Content uploaded by Jan Nehyba
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jan Nehyba on Jul 27, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
130
|
Fih Chapter
CLEAN LANGUAGE AS ADATA COLLECTION TOOL
Jan Nehyba, Petr Svojanovský
is chapter explains and evaluates how the data collection method entitled Clean
Language (described in detail in Chapter3) was implemented in our research. In the
context of pedagogical sciences, it is anew way of interviewing, which helps to gather
data as closely as possible from rst person (Searle, 1992; Varela, 1999). In the context
of our research, we assume that the more we obtain data from the position of the rst
person, the better the quality of data. We understand high-quality data as information
collected rst hand from the world of the research participants, i.e. information that
is aected as little as possible by the researcher’s perspective during the interview.
In line with the denition, we believe that tacit knowledge is less conscious than
other knowledge, and dicult to articulate. It follows that in research interviews, it is
important to ensure as far as possible that the structuring of such knowledge comes
directly from the informants. In other words, the less the researcher intervenes in the
interview, the greater potential for the elicitation of tacit knowledge. Clean language
enables informants to explore what Petitmengin (2014) refers to as the microstructure
of their experience, thus helping them to grasp what is less conscious and dicult to
articulate. Intervention by the researcher in the content of the interview (paraphrasing
or interpreting what has been said or introducing completely new topics) can distract
the informant from accessing essential details that may contribute to awareness and
the ‘naming’ of less obvious aspects of their own experience.
In this chapter, we explain how we understand the term Clean Language and how
we interpret it, and then move to the actual analysis of how we used this method in
practice, i.e. in conducting the research interviews.
5.1 Conducting interviews using Clean Language
Although the method of Clean Language interviewing is based on several clear ideas
(for example, repetition of the participant’s verbal and non-verbal expressions, use of
‘clean’ questions), dierent aspects of this method can be highlighted; for example,
whether emphasis is placed on the natural formulation of questions asked in an inter-
view, or whether more emphasis is placed on the use of strictly ‘clean’ questions. ese
dierences may appear as subtle nuances but they greatly inuence how an interview
is conducted. ere is adierence between strictly adhering to alist of ‘clean’ questions
131
|
and asking questions guided by the informant’s previous answer(s) (although these po-
sitions are not necessarily mutually exclusive). erefore, we consider it important to
present how we think our research team understood and applied the Clean Language
interviewing method. In the following paragraphs, we present those aspects of Clean
Language interviewing we consider important.
e value of ‘clean’ questions in an interview relies on an objective concept of clean-ness.
‘Clean’ questions have no ‘deeper’ meaning and donot demonstrate empathy for the
interviewee. e assumption is that these very specic questions help to systematically
eliminate the interviewer’s own assumptions, so that they donot unduly inuence the
interview. Our use of Clean Language interviewing involved using questions taken
from aclean questions list93. e belief is that these specic questions help us systemati-
cally eliminate our own assumptions, so that they donot inuence us when conducting
an interview. Our view is that Clean Language interviewing helps us, through the use
of clean questions, to “minimize” any assumptions within the questions themselves.
Every question has certain assumptions but clean questions are designed to contain
as few of these assumptions as possible. For example, the question: “What kind of
X is that X?” assumes only some form of existence of X, as opposed to the question:
“What doyou think about X?”, which assumes that the informant has to think, and
not, for example, feel, something about X, etc. (X represent aword or anon-verbal
gesture of interviewee). “What kind of X is that X?” assumes that since the interviewee
has mentioned X, then X will have some qualities which enable the interviewee to
distinguish X from not-X.
Although, through their construction, even clean questions inuence how the inform-
ant approaches their experience, they dothis much less than traditional open interview
questions (see Chapter3). As aresult, the Clean Language interviewer becomes more
self conscious about their own language when conducting aresearch interview. We
see this ‘sensitization’ as the most important benet of Clean Language interviewing.
Clean Language also helps researchers to recognize and minimize, rather than
eliminate, assumptions in relation to individual interviewees that aect the rapport
or the relational level of communication (cf. Hulburt, 2011, Chapter20). is more
contextual and relational concept of clean-ness in an interview represents areturn to the
original idea of the creators of Clean Language, Grove and Panzar “We cannot dene
in advance the grammar, syntax or vocabulary of aclean question. A‘clean’ question is
unique to each client. We can give general rules dening clean questions. Nonetheless,
we have to discover which questions will t which client.” (1989, p.23)
93 ese lists of questions dier from each other (to agreater or lesser extent), both in terms
of quantity and quality (inclusion of adierent type of questions on the list), depending
on the context and practice of each author. For lists of ‘clean‘ questions, see, for example,
Lawley and Tompkins (2000); Harland (2012a); Way (2013); Tosey, Lawley, & Meese (2014);
McCracken (2016).
