Exponent eingeführt und interpretiert
wird. Wenn diese Diagnose zutrifft, sind weitere Zugänge zum Logarithmus gefragt.
2
Der vorliegende Beitrag entwickelt und diskutiert unter Bezugnahme auf das theoretische
Konstrukt der Grundvorstellungen sowie auf die Unterscheidung zwischen operationalen und
strukturellen Auffassungen mathematischer Begriffe vier Grundvorstellungen zum Logarithmus:
(i) die Grundvorstellung des ‚multiplikativen Einpassens, (ii) die Grundvorstellung des
‚Bestimmens der Stellenzahl, (iii) die Grundvorstellung des ‚Herabsetzens der Hierarchiestufe,
sowie (iv) die Grundvorstellung des ‚inversen Exponenten. Drei Interpretationen (i bis iii) deuten
den Logarithmus operational und darüber hinaus in einem Erfahrungsbereich, der den Lernenden
vertraut ist. Zusammen mit (iv), einer strukturellen Grundvorstellung, könnten sie so wird hier
aus theoretischer Sicht argumentiertden Logarithmus für Lernende zugänglicher und
verständlicher machen. In der Tradition der Stoffdidaktik stehend, bereitet dieser Beitrag
mathematisches Professionswissen für das Unterrichten des Logarithmus auf.
Schlüsselwörter Logarithmus · Begriffsbildung · Stoffdidaktik
· Grundvorstellungen · Operational–strukturell · Unterrichtsspezifisches
mathematisches Professionswissen · Sekundarstufe 2
Mathematics Subject Classification (2010): C34 · D44 · D74 · H24
Introduction
In their study on students’ understanding of pH values in chemistry, Watters and
Watters quote the following comment by a student: “I don’t know what the log
actually is, I only know where the button is on my calculator” (Watters and
Watters 2006, p. 280). Such comments are not uncommon — many students do
not understand what logarithms ‘actually are’ and struggle with the concept
(Berezovski 2004; Espedal 2015; DePierro and Garafalo 2008; Kenney 2005;
Williams 2011). This is especially problematic considering the indispensability of
the concept of logarithms for understanding various scientific and mathematical
phenomena (e.g., pH values, decibel scales, financial interest rates, etc.).
Logarithms, then, leave some room for improvement with respect to making
accessible (Kirsch 1977), a guiding principle for the conception and planning of
mathematics lessons.
Why, then, are logarithms meaningless and difficult for many students? And what
mathematical interpretations could make the concept more accessible to students
so that they grasp the concept, experiencing it as meaningful and being able to
apply it to solve mathematical problems? Rooted in the German tradition of
Stoffdidaktik, or ‘subject-matter didactics’ (Sträßer 2014), this paper analyses the
concept of logarithms to understand how it can be made accessible to, and
3
meaningful for, students. In particular, it aims to make explicit the elements
required for the teaching of logarithms, i.e. to unpack some of the related
specialized content knowledge (Ball et al. 2008, p. 400).
One theoretical construct that describes how mathematical concepts can be made
comprehensible to students is that of Grundvorstellungen, or ‘basic models’
(Griesel 1971; vom Hofe 1998; vom Hofe et al. 2006; Oehl 1962). Focusing on
meaningful interpretations of concepts, this construct has enjoyed popularity in
the German-speaking countries since its inception. In international literature,
another theoretical construct on how accessibility can be achieved is more
established, namely that of operationality and structurality (Sfard 1991, 2008).
While accessibility plays an important role in the work of Sfard, she also
emphasises other quite different aspects. Both theoretical constructs have been
applied to concepts such as rational numbers, division or functions (e.g., Jordan
2006; Sfard 1991, 2008). To the best of my knowledge, they have never been
combined to identify the features of a concept that could make it more accessible
to students. This is what this paper aims to do with respect to logarithms.
The first section sets out the theoretical framework, introducing the theoretical
construct of basic models to non-German-language readers, and summarizing the
theoretical construct of operationality and structurality. The second section
presents a historical and mathematical analysis of logarithms as a concept. In
particular, it explains that logarithms and division are mathematical analogous,
and it presents a method by which logarithmic values can be calculated manually.
The third section is on educational background. It looks at the place of logarithms
in the secondary curriculum and in algebra textbooks, and provides an overview
of the body of literature on learners' difficulties and on various proposals as to
how these could be addressed. In particular, it gives some reasons why the
textbook definition of logarithms could be responsible for many difficulties and
mistakes. As will be shown in the forth section, the construct of operationality and
structurality can be used to diversify the construct of Grundvorstellungen: First,
four basic models for logarithms as numbers and operators will be proposed, then
their theoretical potential for the handling of logarithms and for explaining them
operationally or structurally will be analysed. The final section formulates the
main hypotheses, and outlines some future research for investigating them
empirically.
4
In short, this is an argumentative research paper: It analyses the subject matter of
logarithms from a semantic and an epistemological viewpoint, and then, by
combining two different theoretical frameworks, it proposes how logarithms could
be made accessible to learners. It generates no empirical findings but rather
hypotheses, underpinned with theoretical arguments. I thus present a more
thorough theoretical analysis of the subject than in my earlier study (Weber 2013).
1 Two Conceptualisations of Understanding
1.1 ‘Grundvorstellungen’ as Basic Models
The German mathematics educator Kirsch (1977) considers making accessible to
be an important guideline for teaching. Among other aspects, he suggests
including “the surroundings of mathematics” (ibid., p. 104). The theoretical
construct of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ follows this tradition and takes Kirsch’s
suggestion one step further: To make mathematics accessible to learners,
‘Grundvorstellungen’ place mathematical concepts in familiar contexts. The
following presents an outline of some central aspects of the theoretical construct
referred to in section 4, following the works of vom Hofe (1998), vom Hofe et al.
(2006) and Kleine et al. (2005). For a more comprehensive discussion, see vom
Hofe and Blum (2016, in this issue).
1.1.1 A theoretical construct and its use in German-speaking countries
There is a long tradition in German-speaking countries of seeking to ensure that
learning is experienced as something meaningful and not merely mechanical. In
the second half of the 20th century, the theoretical construct of
Grundvorstellungen — translated as ‘basic models’ (cf. 1.1.2) — became
established in German-speaking theories on teaching and learning mathematics. It
was first elaborated for arithmetic and algebra at primary school level, and then
expanded to lower and upper secondary mathematics.
Initially, the construct was mainly seen prescriptively, describing the standard
interpretations of mathematical concepts or theorems to be learned, like the
“sharing and measuring” interpretations for dividing (cf. 1.1.4). For generations,
many German-speaking mathematics educators sought such meaningful
5
interpretations to use as theoretical categories to develop lessons, write textbooks
and structure curricula.1
A different, more empirical view of interpretations of mathematical concepts was
introduced into the German-language discourse by vom Hofe (1998). Vom Hofe
studied Grundvorstellungen more from the perspective of students, taking into
account their actual interpretations and the conceptions guiding their problem
solving and argumentation. Through this descriptive broadening,
Grundvorstellungen “[…] can be used to describe relations between mathematical
contents and the phenomenon of the individual generation of concepts,
characterizing […] constitution of meaning of mathematical concepts based on
familiar contexts and experiences […]” (vom Hofe 1998, p. 320, italics in
original). A similar approach to conceptions guiding students’ problem solving
and argumentation was already being discussed in other countries in the form of
tacit models (Fischbein 1989), concept images (Tall and Vinner 1991), or use
meanings (Usiskin 1991).
1.1.2 ‘Grundvorstellungen’ as basic models
In order to use the theoretical construct ‘Grundvorstellungen’ in an English-
language context, it is first necessary to find an appropriate term for it. To
describe the prescriptive guidelines for structuring lessons (see above), it could be
translated as “basic ideas” (vom Hofe 1998, p. 319). If the focus is more on
individual meaning making, the term is sometimes translated as “individual
images” (vom Hofe 1998, p. 326) or “mental models” (vom Hofe et al. 2006, p.
142; Kleine et al. 2005, p. 229). Because the present paper will elaborate
normative interpretations of logarithms that are prototypical for working
mathematically, the term basic models is preferred here (cf. Selter et al. 2012).
This is in keeping with the English-language tradition where the interpretations of
division as sharing and measuring, for instance, are seen as “models” (Brown
1981, p. 23; Greer 1992, p. 276). As this example suggests, basic models are
standard interpretations of a mathematical concept (division) in the world of
experience, be it real-life experiences (sharing, comparing) or consolidated school
knowledge (subtraction). By having a certain explanatory power, they can serve
1 For a comprehensive overview of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ identified so far for lower secondary
concepts, see Jordan (2006, pp. 148154).
6
students as a basis for meaningful argumentation and for solving tasks, going
beyond arbitrary interpretations of concepts.
1.1.3 Two types of basic models
To conceptualise elementary and advanced concept formation, vom Hofe
distinguishes between primary and secondary basic models, following Fischbein's
distinction between primary and secondary intuitions (1987). If a basic model
refers to real-life experience, vom Hofe speaks of a primary basic model. In a
sense intermediaries between real life and mathematics, they are used to support
mathematical reasoning that is related to reality (Blum 2002) or — in contrast —
the modelling of real-life problems (vom Hofe et al. 2006).
But many advanced concepts, including logarithms, are difficult to interpret in an
everyday context with realistic objects and manual actions. Nevertheless, to make
these kinds of concepts meaningful it is important to interpret them in familiar
contexts. Here it is useful to draw on the school context of mathematical objects
and actions: secondary basic models. Models of this type can be seen as
intermediaries between different modes of representations (e.g., vom Hofe and
Blum 2016, in this issue; Stölting 2008). The secondary basic models for
logarithms that will be identified in section 4, however, are slightly different.
They have less to do with mediating between different modes of representation
than with mediating between contexts of different levels of familiarity: Every
basic model for logarithms will link a non-familiar context of logarithms with
another context that students are likely to be more experienced in, for example
that of measurement (fig. 4–7).2
Finally, it should be noted that basic models, as interpretations, can never cover
all relevant aspects of a mathematical concept. They usually have less universal
validity than mathematical concepts, precisely because they refer to specific
contexts. Thus, in order to have a broad basis for reasoning and understanding, a
2 In this paper, the term “context” is not limited to real-life settings, but is considered more
broadly in the sense of meaningful settings where particular conceptualizations of a
mathematical concept can be activated (e.g., Leuders et al. 2011). Moreover, the term
“familiar” is used to refer to students’ individual experiences and underlying activities (e.g.,
comparing, counting, etc.). In the German tradition, the corresponding construct is ‘subjektiver
Erfahrungsbereich’ (Bauersfeld 1983; vom Hofe 1998), or ‘subjective experiential domain.
7
mathematical concept must be represented by a variety of different basic models:
Each aspect has to be experienced as a phenomenon in its own right.
1.1.4 Basic models for division, and a first research question
Because logarithms will later be shown to be analogous to division (cf. 2.1.2), and
because a variety of different basic models for division have been identified, the
theoretical considerations outlined above will be shortly illustrated.
Although the division of natural numbers can be seen as the sharing of objects
(fair-sharing or partitive basic model), division also occurs in other everyday
contexts. Another, no less important interpretation is the splitting up into groups
of equal size (measurement or quotative basic model), sometimes formulated by
asking how many times the divisor fits or goes into the dividend (fitting-into basic
model). While the fair-sharing model cannot be applied to fractions, the fitting-
into model can:
30 ÷ 1
2= 60
, because
1
2
goes into 30 as an addend sixty times.
