ArticlePDF Available

Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers and Martial Artists: Investigating potential Differences using the Balloon Analogue Risk Task

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study investigated the effects of regular threat exposure on risk-taking by comparing risk-taking propensity between police officers, martial artists, and a control group not regularly exposed to real or simulated threats. The behavioral measure of risk-taking propensity known as the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) was administered to a sample of 205 participants (police officers, martial artists, and controls). Results showed no difference between the groups, which indicates that experience as a police officer and/or a martial artist did not lead to a change in risk taking propensity. The potential importance of risk-taking propensity to a better understanding of decision-making of police officers and martial artists are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ISSN: 2161-0231 (ONLINE)
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers and Martial Artists:
Investigating potential Differences using the Balloon Analogue
Risk Task
Mario S. Staller1, Benjamin Zaiser1, Swen Körner3, Jon C. Cole1
1 University of Liverpool, Department of Psychological Sciences,
Tactical Decision Making Research Group
3 German Sports University Cologne, Institute of Pedagogy and Philosophy
Abstract
This study investigated the effects of regular threat exposure on risk-taking by comparing
risk-taking propensity between police officers, martial artists, and a control group not
regularly exposed to real or simulated threats. The behavioral measure of risk-taking
propensity known as the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) was administered to a sample
of 205 participants (police officers, martial artists, and controls). Results showed no
difference between the groups, which indicates that experience as a police officer and/or a
martial artist did not lead to a change in risk taking propensity. The potential importance of
risk-taking propensity to a better understanding of decision-making of police officers and
martial artists are discussed.
Keywords: martial arts, police use of force, risk-taking propensity
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
2!
Introduction
Police officers are regularly exposed to threats (Jager, Klatt, & Bliesener, 2013). In this
context, taking and managing risk is a common element of the police profession and a routine
action for police officers, especially when decisions with impact on officer and civilian safety
have to be made within split seconds (Beauregard & Michaud, 2015; Hill, 2002). Compared
to police officers, martial artists (including practitioners of combat sports and self-defense)
are not exposed to real-life threats on a regular basis. However, martial artists engage in
simulated, confrontational situations (Staller, Bertram, Althaus, Heil, & Klemmer, 2016) or
physical combat settings (e.g. Branco et al., 2013; Jensen, Roman, Shaft, & Wrisberg, 2013;
Markovic, Vucetic, & Cardinale, 2008). Even though risk-taking behaviors that place an
individual at risk for health or safety outcomes have received particular attention in the
literature (e.g. Banducci, Felton, Dahne, Ninnemann, & Lejuez, 2015; Bornovalova, Gwadz,
Kahler, Aklin, & Lejuez, 2008; Hanson, Thayer, & Tapert, 2014; Lejuez, Simmons, Aklin,
Daughters, & Dvir, 2004; Zuckerman, Ball, & Black, 1990), police officers and martial artists
have not been subject to any investigations so far. Therefore, the study reported with this
article aims to assess whether experience as a police officer or martial artist can consequently
lead to an increase in risk taking behavior. The study compared police officers to martial
artists, as both differ from normal populations due to experiences of being in hostile situations
where risk assessment, management, and mitigation may bring some benefit.
Literature
Risk-taking behaviors can be defined as behaviors that involve some potential for
danger or harm, while simultaneously providing an opportunity to obtain a reward of some
kind (Leigh, 1999). Such behaviors are neither positive nor negative on their own in the sense
that they are neither functional nor dysfunctional. Rather they encompass a broad range of
behaviors that fall along both positive and negative dimensions (Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer,
1999; Foersterling, 1980; Leigh, 1999; Lejuez et al., 2002).
