Content uploaded by Navjot Bhullar
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Navjot Bhullar on Jul 15, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786; doi:10.3390/ijerph14070786 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Article
Into the Woods or a Stroll in the Park: How Virtual
Contact with Nature Impacts Positive and Negative
Affect
Elizabeth McAllister, Navjot Bhullar * and Nicola S. Schutte
School of Behavioural, Cognitive and Social Sciences, University of New England, Armidale 2351, Australia;
libbymcallister@hotmail.com (E.M.); nschutte@une.edu.au (N.S.S.)
* Correspondence: navjot.bhullar@une.edu.au
Received: 23 May 2017; Accepted: 11 July 2017; Published: 14 July 2017
Abstract: This study examined the effects of virtual contact with nature on positive and negative
affect, and investigated the psychological process of perceived restorativeness as a mediator of this
relationship. A sample of 220 Australians aged between 18 and 75 years (M = 49.07, SD = 14.34,
female = 72%) participated in the study. Participants were randomly allocated to one of the three
experimental conditions experienced through video presentations: (1) ‘wild’ nature, (2) ‘urban’
nature, and (3) non-nature control. They then completed measures of perceived restorativeness as
well as positive and negative affect. Compared to the non-nature control condition, the experience
of wild nature resulted in significantly higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative
affect. The experience of urban nature resulted in significantly lower levels of negative affect only
compared to the non-nature control video. Experience of wild and urban nature resulted in greater
perceptions of restorativeness as compared to the non-nature control video. Restorativeness was a
significant underlying psychological mediating path through which nature experience exerted its
influence on affect. These results have the potential to inform nature-based green care interventions
for mental health as well as for urban planning to maximize beneficial effects of natural
environments.
Keywords: wild nature; urban nature; restorativeness; affect; mental health
1. Introduction
Contact with nature has been linked to enhanced mood [1,2] . This is consistent with the Bi ophilia
hypothesis [3] which suggests that humans have an innate desire to affiliate with other living things
and that they derive well-being from experiencing this affiliation. The mechanism by which contact
with different natural systems (e.g., wild and urban nature) improves affect still remains unclear.
Understanding the process by which nature exposure exerts its influence is important; first to inform
clinical practice, as increased contact with nature potentially provides an accessible, cost-effective
intervention for improving mood [4], and second to support urban planning that maximizes the
effectiveness of natural areas in supporting human well-being [5].
1.1. Contact with Nature
Nature is commonly thought of as wild plants, animals, ecosystems, and landscapes in contrast
to human built environments and natural places impacted by human activity. However, in the
modern world, nature exists on a spectrum from places with little evidence of human activity to small
parks in highly urbanized areas [6]. In ecology and environmental psychology, natural features that
are partly the products of human activity, such as urban parks and back yards, are widely considered
to also represent nature [7–9].
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 2 of 11
In the present study, contact with nature was conceptualized as a person experiencing nature,
including nature influenced by humans, by viewing and hearing the sounds of nature in video or still
images [10,11].
1.2. Relationship between Contact with Nature and Affect
A meta-analysis of 32 studies examined the effect of contact with nature on positive affect and
negative affect, and found a moderate increase in positive affect and a smaller but still significant
decrease in negative affect for participants who had contact with nature compared to those who did
not [2]. Another meta-analysis of 17 studies comparing self-reported emotions after undertaking
physical activity in a natural versus a non-natural environment found clear beneficial impacts of
nature compared to non-nature on levels of anger, sadness, and fatigue [1]. This meta-analysis found
an improvement from before to after contact with nature in levels of energy, anxiety, anger, sadness,
and fatigue. A large cross-sectional study in the United Kingdom using a smartphone app titled
“Mappiness” also found that participants responding to questions in outdoor settings and natural
habitats were significantly happier than participants in urban environments [12]. In a cross-sectional
study [13], a negative association was found between green space around residents’ homes and stress
levels (as measured by saliva cortisol levels and self-report).
Many experimental studies have exposed participants to virtual nature in the form of
photographs and videos, due to the difficulties of taking participants outdoors and the fact that real
natural settings feature potential confounding variables such as differing light, temperature, smells,
and sounds. While the effects of nature are more pronounced when real-life nature is encountered,
previous research has shown that virtual nature has similar, if weaker, effects on a range of
psychological variables [10,11,14–17].
