Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87; doi:10.3390/socsci6030087 www.mdpi.com/journal/socsci
Article
Assessing Trust and Effectiveness in Virtual Teams:
Latent Growth Curve and Latent Change
Score Models
Michael D. Coovert 1,* Evgeniya E. Pavlova Miller 2 and Winston Bennett, Jr. 3
1 Department of Psychology, University of South Florida; Tampa, FL 33620, USA
2 State Farm Mutual Automobile Insurance Company, Atlanta, GA 30346, USA; jenny.pavlova@gmail.com
3 Air Force Research Laboratory, Airman Systems Directorate; Dayton, OH 45433, USA;
winston.bennett@us.af.mil
* Correspondence: coovert@mail.usf.edu; Tel.: +1-813-974-0482
Received: 5 June 2017; Accepted: 28 July 2017; Published: 2 August 2017
Abstract: Trust plays a central role in the effectiveness of work groups and teams. This is the case
for both face-to-face and virtual teams. Yet little is known about the development of trust in virtual
teams. We examined cognitive and affective trust and their relationship to team effectiveness as
reflected through satisfaction with one’s team and task performance. Latent growth curve analysis
reveals both trust types start at a significant level with individual differences in that initial level.
Cognitive trust follows a linear growth pattern while affective trust is overall non-linear, but
becomes linear once established. Latent change score models are utilized to examine change in
trust and also its relationship with satisfaction with the team and team performance. In examining
only change in trust and its relationship to satisfaction there appears to be a straightforward
influence of trust on satisfaction and satisfaction on trust. However, when incorporated into a
bivariate coupling latent change model the dynamics of the relationship are revealed. A similar
pattern holds for trust and task performance; however, in the bivariate coupling change model a
more parsimonious representation is preferred.
Keywords: trust; teams; latent growth; latent change scores; distributed teams; cognitive; affective
1. Introduction
Virtual teams have been the focus of increasing research due to their prevalence in
organizations. Even though these types of teams have existed for some time, we lack a clear
understanding of factors related to their effectiveness. There are many advantages to virtual teams
(LeMay 2000); however, available research suggests that virtual team performance is often
substandard to the performance observed in face-to-face teams (Anderson et al. 2007; Thompson and
Coovert 2006). One reason for this inferiority is that those constructs necessary for effective
teamwork, such as trust, cohesion, and shared understanding are under-developed in virtual teams
because of the quality of technology-mediated communications. There are fewer and lower quality
communication cues exchanged during technology-mediated compared to face-to-face interactions
(Daft and Lengel 1986). Work in a virtual environment, however, is a reality in today’s workplace.
Thus, in order for organizations to have fully functional distributed teams we must have an
understanding of the nature and operation of team constructs and processes in the virtual
environment.
Trust is one of the critical constructs associated with team success. Its development is
particularly challenging for virtual teams because team members are often dispersed across
locations and they lack informal, non-task related interactions, and it is those interactions that
facilitate and lead to the development of trust (Cohen and Gibson 2003). Lack of trust in a team can
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 2 of 26
be problematic since it can lead to higher costs, lower team commitment, less willingness to work
with the team, misinterpretation of information, and lack of open communication—especially about
problems facing the team (Wilson et al. 2006). Trust is therefore closely linked to successful team
performance, making it important to understand how we can build and enhance trust between
individuals in technology-mediated environments.
The goal of the present study is to examine, within virtual teams, how trust changes over time,
the impact team effectiveness has on those changes, and if there is a reciprocal influence between
these constructs. The majority of trust research has examined the important relationships trust has
with other constructs of interest, helping us answer the question of why trust is important. With this
study we look to build on that work and answer the question of how trust develops in a virtual
environment, and to quantify the development of trust over time through the estimation of the latent
change in trust within team members as they interact over time.
A main issue with trust research in virtual teams is that studies rarely examine trust as a
dynamic construct. Trust can grow or diminish over time, and those changes impact outcomes of
interest. Trust researchers generally agree that the consequences of an interaction will have an effect
on subsequent trust (e.g., (Mayer et al. 1995; Jarvenpaa and Leidner 1999; McAllister 1995)), with
positive/satisfactory outcomes facilitating an increase in trust, while negative/unsatisfactory
outcomes hamper and even decrease levels of trust. However, this bidirectional relationship
between trust and its outcomes is rarely captured. In order to understand the development of trust
over time, and the impact of trust on outcomes of interest, we propose a model of trust development
that takes the synergistic nature of the relationship into account. We position existing findings
within Social Information Processing Theory (SIP; Walther 1992) and argue for a bi-directional
relationship between trust and two constructs of interest: satisfaction with one’s team and team
performance. Specifically, we examine trust development and how it changes with team
performance and separately trust development and how it changes with satisfaction with the team.
Understanding the dynamic process of trust development will aid in implementing processes,
procedures, and tools that support the development of trust in distributed teams.
Due to the scope of the trust literature, we provide only a selective review the theoretical
background of trust and its development within individuals in teams. This review is necessary to
provide a context for our work.
1.1. What Is Trust?
Research describes trust as essential in relationships that require collaboration and cooperation
(Rusman et al. 2010). For example, trust is central to building alliances (Smith and Barclay 1997), is
key to group participation (Bandow 2001), and facilitates information sharing (Jones and George
1998). Furthermore, the presence of trust is necessary in situations involving risk. Trust is a heuristic,
which allows participation in risky behaviors without a constant engagement in risk-benefit
analyses pertaining to the situation (Riegelsberger et al. 2003). For trust to be a required component
of an interaction, two conditions need to be met: (1) one of the participants in the interaction needs to
understand that the situation involves risk; and (2) participation in the situation is incentivized
(Mayer et al. 1995). Moreover, trust is positively related to satisfaction, performance, and
commitment as well as negatively related to stress (Costa et al. 2001). Trust is critical to effective
communication processes, with low levels of trust leading to negative consequences. An example is
an increased likelihood of ambiguous information being interpreted more negatively (Salas et al.
2005).
The importance of trust is indisputable; its genesis, however, is a source of debate. A review of
the interpersonal trust literature by Lewicki et al. (2006) delineates two general methods for studying
trust. One is a behavioral approach, which describes trust as a rational-choice behavior often
evaluated by observing cooperation behaviors; the second is a psychological perspective which
examines interpersonal states, expectations, attitudes, and dispositions. Within each method, trust is
a dynamic construct whereby it can increase, decrease, or remain unchanged based on the
interaction and outcomes.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 3 of 26
From the psychological perspective, there are three major conceptualizations of trust. First, trust
can be a unidimensional construct whereby it ranges from extreme distrust to high trust (e.g.,
(McAllister 1995)). Secondly, trust is viewed as a two-dimensional construct with trust and distrust
as separate dimensions, allowing for a person to range on each (thus a person can simultaneously
trust and distrust a target; (e.g., (McAllister 1995)). Thirdly, trust has been conceptualized as a
transformational construct that can change quantitatively and also qualitatively over time (e.g.,
(Lewicki and Bunker 1996; Lewicki et al. 2006)).
In the present work, we examine trust as a unidimensional psychological construct and employ
the theoretical perspective advanced by McAllister (1995). We chose this conceptualization for three
reasons: its intuitive theoretical development, the strong empirical evidence supporting its
measurement, and the goals of our study. As defined by McAllister (1995), trust is “the extent to
which a person is confident in, and willing to act on the basis of, works, actions and decisions of
another” (p. 25). To capture the role of trust in an organizational setting, McAllister (1995) proposed
a two-factor model of trust: cognition-based trust and affect-based trust. Cognition-based trust is
derived from knowledge the trustor possessed about the entity to be trusted (trustee). Affect-based
trust captures the emotional ties between the trustor and the trustee. This two-component model of
trust has received strong empirical support (e.g., (Webber 2008; Wilson et al. 2006)) and has become
very influential in the field of trust research. Moreover, a recent review by McEvily and Tortoriello
(2011) of the measurement of trust identified that the McAllister (1995) scale is one of the most
replicated trust measures, further supporting our use of this conceptualization and measurement.
1.2. The Importance of Trust in Virtual Teams
When task completion involves collaboration among individuals, trust among those
individuals is essential. Thus, trust is a central construct of interest when examining the performance
and effectiveness of teams, both face-to-face and virtual. In the case of virtual teams there is an
additional risk imposed by the environment. Computer-mediated environments are considered to
be of higher risk than face-to-face due to the limited interaction among team members and the
reduced amount of information exchanged during the interaction owing to the technological
medium (Riegelsberger et al. 2003).
A virtual team is one whose members are geographically or temporally dispersed. Individuals
in virtual teams communicate exclusively via technology, as they are unable to engage in direct
face-to-face interactions (Cohen and Gibson 2003). Most work teams fall on a continuum of virtuality
(Kirkman and Mathieu 2005); individuals in these teams use technology in varying degrees in order
to accomplish work tasks. On the low virtuality end of the continuum, teams are primarily
face-to-face and use technology only occasionally, while on the high virtuality end teams
communicate exclusively via technology and never meet face-to-face. In the work described here our
focus is on high virtuality teams, where the interaction between team members is exclusively
through technology. We refer to these teams as virtual teams.
Organizations have identified several positive aspects to virtual teams, including greater
flexibility in scheduling and superior access to a diverse workforce (LeMay 2000). Despite these
positive characteristics, there can be downsides. For example, empirical research suggests that
virtual team performance can be inferior to the performance of face-to-face teams (Thompson and
Coovert 2006). Additionally, virtual teams take longer to complete a task, team members experience
more misunderstandings, and more time is spent clarifying ideas (Anderson et al. 2007).
The drawbacks of virtual teams have been attributed to characteristics of the environment.
Participants in a computer-mediated environment often lack the time and the informal interaction
needed to establish personal relationships, resulting in lower levels of trust between team members.
Additionally, technology-mediated environments do not allow for the transmission of the same
quantity and quality of communication cues, as do face-to-face interactions. The resulting
degradation of the information communicated between team members further contributes to lower
levels of understanding and trust (Daft and Lengel 1986). Empirical results support this perspective.