132
|
In this contextual model of clean-ness, the role of rapport (the relational level of the
interview) is crucial for obtaining high-quality content, which for us, is data that is
as close as possible to the rst-person perspective. In our research, rapport was sup-
ported in particular by specic verbal comments. In itself, successful use of exclusively
‘clean’ questions creates asafe environment in which the informant can concentrate
on their own inner world and in this sense supports acertain rapport between the
informant and the researcher. Clean Language interviewing aims to maintain the rap-
port between the informant and their inner world of experience, however, to achieve
this, it is necessary also to maintain researcher-informant rapport94 .
Clean Language interviewing primarily inuences the process of conducting an interview
and aims to minimize inuencing the informant’s experience in terms of content. We can-
not conclude, however, that Clean Language does not inuence the interviewee. On the
contrary from the perspective of social constructionism (Gergen, 1999), the extended
mind (Rowlands, 2010) and in reference to neo-pragmatism (Rorty, 1999), this is not
even possible. e dierence is in how Clean Language interviewing inuences the
informant. It deliberately inuences the interviewee so as to keep their attention in
their own eld of experience95 to be able to see the phenomenon in question from the
closest possible position to their own96. We donot inuence the content of their atten-
tion by adding new topics but inuence what part of their eld of experience they talk
about. erefore, we can refer to this method as asecond person interview, which helps
the interviewee come as close to themselves as possible (however, from our perspec-
tive, we can never cross this border). us, it is about the degree to which we “come
closer”, in our interview, to where the interviewee’s attention is. We also perceive it as
clean when we come close to where the interviewee’s attention is using clean language
syntax97, and then directing their attention to the “edge” of their perception of personal
experience (using aclean question). For example, the informant makes the statement:
“Isee myself connected, how all that pupils’ energy is owing to me.” We keep the
interviewee’s attention on the entire description of their experience by repeating their
words, and subsequently, we direct it to the kind of “connection” it is, although the
94 We are aware from personal experience that experts in Clean Language are able to establish
rapport by using ‘clean’ questions and by non-verbal expression (mirroring, etc.).
95 Cf. Urban (2015, p.44): “Husserl introduced the term eld ... with aconscious reference to an
analogy to common experiential elds such as visual eld, tactile eld, etc.”
96 is assumes adivision between one’s own experience and the experience of another, which
is our personal construct. Our own experience refers to how Iexperience writing these lines
while the experience of another refers to how someone else experiences writing another chap-
ter of this book. erefore, Itry to access this experience of another, to see what this experi-
ence of another looks like. If Iwanted to return to my own experience, it would mean that
Ihave to return to how Iexperience focusing on the experience of another.
97 Syntax is how aresearcher composes aquestion for the informant. In formalized form, it con-
sists of three parts: (1) … and [client’s words]. (2) … and as/when [client’s words], (3) [clean
question]? However, the researchers did not always exactly observe the three parts; sometimes,
they would only use 3. aclean question.
133
|
interviewee’s attention would originally be directed to, for example, elaboration on the
topic of pupils. If our question “hits” where the informant’s attention is or where it is
directed, we can “immerse” them even more in the “stream” of their own experience. As
aresult, it can help the informant access even that content that is not obvious to them98.
However, the aim of the research interview is not only “immersion” but a“balance” of
this immersion and nding information in relation to the research question.
In relation to the topic of clean-ness in an interview, it should be noted that the very
concept of ‘clean-ness’ is ametaphor, and some authors even consider it, in the context
of experimental research, an embodied metaphor that inuences our moral evaluation
(Zhong & Liljenquist, 2006; Schnall, Benton, & Harvey, 2008). is is then represented
with conceptual metaphors: CLEAN IS GOOD and DIRTY IS BAD. We are aware
of this tendency, however, we understand the term ‘clean’ dierently. To conduct an
interview in Clean Language does not mean to conduct agood interview but to come
as close as possible to the informant’s rst-person perspective. We emphasize that an
interview conducted in clean language is dierent, not better. It produces adierent
type of data than interviews conducted in aconventional way (cf. the hermeneutic
conception of understanding as ‘other’, not ‘better’, Grondin, 2007, p.174).