The corresponding chain of argument can be represented as in figure 1: While the
two boxes represent different contexts of division, the arrows indicate how the
basic model mediates between the contexts (adapted from Prediger 2009, p. 170).
This example illustrates what is meant by the assertion that basic models have a
certain explanatory power and can serve as a basis for meaningful argumentation.
What is 30 divided by
1 2
?
30 – 1 2 – – 1 2 =30 – 60 1 2 =0
!!30 ÷1 2 =60
Division as
(additive) fitting-into
How often does
1 2
(as an addend) fit into 30?
Fig. 1 Role of the basic model of fitting-intoas intermediary between two contexts of division
(adapted from Prediger 2009, p. 170)
8
In addition to these primary basic models for division, the repeated subtraction of
equal subtrahends and inverse multiplication are secondary basic models for
division. It should be noted that the interpretation of division as repeated
subtraction leads to the long division algorithm (Brown 1981; Fosnot and Dolk
2001; Greer 1992; Griesel 1971; Padberg and Benz 2011; Radatz et al. 1998).
As mentioned above, no basic models in the sense of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ have
thus far been identified for logarithms.3 This lack gives rise to the following
question:
(Q1) What are some different basic models for the concept of logarithms?
Specifically, which interpretations have the explanatory power to relate
logarithms to familiar mathematical contexts and consolidated experiences?
An arbitrary collection of basic models would not answer this question
satisfactorily. Instead, with regard to their accessibility, different “types” of basic
models are needed. This can be achieved through the construct of operationality
and structurality, which is briefly described below.
1.2 Operational and Structural Conceptions
A quite different perspective is afforded by process-object theories, which
conceptualise understanding as a combination of processes and objects. This is the
basic thinking behind a number of theories, including relational and instrumental
understanding (Skemp 1978), procepts (Gray and Tall 1994), the APOS theory
(Dubinsky 1991; Dubinsky et al. 2002), and operational versus structural
conceptions (Sfard 1991, 2008). As the latter perspective will be fruitful for the
elaboration of basic models for logarithms, the distinction between operational
and structural conceptions will be briefly outlined here, following Sfard (1991,
2008).
3 As early as 1850, a German textbook proposes “Grundvorstellungen zum Logarithmus” (basic
models for logarithms), with the aim of explaining logarithms in a more “comprehensible and
thorough” manner (Matzka 1850). However, he interpreted them as “[…] representatives,
chargés d'affaires, authorised agents […]” (ibid., p. 10, translated by C. W.), so one can hardly
consider these as basic models in the modern sense.
9
1.2.1 Dual nature of mathematical concepts
According to Sfard, mathematical concepts can essentially be conceived in two
ways: operationally or structurally. These two conceptions are not mutually
exclusive, but complementary: Depending on how it is approached or acted upon,
a mathematical content can be thought of from an operational or structural
perspective. In other words, concepts such as fractions, functions, etc. always
embody both of these conceptions. The dual nature of mathematical concepts
offers some conceptual affordances: “[…] the structural approach generates
insight; the operational approach generates result” (Sfard 1991, p. 28).
The genesis of a mathematical concept often begins with a method or an
algorithm, a process. As an example, Sfard (1991) cites the evolution of the
concept of numbers. Whether from the division of natural numbers to positive
fractions, whether from the root extraction of real numbers to imaginary numbers:
it always began with a process. A notation such as
1 2
, for example, was
understood operationally first as division, and only later structurally when it was
read as a symbol for an object.
With reference to Piaget, Sfard (1991) outlines a model for learning in which
students begin by learning operational interpretations of mathematical concepts,
then progress towards their structural interpretation. In her view, new concepts
should not be taught only from a structural perspective, but teaching should take
care to also address complementary, operational aspects. The qualitatively crucial
step to understanding then consists in “realizing” several processes as a new
entity: In reification, a number of previously unconnected process-related aspects,
possibly in different representations, become united in a single object (Sfard 1991,
p. 19).
Sfard has been working on the operationalization of these terms for a long time
and recently began to broaden her originally cognitive view with a discursive
dimension (Sfard 2008). Thinking, and especially concept development, is now
seen as a matter of communication. Operationality and structurality thus become
properties of discourse on a concept: They no longer lie in its nature or
conception but are found instead in the language that is used to talk about and
explain a concept. Sfard therefore defines reification somewhat differently now:
“Reification is the act of replacing sentences about processes and actions with
propositions about states and objects” (ibid., p. 44). This means that the
10
accessibility of concepts could improve if teachers and students were engaged in a
discourse on processes and actions (Sfard 1991).
Sfard is not the only one to suggest that, when learning new concepts, operational
conceptions precede structural ones as consecutive steps to be passed through.
This is also the premise of Dubinsky and his colleagues in their research on
concept formation (Dubinsky 1991; Dubinsky and McDonald 2002). They note,
however, that concepts need not necessarily develop in this order. Gilmore and
Inglis (2008) give empirical evidence justifying this view.
1.2.2 Second research question
As the example of
1 2
illustrates, the distinction between operationality and
structurality is meaningful even for numbers and operators, and not only for
functions. To date, possible ways of realizing a mathematical concept as process
and as object have been illustrated not only for arithmetic expressions, but also for
algebraic expressions and equations (Dubinsky 1991; Gray and Tall 1994; Sfard
1991). For example, understanding the idea of squaring operationally means
describing it as a computation, and understanding it structurally means explaining
it as a “static relation between two magnitudes” (Sfard 1991, p. 6). It seems,
however, that the operational-structural construct has not yet been explicitly
applied to logarithms. In combination with (Q1) above, the second question is as
follows:
(Q2) Which of the identified basic models for logarithms enable a discourse on
processes and actions, and which enable a discourse on states and objects?
This paper will identify several basic models for logarithms that differ with
respect to their operational-structural character. They will be called operational
basic models and structural basic models. Before the two questions can be
answered, the concept of logarithms must first be analysed in its historical and
educational context.
11
2 Historical and Mathematical Analysis of
Logarithms
2.1 History of Logarithms
2.1.1 The invention of Napier and Bürgi
Logarithms were invented in the early 17th century by Napier or Bürgi, depending
on the source. Both were looking for a way to simplify astronomical calculations
of very large numbers. Though acting independently of one another, they both
proceeded in a similar manner, considering the relations between two
progressions: a geometric progression increasing by a constant factor, and an
arithmetic progression increasing by a constant addend. Napier employed a
geometric model of two correlated points moving continuously along two
different lines: One point moves on a (limited) segment at an instantaneous
velocity proportional to the remaining distance, while the other point moves at a
constant velocity on a ray (towards infinity). The distance of the first point from
the starting point thus grows geometrically, while the distance of the second point
grows arithmetically (for details, cf. Berezovski 2007, p. 23f.; Cajori 1913, p. 6f.;
Panagiotou 2011, p. 9; Smith and Confrey 1994, p. 347ff.).
On the basis of this model and after years of manual calculations, both Napier and
Bürgi came up with a pair of corresponding series sufficiently dense for practical
use in computation. These were published in 1614 (Napier) and 1620 (Bürgi)
(Cajori 1913; Panagiotou 2011; Smith and Confrey 1994). Napier’s tables in
particular went on to be widely distributed and were also variously simplified and
amplified. Shortly thereafter, in 1622, the first slide rule was invented by placing
two sliding logarithmic scales next to each other (Stoll 2006).
2.1.2 Logarithms as numbers that count divisions
The meaning of the term “logarithm” is not self-evident; and nor is the meaning of
its etymological translation: “ratio number.” Napier, who coined the term,
explained it as follows (Cajori 1913, p. 7): Two consecutive numbers of the
(geometric) progression
1
,
n
,
n2
, .… have a constant ratio. If the (arithmetic)
progression 0, 1, 2, . . . is mapped onto this progression, the expressions of the
arithmetic progression indicate how often the 1 (of the geometric progression)
12
was multiplied by the ratio
n
. After first speaking of “artificial numbers,” he later
called the numbers of the arithmetic progression the “number of ratios,” or
“logarithm.” In other words, the “number that counts ratios” of 8 to base 2 is 3,
because 1 must be multiplied by 2 three times to yield 8. This makes clear the
mathematical analogy between logarithms and division (fig. 2): While the
division of 8 by 2 asks for the number of addends, the question behind the
logarithm of 8 to base 2 is the number of factors. In other words: Just as divisions
are repeated subtractions, logarithms are repeated divisions. This conception of
logarithms as numbers that count divisions was prevalent during the 17th century,
but became less important in later definitions of logarithms.
log28=?
#
1×!2×!×2
?
" #$ %$ =8
#
8÷2÷!÷2
?
" #$ %$ =1
8÷2=?
#
0+2+!+2
?
" #$ %$ =8
#
82!2
?
" #$ %$ =0
Fig. 2 Mathematical analogy between logarithms and division
2.1.3 Euler’s and other conceptualisations
After Descartes introduced the modern symbolism
an
for powers of numbers in
1637, it was recognised that logarithms could also be interpreted as exponents.
Euler was one of the first to use the exponential property as a definition (Cajori
1913, pp. 13, 46 f., 116):
Resuming the equation
ab=c
, […] we take the exponent
b
such that
the power
ab
becomes equal to a given number
c
; in which case this
exponent
b
is said to be the logarithm of the number
c
. (Euler 1822, p.
63)
For Euler, the logarithm is a particular exponent: The logarithm of
c
to base
a
is
the exponent by which
a
must be raised to yield
c
. Rather than stating directly
what logarithms are, Euler proceeds indirectly by conceiving it in terms of its
inverse, raising to a power (with reference to the exponent). Today’s collections
of formulas follow this indirect definition when they define the logarithm by the
equivalence relation
logab=x:ax=b
.
13
From the perspective of mathematics, this reading of logarithms simplifies the
deductive structure of the earlier conceptualisation, which is why it is widely
used. Using Sfard's terminology (cf. 1.2.1), it expresses a structural
conceptualisation, as it is a static relation that refers to another concept (see 4.4.2).
But, as will be shown later, it could be responsible for many problems faced by
students (cf. 3.4).
Further properties of logarithms were discovered after Euler that can also be used
as definitions and hence are definitional. Since the late 17th century, for example,
it has been known that the (natural) logarithm can be conceived as the area of a
hyperbolic segment or can be expanded in a series (Cajori 1913; Panagiotou
2011). Another reading was added by Cauchy in the 19th century: Logarithmic
functions are (apart from a factor) the only continuous solutions
Φ
of the
functional equation
Φ(xy)=Φ(x)+Φ(y)
(
x>0
,
y>0
). Each of these
equivalent properties discovered in the past highlights a particular aspect of
logarithms and could serve as definition from a logical-deductive perspective.
2.1.4 Various notations reflect various aspects
Napier did not use any abbreviations for logarithms, but as early as 1624 the
abbreviation “Log.” was used (Kepler), and Euler wrote “L.” or “log” (1822, p.
