Content uploaded by Alexander E. G. Lee
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Alexander E. G. Lee on Jul 13, 2017
Content may be subject to copyright.
Submitted 2 May 2017
Accepted 24 May 2017
Published 30 June 2017
Corresponding author
Alexander E.G. Lee, allee@jyu.fi,
aleex19@gmail.com
Academic editor
Lydia Hopper
Additional Information and
Declarations can be found on
page 14
DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462
Copyright
2017 Lee and Cowlishaw
Distributed under
Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0
OPEN ACCESS
Switching spatial scale reveals
dominance-dependent social foraging
tactics in a wild primate
Alexander E.G. Lee1,2,3and Guy Cowlishaw2
1Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
2The Institute of Zoology, Zoological Society of London, London, United Kingdom
3Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions, Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences,
University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland
ABSTRACT
When foraging in a social group, individuals are faced with the choice of sampling their
environment directly or exploiting the discoveries of others. The evolutionary dynamics
of this trade-off have been explored mathematically through the producer-scrounger
game, which has highlighted socially exploitative behaviours as a major potential cost of
group living. However, our understanding of the tight interplay that can exist between
social dominance and scrounging behaviour is limited. To date, only two theoretical
studies have explored this relationship systematically, demonstrating that because
scrounging requires joining a competitor at a resource, it should become exclusive to
high-ranking individuals when resources are monopolisable. In this study, we explore
the predictions of this model through observations of the natural social foraging
behaviour of a wild population of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). We collected data
through over 800 h of focal follows of 101 adults and juveniles across two troops over
two 3-month periods. By recording over 7,900 social foraging decisions at two spatial
scales we show that, when resources are large and economically indefensible, the joining
behaviour required for scrounging can occur across all social ranks. When, in contrast,
dominant individuals can aggressively appropriate a resource, such joining behaviour
becomes increasingly difficult to employ with decreasing social rank because adult
individuals can only join others lower ranking than themselves. Our study supports
theoretical predictions and highlights potentially important individual constraints
on the ability of individuals of low social rank to use social information, driven by
competition with dominant conspecifics over monopolisable resources.
Subjects Animal Behavior, Ecology
Keywords Social dominance, Producer-scrounger, Individual differences, Competition,
Phenotype-limited strategy, Resource defence, Social foraging, Resource ecology
INTRODUCTION
Socially exploitative behaviours occur when individuals make use of the resources of
competitors. A wide range of both theoretical and empirical studies over recent decades
have highlighted such behaviours as a major potential cost of group living (Giraldeau
& Dubois, 2008). Because resources such as food, mates, breeding territories, or safety
from predation generally show variation in their distribution through space or time,
How to cite this article Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), Switching spatial scale reveals dominance-dependent social foraging tactics in a wild
primate. PeerJ 5:e3462; DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462
individuals should benefit from gathering information about their local environment to
improve decision-making (Valone, 1989;Valone, 2006;McNamara, Green & Olsson, 2006).
However, when the personal collection of information requires search effort or risk taking,
selection should favour the avoidance of these costs through the collection and use of
social information, where individuals attend to the behaviours of others in a social group
to exploit their efforts and knowledge (for review, see Valone & Templeton, 2002;Danchin
et al., 2004;Rieucau & Giraldeau, 2011).
The dynamics of these interactions have been formalised as the producer-scrounger
game (Barnard & Sibly, 1981;Barnard, 1984;Vickery et al., 1991). Supported by a wide
range of empirical studies (e.g., Koops & Giraldeau, 1996;Mottley & Giraldeau, 2000;
Morand-Ferron, Giraldeau & Lefebvre, 2007), the producer-scrounger game has emerged
as the prevailing theoretical framework in which to study social foraging decisions
(Vickery et al., 1991;Giraldeau & Caraco, 2000). In this game, producers actively search for
resources, while scroungers instead rely on social information to exploit the discoveries
of producers. The two tactics are considered mutually exclusive. Scrounging is thus under
negative frequency-dependent selection since its success, being dependent on the efforts of
producers, is determined by the relative frequencies of the two tactics within a group. This
dynamic is expected to lead populations to an evolutionarily or behaviourally stable mix
of producing and scrounging (Giraldeau & Dubois, 2008;Fawcett, Hamblin & Giraldeau,
2013). As such, scrounging behaviour has the potential to reduce the per capita rate of
discovery of new resources (Vickery et al., 1991), which may act to reduce average individual
fitness in a population (Coolen, Giraldeau & Vickery, 2007).
The basic producer-scrounger model assumes that an individual’s phenotype has no
influence on its decision or ability to play either tactic. All individuals are essentially
equivalent, and are expected to receive equal payoffs. However, many empirical studies
have shown that an individual’s tactic choice may be strongly influenced or constrained by
its phenotype (e.g., Beauchamp, 2001;Stahl et al., 2001;Di Bitetti & Janson, 2001;Kurvers
et al., 2010). This has potentially important fitness implications, since theory predicts that
phenotype-limited games may not reach an evolutionarily stable mix of strategies, resulting
in differential payoffs across individuals (Parker, 1982).
Since scrounging behaviour represents the exploitation of another’s resource,
one might expect it to be strongly influenced by social dominance. Specifically, the
competitive advantage of high-ranking individuals should allow them to scrounge
from others more easily (Parker, 1974;Maynard Smith & Parker, 1976;Hammerstein,
1981). Despite this expectation, empirical studies have not been unanimous. While
some experiments have demonstrated a clear positive relationship between social
dominance and scrounging behaviour (Stahl et al., 2001;Liker & Barta, 2002;Lendvai, Liker
& Barta, 2006;McCormack, Jablonski & Brown, 2007), a number of other studies have not
(Bugnyar & Kotrschal, 2002;Robinette Ha & Ha, 2003;Beauchamp, 2006;Teichroeb, White
& Chapman, 2015). This conflict might be reconciled by considering more systematically
the spatiotemporal distribution of resources faced by different taxa in both naturalistic
and experimental settings. The competitive benefits of social dominance are expected
to be associated with priority of access to resources, manifest as contest competition
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 2/19
(Kaufmann, 1983;Łomnicki, 2009). Consistent with this, resource defence theory predicts
that individuals should be more aggressive when defending a resource in accordance with
both its value and how easily it can be defended (Grant, 1993;Grant & Guha, 1993;Robb &
Grant, 1998). Empirical studies into dominance and resource defence have demonstrated
higher foraging success for socially dominant individuals only when presented with limited
food patches that are monopolisable (Theimer, 1987;Vahl et al., 2005).