Studies making a direct comparison between the effects of “wild nature” (surrounds where
human influence is not evident) versus “urban nature” (surrounds with clear human influence) have
found varying effects [5,18]. Greater actual and perceived biodiversity have been found to increase
the effect of nature walks on psychological well-being [19–21]. Another study [8] found that perceived
naturalness and perceived restorativeness of the surrounds interact to enhance positive affect after
an outdoor group walk. Such results indicate a positive effect for areas that are perceived as wild
nature compared to urban nature. On the other hand, a meta-analytic study [2] found no significant
differences in positive and negative affect between contact with wild nature and manicured nature.
Similarly, a study examining the well-being benefits of contact with wild woodland, tended
woodland, and parkland found no significant differences among the three [22].
There is evidence that contact with any nature may be more useful in increasing positive emotion
than in reducing negative emotion [2]. Evidence from three experimental studies suggested a
significant connection between contact with nature and increased levels of positive emotion, while
differences in negative emotion were found to be non-significant [11]. A similar finding was observed
in a sample of individuals diagnosed with Major Depressive Disorder [23]. However, it is still unclear
how virtual experience of a wild or urban nature will differentially impact positive and negative
affect, respectively compared with a non-nature control.
1.3. Relationship between Contact with Nature and Restorativeness
The mechanism by which nature influences affective states is an active area of research. While
often investigated, it appears that physical exercise and social contact cannot fully explain the
beneficial effects of nature [1]. Attention restoration theory [7,24] has attracted much research effort
as an explanation for nature’s beneficial effect on positive and negative affect. This theory proposes
that directed attention to a task can only be sustained for so long before fatigue is experienced and
attention lapses. It further posits that restoration of attentional capacity can be achieved in
environments that absorb one’s attention seemingly effortlessly, a process termed fascination [24].
A number of experimental studies have compared participants’ attentional capacity after
viewing photographs of, or visiting, natural versus urban places. These studies provide evidence of
the attentional restoration benefits of even a brief interaction with nature, including virtual nature
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 3 of 11
[25–28]. Additionally, many studies have documented the perceived restorativeness of a natural place
using self-reporting methods [19,29,30]. A study of a sample of 319 young adult students in New
Zealand found that the level of fascination in nature mediated the relationship between time in nature
and positive affect [31]. Experimental research suggests that virtual natural environments impact
restorativeness [16], and a greater perception of restorativeness is found to be associated with more
positive affect and less negative affect [15,26]. Therefore, restorativeness might be the path that
explains the effect of nature experience on affect.
1.4. The Current Study
Using a psychological lens, the current study built on and extended previous research [2,8,11] in
two important ways. First, this study experimentally compared the effects of virtual contact with wild
nature and urban nature on positive and negative affect, respectively. To our knowledge, this direct
comparison of virtual ‘wild’ nature and ‘urban’ nature experience and their differential impacts on
positive and negative affect, respectively, have not been experimentally investigated. Based on
previous research [16], we predicted that exposure to virtual wild nature compared with exposure to
virtual urban nature would result in more positive affect and less negative affect, and greater
perceived restorativeness. In addition, both virtual wild and urban nature experience would result
in more positive affect and less negative affect, and a greater perception of restorativeness compared
to exposure to a non-nature control.
Furthermore, the present study extended previous research by examining the role of perceived
restorativeness as a mediating underlying psychological process explaining the relationship between
the virtual nature experience and positive and negative affect. We predicted that the nature
experience would influence perceived restorativeness, which in turn would influence positive and
negative affect, respectively. More specifically, we predicted that the nature experience would result
in a greater perception of restorativeness, which in turn would result in more positive affect and less
negative affect.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and Procedure
Two hundred and twenty Australian residents ranging in age from 18 to 75 years
(Mean age = 49.07, SD = 14.34, female = 72%) completed the present study. The majority of the sample
(76%) had completed a Bachelor or higher degree.
The study was carried out using the Qualtrics research platform (Qualtrics, Provo, UT, USA).