63). Other forms of notation do not refer to the term “logarithm,” emphasising
instead the analogy to division (fig. 2). In 1778, Burja used the double fraction bar
for the logarithm of
b
to base
a
, and in 1834 Schellbach wrote it with a
tricolon, as
b
a
. These two German mathematicians and teachers would, then,
have written or
8
2=3
, respectively, instead of
log28=3
, explaining
that 8 must be divided by 2 three times to yield 1 (Cajori 1952, p. 112, p. 114).4
4 Contrary to the English-speaking tradition where the obelus sign “
÷
is used as a division
symbol, German-speaking countries use the colon “:” to indicate the division operation (also
used for ratios).
14
2.2 Algorithm for the Manual Calculation of Logarithms
As shown above, the logarithm
logab
indicates “how often” the base
a
is
contained as a factor in
b
, which can be rewritten as
b
alogab=1
. This is why, in
analogy to the standard division algorithm (“long division”), it is possible to
specify a logarithm algorithm that calculates logarithmic values decimal by
decimal (Goldberg 2006). One proceeds by repeated division rather than by
repeated subtraction, and instead of “bringing down” the next number and “adding
a zero” to the remainder (i.e. to multiply it by ten), it is raised to the power of ten
(the base of the decimal system).
The method thus essentially consists of the following steps (for an example of
taking the logarithm of 40 to base 2, see fig. 3):5
· Step 1: Calculate how many times the base goes into the argument as a factor,
and add this number as a new (decimal) place to the result. In the example, 2
goes into 40 five times as a factor without exceeding it, so the result begins
with the number 5.
· Step 2: Next, place the value of “base to the power of calculated position”
underneath the argument of the logarithm and divide the argument by this
value (instead of subtracting). Note the result as a remainder underneath. In the
example,
40 ÷25=1.25
.
· Step 3: Now, exponentiate (instead of multiplying) the remainder by ten and
note the result as the new argument:
1.2510 9.31
· Step 4: If the new argument equals 1 (instead of zero), the process has come to
an end and the result is established. Otherwise, repeating steps 1 to 3 will result
in the next digits of the decimal expansion of the result (3, 2, …).
5 To do and write down division problems like
b÷a
with long division, English-speaking
countries use the symbol
a b
. Read as “
a
is divided into
b
,” the symbol conveys the
quotative interpretation of division (cf. 1.1.4). In contrast, this symbol is not known in German-
speaking countries where the division symbol
b:a
is also used for writing down long
divisions.
15
log240 = !! 5.32!!!!!!!!!!!!
= 40 !!2 = ? ⎯ → 2)!40!!!!!!!!!!!!!
!!!!! ÷32 = 25
( )
1.25
!!!!! 1.2510 9.31
!÷8 = 23
( )
1.16
1.1610 4.57
: 4 = 22
( )
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
Fig. 3 Logarithm algorithm, using the notation
b
a
for logarithms, introduced by Schellbach,
and the notation
a)!b
, referring to the long division symbol
a!b
used in English-speaking
countries to express long divisions in writing
To show the correctness of the algorithm, the entries in figure 3 may be
reformulated as a series of equations, as follows:
log240 =log2(32 1.25) =5+log2(1.25) =5+110 log2(1.2510)5+110 log2(9.31)
5+110 log2(8 1.16) =5.3 +110 log2(1.16) =5.3 +1100 log2(1.1610 )
Two points regarding the logarithm algorithm are worth noting here:
· In contrast to division, logarithms do not (left) distribute over addition:
loga(b
1+b2)logab
1+logab2
. Because of this, in step 1 of the algorithm, one
must consider all digits of the numbers
b
and
a
at once, rather than process
them digit by digit as in long division.
· If
a
and
b
are natural numbers (where
a
does not necessarily have to be less
than
b
), this algorithm allows one to manually calculate the value of
logab
.
This is because only long divisions (step 2) and
b
-fold multiplications (step 3)
need to be calculated in the given set of algorithmic rules.6
Being an algorithm that “generates result(Sfard 1991, p. 28), this logarithm
algorithm is seen as an operational conceptualisation of logarithms (cf. 1.2.1).
6 If
b<a
, the equivalence
logab=1 logba
can be used.
16
3 Educational background of logarithms
This section looks at several aspects of the teaching and learning of logarithms.
First, the place of logarithms in school curricula and algebra textbooks will be
briefly outlined. This is followed by an overview of the research literature about
students’ difficulties and a review of alternative instruction techniques. The last
section looks at why the introduction of logarithms as indirect exponents is
problematic (cf. 2.1.3).
It should be noted that logarithms as numbers or operators must be distinguished
both mathematically and epistemologically from logarithms as functions (Smith
and Confrey 1994). Although both aspects of logarithms appear in upper
secondary level curricula, space precludes discussion of their functional aspect.
This paper is therefore focusing only on logarithms as numbers and operators.
3.1 Logarithms in Secondary Curricula and in School Textbooks
Ever since Euler, logarithms have been included in school curricula. They were
used to simplify complicated manual calculations and remained relevant in
schools for centuries until the introduction of pocket calculators, which have to a
large extent put an end to this tradition. In the German-speaking countries, for
example, the subject has been dropped entirely from curricula for lower achieving
school students. However, at upper secondary level, logarithms are still of some
importance. They usually follow powers (with non-natural exponents) and
exponential functions in grades 10–11 (i.e. age 16–17). Besides the objective of
manipulating expressions, logarithms are taught to solve specific equations,
particularly in the context of exponential growth and decay, and sometimes to
model mathematical and real-life problems. The properties and rules that
determine how logarithms behave are very important here (logarithmic laws,
change of base theorem).
With the broad circulation of the Elements of Algebra (originally published in
German in 1765), the indirect definition
logab=x:ax=b
became the
standard. Even today, many high school textbooks explain logarithms with this
formal relation. As a brief (and unsystematic) examination of some modern
algebra textbooks in German and English suggests, they all use this equivalence
17
relation to introduce logarithms as inverse exponents (e.g., Deller et al. 2000;
Griesel et al. 2004; Hanrahan et al. 2004) or as inverse exponential functions (e.g.,
Neill and Quadling 2000; Murdock et al. 2010). Some of the textbooks examined,
in order to motivate students, do first introduce to a “real-life” phenomenon that
can be solved by logarithms (e.g., Hirsch et al.. 2008), while others interpret
points of a graph of exponential growth in “reverse order,” i.e. by starting with
some
y
-values and reading off their
x
-values (e.g., Holliday et al. 2005; Kuypers
et al. 1995). Even in those instances where a textbook presents such an
interpretation in the introductory section, it rarely makes use of it for
argumentation, for instance to make the logarithmic laws plausible (for an
exception, cf. Lergenmüller and Schmidt 2004). In short, one gets the impression
that the primary interpretation of logarithms conveyed by textbooks is the indirect
conception proposed by Euler.
3.2 Students’ Difficulties and Mistakes: an Overview of Literature
Mathematics education research literature on logarithms (in English and German)
identifies and analyses two different sorts of difficulties faced by students:
specific mistakes in manipulating logarithmic expressions, and more general
problems in understanding the meaning of the logarithmic concept.
3.2.1 Mistakes in manipulating logarithmic expressions
Studies that investigate the handling of logarithms by students generally present
the mistakes made as episodic observations, in a non-exhaustive fashion and
without any theoretical grounding. To date, there seems to be no “systematic
empirical research in this context” (van Dooren et al. 2008, p. 325). Thus all that
can be provided here is a list of some incorrect calculations mentioned in the
literature:
· The expression
log(x+y)
is rewritten as
log x+log y
(Kaur and Boey 1994;
Yen 1999),
log(x)+log( y)
as
xy
(Berezovski 2004),
log(x)log( y)
as
log(x) / log( y)
(Lee and Heyworth 1999),
log(xy)
as
xlog(y)
(Chua and
Wood 2005),
log(x) / log( y)
as
log(x/y)
(Kaur and Boey 1994) or as
x/y
(Chua and Wood 2005), etc.
· When treating logarithmic expressions or solving logarithmic equations, the
logarithm is eliminated or “cancelled out” by dividing by the symbol “
log
18
(Andelfinger 1985, p. 229; Berezovski 2004, p. 54; Kenney 2005, p. 4; Yen
1999, p. 6).
· In order to calculate the value of
logab
, the root
b
a
is extracted (Fermsjö
2014; Leopold and Edgar 2008; Weber 2002).
3.2.2 Difficulties in understanding the meaning of the logarithmic concept
In addition to the kinds of abortive algebraic manipulations listed above, some
authors also report on misconceptions in meanings. Even students capable of
correctly handling logarithmic expressions may labour under such
misconceptions:
· Students conceive logarithms as a “button […] on my calculator” (Watters and
Watters 2006, p. 280), as a special “number as Pi” or simply as a “maths
machine” (Weber 2013, p. 80f.).
· Students experience the problem of determining
log28
without a calculator as
more difficult than that of writing 8 as a power of 2 (Andelfinger 1985).
· Expressions like
loga1
and
logb1
(Kenney 2005) are thought to be different,
while, conversely, expressions like
log10 x
and
ln x
(Kenney 2005) or
log(x+3)
and
log xlog3
(Andelfinger 1985; Senk and Thompson 2006) are
not recognised as being different.
· Logarithms are not recognised as a suitable tool for mathematical decisions and
modelling. For instance, when asked which is the larger of the two numbers
25625
and
26620
, students may reason as follows: “
25625
is bigger because it
has a bigger exponent” (Berezovski 2004, p. 57). Conversely, some students
are unable to articulate the implications for an actual (chemical, biological,
physical) situation where two quantities are logarithmically connected (Watters
and Watters 2006; DePierro and Garafalo 2008).
Difficulties of this nature illustrate what failing to understand logarithms can
mean.
3.2.3 Various underlying causes
These difficulties are given different interpretations in the research literature. In
the literature surveyed for the purposes of this study, three different causes could
be identified:
19
· Students' prior knowledge (mainly the concept of powers or exponents) may be
insufficient, or the new concept may not be adequately integrated into it (Chua
and Wood 2005; Kenney 2005). More detailed analyses identify
misconceptions behind students’ algebraic mistakes such as
log(x+y)=log x+log y
and regard them as an overgeneralization of rules,
with a distinction being made between the overgeneralization of logarithmic
laws (Chua and Wood 2005), the overgeneralization of linearity (van Dooren et
al. 2008) or the overgeneralization of the distributive law of numbers to
operations with the symbol of “log” (Matz 1982). Other authors focus on the
students' visual perception or the visual characteristics of the algebraic
expressions. They explain overgeneralizations of the above type by the fact that
students “misperceive the problem situation” (Lee and Heyworth 1999, p. 228)
or by the “visual salience” of the algebraic transformations, i.e. a “visual
coherence that seems to make the left- and right-hand sides appear naturally
related to one another” (Kirshner and Awtry 2004, p. 242).
· The teachers may not make the background of the symbol sufficiently explicit
(e.g., Kenney 2005), or even evince an insufficient understanding of logarithms
themselves (Berezovski 2007). For example, Berezovski (2007) concludes in
her case study that many of the investigated pre-service secondary teachers had
insufficient subject matter knowledge and limited pedagogical content
knowledge regarding logarithms.
· The dominance of the indirect definition (e.g., Espedal 2015; Fermsjö 2014;
Mulqueeny 2012; Smith and Confrey 1994; Weber 2002; Williams 2011). In
one case study, Williams (2011) reports that her students used the Eulerian
definition as a kind of a one-way transferral to exit the new context of
logarithms, arguing and answering in the better known context of exponents
and not transferring the answer back.