5.2 Analysis of conducting research interviews
In this section we rst emphasize the selection of interviews analyzed and describe
each phase of the analysis. We then move to the ndings and, nally, to the discussion
of the results.
In total, the research team conducted 44 in-depth, unstructured interviews between
September2013 and January2016. e interviews were conducted by seven trained
researchers. All the researchers had been trained in how to conduct interviews using
Clean Language and they consulted with Clean Language experts James Lawley, Penny
Tompkins and Caitlin Walker. ree researchers had additional practical experience
conducting interviews using ‘clean’ questions (researchers 1, 2 and 7) because they had
attended an ocial workshop on Clean Language interviewing that included practical
training. Four researchers had only received several hours of training (researchers 3,
4, 5 and 6). Researchers 1, 2 and 3 each conducted approximately one-quarter of the
total number of interviews, providing approximately three-quarters of the data col-
lected. To evaluate the manner of implementing Clean Language when conducting
research interviews, the researchers randomly (by drawing lots) chose one interview
each, which was subsequently analyzed (i.e. 3interviews in total). e last (fourth)
interview for analysis was randomly selected from the remaining batch of interviews
conducted by one of the remaining researchers (Table4).
98 Cf. the use of trance as elicitation of experience (Lifshitz et al., 2013).
134
|
Table 4
Basic information about research interviews and their analysis.
Number
of research
interviews
Number of
analyzed
interviews
Topic of the analyzed
interview
Informant in
the analyzed
interview
Researcher 1 10 23% 1 subjective conception
of teaching Karel
Researcher 2 12 27% 1 subjective conception
of teaching Ema
Researcher 3 10 23% 1 didactic transformation
of content Františka
Researcher 4, 5, 6, 7 12 27% 1 didactic transformation
of content Ema
Total 44 100% 4
5.3 Phases in the analysis
e analysis of interviews was based on the protocol for ‘clean-ness’ validation when
conducting aresearch interview (Chapter3.5), which sets out four basic categories
evaluating the degree to which the researcher’s questions inuence the content of the
informant’s statements: (1) classically clean; (2) contextually clean; (3) mildly/poten-
tially leading; and (4) strongly leading. One trained researcher began (deductively)
analyzing the questions in the interviews according to these categories. She catego-
rized all other statements (comments) in the interviews, thus gradually (inductively)
creating the typology of the comments. is was the rst reading of the data conducted
by atrained researcher.
e categorization of questions and comments was subjected to re-analysis by two
other researchers (authors of this chapter) is was the second reading. It became ob-
vious that theoretically designed categories for the evaluation of questions were too
vaguely dened, and it was not possible to reliably distinguish in which category each
question belonged. Consequently, we started to approach the analysis inductively.
As far as the comments were concerned, it became apparent that the researcher in-
uenced participant statements to varying degrees—our hypothesis when we began
categorizing the comments according to the degree of inuence on the informant.
e originally categorized questions were revised (the third reading—again conducted
by the researcher based on instruction), and a new typology of questions and new
denitions of categories were created containing individual types of questions. e
comments were also categorized according to the degree of inuence on the partici-
pants’ statements.
135
|
e last phase was the fourth, nal, reading (by the authors of this chapter) where we
examined to what degree our division of individual types of questions and comments
into categories of ‘clean-ness matched that of the researcher’s. e categorizations were
amended and the partial denitions nalized.
Lawley’s original categories (Chapter3.5) were adapted to the context of our research
based on the iterative process described. First, the adaptation included adierent ‘un-
derstanding’ of the scope of each category. We took ‘scope’ to mean what is logically
(and on aregular basis) understood as asummary of objects which fall under agiven
category (e.g. objects falling under Category1 dened by us); in other words the scope
of aterm. Our scope of categories is much broader than Lawley’s. Lawley’s category of
classically clean questions includes only prescribed strictly clean questions, whereas
our Category1 also includes some conversational ways of using clean language. On
the one hand this was due to the fact that we are not as experienced in conducting
clean language interviews, on the other hand, it was aresult of the fact that criteria
other than objectivistically dened clean-ness (where clean is dened only as clean
questions) were also important to us
e results of the analysis are summarized in the following sections—rst adescrip-
tion of the qualitative analysis of questions and comments (types and dierences
among them) followed by an examination of the quantitative analysis (the percentage
of individual categories of questions and comments analyzed in the interviews).
5.4 Qualitative analysis
In this chapter we describe the dierent categories considered in the analysis. e
questions are ordered according to the degree of content-inuencing on the partici-
pant—from Category 1, which includes the least content-leading questions or com-
ments, to Category 4, which includes the most aecting questions. e manner of
categorization is illustrated in specic examples.