Before data collection, approval by the institutional human research ethics review committee (HREC,
approval number HE15-129) was obtained. Participants were recruited from the general population.
Invitations to participate were sent by email to social networks of the researchers. The invitation to
participate was also included in the email newsletter of the Australian Psychological Society, and
circulated to members of two outdoor activity clubs in Queensland, Australia. All participants were
volunteers. Participants were invited to access the study anonymously via Qualtrics. Participants
were randomly assigned to one of the three video experimental conditions and then completed
measures of perceived restorativeness, positive and negative affect, as well as manipulation check,
covariate (depressive symptoms), and demographic variables. Respondents did not receive any
reimbursement or incentive for their participation.
2.2. Experimental Manipulation and Design
Three experimental videos were created for the study to operationalize contact with nature (or
a non-nature control) corresponding to the experimental conditions in this study. Participants were
randomly allocated to wild nature (n = 72), urban nature (n = 76), and non-nature control (n = 72)
conditions.
The videos were filmed in South East Queensland, Australia to create naturalistic representations
of the nature and non-nature settings. Each video was shot in sunny Autumn weather during the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 4 of 11
middle of the day, and after the editing process displayed between 2 min 20 s and 2 min 35 s of
footage with sound. Video and still images of approximately this duration have been found to be
effective in eliciting affective responses to nature and other stimuli [32–34], and were found to be of
optimal length whereby participants are more likely to watch the video in full, while still exerting a
significant effect (see Section 3).
Each video contained four or five scenes of roughly equal length, and panned slowly and
horizontally within each scene. No coastal scenes were included, and inland bodies of water did not
feature prominently. Footage of people was not included except where they were in the distance and
unidentifiable. No explanatory text or music was included and no legible text was visible. The URLs
for the videos are provided in Appendix A.
The wild nature video featured rainforest and dry sclerophyll forest scenes filmed in Mount
Glorious, Brisbane, Australia. A wide range of native tree, epiphyte, and understorey species was
included. Birdsong was clearly audible throughout. Gravel walking tracks were the only evidence of
human activity. The urban nature video featured public parks and gardens in urban contexts in the
Brisbane suburbs of Sandgate and Boondall. Scenes included nature in the form of garden beds, birds,
trees, and shrubs, including non-native species, while indicating the urban context through the
presence of buildings, roads, cars and buses, lamp posts, concrete pathways, and vehicle sounds.
People were sometimes visible in the distance. Finally, the non-nature control condition video
showed urban built environments in the suburbs of South Brisbane and Taigum, and included
minimal plants or animals. Concrete surfaces, roadways, buildings, and cars were visually dominant.
Sounds were primarily road and aircraft noise. People were sometimes visible in the distance.
2.3. Measures
In addition to the demographic information (age, sex, education), we assessed the constructs of
interest through the following measures. All Cronbach’s alphas (αs) reported, used to assess internal
consistency of scales, are for the present study.
2.3.1. Perceived Restorativeness
The 26-item Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) [15,35] is a self-report measure of how
restorative an environment is perceived to be, in the context of attention restoration theory [7]. The
PRS is scored on a seven-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (completely). Example items
include “Being here is an escape experience” and “This place has fascinating qualities”. Participants
were instructed to answer while thinking about the place in the video they had just watched, and
items were reworded accordingly (e.g., “Being there is an escape experience”). (α = 0.97).
2.3.2. Affect
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [36] was used to assess both positive and
negative mood. The PANAS comprises two 10-item scales for positive affect and negative affect,
respectively. Respondents are shown emotion-related words (e.g., “excited” for positive affect, and
“upset” for negative affect) and asked to indicate to what extent they feel that way “right now—that
is, at the present moment”. Responses are measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (very slightly
or not at all) to 5 (extremely). (αpositive affect = 0.91, αnegative affect = 0.82).
2.3.3. Manipulation Check and Covariate
A manipulation check item asked participants to nominate on a four-point scale whether they
had watched all, half, a quarter, or none of the video presented. Previous research found that
depressed mood was strongly associated with positive affect and negative affect [37], therefore, we
assessed participants’ levels of depressive symptoms to control for this confounding variable. We
used a 7-item depression subscale of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21) [38]. The
measure is assessed on a four-point scale from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 5 of 11
much or most of the time). Scores on the scale are summed and then doubled for comparison with
DASS-42 [38]. (α = 0.89).