Being rooted in the tradition of subject-matter didactics, this study concentrates on
this last cause and reconsiders the way logarithms are introduced and defined (cf.
3.4).
3.3 Alternative Instruction Proposals: an Overview of the Literature
Because of the many difficulties this topic can cause for many students, it is
sometimes discussed in teachers’ journals and explored in mathematics education
20
publications. These propose a number of alternative instructional approaches to
supplement or replace traditional instruction and which aim to avoid difficulties
and improve accessibility. It would appear that few of the alternative approaches
have undergone systematic quantitative evaluation or qualitative investigation, so
there is little empirical data regarding the learning of students who were taught
using these approaches. They will now be classified into five groups.
3.3.1 Using history
Most of the proposals go back to the historical roots of the concept, drawing on
Napier and Bürgi. They suggest introducing and explaining logarithms with two
juxtaposed progressions in the form of number lines or the columns of a table, and
teaching students how to reason on the basis of this model (Clark 2006; Fermsjö
2014; Katz 1997; Mulqueeny 2012; Smith and Confrey 1994; Toumasis 1993).
Only a few case studies have been conducted on the impact this kind of logarithm
instruction has on students (Fermsjö 2014; Mulqueeny 2012). Fermsjö (2014), for
instance, reports that this approach was helpful, and that difficulties in
manipulating logarithmic expressions were rarer. However, his students struggled
with some new systematic mistakes (ibid., p. 308).
Weber (not to be confused with the author of this paper) chooses a slightly
different approach, introducing the logarithm
logab
as the number of factors
a
in
b
. In a pilot study with two groups of university students, he explained the
logarithm to one group as the number of factors, while the other group received
traditional instruction (Weber 2002). Both groups were then asked to solve tasks
requiring basic computations (e.g., “What is
logxx
?”) and rules (e.g., “
loga(xr)
can be simplified to what? Why?”) (ibid., p. 1024). The treatment group
performed better on these tasks than the control group. Similarly, Espedal (2015)
developed some teaching material based on repeated division. This material was
presented to students of one high-school class, while another high-school class
received traditional instruction. The “repeated division” group solved tasks such
as “Why is
log1 =0
?” or “Solve
2x+13=5
” significantly better than the control
group (ibid., p. 56). However, several of the known mistakes in manipulating
logarithmic expressions still occurred in both groups.
21
3.3.2 Using alternative language and notation
As already mentioned, it is not immediately clear what is meant by “logarithm” or
logab
” (cf 2.1.). A second group of proposals therefore involves new language
and notation. Examples include the “index” (Brennan 2007, p. 49), the “exponent
seeker” (Bennhardt 2009, p. 48) or the “liftoff function” (Hurwitz 1999, p. 344).
A form of notation like
a!(b)
would borrow from propaedeutic algebra, where
variables are initially represented in the form of placeholders (Hammack and
Lyons 1995). One could also think about introducing the “tree notation,” a
notation adapted from linguistic theory and artificial intelligence (Kirshner and
Awtry 2004, p. 232). I did not find any literature that would propose the use of
historical notations such as or
b
a
(cf. 2.1.4).
3.3.3 Manual calculation
One pitfall of defining a logarithm as an exponent is that students cannot draw on
any pre-existing algebraic procedure to solve an equation such as
2x=10
.
Students mostly solve tasks like these with the “guess and check” strategy; the
teacher’s “solution”
x=log210
can feel like a cop-out and may easily become
entrenched as the “button on my calculator” idea (Watters and Watters 2006, p.
280).
To avoid problems like these, a third group of proposals suggests having students
calculate logarithmic values manually. This can be done using a sequence of
nested intervals, going back to Euler’s demonstration of how
log5
can be
calculated manually (Sandifer 2007). Other proposals suggest utilising slide rules
or logarithm tables (Berezovski 2004, p. 78; Ostler 2013).
3.3.4 Using different conceptualisations
Yet another group of proposals suggests utilising definitional properties other than
the Eulerian definition of logarithms (cf. 2.1.3). Some authors suggest starting
with Cauchy’s property, introducing logarithms as (continuous) functions that
fulfil the property
log(ab)=log(a)+log(b)
and deducing all the properties and
logarithmic laws step by step (Seebeck and Hummel 1959). Other authors use the
22
geometric fact that the area under the hyperbola is a logarithm (Panagiotou 2011),
going back to a proposal by Klein (1932, p. 156).
3.3.5 Applications
A final group comprises suggestions for applied, “real-life” examples. Teachers
present realistic situations, which logarithms can help to calculate (Klüpfel 1981)
or which are to be represented and explored with logarithmic scales (Kirsch 1977;
Rahn 1994; Wood 2005). More elaborated approaches like the Dutch Realistic
Mathematics Education use realistic problem contexts to introduce new concepts,
formalising only gradually. Thus to introduce logarithms, students are first
presented with the concept of exponential growth in several realistic contexts.
Only then are logarithms presented by interpreting the graph of a realistic
exponential function in reverse order (Webb et al. 2011). Following a teaching
experiment at a college, students claimed to have understood the meaning of
logarithms (ibid., p. 51). Whether they also had a better grasp of algebra is not
reported.
3.4 Conclusion
The many alternative teaching proposals on one hand (cf. 3.3), and the many
reported difficulties and mistakes on the other (3.2) suggest that it could be worth
reconsidering the traditional introduction of the concept (3.1). As mentioned
above, this paper takes the position that the indirect definition might be
responsible for many of the difficulties and mistakes, because learners cannot
immediately apply it. Here are some arguments:
1. Euler’s definition reflects a structural conception: Napier and Bürgi had a
certain understanding of the concept when they studied two juxtaposed
progressions. Although logarithms were recognised shortly thereafter to be
exponents, it took over a century until this interpretation ceased to be read as a
property and was used as a definition by Euler. In a way, it reflects the view of
an expert who, after a long period of research, retrospectively chose one of the
definitional properties as a definition, or, using Sfards terminoloy, it mirrors a
reification process that leads to a structural conception (1.2.1).
2. Later definitional properties are less operational: There are other ways of
defining and thus introducing the logarithm, for example as the solution of a
23
certain functional equation (2.1.3). However, this definition is purely
existential and thus even less operational than the Eulerian one. Although some
authors suggest this approach (Seebeck and Hummel 1959), it has been shown
to cause students great difficulty (Sajka 2003). With Kirsch, defining
logarithms as the solution of a functional equation is like “[…] putting the cart
before the horse, which serves only to make access more difficult […]” (1977,
p. 102).
3. Teaching logarithms is similar to teaching division: Although division can
logically be defined as inverse multiplication, it is — at least in the German-
speaking countries — not taught with such logical elegance. Instead, it is
conveyed through a wide range of contexts, namely as fair-sharing and
measurement and as repeated subtractions, leading to long division (2.2). The
ambition of these various approaches is to make division accessible, and to
convey an understanding of it as an autonomous object, possessing various
properties. Thus, since logarithms and division are mathematically analogous
(2.1.1), their teaching can be considered analogously as well: Introducing
logarithms in a more constructive way than as inverse exponents could
probably improve accessibility.
To my knowledge, this conclusion is not supported by any larger empirical study,
but is shared by other authors (3.3). Based on the theoretical construct of basic
models in the sense of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ (1.1), the next section will present
elements of a broader instructional approach that relies on multiple
interpretations, of which the interpretation as inverse exponent is only one.
4 Operational and structural basic models for
logarithms
If it is correct that the indirect definition is not a suitable starting point, this could
have implications for the teaching of logarithms: How, then, could the subject of
logarithms been structured differently?
Based on sections 1 and 2, it is possible to answer research question (Q1),
identifying four basic models for logarithms. All four will be shown to interpret
logarithms within the students’ world of experience in the sense of consolidated
school knowledge. To demonstrate their explanatory power, this section will show
how they can serve students as a basis for meaningful argumentation, i.e. how,
24
with their help, certain textbook tasks can be solved in a meaningful way and
certain typical mistakes and difficulties can be prevented (cf. 1.1.2). The
suggested courses of argumentation are of course prototypical ways of handling a
problem: They show one possible chain of argument. What is important here is
that learners are able to give similar reasons of their own that they find plausible.
Additionally, question (Q2) will be answered by elaborating the extent to which
each basic model can be used to explain logarithms operationally or structurally.
4.1 Logarithms as Multiplicative Measuring
Logarithms and divisions work analogously: While division asks for the number
of addends, logarithms look for the number of factors (cf. 2.1.2). In analogy to the
additive measurement model for division (1.1.4), multiplicative measuring is
proposed as a first basic model here. Reflecting Napier’s idea when he coined the
term “logarithm” and conceptualised it as number that counts divisions, a first
version of a basic model is as follows:
(BM1) The logarithm of a number
b
(to base
a
) indicates how often the base
a
fits into the number
b
as a factor. (Basic model of ‘multiplicative fitting-
into’)
Because counting the factors is equal to counting the divisions by the factor, it can
be slightly reformulated:
(BM1ʹ) The logarithm of a number
b
(to base
a
) indicates how often the number
b
has to be divided by the base
a
to produce
1
. (Basic model of ‘repeated
division’)
This version reflects the idea Burja and Schellbach have had in introducing their
notations modelled on division (2.1.4).
Mathematically, both interpretations are equivalent to the definition of logarithms
as inverse exponents because
ax
can be read as the product of
x
factors of
a
. At
first glance, (BM1) and (BM1ʹ) work for natural numbers
x
only, but this is not a
principal restriction, because there is the logarithm algorithm: Firstly, it reflects
the basic model of multiplicative measuring as it divides by the base
a
repeatedly; secondly, it extends multiplicative measuring as it determines the
decimals of logarithms to any desired accuracy (2.2).
25
4.1.1 Multiplicative measuring as a basic model
To what extent can (BM1) and (BM1ʹ) be considered basic models in the sense of
‘Grundvorstellungen’ (cf. 1.1)? Both interpretations of logarithms refer to a
measuring context, but without indicating any realistic manual actions. That is
why they cannot be considered primary basic models. Now, because students have
compared and measured many realistic things since childhood in everyday and
geometric situations, they are well acquainted with linear measuring. Based on
this concrete experience, and combined with the possibility of calculating
(certain) logarithms manually, the question “How often does the factor
a
fit into
b
multiplicatively?” is quite likely to sound reasonable to students, at least more
so than the question about finding an inverse exponent. In this sense, (BM1) and
(BM1ʹ) are secondary basic models (cf. 1.1.3).
The explanatory power of (BM1) and (BM1ʹ) lies in their prototypical nature; they
can be used to tackle and solve textbook tasks. For example, consider the
evaluation of
log21024
. Students are usually expected to solve it by guessing and
checking. With the aid of multiplicative measuring, however, it can be
reformulated as “How many times does 2 as a factor fit into 1024?” (BM1) or as
“How often does 1024 have to be divided by 2 to produce 1?” (BM1ʹ). Thanks to
this interpretation, the answer can now be determined step by step:
log21024 =10
. This chain of argument can be depicted in a similar manner to
figure 1 (fig. 4).
What is the base 2
logarithm of 1024?
1024 ÷2÷÷2=1024 ÷210 =1
!log21024 =10
(BM1, BM1ʹ)
Logarithms as
multiplicative measuring
How often does 2 as a factor fit into 1024?