Some researchers have suggested that the integration of producer-scrounger and
resource defence theory might elucidate an interesting relationship between socially
exploitative behaviour and dominance (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Giraldeau & Dubois,
2008). Specifically, dominant individuals should benefit disproportionately if they can use
their competitive advantage to ensure that only they can use social information effectively.
Two studies (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Lee et al., 2016) have explored this hypothesis
by modelling the effects of between-individual asymmetries in competitive ability on
producer-scrounger dynamics in a group. They found that when social rank conferred
no competitive advantage to an individual—that is, resources were not monopolisable—
groups converged on basic producer-scrounger equilibria in which all individuals behave
equivalently and receive equal payoffs. In contrast, when individuals could use their social
rank to gain a competitive advantage in monopolising a resource, scrounging behaviour
was strongly associated with dominance, and dominant individuals achieved the highest
payoffs (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Lee et al., 2016). The driving force behind this pattern
was the fact that scrounging behaviour requires that a competitor is joined at a resource
in space and time, forging a causal link between the degree to which contest competition
acts and constraints on an individual’s ability to use social information (Lee et al., 2016).
However, to date there has been no attempt to test these predictions empirically, either in
the laboratory or under naturalistic conditions.
In this study, we explore a key prediction generated by the unification of producer-
scrounger and resource defence theories, namely that there should be a strong link between
social dominance and the scrounger tactic only when resources are monopolisable. We
did this by studying the natural social foraging decisions made by wild chacma baboons
(Papio ursinus) across two spatial scales that are expected to differ in the degree to which
dominant individuals can monopolise food. At the first spatial scale—the ‘patch’—resource
clumps were too large to be monopolised independently, while at the second—the ‘sub-
patch’—resource clumps were smaller and monopolisation was possible (see ‘Methods’
for further details on how these spatial scales were defined). Because our focus was on
naturalistic behaviour, we did not manipulate the information available to individuals
while foraging to manufacture a situation where joining a competitor always represented
the exclusive use of social information, which could accurately be termed scrounging
(Vickery et al., 1991). Rather, we consider the observable joining behaviours of individuals
as they foraged, representing competitive interactions fundamental to the predictions
of producer-scrounger theory (Lee et al., 2016). In this way, we explore the competitive
constraints that social rank may impose on an individual’s ability to use social information
through joining behaviour, and the relation of such constraints to resource monopolisability
in a naturalistic setting in which information use is expected to be important. We make
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 3/19
the following three predictions: (1) joining behaviour should show a strong positive
association with dominance rank at the smaller, sub-patch level but not at the larger, patch
level; (2) individuals should join those to whom they are dominant at the sub-patch level,
but there should be no systematic asymmetry in dominance when joining occurs at the
patch level; and (3) joining should be associated with competitive exclusion (i.e., resource
monopolisation) at the sub-patch level, but not at the patch level.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study site and species
Fieldwork was conducted at Tsaobis Nature Park, Namibia (22◦230S, 15◦450E), during two
three-month periods between August and October in 2012 and 2013. Two groups of chacma
baboons, hereafter referred to as troop ‘J’ (group size and compositions: NJ,2012 =54, adult
females =16, adult males =15, juveniles =23; NJ,2013 =58, adult females =18, adult
males =9, juveniles =31) and troop ‘L’ (NL,2012 =51, adult females =18, adult males
=6, juveniles =27; NL,2013 =62, adult females =19, adult males =11, juveniles =32),
were the focus of study. All baboons were individually recognisable and habituated to the
presence of observers at close proximity. Each group was followed daily from dawn until
dusk (see Huchard et al., 2009 for further information). For each year, data were collected
for all individual baboons >6 months of age (the age at which young baboons begin to
forage independently of their mother) at the start of the study period, resulting in a total
sample of 101 individuals (2012: 54 adults, 43 juveniles; 2013: 50 adults, 41 juveniles).
Differences in the sample of individuals across years were due to death, emigration, or
passing the minimum age threshold.
Chacma baboons are an ideal model system for our study, since they live in large,
stable social groups in which linear dominance hierarchies are clear (Altmann & Altmann,
1973), individuals generally feed at the same time (King & Cowlishaw, 2009), the use
of social information while foraging has been demonstrated in field-based experiments
(Carter, Torrents Ticó & Cowlishaw, 2016), and socially exploitative foraging interactions
are common (King, Isaac & Cowlishaw, 2009;Marshall et al., 2012). Furthermore, our
study troops spent approximately 80% of their foraging time during the study period in a
riparian woodland environment, characterised by large trees including Faidherbia albida,
Salvadora persica,Acacia erioloba,Acacia tortilis, and Prosopis glandulosa. Within this
feeding environment, we defined two spatial scales between which the ability of dominant
individuals to monopolise food were predicted to differ: the patch and the sub-patch.
The patch represents the scale traditionally used in foraging theory and ecology, and
is defined as a spatially discrete unit of a food resource (Wiens, 1976). Here, we refined
this definition such that the operational definition of a patch was a single tree or shrub,
or a collection of conspecific trees or shrubs growing together with a continuous canopy
separated by no more than 1 m (median surface area =156 m2, interquartile range =
28–237 m2;n=59; see Marshall et al., 2012 for further details). In contrast, the sub-patch
was defined as the area in a patch within which an individual could feed without travelling
(i.e., within arm’s reach of a stationary baboon, approximately 2.25 m2). This is equivalent
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 4/19
to the ‘feeding station’ scale that has received some attention in the foraging literature
(Kotliar & Wiens, 1990; see Searle, Hobbs & Shipley, 2005 for a review). Given the large
size of patches compared with sub-patches, dominant individuals should be able to
competitively exclude subordinate others more easily at the latter scale.
Data collection and processing
Information regarding individual social foraging decisions and interactions at each spatial
scale was recorded through focal sampling (Altmann, 1974) on Motorola ES400 Personal
Digital Assistants and Google Nexus 4 Smartphones using a customised data capture
application in the database-driven software Cybertracker v.3.317 (http://cybertracker.org).
Focal follows lasted between 15 and 30 min, and the same individual was not studied more
than once within a 6-hour period. Individuals were selected for focal observation using
a pseudorandom sampling process, which ensured even coverage across different times
of day (based on four consecutive 3-hour time blocks from 06:00 to 18:00) and different
months.