5.4.1 Categorization of questions
Category1: ‘Clean’ questions—variants99
(a) ese questions included only the informant’s exact words supplemented by
some of the clean questions on the list created by Lawley and Tompkins (2000,
pp. 282–283). ese questions are ‘clean’ without depending on the context in
which they are asked (context-independent).
99 For each category of questions or comments where dierent variants are presented (this ap-
plies to Categories 1and 2 for questions and comments), we list the variants that repeatedly
occur in the interviews. ose occurring only exceptionally were not included in the list.
136
|
Example 1
Participant: Well, it was adisappointment that the plan, what Ihad expected, was not
fullled.
Researcher: What kind of disappointment? (question What kind of? in the list by
Lawley and Tompkins)
(b) ese questions were variations of the basic question “What kind of X is it?” becau-
se they donot contain any topics, opinions, ideas, beliefs, etc. that the researcher
would bring into the interview through these questions. is is only avariation of
the wording of the questions.
Example1
Researcher: What is X about for you?
Example2
Researcher: How would you name X?
(c) ese questions contain words that donot contaminate the informant’s statement
in terms of content (at the level of the informant’s external speech). In essence,
they are paraphrases of clean questions in which, however, there is no semantic
shi. ese are clean questions uttered in one’s own words, where these words are
commonly shared expressions of communication.
Example 1
Participant: … When Igo into the classsroom, Ifeel rising tension ... then Igo to the
teacher’s desk and the tension uctuates … and when Isit in the teacher’s chair … it
goes away.
Researcher: And if everything goes like that, what happens next? (aparaphrase of the
question: What happen next? in the list by Lawley and Tompkins).
Category2: Contextually clean questions—variants
(a) Verifying questions—used by the researcher to verify they understand correctly
what the informant is saying. In fact, it is a paraphrase to clarify particular in-
formation in the informant’s statement. is is not an attempt to paraphrase the
meaning of the statement (as is the case in the category of medium-inuencing
questions).
Example1
Participant: e teacher tells me: Could you dothis topic and nothing more.
Researcher: So the teacher tells you what topic you should do, and it is up to you how
many texts, authors, there are?
137
|
Example2
Participant: When Iam in alesson and Ifeel aconnection with apupils.
Researcher: And now precisely you’re talking about maths or are you talking about...?
(b) Introductory questions—used by researchers to initiate an interview with an
informant.
Example 1
Researcher: When Isay “you and teaching”, what does it doto you, what could you say
in that respect?
(c) Questions aimed at the manner of expression—the purpose of these questions
is to invite informants to express themselves using the selected instruction
or technique. ese questions are not a direct focus on the experience of
participants.
Example1
Participant: When you think about it, it’s like Ilooked at it from afar.
Researcher: Now when you look from a distance and look at that, what it was about,
what was created, what’s happening inside you?
Example2
Participant: Is it like asphere with many connections. is is the most concise.
Researcher: And can you draw it here?
Category3: Medium-inuencing questions
ese are questions that contain words the informant has not said and that
introduce potentially new topics or links, or asemantic shi into the interview.
ese questions contain aparaphrase of the student’s statement.
Example1
Participant: Well Ithink that for many there is the eect that if they fail three times in
arow, there will come this: “Iwill fail again anyway.”
Researcher: So the bad marks, the three failures make them give up, saying it doesn’t
matter anymore?
e example above contains aparaphrase, potentially bringing in the new topic that
the subjects do not care. Although this paraphrase may seem to correspond to the
student’s statement in terms of content, this cannot be said with certainty. e inform-
ant could have, in the background of their statement, implicitly perceived adierent
meaning (adierent topic), for example, that the teacher’s marking is unfair.
138
|
A paraphrase is always an interpretation because the same thing said using other
words creates the potential for asemantic shi in these other words. In this respect
, it is dicult to distinguish paraphrasing (medium-inuencing questions) from in-
terpretation (strongly inuencing questions), i.e. the extent to which the meaning of
the informant’s words was or was not changed, and the degree to which the inform-
ant was inuenced by the researcher. e inclusion of aquestion in agiven category
then depends on the researcher’s sensitivity to distinguishing the degree to which the
meaning of the informant’s statements was changed in the question. Disputable cases
(where researchers did not agree on the inclusion of a question in aparticular cat-
egory) were re-discussed among the researchers.