The specific risks that police officers and martial artists have to manage differ from
each other. In the context of policing, the management of risk (with potentially detrimental
outcomes for health and safety) is an everyday task (Hoyle, 1998; Kane, 1999; 2000; Perez
Trujillo & Ross, 2008). On the other hand, martial artists have to deal with risks to health and
safety associated with combat and training settings (Gauthier, 2008; Jensen et al., 2013). In
this context, their decision-making can have serious consequences as well. In situations
where every alternative regularly has drawbacks (Yates & Tschirhart, 2006) and the
environment is uncertain, time pressured, and dynamic (Endsley, Hoffman, Kaber, & Roth,
2007; Lipshitz, Klein, Orasanu, & Salas, 2001; Parent & Verdun Jones, 1998; Staller &
Zaiser, 2015), experienced decision-makers are thought to rely on intuitive decision-making
based on recognition of situational cues (K. G. Ross, Klein, Thunholm, Schmitt, & Baxter,
2004). Accordingly, police officers have to take risks, since the environment is uncertain and
they need to balance the potential positive outcomes of their behavior against potential
negative outcomes associated with the situation. In situations that pose threats to the police
officer or members of the public, the acting officer has to make quick and complex decisions.
The officer has to compare potential courses of action to contextual constraints and the
familiarity of the unfolding situation (Klein, 2008; Klein, Orasanu, Calderwood, & Zsambok,
1993). Correspondingly, experience plays a crucial role and the process by which experts
make decisions based on experience and an increased ability to assess risk (Wickens, 1992).
Furthermore, through the use of heuristics, experts are able to quickly select the best tool or
strategy for a given task in an uncertain world (Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011; Todd,
Gigerenzer, ABC Research Group, 2012). These strategies ignore “part of the information,
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
3!
with the goal of making decisions more quickly, frugally, and/or accurately than more
complex methods” (Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011, p. 454). With this in mind, a propensity
for risk-taking behavior in police officers and martial artists may be affected by past
experiences, where risk-taking has been used effectively. This may lead to the increased
accessibility of heuristics related to risk-taking behavior (Fischer, Guter, & Frey, 2008).
Hypothesis
Given that police officers and martial artists have to manage risks on a regular basis,
it is hypothesized that both groups would show a difference in risk-taking behavior compared
to a control group.
Operationalization
One potential tool for gauging risk-taking behavior within a laboratory setting is the
Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), a computerized assessment of risk taking propensity
(Hopko et al., 2006; Lejuez et al., 2002; Lejuez, Aklin, Zvolensky, & Pedulla, 2003b; Lejuez
et al., 2007; White, Lejuez, & de Wit, 2008). It is one of the most widely used and tested
sequential risk-taking tasks and is proven to be a sensitive measure to real world risk-taking in
different clinical and non-clinical populations (Aklin, Lejuez, Zvolensky, Kahler, & Gwadz,
2005; Banducci et al., 2015; Bornovalova et al., 2008; Campbell, Samartgis, & Crowe, 2013;
Crowley, Raymond, Mikulich-Gilbertson, Thompson, & Lejuez, 2006; Hanson et al., 2014;
Lauriola, Panno, Levin, & Lejuez, 2014; Lawyer, 2013; Lejuez et al., 2002; 2004; 2007;
2003a; Lejuez, Aklin, Bornovalova, & Moolchan, 2005). The BART requires participants to
inflate a series of balloons. The more an individual balloon is inflated, the more money a
participant will earn. The caveat is that all money earned for a given balloon will be lost if
the balloon is inflated too much and explodes. Hence, the BART is able to determine the
likelihood of behaving in a risky manner by measuring the average number of pumps that
balloons are inflated to in the context of earning monetary rewards (Banducci et al., 2015).
Since performance on the BART is associated with substance and alcohol use, smoking,
psychopathy, and risky sex among adults (Campbell et al., 2013; Gonzalez et al., 2012;
Hanson et al., 2014; Hopko et al., 2006; Hunt, 2005; Lejuez et al., 2002; 2003a; Schuster,
Crane, Mermelstein, & Gonzalez, 2012), it can be assumed that it captures adults’ likelihood
of behaving in a risky manner across a variety of contexts (Banducci et al., 2015). Therefore,
performance on the BART serves as an accessible proxy of the participants’ risk-taking
propensity across multiple contexts.
Methods
Participants
A total of 205 participants took part in the study. Police officers were sampled through an
opportunistic sampling method. Martial artists and the control group were recruited via social
media (Facebook). Participants were asked about experience as police officers and in martial
arts and were subsequently assigned to different groups for the study. Two cases of missing
data were identified (police officers with no experience in martial arts) resulting in a final
sample of N = 203.