A patch entry event was recorded whenever the focal individual searched for or consumed
food in a new patch for 5 s or more. While in a patch, the focal individual could move
between sub-patches. A sub-patch entry was recorded when an individual relocated into a
new area of a patch to forage, and either remained stationary for ≥5 s while standing, or
sat for ≥1 s, to forage in this location. In this way, foraging behaviour at each spatial scale
was studied at the level of investment, since entries need not have resulted in successful
food consumption (although in almost all cases did). At each spatial scale, a specific
foraging decision was assigned to every entry event. The decision was defined as ‘produce’
if the patch or sub-patch being entered was unoccupied, and ‘join’ if occupied by a
conspecific. Note that join events at the sub-patch level need not have been preceded by
a join event at the patch level, because (1) a focal individual could enter an unoccupied
patch and subsequently be joined by others, providing opportunities for future join events
at the sub-patch level; and (2) focal follows could begin with the focal individual already
occupying a patch.
For each join event, the number and identity of individuals occupying the resource
was recorded. In cases where visibility was poor, a minimum number of occupants was
estimated and, where known, their identity recorded. Since individuals being joined could
either remain in, or be supplanted from, their patch or sub-patch, we recorded whether
or not a join event was associated with competitive exclusion. We defined supplanting,
representing competitive exclusion, as an approach-retreat interaction (Rowell, 1966;Silk et
al., 2010) at a given patch or sub-patch that resulted in the entry and exit of the approaching
and retreating individuals, respectively.
Since the size of a patch is variable, while sub-patch size is fixed, the relationship between
them is such that at the smallest, or ‘critical’, patch sizes they reach equivalence. With this
in mind, social foraging decisions were included only where a sub-patch structure could
be defined (i.e., where the occupied patch held more than one sub-patch), such that the
sub-patch always represented a smaller spatial scale nested within the patch. Study at the
sub-patch scale thus captured social foraging dynamics at a resolution higher than at the
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 5/19
patch scale, allowing us to avoid conflating processes working at the two different spatial
scales. Specifically, monopolisation of food at the patch scale always required the defence
of an area at least (but generally considerably more than) double that required at the
sub-patch scale. The data were then filtered further to exclude all ambiguous foraging
decisions that could not clearly be classified as either produce or join (<10%). A total of
801 focal hours were carried out across the two study periods on 101 individual baboons
(mean ±s.e. =7.9±0.1 h per individual), resulting in a dataset of 1861 patch entry and
5050 sub-patch entry decisions for analysis. All observers completed a period of intensive
training in the field to ensure high levels of accuracy and consistency in recognising patch
and sub-patch boundaries, entry events, foraging decisions, and competitive exclusion.
Observers were also naïve to the predictions of the study relating to associations between
joining behaviour and social dominance at the two spatial scales.
A dominance hierarchy was generated for each troop-year combination using pairwise
agonistic interactions occurring within each study period. These interactions were collected
both during focal follows and through ad libitum sampling, and were used to make actor-
receiver matrices indicating the number of agonistic interactions occurring between each
dyad in each direction. No dominance interactions occurring during foraging decisions were
included in the matrices. In addition, all interactions involving individuals not yet weaned
from their mother were excluded, because dominance asymmetry at this age is strongly
influenced by the mother’s presence and behaviour (Cheney, 1977). Each actor-receiver
matrix (N2012,J=1010; N2012,L=1,025; N2013,J=833; N2013,L=1,073) was reordered using
Matman 1.1.4 (Noldus Information Technology 2003), optimised by selecting the hierarchy
with the lowest level of conflict (i.e., minimising the number of interactions inconsistent
with the predicted hierarchy) using a heuristic search algorithm with ten thousand
randomisations. Linearity was supported for all four hierarchies (Landau’s corrected
linearity index: h0
2012,J=0.19; h0
2012,L=0.32; h0
2013,J=0.18; h0
2013,L=0.15, p<0.001 in
all cases), highlighting the rarity of interactions inconsistent with the predicted hierarchy
(n2012,J=67 (7%); n2012,L=81 (8%); n2013,J=43 (5%); n2013,L=71 (7%)). Individuals not
yet weaned were then re-entered into the appropriate dominance hierarchy based on their
maternal rank (i.e., one position below their mother, consistent with the well-documented
maternal reinforcement of offspring rank in chacma baboons; Cheney, 1977), producing
complete hierarchies that included all members of the group for each year. To control
for differences in the size of groups within and across years, all absolute ranks (ranging
from 1 to n) were standardised to between 0 (lowest rank) and 1 (highest rank) following
1−((1−r)/(1−n)), where ris the absolute rank of an individual.
Our wholly observational research adhered to the Guidelines for the Use of Animals
in Behavioural Research and Teaching (Animal Behaviour 2012 83:301–309), and our
protocols were assessed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Zoological Society
of London (BPE/0518). Our study was approved by the Ministry of Environment and
Tourism in Namibia (Research Permits 1696/2012 and 1786/2013).
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 6/19
Statistical analyses
Our analysis was divided into three sections consistent with the three main predictions
outlined above. First, we used generalised linear mixed-effects modelling (GLMM) to
explore how the relationship between social dominance and joining behaviour changed
across spatial scales due to differences in resource monopolisability. Our main prediction
was that all individuals would exhibit joining behaviour at the patch scale, but that there
would be a strong positive relationship between rank and joining at the sub-patch scale.
However, since juvenile baboons are often tolerated at feeding sites (e.g., Huchard et al.,
2013), we predicted that this positive relationship (and thus an interaction between spatial
scale and dominance rank) would only hold for adults. While differences between adults
and juveniles were not the focus of this study, it was important to include age class in our
statistical models to fully understand any relationships between resource monopolisability,
contest competition, and joining behaviour. We thus constructed our statistical model with
a three-way interaction between spatial scale (‘patch’ or ‘sub-patch’), dominance rank, and
age class (‘juvenile’ or ‘adult’). The response variable was given as a binary indicator of
the decision at each entry to either ‘produce’ or ‘join’, scored as 0 or 1, respectively. We
fit a binomial error structure to the GLMMs. Model selection was conducted by using a
likelihood ratio test (α=0.05) to judge whether the model with or without the three-way
interaction term provided the better fit to the data, and if the latter, whether those models
with or without two-way interactions between spatial scale, dominance rank, and age class
provided the better fit. Troop and year were included as control fixed effects, and were thus
retained in all models. Focal identity and focal follow number were included as random
intercepts in all models.