2.4. Statistical Analyses
Pearson’s correlation coefficients were used to examine bivariate relationships between key
study variables. Analyses of covariance were conducted to examine group differences on positive and
negative affect, and perceived restorativeness. Mediation analyses were conducted using the
regression-based PROCESS macro (Model 4) [39] to examine the mediating effect of perceived
restorativeness between the experience of nature and positive and negative affect, respectively.
3. Results
3.1. Data Screening
A total of 274 participants commenced the survey and 54 cases (20%) were excluded; 48
participants did not proceed beyond the experimental manipulation (video) and six participants
reported not watching the video and, therefore, failed to meet the manipulation check criterion. The
remaining sample of 220 was used in the present study.
3.2. Preliminary Analyses
Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations of the study variables are presented in
Table 1. Variables were normally distributed with the exception of negative affect; negative affect had
a positive skew and a total sample mean negative affect score of 12.04, well below the mean of 16.00
found in a normative study of PANAS scores in a non-clinical population [40]. As expected,
restorativeness was significantly associated with greater positive affect and lower levels of negative
affect. There were also significant associations in the expected direction between positive and
negative affect, and depressive symptoms, with the exception that higher positive affect was not
significantly associated with lower negative affect.
There were no significant differences between men and women on positive affect, t(218) = 1.50,
p = 0.135, negative affect, t(218) = 0.74, p = 0.462, and perceived restorativeness, t(218) = 0.92, p = 0.361.
Bivariate correlations were significant between age and positive affect, r(218) = 0.21, p < 0.01 and
negative affect, r(218) = −0.30, p < 0.01, with older participants reporting more positive affect and less
negative affect. Age was not significantly related to perceived restorativeness, r(218) = 0.04, p = 0.538,
and there were also no significant experimental group differences in age, F(2, 217) = 0.44, p = 0.644.
Table 1. Bivariate correlations, means, and standard deviations (SD) of key study variables.
Variables 1234
1. Perceived Restorativeness - 0.41 *** −0.17 * 0.09
2. Positive affect - −0.09 −0.19 **
3. Negative affect - 0.34 ***
4. Depressive symptoms -
M 3.42 27.43 12.04 5.66
SD 1.26 8.26 2.85 6.04
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 (two-tailed).
3.3. Analyses of Covariance (ANCOVAs)
Three one-way ANCOVAs were used to examine the effect of experimental manipulation on
perceived restorativeness and positive and negative affect. The experimental condition was used as
the independent variable (IV), while positive affect, negative affect, and restorativeness were used as
dependent variables (DVs), with depressive symptoms serving as a covariate. Univariate analyses
revealed that there were significant group differences on positive affect, F(2, 216) = 5.29, p = 0.006,
partial η2 = 0.05, a small effect size; negative affect, F(2, 215) = 5.51, p = 0.005, partial η2 = 0.05, a small
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 6 of 11
effect size; and perceived restorativeness, F(2, 216) = 60.53, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.36, a large effect
size [41].
Table 2 summarizes the covariate-adjusted means and standard errors for each DV by condition.
For positive affect, the mean score for the wild nature condition was significantly higher than the
mean score for the urban nature condition and the non-nature control condition. There was no
significant difference between urban nature and non-nature control conditions on positive affect.
Therefore, the effect of urban nature on positive affect was not tested in the subsequent mediation
analysis for the relationship between urban nature experience and positive affect.
The wild and urban nature conditions resulted in significantly lower mean negative affect as
compared to the non-nature control, however, the two nature conditions were not significantly
different from each other. As expected, the wild nature experience resulted in significantly greater
perceived restorativeness than the urban nature and non-nature control conditions, and the urban
nature experience led to a greater perception of restorativeness than that of the non-nature condition.
Table 2. Covariate-adjusted means (M) and standard errors (SE) for key study variables for
experimental conditions.