The sample consisted of (a) police officers (n = 72) with no experience in martial
arts, except for police training, (b) martial artists (n = 50), that had regular training experience
in martial arts, and were not part of a police force, (c) police officers with additional martial
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
4!
arts experience (n = 33), and (d) a control group (n = 48) with no experience in the policing
sector and no experience in any martial art or combat sport. Demographical data of
participants is shown in Table 1 (see Table 1).
Materials
The BART required participants to pump up a simulated balloon on the screen by the click of
a mouse. The computer screen shows a small-simulated balloon that gets bigger with every
click of the mouse. Participants are asked to gain as much money as possible on each trial
and cashing out by clicking “Collect $$$” on the screen. Furthermore, a permanent money-
earned display labeled “Total Winnings” and display listing the money that could be earned,
when the participants decided to cash out (“Potential earnings”), and the trial number
(“Balloon number”) are presented on the screen (see Figure 1).
Figure 1. On-screen presentation of the BART
With each click (or pump of the balloon), 5 cents was earned on a temporary account
(“Potential earnings”). When a balloon was pumped past its individual explosion point, a
sound-effect combined with the explosion of the balloon was presented. In that case,
participants lost all money in the temporary account and a new balloon appeared on the
screen. Accordingly, participants had to decide when to stop inflating the balloon. After 30
trials the adjusted average pump count (AAPC) was calculated as the behavioral statistic of
the task. The AAPC is the mean number of pumps on trials that do not end in an explosion
(Pleskac, Wallsten, Wang, & Lejuez, 2008).
Procedure
Participants accessed a website (hosted on www.millisecond.com), which outlined
information regarding the experiment and issued a consent form. If accepted, the Inquisit web
software (Inquisit v4.0.5.0., 2014) was downloaded and screen resolution was automatically
set to 800 x 600. Before testing began, participants were informed that the test should be
completed in a quiet setting. After the tasks were finished, participants were directed to a
website (hosted on www.soscisurey.de), which recorded demographic information and any
experience of being part of a police force or martial art group. Participants were thanked for
their time and presented with debrief information.
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
5!
Analytic Strategy
Group differences were assessed using a one-way ANOVA. The dependent variable was the
AACP, whereas the groups formed the independent variables. Normality of the data sets were
checked using Shapiro-Wilk’s test (Razali & Wah, 2011; Shapiro & Wilk, 1965) and a visual
inspection of histograms, normal Q-Q plots and box plots (Doane & Seward, 2011). The
results showed that the AAPC score of the BART was normally distributed. Statistical
analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 24.0. A significance level of p <
.05 was set.
Results
Means, standard deviations, and 95% confidence intervals of the AAPC are displayed in
Table 2.
Table 2.
Means, Standard Deviations and 95% Confidence Intervals of AAPC Scores
n
M
SD
LL
UL
Police
72
32.26
14.69
28.81
35.72
Martial Arts
50
32.89
13.86
28.95
36.83
Police & Martial Arts
33
29.17
14.13
24.16
34.19
Control
48
30.64
11.51
27.30
33.98
Total
203
31.53
13.66
29.64
33.42
Results of the one-way ANOVA yielded no significant difference between the groups, F(3,
202) = 0.63, p = .600. Means and 95% confidence intervals of the AAPC are depicted in
Figure 2.
Figure 2. Means and 95% confidence intervals of AAPC score
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
6!
Discussion
This study investigated the effects of regular threat exposure on risk taking. In particular, it
inquired whether exposure to real threats, as typical for police officers, or whether regular
exposure to threats associated with training or competition settings, as typical for martial
artists, could lead to an increased propensity for risk-taking. The study demonstrated that
neither police officers nor martial arts practitioners show a propensity for risk-taking as
measured by the BART.
This finding has two implications: First, exposure to situations of mild threat such as
practice and competition activities, as common for martial artists, do not lead to an increase in
risk taking. Second, the exposure to real threat does also not result in an increase in risk-
taking behavior as indicated by the AAPC scores of the participating police officers.
Considering that a substantial component of police use of force and martial arts training
consists of applying reasonable force, the role of training may act as a mediating factor in
situations of real and simulated threat and reduce risk-taking behavior. Yet, further research
is needed to investigate this assumption.
