ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Invasion success is dependent on the ability of a species to discover and exploit novel food resources. Within this context, individuals must be willing to taste novel foods. They must also be capable of evaluating the nutritional content of new foods, and selecting their relative intake in order to fulfil their nutritional needs. Whereas the former capacity is well studied, little is known about the latter capacity. First, using the common myna as a model avian invader species, we quantified the willingness of mynas to taste novel foods relative to familiar ones. Mynas readily tasted high protein (HP) novel foods and consumed them in higher quantities compared to a familiar food. Data showed that at three different levels – mixes, ingredients and macronutrients – intake could not be explained by a random model. In experiment 2, we confirmed that mynas were making their selection based on protein (P) content rather than a selection for novelty per se. When given the choice of three equally unfamiliar foods, mynas again ate disproportionately from the high protein relative to high lipid and high carbohydrate foods. Analysis revealed that mynas consumed amounts of protein that were closer to the ones in their natural diet. Finally, in experiment 3, we measured inter-individual variation in innovation and exploration propensities, and examined associations with inter-individual variation in consumption of specific macronutrients. This analysis revealed that individuals that selected HP pellets were more exploratory and individuals that selected HC pellets were quicker to solve the innovative foraging task. These findings indicate that not only the willingness to taste novel foods, but also the capacity to evaluate their nutritional content, might be central to the myna's substantial ecological success.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1432
Tasting novel foods and selecting nutrient content in a highly
successful ecological invader, the common myna
Chloe Peneaux, Gabriel E. Machovsky-Capuska, David Raubenheimer, Francoise Lermite,
Charlotte Rousseau, Tanya Ruhan, John C. Rodger and Andrea S. Griffin
C. Peneaux (http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2746-5820), F. Lermite, C. Rousseau and A. S. Griffin (andrea.griffin@newcastle.edu.au), School of
Psychology, Univ. of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia. – G. E. Machovsky-Capuska and D. Raubenheimer, Charles Perkins Centre and
Faculty of Veterinary Science, e Univ. of Sydney, Sydney, Australia. – T. Ruhan and J. C. Rodger, School of Environmental and Life Sciences,
Univ. of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia.
Invasion success is dependent on the ability of a species to discover and exploit novel food resources. Within this context,
individuals must be willing to taste novel foods. ey must also be capable of evaluating the nutritional content of new
foods, and selecting their relative intake in order to fulfil their nutritional needs. Whereas the former capacity is well studied,
little is known about the latter capacity. First, using the common myna as a model avian invader species, we quantified
the willingness of mynas to taste novel foods relative to familiar ones. Mynas readily tasted high protein (HP) novel foods
and consumed them in higher quantities compared to a familiar food. Data showed that at three different levels – mixes,
ingredients and macronutrients – intake could not be explained by a random model. In experiment 2, we confirmed that
mynas were making their selection based on protein (P) content rather than a selection for novelty per se. When given the
choice of three equally unfamiliar foods, mynas again ate disproportionately from the high protein relative to high lipid and
high carbohydrate foods. Analysis revealed that mynas consumed amounts of protein that were closer to the ones in their
natural diet. Finally, in experiment 3, we measured inter-individual variation in innovation and exploration propensities,
and examined associations with inter-individual variation in consumption of specific macronutrients. is analysis revealed
that individuals that selected HP pellets were more exploratory and individuals that selected HC pellets were quicker to
solve the innovative foraging task. ese findings indicate that not only the willingness to taste novel foods, but also the
capacity to evaluate their nutritional content, might be central to the myna’s substantial ecological success.
To become established in novel environments, invasive
birds have to face new ecological challenges such as finding
new shelter and recognizing new predators and competitors
(Coleman and Mellgren 1994, Griffin et al. 2016). Within
this context, there is evidence that the ability to exploit
novel food resources is particularly important (Sol et al.
2011, 2012a). Avian species with high numbers of reports
of novel feeding behaviours, including consuming novel
foods and developing novel foraging techniques, are more
likely to become established when introduced to environ-
ments outside their native range than species with fewer such
reports (Sol et al. 2002, 2005, Wright et al. 2010). us,
to date, research has established that success in new envi-
ronments depends heavily upon an invader’s ability to con-
sume novel food, develop new foraging behaviours and/or
use pre-existing ones in novel contexts (Mayr 1965, Sol and
Lefebvre 2000, Sol et al. 2005, 2008, Griffin et al. 2016).
Foods are complex blends of many nutrients each of which
has their own effect on the forager (Raubenheimer et al.
2012). Lipid (L) and carbohydrate (C), in particular, play a
central role in metabolism and energy storage whereas pro-
tein (P) is fundamental to animals’ growth and reproduction
(McWilliams 2011). Nutritional goals can vary among indi-
viduals, for example as a function of age and/or sex (Simpson
et al. 2010), but also among geographically isolated popula-
tions and among species (Tait et al. 2014). e complexities
of nutrient content and their impact on individual health
generate a challenge for invaders. Novel foods are likely to
differ in their nutritional composition and physiological
properties to those foods that invaders have encountered
previously (Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016a). erefore,
not only must invaders be willing to consume novel foods,
they must also have the ability to identify the nutritional
content of these novel foods and combine their proportional
intake to fulfil their nutritional goals (Machovsky-Capuska
et al. 2016a).
Exploration and innovativeness in the foraging context
have been consistently linked to invasion and colonization
of novel habitats in birds (Holway and Suarez 1999, Sol and
Lefebvre 2000, Sol et al. 2002, Russell et al. 2010, Wright
et al. 2010). Nutritional intake might impact behaviour in
several ways. First, it is known that nutritional deficiencies
can produce personality biases in later life (Fraňková 1973,
Barnes et al. 1976, Almeida et al. 1991, 1993, 1994, 1996).
© 2017 e Authors. Journal of Avian Biology © 2017 Nordic Society Oikos
Subject Editor: Paul McDonald. Editor-in-Chief: Jan-Åke Nilsson. Accepted 19 July 2017
Journal of Avian Biology 48: 1432–1440, 2017
doi: 10.1111/jav.01456
1433
For example, low P intake during ontogeny is linked to
increased exploration in adult rats Rattus norvegicus. is
link between low P intake and activity might be maintained
through life as suggested by research in invertebrates. Crickets
Anabrus simplex deficient in P locomote more than crickets
satiated in protein (Simpson et al. 2006). Second, it is pos-
sible that the tendency to forage on certain nutrients specifi-
cally might enhance certain types of foraging behaviours. For
example, urban exploiters, which rely heavily upon human
discards rich in L and C, might have an enhanced motor
diversity due to their frequent handling of packaged human
discards in school playgrounds, supermarket carparks and
around fast food outlets. Higher motor diversity is known to
facilitate innovative foraging (Griffin et al. 2013a, Diquelou
et al. 2016). ese nutrient–behavioural relationships have
yet to be explored in invasive birds.
Classified as one of the world’s 100 worst invasive species
(Lowe et al. 2000), the common ‘Indian’ myna Acridotheres
tristis (recently proposed to be reclassified as Sturnus tristis
by Christidis and Boles (2008); hereafter mynas) is an excel-
lent model to investigate the physiological and behavioural
attributes of ecological invaders (Griffin et al. 2010, 2013a,
b, 2014, Sol et al. 2011, 2012b). Introduced to Australia
late 19th century (Martin 1996, Pell and Tidemann 1997,
Tidemann 2005), mynas are now present along much of the
east coast (Grarock et al. 2013). e species is well known
for its ability to learn about novel environmental stimuli.
Previous research has demonstrated learning of bait avoid-
ance (Feare 2010), the location of human hunters (Dhami
and Nagle 2009), novel predators (Griffin 2008), and dan-
gerous places (Griffin et al. 2010, Griffin and Haythorpe
2011). e species is also known to reduce flight distances in
areas highly frequented by humans (McGiffin et al. 2013).