MacDonnell et al. (2009) compared performance and cohesion in face-to-face and video conferencing
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 4 of 26
teams. Their results indicated that even though there was no significant difference in performance
between the two types of teams, team cohesion ratings for video conferencing teams were
significantly lower than face-to-face teams. The fact that video conferencing teams performed on the
same level as face-to-face teams is likely because video conferencing team members could exchange
task relevant information as effectively over the video conferencing medium as could be done in the
face-to-face format. The differences in cohesion perception, however, suggest that even though the
interactions are similar, there are still disparities occurring in terms of team processes (MacDonnell
et al. 2009). Similar differences in team process were observed by Wilson et al. (2006) for trust and
cooperation. Additionally, Zornoza et al. (2009) demonstrate that trust climate in a group moderates
the relationship between virtuality level and the effectiveness of the group. Finally, an additional
and important finding is provided by Wilson et al. (2006) who demonstrate that even one
face-to-face interaction between virtual team members can mitigate the decrement introduced by
technology and elevate trust and cooperation up to levels occurring in face-to face teams.
Face-to-face and virtual teams are frequently examined as two distinct and separate entities.
Rusman et al. (2010) proposed an integrated view of the trust formation process in both co-located
and virtual project teams. These authors stated the development of trust in virtual teams is especially
challenging because there is rarely an expectation that collaboration would continue in the future,
reducing the necessity for teammates to behave in a trustworthy manner. Additionally, virtual team
members often lack the information necessary to evaluate each other’s trustworthiness. For example,
during a typical interaction, teammates make a trustworthiness assessment of a trustee. This
includes an estimate of the trustworthiness characteristics of benevolence, competence, and integrity
(Mayer et al. 1995). If information on these characteristics is not available, then participants in a
face-to-face interaction would base their assessment on available non-relevant cues such as physical
appearance and body language. Participants in technology-mediated interactions, however, do not
have access to many types of trust cues concerning a target trustee. This makes establishing an initial
level of trust especially challenging. As described by Rusman et al. (2010), once an initial assessment
of trust is performed, the process of further trust development is a cyclical one. First, information is
attended to. Secondly, based on that information and the context of the interaction, the trustor makes
a subsequent trustworthiness assessment and acts accordingly. Thirdly, the outcome of the
interaction is incorporated with the rest of the available information, a new trust assessment is made,
and another action is taken based on that trust assessment thereby continuing the cycle.
With an understanding of the process whereby trust develops in the individuals in a team and
how it can change over time, we now consider the relationship between trust and outcome variables
important to teams. Two important team outcome variables are satisfaction with one’s team and the
performance of one’s team.
1.3. Trust and Satisfaction
Based on the idea that one’s perceptions of both dispositions and observed behaviors comprise
trust, Costa et al. (2001) examine the effect of trust on task performance, satisfaction, relationship
commitment, and stress in face-to-face teams. Their results indicated that both trustworthiness
perceptions and cooperation behaviors play a major role in an assessment of trust in a target trustee.
They demonstrated trust was positively related to perceived team satisfaction and commitment, task
performance, and negatively related to stress (Costa et al. 2001) and continuance commitment (Costa
2003).
This relationship between trust and satisfaction holds in a virtual environment as well. Schiller
et al. (2014) examined the effect of institutional boundaries on trust in virtual teams. They examined
trust as a one-component concept (e.g., (Jarvenpaa and Leidner 1999; Mayer et al. 1995)) and
demonstrated that team trust is positively associated with two aspects of satisfaction—satisfaction
with the team process and satisfaction with the team outcome.
Taken together these findings suggest that team satisfaction is impacted by trust regardless of
the communication environment in which the team operates (face-to-face vs. virtual). It is important
to consider this relationship between trust and satisfaction, since satisfaction with the interaction
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 5 of 26
and its outcome could determine the trustworthiness assessment of the trustee for a subsequent
interaction (Mayer et al. 1995). This calls for a longitudinal design in order to determine the initial
trust to satisfaction link, followed by a quantification of the satisfaction to trust influence.
1.4. Trust and Performance
Trust in face-to-face teams has been determined to have a direct positive influence on
performance, satisfaction, and organizational citizenship behaviors (Dirks and Ferrin 2001). These
results have been supported at both the individual (e.g., (Costa 2003)) and team (e.g., (De Jong and
Elfring 2010; Palanski et al. 2011)) levels. Moreover, prior performance has been shown to have an
influence on trust (Walther 1992; McAllister 1995; Webber 2008), supporting a bi-directional
relationship between trust and performance.
The relationship between trust and performance in virtual teams, however, is less definitive.
Some studies have found a positive relationship between the two (e.g., (Altschuller and
Benbunan-Fich 2010)), while others have found no relationship (e.g., (Aubert and Kelsey 2003)).
Altschuller and Benbunan-Fich (2010) examined the communication process variables that
affect trust and performance in virtual teams. Altschuller and Benbunan-Fich speculate that
interaction experiences such as self-disclosure, and interpersonal perceptions such as virtual
co-presence (one’s subjective feeling that an environment is shared with virtual counterparts), will
positively relate to both team trust and team performance. To explore this hypothesized
relationship, they examined group-level trust in short-lived virtual teams. Their results demonstrate
that trust fully mediates the relationship between virtual co-presence and team performance,
supporting the notion that trust has a positive relationship with team performance in virtual
environments.
Extending the Altschuller and Benbunan-Fich (2010) findings by examining real-world, long
term distributed teams, Peters and Karren (2009) studied the impact of team trust on team
performance in a virtual context. Their results demonstrate a strong relationship between team trust
and perceived team performance, paralleling the earlier findings from co-located teams.
To further specify the mechanisms that take place for trust development and thereby enhance
performance in virtual teams, Crisp and Jarvenpaa (2013) assessed two components of trust: trusting
belief and normative action. Trusting belief is a cognitive element and captures the positive
expectations of the team’s trustworthiness. Normative action, on the other hand, is a behavioral
component and involves “setting and monitoring performance norms in virtual teams” (p. 47). They
found that early trust helps the team engage in normative actions, which further help facilitate the
development of trust, which in turn has a positive effect on team performance.
Summary
We see from literature reviewed in the above two sections that trust is closely related to
satisfaction with one’s team and also team performance. Initial levels of trust influence satisfaction
with one’s team and also influence the team’s performance. Subsequently, satisfaction and
performance are likely to influence later levels of trust. Thus, it is important to examine and quantify
these hypothesized reciprocal relationships as they evolve over time.
1.5. Trust Development in a Virtual Environment—A Proposed Model and Hypotheses
The model we propose builds upon findings in the existing literature about the
trust-performance relationship and situates them within the framework of Social Information
Processing theory (SIP; Walther 1992). According to SIP theory, once team members have worked
together for a sufficient amount of time, they will start communicating non-task relevant
information; this type of information is necessary for the development of affect-based trust. Indeed
Wilson et al.’s (2006) work supports this postulation demonstrating that given enough time,
members of virtual teams can reach the same levels of trust and cooperation as their face-to-face
counterparts. We chose SIP theory to guide our research as our interest is in virtual teams with a
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 6 of 26
relatively short time together. Other approaches, such as media naturalness theory (DeRosa et al.
2004) should be investigated if the virtual teams have a longer duration.
Researchers have been urged to examine team processes in a longitudinal manner, within
task-episodes consisting of concrete inputs and outputs (Jarvenpaa et al. 2004; LePine et al. 2008).
This type of episodic approach lends itself well to the study of how trust and team outcomes (e.g.,
satisfaction, performance) develop over time as a function of each other. It is possible that the
relationships between trust and its outcomes vary depending on the amount of time team members
have spent together. Examining the development of these two constructs by utilizing the
task-episode approach will allow for the examination of such possibilities.
We propose that trust and its outcomes, specifically satisfaction with one’s team and the
performance of the team, will develop in such a way that changes in one will influence the other. We
anticipate that both within and between construct relationships will drive changes in trust.
Intra-construct relationships are defined by the established level of trust (e.g., Trust at time 1), and
how quickly trust is changing (e.g., the rapidity of trust development as specified by its slope).
Inter-construct relationships during a particular task episode will be influenced by trust and in turn
will influence trust (Walther 1992; McAllister 1995). Thus, trust will influence satisfaction with one’s
team, which in turn will influence the subsequent level of trust. This type of model is referred to as a
bivariate coupling model, where trust is coupled to satisfaction, which in turn is coupled to the
subsequent level of trust. It is anticipated that for each trust type, a bivariate coupling model will fit
the data better than either of two simpler models, see hypotheses 4a and 4b. One simpler model is
defined by trust influencing satisfaction with no reciprocal influence of satisfaction on trust and the
second simpler model has satisfaction influencing trust, with no reciprocal influence of trust on
satisfaction.
It is anticipated the same type of relationships will hold between each type of trust and team
performance. Specifically we anticipate that for each type of trust and performance a bivariate
coupling model will better explain the relationships in the data better than either of two simpler
models, see hypotheses 5a and 5b. One simpler model has trust influencing performance, but no
reciprocal influence of performance on trust. The second simpler model has performance influencing
trust but no reciprocal influence of trust on performance.
A specific prediction as to the direction of the above-described relationships is less clear. Social
Information Processing theory stipulates that given successful performance, trust will increase
(Walther 1992). If teams are generally successful in achieving their goals, this success will propagate
trust development, which will influence performance and satisfaction, resulting in positive
relationships. If team performance is not successful, it will have (1) a negative effect on trust (trust
will decrease); or (2) no effect (trust will not change), with relationships being negative or
non-significant, respectively.
In terms of trust development, it is expected the two components of trust, cognition- and
affect-based, will follow different developmental trajectories. Based on prior research (McAllister
1995; Webber 2008; Kuo and Yu 2009) we anticipate that cognition-based trust will be the first to
develop as team members base it on task relevant information exchanged. As team members work
together and cognition-based trust continues to grow, affect-based trust will emerge based on the
established cognition-based trust, and the exchange of relational information. Therefore, we expect
to replicate existing findings:
Hypothesis 1. Affect-based trust will take longer to develop than cognition-based trust.