There are several limitations that have to be acknowledged. First, even though
participants reported their experience as police officers and/or martial artists, the actual
exposure to threats was not assessed. Future studies should consider the difference between
the experience as a police officer and the actual exposure to real life threats (Schmalzl, 2008).
Second, the groups of police officers and martial artists consisted of a broad range of
individuals of the specific population. It could be possible that more specialized groups (e.g.
special operations officers; mixed-martial artists) display differences in the propensity for
risk-taking. Third, since some of the participants completed the test at home via the Internet,
there is the possibility of distractions during testing. Although participants were asked to
make sure they did not get distracted, it cannot be controlled for (Birnbaum, 2004).
The study is one of the first of its kind to employ a behavioural assessment measure
of risk-taking propensity in order to shed light on the decision-making of police officers and
martial artists. Although several questions remain for future research, results from the current
study set the stage for future research examining the potential relationship between risk-taking
propensity and the exposure to threats as a police officer or a martial artist.
About the Authors:
Dr. Mario Staller is a German police officer with more than ten years of experience as a
police use of force, self-defense, and firearms instructor. His main areas of
research are psychological aspects of conflict management in police contexts, skill
development and pedagogical practice in police use of force and self-defense training.
Benjamin Zaiser served as a member of the federal crisis negotiations team of the German
police and as a narcotics agent in both national and international settings. His research
interests include social cognition during critical and major incidents, tactical decision making
in law enforcement contexts, and the pedagogy of corresponding training and education.
Dr. Swen Körner is a tenured professor at the German Sports University Cologne. His
current research is focusing on nonlinear pedagogy, training pedagogy, network analysis and
social system dynamics. He is the head of several ministry funded research projects and has
been a member of the Advisory Council of the German Federal Institute for Sports Science
for many years.
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
7!
Dr. Jon Cole is the director of the Tactical Decision Making Research Group at the
Department of Psychological Sciences at the University of Liverpool. The research group
focuses on (i) the psychology of armed confrontations, (ii) preventing violent extremism and
radicalisation, (iii) terrorist decision making, (iv) crisis negotiation, and (v) Social Media
Intelligence (SOCMINT).
References
Aklin, W. M., Lejuez, C. W., Zvolensky, M. J., Kahler, C. W., & Gwadz, M. (2005).
Evaluation of behavioral measures of risk taking propensity with inner city adolescents.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(2), 215–228.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.007
Banducci, A. N., Felton, J. W., Dahne, J., Ninnemann, A., & Lejuez, C. W. (2015). Maternal
risk taking on the balloon analogue risk task as a prospective predictor of youth alcohol
use escalation. Addictive Behaviors, 49, 40–45.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.011
Beauregard, E., & Michaud, P. (2015). “HIGH-RISK”: A Useful Tool for Improving Police
Decision-Making in Hostage and Barricade Incidents. Journal of Police and Criminal
Psychology, 30(3), 125–138. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-014-9151-4
Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the Internet. Annual Review
of Psychology, 55(1), 803–832. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141601
Bornovalova, M. A., Gwadz, M. A., Kahler, C., Aklin, W. M., & Lejuez, C. W. (2008).
Sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity as mediators in the relationship between
childhood abuse and HIV-related risk behavior. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32(1), 99–109.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.04.009
Branco, B. H. M., Massuça, L. M., Andreato, L. V., Marinho, B. F., Miarka, B., Monteiro, L.,
& Franchini, E. (2013). Association between the rating perceived exertion, heart rate and
blood lactate in successive judo fights (randori). Asian Journal of Sports Medicine, 4(2),
125–130. http://doi.org/10.5812/asjsm.34494
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
2909.125.3.367
Campbell, J. A., Samartgis, J. R., & Crowe, S. F. (2013). Impaired decision making on the
Balloon Analogue Risk Task as a result of long-term alcohol use. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(10), 1071–1081.
http://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2013.856382
Crowley, T. J., Raymond, K. M., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S. K., Thompson, L. L., & Lejuez, C.
W. (2006). A Risk-Taking “Set” in a Novel Task Among Adolescents With Serious
Conduct and Substance Problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(2), 175–183.
http://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000188893.60551.31
Doane, D. P., & Seward, L. E. (2011). Measuring skewness: a forgotten statistic? Journal of
Statistics Education, 19(2), 1–18.