Based on the traditional approach of quantifying dietary
generalism in terms of foods consumed, mynas are docu-
mented to be generalist omnivores. Indeed, the species has
been reported to consume a broad range of foods includ-
ing invertebrates, plants, fruits, and human discards (Moeed
1975, 1976, Sengupta 1976, Pell and Tidemann 1997,
Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016b). Diet generalism is a
well-established predictor of invasion success (Cassey 2002,
Blackburn et al. 2009) and is likely to explain some part of
the myna’s extraordinary worldwide ecological success. On
the other hand, mynas are known to taste novel foods (Sol
et al. 2012b), but it is not known whether they would choose
novel foods when given the choice. It is also not known
whether mynas have the capacity to identify the nutri-
tional composition of novel foods and adjust their relative
consumption to achieve their nutritional goals. Finally, it
is not known whether the tendency to selectively consume
certain nutrients is associated with individual variation in
exploration and/or innovative foraging behaviour.
In the present study we combined behavioural observa-
tions with captive feeding trials and nutritional geometry to
examine food and nutritional selections in common myna
birds. In particular, we aimed to: 1) determine whether
mynas fed with novel foods select their foods, ingredients
and nutrients in non-random proportions, 2) establish the
macronutrient composition of myna diets using high-protein
(HP), high-lipid (HL) and high-carbohydrate (HC) pellets,
and 3) determine whether consumptions of particular
macronutrient were associated with innovation and explora-
tion propensities.
Methods
Subjects and husbandry
A total of 21 birds were caught in and around Newcastle
(NSW, Australia) using a trap specifically designed for this
species (Tidemann and ANU Fenner School 2009). All
twenty-one participated in experiment 1, and a subset of
ten also participated in experiment 2. Upon capture, birds
were transported to the Central Animal House (CAH) at the
Univ. of Newcastle, where they were measured (tarsus, beak
and wing), weighed and individually marked with coloured
plastic leg bands. Birds were then released into outdoor
group aviaries (2.0 1.0 2.0 m, H D W) and treated
for internal parasites during a period of ten days. Mynas were
then transferred to a large flight aviary (2.2 1.2 4.4 m)
where they were held until testing commenced. Food and
water was available ad libitum at all times. Birds were fed
a commercial brand of puppy dog pellets (SUPERCOAT®
Puppy with Real Meat). Dog pellets were used because they
are small enough to be swallowed by mynas easily. Dog pel-
lets were the most widely used food to bait myna traps in
Australia and were originally selected as suitable bait based
on myna food choice tests undertaken in the context of
developing a species-specific trap (Tidemann and ANU
Fenner School 2009). In addition, free-ranging mynas fre-
quently consume dog pellets from suburban back gardens
with dogs (Parsons et al. 2006). Based on these observations
and on veterinarian advice, they were an adequate food to
feed captive-held mynas and have been used since 2006 in
our myna research program.
During the food and macronutrient selection experiments,
birds were held in individual cages (0.6 0.6 0.6 m)
located outdoors but protected from weather conditions by
a roof. Individual cages were equipped with a perch and a
nest box and spatially arranged such that birds were in close
visual and acoustic contact. is arrangement facilitates
mynas’ adjustment to individual housing (Griffin and Boyce
2009, Griffin and Haythorpe 2011, Sol et al. 2012a, Griffin
et al. 2013a).
Procedure
Experiment 1: novel food selection
To measure the food choices of mynas in the presence
of new, unfamiliar foods, 21 mynas were transferred to
individual cages and left for two days to acclimatize to their
new holding conditions. During this time, each myna had
ad libitum access to water, as well as ad libitum access to the
now-familiar dog pellet food.
Over the course of the following three days, birds were
presented with a simultaneous choice of three foods. To
create these foods, we mixed ingredients that differed in
their macronutrient composition: dog pellets (DP, 14.0%
L, 29.0% P and 46.6% C), commercial insectivore powder
named Wombaroo® (W, 12.0% L, 52.0% P and 18.0% C)
and commercial parrot food called Superior Egg & Biscuit
1434
Vetafarm® (EB, 8.0% L, 16.0% P and 63.0% C). e foods
were created manually under laboratory conditions in order
to produce experimental pellets that were visually identical
and novel to the captive birds. e ingredients were selected
with the primary aim of gradually reducing the proportion
of familiar content (dog pellets), and increasing the propor-
tion of novel content. is allowed us to quantify the mynas’
willingness to consume increasingly novel foods. e foods
varied along a gradient of gustatory familiarity, but also dif-
fered in their nutritional composition. e first food (Mix
1), contained solely the birds’ familiar DP and the nutri-
tional composition of the mix (dry mass) was: 15.6% L,
32.4% P and 52.0% C; the second food (Mix 2) contained
different proportions of DP and W and the nutritional
composition of the mix (dry mass) was: 15.2% L, 44.1%
P and 40.6% C; and the third food (Mix 3) contained dif-
ferent proportions of DP, W and EB, and the nutritional
composition of the mix (dry mass) was: 13.5% L, 36.7%
P and 49.7% C.
In order to measure which of the three foods mynas
selected, 7.5 g of each food was placed in one of three small
individual dishes (3 cm diameter, 2 cm deep). e three
dishes were attached to a wooden board so that they could
not be tipped over (see photography in Supplementary
material Appendix 1 Fig. A1). e 3-dish board was then
offered to each bird for three successive 24-h time periods.
Each food was presented in each dish once across the three
testing days in order to avoid specific location preferences.
At the end of each 24-h period, any remaining food was col-
lected and weighed with a high-precision scale. Each cage
was equipped with a removable floor tray that collected
any dropped food. Any food in the tray was collected and
incorporated to the left-over food before weighing. A con-
trol board with equivalent amounts of food was placed in an
adjacent empty cage in order to measure any change in mass
that might have occurred as a consequence of desiccation or
humidity. As weight changes of all control foods placed in
an empty myna cage were negligible (weight changes across
the three days (in percentage of total food consumed): Mix
1: –1.1%; Mix 2: –0.91%; Mix 3: –0.92%), they were not
considered further.
Experiment 2: macronutrient selection
Experiment one manipulated food familiarity, but in doing
so, simultaneously manipulated nutrient content. Hence,
the purpose of experiment 2 was to manipulate macronu-
trient content while holding energy content and familiarity
constant (i.e. all foods were novel). Ten common mynas were
transferred from the group flight aviary to individual cages
identical to those used in experiment 1 and allowed two days
to acclimatize to their new surroundings. As in experiment
1, birds had access to food (DP) and water ad libitum during
acclimatization. Over the course of the following three days,
each bird was presented with a simultaneous choice of three
semi-synthetic pellet foods that were isoenergetic (2600 kcal
g–1), but differed in their macronutrient composition (P, L,
C) (for details see Machovsky-Capuska et al. (2016b)). Dur-
ing tests, 25 g of each food was placed in one of the three
small individual plastic bottles attached to one side of the
cage so that they could not be tipped over (see photography
in Supplementary material Appendix 1 Fig. A1). e three
bottles were then offered to each bird for three successive
24-h time periods, as in experiment 1. At the end of each 24
h period, any remaining semi-synthetic food was collected
and weighed with a high-precision scale.
Experiment 3: behavioural analysis
e aim of experiment 3 was to explore potential
relationships between individual variation in macronutri-
ent intake and individual variation in exploration propensity
and innovativeness.
Innovation test one potential approach to quantifying
innovative foraging is to measure an individual’s propensity
to solve novel foraging problems (Webster and Lefebvre
2001, Griffin and Guez 2014). To this end, each myna was
presented with either a puzzle box consisting of a closed Petri
dish (presented right way up with a flexible handle or upside
with a hook attached to the top part) or a small Plexiglas box
with a lid and a handle (for examples, see schematic in Sup-
plementary material Appendix 1 Fig. A2). Mynas could solve
the extractive foraging problem by pulling the small handle
or the hook off the top of the puzzle box, or, alternatively, by
either levering or pushing the lid upwards. e reward (a dog
pellet or a mealworm) was visible but the apparatus needed
to be opened to access the food.
ese tests were conducted early in the morning following
an overnight food deprivation period. To avoid a neophobia
response to the tasks, the apparatus had been placed in the
same location as the mynas’ daily feeding dishes with an
available food reward the evening before the trial. To begin
the trial, the tasks were presented with one visible reward
inside but unavailable without solving the task. Trials were
video recorded for 30 min. following the introduction of
the apparatus. If the bird failed to make contact with the
task within 30-min the first time round, they were given a
second opportunity at least 0.5–1.5 h later. To investigate
differences between birds in problem-solving propensities,
an innovation score was calculated using latency to solve (s)
minus latency to first contact the apparatus (s). A score of
1801s was attributed to birds that failed to solve the task.