In the following example, the underlined words indicate apotential semantic shi in
the researcher’s question.
Example2
Participant: Ihave already given up on passing on to them everything Iprepare because
that has hardly ever worked out. So Irather hope that about aquarter of what Isay
sticks in their heads ... that maybe in the next class they will be able to repeat or
answer afew follow-up questions ...
Researcher: So if it sticks, at least that quarter, that means that you have something
to follow up in the next class that they will respond to your questions, that they will
actually remember the subject matter, what you had done?
e student’s wording everything Iprepare is paraphrased by the researcher as subject
matter. is represents asemantic reduction, and thus asemantic shi in the state-
ment—everything the student prepares for her class need not, in terms of content,
relate to the subject matter. e student’s wording about aquarter sticks is paraphrased
by the researcher as sticks, at least that quarter. e paraphrase carries an implicit
assumption that if less than aquarter is remembered by pupils, it would be impossible
to follow up on the previous lesson in the next one. e researcher thus introduces
apotentially new link, apresumption about “if-then” causality.
Category4: Strongly inuencing questions
ese questions include words the informant has not said, and explicitly introduce
acompletely new topic or link into the interview. ese questions contain an interpreta-
tion of the student’s statement.
In the rst example, the informant describes her experience in class where pupils are
unable to solve aMath problem without her support (specically her physical presence
and non-verbal signals). e researcher’s response was to encourage the informant to
think about whether she tried to change, to eliminate, this behavior in the pupils in
any way. However, such considerations were not present in the informant’s statements.
By introducing acompletely new topic, the researcher heavily inuences the content.
139
|
Example1
Participant: … they are not able to solve the problem without me sitting there with them
and nodding yes ... it seems to me alot of kids have problems with this.
Researcher: And doyou remove that somehow...?
Asimilar situation is also illustrated in the second example. e researcher introduces
an explicitly new topic in the interview—taking into account what the kids are like in
preparation for classes.
Example2
Participant: … Ithink that Inotice that, what the kids are like and what they doin the
class. Of course Idon’t notice all of them in one class ... but Ihad singled out afew
people [pupils] Iasked [other teachers] about...
Researcher: And then when you know, or you probably must have known, then what—
did you take it into account in preparation or how did you proceed?
5.4.2 Discussion on the categorization of questions
Variations in ‘clean’ questions and Categories 1a, 1c and 2a refer to what Lawley and
Tompkins (2005) describe as a conversational conception of clean language. ‘Clean
conversation’ (dialogue) diers from the use of clean language in that:
1. in ‘clean conversation’, the interviewer intends to achieve something (for themselves);
in the context of research, the intent of aresearcher is to explore the informant’s expe-
rience in acertain “framework”, created by the research question in the interview;
2. it happens in the real world, and therefore it is possible for the interviewer to assu-
me more than in the metaphorical landscape; for example, in clean conversation in
the ordinary world we assume that the laws of physics apply, whereas this need not
be the case in the interviewee’s metaphorical landscape (cf. law of cartoon physics,
Harland, 2012a, p.56);
ese sub-categories (1b, 1c, 2a) refer to what is called, in the context of exploring ex-
perience in exposure interviews (pertaining to methods of examination of experience
that is the closest possible to the rst-person perspective), the deliberate inconsistency
of aquestion (Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel, 2007, p.15). However, this idea goes partially
against the clean language philosophy. Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel claim that if acertain
experience is suciently “robust”, then asking a question repeatedly and dierently
(inconsistently) will lead to asharpening of the meaning of the experience (Hurlburt,
2011, p.161). From the perspective of clean language, we can agree with this only in
relation to the repetition of aquestion (cf. Harland, 2012b) since, as Hulburt himself
says, each of these questions has its advantages and disadvantages and, from the per-
spective of clean language, the greater the consistency of the question, the more the
disadvantages are minimized.
140
|
Hurlburt thus assumes that our experience can be “robust” and we can vary questions
to describe the experience, and place the emphasis on “playful” phenomenological
variations in the questions (cf. Ihde, 2012). is means oering anumber of possible
questions that enable adeeper exploration of the experience from dierent angles.
By contrast, Clean Language assumes apotential for “fragility and uidity” in some
moments of inner experience, which may fall apart aer even the slightest inuence
on content.
5.4.3 Categorization of comments
Category 1: Positively inuencing comments
ese are comments that strengthen the relationship with the student and en-
courage open and detailed exploration of the structure of their own experience.
Although these comments usually also include words the informant has not
said, they focus on the process of the interview, not the content of the interview.