Second, we asked whether join ‘events’ were consistently associated with asymmetries
in social rank at each spatial scale. Our main prediction was that individuals would
consistently join others lower ranked than themselves at the sub-patch level, but would
join others regardless of rank differences at the patch level. Again, we predicted that
the relationship at the sub-patch level would not hold for juvenile individuals. To test
this second set of predictions, we used a randomisation method to compare the joining
behaviour we observed to the patterns of joining that would be expected if individuals
joined others randomly with respect to rank difference. We employed this method because
any relationship between dominance rank and joining frequency demonstrated in our first
analysis would indicate that a crude comparison of the rank difference between joining
and joined individuals could lead us to erroneous conclusions. For example, if high ranked
individuals joined more frequently than low ranked individuals at a given spatial scale, our
data would suggest that individuals on average joined others lower ranked than themselves
at this spatial scale in the case that their actual joining behaviour was random with respect to
rank difference, simply because individuals with above average social rank necessarily have
more individuals subordinate to them than dominant to them. Our observation variable
for this analysis was a binary indicator of whether the joining individual was dominant
or subordinate to the joined individual. For those events where multiple individuals were
joined in a patch (36%) or sub-patch (2%), the direction of their average rank difference
with the focal individual was used. We generated expectations of rank differences under
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 7/19
random joining behaviour with respect to rank difference by randomly resampling from
the appropriate troop only the identity of the joined individual for each observed join
event, and calculating the difference in rank between the actual joiner and this randomly
sampled individual. We repeated this process 10,000 times to generate a distribution of the
expected proportion of join events in which the joining individual would be subordinate
to the joined individual if individuals joined randomly with respect to rank difference. We
defined that our observed estimates for the proportion of events with a subordinate joiner
deviated from random expectations when they fell outside of the 95% tolerance intervals
of the random distribution. We built four sets of random distributions to which we could
compare our observational estimates: one for adult joiners and one for juvenile joiners at
both the patch and the sub-patch scale.
Third, we built a GLMM to establish whether join events at different spatial scales
were associated with differences in the competitive exclusion experienced by the joined
individual. Competitive exclusion was modelled as a binary response variable: individuals
were either supplanted from the resource or were not. Fixed effects were included as an
interaction between spatial scale and age class, and were assessed using likelihood ratio
tests as described above. Since these data were not available for patch level decisions in
2012, only decisions from 2013 were used in this analysis. Troop was included as a control
fixed effect, and so was retained in all models, and focal identity and focal follow number
were included as random intercepts. We predicted that joining behaviour in adults would
cause competitive exclusion of the joined individual at the sub-patch but not the patch
scale, and that joining behaviour in juveniles would result in lower levels of competitive
exclusion at the sub-patch scale compared with adults.
All analyses were conducted in R version 3.0.2. using the lme4 package (Bates et al.,
2013;R Core Team, 2013).
RESULTS
Dominance and social foraging decisions at different spatial scales
At the patch scale, joining behaviour was common regardless of social rank (Table 1;
Fig. 1A), consistent with our predictions. Although there was some increase in joining
with social dominance in adults, even the lowest ranked individuals entered occupied
patches approximately 55% (95% confidence intervals: 41% and 67%) of the time. While
joining was in general much less common at the sub-patch scale, there was a strong positive
relationship in adults between dominance and joining behaviour (twice that at the patch
scale) that was consistent with our predictions (Table 1;Fig. 1B). The lowest ranked adults
had around a 1% probability (95% confidence intervals: 0% and 2%) of joining when
entering a new sub-patch, while mid-ranked and top-ranked adults did so approximately
4% (95% confidence intervals: 3% and 6%) and 11% (95% confidence intervals: 8% and
17%) of the time, respectively.
As predicted, the effects of social rank on the probability of joining were weaker
in juveniles, and this held across both spatial scales such that there was in general no
relationship between dominance and joining frequency in juveniles (Table 1;Fig. 1). In
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 8/19
Figure 1 Predicted relationship between the probability of joining behaviour and dominance rank
at the (A) patch and (B) sub-patch level. For both panels, solid and dashed lines represent the predicted
values for adults and juveniles, respectively. Shaded green (adults) and purple (juveniles) regions are
bounded by upper and lower 95% confidence intervals. Note the difference in scale of the two y-axes,
reflecting the much lower levels of joining across all individuals at the sub-patch compared with patch
scale.
Table 1 Factors predicting the probability of joining behaviour and competitive exclusion associated with joining.
Response NFixed effect βs.e. χ2p
Probability of joining 6,911 Intercept −0.05 0.26
Spatial scale (Sub-patch) −4.51 0.28
Rank 1.10 0.38
Age class (Juvenile) 1.57 0.30
Troop (L) 0.20 0.15
Year (2013) 0.27 0.12
Spatial scale (Sub-patch) * Rank 1.18 0.37 10.30 0.001
Rank * Age class (Juvenile) −1.68 0.54 9.03 0.003
Spatial scale (Sub-patch) * Age class (Juvenile) −0.68 0.21 10.40 0.001
Probability of competitive exclusion 385 Intercept −2.44 0.47
Spatial scale (Sub-patch) 4.55 0.65 164.27 <0.001
Age class (Juvenile) −1.61 0.47 14.06 <0.001
Troop −0.89 0.44
Notes.
Model reference categories: Spatial scale (Patch), Age class (Adult), Troop (J), Year (2012).
addition, juveniles were on average more likely than adults to join at the patch scale,
but this primarily reflected low-ranking juveniles joining much more frequently than
similarly ranked adults when entering a new patch. This effect was somewhat weakened
at the sub-patch scale: low-ranked juveniles joined more frequently when entering a new
sub-patch than similarly ranked adults but the pattern was reversed for high-ranked
juveniles.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 9/19
Combined, our results reflect support for the three possible two-way interactions
between spatial scale, dominance rank, and age class (Table 1). Our lack of support for
a three-way interaction between these effects (likelihood ratio test: χ2=0.70, p=0.41)
reflects the fact that there was some increase in joining with social rank in adults at both
spatial scales. This meant that a stronger effect of dominance in adults at the sub-patch
versus patch level, combined with no clear relationship between joining frequency and
social rank in juveniles at either spatial scale, was captured by a general (i.e., not age
class-specific) increase in joining with dominance at the sub-patch level and a general (i.e.,
not spatial scale-specific) decrease in the effect of dominance on joining in juveniles.