How often does 1024 have to be divided by 2 to produce 1?
26
Fig. 4 Role of the basic model of multiplicative measuringas intermediary between two contexts
of logarithms
Many further problems can be solved by relating them to the context of
multiplicative measurement, such as the simplification of
loga1
,
logaa
,
logaa3
,
etc., or checking the correctness of
logaab=b
: By reformulating the problem as a
question of how many times one number fits into another as a factor, it can be
plausibly solved. Assertions like “
log37
is less than
log57
because 3 is less than
5” (Berezovski 2007, p. 109) should also occur less frequently with this basic
model: As 3 is smaller than 5, 3 fits into 7 more often than 5, regardless of any
calculations. Even expressions like
log20
or
log28
can be recognised as
incalculable with the aid of this basic model, because
0
and
8
can never be
converted to
1
through repeated division by
2
.
This basic model may perhaps even help to make the logarithmic law
loga(bc)=logab+logac
accessible: If
a
fits — as a factor —
m
times into
b
and
n
times into
c
, it has to fit exactly
m+n
times into the product
bc
.
Empirical investigation is of course needed to establish whether students find this
argument plausible or whether it is too sophisticated.
4.1.2 Multiplicative measuring, an operational conception
The basic model of multiplicative measuring can be conveyed to students with the
aid of the logarithm algorithm (cf. 2.2). This consists of a set of computational
rules that can be performed step by step. As such, it constructively generates
results, and makes an operational access to logarithms possible.
4.2 Logarithms as Counting the Number of Digits
For certain kinds of questions and arguments, it can be helpful when basic models
relate a concept to a typical application context and vice versa (cf. 1.1.3). For
example, in the decimal system, one million has seven digits, and its common
logarithm is 6. More generally, the number of digits of any natural number
n
(in
decimal notation) is equal to
log10 n
+1
, so the concept of logarithm can be
interpreted as a number of digits. This interpretation can be generalised to non-
27
natural numbers of digits: Because a power
10n
has
n+1
digits, any number
b
can be said to have the generalised number of digits
log10 b+1
(in decimal
notation). In a nutshell:7
(BM2) The (common) logarithm of a number
b
finds the number of digits of
b
,
minus one. (Basic model of ‘counting the number of digits’)
4.2.1 Counting the number of digits as a basic model
Unlike the basic model of multiplicative measuring (BM1), this interpretation
draws on a typical application context for logarithms, the number of digits.
Students at upper secondary school are familiar with it from early maths lessons,
because counting and positional notation is addressed and practised at length in
primary school.
The potential of this basic model lies not in relating a class of logarithmic
problems to the context of the number of digits but, conversely, in answering
certain questions with the help of logarithms. For instance, consider a task like
“How many digits are there in the power
22000
when written in decimal
notation?” (Deller et al. 2000, p. 78; translation by C.W.). Using basic model
(BM2), it can be expressed differently, leading to a logarithmic expression. Unlike
the example discussed above, the corresponding cycle of argumentation now
begins in a familiar context (fig. 5). In this specific task, one furthermore has to
utilise the logarithmic law
loga(bn)=nlogab
(or (BM3), cf. 4.3), while the
decimals of
log10 2
can in principle be computed by hand (cf. 2.2).
How many digits are there in the
power
22000
when written out in
decimal notation?
The number therefore
has 603 places.
(BM2)
Logarithms as
counting the number of digits
7 This interpretation can be further generalised to arguments written in any non-decimal base, by
changing the base of the logarithm. For example, the base 2 logarithm of a number
b
finds the
number of digits minus 1 in the binary expression of
b
.
28
log10 22000 =2000 log10 2602.06
Fig. 5 Role of the basic model of counting the number of digits’ as intermediary between two
contexts of logarithms
Other problems can also be solved with this basic model. For example, if tasked
with determining which of the numbers
25625
and
26620
is larger (Berezovski
2004), it is possible to compare the number of digits. Students familiar with this
basic model could be less likely to make statements such as “
25625
is bigger
because it has a bigger exponent” (ibid., p. 57). This is another claim that merits
empirical investigation.
4.2.2 Counting the number of digits, an operational conception
The basic models (BM1) and (BM2) are somewhat akin in that they both deal
with logarithms as numbers and place them in a familiar context. While the first
basic model refers to a constructive algorithm and thus emphasises the operational
side most strongly, the second basic model (BM2) does not. Instead, it deals with
an application of logarithms. So although this second basic model cannot be used
to explain logarithms operationally, it could be used to support operational
explanations in that it describes the effect it has on numbers. Because this differs
from mentioning the mathematical properties and relations of a concept, it cannot
be considered a form of structural explaining. In short, (BM2) can be considered
another operational basic model for logarithms as numbers, but to a lesser extent
than (BM1).
4.3 Logarithms as Decreasing the Hierarchy Level
While the law
logaab=b
is plausible on the grounds of the basic model of
multiplicative measuring (cf. 4.1), the logarithmic laws are not convincingly
evident on the basis of this alone. This requires a further basic model, not least to
avoid some of the algebraic difficulties involved in manipulating expressions and
solving exponential and logarithmic equations.
29
Arithmetic operations can be classified according to their hierarchy level:
Because, mathematically speaking, multiplication is — at least for natural
numbers — a repeated addition, it is a second-level operation (e.g., Oehl 1962).
Similarly, the operation of raising to a power is a repeated multiplication, which is
why it can be considered a third-level operation. Now, logarithms fulfil the
property
Φ(xy)=Φ(x)+Φ(y)
(
x>0
,
y>0
) (cf. 2.1.3). As a consequence,
logarithms lower not only second-level operations (multiplication, division) by
one hierarchy level, but also third-level operations (powers, roots). To be more
precise:
(BM3) The logarithm of an expression reduces multiplications and divisions to
additions and subtractions, and it reduces powers and roots to
multiplications and divisions. (Basic model of ‘decreasing the hierarchy
level’)
4.3.1 Decreasing the hierarchy level as a basic model
The third basic model relates to the hierarchy of arithmetic operations as a
familiar context. Seen in this light, (BM3) refers to previous school experience at
primary and lower secondary level, where, for instance, multiplication is
explained as repeated addition. With the aid of this basic model, it is possible to
solve tasks like “expand
log cd
as much as possible” (Deller et al. 2000, p. 74;
translation by C.W.). As in the above scenarios, a possible chain of reasoning
answers the original task by reformulating it in another logarithmic context (fig.
6).
Expand
log cd
as much as possible.
!!!log cd =12(log c+log d)
(BM3)
Logarithms as
decreasing the hierarchy level
Taking square roots as a third-level operation becomes dividing the logarithm
by two, and multiplication as a second-level operation becomes addition
between logarithms.
30
Fig. 6 Role of the basic model of ‘decreasing the hierarchy levelas intermediary between two
contexts of logarithms
This basic model can be used not only to solve similar problems, but maybe also
to make logarithmic laws like
loga(b÷c)=logablogac
plausible and thus more
accessible. There is some theoretical plausibility to the claim that this basic model
could prevent students from making algebraic mistakes such as expanding
logarithmic expressions like
log(x+y)
to
log x+log y
(cf. 3.2.1). It might even
enable them to give reasons why this transformation must be wrong: For the
expansion of
log(x+y)
to be possible, there would have to exist a somehow
“deeper” arithmetic operation than addition, which is a first-level operation.
4.3.2 Decreasing the hierarchy level, an operational conception
In contrast to the former basic models, the third basic model is geared not towards
numbers, but towards algebraic expressions. As such, it interprets logarithms as
operators. Similar to the second basic model (BM2), it does not explain
logarithms operationally, but it likewise provides information about the effect of
logarithms. This is why it is theoretically plausible that this basic model could
support the operational explanation of logarithms in the case of algebraic
expressions, but not their structural explanation. In short, (BM3) can also be
considered an operational basic model, but for logarithms as operators rather than
for logarithms as numbers.
4.4 Logarithms as Inverse Exponents
Euler’s understanding of logarithms is widely used in mathematics, so it would be
intellectually dishonest and irresponsible to withhold it from learners. It therefore
has to be considered as one of several interpretations of logarithms, analogously
to inverse multiplication as one of the basic models of division (cf. 1.1.4). It is the
last basic model for logarithms proposed here:
(BM4) The logarithm of an expression (to base
a
) is the exponent by which the
base
a
must be raised to yield the expression. (Basic model of ‘inverse
exponent’)
31
4.4.1 Inverse exponent as a basic model
With its focus on inversion, this basic model again draws on previous school
knowledge. Students have encountered several inverse phenomena in the course
of their maths lessons: positive and negative numbers, multiplication and division,
etc. Each aspect was experienced as a phenomenon in its own right before it was
logically related to its inverse (cf. 1.1.4).
With the aid of this basic model, the law
alogab=b
is now self-explanatory: As the
logarithm of
b
is an exponent, the base
a
raised by this exponent must be equal
to the original number
b
. This basic model obviously also indicates how to solve
exponential equations: Because an equation such as
2x=40
asks for an exponent,
the question can be translated into another logarithmic context, so the task is to
determine
log240
(fig. 7).
Solve
2x=40
to
three significant figures.
Therefore,
25.32 40
.
(BM4)
Logarithms as
inverse exponents
x=log240 5.32
Fig. 7 Role of the basic model of inverse exponentsas intermediary between two contexts of
logarithms
4.4.2 Inverse exponent, a structural conception
This basic model refers to another concept, the concept of exponents, whether
numbers or expressions. Unlike the previous basic models, it neither implies a
computation procedure (as (BM1) and (BM1ʹ) do) nor names the effects
logarithms can have (as (BM2) and (BM3) do), but rather relates a concept to an
object. This is the view of experts who have studied the operational aspects of the
concept and then reified their experiences to make propositions about states and
32
objects. In other words, the Eulerian property reflects the most structural
conception of logarithms to date. As such, it is less suitable as a starting point for
learners than as the objective of concept formation at school (cf. 3.4)
5 Discussion and outlook
5.1 Summary: Four Basic Models, and Two Hypotheses Regarding
their Relevance
This work identifies various basic models to foster the learning of logarithms. The
main underlying theoretical construct is that of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ (Griesel
1971; vom Hofe 1998; vom Hofe et al. 2006; Oehl 1962). As such, it favours a
multi-perspective approach to mathematical concepts — i.e., that a mathematical
concept must be taught using a variety of different basic models —, an approach
that is known from the teaching of division (Brown 1981; Fosnot and Dolk 2001;
Greer 1992; Padberg and Benz 2011; Radatz et al. 1998). To theoretically
improve the accessibility of the logarithmic concept, the construct of
‘Grundvorstellungen’ is combined with another theoretical construct, namely that
of of operationality and structurality, which emphasises the importance of
operational approaches in the learning of new mathematical concepts (Sfard 1991,
2008).
The paper focuses on logarithms as numbers or operators. First, it begins by
unpacking some mathematical knowledge: Logarithms can be conceptualised as
inverse exponents (i.e. structurally). But they are more than just the opposite of
another concept;
logab
can also be seen as the number of factors
a
in
b
and thus
operationally. Because the indirect definition still prevails in algebra textbooks
despite the many problems reported in educational literature, it may be an
underlying cause of learners’ difficulties. This position is in keeping with other
authors (Fermsjö 2014; Mulqueeny 2012; Smith and Confrey 1994; Weber 2002;
Williams 2011).