(a) Showing understanding and personal involvement
e research analysis categorized only the more apparent expressions of active lis-
tening. One-or-two-word expressions such as hmm and oh, good were not coded as
comments and thus donot inuence the overall frequency of the comments in this
Category.
Example1
Participant: Is like alight bulb … for many people is not clear.
Researcher: Clear, clear. Clear, light bulb. Yes, yes, yes.
Example2
Participant: Ifeel as sun in the middle of classroom.
Researcher: Hm, hm, hm, OK, good.
(b) Stabilization of attention through aliteral replica
Example 1
Participant: Yeah, in physics, the relationship between understanding and learning, Ire-
ally think it’s easy—if there’s no understanding, there’s no learning Ithink.
Researcher: No understanding, no learning {nodding}.
141
|
(c) Assurance leading to openness
Example 1
Participant: It’s illogical what Isay. Iknow … Ishould only use the correct term, that we
learned in school.
Researcher: … it doesn’t have to be completely logical ... if something is not right, you
maybe correct yourself or don’t correct yourself {gesticulating} … simply if it isn’t
exactly as you have it, it’s not aproblem.
Category 2: Context-bound comments—neutral
(a) Pre-framing the interview—explanation of what the research is about, how
the interview will be conducted, etc.
Example 1
Researcher: ... Iwill be asking something, you will try to reply, just note that some of the
questions may sound abit strange ... whatever crosses your mind, whether it’s athou-
ght, afeeling, some whatever, it belongs here, that’s why we’re here ...
(b) Rening the instructions—these comments are the researcher’s attempt to direct
the participant’s attention so that it conforms with the research question. No new
content is introduced, only adeveloping of what has already been said.
Example1
Researcher: Iwould come back to you saying you explain it to them in very simple terms.
Example2
Researcher: Elaborate.
(c) Comment associated with instruction/technique
Example 1
Researcher: … we would try automatic writing, which means that on the topic Igive …
you will write for three minutes without having anything for preparation, and what
is important is that your hand must not stop...
Category 3: Inuencing comments
ese are comments that contain words the informant has not said and that introduce
potentially new topics or links, or asemantic shi into the interview. ese comments
typically contain aparaphrase of the student’s statement.
142
|
Example 1
Participant: But Iknow that when Igo away, some of them solve the problem, and then
Icome back and say yes, great, let’s go on, or [Isay] Ithink we could doit abit die-
rently, abit better or it’s not supposed to be this way.
Researcher: Yeah, yeah, so actually you say it something like you don’t say, yeah, this is
wrong but let’s try it like...
In this interview, the researcher’s paraphrase introduces apotentially new topic into
the interview by emphasizing the level of feedback in the student’s statement. As is ap-
parent from previous statements, the student particularly emphasizes the inuence of
her presence next to pupils when they have to solve amathematical problem: “without
attracting attention, Igo, for example, to have asip of water and Itry to go away to
make them try on their own..., not the way of providing pupils with feedback.”
Category4: Strongly inuencing comments
ese are comments that contain words the informant had not said and that explicitly
introduce acompletely new topic or link into the interview. ese comments contain
an interpretation of the student’s statement. In the rst example, the rst part of the
utterance is asummary of the contents of the informant’s statements so far, (this is
not inuencing because it contained words and semantic links used by the informant.
However, in the second part of the utterance, the researcher has interpreted the stu-
dent’s statement. e researcher thus created anew semantic link with an unexpected
situation and conrmation of the teacher role.
Example1
Researcher: We talked about unexpected situations, about situations which throw you
o your teacher role and return you to the other one. Now actually, in turn, again an
unexpected situation which reassured you in that role.
In the second example, it is aform of evaluation of the student’s statement and apres-
entation of the interviewer’s own opinion. Both the evaluation and the opinion intro-
duce anew semantic perspective, new links, into the interview.
Example2
Participant: … better if they admit they don’t understand it, and they doadmit that in
the seventh grade … then Itried to explain that further or explain it in adierent
way. Which Ithink is probably better, but that’s the seventh grade, not sixth grade.
Researcher: Hmm, never mind, it’s in general like that, Ithink that also in the sixth class,
even if this happened, it would probably have the same course.