Furthermore, it is assumed the growth trajectory of cognitive trust is linear; although this has
yet to be empirically tested. Since cognitive trust develops first, followed by affective trust, this
implies the latent growth of affective trust is non-linear. We will empirically test each assumption
via latent growth modeling.
Building on what we know so far about the development of trust in both virtual and
face-to-face teams, we propose a dynamic model of trust development. The model stipulates trust
development and team outcomes are closely connected to each other and they should be studied
together. This is especially true in teams such as ours where task interdependence is high (Alge et al.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 7 of 26
2003). Methodologically, the model is a Latent Change Score (LCS) model (McArdle 2009; Ferrer and
McArdle 2010; Geiser 2013) and is theoretically positioned within Social Information Processing
theory (Walther 1992). Based on SIP theory, interactions between team members influence the
development of trust within each individual. Thus, it is expected that if prior interactions were
successful, trust would increase within each individual; if they were not, it would not change or it
might even decrease.
It is important to point out that latent change score models operate by estimating the amount of
the latent construct of interest (e.g., trust) within each individual. This involves estimating the factor
scores of the construct for each individual at each point in time (e.g., task episode). The manner in
which this is done is to constrain the measurement models factor loading and intercepts so the
observed changes are a function of the change in the latent constructs, not due to changes in the
measurement scales. See McArdle (2009) Ferrer and McArdle (2010) and Geiser (2013) for details.
Therefore, based on the proposed model it is expected that:
Hypothesis 2. Affective trust: A latent change score model with strong factorial invariance will fit better than a
less restrictive autoregressive model.
Hypothesis 3. Cognitive trust: A latent change score model with strong factorial invariance will fit better than a
less restrictive autoregressive model.
Hypothesis 4a. A bivariate coupling model linking affective trust and satisfaction will explain the relationships
in the data better than either of two simpler models.
Hypothesis 4b. A bivariate coupling model linking cognitive trust and satisfaction will explain the relationships
in the data better than either of two simpler models.
Hypothesis 5a. A bivariate coupling model linking affective trust and performance will explain the relationships
in the data better than either of two simpler models.
Hypothesis 5b. A bivariate coupling model linking cognitive trust and performance will explain the
relationships in the data better than either of two simpler models.
To ease discussion, we use the term team effectiveness as defined by the combination of team
performance and satisfaction with one’s team. It is expected that when team effectiveness is high
there will be an increase in trust from an initial point to a subsequent point in time (e.g., Time 1 to
Time 2). If effectiveness levels are low, as when a team is performing poorly, there will be no change
or even a decrease of trust from an initial to a subsequent point (e.g., Time 2 to Time 3).
2. Methodology
2.1. Participants
Two hundred and ninety seven (N = 297) participants took part in the study, composing a total
of 99 teams. All teams consisted of three participants. The final sample was 76% female with a mean
age of 21.22 years (SD = 4.21). Participants identified themselves as: Caucasian, 45%; Black, 13%;
Hispanic, 19%; Other 18%; and 5% did not report their ethnicity. Participants were undergraduate
students at a large southeastern research university and were incentivized to participate in the study
by receiving class credit. The Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study and treatment of
subjects. The IRB strictly adheres to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and the National
Institutes of Health regarding the ethical treatment of research participants.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 8 of 26
2.2 Materials
2.2.1. Search and Rescue Task
A search and rescue task running on the Distributed Dynamic Decision-making, DDD 4.1
simulation engine (Aptima, Inc., Woburn, MA, USA), was utilized for this study. The task required
team members to work together by sharing information and resources in order to complete a search
and rescue mission. Each team member possessed a limited set of resources (medical, mechanical,
and navigational), with the distribution of those resources being the same across team members.
Based on prior task calibration, the total available resources was sufficient to complete each mission.
Team members used their resources to accomplish tasks necessary to facilitate mission completion.
The tasks included: find and rescue a lost party, repair a communications antenna, and recover a lost
item (e.g., a UAV). To evaluate performance, each participant received points for completing certain
actions. Actions include uncovering mission critical information, fixing broken equipment to
facilitate further movement, and helping other parties (team mates) along the way.
Team members communicated with one another using a built-in synchronous text-based chat
tool. This allowed for the exchange of unique information, coordination of tasks, and discussions of
whether to expend resources on activities. The content of those chat logs was not analyzed for this
study.
Each team consisted of four individuals, with three members (action members) being
responsible for accomplishing the task at hand, and a fourth member (resource member) conveyed
external information and provided extra resources when requested. We attempted to staff the four
roles with research participants, but in cases when only three arrived for the study, a research
assistant filled the role of the resource member. The use of a research assistant for the resource team
member was deemed appropriate due to the nature of the role. Team performance was determined
by the actions of the three action members; the influence of the resource member on overall team
performance was negligible.
Each team completed three different missions. The order of the missions was counterbalanced.
2.2.2. Team Trust
Team trust was assessed using the trust scale developed by McAllister (1995). The scale
determined trust on two dimensions—affect-based trust and cognition-based trust (Cronbach’s α =
0.8–0.9 and α = 0.84–0.93, respectively). Both cognition- and affect-based trust was assessed with four
items targeting trust in one’s teammates. Participants indicated their responses on a 5-point Likert
scale.
2.2.3. Team Satisfaction
Team satisfaction was assessed using a team satisfaction scale adapted from Lancellotti and
Boyd (2008). The scale contained three items, which assessed individual desire to be a part of the
team (Cronbach’s α = 0.85–0.88). Respondents had to indicate their level of agreement on a 7-point
Likert scale.
2.2.4. Performance
Each individual in the team received a score based on the number of tasks completed (e.g., read
a seismic monitor, repaired a mechanical device, provided medical assistance). A team score was
derived based on the sum of the three action members’ scores.
2.3. Procedure
Participants were recruited via a university online recruiting system and signed up for a session
of their choice. Interaction between the participants prior to the study was minimized. Once
participants arrived in the lab for the study, they completed a consent form that outlined the
assessments and the task. After consent was obtained the participants completed the first trust
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 9 of 26
assessment (baseline). Then, participants watched a 10-min training video covering functionality of
the computer-simulation and the role of each participant. Following the training video participants
completed a demographic survey and a training mission. The training mission lasted approximately
15 min and enabled training to competency on task actions. Once training was completed, the first
mission commenced. The participants had 40 min to complete the mission. The majority of teams
utilized all the allotted time. Upon mission completion or when time ran out, participants filled out
the team satisfaction assessment. The measurement period was terminated once the participants
completed the satisfaction survey. The second measurement period then commenced. Participants
filled out the second trust assessment (Time 1) followed by participating in the second mission and
subsequently completing the team satisfaction survey. The third measurement period was identical
to the second, with trust assessment (Time 2), task performance, and satisfaction measurement. At
the end of the third measurement period the participants filled out the trust assessment (Time 3) one
last time, then they were debriefed and thanked for their participation. (The McAllister (1995) scales
are readily available in the literature. Two items from the scale measuring satisfaction with
teammates (Lancellotti and Boyd (2008) are: 1. My teammates approach the mission with professionalism
and dedication. 2. I can rely on my teammates not to make my job more difficult by careless work. We are
happy to provide all scales upon request.)
2.4. Analysis Strategy
Below we test a series of models with varying degrees of freedom. Because degrees of freedom
are linked to power, we are conservative and our sample size results in power in excess of 0.90 for all
models (MacCallum et al. 1996).
To examine the trajectory of trust development we utilize Latent Growth Curve Modeling
(LGCM), since it allows for the assessment of a specific type of change over time, such as linear or
quadratic. Latent Growth Curve Modeling is advantageous as it corrects for random measurement
error, thereby allowing for estimation of interindividual differences in true intraindividual change in
trust over time.
To examine the dynamic trust-satisfaction and trust-performance relationships we employ a
Latent Change Score (LCS) modeling technique. The application of a LCS model is preferred to other
modeling approaches for several reasons. First, as a structural equation model, it allows for the
study of latent-variable and measurement-variable (indicator) relationships. Secondly, by using LCS
modeling we can examine possible sources of changes in the constructs of interest. Thirdly, LCS
models include coupling parameters that capture the time-dependent effect of one construct on the
change of another, thereby allowing for the examination of dynamic processes (McArdle 2009).
Latent change models take a different approach from autoregressive models in that change is
measured directly through latent difference variables (McArdle and Hamagami 2004; Raykov 1993).
To test the proposed models, we followed the recommendations in Geiser (2013) and McArdle
(2009):
(1) Examine a confirmatory factor model reflecting the measurement of the construct over occasions.
Examine factor intercorrelations to assess (1) latent state (intercorrelations close to 1.0); (2) latent
state trait (small to medium intercorrelations); or (3) an autoregressive model (decreasing
intercorrelations over time) (Geiser 2013, p. 84).
(2) Examine an autoregressive model in which the temporal sequence is preserved.
(3) Examine an indicator specific latent variable model that accounts for specific (non-random)
indicator variance.
(4) Examine a model involving invariant factor loadings.
(5) Examine a full latent change model.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 10 of 26
3. Results
3.1. Latent Growth Curve Analysis: Test of Hypothesis 1
Based on a review of the literature, our hypothesis (H1) regarding the change in trust among
individuals is that affective trust takes longer to develop, so we predict a linear growth model will
not fit over all sessions, but will fit the latter segments (once cognitive trust is established). Since
cognitive trust develops first it is hypothesized to follow a linear growth pattern. To test this we
conducted second-order latent growth curve modeling.
3.1.1. Affective Trust
Fit of a linear growth curve model to the four time points for affective trust is quite poor; Χ2 (115) =
424.216, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.095, CFI = 0.907, TLI = 0.903, SRMR = 0.125. The growth process for
affective trust is significantly different from zero: MINTERC = 3.085, z = 55.675, p < 0.001. There is also
significant variability in the initial latent scores, VarINTERC = 0.430, z = 5.731, p < 0.001. The mean for the
latent slope factor is significantly different from zero, indicating trust increases over time, MLINEAR =
0.209, z = 10.684, p < 0.001. The slope factor variance is also significant, VarLINEAR = 0.044, z = 3.23, p <
0.001, indicating significant variability in the slopes of the individual growth curves; thus over time
affective trust is not increasing to the same extent in all team members.