The paper therefore develops several basic models in the sense of
‘Grundvorstellungen’: the basic model of ‘multiplicative measuring’ (BM1), the
basic model of ‘counting the number of digits’ (BM2), the basic model of
‘decreasing the hierarchy level’ (BM3), and the basic model of ‘inverse exponent’
(BM4). They all support meaningful mathematical reasoning, because they have a
33
certain explanatory power when it comes to argumentation or solutions to specific
tasks. All of them vary not only in terms of their mathematical character (number,
operator), but also in terms of their process-object character. They can therefore
be structured as follows:
· On one hand, (BM1), (BM1ʹ) and (BM2) refer to logarithms as numbers.
(BM3), however, refers to logarithms as operators, while (BM4) refers to both
numbers and operators.
· On the other hand, (BM1) and (BM1ʹ) are more closely related to the
operational conception of logarithms (as they generate result), whereas (BM4)
is more closely related to its structural conception (as it refers to another
concept). The remaining two basic models, (BM2) and (BM3), can potentially
be useful to bridge operational and structural conceptions of logarithms,
emphasizing the effects of logarithms.
The presentation of basic models for logarithms in this paper goes hand in hand
with hypotheses. In short, I claim that the interplay between the four basic models
can make logarithms accessible and foster understanding. This means: They
endow learners with a solid basis for argumentation, firstly to solve tasks and to
remember laws, and secondly to reduce mistakes and difficulties. Various
theoretical arguments are given in support of these two hypotheses.
5.2 Implications for Future Research
The primary purpose of this paper is to suggest elements of an approach that
makes logarithms accessible and meaningful for students. Many arguments are
made at a theoretical level here without a specific focus on empirical evidence.
However, preliminary observations based on my teaching experience suggest that
the four basic models may have the potential to help students generate meaning
about logarithms (Weber 2013). As encouraging as the corresponding indications
may appear, empirical research is needed to investigate the affordances and the
limitations of the basic models introduced here. For example: Does the plurality
of basic models for logarithms foster or hinder understanding? Is the sequence of
the basic models of importance for the teaching and learning of the concept? Is it
advisable, as Sfard and others suggest (cf. 1.2.1), to introduce logarithms using
the most operational conception (BM1), gradually moving to the most structural
one (BM4), or is it not advisable (cf. Gilmore and Inglis 2008)? A further concern
34
is that the multiple interpretations offered to students may not be reified (cf. Sfard
1991; 2008) as a new, single entity, but that learners may instead use them in a
compartmentalised fashion. Qualitative case studies, and quantitative large-scale
implementations, can help to explore and answer questions like these, and thus to
investigate the above hypotheses.
Secondly, the implications for teaching are not clarified for either school or
teacher education. For example, it is not obvious in what way and in which order
the various models have to be taught so that they can become reified in a single
object, the logarithm. Again, one could take inspiration in the teaching of division,
whether from textbooks for pupils (e.g., Wittmann and Müller 2004) or for pre-
service teachers (e.g., Padberg and Benz 2011; Radatz et al. 1998). Future
investigation must clarify to what extent such knowledge about instructing
primary-level pupils and teachers can be transferred to secondary-level students
and teachers, respectively. This is the second field where empirical research is
indispensable.
In summary, making logarithms accessible is a multilayered task. The work
reported here can be considered a first step towards this objective, laying some
theoretical foundations for further investigation.
References
Andelfinger, B. (1985). Exponentielle und logarithmische Probleme. In B. Andelfinger (Ed.),
Didaktischer Informationsdienst Mathematik (pp. 227231). Soest: Landesinstitut für
Schule und Weiterbildung.
Ball, D., Thames, M., & Phelps, G. (2008). Content knowledge for teaching: what makes it
special? Journal of Teacher Education, 59 (5), 389407.
Bauersfeld, H. (1983). Subjektive Erfahrungsbereiche als Grundlage einer Interaktionstheorie des
Mathematiklernens und -lehrens. In H. Bauersfeld et al. (Eds.), Analysen zum
Unterrichtshandeln (pp. 156). Köln: Aulis Deubner.
Bennhardt, D. (2009). Der Logarithmus: tradierte Fachbegriffe oder sinnstiftende Kreatitivität?
Praxis der Mathematik in der Schule, 51 (10), 4445.
Berezovski, T. (2004). An inquiry into high school students’ understanding of logarithms.
Unpublished master thesis. Simon Fraser University.
Berezovski, T. (2007). Exposing pre-service secondary mathematics teachers’ knowledge through
new research designed methodology. Unpublished dissertation thesis. Simon Fraser
University.
35
Blum, W. (2002). On the role of ‘Grundvorstellungen’ for reality-related proofs examples and
reflections. In M. A. Mariotti (Ed.), Proceedings of the Third Conference of the European
Society for Research in Mathematics Education. Università di Pisa.
Brennan, M. (2007). The role of language in the demise of logs. The Irish Math Teacher
Association Newsletter, 107, 4451.
Brown, M. (1981). Number operations. In K. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of
Mathematics: 1116 (pp. 2347). London: Murray.
Cajori, F. (1913). History of the exponential and logarithmic concepts. The American
Mathematical Monthly, 20 (1), 514, 20 (2), 3547, 20 (4), 107117.
Cajori, F. (1952). A history of mathematical notation, vol. 2. Chicago: Open Court.
Carter, J.A. (2012): Algebra 2. Columbus, OH: Glencoe & McGraw-Hill.
Chua, B., & Wood, E. (2005). Working with logarithms: students’ misconceptions and errors. The
Mathematics Educator, 8, 2, 5370.
Clark, K. (2006). Investigating teachers’ experiences with the history of logarithms: a collection of
five case studies. Unpublished dissertation. University of Maryland.
Deller, H., Gebauer, P., & Zinn, J. (2000): Algebra 2 Aufgaben. Zürich: Orell Füssli.
Van Dooren, W., de Bock, D., Janssens, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2008). The linear imperative. An
inventory and conceptual analysis of students’ overuse of linearity. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 39 (3), 311342.
Dubinsky, E. (1991). Reflective abstraction in advanced mathematical thinking. In D. O. Tall
(Ed.), Advanced mathematical thinking (pp. 95123). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Dubinsky, E., & McDonald, M. A. (2002). APOS: A constructivist theory of learning in
undergraduate mathematics education research. In D. Holton et al. (Hrsg.), The teaching
and learning of mathematics at university level (pp. 275282). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Espedal, B. (2015). En meningsfull tilnærming til logaritmer En designstudie om introduksjon av
logaritmer gjennom repetert divisjon. Unpublished master thesis. University of Agder.
Euler, L. (1822). Elements of algebra. London: Longman, Orme (Original German edition: 1765).
Fermsjö, R. (2014). Exploring logarithms using number lines. In S. Oesterle, C. Nicol, P.
Liljedahl, & D. Allan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Joint Meeting of PME 38 and PME-NA 36
(vol. 6, p. 308). Vancouver: PME.
Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Fischbein, E. (1989). Tacit models and mathematical reasoning. For the learning of mathematics,
9 (2), 914.
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young mathematicians at work: Constructing multiplication and
division. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Greer, B. (1992). Multiplication and division as models of situations. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.),
Handbook for research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 276295). New York:
Macrnillan.
Gilmore, C., & Inglis, M. (2008). Process- and object-based thinking in arithmetic. In O. Figueras,
J. L. Cortina, S. Alatorre, T. Rojana & A. Sepulveda (Eds.), Proceedings of the Joint
Meeting of PME 32 and PME-NA XXX (vol. 3, pp. 7380). Morelia: Cinvestav-UMSNH.
36
Goldberg, M. (2006). Computing logarithms digit-by-digit, International Journal of Mathematical
Education in Science and Technology, 37 (1), 109114.
Gray, E. M., & Tall, D. (1994). Duality, ambiguity and flexibility: A „proceptual“ view of simple
arithmetic. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25 (1), 116140.
Griesel, H. (1971). Mengen, Zahlen, Relationen, Topologie: Die Neue Mathematik für Lehrer und
Studenten, vol. 1, Hannover: Schroedel.
Griesel, H., & Postel, H., & Suhr, F. (2004). Elemente der Mathematik 10. Braunschweig:
Schroedel.
Hammack, R., & Lyons, D. (1995). A simple way to teach logarithms. The Mathematics Teacher,
88 (5), 374375.
Hanrahan, V., Matthews, J., Porkess, R., & Secker, P. (2004). AS Pure Mathematics Core 1 &
Core 2. London: Hodder Murrays.
Hirsch, C., Fey, J., Hart, E., Schoen, H., & Watkins, A. (2008). Core-Plus Mathematics:
Contemporary Mathematics in Context, Course 2. New York, NY: Glencoe / McGrawHill.
Vom Hofe, R. (1998). On the generation of basic ideas and individual images: Normative,
descriptive and constructive aspects. In A. Sierpinska, & J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Mathematics
Education as a Research Domain: A Search for Identity. London: Kluwer, 317331.
Vom Hofe, R., & Blum, W. (2016). “Grundvorstellungen” as a Category of Subject-Matter
Didactics. Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik, doi: 10.1007/s13138-016-0107-3 (in this
themed issue)
Vom Hofe, R., Kleine, M., Blum, W., & Pekrun, R. (2006). The effect of mental models
(„Grundvorstellungen“) for the development of mathematical competencies. In M. Bosch
(Ed.), Proceedings oft he Fourth Congress of the European Society for Research in
Mathematics Education (pp. 142151). Universitat Ramon Llull.
Holliday, B., Cuevas, G., Moore-Harris, B., Carter, J., Marks, D., Casey, R., Day, R., & Hayek, L.
(2005). Glencoe Mathematics: Algebra 2, California edition. New York, NY: Glencoe /
McGraw-Hill.
Hurwitz, M. (1999). We have liftoff! Introducing the logarithmic function. The Mathematics
Teacher, 92 (4), 344345.
Jordan, A. (2006). Mathematische Bildung von Schülern am Ende der Sekundarstufe 1 Analysen
und empirische Untersuchungen. Hildesheim: Franzbecker.
Katz, V. (1997). Some ideas on the use of history in the teaching of mathematics. For the
Learning of Mathematics, 17 (1), 62 63.
Kaur, B., & Boey, H. (1994). Algebraic misconceptions of first year college students. Focus on
Learning Problems in Mathematics, 16 (4), 4358.
Kenney, R. (2005). Students’ understanding of logarithmic function notation. In G. Lloyd, M.
Wilson, J. Wilkins, & S. Behm (Eds.) (2005). Proceedings of the 27th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education (8 pages). Roanoke, VA: PME-NA
37
Kirsch, A. (1977). Aspects of simplification in mathematics teaching. In H. Athen & H. Kunle
(Eds.), Proceedings of the third international congress on mathematical education (pp. 98
119). Karlsruhe: Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik.
Kirshner, D., & Awtry, T. (2004). Visual Salience of Algebraic Transformations. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 35 (4), 224257.
Klein, F. (1932). Elementary mathematics from an advanced standpoint: Arithmetic algebra
analysis. London: MacMillan (Original German edition: 1908).
Kleine, M., Jordan, A., & Harvey, E. (2005). With the focus on GrundvorstellungenPart 1: a
theoretical integration into current concepts. ZDM Mathematical Education, 37, 226-233.