Social constraints on joining behaviour at different spatial scales
At the patch scale, there was no evidence that adult individuals joined others systematically
higher or lower ranked than themselves compared with the rank asymmetries expected
if individuals joined others randomly with respect to their rank difference (Fig. 2A,
Npatch,adult =313). Although adults were less likely to join individuals dominant to them
at this spatial scale, this could be explained under joining behaviour that was random with
respect to rank difference by the finding in our first analysis that the frequency of joining
behaviour increased slightly with increasing rank (Fig. 1A).
At the sub-patch scale, adult individuals joined individuals dominant to themselves in 9%
of join events, and so were less likely to do so than would have been expected if individuals
joined others randomly with respect to rank difference (Fig. 2B;Nsub-patch, adult =154) and
given the fact that high ranked individuals are much more likely than low ranked individuals
to join others at this spatial scale (Fig. 1B). We thus found support for our prediction that
adult individuals would consistently join others lower ranked than themselves at the
sub-patch level.
Comparisons between observed and random joining behaviour for juveniles were
broadly similar to those for adults. At the patch scale, the rank asymmetries at observed join
events did not deviate from expectations under joining behaviour random with respect to
rank difference (Fig. 2C;Npatch,juvenile =349). At the sub-patch scale, we observed juveniles
joining others to whom they were subordinate in 49% of cases. Although this shows that
juveniles were frequently able to join others dominant to themselves, they nonetheless
did so less often than expected under random joining (Fig. 2D;Nsub-patch, juvenile =147).
Despite the fact that our first analysis found no relationship between dominance and
joining frequency for juveniles at the sub-patch level (Fig. 1B), the random distribution at
this spatial scale reflects the fact that juveniles are usually below average in rank.
Competitive exclusion at different spatial scales
Joining caused competitive exclusion at the sub-patch scale much more than it did at the
patch scale (Table 1). In adults, joining at the patch scale was associated with competitive
exclusion in 9% of cases. This figure increased to 79% at the sub-patch scale. Joining by
juveniles was less likely to result in competitive exclusion at both spatial scales (patch: 3%;
sub-patch: 51%).
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 10/19
Figure 2 Comparison of observed dominance asymmetry during join events with simulated joining
behaviour that is random with respect to rank difference. Probability density distributions show expecta-
tions for the proportion of join events in which the joining individuals would be subordinate to the joined
individual if their behaviour was random with respect to the rank of the joined individual. The distribu-
tions are for adults (A) and juveniles (B) at the patch level, and adults (C) and juveniles (D) at the sub-
patch level, generated through 10,000 iterations of randomly selecting the individual to be joined at each
join event. For each distribution, dotted vertical lines indicate the 95% tolerance intervals and solid verti-
cal lines indicate our observed value.
DISCUSSION
We provide empirical evidence that joining behaviour should be more strongly related to
social rank when the competitive asymmetries associated with dominance are stronger,
in support of previous theoretical predictions (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Lee et al., 2016).
We show that changes in resource monopolisability can mediate this shift in competitive
asymmetry through changes in competitive exclusion at different spatial scales. At the
larger patch scale, adults could join others regardless of any differences in their social
ranks, and did so frequently. At the smaller sub-patch scale, joining was a rarer event—
likely reflecting the higher finder’s share at this spatial scale (Vickery et al., 1991)—but also
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 11/19
represented a more exclusive tactic, almost non-existent in the lowest-ranking individuals
but increasing in probability with social dominance. When resources are monopolisable,
socially subordinate individuals may thus be constrained in their ability to exploit available
social information when its use requires joining behaviour, as is assumed by producer-
scrounger theory. Since the size of a resource is expected to influence its economic
defensibility (Grant, 1993), our study supports calls to unify producer-scrounger and
resource defence theory in order to better understand the relationship between dominance
and socially exploitative behaviours (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Dubois, Giraldeau & Grant,
2003;Dubois & Giraldeau, 2005;Giraldeau & Dubois, 2008). However, our study also
highlights two areas in which our understanding of the evolutionary ecology of animal
information use is still lacking, requiring further theoretical and empirical developments.
First, we showed a strong increase in the frequency of joining at higher ranks for adults
at the sub-patch scale, where single individuals could use their dominance to exclude
competitors. However, the pattern we observed was weaker than that predicted by Barta
& Giraldeau (1998), who suggested a complete absence of scrounging in all but the most
dominant individuals when resources could be effectively monopolised. We found that
even middle- and low-ranking adults can scrounge from others, provided that the others
they join are even lower ranked than themselves. The failure of the producer-scrounger
model to predict this pattern likely reflects two of its assumptions. Specifically, the model
is built such that any individual playing scrounger can access all the discoveries of others,
since (1) resources are assumed to be so rare that they are discovered one-at-a-time and (2)
scroungers can access perfect social information and so detect each discovery (Vickery et al.,
1991;Giraldeau & Caraco, 2000; cf. Ohtsuka & Toquenaga, 2009). When resources can be
monopolised, these conditions mean that only the highest-ranked individuals will benefit
from scrounging behaviour. However, in many groups of social foragers multiple patches
can be discovered at the same time (and simultaneous discoveries or options will likely be the
norm for many other types of resource too). As such, scrounging individuals will be unable
to access all resource discoveries even if they have perfect social information (Ohtsuka &
Toquenaga, 2009;Afshar & Giraldeau, 2014). Furthermore, since individuals within social
groups are unlikely to be in close proximity at all times (Krause & Ruxton, 2002; for this
study population, see Cowlishaw, 1999;Castles et al., 2014), scrounging individuals are
unlikely to possess perfect social information regarding all discoveries occurring at the
group level (Barta, Flynn & Giraldeau, 1997;Hirsch, 2007), and any temporal and energetic
costs associated with scrounging may vary both within and between individuals.