143
|
5.4.4 Discusion of the categorization of comments
Paradoxically our divergence from the traditional concept of Clean Language inter-
viewing in research, which deliberately does not work with comments, is most evident
in the comments. Categories 1a and 1c could seem undesirable from the perspective of
traditional Clean Language interviewing, while 1b best corresponds to the philosophy
of Clean Language interviewing.
e reason is that Category 1b uses only the rst two phases of the syntax of Clean
Language interviewing—the third part is not used (for details on syntax see Chapter 5.1
Conducting interviews using Clean Language). By including only two phases of syntax,
the question is missing and the repetition is only adeclarative sentence, i.e. acom-
ment. e comment does not include anything that would appear to contaminate the
respondent’s statement, but, on the contrary (from our experience), this repetition
reinforces the informant’s “immersion” in their own experience. is technique is
commonly used in other methods of interviewing close to the rst-person position
(cf. Gendlin, 2004 or Petitmengin & Bitbol, 2009). e purpose of most of the other
comments in Categories 1 and 2 is either to reinforce the relationship between the
informant and the researcher or to frame or pre-frame the space for the research in-
terview. is contributes to creating an atmosphere of trust and a secure interview
environment. Understandably, these comments can be perceived to have asuggestive
form but they are suggestive in relation to the process, not the content of the interview.
Categories 3 and 4 are comments we can label as undesirable in the context of an
interview because they unnecessarily stie topics brought up by the informant.
In summary, we can say that positively inuencing comments help obtain data from
aposition close to the rst person. Neutral comments help maintain the research inter-
view process in desirable dynamics. Inuencing and strongly inuencing comments
are undesirable in an interview because they have the potential to alter the focus of
the interview so that it is not in harmony with the informant’s previous statement(s).
5.5 Quantitative analysis
In this section we proceed to the quantication of the data analyzed. Researchers 1 and 2
conducted half of the research interviews (22 out of 44) asking on average 82% completely
‘clean’ questions (Category1) in arandomly selected interview. Researcher3 conducted
about aquarter of the interviews, but used n only 24% of all questions asked in the
interview analyzed were Category 1 ‘clean’ questions. Arepresentative of the researchers
who conducted about aquarter of the research interviews combined (Researcher4) used
Category1 questions in 57% of cases (Table5, Figure6). With some degree of bias, it
may be deduced that these statistics also represent the level of clean-ness evident in the
clean language used in the research interviews that were not analyzed.
144
|
Table 5
e frequency of questions in each category of clean-ness in the interviews analyzed (absolute
numbers)
Clean-ness rating of questions
Interview:
researcher 1
+ Karel
Interview:
researcher 2 +
Ema
Interview:
researcher 3
+ Františka
Interview:
researcher 4
+ Ema
Category 1:
Classically clean 57 65 20 66
Category 2:
Contextually clean 11988
Category 3:
Mildly leading 1 5 22 32
Category 4:
Strongly leading 0 1 32 10
Total 69 80 82 116
Interview length 80 minutes 95 minutes 85 minutes 77 minutes
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
Interview: researcher 1 Interview: researcher 2 Interview: researcher 3 Interview: researcher 4
83% 81%
24%
57%
16%
11% 10% 7%
1%
6%
27% 28%
0% 1%
39%
9%
ategor 1: Classically clean ategor 2: Contextually clean
3: Midly leading
4: Strongly leading
+ Karel
+ Ema + Františka + Ema
Figure 6. e frequency of questions in each category of clean-ness in the interviews
analyzed (percentage)
As far as the analyzed comments are concerned, Table 6 and Figure 7 show that
Researcher3 and Researcher 4 had ahigher number of comments in Categories 3
and 4 than the rst two researchers, who are more experienced in conducting Clean
Language interviewing.
145
|
Table 6
e frequency of comments in each category of clean-ness in the interviews analyzed (abso-
lute numbers)
Clean-ness rating of comments
Interview:
researcher 1
+ Karel
Interview:
researcher 2
+ Ema
Interview:
researcher 3
+ Františka
Interview:
researcher 4
+ Ema
Category 1:
Positively inuencing comments 18 38 26 34
Category 2:
Context-bound comments 12 15 8 22
Category 3:
Inuencing comments 2 0 15 15
Category 4:
Strongly inuencing comments 0 0 26 15
Total 32 53 75 86
Interview length 80 minutes 95 minutes 85 minutes 77 minutes
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
Interview: researcher 1 Interview: researcher 2 Interview: researcher 3 Interview: researcher 4
56.3%
71.7%
34.7%
39.5%
37.5%
28.3%
10.7%
25.6%
6.3%
0.0%
20.0% 17.4%
0.0%
34.7%
17.4%
Category 1: Positively influencing comments
Category 2: Context-bound comments
Category 3: Influencing comments
Category 4: Strongly influencing comments
+ Karel (Participant 1)
+ Ema (Participant 2) + Františka (Participant 3) + Ema (Participant 4)
0.0%
Figure 7. e frequency of comments in each category of clean-ness in the interviews
analyzed (percentage)
146
|
5.5.1 Discussion of the quantitative analysis
Overall, it can be said that Researchers 1 and 2 not only used more Category 1
questions but also signicantly fewer Category 3 and 4 comments. On the contrary,
Researchers 3 and 4 used ‘clean’ questions less and used signicantly more Category
3 and 4 comments. We interpret these results in relation to the level of experience
of the researchers in applying clean language to interviews. Researchers 1 and 2
have more intensive experience with the use of clean language, not only in research
interviews but also in coaching and therapeutic interviews, interviews focused on
reective practice. is leads us to the conclusion that, in order to master the applica-
tion of clean language in research interviews, training for researchers is necessary.