Exploration test – exploration propensity was assessed in
an unfamiliar room under artificial light (Dingemanse et al.
2002) (see room’s plan in Supplementary material Appendix
1 Fig. A3). e room included five artificial trees each bearing
five perches. e ground was divided in four areas (87 116
cm). Birds were moved from the aviary to an individual cage
placed on wheels. e individual cages were covered with a
large piece of opaque material and rolled into the unfamil-
iar testing room. e opaque material was gently lifted and
the bird left alone to acclimate 10 min to this environment
before the experimentor remotely opened the doors of their
cage (1 27 55 cm). e bird was given a 15-min latency
to exit its home-cage, if it had not departed from the cage by
the end of that period the experimenter approached the cage
to induce exit. Once the bird exited the cage, it was given 10
min to move freely around the room. Birds had the ability
to return to the home cage during the test, however the nest
box remained closed at all times during testing. White noise
was played back through a loudspeaker to mask the sounds
of birds in a nearby room.
1435
JMP® ver. 10 (SAS Inst., Cary, NC, 1989-2007.10) (for
PCA). Two-tailed tests were used throughout and alpha
levels were set at 0.05. All visual representations, including
RMT models, were created with SPSS Statistics 21 (IBM
2012).
Data deposition
Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: < http://
dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.3q7k6 > (Peneaux et al. 2017).
Results
Experiment 1
e amount of food consumed by mynas differed signifi-
cantly across the three food mixes (Mix 1: median 12.03 g,
range 17.53 g (4.56–22.09 g); Mix 2: median 17.59 g,
range 15.08 g (6.36–21.44 g); Mix 3: median
16.20 g, range 16.05 g (5.81–21.86 g); approximative gen-
eral independence test: n 21, c² 8.75, p 0.01, Fig. 1a).
Mix 1 was consumed in significantly smaller quantities than
Mix 3 and Mix 2 (approximate 2-sample permutation test
with Bonferroni–Holm correction: Mix 1/2: p 0.04;
Mix 2/3: p 0.04), indicating that the food novel in its
appearance, but most familiar in terms of its content (DP),
was the least consumed food. Mixes 2 and 3, which were
equally novel in appearance, but contained increasing propor-
tions of novel contents, were not consumed in significantly
different proportions (approximate 2-sample permutation
test with Bonferroni–Holm correction: p 0.98).
e amount consumed of each of the three ingredients
and nutrients also differed significantly after the three days
of experimentation (approximative general independence
test: ingredients: n 21, c² 38.67, p 0.0001, Fig. 1b;
nutrients: n 21, c² 39.75, p 0.0001, Fig. 1c), showing
that mynas fed non-randomly from their foods dishes at the
three differente scales analysed.
e macronutrient composition of the diet (dry mass)
estimated during the experiment 1 was 37.3 % P ( 0.2 SE):
14.8 % L ( 0.1 SE): 47.9% C ( 0.2 SE).
Experiment 2
e amount consumed of HL, HP and HP foods differed
significantly (HL: median 4.77 g, range 17.21 g
(0.55–17.75 g); HP: median 30.19 g, range 23.44 g
(20.08–43.52 g); HC: median 7.00 g, range 24.40 g
(2.19–26.58 g); approximative general independence test:
n 10, c² 15; p 0.0001). HP pellets were consumed
in significantly greater quantities than HL and HC pellets
(approximate 2-sample permutation test with Bonferroni–
Holm correction: HP/HL: p 0.01; HP/HC: p 0.01).
e macronutrient composition of the diet (dry mass)
estimated during the experiment 2 was 59.8% P ( 0.8 SE):
22.0 % L ( 0.7 SE): 18.4% C ( 1.0 SE).
e RMT model showed the differences of the P:L:C ratios
of the diets estimated for experiments 1 (P:L:C 2.5:1.0:3.2)
and 2 (P:L:C 2.7:1.0:0.8) with previously reported
In order to determine exploration tendency, several
variables were scored from the videos, including: number
of trees, perches and ground areas visited. To assess the
bird’s activity, we scored the number of movements (flights
between trees, hops between perches, walks on the ground
and returns to cage). e latency to exit the cage was also
scored.
Statistical analyses
Variables scored for the exploration (i.e. number of move-
ments, number of zones visited, number of floor-zones
visited, number of trees visited, number of branches vis-
ited, and latency to exit home cage) were compiled in a
principal component analysis (PCA), using the correla-
tion matrix and a Varimax rotation, in order to reduce the
number of independent variables and obtain an individual
exploration score for each bird. Values on the first prin-
cipal component were used as exploration score for each
individual bird.
Results were analysed using non-parametric statistics in
the form of permutation test (sometimes called a random-
ization test, a resampling method similar to bootstrapping
(Good 2000)). In experiment 1, we tested whether mynas
ate randomly from the novel foods provided by comparing
the consumption of foods, ingredients and nutrients with a
null expectation in which mynas ate equal amounts using
approximative general independence tests (repeated mea-
sures). In experiment 2, the same approach was used to com-
pare the amounts of each HP, HL and HC foods consumed
by the birds. Descriptive statistics for food consumption
were also presented as median and range. Significant effects
were followed up with post-hoc pairwise comparisons using
approximate 2-sample permutation tests stratified by indi-
viduals and p-values were corrected using the Bonferroni–
Holm correction (noted p’).
For each semi-synthetic food consumed in experiment
2, correlations between the total amount of macronutrient
(P, C, and L) consumed and behavioural data were examined
using another form of permutation test, a general Indepen-
dence test.
Following Raubenheimer (2011), we used nutritional
geometry (right-angled mixture triangles, RMT) to portray
the experimental food choices of mynas faced with unfamiliar
foods. For experiment 1, we determined food consumption
and diet of mynas at three different scales (foods, ingredients
and nutrients) and we compared observed intakes against a
null hypothesis that mynas consumed equal amounts. For
experiment 2, we obtained the macronutrient composition
of diets (expressed as P:L:C dry mass ratios) from the con-
sumption of semi-synthetic foods. We then compared our
results with the 1) null hypothesis of consumption of equal
amounts (dry mass), 2) macronutrient composition (dry
mass) of the diet of free-ranging mynas as determined by
stomach content analyses (Sengupta 1976, see Machovsky-
Capuska et al. 2016b for more details), and 3) macronutri-
ent composition (dry mass) of the diet obtained using an
experimental ‘cafeteria’ design (Machovsky-Capuska et al.
2016b).
All statistical analyses were carried out using R ver. 3.1.1
(R Development Core Team) (for Permutation tests) and
1436
Experiment 3
Correlation analysis revealed the existence of two relation-
ships between behavioural traits and the consumption of
specific macronutrients (Fig. 3, see full correlation table and
scatter plot panel of the correlation analysis in Supplemen-
tary material Appendix 2 Table A1 and Fig. A2). Individuals
that consumed larger total amounts of carbohydrate pellets
in a three-way choice between C, L and P were faster to
solve a novel extractive foraging task (Fig. 3a; approximate
general independence test: n 10, Z –1.89, p 0.04).
We found no evidence of a relationship between innovation
latencies and intake of the other two macronutrients (P and
L) (approximate general independence test: protein: n 10,
Z –0.63, p 0.57; lipid: n 10, Z 1.37, p 0.17).
Behavioural variables used to describe levels of exploration
were aggregated into a PCA. e KMO test suggested the
matrix was appropriate for use in a PCA (KMO 0.74). e
first axis explained 58.2% of the variance in the data and was
considered to be a good summary of the data (Budaev 2010).
Values on this first axis were used as the exploration score of
each individual, where high scores indicated high tendency to
explore (Table 1). Correlational analyses between each indi-
vidual’s PC1 score and its relative intake of each macronutri-
ent during experiment 2 revealed an association between an
individual’s relative protein intake and exploration tendency.
Mynas that consumed more protein had higher explora-
tion PC scores (Fig. 3b; approximate general independence
test: n 10, Z 1.91, p 0.04). No relationships were
found between an individual’s exploration score and either
its carbohydrate (approximate general Independence test:
n 10, Z –0.59, p 0.58); or its lipid intake (approximate
general independence test: n 10, Z –1.31, p 0.20).