Condition Positive Affect Negative Affect Perceived
Restorativeness
M SE M SE M SE
Wild nature (n = 72) 29.92 a 0.94 11.77
a 0.31 4.18
a 0.12
Urban nature (n = 76) 25.88 b 0.92 11.51
a 0.30 3.69
b 0.11
Non-nature control (n = 72) 26.58 b 0.94 12.87
b 0.31 2.38
c 0.12
Means in each column with different superscripts (a, b, c) are significantly different from each other,
using Sidak correction (p < 0.05).
3.4. Mediation Analyses
To test the hypothesis that perceived restorativeness would mediate the effect of nature
experience on positive and negative affect, respectively, we conducted three sets of regression-based
mediation analyses using the PROCESS model [39]. This approach uses accelerated bootstrapped
bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals (BCa CIs) for assessing the significance of indirect paths. For
all analyses, the IV was created using a dummy coded variable: participants in the relevant nature
(wild or urban) condition were assigned a value of 1 and participants in the control (non-nature)
condition were assigned a value of 0; perceived restorativeness served as a two mediator, and
depressive symptoms served as a covariate.
For the first analysis, summarized in Figure 1, contact with wild nature was the IV and positive
affect was the DV. Overall, the model explained 26% of the variance in positive affect, F(3, 140) =
16.41, p < 0.001. The effect of the wild nature experience on positive affect was mediated by perceived
restorativeness, after controlling for depressive symptoms. The 95% BCa CIs for the indirect effect of
5.39 (SE = 1.10) ranged from 3.42 to 7.75 for perceived restorativeness, and a Sobel test of z = 4.61,
p < 0.001 indicated a significant mediation.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 7 of 11
Positive Affect
Wild Nature
Experience −0.184ns (3.55**)
Negative Affect
Urban Nature
Experience −0.73ns (−1.35**)
−0.48*
Figure 1. Perceived restorativeness as a mediator of the relationship between the virtual wild nature
experience and positive affect. All paths are unstandardized beta coefficients [39]. The value in
parenthesis represents the relationship between the wild nature experience and positive affect prior
to controlling for perceived restorativeness. N = 144, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, ns = not significant.
For the second analysis, contact with wild nature served as the IV and negative affect as the DV.
Overall, this model explained 12% of the variance in negative affect, F(3, 140) = 6.40, p < 0.001. The
results revealed that the effect of the wild nature experience on negative affect was unrelated to
perceived restorativeness, after controlling for depressive symptoms. The 95% BCa CIs for the
indirect effect of −0.48 (SE = 0.40) ranged from −1.32 to 0.27, and a Sobel test of z = −1.19, p = 0.24
suggested a statistically non-significant mediation.
The final analysis, summarized in Figure 2, used contact with urban nature as the IV and
negative affect as the DV. Overall, this model explained 18% of the variance in negative affect,
F(3, 144) = 6.82, p < 0.001. The effect of the urban nature experience on negative affect was mediated
by perceived restorativeness, after controlling for depressive symptoms. The 95% BCa CI for the
indirect effect of −0.62 (SE = 0.31) ranged from −1.32 to −0.08, and a Sobel test of z = −2.00, p = 0.045.
Figure 2. Perceived restorativeness as a mediator of the relationship between the virtual urban nature
experience and negative affect. All paths are unstandardized beta coefficients [39]. The value in
parenthesis represents the relationship between the wild nature experience and positive affect prior
to controlling for perceived restorativeness. N = 148, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, ns = not
significant.
Perceived
Restorativeness
1.81*** 2.97***
Perceived
Restorativeness
1.31***
−1.84ns (3.55)**
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 8 of 11
4. Discussion
The present study compared the impact of virtual contact with wild nature and urban nature to
a non-nature control on positive and negative affect as well as perceived restorativeness. It also
examined whether perceptions of restorativeness might be the path that links the relationship
between virtual contact with nature and affect.
4.1. Effects of Contact with Wild and Urban Nature
The results add new detail to the understanding of well-being outcomes after virtual contact
with different types of nature. Contact with wild nature resulted in significantly more positive affect
benefit compared to contact with urban nature and the non-nature control condition. Contrary to
previous research [2,42], virtual contact with urban nature was not significantly different from the
non-nature control in eliciting positive affect. The present results suggest that only the wild nature
experience enables elicitation of positive emotional states such as excitement, strength, and
inspiration (as assessed by the positive affect subscale of PANAS) [36].