is should include not only developing an understanding how the method works
but also repeated practice in asking questions, supported by feedback from amore
experienced practitioner. It can be assumed that more intense training will also lead
to areduction in the number of Category 3 and 4 comments. For researchers not
suciently familiar with amethod, it is important to concentrate when conducting
interviews on the exact process of asking questions. is may detract the researcher
from the interviewee’s lived experience and, at the same time, the informant’s atten-
tion may be detracted from the topic reected on.
Example1
So how... Idon’t know how to articulate this but what does it look like?
Example2
Well, um, what would that be, um, Idon’t know what to call it, Idon’t want to call it
indents or something similar, simply what is that you’d like to achieve, like this?
5.6 Conclusion
Overall, there are anumber of conclusions that can be drawn from our evaluation of
the use of Clean Language interviewing for data collection in research.
• We dened the categories and sub-categories of clean-ness (1 to 4) for the questions
asked. Further research would be necessary, which would also proceed inductively,
and which could independently identify more categories. Such categories could be
subsequently compared, which could lead to arenement of the categories. e
rened categories could then be used for deductive coding of interviews for the
purposes of evaluating the interviews conducted.
• We dened the categories and sub-categories of clean-ness (1 to 4) for the com-
ments used.
147
|
• We discussed the nature of the categories of questions and comments in relation
to other interviewing methods that are also close to the rst-person perspective in
practice.
• We quantied each category of questions and comments for four interviews to gain
an overview of the extent to which we were able to adhere to clean-ness in data
collection. When interviewing using clean language, less experienced researchers
had not only alower number of Category1 clean questions but also asignicantly
higher number of Category 3 and 4 comments than those researchers who had un-
dergone longer training.
• A seemingly trivial nding, yet important from our perspective, revealed by the
quantitative analysis t was that the quality of an interview (and thus also the expec-
ted quality of data) depends on the level of experience of the researchers.
Auseful extension of the results of the evaluation could be the use of conversational
analysis. is could help us nd out how questions and comments inuence inform-
ant statements. For example, we judge from subjective observations that informants
are being educated while being interviewed. Researchers repeatedly experienced that,
aer acertain number of interviews, informants were able to predict what question
the researcher would ask. ey would oen ask and answer the question themselves.
We assume that this experience goes hand in hand with becoming more sensitive to
descriptions and the reection of one’s own experience. is could be seen as anatural
eect of long-term use of Clean Language interviewing. In the context of the examina-
tion of the experience of human consciousness, in some interview methods, for ex-
ample, descriptive experience sampling (Hulburt, 2011), informants are trained to be
able to capture their inner experience. is is an inspirational idea because it not only
educates the researcher but also the participant in how to approach their experience.
It seems that Clean Language is indirectly responsible for this.
Our experience shows that, at the beginning of an interview, ‘clean’ questions are
perceived as unnatural by the informants (they are puzzled by what they perceive as
‘strange’ questions). e informants then tend not to focus on the content of their
experience but instead comment on the actual question (is oen happened with the
question “What kind of X is that X?”).
However, in our concept of Clean Language interviewing, the clean-ness of aquestion
is not the same as the naturalness of aquestion. If we equated clean-ness with natural-
ness, the informant could perceive some questions as ‘clean’ even though they contain
many assumptions. It is not important for aresearcher to explain to the informant
which question is clean and which is not. In the interview process, the informant
gradually (even subconsciously) learns the interviewing logic and becomes more sen-
sitive to their own experience. As discussed above, this also brings us, the researchers,
closer to the relational and contextual concept of clean-ness in an interview.