Discussion
In this study, we aimed to analyse mynas’ food and nutri-
tional selection through the presentation of novel foods. We
experimental feeding trials for mynas (P:L:C 6.3:1.0:0.1)
and also with the reconstructed natural diet (P:L:C
3.3:1.0:1.4, see Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016b for more
details) (Fig. 2).
Figure 1. Right-angled mixture triangle showing (as a % of dry weight) the foraging choices of captive mynas during the three days at
different scales. (a) Myna diet (black hollow circles) clusters around the range of Mix 3 intakes, (b) the model shows the region of
ingredients’ space (dotted line) that was accessible to the birds (black hollow circles) given the three mixes (black hollow triangles) they were
provided during the experiment and (c) nutritional niche accessible to mynas delineated by dotted lines as defined by the three different
mixtures (black filled squares) offered to the birds. e three plots compare the consumption of foods with a null hypothesis that mynas
consume equal amounts (black filled circles).
Figure 2. Right-angled mixture triangle showing (as a % of dry
mass) the macronutrient preferences in common mynas. Empty
circles represent macronutrient intakes of individual birds and the
squares represent macronutrient compositions of the three semisyn-
thetic food pellets offered during the captive feeding trials (hollow
blue square HP diet; solid green square HC diet; solid grey
square HL diet). e green star represents the macronutrient
composition of the diet estimated in experiment 1; the gold circle
represents the macronutrient composition of the diet estimated in
experiment 2; the solid red circle represents the natural diet esti-
mated from Sengupta (1976, for more details see Machovsky-
Capuska et al. 2016b) and the hollow red circle represents the diet
estimated from free-ranging mynas obtained from Machovsky-
Capuska et al. (2016b). e black triangle represents the null
hypothesis for a balance dietary nutrient intake consuming equal
amounts of the three macronutrients.
1437
intended to identify food selection patterns and also whether
the patterns of consumption of some macronutrients were
correlated with innovation and exploration propensities. In
experiment 1, birds showed significantly less interest for a
familiar dog food relative to two other foods, despite the dog
food mix being the most familiar in terms of content. Our
analyses also showed that mynas fed in non-random pro-
portions at three different scales analysed: foods, ingredients
and nutrients. In experiment 2, mynas selectively consumed
HP when simultaneously offered a choice between HP, HC,
Figure 3. Correlations between: (a) mean ( SE) innovation score and total amount of HC pellets after a 3-d period (g); (b) mean ( SE)
exploration score and total amount of HP pellets consumed after a 3-d period (g). Innovation score solving latency – first contact
latency.
Table 1. Orthogonally (Varimax) rotated component loadings on
first axis for the exploration test. Bold indicates variables contribut-
ing to a component’s meaning.
Behavioural variables PC1
Number of trees visited 0.87
Number of branches visited 0.97
Total number of zones visited (ground air) 0.93
Number of movements 0.87
Latency to first exit the cage –0.09
Number of floor-zones visited 0.42
1438
e present study revealed that mynas consuming higher
amounts of HP foods were more exploratory when con-
fronted with a novel environment. Moreover, individual’s
tendency to explore a novel space is a repeatable trait in
mynas (Lermite et al. 2017). To reduce the impacts of behav-
iour unrelated to exploration during this open-field test, spe-
cific methodologies were used (i.e. individuals were not food
deprived and were free to exit home cage) to limit the expres-
sion of stress-related responses and facilitate information
acquisition (Dingemanse et al. 2002, Mettke-Hofmann et al.
2002, but see discussions in Carter et al. 2013 and Lermite
et al. 2017). Together, these findings suggest that mynas with
higher exploratory tendencies are likely to be more sensitive
to dietary fluctuations in protein. A relationship between
low-protein diet and exploration has been demonstrated
in rats (Almeida et al. 1991, 1993, 1994, 1996). A lack of
dietary-protein has been suggested to cause an increase of
‘impulsiveness’, driving malnourished rats to explore more
open-arm novelty (Almeida et al. 1991, 1993, 1994, 1996).
is behavioural pattern reflects a decrease in anxiety in
the protein-deficient individuals. Protein-malnutrition
is suspected by the authors to cause deleterious effects on
brain structures underlying inhibitory behaviours in situ-
ations promoting anxiety (Almeida et al. 1994). Hence,
lower anxiety levels induced by protein malnutrition could
act as a mediator for higher levels of exploration in these
protein-seeking birds. Moreover, in the context of an urban
environment, proteins are expected to be rare and extremely
valuable (Eagle and Pelton 1983, Pierotti and Annett 1987,
Murphy 1993, Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016b). Foraging
for HP foods in urban environments (e.g. insects) would
therefore require longer periods of time spent exploring the
environment, which could in turn produce a feedback loop
reinforcing the expression of exploratory behaviour in these
P deficient individuals.
e present study also revealed that mynas consuming
higher amounts of HC foods were faster to solve a novel
extractive foraging task. Innovative foraging behaviour has
been linked to the ability to use a greater variety of motor
actions in a foraging context (Griffin et al. 2014). Urban
mynas are often seen foraging in school playgrounds,
food outlets and supermarket carparks (Sol et al. 2012b).
Anthropogenic food sources are often wrapped in differ-
ent packaging that are likely to substantially challenge and
enhance mynas motor diversity skills. Human discards
contain high-levels of C relative to P (Pierotti and Annett
1987), and birds adapted to access and consume these food
items are likely to develop a taste for HC foods. is might
explain the present association between innovative foraging
and consumption of C found here.
e mechanisms underlying food and nutritional choices
and post-ingestive processing in invaders are likely to be
a key to understand how these species succeed in novel
environments (Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016b). Here, we
demonstrated that common mynas select their foods based
on their nutritional composition. e tendency to sample
unknown foods to fulfil nutritional requirements is likely to
contribute to the invasion success of mynas, but this remains
to be tested. Our findings linking macronutrient intake
and exploration and foraging innovation highlight the
HL pellets. e RMT analyses also revealed that mynas
consumed different amounts of protein to their natural diet
described by Machovsky-Capuska et al. (2016b). Finally,
experiment 3 revealed the existence of two nutrient–behav-
ioural relationships. Mynas that consumed greater amounts
of HP pellets had a stronger tendency to explore whereas
those mynas that ate more HC pellets were faster to solve a
novel foraging problem.
e history of captive myna feeding in the present study,
together with their patterns of food choices during test-
ing, strongly suggest that mynas selectively consume novel
foods relatively to the nutritional content. In experiment 1,
we showed that when mynas were given a choice between
foods containing a gradually reduced proportion of famil-
iar content (DP), they chose to consume higher amount of
the foods that contained increased proportion of unfamiliar
content (EB and W). Birds actually chose novel combination
of HP foods.
Experiment 2 confirmed that it was not novelty per se
that was driving this selection, but rather protein content,
by showing that mynas selected HP relative to HC or HL
when all three foods were equally novel. ese observations
are consistent with the findings of Machovsky-Capuska et al.
(2016b). In that study, free-ranging urban mynas selectively
chose to consume almost exclusively protein-enriched foods,
leading the authors to speculate that free-ranging mynas
might be protein deficient. Here, we extend these findings
by confirming that mynas are willing to consume novel
foods in such proportions that would enable them to reach
their nutritional goals.
Animals are likely to detect certain nutrients using
olfactory and gustatory cues (Simpson and Raubenheimer
2012). For example, gallinaceous chicks display an unlearned
predisposition to peck at foods (Suboski and Bartashunas
1984), which presumably enables them to perceptually
locate salt and glucose. In contrast, the identity of plants
containing HP, which lacks a perceptual signature, is socially
transmitted from mother to chick in gallinaceous chicks
(Allen and Clarke 2005). As our mynas were individually
held and all foods were visually identical and offered ran-
domly, mynas were not able to mimic each other’s food
choices. Furthermore, our novel foods had no resemblance to
insects (that are also HP foods), meaning that mynas could
not have been able to rely on previous experience to select
foods more likely to contain protein. Hence, their detec-
tion and selection of HP foods were likely to be triggered
by different physiological mechanisms including systemic
nutrient sensing mechanisms, neural circuits that control
feeding behaviour, hormonal feedback from body reserves
(Morton and Schwartz 2011) and also post-ingestive regula-
tory responses that assist in the adjustment of imbalanced
nutrients (Simpson and Raubenheimer 2012). A challenge
ahead should aim to determine the mechanisms by which
mynas sense nutrients and in particular detect HP foods.