The results for negative affect suggest that virtual contact with either wild nature or urban nature
significantly decreased negative affect, compared to a non-nature control, which is consistent with
previous research [22]. There was no significant difference between the virtual experience of a wild
or urban nature on negative affect. These results suggest that urban nature is as effective as wild
nature in the alleviation of negative affect. This is a useful finding in the development of evidence for
nature therapy. If contact with an urban park is as efficacious in easing feelings of distress, shame, or
anxiety as travelling to a national park, more people may be able to experience the benefit of nature
in reducing negative affect more often.
The present study found small effect sizes for group differences in positive affect and negative
affect, which is consistent with previous research [2,11]. The results of the present study indicated
that exposure to virtual nature for 2–3 min can produce significant improvements in affective states.
Our results add further evidence to the known beneficial effects of nature experience in improving
mood. More specifically, it provides evidence of only wild nature experience increasing positive
affect and either wild or urban nature experience lessening negative affect compared to a non-nature
control. This finding has practical implications, as viewing short virtual nature videos could provide
easily accessible and low-cost intervention to improve mood and mental health.
4.2. Mediation Analyses
For the relationship between virtual contact with wild nature and positive affect, the results
support the hypothesis that perceived restorativeness is a path, or mediator, explaining this
relationship. This is consistent with previous research showing perceived restorativeness as a path
that provides a link between nature experience and affect [16,31], and provides further evidence that
restoration of attention can increase positive affect. Perceived restorativeness also mediated the
relationship between contact with urban nature and negative affect. This suggests that the
mechanisms for the well-being effect of nature varies depending on the type of nature and is not the
same for positive affect and negative affect.
4.3. Limitations and Future Research
The present study had some limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the current
findings. Participants in the study sample reported low levels of negative affect. This floor effect may
have not allowed a full test of the impact of nature in alleviating negative affect. Future work in this
area may benefit from a process to induce negative affect prior to contact with nature, as has been
used in some previous studies [43]. In investigating the impact of nature on negative affect, future
research might also focus on populations of individuals high in negative affect, such as those
experiencing anxiety. In the present study, we did not measure positive and negative affect at
baseline, therefore, it was not possible to investigate the change in positive and negative affect in
response to a virtual experience of wild or urban nature. Nevertheless, the random assignment of
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 9 of 11
participants to different experimental conditions ensures no such systematic influences on our
results.
The specific operationalization of constructs used in the present study may also represent some
limitations that can be addressed by future research. Measuring attentional capacity directly with
cognitive tests, rather than through a self-report measure of restorativeness, would potentially
improve validity. The videos used for the experimental manipulation were constructed to represent
wild nature, urban nature, and a non-nature urban control condition. Each represented condition is
in an Australian setting, therefore, replication of results with other nature and urban settings would
increase confidence in the generalizability of the results.
Studies which integrate consideration of eudaimonic and hedonic well-being may be useful in
generating a more complete picture of psychological benefits of nature contact. The benefit of nature
for eudaimonic well-being is an active area of research [44], but has attracted less research effort to
date than subjective well-being. Future studies may also investigate the well-being effects of
environments such as beaches, coral reefs, riverine areas, and alpine areas, as research into the
psychology of contact with ecosystems other than forests and woodlands is rare [45]. Investigating
benefits of nature contact for those diagnosed with mood or other psychological disorders is also a
valuable research direction supporting clinical applications.
5. Conclusions
The findings of the present study indicate that even brief, virtual nature experience can enhance
affect, and adds to the evidence of the benefits of increased green space in urban areas to reduce
negative emotional states, thus promoting mental health. These findings provide support for
attention restoration theory, with evidence that improvements in mood resulting from nature
experience may be facilitated by the restorative impact of nature on attentional capacity.
Author Contributions: Elizabeth McAllister and Navjot Bhullar conceptualized and designed the study.