Endsley, M. R., Hoffman, R. R., Kaber, D., & Roth, E. (2007). Cognitive Engineering and
Decision Making: An Overview and Future Course. Journal of Cognitive Engineering
and Decision Making, 1(1), 1–21. http://doi.org/10.1177/155534340700100101
Fischer, P., Guter, S., & Frey, D. (2008). The effects of risk-promoting media on inclinations
toward risk taking. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 30(3), 230–240.
http://doi.org/10.1080/01973530802375029
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
8!
Foersterling, F. (1980). Sex differences in risk taking: effects of subjective and objective
probability of success. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6(1), 149–152.
http://doi.org/10.1177/014616728061023
Gauthier, J. (2008). Ethical and Social Issues in Combat Sports: Should Combat Sports Be
Banned? In Combat Sports Medicine (pp. 73–88). London: Springer London.
http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84800-354-5_5
Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of
Psychology, 62(1), 451–482. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346
Gonzalez, R., Schuster, R. M., Mermelstein, R. J., Vassileva, J., Martin, E. M., & Diviak, K.
R. (2012). Performance of young adult cannabis users on neurocognitive measures of
impulsive behavior and their relationship to symptoms of cannabis use disorders. Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(9), 962–976.
http://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.703642
Hanson, K. L., Thayer, R. E., & Tapert, S. F. (2014). Adolescent marijuana users have
elevated risk-taking on the balloon analog risk task. Journal of Psychopharmacology,
28(11), 1080–1087. http://doi.org/10.1177/0269881114550352
Hill, J. (2002). High-speed police pursuits: Dangers, dynamics, and risk reduction. FBI Law
Enforcement Bulletin, 71(7), 14–18. http://doi.org/10.1037/e320552004-003
Hopko, D. R., Lejuez, C. W., Daughters, S. B., Aklin, W. M., Osborne, A., Simmons, B. L.,
& Strong, D. R. (2006). Construct validity of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART):
Relationship with MDMA use by inner-city drug users in residential treatment. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28(2), 95–101.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-006-7487-5
Hoyle, C. (1998). Negotiating domestic violence: Police, criminal justice, and victims.
Oxford, UK: Clarendon.
Hunt, M. K. (2005). Construct Validity of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART):
Associations With Psychopathy and Impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416–428.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1073191105278740
Inquisit 4.0.5.0. (2014). Inquisit 4.0.5.0. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software LLC.
Jager, J., Klatt, T., & Bliesener, T. (2013). NRW-Studie: Gewalt gegen Polizeibeamtinnen und
Polizeibeamte [North Rhine-Westphalian study: Violence against police officers]. Kiel:
Institut für Psychologie, Christian-Albrechts-Universität.
Jensen, P. R., Roman, J., Shaft, B., & Wrisberg, C. (2013). In the cage: MMA fighters'
experience of competition. Sport Psychologist, 27(1), 1–12.
Kane, R. J. (1999). Patterns of arrest in domestic violence encounters. Journal of Criminal
Justice, 27(1), 65–79. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2352(98)00037-3
Kane, R. J. (2000). Police responses to restraining orders in domestic violence incidents:
Identifying the custody-threshold thesis. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27(5), 561–580.
http://doi.org/10.1177/0093854800027005002
Klein, G. A. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Human Factors, 50(3), 456–460.
http://doi.org/10.1518/001872008X288385
Klein, G. A., Orasanu, J., Calderwood, R., & Zsambok, C. E. (1993). Decision making in
action: Models and methods. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Lauriola, M., Panno, A., Levin, I. P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2014). Individual differences in risky
decision making: A meta-analysis of sensation seeking and impulsivity with the Balloon
Analogue Risk Task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(1), 20–36.
http://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1784
Lawyer, S. (2013). Risk taking for sexual versus monetary outcomes using the Balloon
Analogue Risk Task. The Psychological Record, 63(4), 803–820.
http://doi.org/10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.4.007
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
9!