Also, further experiments will be undertaken to rule out the
alternative possibility that mynas only demonstrated taste
preferences for particular foods instead of actively regulat-
ing their macronutrient intake, although a large body of
literature spanning many animal taxa suggest this is unlikely
(Simpson and Raubenheimer 2012).
1439
Diquelou, M. C., Griffin, A. S. and Sol, D. 2016. e role of motor
diversity in foraging innovations: a cross-species comparison
in urban birds. – Behav. Ecol. 27: 584–591.
Eagle, T. C. and Pelton, M. R. 1983. Seasonal nutrition of black
bears in the Great Smoky Mountains National Park. – Int.
Conf. Bear Res. Manage. 5: 94–101.
Feare, C. J. 2010. Invasive bird eradication from tropical oceanic
islands. – Aliens 30: 12–19.
Fraňková, S. 1973. Effect of protein-calorie malnutrition on the
development of social behavior in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 6:
33–43.
Good, P. 2000. Permutation tests: a practical guide to resampling
methods for testing hypotheses. – Springer.
Grarock, K., Lindenmayer, D. B., Wood, J. T. and Tidemann, C.
R. 2013. Using invasion process theory to enhance the under-
standing and management of introduced species: a case study
reconstructing the invasion sequence of the common myna
(Acridotheres tristis). – J. Environ. Manage. 129: 398–409.
Griffin, A. S. 2008. Socially acquired predator avoidance: is it just
classical conditioning? – Brain Res. Bull. 76: 264–271.
Griffin, A. S. and Boyce, H. M. 2009. Indian mynahs, Acridotheres
tristis, learn about dangerous places by observing the fate of
others. – Anim. Behav. 78: 79–84.
Griffin, A. S. and Haythorpe, K. 2011. Learning from watching
alarmed demonstrators: does the cause of alarm matter?
– Anim. Behav. 81: 1163–1169.
Griffin, A. S. and Guez, D. 2014. Innovation and problem solving:
a review of common mechanisms. – Behav. Process. 109:
121–134.
Griffin, A. S., Boyce, H. M. and MacFarlane, G. R. 2010. Social
learning about places: observers may need to detect both social
alarm and its cause to learn. – Anim. Behav. 79: 459–465.
Griffin, A. S., Guez, D., Lermite, F. and Patience, M. 2013a.
Tracking changing environments: innovators are fast, but not
flexible learners. – PLoS One 8: e84907.
Griffin, A. S., Lermite, F., Perea, M. and Guez, D. 2013b. To
innovate or not: contrasting effects of social groupings on safe
and risky foraging in Indian mynahs. – Anim. Behav. 86:
1291–1300.
Griffin, A. S., Diquelou, M. and Perea, M. 2014. Innovative
problem solving in birds: a key role of motor diversity. – Anim.
Behav. 92: 221–227.
Griffin, A. S., Guez, D., Federspiel, I., Diquelou, M. and Lermite,
F. 2016. Invading new environments: a mechanistic framework
linking motor diversity and cognitive processes to invasion
success. – In: Sol, D. and Weis, J. S. (eds), Biological invasions
and animal behaviour. Cambridge Univ. Press, pp. 26–46.
Holway, D. A. and Suarez, A. V. 1999. Animal behaviour: an
essential component of invasion biology. – Trends Ecol. Evol.
14: 328–330.
IBM 2012. IBM SPSS statistics for Windows, version 21.0. – IBM,
Armonk, NY.
Lermite, F., Peneaux, C. and Griffin, A. S. 2017. Personality and
problem-solving in common mynas (Acridotheres tristis).
– Behav. Process. 134: 87–94.
Lowe, S., Browne, M., Boudjelas, S. and De Poorter, M. 2000.
100 of the world’s worst invasive alien species: a selection from
the global invasive species database. – e invasive species
specialist group (SSC/IUCN).
Machovsky-Capuska, G. E., Senior, A. M., Simpson, S. J. and
Raubenheimer, D. 2016a. e multidimensional nutritional
niche. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 31: 355–365.
Machovsky-Capuska, G. E., Senior, A. M., Zantis, S. P., Barna, K.,
Cowieson, A. J., Pandya, S., Pavard, C., Shiels, M. and
Raubenheimer, D. 2016b. Dietary protein selection in a free-
ranging urban population of common myna birds. – Behav.
Ecol. 27: 219–227.
importance of nutrition in the development of exploratory
and behaviourally flexible phenotypes in a very successful
invader.
Acknowledgements is project has been assisted by the New South
Wales Government through its Environmental Trust.
Permits – All husbandry and experimental procedures were approved
by the Animal Care and Ethics Committee of the Univ. of Newcastle
(Animal Research Authorities A-2011-127, A-2014-424, and
A-2014-425).
References
Allen, T. and Clarke, J. A. 2005. Social learning of food preferences
by white-tailed ptarmigan chicks. Anim. Behav. 70:
305–310.
Almeida, S. S., de Oliveira, L. M. and Graeff, F. G. 1991. Early
life protein malnutrition changes exploration of the elevated
plus-maze and reactivity to anxiolytics. – Psychopharmacology
103: 513–518.
Almeida, S. S., Garcia, R. A. and de Oliveira, L. M. 1993. Effects
of early protein malnutrition and repeated testing upon
locomotor and exploratory behaviors in the elevated
plus-maze. – Physiol. Behav. 54: 749–752.
Almeida, S. S., Garcia, R. A., Cibien, M. M., De Araujo, M.,
Moreira, G. M. and De Oliveira, L. M. 1994. e ontogeny
of exploratory behaviors in early-protein-malnourished rats
exposed to the elevated plus-maze test. – Psychobiology 22:
283–288.
Almeida, S. S., Tonkiss, J. and Galler, J. R. 1996. Prenatal
protein malnutrition affects exploratory behavior of female
rats in the elevated plus-maze test. – Physiol. Behav. 60:
675–680.
Barnes, R. H., Levitsky, D. A., Pond, W. G. and Moore, U. 1976.
Effect of postnatal dietary protein and energy restriction on
exploratory behavior in young pigs. – Dev. Psychobiol. 9:
425–435.
Blackburn, T. M., Cassey, P. and Lockwood, J. L. 2009. e role
of species traits in the establishment success of exotic birds.
– Global Change Biol. 15: 2852–2860.
Budaev, S. V. 2010. Using principal components and factor analysis
in animal behaviour research: caveats and guidelines.
– Ethology 116: 472–480.
Carter, A. J., Feeney W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G. and
Heinsohn, R. 2013. Animal personality: what are behavioural
ecologists measuring? Biol. Rev. Camb. Phil. Soc. 88:
465–475.
Cassey, P. 2002. Life history and ecology influences establishment
success of introduced land birds. – Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 76:
465–480.
Christidis, L. and Boles, W. E. 2008. Systematics and taxonomy
of Australian birds. – CSIRO.
Coleman, S. L. and Mellgren, R. L. 1994. Neophobia when feeding
alone or in flocks in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. – Anim.
Behav. 48: 903–907.
Dhami, M. K. and Nagle, B. 2009. Review of the biology and
ecology of the common myna (Acridotheres tristis) and some
implications for management of this invasive species. – Pacific
Invasives Initiative.
Dingemanse, N. J., Both, C., Drent, P. J., van Oers, K. and van
Noordwijk, A. J. 2002. Repeatability and heritability of
exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild. – Anim.
Behav. 64: 929–938.
1440
Sengupta, S. 1976. Food and feeding ecology of the common myna,
Acridotheres tristis. – Proc. Indian Natl Sci. Acad. 42: 338–345.
Simpson, S. J. and Raubenheimer, D. 2012. e nature of nutri-
tion: a unifying framework from animal adaptation to human
obesity. – Princeton Univ. Press.
Simpson, S. J., Sword, G. A., Lorch, P. D. and Couzin, I. D. 2006.
Cannibal crickets on a forced march for protein and salt.
– Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103: 4152–4156.