Elizabeth McAllister conducted the study. Both Elizabeth McAllister and Navjot Bhullar analyzed the data and
contributed equally in preparing this paper. Nicola S. Schutte contributed to the analysis of the data and writing
of the paper.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Appendix A
Wild nature video URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=qHZ3rV6TzMs
Urban nature video URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=RZMazVelWH4
Non-nature control video URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4nfG3xo3mBU
References
1. Bowler, D.E.; Buyung-Ali, L.M.; Knight, T.M.; Pullin, A.S. A Systematic Review of Evidence for the Added
Benefits to Health of Exposure to Natural Environments. BMC Public Health 2010, 10, 456.
2. McMahan, E.A.; Estes, D. The Effect of Contact with Natural Environments on Positive and Negative affect:
A Meta-Analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015, 10, 507–519.
3. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984.
4. Townsend, M.; Weerasuriya, R. Beyond Blue to Green: The Benefits of Contact with Nature for Mental Health and
Well-Being; Beyond Blue Limited: Melbourne, Australia, 2010.
5. Shanahan, D.F.; Lin, B.B.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Dean, J.H.; Barber, E.; Fuller, R.A. Toward Improved Public
Health Outcomes from Urban Nature. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, 470–477.
6. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Daily, G.C. The Impacts of Nature Experience on Human Cognitive Function
and Mental Health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012, 1249, 118–136.
7. Kaplan, S.; Kaplan, R. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press:
Cambridge, UK, 1989.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 10 of 11
8. Marselle, M.R.; Irvine, K.N.; Lorenzo-Arribas, A.; Warber, S.L. Moving Beyond Green: Exploring the
Relationship of Environment Type and Indicators of Perceived Environmental Quality on Emotional Well-
being Following Group Walks. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 106–130.
9. Miller, G.T.; Spoolman, S.E. Living in the Environment, 17th ed.; Brooks/Cole: Belmont, CA, USA, 2012.
10. Hartig, T.; Book, A.; Garvill, J.; Olsson, T.; Garling, T. Environmental Influences on Psychological
Restoration. Scand. J. Psychol. 1996, 37, 378–393.
11. Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M.; Bruehlman-Senecal, E.; Dolliver, K. Why is Nature Beneficial? The Role of
Connectedness to Nature. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 607–643.
12. MacKerron, G.; Mourato, S. Happiness is Greater in Natural Environments. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23,
992–1000.
13. Ward-Thompson, C.; Roe, J.; Aspinall, P.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A.; Miller, D. More green space is linked to
less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 105,
221–229.
14. Daniel, T. Measuring the Quality of the Natural Environment: A Psychophysical Approach. Am. Psychol.
1990, 45, 633–637.
15. Hartig, T.; Korpela, K.; Evans, G.; Garling, T. A Measure of Restorative Quality in Environments. Scand.
Hous. Plan. Res. 1997, 14, 175–194.
16. Schutte, N.S.; Bhullar, N.; Stilinovic, E.J.; Richardson, K. The Impact of Virtual Environments on
Restorativeness and Affect. Ecopsychology 2017, 9, 1–7.
17. Valtchanov, D.; Ellard, C. Physiological and Affective Responses to Immersion in Virtual Reality: Effects of
Nature and Urban settings. J. Cyberther. Rehabil. 2010, 3, 359–374.
18. Jorgensen, A.; Gobster, P.H. Shades of Green: Measuring the Ecology of Urban Green Space in the Context
of Human Health and Well-being. Nat. Cult. 2010, 5, 338–363.
19. Carrus, G.; Scopelliti, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Colangelo, G.; Ferrini, F.; Salbitano, F.; Sanesi, G. Go Greener, Feel
Better? The Positive Effects of Biodiversity on the Well-being of Individuals Visiting Urban and Peri-urban
Green Areas. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 134, 221–228.
20. Dallimer, M.; Irvine, K.N.; Skinner, A.M.J.; Davies, Z.G.; Rouquette, J.R.; Maltby, L.L.; Warren, P.H.;
Armsorth, P.R.; Gaston, K.J. Biodiversity and the Feel-Good Factor: Understanding Associations Between
Self-reported Human Well-being and Species Richness. Bioscience 2012, 62, 47–55.