Given that affective trust is hypothesized to emerge later than cognitive, we fit a second order
linear growth model to the last three time points (removing baseline from the above analysis). Fit of
the linear growth model is quite good, Χ2 (58) = 136.903, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.068, CFI = 0.970, TLI =
0.966, SRMR = 0.047. The growth process for trust is significantly different from zero: MINTERC = 3.203,
z = 51.654, p < 0.001. There is also significant variability in the initial latent scores, VarINTERC = 0.760, z =
7.611, p < 0.001. The mean for the latent slope factor is significantly different from zero, indicating
affective trust increases over time, MLINEAR = 0.257, z = 11.047, p < 0.001. The slope factor variance is
not significant, VarLINEAR = 0.016, z = 390, p < ns, indicating nonsignificant variability in the slopes of
the individual growth curves; thus affective trust is increasing to the same extent in all team
members over time. The second-order growth factors account for considerable variance in the latent
affective trust states, R2 values of 0.797, 0.849, and 0.949 for time 1, time 2, and time 3, respectively,
indicating good fit of the linear growth model to the data. The remaining 20.3% (baseline) to 5.1%
(time 3) of the latent state variance is due to reliable occasion-specific variability demonstrating that
occasion-specific fluctuations were more important in the beginning than at the end of the study.
3.1.2. Cognitive Trust
The linear growth curve model for cognitive trust fits reasonably well, Χ2 (106) = 232.544, p <
0.001, RMSEA = 0.063, CFI = 0.963, TLI = 0.958, SRMR = 0.089. The growth process for trust is
significantly different from zero: MINTERC = 3.452, z = 74.392, p < 0.001. There is also significant
variability in the initial latent scores, VarINTERC = 0.359, z = 6.805, p < 0.001. The mean for the latent
slope factor is significantly different from zero, indicating cognitive trust increases over time, MLINEAR =
0.185, z = 10.319, p < 0.001. The slope factor variance is also significant, VarLINEAR = 0.045, z = 4.650, p <
0.001, indicating significant variability in the slopes of the individual growth curves; thus cognitive
trust is not increasing to the same extent in all team members over time. The second-order growth
factors account for between 42.9% and 97.5% of the variance in the latent cognitive trust states, R2
values of 0.429, 0.740, 0.806, and 0.975 for baseline, time 1, time 2, and time 3, respectively. This
indicates good fit of the linear growth model to the data. The remaining 57.1% (baseline) to 2.5% (time 3)
of the latent state variance is due to reliable occasion-specific variability demonstrating that
occasion-specific fluctuations were more important in the beginning than at the end of the study. See
Figure 1 for the plot of the latent means over time.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 11 of 26
Figure 1. Latent growth curves for cognitive and affective trust.
3.1.3. Summary
Our analysis provides support for hypothesis one. In these virtual teams individuals start out
with different levels of cognitive trust and while it grew at a significant rate and in a linear fashion,
individual growth of cognitive trust occurs at different rates. Overall affective trust is non-linear due
to the fact that it is dependent on the establishment of cognitive trust. Once cognitive trust is in
place, affective trust develops at a significant rate, providing support for Hypothesis 1. Individuals
start with different levels of affective trust and it grows at a significant pace. It is important to note
that although the development of trust occurs according to theoretical prediction, causality has not
been conclusively demonstrated.
We now move to examine how the different types of trust influence satisfaction with one’s team
and team performance; and if, over time, there is a reciprocal relationship between trust and
satisfaction or trust and performance.
3.2. Overview of Latent Change Analysis
Analyses of models concerned with change proceed in a sequential fashion, allowing for the
comparison of models of interest to substantive baseline models. Following recommendations in
(McArdle 2009; Ferrer and McArdle 2010; Geiser 2013), we begin with a confirmatory factor model
reflecting the measurement of the construct over occasions. In our case this refers to the items from
the McAllister (1995) trust scale reflecting affective (4 items) and cognitive (4 items) trust. We then
examine an autoregressive model in which the temporal sequence is preserved (e.g., baseline trust
followed by measurement occasions 1, 2, and 3). Next, as we are using the same scale over time, it is
important to account for specific (non-random) indicator variance. As such, we add a model that
includes indicator-specific latent variables (Geiser 2013, p. 88). Following next is a model involving
invariant factor loadings (weak factorial invariance). We end with a full latent change model, which
adds invariant intercepts (strong factorial invariance) to the weak factorial invariant model. This is
our primary model of interest. It is the comparison of the full latent change model to the preceding
baseline models that provides a test of Hypotheses 2 for affective trust and Hypothesis 3 for
cognitive trust.
3.3. Latent State Analysis of Trust
As described by (Geiser 2013, p. 145) “Latent difference variables represent interindividual
differences in true intraindividual change over time—that is, change scores corrected for random
measurement error.” As such, this is a direct method for estimating change, as opposed to the
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 12 of 26
indirect approach taken by autoregressive models. Since a latent difference between two variables
(e.g., trust at time 1; trust at time 2) is itself a latent variable in the model, this makes it possible to
directly study interindividual differences in intraindividual change.
3.3.1. Hypothesis 2: Assessment of Affective Trust via Latent Change Score Modeling
As described above, a sequence of models is examined beginning with the least restrictive
(confirmatory factor analysis) and incrementally becoming more restrictive until concluding with a
latent change model. The fit of the different models are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Latent state analysis of trust.
Model Χ2 df RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Affective Trust
Null 3459.85 120
Confirmatory factor model. Four
intercorrelated trust factors 599.84 98 0.131 0.850 0.816 0.063
Latent autoregressive model 608.14 101 0.130 0.848 0.820 0.063
Baseline -> T1 -> T2 -> T3
Autoregressive adding indicator
specific latent variables 168.16 86 0.057 0.975 0.966 0.037
Autoregressive, indicator specific
LVs, adding invariant factor loadings 228.76 97 0.068 0.961 0.951 0.087
Latent change model: invariant factor
loadings and intercepts 272.24 101 0.076 0.949 0.939 0.086
Cognitive Trust
Null 3498.94 120
Latent change model: invariant factor
loadings and intercepts 217.756 101 0.062 0.965 0.959 0.082
Confirmatory Factor Model
The confirmatory factor model examines affective trust measured on four occasions with the
identical indicators (same four items from McAllister’s scale) on each occasion. Fit of the model is
poor, Χ2 (98) = 599.84, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.131, CFI = 0.850, TLI = 0.816, SRMR = 0.063. Examining
the interfactor correlations of the confirmatory factor model, we see they are of modest magnitude
and decline with increasing temporal distance (baseline to time 1 = 0.425, baseline to time 2 = 0.380,
baseline to time 3 = 0.362) thus indicating the influence of both occasion variance (as opposed to full
trait variance) and the presence of an autoregressive process.
Latent Autoregressive Model
We begin adding constraints, first by replacing the inter-factor correlations with structural
parameters between the trust latent variables, linking them in sequence from measurement at:
baseline to time 1, time 1 to time 2, and time 2 to time 3. This model, more restrictive than the first,
fits poorly and about the same level as the intercorrelated confirmatory factor model, Χ2 (101) =
608.14, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.130, CFI = 0.848, TLI = 0.820, SRMR = 0.063.
Indicator Specific Latent Variable Model
A third model accounts for using the same scale on multiple measurement occasions through
adding indicator specific latent variables (specific method factors). The indicator-specific latent
variable approach is preferred to simply allowing the errors to correlate as it does not confound
indicator specific effects with random measurement error (Geiser 2013, p. 89). Fit of this model to the
data results in: Χ2 (86) = 168.16, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.057, CFI = 0.975, TLI = 0.966, SRMR = 0.037. The
addition of indicator specific method factors is a significant improvement from the sequential
model, nested model comparison Χ2 (15) = 439.98, p < 0.001.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 13 of 26
Factorial Invariance Model
A fourth model layers further restrictions by constraining factor loading to be invariant. This
weak factorial invariance (Widaman and Reise 1997; Meredith and Horn 2001) ensures the measured
variables of the measurement instrument did not change their relationship to the affective trust
latent variable over the course of the study. The resulting difference in variance is a function of the
latent construct being influenced by contextual sources; here participation in the task. The additional
parameter restrictions affect the overall fit of the model, Χ2 (97) = 228.76, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.068,
CFI = 0.961, TLI = 0.951, SRMR = 0.087. The model with weak factorial invariance fits somewhat less
well than the model with fewer restrictions although the overall fit is reasonable.
Latent Change of Affective Trust
The model of primary interest is a latent change model. Described above, the latent change
representation has many advantages over autoregressive and second order autoregressive models.
To test a latent change model we take the weak factorial invariance model and include the restriction
that the intercepts of the observed variables are invariant over occasions, imposing strong factorial
invariance (Widaman and Reise 1997; Meredith and Horn 2001) on the model. Then latent variables
representing the change in the latent construct at each point in time is included. The
parameterization allows for the estimation of the influence of each preceding time and the difference
on subsequent time points. For example, trust at time 1 is equal to baseline trust, plus the difference
between baseline (time 0) and time 1; time 2 trust is equal to time 1 trust plus the difference between
time 1 and time 2; and so forth for trust at time 3. The model fit is acceptable, Χ2 (101) = 272.33, p <
0.001, RMSEA = 0.076, CFI = 0.949, TLI = 0.939, SRMR = 0.086. Given its reasonable fit, this is the
preferred model moving forward as it provides the theoretical advantages described above, notably
the ability to estimate error free latent means. Thus, support is found for Hypothesis 2.
3.3.2. Hypothesis 3: Assessment of Cognitive Trust via Latent Change Score Modeling
Latent Change of Cognitive Trust
Having examined a series of baseline models and concluding with the latent change model for
affective trust (see Table 1), we took the same approach for assessing cognitive trust. The result
across baseline models is similar, as such we report only the latent change solution here (see Table 1
for comparisons). Compared to the affective trust solution, fit of the latent change cognitive trust
model is somewhat better, Χ2 (101) = 217.756, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.062, CFI = 0.965, TLI = 0.959,
SRMR = 0.082. See again Table 1. With the fit of this model, support is found for Hypothesis 3.