Simpson, S. J., Raubenheimer, D., Charleston, M. A. and Clissold,
F. J. 2010. Modelling nutritional interactions: from individuals
to communities. – Trends Ecol. Evol. 25: 53–60.
Sol, D. and Lefebvre, L. 2000. Behavioural flexibility predicts
invasion success in birds introduced to New Zealand. – Oikos
90: 599–605.
Sol, D., Timmermans, S. and Lefebvre, L. 2002. Behavioural
flexibility and invasion success in birds. – Anim. Behav. 63:
495–502.
Sol, D., Duncan, R. P., Blackburn, T. M., Cassey, P. and Lefebvre,
L. 2005. Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds
to novel environments. – Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102:
5460–5465.
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M. and Lefebvre, L. 2008. Brain size
predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel
environments. – Am. Nat. 172: S63–S71.
Sol, D., Griffin, A. S., Bartomeus, I. and Boyce, H. 2011. Exploring
or avoiding novel food resources? e novelty conflict in an
invasive bird. – PLoS One 6: e19535.
Sol, D., Griffin, A. S. and Bartomeus, I. 2012a. Consumer and
motor innovation in the common myna: the role of motivation
and emotional responses. – Anim. Behav. 83: 179–188.
Sol, D., Bartomeus, I. and Griffin, A. S. 2012b. e paradox of
invasion in birds: competitive superiority or ecological
opportunism? – Oecologia 169: 553–564.
Suboski, M. D. and Bartashunas, C. 1984. Mechanisms for social
transmission of pecking preferences to neonatal chicks.
– J. Exp. Psychol. Behav. Process. 10: 182–194.
Tait, A. H., Raubenheimer, D., Stockin, K. A., Merriman, M. and
Machovsky-Capuska, G. E. 2014. Nutritional geometry and
macronutrient variation in the diets of gannets: the challenges
in marine field studies. – Mar. Biol. 161: 2791–2801.
Tidemann, C. R. 2005. Indian mynas – can the problems be
controlled? – In: Proceedings of the 15th National Urban
Animal Management Conference, pp. 19–21.
Tidemann, C. R. and ANU Fenner School 2009. Common myna.
< http://fennerschool-associated.anu.edu.au/myna/index.html >.
Webster, S. and Lefebvre, L. 2001. Problem solving and neophobia
in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados.
– Anim. Behav. 62: 23–32.
Wright, T. F., Eberhard, J. R., Hobson, E. A., Avery, M. L. and
Russello, M. A. 2010. Behavioral flexibility and species
invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis. – Ethol. Ecol.
Evol. 22: 393–404.
Martin, W. K. 1996. e current and potential distribution of the
common myna Acridotheres tristis in Australia. – Emu 96:
166–173.
Mayr, E. 1965. e nature of colonizations in birds. In: Baker,
H. G. and Stebbins, G. L. (eds), e genetics of colonizing
species. Academic Press, pp. 29–47.
McGiffin, A., Lill, A., Beckman, J. and Johnstone, C. P. 2013.
Tolerance of human approaches by common mynas along an
urban–rural gradient. – Emu 113: 154–160.
McWilliams, S. 2011. Ecology of vertebrate nutrition. – John
Wiley and Sons.
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Winkler, H. and Leisler, B. 2002. e sig-
nificance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia
in parrots. – Ethology 108: 249–272.
Moeed, A. 1975. Diets of nestlings starlings and mynas at Havelock
North, Hawke’s Bay. – Notornis 22: 291–294.
Moeed, A. 1976. Foods of the common myna (Acridotheres tristis)
in central India and in Hawke’s Bay, New Zealand. – Notornis
23: 246–249.
Morton, G. J. and Schwartz, M. W. 2011. Leptin and the central
nervous system control of glucose metabolism. – Physiol. Rev.
91: 389–411.
Murphy, M. E. 1993. e protein requirement for maintenance in
the white-crowned sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii.
– Can. J. Zool. 71: 2111–2120.
Parsons, H., Major, R. E. and French, K. 2006. Species interactions
and habitat associations of birds inhabiting urban areas of
Sydney, Australia. – Austral Ecol. 31: 217–227.
Pell, A. S. and Tidemann, C. R. 1997. e impact of two
exotic hollow-nesting birds on two native parrots in savannah
and woodland in eastern Australia. – Biol. Conserv. 79:
145–153.
Peneaux, C., Machovsky-Capuska, G. E., Raubenheimer, D.,
Lermite, F., Rousseau, C., Ruhan, T., Rodger, J. C. and Griffin,
A. S. 2017. Data from: Tasting novel foods and selecting
nutrient content in a highly successful ecological invader, the
common myna. Dryad Digital Repository, < http://dx.doi.
org/10.5061/dryad.3q7k6 >.
Pierotti, R. and Annett, C. 1987. Reproductive consequences of
dietary specialization and switching in an ecological generalist.
In: Kamil, A. C., Krebs, J. R. and Pulliam, H. R. (eds),
Foraging behavior. Springer, pp. 417–422.
Raubenheimer, D. 2011. Toward a quantitative nutritional
ecology: the right-angled mixture triangle. – Ecol. Monogr.
81: 407–427.
Raubenheimer, D., Simpson, S. J. and Tait, A. H. 2012. Match
and mismatch: conservation physiology, nutritional ecology
and the timescales of biological adaptation. Phil. Trans. R.
Soc. B 367: 1628–1646.
Russell, J. C., McMorland, A. J. and MacKay, J. W. 2010.
Exploratory behaviour of colonizing rats in novel environments.
– Anim. Behav. 79: 159–164.
Supplementary material (Appendix JAV-01456 at < www.
avianbiology.org/appendix/jav-01456 >). Appendix 1–2.
... We tested the hypotheses that dietary carotenoid and macronutrients should contribute to the quality of a carotenoid signal, and that animals have the ability to detect and consume foods containing nutrients that enhance carotenoid signal quality. The common (Indian) myna (Acridotheres tristis) is a passerine that displays colored skin features and is known for its ability to select foods of differential macronutrient content (Machovsky-Capuska et al., 2016a;Peneaux et al., 2017Peneaux et al., , 2020. These features make it an ideal model species to test our hypotheses. ...
... They are behaviorally monogamous, forming life-long bonds with their partners (Counsilman, 1974), and it is reasonable to assume that this carotenoid-based skin display functions as an indicator of individual quality (Endler, 1980;Svensson and Wong, 2011;Weaver et al., 2018). They are also generalist omnivores and previous work has demonstrated that mynas can select foods based on the food's nutritional content and on their own physiological needs (Machovsky-Capuska et al., 2016a;Peneaux et al., 2017). ...
... These results suggest that males could ingest more L in the presence of dietary carotenoids to facilitate carotenoid uptake for transport to and/or deposition in the integument. Mynas are known for selecting foods and macronutrients based on their nutritional needs, even when no perceptual signatures such as olfactory and/or gustatory cues are present (Machovsky-Capuska et al., 2016a;Peneaux et al., 2017). It is also known that males can select food items with high carotenoid content during feather production (Senar et al., 2010;Walker et al., 2014). ...
Article
Producing colored signals often requires consuming dietary carotenoid pigments. Evidence that food deprivation can reduce coloration, however, raises the question of whether other dietary nutrients contribute to signal coloration, and furthermore, whether individuals can voluntarily select food combinations to achieve optimal coloration. We created a 2-way factorial design to manipulate macronutrient and carotenoid access in common mynas ( Acridotheres tristis ) and measured eye patch coloration as a function of the food combinations individuals selected. Mynas had access to either water or carotenoid-supplemented water and could eat either a standard captive diet or choose freely between three nutritionally defined pellets (protein, lipid, carbohydrate). Mynas supplemented with both carotenoids and macronutrient pellets had higher color scores than control birds. Male coloration tended to respond more to nutritional manipulation than females, with color scores improving in macronutrient- and carotenoid-supplemented individuals compared to controls. All mynas consuming carotenoids had higher levels of plasma carotenoids, but only males showed a significant increase by the end of the experiment. Dietary carotenoids and macronutrient intake consumed in combination tended to increase plasma carotenoid concentrations the most. These results demonstrate for the first time that consuming specific combinations of macronutrients along with carotenoids contribute to optimizing a colorful signal and point to sex-specific nutritional strategies. Our findings improve our knowledge of how diet choices affect signal expression and, by extension, how nutritionally impoverished diets, such as those consumed by birds in cities, might affect sexual selection processes and ultimately population dynamics.