21. Fuller, R.A.; Irvine, K.N.; Devine-Wright, P.; Warren, P.H.; Gaston, K.J. Psychological Benefits of
Greenspace Increase with Biodiversity. Biol. Lett. 2007, 3, 390–394.
22. Van den Berg, A.E.; Jorgensen, A.; Wilson, E.R. Evaluating Restoration in Urban Green Spaces: Does Setting
Type Make a Difference? Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 127, 173–181.
23. Berman, M.G.; Kross, E.; Krpan, K.M.; Askren, M.K.; Burson, A.; Deldin, P.J.; Kaplan, S.; Sherdell, L.;
Gotlib, I.H.; Jonides, J. Interacting with Nature Improves Cognition and Affect for Individuals with
Depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 140, 300–305.
24. Kaplan, S. The Restorative Benefits of Nature: Toward an Integrated Framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995,
15, 169–182.
25. Berto, R. Exposure to Restorative Environments Helps Restore Attentional Capacity. J. Environ. Psychol.
2005, 25, 249–259.
26. Hartig, T.; Evans, G.; Jamner, L.; Davis, D.; Gärling, T. Tracking Restoration in Natural and Urban Field
Settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123.
27. Laumann, K.; Gärling, T.; Stormark, K.M. Selective Attention and Heart Rate Responses to Natural and
Urban Environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 125–134.
28. Lee, K.E.; Williams, K.J.H.; Sargent, L.D.; Williams, N.S.G.; Johnson, K.A. 40-second Green Roof Views
Sustain Attention: The Role of Micro-breaks in Attention Restoration. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 42, 182–189.
29. Hipp, J.A.; Ogunseitan, O.A. Effect of Environmental Conditions on Perceived Psychological
Restorativeness of Coastal Parks. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 421–429.
30. Tyrväinen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The Influence of Urban Green
Environments on Stress Relief Measures: A Field Experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9.
31. Sato, I.; Conner, T. The Quality of Time in Nature: How Fascination Explains and Enhances the Relationship
Between Nature Experiences and Daily Affect. Ecopsychology 2013, 5, 197–204.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 786 11 of 11
32. Hartmann, P.; Apaolaza-Ibanez, V. Beyond Savanna: An Evolutionary and Environmental Psychology
Approach to Behavioral Effects of Nature Scenery in Green Advertising. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30,
119–128.
33. Nittono, H.; Fukushima, M.; Yano, A.; Moriya, H. The Power of Kawaii: Viewing Cute Images Promotes a
Careful Behavior and Narrows Attentional Focus. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e46362.
34. Saroglou, V.; Buxant, C.; Tilquin, J. Positive Emotions as Leading to Religion and Spirituality. J. Posit.
Psychol. 2008, 3, 165–173.
35. Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Bowler, P.A. Further Development of a Measure of Perceived Environmental
Restorativeness; Uppsala Universitet: Uppsala, Sweden, 1997.
36. Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and
Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063–1070.
37. Watson, D.; Naragon-Gainey, K. On the Specificity of Positive Emotional Dysfunction in Psychopathology:
Evidence from the Mood and Anxiety Disorders and Schizophrenia/Schizotypy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30,
839–848.
38. Lovibond, S.H.; Lovibond, P.F. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd ed.; Psychology
Foundation: Sydney, Australia, 1995.
39. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based
Approach; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013.
40. Crawford, J.R.; Henry, J.D. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): Construct Validity,
Measurement Properties and Normative Data in a Large Non-clinical Sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2004, 43,
245–265.
41. Cohen, J.W. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates:
Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988.
42. White, M.; Alcock, I.; Wheeler, B.W.; Depledge, M.H. Would You Be Happier Living in a Greener Urban
Area? A Fixed-effects Analysis of Panel Data. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 920–928.
43. Brown, D.; Barton, J.; Gladwell, V. Viewing Nature Scenes Positively Affects Recovery of Autonomic
Function Following Acute Mental Stress. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2013, 47, 5562–5569.
44. Kamitsis, I.; Francis, A.J.P. Spirituality Mediates the Relationship between Engagement with Nature and
Psychological Wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 136–143.
45. Han, K.-T. Responses to Six Major Terrestrial Biomes in terms of Scenic Beauty, Preference, and
Restorativeness. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 529–556.
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).