Kluepfel, C. (1981). When are logarithms used? Mathematics Teacher, 74 (4), 250253.
Kuypers, W., Lauter, J., & Wuttke, H. (1995). Mathematik 10. Schuljahr. Berlin: Cornelsen.
Lee, F., & Heyworth, R. (1999). Errors due to misperception and the default-value model. In G.
Cumming, T. Okamoto, & L. George (Eds.), Advanced research in computers and
communications in education (pp. 228231). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
Leopold, D. G., & Edgar, B. (2008). Degree of Mathematics Fluency and Success in Second-
Semester Introductory Chemistry. Journal of Chemical Education, 85 (5), 724–731.
Lergenmüller, A., & Schmidt, G. (2004). Mathematik Neue Wege 10. Braunschweig: Schroedel.
Leuders, T., Hußmann, S., Barzel, B., & Prediger, S. (2011). “Das macht Sinn!” Sinnstiftung mit
Kontexten und Kernideen. Praxis der Mathematik in der Schule, 37, 29.
Matz, M. (1982). Towards a process model for high school algebra errors. In D. Sleeman, & J. S.
Brown (Eds.), Intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 2550). London: Academic Press.
Matzka, W. (1850). Elementarlehre von den Logarithmen. Prag: Calve’sche Buchhandlung.
Mulqueeny, E. (2012). How do students acquire an understanding of logarithmic concepts?
Unpublished dissertation thesis. Kent State University.
Murdock, J., Kamischke, E., & Kamischke, E. (2010). Discovering advanced algebra an
investigative approach. Emeryville, CA: Key Curriculum.
Neill, H., & Quadling, D. (2000). Pure Mathematics 1 & 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Oehl, W. (1962). Der Rechenunterricht in der Grundschule. Hannover: Schroedel.
Ostler, E. (2013). Exploring logarithms with a ruler. Mathematics Teacher, 106 (9), 669673.
Padberg, F., & Benz, C. (2011). Didaktik der Arithmetik. Heidelberg: Springer
Panagiotou, E. (2011). Using history to teach mathematics: the case of logarithms. Science
Education, 20 (1), 135.
DePierro, E., & Garafalo, F. (2008). Helping students make sense of logarithms and logarithmic
relations. Journal of Chemical Education, 85 (9), 12261228.
Prediger, S. (2009). Verstehen durch Vorstellen. In T. Leuders, L. Hefendehl-Hebeker, & H.-G.
Weigand (Eds.), Mathemagische Momente (pp. 166175). Berlin:Cornelsen.
Radatz, H., Schipper, W., Dröge, R., & Ebeling, A. (1998). Handbuch für den
Mathematikunterricht: 2. Schuljahr. Hannover: Schroedel.
Rahn, J., & Berndes, B. (1994). Logarithms to Explore Power and Exponential Functions. The
Mathematics Teacher, 87 (3), 161170.
38
Sajka, M. (2003). A secondary school student’s understanding of the concept of functiona case
study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 53 (3), 229254.
Sandifer E. (2007). Finding logarithms by hand. In E. Sandifer (Ed.), How Euler did it (pp.121
126). Washington, D.C.: The Mathematical Association of America.
Seebeck, C., & Hummel, P. (1959). Logarithmic and exponential functions a direct approach.
The Mathematics Teacher, 52 (6), 439443.
Senk, S., & Thompson, D. (2006). Strategies used by second-year algebra students to solve
problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37 (2), 116128.
Selter, C., Prediger, S., Nührenbörger, M., & Hußmann, S. (2012). Taking away and determining
the differencea longitudinal perspective on two models of subtraction and the inverse
relation to addition. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79 (3), 389408.
Sfard, A. (1991). On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: Reflections on processes and
objects as different sides of the same coin. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22, 136.
Sfard, A. (2008). Thinking as communicating: Human development, the growth of discourses and
mathematizing. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Skemp, R. (1978). Relational understanding and instrumental understanding. Arithmetic Teacher,
26, 915.
Smith, E., & Confrey, J. (1994). Multiplicative structures and the development of logarithms: what
was lost by the invention of functions? In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of
multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 331360). Albany, NY: State
University of New York Press.
Stoll, C. (2006). When slide rules ruled. Scientific American, 5, 8087.
Stölting, P. (2008). Die Entwicklung funktionalen Denkens in der Sekundarstufe 1 vergleichende
Analysen und empirische Studien zum Mathematikunterricht in Deutschland und
Frankreich. Unpublished dissertation thesis. Universität Regensburg.
Sträßer, R. (2014). Stoffdidaktik in mathematics education. In S. Lerman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Mathematics Education (pp. 566570). Dordrecht: Springer.
Tall, D., & Vinner, S. (1991). Concept image and concept definition in mathematics with
particular reference to limits and continuity. Educational studies in mathematics, 12 (2),
151169.
Toumasis, C. (1993). Teaching logarithms via their history. School Science and Mathematics, 93
(8), 428434.
Usiskin, Z. (1991). Building mathematics curricula with applications and modelling. In M. Niss,
W. Blum, & I. Huntley (Eds.), Teaching of mathematical modelling and applications (pp.
3045). Chichester: Horwood.
Watters, D., & Watters, J. (2006). Student understanding of pH. Biochemistry and Molecular
Biology Education, 34 (4), 278284.
Webb, D., van der Kooij, H., Geist, M. (2011). Design research in the Netherlands: Introducing
logarithms using realistic mathematics education. Journal of Mathematics Education at
Teachers College, 2, 4752.
39
Weber, C. (2013). Grundvorstellungen zum Logarithmus Bausteine für einen verständlichen
Unterricht. In H. Allmendinger, K. Lengnink, A. Vohns, & G. Wickel (Eds.), Mathematik
verständlich unterrichten (pp. 7998), Wiesbaden: Spektrum.
Weber, K. (2002). Developing students’ understanding of exponents and logarithms. In D.
Mewborn, P. Sztajn, D. White, H. Wiegel, R. Bryant, & K. Nooney (Eds.), Proceedings of
the twenty-fourth annual meeting of the North American Chapter of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (vol. 1, pp. 10191027). Columbus,
OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.
Williams, H. (2011). A conceptual framework for student understanding of logarithms.
Unpublished master thesis. Brigham Young University.
Wittmann, E., & Müller, G. (2004). Das Zahlenbuch 2. Leipzig: Klett.
Wood, E. (2005). Understanding logarithms. Teaching Mathematics and its Applications, 24 (4),
2005.
Yen, R. (1999). Reflections on higher school certificate examinations: learning from their
mistakes, HSC 1998. Reflections, 24 (3), 38.
... We start with the 'Basic Models' (Vom Hofe et al. 2006, cited by Weber, 2016 using a theoretical construction that describes how mathematical concepts can be made understandable for students based on different interpretations; the study of the Basic Models of Logarithms (Weber, 2016(Weber, , 2017 and the review of the state of the art of this topic. A proposal is made for the training of mathematics teachers that answers the question: What can be an alternative Hypothetical Learning Path of the logarithmic function that allows future teachers to interact with the results of research in mathematics education and deconstruct the concept of logarithmic function? ...
... We take verbatim the following Basic Models (BM) proposed in Weber (2016;2017): ...
... In this sense, Models (BM1) and (BM1') are more closely related to the operational conception of logarithms, since they generate results, while (MB4) is more closely related to their structural conception and their structure refers to another concept, that of exponent. Weber (2016) highlights that, from an epistemological point of view, conceiving logarithmic functions as inverse functions of exponentials reflects the structural vision of experts who have reified their experiences. As for the two remaining Basic Models (BM2) and (BM3), they can be potentially useful to unite the operational and structural conceptions of logarithms, emphasizing the effects of logarithms. ...
Article
Full-text available
La enseñanza y el aprendizaje de la función logarítmica ha sido objeto de estudio en Educación Matemática, evidenciándose diversas dificultades en la comprensión de este concepto tanto por parte de los docentes como de los estudiantes, las cuales han sido detectadas o analizadas a través de entrevistas y propuestas de trabajo en el aula, entre otros. En este caso se procede desde la necesidad de intervenciones que impliquen directamente a los profesores, se retoman las ideas desde la didacta de la matemática respecto a los Modelos Básicos y con la hipótesis que subyace, concerniente a que el enfoque único en la enseñanza de la función logarítmica como la inversa de la función exponencial, no es suficiente y además éste puede coadyuvar a generar dificultades en su aprendizaje. Así, en este proyecto al abordar la formación de profesores, se elabora una Trayectoria Hipotética de Aprendizaje, de manera que ellos tengan la oportunidad de deconstruir este concepto en su proceso de formación. Aprovechando los resultados de indagaciones en educación matemática, a partir de una investigación documental y con perspectiva fenomenológica, se procesan datos cualitativos con un análisis descriptivo interpretativo recurriendo a la noción de Trayectoria Hipotética de Aprendizaje. En esta Trayectoria Hipotética de deconstrucción del concepto se proponen objetivos, tareas, preguntas y experiencias; con una exigencia cognitiva que les permita a los profesores en ejercicio o en formación inicial, caracterizar estas funciones mediante diversos elementos matemáticos. Así, a partir del estudio y análisis de las publicaciones de la comunidad académica, se seleccionan y se hace uso en parte de las propuestas y hallazgos, que de acuerdo con los análisis de los investigadores abonan una posibilidad de que el profesor pueda transitar por los diferentes Modelos Básicos y al mismo tiempo los investigadores estén insertos tangencialmente en el rol de curadores en educación.
... Its history and uses in mathematics date back to 1634 when the concept was developed by a Scottish mathematician by name John Napier (Smith, 2000). Even though the concept constantly changes (Villarreal-Calderon, 2012), logarithm uses include to compare, measure, forecast, explain, illustrate, and interpret values (Weber, 2016), and importantly, it can be useful to model quantitative relationships, whereby students are "supported in conceptualizing quantities, their relationships and how they vary together" (Kuper & Carlson, 2019, p. 2). Thus, as part of subject competency, logarithm form key aspect of useful PMT content knowledge. ...
... Logarithm has a reputation for being difficult (Dintarini, 2018). This is probably because of its presentation as the inverse of exponential functions (Weber, 2016). Students perceive logarithm as irrelevant and confuse exponential and logarithmic laws (Campo-Meneses et al., 2021) and they tend to skip steps or fail to understand the meaning of the concept. ...
Article
Full-text available
Pre-service mathematics teachers’ (PMTs) subject competency continues to engage scholars and researchers. Understanding level of knowledge of concepts that PMTs bring to their learning in university is crucial to developing their teacher knowledge. This article examines genetic decomposition of schemas PMTs in one university in South Africa build (to know about rules) for solving logarithmic equations. A mixed methods approach, and the action-process-object-schema (APOS) theory were employed to examine mental construction the 19 purposively selected PMTs that responded to a 90-minute simple logarithm research task (LRT) made while solving problems. Analysis of task scripts using percentage score forms the basis of the qualitative phase of the research. Individual interview was useful to elicit PMTs’ views and perceptions of their encountered difficulties in solving LRT problems. One common difficulty was proving the logarithmic equation. This highlights gaps in PMTs’ prior knowledge of logarithmic concepts and basic rules. Implications of the findings for PMT subject competency were discussed.