... When given the choice between foods with different nutritional content, urban mynas have been showed to selectively consume and fight for foods enriched in protein (Machovsky-Capuska et al., 2016). A follow-up study demonstrated that mynas kept on a low protein diet in captivity subsequently prioritised intake of foods with a high protein content (Peneaux et al., 2017). Further work has also shown that captive mynas on a low-lipid diet would prioritise intake in lipids (Gumede and Downs, 2020). ...
... Phenotypical shifts towards higher aggression and dispersal in response to a lack of breeding resources or detrimental environmental conditions have been found in other species (Aguillon and Duckworth, 2015;Duckworth and Badyaev, 2007;Hui et al., 2012). Moreover, previous work has demonstrated a link between nutritional intake and exploratory phenotypes in mynas, pointing to the potential role of poor-quality urban foods in range expansion (Peneaux et al., 2017). ...
Article
Theory suggests that overcrowding and increased competition in urban environments might be detrimental to individual condition in avian populations. Unfavourable conditions could be compounded by changes in dietary niche with additional consequences for individual quality of urban birds. We analysed the isotopic signatures, signal coloration, body condition, parasitic loads (feather mites and coccidia), and immune responsiveness of 191 adult common (Indian) mynas (Acridotheres tristis) captured in 19 localities with differing levels of urbanization. The isotopic signature of myna feathers differed across low and high urbanized habitats, with a reduced isotopic niche breadth found in highly urbanized birds. This suggests that birds in high urban environments may occupy a smaller foraging niche to the one of less urbanized birds. In addition, higher degrees of urbanization were associated with a decrease in carotenoid-based coloration, higher ectoparasite loads and higher immune responsiveness. This pattern of results suggests that the health status of mynas from more urbanized environments was poorer than mynas from less modified habitats. Our findings are consistent with the theory that large proportions of individual birds that would otherwise die under natural conditions survive due to prevailing top-down and bottom-up ecological processes in cities. Detrimental urban ecological conditions and search for more favourable, less crowded habitats offers the first reasonable explanation for why an ecological invader like the common myna continues to spread within its global invasive range.
... It is essential that, when establishing populations after colonization, invasive species be good at searching and consuming novel food resources in new environments [12,43]. With wide niche breadth, generalists can consume a broad range of foods and survive better in novel environmental conditions than more specialized species [7,44,45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Animals can expand distributions in response to climatic and environmental changes, but the potential expansive ability of a source population is rarely evaluated using designed experiments. Group foraging can increase survival in new environments, but it also increases intraspecific competition. The trade-off between benefit and conflict needs to be determined. The expanding Light-vented Bulbul Pycnonotus sinensis was used as a model to test mechanisms promoting successful expansion. Social foraging and its advantages were evaluated using lab-designed feeding trials. Consuming novel foods was compared between bulbuls and a sympatric, nonexpansive relative species, the finchbill Spizixos semitorques, from native areas at both solitary and social levels. Bulbuls increased their eating times when transferred from solitary to group, whereas social context did not affect finchbills. Bulbuls were significantly more likely to eat with their companions than finchbills when in a group. Thus, exploring food resources in a bulbul source population was facilitated by social context, indicating that social foraging is an important means by which birds successfully expand and respond to environmental changes. This research increases understanding of successful expansion mechanisms and will consequently help predict invasive potentials of alien species.
... Typically, energy maximisation was an assumption rather than a prediction to be tested (e.g. Albrecht et al., 2018;Martin, 1985;Sayers et al., 2010), while the nutrient balance model was applied only to one species of fruit-eating birds, using artificial foods in urban environments (Machovsky-Capuska et al., 2016;Peneaux et al., 2017). To our knowledge, this is the first study that analyses nutrient balance in an assemblage of fruit-eating bird species. ...
Article
According to diet‐regulation hypotheses, animals select food to regulate the intake of macronutrients or maximise energy feeding efficiency. Specifically, the nutrient balance model proposes that foraging is primarily a process of balancing multiple nutrients to achieve a nutritional intake target, while the energy maximisation model proposes that foraging aims to maximise energy. Here, we evaluate the adjustment of fruit diets (the fruit‐derived component of the diets) to nutritional and energy intake targets, characterizing the nutrient balance and energy maximisation strategies across fruit‐eating bird species with different fruit‐handling behaviours ("gulpers", which swallow whole fruits, and "mashers", which process the fruit in the beak) in subtropical Andean forests. Food‐handling behaviour determines the food intake rate and, consequently, influences animal efficiency to obtain nutrients and energy. We used extensive field data from the diet of fruit‐eating birds to test how species adjust their food intake. We used nutritional geometry to explore macronutrient balance and the effectiveness framework to explore energy‐acquisition effectiveness. Observed diets showed a good fit with predictions of a diet balanced in macronutrient proportions. With few exceptions, diets clustered near an optimal macronutrient mixture and did not differ from each other in terms of maximising energy intake. Moreover, when comparing our results with a random diet based on local fruit availability, birds tended to fit better to the nutritional target, and less to the energy target, than expected from a random diet. Fruit‐handling behaviour did not affect the ability of bird species to reach a nutritional target but it affected species energy acquisition, which was lower in mashers than in gulpers. This study explores for the first time different diet‐regulation strategies in wild fruit‐eating birds, and supports the argument that the diet reflects a specific regulation of macronutrients. Understanding why birds select fruits is a complex question requiring multiple considerations. The nutrient balance model explains the relevance of nutrient composition in the fruit selection by fruit‐eating birds, although it is still necessary to determine its relative importance with respect to other dietary drivers.
... protein). 35,36 On a low-protein and low-carotenoid diet, controlled observations in captivity have revealed that eye patch coloration progressively decreases as time in captivity increases, until all coloration disappears and only a white skin surface is visible (C. Peneaux, personal communication; Figure 1). ...
Article
Full-text available
Conspicuous coloured displays from ultraviolet to bright red have been documented in many species throughout the animal kingdom. These colours often occur as sexual signals and can be incorporated into different types of integuments (e.g. scales, feathers, skin). Two main mechanisms are known to produce coloured integuments: pigmentation and tissue structure. Although pigmental and structural coloration are separate mechanisms and can occur independently, some coloured displays might emerge from a combination of both. Here, we demonstrate, using biochemical, optical and morphological methodologies, that the yellow coloration of the skin located around the eye of Common (Indian) Mynas ( Acridotheres tristis) is produced by both light-reflecting nanostructures and light-absorbing carotenoid pigments. Our analysis confirms that nanostructured collagen in the avian dermis work in combination with carotenoid pigments to produce vivid integumentary colours. Identifying the mechanisms behind the production of a coloured signal provides a basis for predicting how a signal’s function might be influenced by environmental factors such as fledgling nutrition.
... Free-ranging urban Common (Indian) Mynas Acridotheres tristis select almost exclusively a high-protein food, and compete to gain access to it, when given the choice between high-protein, high-carbohydrate and high-lipid artificial pellets (Machovsky-Capuska et al. 2016). Moreover, protein-deprived mynas selectively consumed novel foods containing higher proportions of protein (Peneaux et al. 2017). Urban habitats may therefore be deficient in protein-rich foods, making this macronutrient a potentially limiting resource for ornamental production. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the past thirty years, carotenoid‐based animal signals have been an intense focus of research because they can potentially broadcast an honest reflection of individual reproductive potential. Our understanding of the underpinning physiological functions of carotenoid compounds is still emerging, however. Here, we argue that wildlife researchers and managers interested in assessing the impact of environmental quality on animal populations should be taking advantage of the signalling function of carotenoid‐based morphological traits. Using birds as a model taxonomic group, we build our argument by first reviewing the strong evidence that the expression of avian carotenoid displays provides an integrated measure of a multitude of diet‐ and health‐related parameters. We then present evidence that human‐induced rapid environmental change (hirec) impacts the expression of carotenoid signals across different critical periods of a bird’s lifetime.