Leigh, B. C. (1999). Peril, chance, adventure: Concepts of risk, alcohol use and risky behavior
in young adults. Addiction, 94(3), 371–383. http://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-
0443.1999.9433717.x
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Richards, J. B., Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., &
Read, J. P. (2003a). The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and
nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 26–33.
http://doi.org/10.1037//1064-1297.11.1.26
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Pedulla, C. M. (2003b). Evaluation of the
Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) as a predictor of adolescent real-world risk-taking
behaviours. Journal of Adolescence, 26(4), 475–479. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-
1971(03)00036-8
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W., Bornovalova, M., & Moolchan, E. T. (2005). Differences in risk-
taking propensity across inner-city adolescent ever- and never-smokers. Nicotine &
Tobacco Research, 7(1), 71–79. http://doi.org/10.1080/14622200412331328484
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W., Daughters, S., Zvolensky, M., Kahler, C., & Gwadz, M. (2007).
Reliability and validity of the youth version of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART-
Y) in the assessment of risk-taking behavior among inner-city adolescents. Journal of
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(1), 106–111.
http://doi.org/10.1080/15374410709336573
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al.
(2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk
Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 75–84.
http://doi.org/10.1037//1076-898X.8.2.75
Lejuez, C. W., Simmons, B. L., Aklin, W. M., Daughters, S. B., & Dvir, S. (2004). Risk-
taking propensity and risky sexual behavior of individuals in residential substance use
treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 29(8), 1643–1647.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.02.035
Lipshitz, R., Klein, G. A., Orasanu, J., & Salas, E. (2001). Taking stock of naturalistic
decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 14(5), 331–352.
http://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.381
Markovic, G., Vucetic, V., & Cardinale, M. (2008). Heart rate and lactate responses to
taekwondo fight in elite women performers. Biology of Sport, 25(2), 135–146.
Parent, R. B., & Verdun Jones, S. (1998). Victim-precipitated homicide: police use of deadly
force in British Columbia. Policing: an International Journal of Police Strategies &
Management, 21(3), 432–448. http://doi.org/10.1108/13639519810228741
Perez Trujillo, M., & Ross, S. (2008). Police response to domestic violence: Making decisions
about risk and risk management. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23(4), 454–473.
http://doi.org/10.1177/0886260507312943
Pleskac, T. J., Wallsten, T. S., Wang, P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2008). Development of an
automatic response mode to improve the clinical utility of sequential risk-taking tasks.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 555–564.
http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014245
Razali, N. M., & Wah, Y. B. (2011). Power comparisons of shapiro-wilk, kolmogorov-
smirnov, lilliefors and anderson-darling tests. Journal of Statistical Modeling and
Analytics, 2(1), 21–33.
Ross, K. G., Klein, G. A., Thunholm, P., Schmitt, J. F., & Baxter, H. C. (2004). The
recognition-primed decision model. Military Review, (4), 6–10.
Schmalzl, H. P. (2008). Einsatzkompetenz [Competence on duty]. Frankfurt: Verlag für
Polizeiwissenschaft.
Risk-taking Behavior in Police Officers & Martial Artists/Staller/Zaiser/ Körner/Cole
!
!!!"#$%&'"()*+,-./0/12.3/,4567589,:4;<5=6,4>,6=?,85>4<@8A85BC,D46;A8,0C,5;AE8<,.
10!
Schuster, R. M., Crane, N. A., Mermelstein, R., & Gonzalez, R. (2012). The influence of
inhibitory control and episodic memory on the risky sexual behavior of young adult
cannabis users. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(05), 827–
833. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712000586
Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete
samples). Biometrika, 52(3-4), 591–611. http://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/52.3-4.591
Staller, M. S., & Zaiser, B. (2015). Developing Problem Solvers: New Perspectives on
Pedagogical Practices in Police Use of Force Training. The Journal of Law Enforcement,
4(3), 1–15.
Staller, M. S., Bertram, O., Althaus, P., Heil, V., & Klemmer, I. (2016). Selbstverteidigung in
Deutschland – Eine empirische Studie zu trainingsdidaktischen Aspekten von 103
Selbstverteidigungssystemen [Self-defence in Germany - A empirical study of
pedagogical aspects in 103 self-defence systems]. In M. J. Meyer (Ed.), Martial Arts
Studies in Germany - Defining and Crossing Disciplinary Boundaries (pp. 51–56).
Hamburg: Czwalina.