Under the conditions of simultaneous discoveries and imperfect access to social
information, the most dominant individuals will be unable to monopolise all resource
discoveries, regardless of the economic defensibility of single resource patches. Instead,
as shown here, the difference in rank between individuals across a hierarchy should play
an important role in mediating scrounging behaviour. This finding is consistent with the
predictions of a recent game theoretic model (Lee et al., 2016), which proposes that if the
highest ranking animals in a group do not detect a particular patch discovery, and/or are
occupied at another discovery, then middle and lower ranking animals can benefit from
scrounging, provided that the producers from whom they are scrounging are lower ranking
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 12/19
than themselves. Our result that joining at the sub-patch level still increases with social
rank likely reflects the fact that higher ranked individuals have more competitors who are
subordinate to them. The most dominant individual should thus be unconstrained in its
ability to act upon opportunities to scrounge, and constraints should increase down the
dominance hierarchy. Furthermore, theory predicts that if more dominant individuals are
more effective at monopolising other’s resource discoveries, then they should continue
to scrounge even under conditions that drive lower-ranked individuals to switch to the
producer tactic (e.g., when the finder’s share is large; Lee et al., 2016). Individuals may
also benefit from positioning themselves so as to maximise scrounging opportunities
(Barta, Flynn & Giraldeau, 1997;Di Bitetti & Janson, 2001;Hirsch, 2007). Since dominant
individuals may be better able to secure more central positions in the group, this may
improve their ability to detect, and increase their proximity to, the discoveries of others
(Barta, Flynn & Giraldeau, 1997;King, Isaac & Cowlishaw, 2009), and further reinforce the
effects of dominance on scrounging behaviour.
Second, we showed that the relationship between social dominance and joining is
strongly influenced by age class. In stark contrast to adults, juvenile baboons showed no
general relationship between social dominance and joining, particularly at the sub-patch
level. While an adult baboon’s ability to join competitors at this spatial scale depended
strongly on their relative dominance, juveniles were less constrained by their social status.
Indeed, juvenile behaviour accounted for almost all instances where a subordinate joined
a higher-ranking individual at the sub-patch level. There are two likely explanations for
this pattern. Firstly, rank acquisition in chacma baboons is mediated primarily through
maternal reinforcement. Specifically, a mother will use aggressive behaviours to establish
the dominance of her developing offspring over others subordinate to her (Cheney, 1977;
Holekamp & Smale, 1991;Lea et al., 2014). Consequently, social rank during early life
might be particularly sensitive to context, such that social interactions between juveniles
may only reflect rank differences between their mothers in the presence of the dominant
mother. In the absence of the dominant mother, older but ‘subordinate’ juveniles might
use their larger size to join younger, smaller, but more ‘dominant’ competitors. Secondly,
there is evidence that juvenile baboons, like the juveniles of several other primate species
(Janson, 1985), are more frequently tolerated at feeding sites than adults. The presence
of co-foraging juveniles may impose only a minimal direct cost to adults, but permitting
close kin access to resources may provide inclusive fitness benefits. In particular, father-
offspring relationships in chacma baboons afford juveniles access to high-quality feeding
sites (Huchard et al., 2013). Such toleration may mean that, in addition to better access to
monopolisable resources, low-ranked juveniles may not be constrained in their ability to
use social information in the same way that similarly ranked adults will be. Our findings
do not provide strong evidence in support of either tolerance or juvenile rank instability
in disrupting the expected positive relationship between social rank and joining behaviour
when resources are monopolisable. However, we might expect juvenile rank stability to
disrupt rank asymmetries but still to involve competitive exclusion when resources are
monopolisable, while toleration of juveniles should result in reduced levels of competitive
exclusion. The fact that the probability of competitive exclusion was reduced for juveniles
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 13/19
compared with adults at the sub-patch scale might suggest that toleration is playing a bigger
role than juvenile rank instability in our observations.
Our study demonstrates the way in which the monopolisability of resources may drive
social constraints on a subordinate individual’s ability to use joining behaviour to access
them. We also show that such constraints may be relaxed in juveniles. Since resources
generally show some uncertainty in their distribution through space and time, information
use is likely to play a key role in resource acquisition. Our study illustrates how competitive
processes associated with dominance might facilitate or constrain an individual’s ability
to benefit from collecting social information when its use requires joining behaviour,
as in the producer-scrounger framework (Barta & Giraldeau, 1998;Lee et al., 2016). An
important step in future research will be to develop frameworks that simultaneously
consider how resource distributions underpin (1) the strength and type of competition
between individuals, (2) the benefits of collecting information socially versus personally,
and (3) the rate at which such information becomes out-dated. This approach will elucidate
the environmental conditions that should generate interdependencies between contest
competition and social information use by highlighting when social information use
should be dependent on joining behaviour at a resource. These insights will allow better
characterisation of the ways in which competition can modulate relationships between an
individual’s ability to use information and its access to resources, and the implications of
such modulations for the dynamics of natural populations.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to say a big thank you to James Ounsley, Cassandra Raby, Rebecca Boulton,
Matthis Petit, Eveline Rijksen, Miles Keighley, Maddie Castles, Stef Oberprieler, Alice
Baniel, Stella Diamant, Katie Hatton, Julien Collet, Chris Smith, Boris Granovskiy, Caitlin
Miller, Alecia Carter, Elise Huchard, Hannah Wilmot, and Willem Odendaal for their work
and support in the field. Thanks also to Tim Coulson, Marcus Rowcliffe, Ben Sheldon,
David Macdonald, Daniel van der Post, Andrés López-Sepulcre, Alexander Weiss, and
three anonymous reviewers for constructive comments and discussions. Permission to
work at the field site was kindly granted by Tsaobis Nature Park, the Wittreich and Snyman
families, and the Ministry of Lands and Resettlement. We also thank the Gobabeb Training
and Research Centre for affiliation in Namibia. This study is a publication of the ZSL
Institute of Zoology’s Tsaobis Baboon Project.
ADDITIONAL INFORMATION AND DECLARATIONS
Funding
This work was supported by a Natural Environment Research Council Quota Studentship
(NE/J500409/1) awarded to AEGL. There was no additional external funding received for
this study. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision
to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 14/19
Grant Disclosures
The following grant information was disclosed by the authors:
Natural Environment Research Council Quota Studentship: NE/J500409/1.
Competing Interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Author Contributions
•Alexander E.G. Lee conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments,
analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper,
prepared figures and/or tables.
•Guy Cowlishaw conceived and designed the experiments, contributed reagents/materi-
als/analysis tools, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Animal Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body
and any reference numbers):
Our wholly observational research adhered to the Guidelines for the Use of Animals
in Behavioural Research and Teaching (Animal Behaviour 2012 83:301–309), and our
protocols were assessed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Zoological Society
of London (BPE/0518).