... In our study, Common Mynas showed dietary flexibility, but showed the least preference for the carbohydrate high diet, which was relatively high in sucrose. However, the maintenance diet, which consisted of fruit and vegetables may have affected the lack of preference for the high carbohydrate/sucrose food, which differed from the findings of Peneaux et al. (2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Common Mynas Sturnus tristis, previously known as Acridotheres tristis, are considered among the world’s worst most invasive species. However, relatively little is known about the factors that affect their persistence and spread in new environments. They have been observed feeding on a wide range of foods, including anthropogenic foods in urban areas. Their diet preferences are relatively unknown. Therefore, we investigated the macronutrient preferences of Common Mynas in captivity. Common Mynas (n = 10) were given a pairwise choice of three different diets (high lipid, high protein, and high carbohydrate diets) in the laboratory to determine their preference. Common Mynas showed a preference for the high lipid food, followed by high protein, with the high carbohydrate food least preferred. Consequently, this suggests Common Mynas preferred food high in lipids compared with proteins and carbohydrates, but additional study is needed to confirm this. Implications are Common Mynas should not be a problem for South African agricultural areas, because this industry generally provides relatively few dietary items high in fat, so we expect the Common Myna will continue to be distributed mainly in urban areas of South Africa where anthropogenic foods relatively high in fat are more common.
... It may also help us to determine what nutrients may be limiting for a particular species in natural habitats (Coogan et al., 2014;Aryal et al., 2015;Coogan et al., 2018b). Such analyses have, for example, been made on several mammal and bird species (Panthi et al., 2015;Peneaux et al., 2017;Coogan et al., 2018b). ...
Article
1. Harvestmen are predators, although they suspected of being partly omnivorous by including fruits in their diet. 2. In the present study, the right‐angled mixture triangle (RMT) was used to analyse the macronutritional niche of the harvestman Opilio canestrinii, an invasive species in northern Europe. The study design followed a double‐test procedure in which the animals were subjected to two self‐selection tests: the first one immediately after being caught in the field and the second after 1 week of ad libitum feeding during which the animals became satiated and nutritionally balanced. A comparison of results from the two tests indicates whether the animals were food limited in the field, and whether they were limited by a particular macronutrient. 3. Females were found to be food limited in the field, whereas males were not. Opilio canestrinii had a target intake of 28% lipid : 52% protein : 20% carbohydrate [i.e. with a considerable proportion of carbohydrate (sugar) in the diet]. Both sexes were non‐protein limited in the field, with sugar being more limiting than lipid. 4. The results indicate considerable inclusion of plant‐derived sugar in the natural diet. This conclusion was supported by a separate experiment showing enhanced performance (survival, gain in mass) in animals whose only energy supply was fresh fruit. 5. It is concluded that the harvestman is best characterized as an omnivorous predator. The present study investigated the macronutrient intake of the harvestman Opilio canestrinii using the right‐angled‐triangle method combined with a double‐test procedure followed up by a fruit experiment. The results indicated a target intake of 28% lipid : 52% protein : 20% carbohydrate when in balance and with food and sugar limited in nature. This suggests a more omnivorous lifestyle than earlier expected, which was supported in the fruit experiment where harvestman showed a capability to eat fruit as a food source
Article
Humans are translocating species beyond their native ranges increasingly fast. These translocations create a natural experiment to explore the role of cognition in invasiveness. Alien vertebrate species face many behavioral challenges upon introduction to novel environments. But here, we focus on how alien species might use cognition to find and adopt new foods. Cognitive processes are particularly well suited to this challenge, a prediction supported by large-scale comparative analyses of alien species’ historical introductions. Here, we parse the steps involved in approaching, handling, tasting, and evaluating novel food sources and, for each one, describe which cognitive abilities are the most relevant. In bringing attention to the functional importance of innovative feeding both conceptually and empirically, synthesizing the cognitive processes involved, highlighting the current void of knowledge, and arguing that alien species are particularly well suited to controlled experimental cognitive studies, this piece scaffolds future experimental cognitive research.
Article
Full-text available
The capacity to behave innovatively facilitates adaptation to changing environmental conditions and accelerates speciation rates. Innovation tendencies show substantial variation both among and within species, but the sources of this variation remain poorly understood. There has been much debate on the role of cognition and significant amounts of empirical research on the influence of motivational and state-dependent processes, but the prediction that innovation might also be facilitated by motor processes has only recently begun to gain traction. Here, we measured innovative foraging in 7 common urban avian species under free-ranging conditions and explored the role of motor flexibility as well as several potential other predictors of innovation such as motivation and morphology. Species differed significantly in their tendency to forage innovatively, with a true corvid, the Australian raven, Corvus coronoides, outperforming all other species. Across species, motor flexibility was the strongest predictor of the capacity to forage innovatively. Our results extend previous work demonstrating the role of motor diversity in individual differences in the tendency to forage innovatively and provide the impetus for future research on links between motor and cognitive flexibility.
Article
Full-text available
Anthropogenic environments can offer rich sources of energy to urban wildlife, but little is known about how they impact on nutritional balance and food selection. Common mynas (Sturnus tristis) provide a powerful model system for testing the nutritional constraints and priorities of an invasive species that has successfully adapted to urban environments. Here, we use behavioral observations, field-based feeding trials, videography, and the right-angled mixture triangle model (RMT) to examine the macronutrient preferences of these invasive birds. Our behavioral observations showed that mynas consumed insects (40.6%), worms (33.2%), human discards (17.6%), and plants (8.6%). Our feeding trials using nutritionally defined foods showed that mynas had a clear preference for food dishes containing only high-protein (HP) pellets over high-lipid (HL) or high-carbohydrate (HC) pellets. In addition, mixed feeders were also presented in 2 combinations: 1) contained equal proportions of HP and HC pellets and 2) equal proportions of HP and HL pellets. HP pellets were selectively consumed from both mixed feeders, this involving an increase in feeding time. Overall, the RMT showed that mynas consumed a higher proportion of protein from the feeders than in their natural diet. Furthermore, the majority of our observations of birds feeding at the dishes containing HP foods ended in intraspecific aggression, suggesting that protein is a contestable resource. These results suggest that mynas at our urban study site are deficient in protein relative to fats and carbohydrates.
Article
The dietary generalist-specialist distinction plays a pivotal role in theoretical and applied ecology, conservation, invasion biology, and evolution and yet the concept remains poorly characterised. Diets, which are commonly used to define niche breadth, are almost exclusively considered in terms of foods, with little regard for the mixtures of nutrients and other compounds they contain. We use nutritional geometry (NG) to integrate nutrition with food-level approaches to the dietary niche and illustrate the application of our framework in the important context of invasion biology. We use an example that involves a model with four hypothetical nonexclusive scenarios. We additionally show how this approach can provide fresh theoretical insight into the ways nutrition and food choices impact trait evolution and trophic interactions.
Chapter
The foraging behavior of animals in complex environments is affected by conflicting demands (cf. Sih 1980, Martindale 1982, 1983, Cerri & Fraser 1983) and may be dependent on more than one aspect of prey quality (Pulliam 1975, Westoby 1978, Nicotri 1980, Breitwisch et al. 1984). Various currencies have been suggested that could affect the dietary selection of a foraging animal. These include energy intake per unit of time (Schoener 1971), maximizing intake of an essential nutrient (Goss-Custard 1981), and mixing nutrient intake to ensure adequate nutrition (Westoby 1978, Nicotri 1980).
Article
Nutrition has long been considered more the domain of medicine and agriculture than of the biological sciences, yet it touches and shapes all aspects of the natural world. The need for nutrients determines whether wild animals thrive, how populations evolve and decline, and how ecological communities are structured.The Nature of Nutritionis the first book to address nutrition's enormously complex role in biology, both at the level of individual organisms and in their broader ecological interactions. Stephen Simpson and David Raubenheimer provide a comprehensive theoretical approach to the analysis of nutrition--the Geometric Framework. They show how it can help us to understand the links between nutrition and the biology of individual animals, including the physiological mechanisms that determine the nutritional interactions of the animal with its environment, and the consequences of these interactions in terms of health, immune responses, and lifespan. Simpson and Raubenheimer explain how these effects translate into the collective behavior of groups and societies, and in turn influence food webs and the structure of ecosystems. Then they demonstrate how the Geometric Framework can be used to tackle issues in applied nutrition, such as the problem of optimizing diets for livestock and endangered species, and how it can also help to address the epidemic of human obesity and metabolic disease.