3.4. Latent State Analysis of Satisfaction
We are interested in the manner in which satisfaction with one’s team changes as the team
works together over time and the extent to which it may be influenced by changes in trust.
Following the system described above for analyzing a sequence of models, we applied the same
strategy to the analysis of the satisfaction construct. We first test the confirmatory factor model,
followed by the latent autoregressive model, in the third model we add indicator specific factors to
account for method variance in the indicators over time, a fourth model tests for weak factor
invariance, and the fifth model is the latent change model with strong factor invariance.
All models are summarized in Table 2. A similar patter to the one for affective trust (see again
Table 1) emerges. Fit is poor for the CFA and latent autoregressive models. The first significant
improvement occurs with the addition of the indicator specific latent variables Χ2 (7) =109.37, p <
0.001. As with the trust construct, adding constraints in terms of invariant factor loadings decreases
fit of the model. According to RMSEA, the latent change score models with strong factorial
invariance fit slightly better than the autoregressive model with invariant factor loadings. Although
the autoregressive model with indicator specific latent variables fit better than the latent change
model, Χ2 (7) = 25.32, p < 0.01; the advantage of the latent change model is it provides unbiased
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 14 of 26
estimates of the latent variable means, and this is necessary as we want to examine the influence of
the change in satisfaction on trust. Satisfaction at time 1 with one’s team is M = 3.707. The latent
change score between time 2 and time 1 is, diff = 0.088, which results in the latent mean for time 2
being M = 3.795. A much larger change in satisfaction occurs between time 3 and time 2, diff = 0.247,
with the resulting time 3 satisfaction latent mean M = 4.042.
Table 2. Latent state analysis of satisfaction.
Model Χ2 df p RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Null 2378.20 36 0.000
Confirmatory factor model. Three
intercorrelated satisfaction factors 146.72 24 0.000 0.131 0.948 0.921 0.057
Latent autoregressive model 150.35 25 0.000 0.130 0.946 0.923 0.060
S1 -> S2 -> S3
Autoregressive adding indicator specific
latent variables 40.98 18 0.002 0.066 0.990 0.980 0.035
Autoregressive, indicator specific LVs,
adding invariant factor loadings 60.94 22 0.000 0.077 0.983 0.987 0.064
Latent change model: invariant factor
loadings and intercepts 66.30 25 0.000 0.075 0.982 0.975 0.064
Summary
Like trust, satisfaction with one’s team is a state (as opposed to trait) construct and can be
expected to change over time. We modeled it with the latent change modeling approach and the
resulting model has reasonable properties. As we now have the latent change models developed for
affective trust and satisfaction with one’s team, we now couple these models to determine the extent
to which trust influences satisfaction over time and satisfaction also impacts trust.
3.5. Testing the Relationships between Trust and Satisfaction
3.5.1. Hypothesis 4a: Affective Trust and Satisfaction
Having developed two latent change score models, one for how affective trust changes over
time and the second for how satisfaction with one’s team changes over time, we now link them in
order to investigate the relationship between changes in trust and changes in satisfaction over time.
Fit of the following models is summarized in Table 3 below.
Table 3. Examining the relationship between changes in affective trust and changes in satisfaction
with one’s team.
Model Χ2 df p RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Change in affective trust influencing satisfaction
Null 6300.69 300 0.000
A. Change in trust directly
influencing satisfaction (see
Figure 2)
666.16 269 0.000 0.070 0.934 0.926 0.178
B. Change in trust influencing
change in satisfaction 719.99 271 0.000 0.075 0.925 0.917 0.216
Model comparison, model A
vs. model B 53.83 2 0.001
Change in satisfaction influencing affective trust
C. Change in satisfaction
directly influencing trust (see
Figure 3)
694.96 268 0.000 0.073 0.929 0.920 0.215
D. Change in satisfaction
influencing change in trust 717.20 273 0.000 0.074 0.926 0.919 0.217
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 15 of 26
Model comparison, model C
vs. model D. 22.24 5 0.001
Bi-variant coupling change model
E. Combines parameters:
Change in trust influencing
satisfaction (model A) with
Change in satisfaction
influencing trust (model C),
(see Figure 4).
652.096 267 0.000 0.070 0.936 0.928 0.165
Model E vs. Model A 14.064 2 0.001
Model E vs. Model C 42.860 1 0.001
Change in Affective Trust Influencing Satisfaction
The first model of interest examines how the latent change in affective trust at each of three time
points influences satisfaction with one’s team at the same points in time, see Figure 2. The model fits
reasonably well, Χ
2
(269) = 666.16, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.070, CFI = 0.934, TLI = 0.926, SRMR = 0.178. A
second model asks the question, does the change in affective trust directly influence the change in
satisfaction one has with the team. This model, while not fitting quite as well as the previous one, fits
at an acceptable level (RMSEA, CFI and TLI now in acceptable range; SRMR still indicating poor fit):
Χ
2
(271) = 719.99, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.075, CFI = 0.925, TLI = 0.917, SRMR = 0.216. A chi-square
difference test comparing the two models is significant, Χ
2
(2) = 53.83, p < 0.001; this indicates the
model with the change in latent affective trust directly influencing satisfaction with one’s team
(Figure 2) fits significantly better than the alternative.
Figure 2. Latent change in trust influencing satisfaction with one’s team. Standardized parameter
estimates for affective trust are not bracketed, those for cognitive trust are given in brackets. All
estimates are significant p < 0.01. For clarity, the measurement model and indicator specific latent
variables are not presented.
Change in Satisfaction Influencing Change in Trust
In addition to examining relationships where affective trust influences satisfaction, it is also
reasonable to question if change in satisfaction with one’s teammates influences affective trust. We
test two models similar to the ones just described. One model has a change in satisfaction directly
influencing affective trust; see Figure 3. The model fit is similar to the other models, Χ
2
(268) = 694.96,
p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.073, CFI = 0.929, TLI = 0.920, SRMR = 0.215. The second model examines the
influence of change in satisfaction on changes in affective trust. Fit of this model is slightly worse
than the previous, Χ
2
(273) = 717.20, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.074, CFI = 0.926, TLI = 0.919, SRMR = 0.217.
A chi-square difference test reveals the model with change in satisfaction directly influencing
affective trust is preferred, Χ
2
(5) = 22.24, p < 0.001, see again Figure 3.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 16 of 26
Figure 3. Latent change in satisfaction influencing trust. Standardized parameter estimates for
affective trust are not bracketed; those for cognitive trust are given in brackets. All estimates are
significant p < 0.01. For clarity, the measurement model and indicator specific latent variables are not
presented.
Bivariate Coupling of Latent Change
Following McArdle (2009), we refer to models where the change scores in the lower and upper
panel influence variables in the opposite panel as bivariate coupling models. We now test a model
that includes the change in affective trust impacting satisfaction with one’s team (Figure 2, model A
in Table 3) with change in satisfaction influencing affective trust (Figure 3, model C in Table 3). Figure 4
presents the parameters and estimates. Overall this model fits better than any of the previous
models, Χ
2
(267) = 652.096, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.070, CFI = 0.936, TLI = 0.928, SRMR = 0.165 and
chi-square difference tests reveal the fit is significantly better than the other preferred models, see
bottom two rows of Table 3. Thus, hypothesis 4a is supported, a bivariate coupling model between
affective trust and satisfaction with one’s team fits better than the simpler non-bivariate coupled
models.
Figure 4. Bivariate coupling model. Latent change in trust influencing satisfaction and latent change
in satisfaction influencing trust. Standardized parameter estimates for affective trust are not
bracketed; those for cognitive trust are given in brackets. All estimates are significant p < 0.01. For
clarity, the measurement model and indicator specific latent variables are not presented.
3.5.2. Hypothesis 4b: Cognitive Trust and Satisfaction
The second type of trust we examine is cognitive. The focus of cognitive trust is on
professionalism, competence, and quality of work. As the latent growth curve analysis
demonstrated, there is a different growth trajectory for cognitive compared to affective trust where
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 17 of 26
cognitive trust’s overall trajectory is linear and the overall trajectory for affective is non-linear; only
becoming linear after some time has passed.
We are interested in the same change models and influences as with affective trust. We begin
with the test of strong factorial invariance. The fit of the model is reasonable, Χ2 (101) = 217.756, p <
0.001, RMSEA = 0.062, CFI = 0.965, TLI = 0.959, SRMR = 0.082. We move to examining a series of
models testing the relationships between cognition-based trust and satisfaction with one’s team. Fit
of each model is summarized in Table 4.
Table 4. Examining the relationship between changes in cognitive trust and changes in satisfaction
with one’s team.
Model Χ2 df
p RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Change in trust influencing satisfaction
Null 6298.62 300
A. Change in trust
directly influencing
satisfaction. See
Figure 2, parameter
estimates in [ ].
584.423 269 0.000 0.063 0.947 0.941 0.201
Change in satisfaction influencing trust
C. Change in
satisfaction directly
influencing trust. See
Figure 4 parameter
estimates in [ ].
599.18 268 0.000 0.065 0.945 0.938 0.214
Bi-variant coupling change model
E. Combines
parameters: Change
in trust influencing
satisfaction (model
A) with Change in
satisfaction
influencing trust
(model C).
577.72 267 0.000 0.063 0.948 0.942 0.185
Model A vs. Model E 6.703 2 0.048
Model C vs. Model E 22.46 1 0.001
Change in Cognitive Trust Influencing Satisfaction
Taking what we learned from our analysis between affective trust and satisfaction, we analyzed
three models depicting the relationships between cognitive trust and satisfaction with one’s team.