Todd, P. M., Gigerenzer, G., ABC Reserach Group. (2012). Ecological rationality:
Intelligence in the real world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
White, T. L., Lejuez, C. W., & de Wit, H. (2008). Test-retest characteristics of the Balloon
Analogue Risk Task (BART). Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(6),
565–570. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014083
Wickens, C. D. (1992). Engineering psychology and human performance. London, UK:
Harper Collins.
Yates, J. F., & Tschirhart, M. D. (2006). Decision-making expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N.
Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise
and expert performance (pp. 421–438). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zuckerman, M., Ball, S., & Black, J. (1990). Influences of sensation seeking, gender, risk
appraisal, and situational motivation on smoking. Addictive Behaviors, 15(3), 209–220.
http://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(90)90064-5
!
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
We review the progress of naturalistic decision making (NDM) in the decade since the first conference on the subject in 1989. After setting out a brief history of NDM we identify its essential characteristics and consider five of its main contributions: recognition-primed decisions, coping with uncertainty, team decision making, decision errors, and methodology. NDM helped identify important areas of inquiry previously neglected (e.g. the use of expertise in sizing up situations and generating options), it introduced new models, conceptualizations, and methods, and recruited applied investigators into the field. Above all, NDM contributed a new perspective on how decisions (broadly defined as committing oneself to a certain course of action) are made. NDM still faces significant challenges, including improvement of the quantity and rigor of its empirical research, and confirming the validity of its prescriptive models. Copyright © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Article
Full-text available
Police use of force training is a crucial guarantor of officer safety when preparing law enforcement professionals to prevent and deal with physical confrontations. Current research on the efficiency of use of force application in the line of duty revealed that acquired skills do not necessarily transfer to real world incidents (Jager, Klatt, & Bliesener, 2013; Renden, Nieuwenhuys, Savelsbergh, & Oudejans, 2015). Based on (a) a review of police use of force literature with a focus on the transferability of skills and (b) a consideration of modern approaches of skill development in the sports domain, we argue that designing learning environments based on sound pedagogical practices in the use of force domain improves the transferability of skills taught in training, without the expense of adding additional training time or employing expensive training strategies. It is also suggested that improving police use of force training with a more inclusive approach, emphasizing the acquisition and development of motor skills (i.e., handgun proficiency or self-defense techniques) in conjunction with the associated decision-making skills.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the study is to examine several combinations of risk factors known to the police – i.e., perpetrator and situational characteristics of the incidents – leading to two possible outcomes in domestic hostage and barricade incidents (HBI): autoaggressive and heteroaggressive behaviors from the perpetrator. Using a series of conjunctive analyses on a sample of 534 hostage and barricade incidents, results show that, depending on the specific combination of factors, the likelihood of violence could very much vary, suggesting that the checklist approach is not adequate for risk assessment in hostage and barricade incidents. HIGH-RISK represents a decision support system that can be easily integrated to any crisis-intervention structure (CIS) or police unit dealing with hostage and barricade incidents.
Book
This book examines the factors which shape the criminal justice response to domestic violence in the light of policy changes at the beginning of the 1990s which aimed to increase arrest rates. In particular, the book discusses the needs and expectations of victims and examines how their choices impact on decisions made by police and prosecutors. Many books on the criminal justice response to domestic violence start from the premise that withdrawal of complaints by victims and the subsequent discontinuance of cases, represents some kind of failure on the part of the agencies involved and that victims would benefit from greater determination by police to prosecute offenders wherever possible. Implicit in this approach is the assumption that the criminal justice system as it presently operates is capable of responding effectively to the needs of victims of domestic violence. This book throws doubt on the validity of these assumptions.
Article
The present analysis examines the extent to which police officers arrest domestic violence offenders who violate restraining orders. The study develops a theoretical framework, referred to as the custody-threshold thesis, that considers the decision to arrest to be a function of the officers' goals to satisfy one of several purposes of custody. Findings from logistic regression modeling show that restraining-order violations in domestic violence incidents have the greatest impact on arrest probability when risk of injury to the victim is low, but that as risk increases, the predictive strength of restraining-order violations diminishes. Findings support the custody-threshold thesis, suggesting important policy implications, which are identified and discussed.