Field Study Permissions
The following information was supplied relating to field study approvals (i.e., approving
body and any reference numbers):
Our study was approved by the Ministry of Environment and Tourism in Namibia
(Research Permits 1696/2012 and 1786/2013).
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
All data and code associated with this research are freely available in the Oxford
University Research Archive (DOI: 10.5287/bodleian:GPr2aK1ng).
Supplemental Information
Supplemental information for this article can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/
peerj.3462#supplemental-information.
REFERENCES
Afshar M, Giraldeau L-A. 2014. A unified modelling approach for producer–
scrounger games in complex ecological conditions. Animal Behaviour 96:167–176
DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.07.022.
Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour
49:227–265 DOI 10.1163/156853974X00534.
Altmann SA, Altmann J. 1973. Baboon ecology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 15/19
Barnard CJ. 1984. Producers and scroungers: strategies of exploitation and parasitism.
London: Chapman & Hall.
Barnard CJ, Sibly RM. 1981. Producers and scroungers: a general model and its
application to captive flocks of house sparrows. Animal Behaviour 29:543–550
DOI 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80117-0.
Barta Z, Flynn R, Giraldeau L-A. 1997. Geometry for a selfish foraging group: a
genetic algorithm approach. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: : Biological Sciences
264:1233–1238 DOI 10.1098/rspb.1997.0170.
Barta Z, Giraldeau L-A. 1998. The effect of dominance hierarchy on the use of alternative
foraging tactics: a phenotype-limited producing-scrounging game. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology 42:217–223 DOI 10.1007/s002650050433.
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2013. lme4: linear mixed-effects models using
Eigen and S4. R package version 1.0-5.
Beauchamp G. 2001. Consistency and flexibility in the scrounging behaviour of zebra
finches. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79:540–544 DOI 10.1139/cjz-79-3-540.
Beauchamp G. 2006. Phenotypic correlates of scrounging behavior in Zebra finches: role
of foraging efficiency and dominance. Ethology 112:873–878
DOI 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01241.x.
Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K. 2002. Scrounging tactics in free-ranging ravens, Corvus corax.
Ethology 108:993–1009 DOI 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002.00832.x.
Carter A, Torrents Ticó M, Cowlishaw G. 2016. Sequential phenotypic constraints on
social information use in wild baboons. eLife 5:e13125 DOI 10.7554/eLife.13125.
Castles M, Heinsohn R, Marshall HH, Lee AEG, Cowlishaw G, Carter AJ. 2014. Social
networks created with different techniques are not comparable. Animal Behaviour
96:59–67 DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.07.023.
Cheney DL. 1977. The acquisition of rank and the development of reciprocal alliances
among free-ranging immature baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
2:303–318 DOI 10.1007/BF00299742.
Coolen I, Giraldeau L-A, Vickery W. 2007. Scrounging behavior regulates population
dynamics. Oikos 116:533–539 DOI 10.1111/j.2006.0030-1299.15213.x.
Cowlishaw G. 1999. Ecological and social determinants of spacing behaviour in desert
baboon groups. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 45:67–77
DOI 10.1007/s002650050540.
Danchin E, Giraldeau L-A, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. 2004. Public information: from nosy
neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487–491 DOI 10.1126/science.1098254.
Di Bitetti MS, Janson CH. 2001. Social foraging and the finder’s share in capuchin
monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour 62:47–56 DOI 10.1006/anbe.2000.1730.
Dubois F, Giraldeau L-A. 2005. Fighting for resources: the economics of defense and
appropriation. Ecology 86:3–11 DOI 10.1890/04-0566.
Dubois F, Giraldeau L-A, Grant JWA. 2003. Resource defense in a group-foraging
context. Behavioral Ecology 14:2–9 DOI 10.1093/beheco/14.1.2.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 16/19
Fawcett TW, Hamblin S, Giraldeau L-A. 2013. Exposing the behavioral gam-
bit: the evolution of learning and decision rules. Behavioral Ecology 24:2–11
DOI 10.1093/beheco/ars085.
Giraldeau L-A, Caraco T. 2000. Social foraging theory. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Giraldeau L-A, Dubois F. 2008. Social foraging and the study of exploitative behavior.
Advances in the Study of Behavior 38:59–104 DOI 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00002-8.
Grant JWA. 1993. Whether or not to defend? The influence of resource distribution.
Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 23:137–153
DOI 10.1080/10236249309378862.
Grant JWA, Guha RT. 1993. Spatial clumping of food increases its monopolization
and defense by convict cichlids, Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum. Behavioral Ecology
4:293–296 DOI 10.1093/beheco/4.4.293.
Hammerstein P. 1981. The role of asymmetries in animal contests. Animal Behaviour
29:193–205 DOI 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80166-2.
Hirsch BT. 2007. Costs and benefits of within-group spatial position: a feeding competi-
tion model. The Quarterly Review of Biology 82:9–27 DOI 10.1086/511657.
Holekamp KE, Smale L. 1991. Dominance acquisition during mammalian social
development: the ‘‘inheritance’’ of maternal rank. American Zoologist 317:306–317
DOI 10.1093/icb/31.2.306.
Huchard E, Alvergne A, Féjan D, Knapp LA, Cowlishaw G, Raymond M. 2009.
More than friends? Behavioural and genetic aspects of heterosexual associa-
tions in wild chacma baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64:769–781
DOI 10.1007/s00265-009-0894-3.
Huchard E, Charpentier MJ, Marshall HH, King AJ, Knapp LA, Cowlishaw G. 2013.
Paternal effects on access to resources in a promiscuous primate society. Behavioral
Ecology 24:229–236 DOI 10.1093/beheco/ars158.
Janson CH. 1985. Aggressive competition and individual food consumption in wild
brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
18:125–138 DOI 10.1007/BF00299041.
Kaufmann J. 1983. On the defnintions and functions of dominance and territoriality.
Biology Review 58:1–20 DOI 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1983.tb00379.x.
King AJ, Cowlishaw G. 2009. All together now: behavioural synchrony in baboons.
Animal Behaviour 78:1381–1387 DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.009.
King AJ, Isaac NJB, Cowlishaw G. 2009. Ecological, social, and reproductive factors
shape producer-scrounger dynamics in baboons. Behavioral Ecology 20:1039–1049
DOI 10.1093/beheco/arp095.
Koops MA, Giraldeau L-A. 1996. Producer–scrounger foraging games in starlings: a
test of rate-maximizing and risk-sensitive models. Animal Behaviour 51:773–783
DOI 10.1006/anbe.1996.0082.