... A logaritmus fogalmának megértése számos nehézséget okoz, ahogyan azt több kutató is megállapította (Chua & Wood, 2005;Kelecsényi et al., 2023;Kenney & Kastberg, 2013;Mulqueeny, 2012;Weber, 2016;Williams, 2011). A szokásos tankönyvek hagyományosan Euler indirekt definíciója alapján mutatják be a logaritmust: ...
Article
In our research, we investigated how we can categorise the errors made by first-year university students in logarithmic tasks. We conducted a survey among first-year engineering students of computer science and logistics engineering at Neumann János University. A short self-made test was used for the research. Logarithmic errors were categorized into classes based on a two-dimensional categorization, the type of error and its possible source. We found that the two-dimensional categorization was able to classify most of the typical logarithmic errors listed in the literature, as well as new errors. As a result of our investigation, it became clear that it is not possible to categorise the types of occurring errors without identifying the underlying sources of these errors. As a general suggestion for similar studies, it is worthwhile to use multidimensional categorization, where one aspect tries to reveal the underlying source or problem solving strategy in addition to the visible error type.
... Heutzutage gibt es eine Vielzahl didaktischer Arbeiten, die sich in der Tradition Kleins mit komplexen mathematischen Begriffen beschäftigen. Untersuchungen befassen sich beispielsweise mit Grundvorstellungen zu Variablen (Malle und Wittmann 1993), Funktionen (Stölting 2008, Logarithmen (Weber 2016), zu Grenzwerten, Ableitungen und Integralen (Blum und Kirsch 1996;Greefrath et al. 2016 Konstruktiver Aspekt: Der Unterschied zwischen den tatsächlich ausgebildeten Vorstellungen und den normativ intendierten Grundvorstellungen lässt sich auf unterschiedliche Weise erklären. Eine zentrale Frage der didaktischen Forschung liegt darin, worauf diese Divergenzen zurückzuführen sind und wie sie sich beheben lassen (vgl. ...
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung Zu den allgemeinen Grundvorstellungen zum Funktionsbegriff zählen die Zuordnungsvorstellung , die Kovariationsvorstellung und die Objektvorstellung . Diese Vorstellungen können auf alle Funktionsklassen gleichermaßen angewendet werden. In jeder Funktionsklasse sind diese Vorstellungen allerdings mit charakteristischen Sachzusammenhängen sowie Denk- und Handlungsweisen verbunden, die wiederum als Ausgangspunkt neuer, differenzierter Grundvorstellungen dienen können. In dem vorliegenden Artikel wird eine Vorgehensweise vorgestellt, mit der eine Ausdifferenzierung der allgemeinen Grundvorstellungen zum Funktionsbegriff durchgeführt und funktionsklassenspezifische Grundvorstellungen hergeleitet werden können. Grundlage des Verfahrens bildet eine didaktisch orientierte Sachanalyse in Verbindung mit der didaktischen Phänomenologie Freudenthals und Aspekten der Genese mathematischer Begriffe. Angewendet wird dieses Verfahren schließlich auf die Funktionsklasse der trigonometrischen Funktionen, wodurch sechs normative Grundvorstellungen zum Sinus identifiziert werden.
... The concept of logarithms provides a challenge for many students [15]- [17]. The card set was designed to be more challenging than the previous ones, as this was the last in the series of decks. ...
Article
We present the game Math-teur, a mathematical card game used successfully at the John von Neumann University for teaching remedial mathematics as part of a complex university catch-up program. In the paper we describe the game itself and summarize the considerations used for the game design.
... (Ganesan & Dindyal, 2014) stated that students' errors in logarithms can be related to their understanding of the topic which implies knowledge of their mathematical thinking and mental constructs. (Weber, 2016) focuses on logarithms as numbers or operators, where logarithms can be conceptualized as inverse exponents (i.e. structurally). ...
Article
Full-text available
This research aims to determine the factors that cause students' difficulties in understanding the concept of logarithms using the factor analysis method. The samples taken were 120 class X students of SMA N 2 Percontohan Karang Baru Aceh Tamiang. Data was obtained using diagnostic test instruments for logarithmic material based on aspects of logarithms as objects, as processes, as functions, and in contextual problems. From these four aspects, Author 2 was able to identify 20 variables that caused students difficulty in understanding the concept of logarithms. The research results show that there are three factors that cause students' difficulties in understanding the concept of logarithms. These factors are (a) logarithmic expressions (b) logarithmic functions, and (c) logarithmic calculations. The logarithmic expression factor is the dominant factor which has an eigenvalue of 10.282 which is able to explain a variance value of 54.115%.
... Weber [18] also highlighted how inadequate pupils' comprehension is and how challenging it is for them to grasp the material. Weber created exercises and assignments to help students grasp concepts better. ...
Article
Full-text available
The motive of the study was to find factors that influence the teaching and learning of logarithms at the senior High schools in the Kwahu East Municipality. The study employed a descriptive survey where a questionnaire was administered to Eighty-Six (86) teachers. The sample was selected using the Census method. After the SPSS software was used to analyse the data, the descriptive analysis revealed that most teachers positively perceive logarithms. Factors influencing Senior High School mathematics teachers' teaching of logarithm were identified. Teachers believe that suitable teaching methods can influence students' perceptions. Strategies to encourage positive perceptions should be promoted among teachers, and recommendations include developing innovative methods for teaching logarithms in Senior High School mathematics.
... Heutzutage gibt es didaktische Arbeiten, die sich auf normativer und deskriptiver Ebene mit Inhalten der Sekundarstufe II beschäftigen. Untersuchungen befassen sich mit Grundvorstellungen zu Variablen (Malle & Wittmann 1993), Funktionen (Stölting 2008), Logarithmen (Weber 2016), zu Grenzwerten, Ableitungen und Integralen (Greefrath et al. 2016), zum Sinus (Frohn & Salle 2017), zu Gleichungen (Hischer 2020) (Greefrath et al. 2016, S. 17) Passende Phänomene zu einem mathematischen Begriff zu finden, die diesen zugänglich machen, wird umso schwieriger, je komplexer der mathematische Begriff ist. Das hat zur Folge, dass Grundvorstellungen im Primarbereich wesentlich elaborierter und besser durchdrungen sind als in der höheren Mathematik. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study is a qualitative multiple-case study research aimed to analyze the students' conceptual understanding of logarithms using APOS (Action-Process-Object-Schema) theory. Using the framework, we analyzed interview results of eight math students with varying performance levels. It revealed that despite a common progression of understanding of logarithms, two important APOS mechanisms show the point of divergence among performance groups. One is the efficient transformation of logarithms to exponents (interiorization), and another is the relation of the logarithm process with other important processes such as functions and equations (coordination). Both mechanisms are important to see logarithm as an object, more than an operation needed to be solved by the student. This result provides insights for teachers in developing APOS-based lessons that can be implemented in the classroom.
Article
We have taught logarithms in high school and college algebra and in introductory calculus courses. Before we began using the method of teaching logs described in this article, we found that many students had difficulties mastering the concept, more so than with other functions. Other teachers reported similar experiences.
Article
Power functions and exponential functions often describe the relationship between variables in physical phenomena. Power functions are equations of the form y = kx ⁿ (see fig. 1 ), where k is a nonzero real number and n is a nonzero real number not equal to 1. Exponential functions are equations of the form y = kb x (see fig. 2 ), where k is a nonzero real number and b is a positive real number. Students should be able visually to recognize these functions so that they can easily identify their appearance when experimental data are graphed. When physical phenomena appear to describe exponential and power functions, logarithms can be used to locate approximate functions that represent the phenomena.
Article
With the widespread use of pocket calculators, no one would use a table of logarithms to multiply or raise a number to a power. Of course, the calculator raises numbers to arbitrary powers by using logarithms internally, but why should students learn logarithms if they don't plan to design calculators? Certainly the presence of the log key on many calculators will do away entirely with the need for tables of logarithms. But when is the log key to be used?
Article
The Curriculum and Evaluation of Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM 1989) targeted the concept of function as one that should receive increased attention. A function can be defined as a rule that assigns to every input value exactly one output value. To make it easier for novices to recognize a function, we should select the rule, the input, and the output to be straightforward.
Article
A direct approach to logarithms rather than the indirect approach as an inverse of an exponential function has many advantages for teaching secondary school students.
Article
One of the central issues that has long captivated research efforts in mathematical education concerns the question of what mental representations people have of mathematical content. That is, what meaning do people associate with mathematical content, and what role do these mental representations play in teaching and learning mathematics? To this end, a number of different categories concerning mental representations have been developed, including, e. g. “intuition”, “use meaning” or “concept image”. One approach, stemming from the German Stoffdidaktik (subject-matter didactics) tradition, is constituted by the didactic category of Grundvorstellung(en). In this contribution we begin by investigating the origins and the development of the concept of Grundvorstellung in the German-speaking research literature on mathematics education. Then, we summarize the main ideas of the concept, particularly concerning its normative, descriptive, and constructive use as well as the distinction between primary and secondary Grundvorstellungen. Following this, a number of typical areas of application will be considered. We will then discuss the use of Grundvorstellungen in the construction and classification of items for quantitative assessment, as well as the role of Grundvorstellungen as a descriptive and explanatory category in empirical classroom research.
Thesis
Les Dépendances fonctionnelles sont omniprésentes dans la vie quotidiennes, mais les résultats de plusieurs études montrent que les élèves ont des difficultés à traiter des problèmes de ce domaine. Ce travail compare l'approche des dépendances fonctionnelles en France et en Allemagne, à l'exemple de la Bavière.Dans la première partie la pensée fonctionnelle est définie clairement et est reliée aux cadres théoriques pertinents utilisés en France et en Allemagne, comme les Grundvorstellungen (Vom Hofe), les registres sémiotiques(Duval) ou le concept image (Vinner). Les outils nécessaires pour les analyses de ce travail y sont également développésLes chapitres suivants sont dédiés à la comparaison des programmes et des livres de classe. Le but est d'éclaircir dans quelle mesure les élèves des deux pays sont aidés dans le développement de la pensée fonctionnelle.Puis il est essayé d'identifier en pratique les points forts et les points faibles reconnus lors des analyses des curriculums. Deux approches différentes ont été choisies pour étudier comment les élèves travaillent avec la pensée fonctionnelle et quels problèmes surgissent lors de ce travail: - des analyses quantitatives des données de PISA et PALMA sont faites et amènent à démontrer certaines relations avec les résultats des analyses précédentes.-Les analyses quantitatives d'une étude d'interviews réalisé en France et en Allemagne documentant certains points forts et points faibles identifiés auparavant. Finalement des propositions sont faites sur la bases des résultats de ce travail pour essayer d'éviter les points faibles des deux pays et pour chercher à profiter des points forts.
Chapter
According to present-day knowledge, the process of mathematical problem solving, even at the university level, is always combined with intuitive images and assumptions which unconsciously affect the outcome. This has been shown, in particular, by Fischbein, in several investigations (1979, 1983, 1987, 1989, 1990). There seems to be no thinking without images, in this sense. At best these images can affect mathematical thinking positively. But they can also lead us astray — as Fischbein has shown as well — if inadequate images become fixed as unconsciously effective ‘tacit models’. Therefore the question arises how to deal with this intuitive level. For instance, does one assume that adequate images develop automatically with an appropriate formal contact with mathematics, or does one consciously follow and support the generation of adequate images and ideas?