The first model is depicted in Figure 2, where change in cognitive trust directly influences
satisfaction. The model fits reasonably well, Χ2 (269) = 584.423, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.063, CFI = 0.947,
TLI = 0.941, SRMR = 0.201. The parameter estimates for this model are provided in Figure 2, see
estimates in [brackets]. The second model we examined specifies that change in satisfaction will
impact cognitive trust; again see Figure 3. This model also fits reasonably well, Χ2 (268) = 599.18, p <
0.001, RMSEA = 0.065, CFI = 0.945, TLI = 0.938, SRMR = 0.214. Parameter estimates for cognitive trust
are given in brackets in the figure. The final model combines the previous two, and states that
change in cognitive trust impacts satisfaction with one’s team; and change in satisfaction, in turn,
influences trust, see again Figure 4. Fit of this model is Χ2 (267) = 577.72, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.063,
CFI = 0.948, TLI = 0.942, SRMR = 0.185. Parameter estimates are provided in Figure 4, see bracketed
values. Nested model chi-square comparisons of this bivariate coupling model to the previous
models are significant; see bottom two rows of Table 4. This means the bivariate coupling model
does a better job of explaining the observed variance between cognitive trust and satisfaction than
does a model with just change in cognitive trust influences satisfaction, or the model where change
in satisfaction influences cognitive trust. Hypothesis 4b is supported.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 18 of 26
Summary
It is important to understanding how trust changes over time and influences satisfaction with
one’s team. Similarly, it is essential to recognize how satisfaction with one’s team changes over time
and influences trust in the team. For each type of trust (affective, cognitive) and satisfaction with
one’s team we examined a series of change models, beginning with correlated factor and
progressing through latent change with strong factorial invariance (Meredith and Horn 2001).
Strong factorial invariance imposes restrictions on the factor loadings such that they are invariant
over time. Similarly, intercepts for the indicator variables are constrained to zero, and remain fixed
over time. The result is the properties of the measurement instrument do not change over time and
therefore do not influence the estimation of parameters associated with the latent constructs of
interest (here trust and satisfaction).
Once the latent change models for each type of trust and satisfaction were established we
examined a series of models looking at the influence of changes in latent trust on satisfaction,
followed by models specifying changes in satisfaction impacting trust. Finally, we coupled the latent
change representations and the resulting model captures how, over time in distributed groups,
changes in trust influence satisfaction with the group, and how changes in satisfaction influence
trust. This bivariate coupling between each type of trust and satisfaction with one’s team reveals the
dynamic nature of the linkages and influence between change in trust and satisfaction as the team
works together over time.
3.6. Trust and Performance
Having established the relationship between trust and satisfaction with one’s team, we want to
test if the same relationship exists between trust and performance. That is, for each type of trust, do
changes in trust influence performance, and do changes in performance influence trust. The model is
similar to Figures 2–4, except the satisfaction with teammates latent variables are replaced by
performance, where performance has two indicators: individual and team score on the task.
3.6.1. Hypothesis 5a: Affective Trust and Performance
The first model we examine states that latent change in affective trust over time influences
performance over time. We fit this model to the data, and the overall fit is reasonable, Χ2 (201) =
426.417, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.061, CFI = 0.937, TLI = 0.927, SRMR = 0.084. Examination of the
parameters reveals that change in trust at times two and three influencing performance are
non-significant, see Figure 5. In terms of parsimony we fixed those two parameters to zero and
re-estimated the model. Overall fit changed very little, Χ2 (203) = 426.704, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.061, CFI
= 0.937, TLI = 0.929, SRMR = 0.083. A nested model chi-square indicates the models are not
significantly different from one another Χ2 (2) = 0.287, p = 0.866, so we prefer the more parsimonious
structure.
To rule out the possibility that performance influences changing levels in affective trust, we
tested a model reversing the direction of influence so over the three time periods, performance
influences change in affective trust (in Figure 5 instead of regressing performance on change, regress
change on performance). Model fit is significantly poorer, with the chi-square being over 100 points
larger and the descriptive measures of fit indicating worse fit across the board.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 19 of 26
Figure 5. Bivariate coupling model. Latent change in trust influencing performance and change in
performance influencing trust. Standardized parameter estimates for affective trust are not
bracketed; those for cognitive trust are given in brackets. All estimates are significant p < 0.01 unless
otherwise noted. * p < 0.023, parameter estimate from parsimonious solution, model H in Table 5. For
clarity, the measurement model and indicator specific latent variables are not presented.
Table 5. Examining the relationship between changes in affective and cognitive trust and performance.
Trust Type Model Χ
2
df
pRMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Change in trust influencing performance
Affective Null 3800.56 231
Cognitive Null 3859.65 231
Affective A. Change in trust
influencing performance. 426.704 203 0.000 0.061 0.937 0.929 0.083
Cognitive B. Change in trust
influencing performance. 393.080 204 0.000 0.056 * 0.948 0.941 0.087
Change in performance influencing trust
Affective C. Change in performance
influencing trust. 408.47 202 0.000 0.059 0.942 0.934 0.082
Cognitive D. Change in performance
influencing trust 370.686 203 0.000 0.053 * 0.954 0.947 0.086
Bi-variant coupling change model
Affective
E. Combines parameters:
Change in trust influencing
performance with Change
in satisfaction influencing
trust
426.417 201 0.000 0.061 0.937 0.927 0.084
F. Parsimonious. Same as
model E, but fix non
significant paths to zero.
427.101 205 0.000 0.060 0.938 0.930 0.083
Model E vs. Model F 0.684 4 ns
Cognitive
G. Combines parameters:
Change in trust influencing
performance with change
in satisfaction influencing
trust
392 202 0.000 0.056 * 0.948 0.940 0.088
H. Parsimonious. Same as
model G, but fix non
significant paths in model
B and D to zero.
370.788 204 0.000 0.052 * 0.954 0.948 0.086
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 20 of 26
Summary
As the overall model fit quite well and hypothesis 5a is supported, the relationship between
affective trust and performance is less straightforward than the relationship found between affective
trust and satisfaction with one’s teammates. Change in affective trust directly influences
performance only at time one; moreover, changes in performance do not influence trust. We now
move to examine the relationships between cognitive trust and performance.
3.6.2. Hypothesis 5b: Cognitive Trust and Performance
As before, we begin with a model that has parameters linking change in cognitive trust and
performance. The overall model fits quite well Χ2 (204) = 393.080, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.056, CFI =
0.948, TLI = 0.941, SRMR = 0.087; however, the parameters linking change in trust to performance are
not significant, see Figure 5.
The next model determines if change in performance influences trust. The model fit is quite
acceptable, Χ2 (203) = 370.686, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.053, CFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.947, SRMR = 0.086. In
this case, the parameter linking change in performance from time 1 to time 2 and cognitive trust is
significant, whereas the following change in performance does not significantly influence trust at
time three.
Hypothesis 5b is supported as the full bivariate coupling change model fit, with fit parameters:
Χ2 (202) = 392.042, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.056, CFI = 0.948, TLI = 0.940, SRMR = 0.088. While the overall
fit of the model is quite good, none of the bivariate coupling parameters are significant. We reran the
model fixing all bivariate coupling parameters to zero except the one from change in performance
(time 1 to time 2) and trust at time 2 as this was significant in the change in performance influencing
cognitive trust model described above. The parsimonious model fits quite well, Χ2 (204) = 370.788, p
< 0.001, RMSEA = 0.052, CFI = 0.954, TLI = 0.948, SRMR = 0.086.
Summary
The relationship between cognitive trust and performance is complex. Similar to affective trust,
cognitive trust behaves differently. Unlike affective trust, there is no relationship between change in
cognitive trust and it influencing performance at time 1. Change in performance between time 1 and
time 2 directly influences cognitive trust at time 2. Change in performance between time 2 and 3 has
no influence on cognitive trust at time three.
3.7. Overall Summary of Results
Using the latent growth curve modeling approach allows for the estimation of error free latent
means for our model. Graphically (see again Figure 1) the two types of trust appear different in their
initial level and growth. Latent growth curve modeling confirmed the difference, with cognitive
trust growing from the outset in a linear fashion. Affective trust, however, is overall non-linear, but
has a strong linear component once cognitive trust is established.
3.7.1. Foundational Latent Change Analyses
Our interest is in the nature of trust in distributed teams, how it changes over time as the
individuals interact, and how it relates to satisfaction with one’s teammates and performance.
Affective and cognitive trust are likely to behave differently, we therefore analyzed separately for
each trust type. The relatively modest intercorrelations for trust factors confirmed we are dealing
with state-like as opposed to trait-like constructs. For each trust type the intercorrelations decreased
with increasing temporal distance, thereby confirming an autoregressive process.
Following a procedure outlined by McArdle (2009) and Geiser (2013) we examined a series of
baseline models proceeding from those with fewer constraints to models with more constraints. The
first is a confirmatory factor model followed by a latent autoregressive model. These fit poorly.
Adding indicator specific latent variables to account for using the same scale over multiple
measurement occasions increased the fit dramatically. The final models added constraints on factor
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 21 of 26
loadings (weak factorial invariance) and intercepts (strong factor invariance). These models fit in an
acceptable although mediocre range. The final representation added latent variables for change.
Overall fit of a latent change representation for affective and cognitive trust is good; again see Table 1.
Following the same procedure we used for analyzing trust, we examined the change in
satisfaction over time. As with each type of trust, the latent change model fit reasonably well.
3.7.2. Models of Theoretical and Substantive Interest
We are interested in how affective and cognitive trusts are related over time to satisfaction with
one’s teammates and also performance in the team.
Satisfaction
Latent change models for both affective and cognitive trust provide a reasonable explanation of
the interrelationships in the data. As seen in Figure 2 where change in trust influences satisfaction,
the structural parameters are moderate to large and all are significant. Similarly in Figure 3 change
in satisfaction influences trust. Again, the parameters are moderate to high, and all are significant. In
the full bivariate coupling model the relationships all hold, so we see that for both affective and
cognitive trust, change in trust influences satisfaction, and change in satisfaction impacts trust,
providing support for Hypothesis 4a and 4b. Furthermore, all individual parameters in the models
are significant.