Kotliar NB, Wiens JA. 1990. Multiple scales of patchiness and patch structure: a
hierarchical framework for the study of heterogeneity. Oikos 59:253–260.
Krause J, Ruxton GD. 2002. Living in groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 17/19
Kurvers RHJM, Van Oers K, Nolet BA, Jonker RM, Van Wieren SE, Prins HHT,
Ydenberg RC. 2010. Personality predicts the use of social information. Ecology
Letters 13:829–837 DOI 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01473.x.
Lea AJ, Learn NH, Theus MJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2014. Complex sources of variance
in female dominance rank in a nepotistic society. Animal Behaviour 94:87–99
DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.019.
Lee AEG, Ounsley JP, Coulson T, Rowcliffe JM, Cowlishaw G. 2016. Information use
and resource competition: an integrative framework. Proceedings of the Royal Society
B: : Biological Sciences 283:20152550 DOI 10.1098/rspb.2015.2550.
Lendvai ÁZ, Liker A, Barta Z. 2006. The effects of energy reserves and dominance on the
use of social-foraging strategies in the house sparrow. Animal Behaviour 72:747–752
DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.032.
Liker A, Barta Z. 2002. The effects of dominance on social foraging tactic use in house
sparrows. Behaviour 139:1061–1076 DOI 10.1163/15685390260337903.
Łomnicki A. 2009. Scramble and contest competition, unequal resource allocation, and
resource monopolization as determinants of population dynamics. Evolutionary
Ecology Research 11:371–380.
Marshall HH, Carter AJ, Coulson T, Rowcliffe JM, Cowlishaw G. 2012. Exploring
foraging decisions in a social primate using discrete-choice models. The American
Naturalist 180:481–495 DOI 10.1086/667587.
Maynard Smith J, Parker GA. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal Behaviour
24:159–175 DOI 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80110-8.
McCormack JE, Jablonski PG, Brown JL. 2007. Producer-scrounger roles and joining
based on dominance in a free-living group of Mexican jays (Aphelocoma ultrama-
rina). Behaviour 144:967–982 DOI 10.1163/156853907781492717.
McNamara JM, Green RF, Olsson O. 2006. Bayes’ theorem and its applications in animal
behaviour. Oikos 112:243–251 DOI 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.14228.x.
Morand-Ferron J, Giraldeau L-A, Lefebvre L. 2007. Wild Carib grackles play a producer
scrounger game. Behavioral Ecology 18:916–921 DOI 10.1093/beheco/arm058.
Mottley K, Giraldeau L-A. 2000. Experimental evidence that group foragers can converge
on predicted producer-scrounger equilibria. Animal Behaviour 60:341–350
DOI 10.1006/anbe.2000.1474.
Ohtsuka Y, Toquenaga Y. 2009. The patch distributed producer-scrounger game. Journal
of Theoretical Biology 260:261–266 DOI 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.06.002.
Parker GA. 1974. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of
Theoretical Biology 47:223–243 DOI 10.1016/0022-5193(74)90111-8.
Parker GA. 1982. Phenotype-limited evolutionarily stable strategies. In: King’s College
Sociobiology Group, ed. Current problems in sociobiology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press, 173–201.
R Core Team. 2013. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R
Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at http:// r-project.org/ .
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 18/19
Rieucau G, Giraldeau L-A. 2011. Exploring the costs and benefits of social information
use: an appraisal of current experimental evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366:949–957 DOI 10.1098/rstb.2010.0325.
Robb SE, Grant JWA. 1998. Interactions between the spatial and temporal clumping
of food affect the intensity of aggression in Japanese medaka. Animal Behaviour
56:29–34 DOI 10.1006/anbe.1998.0735.
Robinette Ha R, Ha JC. 2003. Effects of ecology and prey characteristics on the use of
alternative social foraging tactics in crows, Corvus caurinus. Animal Behaviour
66:309–316 DOI 10.1006/anbe.2003.2182.
Rowell TE. 1966. Hierarchy in the organization of a captive baboon group. Animal
Behaviour 14:430–443 DOI 10.1016/S0003-3472(66)80042-8.
Searle KR, Hobbs NT, Shipley LA. 2005. Should I stay or should I go? Patch
departure decisions by herbivores at multiple scales. Oikos 111:417–424
DOI 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2005.13918.x.
Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wit-
tig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2010. Strong and consistent social bonds
enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology 20:1359–1361
DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067.
Stahl J, Tolsma PH, Loonen MJJE, Drent RH. 2001. Subordinates explore but dominants
profit: resource competition in high Arctic barnacle goose flocks. Animal Behaviour
61:257–264 DOI 10.1006/anbe.2000.1564.
Teichroeb JA, White MMJ, Chapman CA. 2015. Vervet (Chlorocebus pygery-
thrus) intragroup spatial positioning: dominants trade-off predation risk for
increased food acquisition. International Journal of Primatology 36:154–176
DOI 10.1007/s10764-015-9818-4.
Theimer TC. 1987. The effect of seed dispersion on the foraging success of dominant and
subordinate dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis. Animal Behaviour 35:1883–1890
DOI 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80081-7.
Vahl WK, Lok T, Van der Meer J, Piersma T, Weissing FJ. 2005. Spatial clumping of
food and social dominance affect interference competition among ruddy turnstones.
Behavioral Ecology 16:834–844 DOI 10.1093/beheco/ari067.
Valone TJ. 1989. Group foraging, public information, and patch estimation. Oikos
56:357–363 DOI 10.2307/3565621.
Valone TJ. 2006. Are animals capable of Bayesian updating? An empirical review. Oikos
112:252–259 DOI 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2006.13465.x.
Valone TJ, Templeton JJ. 2002. Public information for the assessment of quality: a
widespread social phenomenon. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences 357:1549–1557 DOI 10.1098/rstb.2002.1064.
Vickery WL, Giraldeau L-A, Templeton JJ, Kramer DL, Chapman CA. 1991. Pro-
ducers, scroungers, and group foraging. The American Naturalist 137:847–863
DOI 10.1006/anbe.1996.0014.
Wiens JA. 1976. Population responses to patchy environments. Annual Review of Ecology
and Systematics 7:81–120 DOI 10.1146/annurev.es.07.110176.000501.
Lee and Cowlishaw (2017), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.3462 19/19