Performance
In each case, overall fit of the bivariate change model to the data for affective and cognitive trust
to performance is significant, providing support for Hypotheses 5a and 5b. Unlike satisfaction,
however, the influence of trust on performance is less pronounced. This can be seen in the individual
parameters of the models. For affective trust, change in trust from baseline to time one does
influence performance at time one, but neither of the other two trust change scores significantly
influence performance. Similarly, none of the cognitive change scores influence performance. In
considering the influence of change in performance on trust, only for cognitive trust does the
difference between time two and time one influence trust at time two. The time two to time three
differences is not significant for either type of trust.
3.7.3. General Summary
We see that the latent change score method is a useful strategy for studying change in trust in
distributed teams. The models are helpful for understanding the change in trust over time. Since the
technique provides error free estimates of latent variable means, it is useful to further analyze factor
scores. Here we see that the type of trust interacts with time, as there are different structures to the
latent change scores. For both affective and cognitive trust, the bivariate coupling models
demonstrated that over time, change in trust influences satisfaction with one’s teammates; and
change in satisfaction with one’s teammates influences trust. When considering performance,
however, the situation is different. For affective trust, early change in trust influences early
performance; whereas for cognitive trust early change in performance influences early trust.
4. Discussion and Conclusions
Modern technologies enable seamless communications providing individuals within today’s
organizations the unprecedented ability to communicate across temporal and geographical
boundaries. Yet, regardless of the sophistication of the hardware and software, fundamental human
characteristics, such as trust, lay at the very foundation of successful interactions. As discussed in the
introduction, the state of knowledge regarding trust in virtual teams is limited. Our work is focused
on building a solid foundation of knowledge regarding the development and growth of trust in
virtual teams. We believe this is best accomplished, at least in part, through the use of controlled
experiments and appropriate quantitative methods.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 22 of 26
Our latent growth curve analyses demonstrate that cognitive and affective trust are both
present in nontrivial levels when virtually distributed individuals are asked to work together to
complete a mission. Yet for each trust type, there are considerable individual differences in the initial
levels (significant differences in the intercepts). Once task work commences, cognitive trust follows a
linear trend with trust increasing within individuals at different rates. On the whole, this is good
news for organizations that pull together individuals for project teams and need those teams to work
effectively from the start. One potential downside, however, would be a situation where one or more
members might be a cybersecurity threat to the organization and the early trusting by the group
enables or facilitates nefarious acts.
The increase in early cognitive trust supports the notion that it is laying the groundwork for
subsequent development of affective trust. Affective trust takes additional time to develop beyond
the baseline level. Once established, however, affective trust follows a linear growth trend and
increases within individuals at the same rate. This is different from cognitive trust where individuals
develop it at different rates throughout task completion. With affective trust associated with
satisfaction, cohesion, performance and extra role behaviors such as organizational citizenship, its
growth and development is important for organizations to attend to.
Our work with latent change score models strengthens and adds to the trust literature. The base
latent change models, while highly constrained, fit the data quite well. All structural parameters are
large and significant indicating that preceding levels of trust and change in trust are each significant
predicators of subsequent trust. Estimating parameters via latent change scores allow for the clear
prediction of trust levels in virtual teams at various points in the team’s evolution. Another use these
parameters would be to seed parameters of computational models used to simulate artificial
teammates in organizational contexts such as training.
The relationship of both cognitive and affective trust with satisfaction is more complex than
earlier believed. If we look simply at the change in trust influencing satisfaction (Figure 2), as trust
changes over time, change in trust has a positive influence on satisfaction with one’s team. Similarly,
change in satisfaction over time continues to positively influence subsequent levels of trust.
Examination of the bivariate coupling change model (Figure 4), however, reveals a diminishing
influence of trust on satisfaction from the first change in trust influence on satisfaction to the second.
The relationship is negative at the third change in trust episode, perhaps indicating greater demands
on the satisfaction criterion space beyond that provided by trust.
The relationship of trust with performance is somewhat different. While the bivariate coupling
change models fit the data significantly better than the uncoupled models, parsimony argues for a
simpler representation. This is because change in affective trust influences only early task
performance. Change in cognitive trust, however, does not influence either early or later task
performance. Similarly, early change in performance influences cognitive trust. This complex
relationship that develops over time may be providing support for the evolution of trust similar to
that described by Lewicki and Bunker (1996). Our findings argue for structuring team tasks in such a
fashion to allow early successful performance to build early cognitive trust among members.
4.1. Theoretical and Practical Implications
The results of this study have several important implications. First, these findings support the
notion that both cognitive and affective trust is present early in the team’s lifecycle. The two types of
trust have different developmental trajectories, suggesting that based on the maturity of the team,
organizations may want to utilize different techniques for building trust. Additionally, there are
differences in developmental trajectories across group members, suggesting that the rates at which
trust changes is different across individuals. It is important to understand the factors that determine
those inter-individual differences.
Secondly, the results of this study indicate the two components of trust have different
relationships with outcomes of interest, in this case performance and team satisfaction. The impact
of changes in affective and cognitive trust on those outcomes is different, and changes over time.
This is important for interventions that organizations may consider implementing. Understanding
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 23 of 26
whether an intervention is aimed at enhancing cognitive or affective trust can help researchers and
practitioners target the effect it may have on the outcome of interest.
Thirdly, these findings support the view that by examining trust as cognitive and affective
constructs, researchers can further understand important relationships between trust type and
outcomes. The two types of trust have different developmental trajectories and unique relationships
with the outcomes examined here. This more refined understanding of trust can help both
researchers and practitioners gain a deeper understanding of team processes, allowing for the ability
to impact those processes when needed.
Lastly, findings from this study demonstrate trust can develop among team members despite
the low media richness of the communication tool utilized by team members. In this study, team
members communicated via a text-based, synchronous communication chat tool. This type of
communication medium is widely used in organizations for a number of reasons including its
convenience and cost effectiveness. Text chat tools are inexpensive, widely available, and utilize less
bandwidth compared to other tools such as video-conferencing, making them attractive as a
communication tool. Even though text-based chat tools are located toward the lean end of the media
richness spectrum, the results of the present study suggest they are an effective communication tool.
Team members were able to complete their tasks as well as establish relationships with one another
as indicated by their increasing trust scores.
4.2. Limitations and Future Directions
The sample of this study consisted of undergraduate students; as such caution should be
exercised when generalizing these findings to an organizational setting. This study is a first step in
uncovering these dynamic relationships between trust, team satisfaction, and performance. Thus,
future studies need to extend this work to an organizational context.
Throughout the study, the researchers monitored the experimental teams’ progress to ensure all
players participated in the mission, however their degree of motivation and effort could not be
assessed. Based on participant reactions during the debriefing sessions, the participants generally
found the task engaging and appeared to have done their best. Also our task simulated only high
cycle interactions (completion of three missions within forty minutes). Replication of the study with
organizational teams in a higher-stakes environment and longer durations is advisable.
In this study, we examined trust as a psychological construct that develops between people
based on their experiences and interactions with one another. In virtual teams, however, two types
of trust are at play—interpersonal trust and trust in the utilized technology. This study examined the
former and did not address the latter. As complex technology is layered onto human interactions
trust in both the human agent as well as in the technology become critical for effective system design
and subsequent interactions (Lee and See 2004). Future studies should examine the effects of both
trust in team members and trust in technology on team outcomes.
The focus of this study was to examine the direct relationships as it unfolds over time between
trust and two effectiveness concepts, team satisfaction and performance. Prior studies have found
the relationship between trust and performance could be mediated through different team processes
(de Jong and Elfring 2010). Future studies should examine how trust interacts with these team
processes in order to increase effectiveness. In particular, communication has been identified as
being important to teamwork (Powell et al. 2004). For this study, we did not have the chat logs
generated during the sessions available for analyses. With the advancement of text analytic
technology, future studies should aim to utilize available communication data to examine the
patterns of communication as well as information sharing behaviors.
Our work has focused on distributed teams who do not see one another. This is commonplace
in the global world of work in which we live today. Much of what is known about how trust
develops, however, is based upon studies conducted in face-to-face teams. In those teams trust
develops through those interactions in which individuals incorporate both subtle verbal and
nonverbal meaning. Future research needs to build upon our work utilizing a dimension of
virtuality.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 24 of 26
Lastly, this study examined the development of trust and effectiveness in a naturalistic setting
with no trust manipulations. Team members were encouraged to work together and be cooperative.
However, team dynamics change as a function of team member behaviors, and in turn influence
trust and team performance. Future studies should examine how competition influences the
development of the relationships between trust and effectiveness in virtual teams.
In conclusion, the world continues to become ever more connected. As organizations exploit
this connectivity through the use of distributed teams it becomes increasingly important to
understand the factors that influence effectiveness in these teams. We see that for two types of
effectiveness, team satisfaction and task performance, trust plays a foundational role. By utilizing
latent growth curve and latent change score models, our work provides practitioners and
theoreticians a solid foundation upon which to continue to build.
Acknowledgments: This work was supported by a National Research Council Fellowship and an AFRL Chief
Scientists Seedling Award to the first author. Support was also received from contract number PO-JN42903. An
earlier version of this work was presented at the 31st Annual Meeting of the Society of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology. We thank Marijana Arvan and David Coovert for their assistance with the data
collection.
Author Contributions: All three authors conceived and designed the experiments; Miller and Coovert
performed the experiments; Coovert and Miller analyzed the data; Coovert, Miller and Bennett contributed to
the writing of the paper.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest. The founding sponsors had no role in the
design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in
the decision to publish the results.
References
(Alge et al. 2003) Alge, Bradley J., Carolyn Wiethoff, and Howard J. Klein. 2003. When does the medium matter?
Knowledge-building experiences and opportunities in decision-making teams. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes 9: 26–37.
(Altschuller and Benbunan-Fich 2010) Altschuller, Shoshana, and Raquel Beneunan-Fich. 2010. Trust,
performance, and the communication process in ad hoc decision-making virtual teams. Journal of
Computer-Mediated Communication 16: 27–47. doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2010.01529.x.
(Anderson et al. 2007) Anderson, A. H., R. McEwan, J. Bal, and J. Carletta. 2007. Virtual team meetings: An
analysis of communication and context. Computers in Human Behavior 23: 2558–80.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2007.01.001.
(Aubert and Kelsey 2003) Aubert, Benoit A., and Barbara L. Kelsey. 2003. Further understanding of trust and
performance in virtual teams. Small Group Research 34: 575–619. doi:10.1177/1046496403256011.
(Bandow 2001) Bandow, Diane. 2001. Time to create sound teamwork. The Journal of Quality and Participation 24:
41–47.
(Cohen and Gibson 2003) Cohen, Susan, and Christina Gibson. 2003. In the beginning: Introduction and
framework. In Virtual Teams that Work: Creating Conditions for Virtual Team Effectiveness. Edited by Susan
Cohen and Christina Gibson. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 1–13.
(Costa 2003) Costa, Ana N. 2003. Work team trust and effectiveness. Personnel Review 32: 605–22.
(Costa et al. 2001) Costa, Ana N., Robert A. Roe, and Tharsi Taillieu. 2001. Trust within teams: The relation with
performance effectiveness. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 10: 225–44.
(Crisp and Jarvenpaa 2013) Crisp, C. Brad, and Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa. 2013. Swift trust in global virtual teams:
Trusting beliefs and normative actions. Journal of Personnel Psychology 12: 45–56.
doi:10.1027/1866-5888/a000075.
(Daft and Lengel 1986) Daft, Richard L., and Rovert H. Lengel. 1986. Organizational information requirements,
media richness and structural design. Management Science 32: 554–71.
(De Jong and Elfring 2010) De Jong, Bart A., and Tom Elfring. 2010. How does trust affect the performance of
ongoing teams? The mediating role of reflexivity, monitoring, and effort. Academy of Management Journal
53: 535–49.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 25 of 26
(DeRosa et al. 2004) DeRosa, Darleen M., Donald A. Hantula, Ned Kock, and John D’Arcy. 2004. Trust and
leadership in virtual teamwork: A media naturalness perspective. Human Resource Management 43: 219–32.
(Dirks and Ferrin 2001) Dirks, Kurt T. and Donald L. Ferrin. 2001. The role of trust in organizational settings.
Organizational Science 12: 450–67.
(Ferrer and McArdle 2010) Ferrer, Emilio, and John J. McArdle. 2010. Longitudinal modeling of developmental
changes in psychological research. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 149–54.
doi:10.177/0963721410370300.
(Geiser 2013) Geiser, Christian. 2013. Data analysis with Mplus. New York: Guilford Press.
(Jarvenpaa and Leidner 1999) Jarvenpaa, Sirkka L., and Dorthy E. Leidner. 1999. Communication and trust in
global virtual teams. Journal of Computer Mediated Communication 3: 1–36.
(Jarvenpaa et al. 2004) Jarvenpaa, Sirkka L., Thomas R. Shaw, and D. Sandy Staples. 2004. Toward
Contextualized Theories of Trust: The Role of Trust in Global Virtual Teams. Information Systems Research
15: 250–67.
(Jones and George 1998) Jones, Gareth R., and Jennifer M. George. 1998. The experience and evolution of trust:
Implications for cooperation and teamwork. Academy of Management Review 23: 531–46.
(Kirkman and Mathieu 2005) Kirkman, Bradley L., and John E. Mathieu. 2005. The dimensions and antecedents
of team virtuality. Journal of Management 31: 700–18.
(Kuo and Yu 2009) Kuo, Feng-yang and Chai-ping Yu. 2009. An exploratory study of trust dynamics in
work-oriented virtual teams. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 14: 823–54.
(Lancellotti and Boyd 2008) Lancellotti, Matthew P., and Thomas Boyd. 2008. The Effects of Team Personality
Awareness Exercises on Team Satisfaction and Performance. Journal of Marketing Education 30: 244–54.
(Lee and See 2004) Lee, John D. and Katrina A. See. 2004. Trust in automation: Designing for appropriate
reliance. Human Factors 46: 50–80. doi:10.1518/hfes.46.1.50_30392.
(LeMay 2000) LeMay, Elaine A. 2000. Virtual Teams: Work Processes, Communication, and Team Development.
Ph.D. Dissertation, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA.
(LePine et al. 2008) LePine, Jeffery A., Ronald F. Piccolo, Christine L. Jackson, John E. Mathieu, and Jessica R.
Saul. 2008. A meta-analysis of teamwork process: Tests of a multidimensional model and relationships
with team effectiveness criteria. Personnel Psychology 61: 273–307.
(Lewicki and Bunker 1996) Lewicki, Roy J., and Barbara Benedict Bunker. 1996. Developing and maintaining
trust in work relationships. In Trust in Organizations: Frontiers of Theory and Research. Edited by Roderick M.
Kramer and Tom R. Tyler. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, pp. 114–39.
(Lewicki et al. 2006) Lewicki, Roy J., Edward C. Tomlinson, and Nicole Gillespie. 2006. Models of interpersonal
trust development: Theoretical approaches, empirical evidence, and future directions. Journal of
Management 32: 991–1024. doi:10.1177/0149206306294405.
(MacCallum et al. 1996) MacCallum, Robert C., Michael Browne, and Hazuki Sugawara. 1996. Power analysis
and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods 1: 130–49.
(MacDonnell et al. 2009) MacDonnell, Rhiannon, Thomas O’Neill, Theresa Kline, and Laura Hambley. 2009.
Bringing group-level personality to the electronic realm: A comparison of face-to-face and virtual contexts.
The Psychologist-Manager Journal 12: 1–24.
(Mayer et al. 1995) Mayer, Roger, Davis, James, and F. David Schoorman. 1995. An integrative model of
organizational trust. Academy of Management Review 20: 709–36.
(McAllister 1995) McAllister, Daniel J. 1995. Affect-and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal
cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal 38: 24–59.
(McArdle 2009) McArdle, John J. 2009. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal
data. Annual Review of Psychology 60: 577–605.
(McArdle and Hamagami 2004) McArdle, John J., and Fumiaki Hamagami. 2004. Methods for dynamic change
hypotheses. In Recent Developments on Structural Equation Models. Edited by K. van Montfort, J. Oud and A.
Satorra. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 295–335.
(McEvily and Tortoriello 2011) McEvily, Bill, and Marco Tortoriello. 2011. Measuring trust in organisational
research: Review and recommendations. Journal of Trust Research 1: 23–63.
doi:10.1080/21515581.2011.552424.
(Meredith and Horn 2001) Meredith, William, and John Horn. 2001. The role of factorial invariance in modeling
growth and change. In New Methods for the Analysis of Change. Edited by L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer.
Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 203–40.
Soc. Sci. 2017, 6, 87 26 of 26
(Palanski et al. 2011) Palanski, Michael E., Surinder S. Kahai, and Francis J. Yammarino. 2011. Team virtues and
performance: An examination of transparency, behavioral integrity, and trust. Journal of Behavioral Ethics
99: 201–16. doi:10.1007/s10551-010-0650-7.
(Peters and Karren 2009) Peters, Linda, and Ronald J. Karren. 2009. An examination of the roles of trust and
functional diversity on virtual team performance ratings. Group and Organization Management 34: 479–504.
(Powell et al. 2004) Powell, Anne, Gabriele Piccoli, and Blake Ives. 2004. Virtual teams: Team control structure,
work processes, and team effectiveness. Information Technology & People 17: 359–79.
(Raykov 1993) Raykov, Tenko. 1993. A structural equation model for measuring residualized change and
discerning patterns of growth or decline. Applied Psychological Measurement 17: 53–71.
(Riegelsberger et al. 2003) Riegelsberger, Jens, M. Angela Sasse, and John D. McCarthy. 2003. The researcher’s
dilemma: Evaluating trust in computer-mediated communication. International Journal of Human-Computer
Studies 58: 759–81. doi:10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00042-9.
(Rusman et al. 2010) Rusman, Ellen, Jan van Bruggen, Peter Sloep, and Rob Koper. 2010. Fostering trust in
virtual project teams: Towards a design framework grounded in a TrustWorthiness ANtecedents (TWAN)
schema. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 68: 834–50.
(Salas et al. 2005) Salas, Eduardo, Dana E. Sims, and C. Shawn Burke. 2005. Is there a “Big Five” in teamwork?
Small Group Research 36: 555–99.
(Schiller et al. 2014) Schiller, Shu Z., Brian E. Mennecke, Fiona Fui-Hoon Nah, and Andy Luse. 2014.
Institutional boundaries and trust of virtual teams in collaborative design: An experimental study in a
virtual world environment. Computers in Human Behavior 35: 565–77. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.051.
(Smith and Barclay 1997) Smith, J. Brock, and Donald W. Barclay. 1997. The effect of organizational differences
and trusting on the effectiveness of selling partner relationships. Journal of Marketing 61: 3–21.
(Thompson and Coovert 2006) Thompson, Lori, and Michael D. Coovert. 2006. Understanding and developing
virtual computer-supported cooperative work teams. In Creating High-Tech Teams: Practical Guide to Work
Performance and Technology. Edited by Clint. Bowers, Eduardo. Salas and Florien Jentsch. Washington:
APA, pp. 213–41.
(Walther 1992) Walther, Joseph B. 1992. Interpersonal Effects in Computer-Mediated Interaction—A Relational
Perspective. Communication Research 19: 52–90.
(Webber 2008) Webber, Shelia S. 2008. Development of cognitive and affective trust in teams: A longitudinal
study. Small Group Research 39: 746–69.
(Widaman and Reise 1997) Widaman, Keith F., and Steven P. Reise. 1997. Exploring the measurement
invariance of psychological instruments: Applications in the substance use domain. In The Science of
Prevention: Methodological Advances from Alcohol and Substance Abuse Research. Edited by Kendall. J. Bryant,
Michael. Windle and Stephen G. West. Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 281–324.
(Wilson et al. 2006) Wilson, Jeanne M., Susan G. Straus, and Bill McEvily. 2006. All in due time: The development
of trust in computer-mediated and face-to-face teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
99: 16–33.
(Zornoza et al. 2009) Zornoza, Ana, Virginia Orengo, and Vicente Peñarroja. 2009. Relational capital in virtual
teams: The role played by trust. Social Science Information 48: 257–81.
© 2017 by the authors. Submitted for possible open access publication under the
terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).